Kathrin C. J. Eschmann's research while affiliated with Cardiff University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists the scientific contributions of an author, who either does not have a ResearchGate profile, or has not yet added these contributions to their profile.

It was automatically created by ResearchGate to create a record of this author's body of work. We create such pages to advance our goal of creating and maintaining the most comprehensive scientific repository possible. In doing so, we process publicly available (personal) data relating to the author as a member of the scientific community.

If you're a ResearchGate member, you can follow this page to keep up with this author's work.

If you are this author, and you don't want us to display this page anymore, please let us know.

Publications (11)


Curiosity Satisfaction Increases Event-related Potentials Sensitive to Reward
  • Article

January 2024

·

16 Reads

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

·

Axel Mecklinger

·

Kathrin C J Eschmann

·

[...]

·

Matthias J Gruber

Successful learning depends on various factors such as depth of processing, motivation, or curiosity about information. A strong drive to learn something or the expectation of receiving a reward can be crucial to enhance learning. However, the influence of curiosity on the processing of new information and its similarity with reward processing is not well understood. This study examined whether states of curiosity influence specific ERPs associated with reward processing and whether these ERPs are related with later memory benefits. In an initial screening phase, participants indicated their curiosity and confidence in prior knowledge about answers to various trivia questions. In a subsequent study phase, we targeted different time windows related to reward processing during the presentation of trivia answers containing the reward positivity (RewP; 250-350 msec), the P3 (250-500 msec), and the late-positive-potential (600-1000 msec). In a following surprise memory test, we found that participants recalled more high- than low-curiosity answers. The RewP, P3, and late-positive-potential showed greater positive mean amplitudes for high compared with low curiosity, reflecting increased reward processing. In addition, we found that the RewP and the P3 showed more positive mean amplitudes for later recalled compared with later forgotten answers, but curiosity did not modulate this encoding-related results. These findings support the view that the satisfaction of curiosity resembles reward processing, indicated by ERPs.

Share

Figure 1. Experimental Procedure. (A) In the screening phase, trivia questions were presented and evaluated by participants according to their knowledge confidence and perceived level of curiosity without reading the corresponding answers. (B) In the following study phase, participants are presented with a subset of these questions again and eleven seconds later with their corresponding answers. Between questions and answers, faces were presented, which were not investigated in the present study. (C) At last, participants were asked to recall all answers in a surprise memory test. The EEG was recorded during the study phase and ERPs were computed time-looked to the onset of the answer.
Curiosity as an amplifier of reward-related feedback processing
  • Preprint
  • File available

May 2023

·

112 Reads

Successful learning depends on various factors such as depth of processing, motivation, or curiosity about information. A strong drive to learn something or the expectation of receiving a reward can be crucial to enhance learning. However, the influence of curiosity on the processing of information-related feedback and its similarity with reward processing is not well understood. This study examined whether states of curiosity influence specific event-related-potentials (ERPs) associated with reward processing and whether these ERPs are related with later memory benefits. In an initial screening phase, participants indicated their curiosity and confidence about answers to various trivia questions. In a subsequent study phase, we targeted different time windows related to feedback processing during the presentation of trivia answers containing the Reward-Positivity (RewP; 250 - 350 ms), the P3 (250 - 500 ms), and the Late-Positive-Potential (LPP; 600 - 1000 ms). In a delayed surprise memory test, we found that participants recalled more high-than low-curiosity answers. The RewP, P3 and LPP showed greater positive mean amplitudes for high compared to low curiosity, reflecting increased feedback processing. In addition, we found that the RewP and the P3 showed more positive mean amplitudes for later recalled compared to later forgotten answers, but curiosity did not modulate this encoding-related finding. These findings support the view that curiosity can act as an amplifier of reward-related feedback processing and opens the investigation of further curiosity-related ERP studies.

Download

Figure 2. Coronal, sagittal, and axial view of the ROIs from the PACE framework that were used for intrinsic functional connectivity analysis. The ventral tegmental area (VTA) is displayed in dark blue, the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) in bright blue, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) in dark green, the hippocampus in bright green, and the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) in grey. The figure was created with the multi-image analysis GUI Mango (ric.uthscsa.edu/mango).
Intrinsic functional connections determine how curiosity and prediction errors enhance memory

April 2023

·

110 Reads

Individuals differ in the way they seek information, acquire knowledge, and form memories. Neural fingerprints of intrinsic functional connectivity distinguish between individuals and predict inter-individual differences in task performance. Both curiosity - the desire to acquire new information - and information prediction errors (IPEs) - the mismatch between information and previous expectations - enhance memory but differ considerably between individuals. The present study assessed whether inter-individual differences in functional connectivity measured using resting-state fMRI determine the extent to which individuals benefit from memory-enhancing effects of curiosity and IPEs. We found a double dissociation between individual differences in mesolimbic functional connectivity, which accounted for curiosity-driven but not IPE-related memory enhancements, and individual differences in cingulo-hippocampal functional connectivity, which predicted IPE-driven but not curiosity-related memory enhancements. These novel findings on how inter-individual differences in dissociable intrinsic functional networks determine memory enhancements stress the need to account for these differences in theoretical frameworks of curiosity and memory.


Overview of the neurofeedback training schedule. Neurofeedback consisted of fives minutes of resting EEG followed by six 5-minute training blocks. Training-induced enhancement in flow experience and performances in a finger tapping and an n-back task were investigated from one pretraining to two posttraining sessions
Neurofeedback training results over all participants and NFT scores for responders and non-responders. (a) Overall, participants were able to increase their FM theta activity relative to the baseline during NFT. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean. (b) Participants’ NFT score, that is, the percentage change of FM theta activity from the first two training blocks of different upregulation strategy usage to the remaining four training blocks of preferred strategy usage, was used to determine participants’ NFT success. Participants with a NFT score greater than zero were classified as responders, whereas participants with a NFT score smaller than zero were classified as non-responders. Thus, the NFT score reflects the ability to use the chosen strategy to increase FM theta activity successfully
Training-induced changes in motor performance and flow experience. (a) Motor performance increased from pretest to posttest and to follow-up session for all participants as well as for responders relative to non-responders. (b) Participants’ NFT score, that is, the percentage change of FM theta activity from the first two training blocks to the remaining four training blocks, significantly predicted pre- to posttest enhancement of motor performance. (c) Global flow experience during finger tapping increased across all pre- and posttraining sessions but differed between responders and non-responders only at posttest. (d) The NFT score significantly predicted pre- to posttest flow enhancement−even if motor performance enhancement and joy felt after NFT was controlled for. (e) Responders indicated to have clearer goals and a greater autotelic experience compared to non-responders during finger tapping at posttest. (f) Participants with a smaller FM theta change during NFT showed a greater flow enhancement from posttest to follow-up session. However, this relationship was not significant when controlling for respective motor performance enhancement. Error bars indicate standard error of the overall and group means. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001
Transfer to cognitive control measured with a visual n-back task. (a) Pr scores in the 2-back condition increased from pretest to posttest but remained constant from posttest to the follow-up measurement. (b) Reaction times in the 2-back condition and (c) in the 0-back condition decreased from pretest to posttest but remained constant between posttest and follow-up measurement. There were no differences between responders and non-responders, indicating that performance enhancements are solely based on task repetition. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001
Theta Neurofeedback Training Supports Motor Performance and Flow Experience

September 2022

·

293 Reads

·

9 Citations

Journal of Cognitive Enhancement

Flow is defined as a cognitive state that is associated with a feeling of automatic and effortless control, enabling peak performance in highly challenging situations. In sports, flow can be enhanced by mindfulness training, which has been associated with frontal theta activity (4-8 Hz). Moreover, frontal-midline theta oscillations were shown to subserve control processes in a large variety of cognitive tasks. Based on previous theta neurofeedback training studies, which revealed that one training session is sufficient to enhance motor performance, the present study investigated whether one 30-minute session of frontal-midline theta neurofeedback training (1) enhances flow experience additionally to motor performance in a finger tapping task, and (2) transfers to cognitive control processes in an n -back task. Participants, who were able to successfully upregulate their theta activity during neurofeedback training (responders), showed better motor performance and flow experience after training than participants, who did not enhance their theta activity (non-responders). Across all participants, increase of theta activity during training was associated with motor performance enhancement from pretest to posttest irrespective of pre-training performance. Interestingly, theta training gains were also linked to the increase of flow experience, even when corresponding increases in motor performance were controlled for. Results for the n -back task were not significant. Even though these findings are mainly correlational in nature and additional flow-promoting influences need to be investigated, the present findings suggest that frontal-midline theta neurofeedback training is a promising tool to support flow experience with additional relevance for performance enhancement.



Multiple regression of curiosity and anxiety measured during the COVID-19 pandemic positively predicting the frequency of real-life information seeking during the first month of lockdown.
Curiosity and Mesolimbic Functional Connectivity Drive Information Seeking in Real Life

August 2022

·

74 Reads

·

8 Citations

Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience

Curiosity reflects an individual’s intrinsic motivation to seek information in order to close information gaps. In laboratory-based experiments, both curiosity and information seeking have been associated with enhanced neural dynamics in the mesolimbic dopaminergic circuit. However, it is unclear whether curiosity and dopaminergic dynamics drive information seeking in real life. We investigated (i) whether curiosity predicts different characteristics of real-life information seeking and (ii) whether functional connectivity within the mesolimbic dopaminergic circuit is associated with information seeking outside the laboratory. Up to 15 months before the COVID-19 pandemic, curiosity and anxiety questionnaires, and a 10-minute resting-state fMRI session were conducted. In a follow-up survey early during the COVID-19 pandemic, participants repeated the questionnaires and completed an additional questionnaire about their COVID-19-related information seeking. Individual differences in curiosity but not anxiety were positively associated with the frequency of information-seeking behaviour. Additionally, the frequency of information seeking was predicted by individual differences in resting-state functional connectivity between the ventral tegmental area and the nucleus accumbens. The present translational study paves the way for future studies on the role of curiosity in real-life information seeking by showing that both curiosity and mesolimbic dopaminergic functional network support real-life information-seeking behaviour.


Curiosity and mesolimbic functional connectivity drive information seeking in real life

January 2022

·

115 Reads

Curiosity reflects the intrinsic motivation of an individual to seek information in order to close information gaps. Laboratory-based experiments have shown that both curiosity and information seeking are associated with enhanced neural dynamics in the mesolimbic dopaminergic circuit. However, it is unclear whether curiosity and its associated neural dynamics in the dopaminergic circuit drive information seeking in real life. The present study investigated (i) whether curiosity traits predict different characteristics of real-life information seeking and (ii) whether functional connectivity within the mesolimbic dopaminergic circuit is associated with information seeking outside of the laboratory. Up to 15 month before the COVID-19 pandemic, we conducted curiosity and anxiety questionnaires as well as a 10-minute resting-state fMRI session. In a follow-up survey early during the COVID-19 pandemic, participants repeated the questionnaires and filled out an additional questionnaire about their COVID-19-related information seeking. Curiosity but not anxiety remained stable over time. Individual differences in curiosity were positively associated with the frequency of information-seeking behaviour. Anxiety during the pandemic was not linked to any characteristics of real-life information seeking. Interestingly, the frequency of information seeking was also independently predicted by individual differences in resting-state functional connectivity between the ventral tegmental area and the nucleus accumbens. The present translational study paves the way for future studies on the role of curiosity in real-life information seeking by showing that curiosity drives information seeking in real-life situations and that the curiosity-promoting mesolimbic dopaminergic functional network supports the frequency of real-life information-seeking behaviour. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Curiosity is a key driver of learning and information seeking in everyday life. However, the temporal stability of curiosity traits, their relationship to real-life information seeking, and the associated dopaminergic brain activity are poorly understood. The present study provides evidence that curiosity traits are stable over time – even through a major event, such as the COVID-19 pandemic – and that both curiosity and intrinsic functional connectivity within the mesolimbic dopaminergic circuit are associated with the frequency of real-life information seeking. These findings contribute to a better understanding of cognitive and neural differences that shape how individuals seek out information and may offer the opportunity to help individuals with suboptimal information-seeking behaviour that negatively affects their well-being or mental health.


Improving cognitive control: Is theta neurofeedback training associated with proactive rather than reactive control enhancement?

June 2021

·

152 Reads

·

18 Citations

Psychophysiology

Frontal-midline (FM) theta activity (4-8 Hz) is proposed to reflect a mechanism for cognitive control that is needed for working memory retention, manipulation, and interference resolution. Modulation of FM theta activity via neurofeedback training (NFT) demonstrated transfer to some but not all types of cognitive control. Therefore, the present study investigated whether FM theta NFT enhances performance and modulates underlying EEG characteristics in a delayed match to sample (DMTS) task requiring mainly proactive control and a color Stroop task requiring mainly reactive control. Moreover, temporal characteristics of transfer were explored over two posttests. Across seven 30-min NFT sessions, an FM theta training group exhibited a larger FM theta increase compared to an active control group who upreg-ulated randomly chosen frequency bands. In a posttest performed 13 days after the last training session, the training group showed better retention performance in the DMTS task. Furthermore, manipulation performance was associated with NFT theta increase for the training but not the control group. Contrarily, behavioral group differences and their relation to FM theta change were not significant in the Stroop task, suggesting that NFT is associated with proactive but not reactive control enhancement. Transfer to both tasks at a posttest one day after training was not significant. Behavioral improvements were not accompanied by changes in FM theta activity, indicating no training-induced modulation of EEG characteristics. Together, these findings suggest that NFT supports transfer to cognitive control that manifests late after training but that other training-unspecific factors may also contribute to performance enhancement.


Improving episodic memory: Frontal-midline theta neurofeedback training increases source memory performance

August 2020

·

192 Reads

·

31 Citations

NeuroImage

Cognitive and neurofeedback training (NFT) studies have demonstrated that training-induced alterations of frontal-midline (FM) theta activity (4-8 Hz) transfer to cognitive control processes. Given that FM theta oscillations are assumed to provide top-down control for episodic memory retrieval, especially for source retrieval, that is, accurate recollection of contextual details of prior episodes, the present study investigated whether FM theta NFT transfers to memory control processes. It was assessed (1) whether FM theta NFT improves source retrieval and modulates its underlying EEG characteristics and (2) whether this transfer extends over two posttests. Over seven NFT sessions, the training group who trained individual FM theta activity showed greater FM theta increase than an active control group who trained randomly chosen frequency bands. The training group showed better source retrieval in a posttraining session performed 13 days after NFT and their performance increases from pre- to both posttraining sessions were predicted by NFT theta increases. Thus, training-induced enhancement of memory control processes seems to protect newly formed memories from proactive interference of previously learned information. EEG analyses revealed that during pretest both groups showed source memory specific theta activity at frontal and parietal sites. Surprisingly, training-induced improvements in source retrieval tended to be accompanied by less prestimulus FM theta activity, which was predicted by NFT theta change for the training but not the control group, suggesting a more efficient use of memory control processes after training. The present findings provide unique evidence for the enhancement of memory control processes by FM theta NFT.



Citations (5)


... The starting point is that curiosity is the drive that leads a person to attempts to fill gaps in their knowledge, termed deprivation curiosity by Lydon-Staley et al., (2021). Eschmann et al. (2023) found that curiosity was one of the "key drivers of information seeking in real life" (p. 6), while van Lieshout et al. (2021), a study in which the subjects participated in a lottery experiment, found that curiosity "increased with the uncertainty of information" but that "the effect of outcome uncertainty on curiosity was not reliably different for gains and losses, indicating that these two effects seemed to operate largely independent of each other" (p. ...

Reference:

Approaches to information-seeking behaviour in psychology: a comparison of early and contemporary studies
Curiosity and Mesolimbic Functional Connectivity Drive Information Seeking in Real Life

Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience

... Computationally, this amounts to a high precision weighting on the likelihood mapping between sensory evidence and inferred perceptual states, encoded within the A tensor in POMDP schema. The experience is therefore dominated by presently incoming, action-relevant sensory data, which inhibits all forms of mental time-travel, including planning and retrospection (Klasen et al., 2012;Yoshida et al., 2014;Katahira et al., 2018;Eschmann et al., 2022). Crucially, this constraint distinguishes flow states from more quotidian examples of pragmatic behaviour which, in and of their own execution do not yield pre-reflective self-awareness and therefore might seem similar to flow states. ...

Theta Neurofeedback Training Supports Motor Performance and Flow Experience

Journal of Cognitive Enhancement

... The durations of neurofeedback-interventions varied between one day with only one neurofeedback session (i.e., Chen et al., 2022;Rozengurt et al., 2017Rozengurt et al., , 2016Shtoots et al., 2021) and four weeks including 12 sessions (i.e., Wang and Hsieh, 2013). On average, multi-session neurofeedback-intervention studies comprised 7.5 sessions (SD: ± 2.6) (i.e., Brandmeyer and Delorme, 2020;Egner and Gruzelier, 2004;Enriquez-Geppert et al., 2014a, 2014bEschmann et al., 2020;Eschmann and Mecklinger, 2022;Lutzenberger et al., 1976;Reis et al., 2016;Tseng et al., 2021;Wang and Hsieh, 2013). ...

Improving cognitive control: Is theta neurofeedback training associated with proactive rather than reactive control enhancement?
  • Citing Article
  • June 2021

Psychophysiology

... Since none of the identified 14 studies reported these statistics comprehensively, all authors were contacted via email, but only one workgroup provided the requested data (Chen et al., 2022). Therefore, and in extension to our preregistered protocol, in eight cases (i.e., Brandmeyer and Delorme, 2020;Enriquez-Geppert et al., 2014b, 2014aEschmann et al., 2020;Reis et al., 2016;Rozengurt et al., 2016;Shtoots et al., 2021;Wang and Hsieh, 2013) effect sizes were calculated on the basis of the reported test statistics (F-values, t-values and p-values). In the remaining two cases (i.e., Rozengurt et al., 2017;Tseng et al., 2021), in which no test statistics of effects were reported in the text and we received no reply from the authors, effect sizes were extracted graphically using GetData Graph Digitizer ("GetData Graph Digitizer", 2013). ...

Improving episodic memory: Frontal-midline theta neurofeedback training increases source memory performance

NeuroImage

... They found monotonic depth-related power increases in the major principal component of the theta band, but monotonic depth-related power decreases in the beta band and the second component in the gamma1 band. Furthermore, they reported that these changes occurred in the frontotemporal and parietal for theta and central for beta and gamma1 bands, suggesting that deeper states of meditation are accompanied by stronger cognitive control and attention [50][51][52][53]. ...

Topographical differences of frontal-midline theta activity reflect functional differences in cognitive control abilities
  • Citing Article
  • June 2018

Brain and Cognition