ArticlePDF Available

Positive and Negative Consequences of Alcohol Consumption in College Students

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

While the negative consequences of alcohol use in college students are well known, less is known about the relationships of these consequences to alcohol consumption patterns. Further, almost no research has been conducted examining students' positive alcohol-related consequences. The current study examines the nature and frequency of positive and negative alcohol-related consequences, the relationship of these consequences to alcohol consumption patterns, and the impact of these consequences on subsequent drinking intentions. Findings indicate that college student drinking does indeed involve many negative consequences, some of which are quite serious, but that students also experience many positive consequences. In fact, they report their encounters with positive consequences as being more extreme and more frequent than their encounters with negative consequences. Further, consuming more alcohol is related to experiencing more positive and more negative consequences, as well as more extremely positive positive encounters, but not more extremely negative negative ones. Finally, participants reported that their positive and negative consequences would influence their future drinking decisions in a number of different ways. Future research directions and implications for interventions are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Positive and negative consequences of
alcohol consumption in college students
Crystal L. Park*
Department of Connecticut, University of Connecticut, 406 Babbidge Road Unit 1020, Storrs, CT 06296, USA
Abstract
While the negative consequences of alcohol use in college students are well known, less is known
about the relationships of these consequences to alcohol consumption patterns. Further, almost no
research has been conducted examining students’ positive alcohol-related consequences. The current
study examines the nature and frequency of positive and negative alcohol-related consequences, the
relationship of these consequences to alcohol consumption patterns, and the impact of these
consequences on subsequent drinking intentions. Findings indicate that college student drinking does
indeed involve many negative consequences, some of which are quite serious, but that students also
experience many positive consequences. In fact, they report their encounters with positive
consequences as being more extreme and more frequent than their encounters with negative
consequences. Further, consuming more alcohol is related to experiencing more positive and more
negative consequences, as well as more extremely positive positive encounters, but not more
extremely negative negative ones. Finally, participants reported that their positive and negative
consequences would influence their future drinking decisions in a number of different ways. Future
research directions and implications for interventions are discussed.
D2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Alcohol consumption; College students; Positive and negative consequences
1. Introduction
Heavy alcohol consumption, often a normative part of the college experience (Chen &
Kandel, 1995), is associated with a multitude of negative consequences, including problems
with academics, interpersonal relations, and the legal system (e.g., Syre, Pesa, & Cockley,
0306-4603/$ – see front matter D2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.08.006
* Tel.: +1-860-486-3520.
E-mail address: clpark@uconnvm.uconn.edu (C.L. Park).
Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 311321
1999). These negative consequences appear to be a particularly problematic aspect of college
student drinking (Wechsler, Davenport, Dowdall, Moeykens, & Castillo, 1994). However, little
research has examined students’ own perceptions of the consequences of their drinking (cf.
Nystrom, 1992; Sadava & Pak, 1993).
The present study examines these alcohol-related consequences in greater detail, focusing on
positive consequences as well as negative ones. Specifically, this study examines the nature and
frequency of positive and negative alcohol-related consequences, the relationship of these
consequences to alcohol consumption patterns, and the impact of these consequences on
subsequent drinking intentions. Special attention is given to potential gender differences.
1.1. Nature and frequency of positive and negative consequences of alcohol consumption
The negative consequences of undergraduate drinking, including personal injuries,
unplanned sexual activity, and legal problems, have been well documented in national
samples (Wechsler, Dowdall, Davenport, & Castillo, 1995). Nevertheless, the literature
indicates that college students tend to hold very positive expectancies for their own alcohol
use (e.g., Kushner, Sher, Wood, & Wood, 1994). Moreover, these positive expectancies
persist and strengthen, with refinements, as students gain more experience with alcohol (e.g.,
Smith, Goldman, Greenbaum, & Christiansen, 1995). Because few studies have examined the
positive consequences of drinking, it is not clear whether students actually experience
positive alcohol-related consequences that reinforce their expectancies.
The present study asked students to report the levels of positive and negative alcohol-
related events that they had recently experienced. Further, students were asked to describe and
rate the intensity of their most negative and most positive recent alcohol-related experience.
Given the high levels of continued drinking by college undergraduates in spite of its
documented negative effects, it was hypothesized that positive consequences would occur
at least as frequently as negative consequences, and that students would rate their most
positive experiences as more positive than they would rate their most negative experiences as
negative.
1.2. Alcohol consumption and consequences
Research has shown that the relationship between drinking amount (quantity and
frequency) and negative consequences is consistent and substantial, but also that these
constructs are distinct (e.g., Bonin, McCreary, & Sadava, 2000; Camatta and Nagoshi,
1995). The present study hypothesized that higher levels of alcohol consumption would
be moderately related to higher levels of both negative consequences and positive
consequences.
1.3. Alcohol-related experiences and future drinking behavior
Many students continue to drink heavily in spite of encountering negative consequences,
although some students may alter their drinking habits in light of their experiences. This
C.L. Park / Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 311–321312
study examined relations among students’ positive and negative alcohol-related consequen-
ces and the intensity of their recent most positive and negative alcohol-related experiences
with their intentions to change their drinking in light of their experiences. Given that
alcohol consumption tends to be immediately positively reinforcing and its negative effects
are less immediate and seem to be less salient (Leigh, 1987), it was hypothesized that
positive consequences will be more influential than negative consequences in future
drinking intentions.
2. Method
Participants were 263 undergraduates (104 men, 159 women; mean age of 19.1; 3%
African American, 1% Asian, 95% Caucasian, 1% Latino) drawn from the psychology
department participant pool. All participants received research credit for their class.
2.1. Measures
2.1.1. Alcohol consumption
Participants were asked how often they consumed alcohol in the past month and how many
drinks they consumed at a typical sitting in the past month. Three aspects of heavy drinking in
the past month were assessed: frequency of binge drinking (5 or more drinks per sitting),
frequency of feeling lightheaded because of drinking, and frequency of being drunk (from
Kushner et al., 1994).
2.1.2. Most positive and most negative recent alcohol-related experience
Participants were asked a series of questions about their most negative alcohol-related
experience in the past 2 months, including a description of the encounter, ratings of the
intensity of this event and future intentions (‘‘How much does this negative consequence play
into your decisions about drinking now?’’) from 1 = not at all to 5 = extremely, and an open-
ended question asking ‘‘How?’’ A parallel set of questions was asked about their most
positive alcohol-related consequence. Participants’ descriptions of their recent most positive
and negative consequences and of how these consequences will play into their decisions about
drinking were sorted by two raters who independently categorized the responses. Their initial
agreement rate was 87%. In discordant cases, raters discussed and mutually decided on the
category for each item.
2.1.3. Negative consequences associated with drinking
Negative consequences associated with drinking were assessed by the Negative Alcohol
Consequences Scale (Wechsler et al., 1994), which asks participants the extent to which
they experienced each of the 12 negative consequences in the past 2 months as a result of
their own alcohol use (e.g., ‘Do something you later regretted’’) from 1 = never to 5 = very
frequently. Alpha in the present sample was .78.
C.L. Park / Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 311–321 313
2.1.4. Positive consequences associated with drinking
Because positive consequences of alcohol use have rarely been assessed, a measure
parallel to Wechsler’s negative consequences scale was devised to assess positive con-
sequences. This scale used the same rating format as Wechsler’s scale, but the 12 items were
taken from the expectancies measure of Kushner et al. (1994). The three highest loading items
on each of the four subscales (tension reduction, performance enhancement, activity
enhancement, and social lubrication) were converted into positive consequence items, and
participants were asked to rate the frequency with which they experienced each consequence.
Examples included ‘‘forgot my worries,’’ ‘‘felt more sexy,’’ and ‘‘had better ideas.’’ Alpha in
the present sample was .90.
3. Results
3.1. Alcohol consumption versus abstinence
Of the 270 students in the study, 28 (10%) reportedthat they had not consumed alcohol in the
past month. The analyses in this paper were conducted only with the 247 students (99 men, 142
women, and 6 who did not identify their gender) who reported that they had consumed alcohol
in the past month.
3.2. Nature and frequency of negative and positive experiences
Students reported experiencing more positive alcohol-related consequences than negative
alcohol-related consequences [M= 2.70 vs. 1.81, t(241) = 18.02, P< .001], and the frequency of
experiencing positive and negative consequences were fairly strongly correlated (r=.47,
P< .001). The most negative experiences in the last 2 months were hangovers/sickness and
kissing/sexual activity (see Table 1). The most positive experiences were having fun/socializing
and expressing oneself (see Table 2). Men reported experiencing higher mean levels of positive
[M= 2.83 vs. 2.50, t(239) = 2.29, P< .001] and negative [M= 1.97 vs. 1.69, t(239) = 3.39
P< .001] consequences than women.
Students reported that the degree to which their most positive consequence was positive
was significantly greater than the degree to which their most negative consequence was
negative [M= 4.03 vs. 3.24, t(222) = 7.65, P< .001]. Men and women did not differ on ratings
of the extremes of their most positive or negative recent alcohol-related experience (ts < 0.87,
Ps > .34).
3.3. Relations between positive and negative alcohol-related consequences and drinking
outcomes
Bivariate correlational analyses indicated that mean levels of experiencing negative and
positive consequences were related to higher levels of all of the alcohol consumption
C.L. Park / Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 311–321314
variables, supplementing results previously reported with this sample (Park & Levenson, in
press). A similar pattern, although with much weaker correlations, was found for the degree
to which the most positive event was positive. However, the degree to which the most
negative event was negative was unrelated to any of the alcohol consumption variables (see
Table 3). When these correlations were conduced separately by gender, almost identical
patterns resulted for men and women.
Table 1
Type and frequency of most negative experiences
Category Number of responses
(percentage of total respondents)
Examples
Men (n= 94) Women (n= 133)
Being sick/hangover 30 (27.8) 52 (32.5) ‘‘Collapsing in my room and vomiting all over
the carpet’’ ‘‘Got a migraine from a hangover’’
Sexual activity/kissing 17 (15.7) 21 (13.1) ‘‘Getting together with a girl and not
remembering the next morning’’ ‘‘Sexual
relations with a guy I’d just met’’
Fight/argument 13 (12.0) 9 (5.6) ‘‘Got in a big fight with my girlfriend’
‘‘Beat up some guy’
School problems 5 (4.6) 13 (8.1) ‘Got drunk the night before an exam and
failed it’’ ‘‘Schoolwork piled up because I
put it off to drink’’
Consequences due to
another person’s
drinking/taking
care of others
9 (8.3) 24 (15.0) ‘Had to take care of a severely intoxicated frosh
girl’’ ‘‘Had to sit up all night with my roommate’’
Accident 1 (0.9) 3 (1.9) ‘‘Big party with people packed on a deck which
snapped off the house’’ ‘‘Fell and sprained
my ankle’’
Aberrant behavior/
said or did
something
should not have/
out of control
12 (11.1) 22 (13.8) ‘‘Embarrassing my girlfriend at a sorority function
by belching in one of her big sister’s ears’’
‘‘Got angry and broke things in a fit of rage’
Trouble with
authorities/legal
7 (6.5) 3 (1.9) ‘‘Arrested for minor consumption’’ ‘‘Stolen
property’’
Blackout 3 (2.8) 4 (2.5) ‘‘Blacked out and didn’t remember things I did’
Drinking and driving 2 (1.9) 4 (2.5) ‘‘Driving home in a very expensive car’’ ‘‘Lost
control of car’’
Bout of depressed mood 2 (1.9) 0 (0.0) ‘‘Depression, wallowing in my failures’’ ‘‘Felt low
about myself because emotions were running’’
Other 6 (5.5) 4 (2.5) ‘‘Was drunk and had nothing to do. I was bored’’
None 1 (0.9) 9 (5.6)
Total 108 160
Total responses are greater than total number of participants who answered because some respondents had a
compound response (e.g., ‘‘I blacked out and said things I normally wouldn’t have, and was involved in a sexual
encounter I regretted’’).
C.L. Park / Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 311–321 315
Table 2
Types and frequency of most positive experiences
Category Number of responses
(percentage of total respondents)
Examples
Men (n= 93) Women (n= 130)
Met new
friends
8 (7.9) 17 (13.1) ‘‘I have met a lot of fraternity people’’ ‘‘Met a lot
of new people at a party’’
Great fun/
socializing
48 (47.5) 73 (56.2) ‘‘Had a blast dancing at a bar with friends’’ ‘‘Had a good
time at a party that wouldn’t have been as much fun sober’’
Romantic
encounter
11 (10.9) 8 (6.2) ‘‘I met my current boyfriend due to lack of social
inhibition’’ ‘‘Flirting with a girl I like when we were
both drunk’’
Sex 8 (7.9) 2 (1.5) ‘‘I went home with a ‘hotty’’’ ‘‘Got with a woman’’
Stress relief 6 (5.9) 2 (1.5) ‘‘Forgot problems of past year with my health’’
‘‘Relieved stress so I could have fun with my friends’
Expressed
myself
14 (13.7) 23 (17.7) ‘‘Was able to express my true feelings to a girl’
‘‘Spoke true feelings to certain people’
Celebrated 0 (0.0) 2 (1.5) ‘‘Many toasts to my parents on their 25th wedding
anniversary’’
Controlled
drinking/
myself
4 (4.0) 2 (1.5) ‘‘Went to a party where alcohol was available, but I
didn’t drink, I restrained myself’’ ‘‘Controlled drinking’
Nice meal 2 (2.0) 2 (1.5) ‘‘Having a glass of wine with my family at Thanksgiving
dinner’’
Other 0 (0.0) 3 (2.3)
None 5 (5.0) 12 (9.2)
Total 101 146
Total responses are greater than total number of participants who answered because some respondents had a
compound response (e.g., ‘‘Had a great time with friends and was able to express feelings I normally wouldn’t
express’’).
Table 3
Correlations between recent positive and negative alcohol-related consequences and past month drinking
outcomes
Mean level
of negative
consequences
Mean level
of positive
consequences
Degree to
which most
negative occurrence
was negative
Degree to
which most
positive occurrence
was positive
Alcohol consumption frequency .58*** .38*** .08 .17***
Alcohol consumption quantity .48*** .36*** .10 .15*
Frequency of binge drinking .61*** .41*** .09 .19**
Frequency of being drunk .61*** .38*** .07 .22**
Frequency of feeling lightheaded .57*** .40*** .10 .20**
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.
C.L. Park / Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 311–321316
Table 4
Correlations among positive and negative consequences, extent of most positive and negative consequence, and
influence of most positive and negative consequence on future drinking decisions
Mean level
of positive
consequences
Degree to
which most
negative
occurrence
was negative
Extent of
influence of
most negative
occurrence
Degree to
which most
positive
occurrence
was positive
Extent of
influence of
most positive
occurrence
Mean level of
negative consequences
.54*** .18** .02 .10 .18**
Mean level of
positive consequences
– .09 .15 .31*** .37***
Degree to which most
negative occurrence
was negative
.54*** .22*** .35***
Extent of influence of
most negative occurrence
– .09 .17**
Degree to which most positive
occurrence was positive
– .41***
Extent of influence of
most positive occurrence
Table 5
Types of influence most negative alcohol-related event will have
Category Number of responses
(percentage of responses)
Examples
Men (n= 86) Women (n= 122)
Drink less/try
to drink less
19 (22.1) 31 (25.4) ‘I try to drink less’’ ‘‘Make sure
I don’t OD’’
Am more careful
when I drink now
11 (12.8) 16 (13.1) ‘I know I drank too much that
night and should be careful’’ ‘‘Make
sure I handle my alcohol well’’
Think more 10 (11.7) 6 (4.9) ‘‘I try to think of consequences
ahead of time’’ ‘‘Have to be more
aware of my actions sometimes’’
Change the context in
which drinking occurs
12 (14.0) 23 (18.9) ‘No more parties at my house’’
‘‘Makes me realize I must always
bring condoms with me when I’m
drinking’’
Change views of
alcohol or self
5 (5.8) 1 (0.8) ‘‘Reaffirmed by belief that alcohol
is not beneficial’’ ‘‘Know I can take
care of a big, drunk dude’’
Other 3 (3.5) 3 (2.6) ‘‘Try to forget’’
Little influence 6 (7.0) 6 (5.0) ‘‘Not really; I made a mistake’
No influence 20 (23.3) 36 (29.5) ‘Not at all’’ ‘‘It doesn’t; I am going
to drink anyway’’
Total number of responses 86 122
C.L. Park / Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 311–321 317
3.4. Positive and negative alcohol-related consequences and influence on future drinking
intentions
Regarding the influence of their most positive and negative experience on their future
drinking behavior, students reported being more strongly influenced by their most positive
experience than their most negative experience [M= 3.18 vs. 2.45, t(222) = 6.38, P< .001].
Men and women did not differ in the extent to which their most positive consequence would
influence their future drinking [M= 3.17 vs. 3.10, t(222) = 0.35, P>.72], but women reported
that their negative experience would be more influential than did men [M= 2.74 vs. 2.14,
t(222) = 3.62, P< .001].
Regarding relations between alcohol consequences and future drinking intentions, Table
4lists correlations among frequency of positive and negative consequences and character-
istics of most positive and negative alcohol-related events with influences on future
drinking. The more negative the most negative event, the more it would influence future
drinking (r=.54). The same was true, but to a lesser extent, for the positivity of the
positive event (r=.41). The positivity of the most positive event was not related to the
extent of influence of the most negative event (r=.09), but the negativity of the most
negative event was related to the extent of influence of the most positive event (r=.35).
Separate correlational analyses by gender produced almost identical patterns for men and
women.
Table 6
Types of influence most positive alcohol-related event will have
Category Number of responses
(percentage of responses)
Examples
Men (n= 81) Women (n= 110)
Reinforce positive
social aspects of drinking
12 (14.8) 23 (21.0) ‘‘It’s a good way to meet people’
‘‘It helps me to be more likeable’
Reinforce fun aspects
of drinking
19 (23.5) 19 (17.3) ‘‘I know I drank too much that night
and should be careful’’ ‘‘Make sure I
handle my alcohol well’’
Reinforce ideas of trust/
control regarding alcohol
4 (4.9) 12 (11.0) ‘‘I know I can trust myself not to drink
too much even when being pressured’’
‘‘I see I can have a good time drinking
but I don’t have to be falling down drunk’’
Reinforce ability
to communicate
better with alcohol
12 (14.8) 23 (21.0) ‘‘I know it helps me express myself’’ ‘‘I will
drink if I wish to communicate better’’
Desire to drink more/
repeat the experience
14 (17.3) 17 (15.5) ‘‘When you party and have a good time, you
want to do it all the time’’ ‘‘Makes me want to
drink at parties’’
Other 2 (2.5) 1 (0.9) ‘‘Feel good that I can make others happy’
Little influence 6 (7.4) 6 (5.5) ‘‘Not much. I still go out like I did before’’
No influence 17 (21.0) 25 (23.0) ‘‘Not at all, it was just a really fun thing to do’
Total 81 110
C.L. Park / Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 311–321318
Students’ reports of how their most negative and most positive occurrences will be related
to their future drinking indicate that many students report planning to drink less or more
carefully as a result of their negative experience but that they are also encouraged to drink
more as a result of their most positive alcohol-related consequence and that their positive
alcohol expectancies have been strengthened (see Tables 5 and 6).
4. Discussion
These results suggest that college student drinking does indeed involve many negative
consequences, some of which are quite serious, but that students also experience many
positive consequences. In fact, they report their encounters with positive consequences as
being more extreme and more frequent than their encounters with negative consequences.
Further, consuming more alcohol is related to more positive and more negative consequences
as well as more extremely positive positive encounters, but not more extremely negative
negative ones. Finally, students reported that their positive and negative consequences would
influence their future drinking decisions in a number of different ways.
The negative consequences of college student drinking have recently received a great
deal of attention (e.g., Wechsler, 2000). Importantly, although alcohol consumption was
related to the frequency of encountering negative experiences, the alcohol consumption
variables were unrelated to the negativity of the most negative encounter. These results
are consistent with those of Sadava and Pak (1993), who noted that negative con-
sequences are only modestly linked to heavy alcohol consumption. Most students reported
that their most negative consequence would influence their future drinking, with many
students intending to drink less or more carefully or to think more when drinking in the
future.
The positive consequences of alcohol, while clearly on the minds of many students (as
evidenced in studies of positive expectancies), have rarely been the focus of research in the
area of college student drinking. The present findings suggest that students experience a
variety of positive consequences, that these positive consequences are in many ways more
significant to them than negative ones, and that positive consequences appear to reinforce
their positive expectancies regarding alcohol. Further, as hypothesized, the most extremely
positive positive consequences were reported as being more influential in their future
decisions regarding drinking. These findings are consistent with Leigh and Stacy’s (1998)
observation that alcohol’s inherently reinforcing properties increase the likelihood that
positive affect will be encoded every time alcohol is consumed. Conversely, negative affect
is less probable and therefore less likely to be associated with alcohol. Further, they
reported evidence that positive outcome associations are more strongly related to drinking
habits than are negative outcome associations, implying that heavier alcohol use leads to
stronger associations with positive than with negative outcomes. In the present study, many
students reported quite directly their plans or desires to drink again or to engage in the
same behaviors that led to their most positive consequence, while many others reported that
the positive consequences reinforced their beliefs that alcohol use leads to more fun, better
C.L. Park / Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 311–321 319
times with friends, less tension, and easier socializing. Such reinforcement of positive
expectancies may lead to continued drinking in the future as well.
While this study examined gender differences, the results generally indicated that men and
women were more alike than different in their experiences of positive and negative alcohol-
related consequences. Correlation patterns among the variables did not evidence gender
differences, although men reported experiencing more positive and more negative alcohol-
related consequences, consistent with previous research (Nystrom, 1992), and women
reported being more influenced in their drinking decisions by their most negative conse-
quence than did men.
Several limitations of this study should be noted. Participants were just one group of
primarily white middle class college students. Future studies should include more socio-
economically and ethnically diverse samples. In assessing consequences and attitudinal and
behavioral changes, it would be helpful to have more sophisticated measurement tools.
Further, the cross-sectional design of the present study limits the ability to make causal
inferences; longitudinal research would be informative in examining how these variables are
related over time. In particular, repeated assessments of alcohol use, consequences, and
subsequent alcohol use would better model the relationships among these variables. A daily
diary methodology might be especially helpful in this regard (e.g., Hussong, Hicks, Levy, &
Curran, 2001).
Future research on college student drinking should attend to the consequences of drinking
as outcomes separate from (and possibly more important than) alcohol consumption
(McCreary & Sadava, 1998; Plant, 1999) and should examine positive as well as negative
outcomes. Further, it appears that some psychosocial factors may make students relatively
more or less likely to experience positive as well as negative alcohol-related consequences.
For example, Sadava and Pak (1993) found that certain variables, such as stress, coping, and
depression, were related to experiencing negative consequences, even when controlling for
the amount of alcohol consumed. Such factors should be identified. Finally, longitudinal
research should examine the sequences of alcohol consumption, consequences, and changes
in alcohol consumption to better understand the interplay of expectancies and experience.
Campus intervention efforts should also be informed by these findings. College students
not only tend to hold positive expectancies for alcohol use (Kushner et al., 1994), but they
actually have positive experiences as results of this use. Interventions involving expectancy
challenges have been shown to be somewhat effective in experimental contexts (e.g., Darkes
& Goldman, 1998), but changing expectancies in meaningful long-term ways has proven to
be difficult (e.g., Corbin, McNair, & Carter, 2001). This may be due, in part, to students’
encounters with positive experiences that reinforce their positive expectancies, which then
lead them to experience further positive consequences in a positive feedback cycle.
Intervention programs should be tempered with an awareness of the students’ likely
encounters with positive as well as negative experiences. Finally, given the semi-independ-
ence of alcohol consumption and negative consequences, intervention goals may include not
only reducing alcohol use, but also reducing the frequency and severity of negative
consequences, which may require different intervention strategies. For example, it may be
helpful to have students focus on the context of their negative consequences and to work with
C.L. Park / Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 311–321320
them on ways that they can change their drinking behaviors to minimize future negative
consequences.
References
Bonin, M. F., McCreary, D. R., & Sadava, S. W. (2000). Problem drinking behavior in two community-based
samples of adults: Influence of gender, coping, loneliness, and depression. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors,
14, 151 161.
Camatta, C.D., & Nagoshi, C.T. (1995). Stress, depression, irrational beliefs, and alcohol use and problems in a
college student sample. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,19(1), 142– 146.
Chen, K., & Kandel, D. B. (1995). The natural history of drug use from adolescence to the mid-thirties in a general
population sample. American Journal of Public Health,85, 41 47.
Corbin, W.R., McNair, L.D., & Carter, J.A. (2001). Evaluation of a treatment-appropriate cognitive intervention
for challenging alcohol outcome expectancies. Addictive Behaviors,26, 475 488.
Darkes, J., & Goldman, M. S. (1998). Expectancy challenge and drinking reduction: Process and structure in the
alcohol expectancy network. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology,6, 64– 76.
Hussong, A. M., Hicks, R. E., Levy, S. A., & Curran, P. J. (2001). Specifying the relations between affect and
heavy alcohol use among young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,110, 449 466.
Kushner, M. G., Sher, K. J., Wood, M. D., & Wood, P. K. (1994). Anxiety and drinking behavior: Moderating
effects of tension-reduction alcohol outcome expectancies. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research,
18, 852 860.
Leigh, B. C. (1987). Evaluations of alcohol expectancies: Do they add to prediction of drinking patterns?
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors,1, 135 139.
Leigh, B. C., & Stacy, A. W. (1998). Individual differences in memory associations involved the positive and
negative outcomes of alcohol use. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors,12, 39 46.
McCreary, D. R., & Sadava, S. W. (1998). Stress, alcohol use and alcohol-related problems: The influence of
negative and positive affect in two cohorts of young adults. Journal of Studies on Alcohol,61, 466 474.
Nystrom, M. (1992). Positive and negative consequences of alcohol drinking among young university students in
Finland. British Journal of Addictions,87, 715 722.
Park, C. L., & Levenson, M. R. (2001). Drinking to cope among college students: Prevalence, problems, and
coping processes. Journal of Studies on Alcohol,63, 486 497.
Plant, M. (1999). Patterns and consequences matter. Addiction Research,7, 463– 468.
Sadava, S. W., & Pak, A. W. (1993). Stress-related problem drinking an alcohol problems: A longitudinal study
and extension of Marlatt’s model. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science,25, 446 464.
Smith, G. T., Goldman, M. S., Greenbaum, P. E., & Christiansen, B. A. (1995). Expectancy for social facilitation
from drinking: The divergent paths of high-expectancy and low-expectancy adolescents. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology,104, 32– 40.
Syre, T. R., Pesa, J. A., & Cockley, D. (1999). Alcohol problems on college campuses escalate in 1997 1998:
Time for action. College Student Journal,33, 82 86.
Wechsler, H. (2000). Binge drinking: Should we attack the name or the problem? Chronicle of Higher Education,
18, B12 B13.
Wechsler, H., Davenport, A., Dowdall, G., Moeykens, B., & Castillo, S. (1994). Health and behavioral conse-
quences of binge drinking in college: A national survey of students at 140 campuses. Journal of the American
Medical Association,272, 1672 1677.
Wechsler, H., Dowdall, G. W., Davenport, A., & Castillo, S. (1995). Correlates of college student binge drinking.
American Journal of Public Health,85, 921 926.
C.L. Park / Addictive Behaviors 29 (2004) 311–321 321
... However, to gain a more nuanced understanding of how trait mindfulness and drinking motivations are related to alcohol-related consequences, researchers have begun to expand their conceptualization and measurement of alcoholrelated consequences. For example, Park (2004) was the first to examine positive alcohol-related consequences in college students and found positive consequences to be more frequent than negative consequences, and that positive consequences reinforced their positive expectancies of alcohol use, which could in turn predict future alcohol use, indicating the importance of including positive consequences in future research [33]. ...
... However, to gain a more nuanced understanding of how trait mindfulness and drinking motivations are related to alcohol-related consequences, researchers have begun to expand their conceptualization and measurement of alcoholrelated consequences. For example, Park (2004) was the first to examine positive alcohol-related consequences in college students and found positive consequences to be more frequent than negative consequences, and that positive consequences reinforced their positive expectancies of alcohol use, which could in turn predict future alcohol use, indicating the importance of including positive consequences in future research [33]. ...
... Another study of first-year undergraduate students utilized latent class analysis and found that their sample could be categorized into four different classes in regards to negative alcohol-related consequences: 1) very few or no negative alcohol-related consequences; 2) academic problems, 3) injured self, and 4) severe problems [35]. Thus, while literature suggests there may be distinct subfactors of alcohol-related consequences, no study has examined the factor structure of the Positive and Negative Consequences of Alcohol Scale (PNCAS) [33] and tested whether these subfactors have differential relationships to trait mindfulness and drinking motivations. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background This study aimed to determine if motivations to use alcohol (coping and social motivations) mediate the relationship between trait mindfulness and a variety of alcohol-related consequences and to determine if the relationship between motivations to use alcohol and alcohol-related consequences is moderated by alcohol use. We determined the factor structure of positive and negative consequences of alcohol use and used this structure as outcomes across eight moderated mediation models. Methods Data were obtained from 296 undergraduate students to confirm the alcohol-related consequences factor structure and to test eight moderated-mediation models. Results Four alcohol-related consequences scales (romantic/sexual, positive, mild negative, and severe negative consequences) were confirmed. The motive of drinking to cope significantly mediated the relationship between trait mindfulness and all four of the alcohol-related consequences scales. Drinking to socialize did not significantly mediate the relationship between trait mindfulness and all of the alcohol-related consequences scales. Conclusions The identified four-factor structure suggests that alcohol-related consequences should be assessed in a more specific manner. Additionally, different motivations for alcohol use relate differentially to trait mindfulness and different alcohol-related consequences; drinking to cope is particularly problematic for this population. Future research on the usefulness of promoting mindfulness to reduce problematic drinking appears warranted.
... In 2019, among people aged 18-25 in the US, an estimated 54.3% consumed alcohol in the past month and over a third (34.3%) of people in this age group binge-drank in the past month, with 8.4% engaging in heavy alcohol use (defined as binge drinking five or more times in the past month) ( Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, 2020 ). Despite some positive social aspects of alcohol consumption, adverse effects commonly include hangover, blackout, regretted or unwelcome behavior including sexual activities, arguments or fights, and injury ( Palamar et al., 2014 ;Park, 2004 ;Park and Grant, 2005 ). Hangover, although not typically grave, is particularly common, with studies of college students finding that 27.8-32.5% have gotten sick or hungover after consumption ( Park, 2004 ). ...
... Despite some positive social aspects of alcohol consumption, adverse effects commonly include hangover, blackout, regretted or unwelcome behavior including sexual activities, arguments or fights, and injury ( Palamar et al., 2014 ;Park, 2004 ;Park and Grant, 2005 ). Hangover, although not typically grave, is particularly common, with studies of college students finding that 27.8-32.5% have gotten sick or hungover after consumption ( Park, 2004 ). This is noteworthy as many adverse effects queried in the present study, such as headache, nausea, and tachycardia, are commonly associated with hangover ( Vatsalya et al., 2018 ), and can be predicted by higher frequency, quantity, and speed of alcohol consumption ( Carpenter and Merrill, 2021 ;Park, 2004 ;Park and Grant, 2005 ). ...
... Hangover, although not typically grave, is particularly common, with studies of college students finding that 27.8-32.5% have gotten sick or hungover after consumption ( Park, 2004 ). This is noteworthy as many adverse effects queried in the present study, such as headache, nausea, and tachycardia, are commonly associated with hangover ( Vatsalya et al., 2018 ), and can be predicted by higher frequency, quantity, and speed of alcohol consumption ( Carpenter and Merrill, 2021 ;Park, 2004 ;Park and Grant, 2005 ). Alcohol is also commonly used in combination with other drugs, which can often increase risk of an adverse effect or hospitalization ( Palamar et al., 2019b ;SAMHSA, 2022 ) and consequently makes the drug uniquely dangerous. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Research investigating adverse effects from drug use has focused extensively on poisonings and mortality. This study focuses on drug-related adverse effects not necessarily resulting in hospitalization or death among a population known for high prevalence of party drug use-electronic dance music (EDM) nightclub and festival attendees. Methods: Adults entering EDM venues were surveyed in 2019-2022 (n = 1952). Those reporting past-month use of a drug were asked whether they had experienced a harmful or very unpleasant effect after use. We examined 20 drugs and drug classes with a particular focus on alcohol, cannabis, cocaine, and ecstasy. Prevalence and correlates of adverse effects were estimated. Results: Almost half (47.6%) of adverse effects involved alcohol and 19.0% involved cannabis. 27.6% of those using alcohol reported an adverse effect, while 19.5%, 15.0%, and 14.9% of participants reported an effect from use of cocaine, ecstasy, and cannabis, respectively. Use of less prevalent drugs, such as NBOMe, methamphetamine, fentanyls, and synthetic cathinones, tended to be associated with higher prevalence of adverse effects. The most consistent risk factor was younger age, while past-month use of a greater number of drugs was often a protective factor against adverse effects. For most drugs, taking too much was the most common perceived reason for the adverse effect, and visiting a hospital after use was most prevalent among those experiencing an adverse effect from cocaine (11.0%). Conclusions: Adverse drug effects are common in this population and results can inform prevention and harm reduction in this population and the general population.
... Although most research on substance use focuses on negative outcomes more than positive ones, positive drug effects may be especially important for understanding future patterns of use (Davidson & Schenk, 1994;Fergusson et al., 2003;Le Strat et al., 2009). For example, young adults' drinking behavior tends to be more influenced by positive alcohol-related experiences than negative ones Park, 2004). The finding that negative effects were not sufficient to reduce CUD risk is also consistent with LCAs showing higher rates of CUD among individuals who experienced a range of cannabis effects, even when some were negative (Grant et al., 2005;Scherrer et al., 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
Operant conditioning and social learning theories suggest that positive cannabis use–related outcomes are a primary contributor to maintained use and risk for dependence. However, currently there does not exist a reliable, validated measure of positive cannabis-related outcomes. This study sought to develop and psychometrically evaluate the Positive Outcomes of Cannabis Use Scale (POCUS). We collected three samples, college students ( N = 883), community adults ( N = 214), and college students ( N = 615), of predominantly White adults in the United States who completed an online survey. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses evaluated scale structure and identified four factors: social enhancement, mood enhancement, cognitive enhancement, and sexual enhancement. Positive outcomes were positively associated with recent use, controlling for expectancies and negative outcomes. Positive outcomes were also differentiated from positive expectancies and more influential in predicting typical use frequency. Findings indicate that the POCUS is psychometrically sound and clinically useful for measuring positive cannabis use–related outcomes among predominantly White adults in the United States.
Article
Generalizing from past experiences to novel situations is critical for adaptive behavior, whereas overgeneralization can promote maladaptive responses (e.g., context-inappropriate fear in anxiety). Here, we propose that overgeneralizing alcohol-related associations characterizes risky drinking. We conducted two online experiments assessing generalization of alcohol-related gains (Study 1) and losses (Study 2) among individuals who engaged in light or risky patterns of drinking (Study 1: N = 88, 24-44 years old; Study 2: N = 87, 21-44 years old). After learning to associate cards with alcohol and non-alcohol-related outcomes, participants chose whether to play with cards varying in perceptual similarity to those shown during conditioning. Finally, participants completed a surprise recognition memory test for all outcomes. Although both groups showed comparable conditioning, we found that risky drinkers overgeneralized alcohol-related gains and losses. Risky drinkers also showed a bias toward recognizing alcohol-related images. These results indicate a novel role for overgeneralization of alcohol-related gains and losses as a mechanism associated with risky drinking.
Thesis
Full-text available
The aim of this study is to determine to what extent adolescents' cyber victimization, peer bullying, dark triad personality traits, risk behavior and prosocial behaviors predict cyberbully and non-cyberbully adolescents. This research is a descriptive quantitative research conducted in a cross-sectional design and designed in an exploratory correlational model. A total of 952 students studying at 9 faculties and 1 vocational school at İnönü University in the 2021-2022 academic year participated in the research. Revised Cyberbullying/Victimization Inventory 2, Peer Bullying Scale, Short Dark Triad Scale Turkish Form, Risk Behaviors Scale, Prosocial Tendencies Scale and personal information form were used as data collection tools in the study. The data of the study were analyzed by percent-frequency analysis and logistic regression analysis. Before performing the logistic regression analysis, it was determined that the sample was large enough, outlier data were not included in the analysis, and the correlation coefficients between the variables were examined and it was seen that there was no multicollinearity problem. As a result of the research, it was determined that 47% of the adolescents in the sample were involved in the cyberbullying process as a bully, victim or bully/victim (both bully and victim); 53% of them were not involved in cyberbullying. It was determined that 30% of the adolescents are cyberbullies, 40% are cyber victims, 7% are only cyberbullies, 17% are only cyber victims, and 23% are bullies/victims. In the logistic regression model obtained, it was determined that there was a relationship between cyberbullying and the predictive variables in the study, that the goodness of fit of the model was sufficient and it explained 25% of the variance in cyberbullying. It was determined that the model accurately classified cyberbully and non-cyberbully adolescents at the rate of 79%, and cyber victimization, peer bullying, alcohol use, antisocial behaviors and nutrition habits made significant contributions to the prediction of cyberbully and non-cyberbully adolescents.
Chapter
Bu araştırmanın amacı ergenlerdeki riskli davranışlar ve prososyal davranışların ergenlerin siber zorba olup olmadığını ne düzeyde öngördüğünü incelemektir. Bu araştırma kesitsel desende gerçekleştirilmiş betimsel bir nicel araştırma olup keşfedici korelasyonel modelde tasarlanmıştır. Araştırmaya 2021-2022 eğitim öğretim yılında İnönü Üniversitesi’nin 9 fakülte ve 1 meslek yüksekokulunda öğrenim gören toplam 952 öğrenci katılmıştır. Araştırmada veri toplama araçları olarak Yenilenmiş Siber Zorbalık/Mağduriyet Envanteri 2, Riskli Davranışlar Ölçeği, Olumlu Sosyal Davranış Eğilimi Ölçeği ve kişisel bilgi formu kullanılmıştır. Araştırmanın verileri lojistik regresyon analizi ile incelenmiştir. Lojistik regresyon analizi gerçekleştirilmeden önce örneklemin yeterli büyüklükte olduğu belirlenmiş, uç veriler belirlenerek analize dahil edilmemiş ve değişkenler arasındaki korelasyon katsayıları incelenerek çoklu bağlantı problemi olmadığı görülmüştür. Siber zorba olan ergenler “1” olarak, siber zorba olmayan ergenler “0” olarak kodlanmış ve lojistik regresyon analizinin standart (enter) yöntemi kullanılarak analiz gerçekleştirilmiştir. Elde edilen veriler SPSS 22 programı aracılığıyla analiz edilmiştir. Araştırma sonucunda elde edilen lojistik regresyon modelinde siber zorbalık ile araştırmada ele alınan yordayıcı değişkenler arasında ilişki olduğu belirlenmiştir. Lojistik regresyon modelinin uyum iyiliğinin yeterli düzeyde olduğu tespit edilmiştir. Modelin, siber zorbalıktaki varyansın %9.3’ünü açıkladığı saptanmıştır. Elde edilen modelin siber zorba olan ve olmayan ergenleri %70.4 oranında doğru sınıflandırdığı bulunmuştur. Modelde yer alan yordayıcı değişkenlerden riskli davranışların alt boyutlarından antisosyal davranışlar, alkol kullanımı ve beslenme alışkanlıkları değişkenlerinin siber zorba olan ve olmayan ergenlerin öngörülmesine anlamlı katkılar sağladığı belirlenmiştir. Riskli davranışların diğer alt boyutları olan sigara kullanımı, intihar eğilimi ve okul terki değişkenleri ile prososyal davranışların hiçbir alt boyutunun siber zorba olan ve olmayan ergenlerin öngörülmesine anlamlı katkılar sağlamadığı saptanmıştır.
Article
During the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic, people faced life-threatening uncertainties with socio-psychological implications while increased alcohol usage has been found to be a maladaptive form of coping induced by pandemic-related stress and isolation. Despite this, alcohol has also been found to increase people’s willingness to socialize and reduce stress. We employ visual self-narrative to investigate a research gap into the usage of alcohol thereby examining the varying roles alcohol has played in people’s lives. Photographs offer deeper understanding through collection of perishable data into social phenomena; and allow inquiry into sensitive topics with vulnerable populations. Our study, which found alcohol can play a helpful role in coping, further suggests photographic data should be further considered in public health research.
Article
Full-text available
Tested and extended G. A. Marlatt's (1979) model of stress-related substance abuse within a longitudinal design, following 391 university students during the transitional year of their graduation from university. 348 of the Ss comprised the final sample. Greater consumption of alcohol and a greater frequency of intoxication, as well as changes toward higher scores on these measures were related to and predicted by higher levels of perceived stress and stressful life events, greater external locus of control, social supports and life satisfaction, more opportunities for heavy drinking in a supportive social context, and attributing greater importance to coping functions of drinking. Higher levels of alcohol-related problems amongst drinkers, after accounting for the effect of consumption levels on problems, were related to these variables as well, and particularly to depression and social supports. (French abstract) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
In previous studies of memory association and alcohol use, the authors found that positive outcomes of drinking were associated in memory with alcohol concepts among heavier drinkers. The present study examined whether previous alcohol use predicted associative memory responses to negative, as well as positive, outcomes of drinking. It also examined whether the normative frequency of positive and negative outcomes affected responses. In the present sample of 609 students, previous alcohol use, normative frequency of outcome, and the number of outcomes listed strongly predicted alcohol-related associative responses to positive outcomes and, to a lesser extent, negative outcomes. The results suggest that repetitive alcohol use strengthens the association in memory between alcohol concepts and both positive and negative outcomes. However, thoughts about potential positive outcomes may be more readily cued by situational factors and events. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
288 adults and 265 university students responded to a questionnaire concerning beliefs about the effects of alcohol, which included a measure of evaluation of alcohol effects. Analyses were performed with measures of frequency of drinking episodes, usual quantity consumed/episode, frequency of intoxication and consumption of 8 or more drinks at one sitting, and estimated number of drinks per year. Evaluation of the undesirable consequences of drinking added significantly to the prediction of quantity-based drinking variables. It is suggested that perceived desirability of alcohol effects is an important factor in understanding the relationship of expectancies to drinking. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Objective. —To examine the extent of binge drinking by college students and the ensuing health and behavioral problems that binge drinkers create for themselves and others on their campus.Design. —Self-administered survey mailed to a national representative sample of US 4-year college students.Setting. —One hundred forty US 4-year colleges in 1993.Participants. —A total of 17592 college students.Main Outcome Measures. —Self-reports of drinking behavior, alcohol-related health problems, and other problems.Results. —Almost half (44%) of college students responding to the survey were binge drinkers, including almost one fifth (19%) of the students who were frequent binge drinkers. Frequent binge drinkers are more likely to experience serious health and other consequences of their drinking behavior than other students. Almost half (47%) of the frequent binge drinkers experienced five or more different drinking-related problems, including injuries and engaging in unplanned sex, since the beginning of the school year. Most binge drinkers do not consider themselves to be problem drinkers and have not sought treatment for an alcohol problem. Binge drinkers create problems for classmates who are not binge drinkers. Students who are not binge drinkers at schools with higher binge rates were more likely than students at schools with lower binge rates to experience problems such as being pushed, hit, or assaulted or experiencing an unwanted sexual advance.Conclusions. —Binge drinking is widespread on college campuses. Programs aimed at reducing this problem should focus on frequent binge drinkers, refer them to treatment or educational programs, and emphasize the harm they cause for students who are not binge drinkers.(JAMA. 1994;272:1672-1677)
Article
One-hundred thirty-five alcohol-using college students completed a questionnaire on their levels of alcohol use, moderate-to-severe problems with alcohol use, the Eysenck 1.7 measure of impulsiveness and venturesomeness, the Zung depression scale, the “Hassles and Uplifts” scale of life stresses, and a scale of proneness to irrational beliefs. Impulsivity and venturesomeness were significantly correlated with quantity-frequency of alcohol use, but not with the occurrence of alcohol use problems; whereas depression, stress, and irrational beliefs were significantly correlated with alcohol problems, but not with alcohol use. Multiple regression analyses indicated that the effect of stress on alcohol problems was mediated by depression, whereas the effect of depression, in turn, was mediated by irrational beliefs.
Article
The current study evaluated an alcohol expectancy challenge (EC) that did not require alcohol administration and could therefore be implemented in a treatment setting. Participants in the treatment group directly challenged alcohol expectancies endorsed on an expectancy questionnaire. A total of 62 male and female undergraduates completed the study (32 control participants, 30 EC participants). Self-report questionnaires were collected pre- and post-intervention, and alcohol logs were kept during the study. The EC resulted in significant reductions in alcohol expectancies across multiple expectancy dimensions. Although the analysis for alcohol consumption was not significant, there was a trend toward better outcomes for male participants in the EC condition. In contrast to study hypotheses, women in the EC condition increased their alcohol consumption from pre to post-test to a greater degree than did control participants.
Article
Expectancies' mediational (control) role in alcohol consumption has been supported by both correlational and experimental evidence (J. Darkes & M. S. Goldman, 1993; M. S. Goldman, P. E. Greenbaum, & J. Darkes, 1997; L. Roehrich & M. S. Goldman, 1995). This study assigned participants (n = 54) to 1 of 2 expectancy challenges targeting the expectancy dimensions of either arousal or sociability identified by B. C. Rather and M. S. Goldman (1994), or to a no-treatment control, to examine the relationship of the structure and process of change in alcohol expectancies. Both challenges resulted in reduced consumption and expectancies immediately posttreatment and 6 weeks later after a short "booster" session. These results may reflect the lack of "discrete" expectancy structure and provide further support for the exploration of these methods as a possible prevention strategy.