ArticlePDF Available

Empathy for Pain Involves the Affective But Not Sensory Components of Pain

Authors:

Abstract

Our ability to have an experience of another's pain is characteristic of empathy. Using functional imaging, we assessed brain activity while volunteers experienced a painful stimulus and compared it to that elicited when they observed a signal indicating that their loved one—present in the same room—was receiving a similar pain stimulus. Bilateral anterior insula (AI), rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), brainstem, and cerebellum were activated when subjects received pain and also by a signal that a loved one experienced pain. AIand ACC activation correlated with individual empathy scores. Activity in the posterior insula/secondary somatosensory cortex, the sensorimotor cortex (SI/MI), and the caudal ACC was specific to receiving pain. Thus, a neural response in AIand rostral ACC, activated in common for “self” and “other” conditions, suggests that the neural substrate for empathic experience does not involve the entire “pain matrix.” We conclude that only that part of the pain network associated with its affective qualities, but not its sensory qualities, mediates empathy.
The time-averaged pulsed flux density of the
pulsars is about 1.8 mJy (1 Jy 10
–26
Wm
–2
Hz
–1
) at 1390 MHz (Table 1), compared with
a total flux density at this frequency of 7 mJy
(3). The 5-mJy unpulsed emission probably
arises in the impact region described above.
We find it remarkable, with much of the
magnetosphere of B blown away by the wind
of A, that B still works as a pulsar. This
suggests that the radio emission is probably
generated close to the neutron star, providing
a direct constraint on the emission height.
Conclusion. We have detected the binary
companion of the millisecond pulsar
J07373039 as a pulsar, making this the first
known double-pulsar system. This discovery
confirms the neutron-star nature of the compan-
ions to recycled pulsars in eccentric binary sys-
tems and validates the suggested evolutionary
sequences in which a companion star, having
spun up the pulsar, forms a young pulsar in a
supernova explosion (31). The highly relativis-
tic nature of this compact system opens up
opportunities for much more stringent tests of
relativistic gravitation than have been possible
previously. Not only have we already measured
four quantities attributable to, and consistent
with, general relativity, but the mass ratio Ris a
new high-precision constraint that is indepen-
dent of gravitational theories. Within a year or
so, we expect to measure the orbital decay due
to emission of gravitational radiation. If the
intrinsic value due to gravitational-wave damp-
ing can be extracted, it will allow tests of radi-
ative aspects of gravitational theories mixed
with strong-field effects. On somewhat longer
time scales of a few years, we expect to detect
several other relativistic effects, such as geodet-
ic precession of the pulsars’ spin axes, spin-
orbit coupling, and other deviations, making
this a superb test bed for relativity.
The detection of the companion as a pul-
sar also opens up the possibility of using each
pulsar to probe the magnetosphere of the
other. The energy flux from the millisecond
pulsar is strongly affecting the pulse emission
process in the companion, and eclipses of the
millisecond pulsar by the companion are also
seen. Future measurements of orbital varia-
tions in pulse shapes, amplitudes, polariza-
tion, and timing over a range of radio fre-
quencies will give fascinating insights into
magnetospheric processes in pulsars.
References and Notes
1. J. H. Taylor, Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. A 341,
117 (1992).
2. R. A. Hulse, J. H. Taylor, Astrophys. J. 195, L51 (1975).
3. M. Burgay et al., Nature 426, 531 (2003).
4. V. Kalogera et al.,Astrophys. J., in press; preprint is avail-
able at http://arxiv.org/abs/astro-ph/0312101.
5. Receiver bandwidths used were 64 MHz at 680 MHz,
256 MHz at 1390 MHz, and 576 MHz at 3030 MHz.
6. More information is available at http://pulsar.
princeton.edu/tempo/.
7. T. Damour, J. H. Taylor, Phys. Rev. D 45, 1840 (1992).
8. J. H. Taylor, J. M. Weisberg, Astrophys. J. 345, 434 (1989).
9. I. H. Stairs, S. E. Thorsett, J. H. Taylor, A. Wolszczan,
Astrophys. J. 581, 501 (2002).
10. M. Bailes, S. M. Ord, H. S. Knight, A. W. Hotan,
Astrophys. J. 595, L49 (2003).
11. T. Damour, N. Deruelle, Ann. Inst. Henri Poincare´ 44,
263 (1986).
12. T. Damour, J. H. Taylor, Astrophys. J. 366, 501 (1991).
13. T. Damour, N. Deruelle, Ann. Inst. Henri Poincare´ 43,
107 (1985).
14. This is correct up to so-called first post-Newtonian [(v/c)
2
]
order (7), and any intersection of the PK-parameter lines
must be located on the R line, which will only deviate from
that shown in Fig. 3 by corrections of order (v/c)
4
.
15. This is correct because of the usage of the DD timing
model in our analysis and its definition of mass (11,13).
16. T. Damour, R. Ruffini, C. R. Acad. Sci. Paris Ser. A 279,
971 (1974).
17. B. M. Barker, R. F. O’Connell, Astrophys. J. 199, L25 (1975).
18. J. M. Weisberg, R. W. Romani, J. H. Taylor, Astrophys.
J. 347, 1030 (1989).
19. J. M. Weisberg, J. H. Taylor, Astrophys. J. 576, 942 (2002).
20. M. Kramer, Astrophys. J. 509, 856 (1998).
21. T. Damour, G. Scha¨fer, Nuovo Cimento 101, 127 (1988).
22. N. Wex, Class. Quantum Gravity 12, 983 (1995).
23. D. Bhattacharya, E. P. J. van den Heuvel, Phys. Rep.
203, 1 (1991).
24. G. S. Bisnovatyi-Kogan, B. V. Komberg, Sov. Astron.
18, 217 (1974).
25. N. Shibazaki, T. Murakami, J. Shaham, K. Nomoto,
Nature 342, 656 (1989).
26. Z. Arzoumanian, J. M. Cordes, I. Wasserman, Astro-
phys. J. 520, 696 (1999).
27. E. P. J. van den Heuvel, C. de Loore, Astron. Astrophys.
25, 387 (1973).
28. B. P. Flannery, E. P. J. van den Heuvel, Astron. Astro-
phys. 39, 61 (1975).
29. S. E. Thorsett, D. Chakrabarty, Astrophys. J. 512, 288
(1999).
30. R. N. Manchester, J. H. Taylor, Pulsars (Freeman, San
Francisco, 1977).
31. G. Srinivasan, E. P. J. van den Heuvel, Astron. Astro-
phys. 108, 143 (1982).
32. J. H. Taylor, J. M. Cordes, Astrophys. J. 411, 674
(1993).
33. We would like to thank J. Sarkissian and other members
of the Parkes multibeam team for their kind help with
making the observations described in this paper and N.
Wex and G. Scha¨fer for useful discussions. Extensive use
was made of the PSRCHIVE pulsar analysis system
developed by A. Hotan and colleagues (see http://
astronomy.swin.edu.au/pulsar). The Parkes radio tele-
scope is part of the Australia Telescope, which is funded
by the Commonwealth of Australia for operation as a
National Facility managed by CSIRO. M.B., A.P., and N.D.
acknowledge financial support from the Italian Ministry
of University and Research (MIUR) under the national
program Cofin 2001. F.C. is supported by NSF, NASA,
and National Radio Astronomy Observatory. D.R.L. is a
University Research Fellow funded by the Royal Society.
12 December 2003; accepted 30 December 2003
Published online 8 January 2004;
10.1126/science.1094645
Include this information when citing this paper.
Empathy for Pain Involves the
Affective but not Sensory
Components of Pain
Tania Singer,
1
* Ben Seymour,
1
John O’Doherty,
1
Holger Kaube,
2
Raymond J. Dolan,
1
Chris D. Frith
1
Our ability to have an experience of another’s pain is characteristic of empathy. Using
functional imaging, we assessed brain activity while volunteers experienced a painful
stimulus and compared it to that elicited when they observed a signal indicating that
their loved one—present in the same room—was receiving a similar pain stimulus.
Bilateral anterior insula (AI), rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), brainstem, and
cerebellum were activated when subjects received pain and also by a signal that a loved
one experienced pain. AI and ACC activation correlated with individual empathy scores.
Activity in the posterior insula/secondary somatosensory cortex, the sensorimotor
cortex (SI/MI), and the caudal ACC was specific to receiving pain. Thus, a neural response
in AI and rostral ACC, activated in common for “self” and “other” conditions, suggests
that the neural substrate for empathic experience does not involve the entire “pain
matrix.” We conclude that only that part of the pain network associated with its
affective qualities, but not its sensory qualities, mediates empathy.
Human survival depends on the ability to
function effectively within a social context.
Central to successful social interaction is
the ability to understand others intentions
and beliefs. This capacity to represent men-
tal states is referred to as “theory of mind”
(1) or the ability to “mentalize” (2). Empa-
thy, by contrast, broadly refers to being
able to understand what others feel, be it an
emotion or a sensory state. Accordingly,
empathic experience enables us to under-
stand what it feels like when someone else
experiences sadness or happiness, and also
pain, touch, or tickling (3).
Even though empathy has been exten-
sively discussed and investigated by philos-
ophers and social scientists, only recently
has it become a focus for neuroscience
(38). Influenced by perception-action
models of motor behavior and imitation (9),
1
Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, In-
stitute of Neurology, University College of London, 12
Queen Square, WC1N 3AR London, UK.
2
Headache
Group, Institute of Neurology, University College of
London, Queen Square, WC1N 3BG London, UK.
*To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-
mail: t.singer@fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk
RESEARCH ARTICLES
www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 303 20 FEBRUARY 2004 1157
Preston and de Waal (8) proposed a model
of empathy that incorporates most theoret-
ical accounts of, as well as empirical find-
ings on, empathy. The key suggestion is
that observation or imagination of another
person in a particular emotional state auto-
matically activates a representation of that
state in the observer, with its associated
autonomic and somatic responses (auto-
maticrefers to a process that does not
require conscious and effortful processing
but can nevertheless be inhibited or con-
trolled). The philosopher Susanne Langer
has described it as an involuntary breach of
individual separateness (10).
Recent neuroimaging studies have ex-
plored the neural correlates of empathic
experience by measuring brain activity
while subjects watched video clips showing
actors telling personal stories with neutral
or sad content accompanied by sad and
happy facial expressions (11), by comparing
brain activity associated with the imitation
of and the mere observation of pictures
showing different emotional facial expres-
sions (12), and by comparing neural re-
sponses elicited by watching videos of
faces with emotional expressions of disgust
and pleasure with responses induced by
smelling aversive and pleasant odors (13).
Whereas the first study did not permit the
identification of shared networks underly-
ing emotions in self and others, the latter
studies report activation in areas previously
identified in the perception and production
of facial emotional expressions (action rep-
resentation and emotional systems) as well
as during the smelling of aversive odors
(insula). These results suggest that regions
associated with feeling an emotion can be
activated by seeing the facial expression of
the same emotion, a phenomenon described
as emotional contagion.
A paradigm to study empathy in
vivo. The present study extends previous
findings in a number of ways. First, we
tested for shared and unique networks for
experienced and empathic pain. More spe-
cifically, we provide evidence for pain-
related empathic responses and demon-
strate that empathic experience does not
involve activation of an entire pain matrix,
but only of that component associated with
the affective dimension of pain experience.
Moreover, we show a relation between em-
pathy-related brain activity and individual
difference in empathy as assessed by com-
monly used empathy scales. We also
demonstratein contrast to accounts of
emotional contagionthat empathic re-
sponses can be elicited automatically in the
absence of an emotional cue (such as facial
emotional expressions) through mere pre-
sentation of an arbitrary cue that signals the
feeling state of another person.
We used functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI) to explore both common
and unique brain circuitry involved when
we experience pain in ourselves, as well as
when we observe someone else feeling
pain. Previous studies on pain have re-
vealed that noxious stimuli consistently ac-
tivate the secondary somatosensory cortex
(SII), insular regions, the anterior cingulate
cortex (ACC), the movement-related areas
such as the cerebellum and supplementary
motor areas and, less robustly, the thalamus
and the primary somatosensory cortex (SI)
(14,15). This pain-related network, which
is commonly referred to as the pain matrix,
served to define regions of interest in the
present study.
To investigate pain-related empathy, we
developed a paradigm that allows the in-
vestigation of empathic experience in vivo,
with the object of empathy being a real
person present in the same room. More
specifically, we investigated pain-related
empathy in 16 couples, under an assump-
tion that couples are likely to feel empathy
for each other. We assessed brain activity
in the female partner while painful stimu-
lation was applied to her or to her partners
right hand through an electrode attached to
the back of the hand. The partner was
seated next to the MRI scanner and the
right hand of each subject was placed on a
tilted board, allowing the female partner
with help of a mirror system to see her and
her partners right hand. On a large screen
situated behind the board, cues were pre-
sented in random order indicating whether
she (self) or her partner (other) would get
low (no pain condition) or high (pain con-
dition) stimulation. We were especially in-
terested in comparing pain-related brain ac-
tivity (assessed by the difference between
trials involving painful and nonpainful
stimulation) in the context of selfand
other.Questionnaires administered after
scanning served to validate measurements
of individual pain threshold made before
scanning, to obtain subjective evidence for
empathic experience during scanning, and
to assess stable individual differences in
empathy in order to determine whether
these scores predict the amplitude of
empathy-related brain activity.
Analysis of pain intensity ratings after
Fig. 1. Pain-related acti-
vation associated with
either experiencing pain
in oneself or observing
one’s partner feeling
pain. Areas in green rep-
resent significant activa-
tion (P0.001) for the
contrast pain–no pain in
the “self” condition and
areas in red for the con-
trast pain–no pain in the
“other” condition. The
results are superimposed
on a mean structural
scan of the 16 subjects.
Activations are shown
on sagittal (A and B) and
axial (C and D) slices. (A)
Activation in ACC and
cerebellum. (B) Bilateral
insula cortex extending
into lateral prefrontal
cortex, left posterior in-
sula extending into sec-
ondary somatosensory
cortex (SII), bilateral oc-
cipital cortex, and fusi-
form cortex. (C) Bilateral
insula and mediodorsal
thalamus. (D) Middle
and lateral cerebellum/
fusiform gyrus. For coor-
dinates of peak activa-
tions from “self” and
“other” conditions, see
tables S1 and S2.
RESEARCH ARTICLES
20 FEBRUARY 2004 VOL 303 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org1158
scanning confirmed the individual thresh-
olds for nonpainful and painful stimulation
determined before scanning (fig. S1) (16 ).
In addition, the unpleasantness ratings in-
dicated empathic involvement of the sub-
jects. Nonpainful trials were rated as being
significantly less unpleasant than painful
trials, irrespective of whether the pain was
applied to themselves or to the partner
[main effect of pain: F (1, 15) 19.93, P
0.001; main effect of self/other: F (1, 15)
0.12, P0.73)].
Does empathizing with pain in others
activate the entire pain matrix? Compar-
ison of brain activity associated with painful
and nonpainful trials in the selfcondition
revealed increased activity in contralateral
SI/MI, in bilateral SII with a peak activa-
tion in contralateral posterior insula ex-
tending into SII, in bilateral mid and ante-
rior insula, in ACC [caudal and posterior
rostral zones, using Picard and Stricks ter-
minology, (17 )], in right ventrolateral and
mediodorsal thalamus, brainstem, and mid
and right lateral cerebellum (Fig. 1, A to D,
green). These regions have all been identi-
fied as responding to painful stimuli in
previous imaging studies (14,15). Many
structures in this pain matrix were also
activated when pain was applied to the
partner, that is, in the absence of somato-
sensory stimulation (Fig. 1, A to D, red).
When comparing painful with nonpainful
trials in the context of other,increases in
brain activity were observed in the ACC
(anterior and posterior rostral zones), the
anterior insula (AI) bilaterally with an ex-
tension into inferior prefrontal cortex, the
cerebellum, and the brainstem. In addition,
significant activations were observed in the
ventral and dorsal visual stream, including
bilateral fusiform cortex, lateral occipital
and right posterior superior temporal sul-
cus, the left inferior parietal cortex, and the
left superior frontal cortex.
To test for common networks activated
by pain in the self and other conditions, we
performed a conjunction analysis (Fig. 2, A
and B), as well as a more conservative
masking procedure in which we masked the
simple contrast painno pain in others by
the simple contrast painno pain in self.
Both analyses revealed a network common
to pain in self and other conditions that
comprised ACC (caudal and posterior ros-
tral zones), bilateral middle insula and AI
(with a peak activation in the right AI),
brainstem, and lateral cerebellum.
We also tested for an interaction in or-
der to identify regions where there were
stronger pain-related activations in the self
as compared to the other condition. These
analyses confirmed that the contralateral
activations in SI/MI and SII/posterior insu-
la, as well as a region in caudal ACC, were
specific to the pain experience in the self
condition (Fig. 3). In contrast, extrastriate
visual related activations were specific to
the empathy condition.
Inspection of the time courses of pain-
related activity for self and others suggest
two peaks, probably as the result of an instant
response to the anticipation cue followed by
another response to the delivery of the pain
stimulus 3.5 s later. Accordingly, pain-related
activations for self in ACC and AI peak first
early, around 2 to 4 s, and again around 8 to
12 s (Fig. 2C). Activation specific to pain in
self in SI, SII, and caudal ACC seems only to
peak late, around 8 to 12 s, probably regis-
tering the actual receipt of the painful stimu-
lus (Fig. 3, A to C). Although the present
study was not designed to test differences
between anticipation and receipt of pain in
self and others, the present data suggest that
both anticipation and experience of pain in-
volve ACC and AI and that activation of
these networks is involved in understanding
the pain of others.
Do people scoring higher in empathy
show higher pain-related brain activity? To
explore further the role of anterior insular
cortex and ACCthe two major regions of
the pain matrix identified as being also
involved when empathizing with the pain
of otherswe determined whether individ-
ual differences in empathy, assessed by two
questionnaires, covary with brain activity
elicited in the empathy conditions (painno
pain in the othercondition). As Fig. 4
illustrates, individual differences in empa-
thy as measured by two empathy scales, the
Balanced Emotional Empathy Scale (18,
19) and the Empathic Concern Scale (a
subscale of the Interpersonal Reactivity In-
dex) (20), showed significant covariation
with activity in ACC (posterior rostral
zone) and left AI, but not right AI. Thus,
subjects who scored higher on general em-
pathy scales showed stronger activations in
areas significantly activated when the sub-
jects perceived their partner as being in
pain. In addition, an anterior part of ACC
(anterior rostral zone adjacent to paracin-
gulate sulcus) and lateral right cerebellum
showed significant covariation with self-
rated individual differences in empathy.
Fig. 2. Shared networks observed when pain was applied to self or to the partner. (A) and (B)
illustrate results of a conjunction analysis between the contrasts pain–no pain in the context
of self and other at P0.001. Results are shown on sagittal (A) and coronal (B) sections of
the mean structural scan. Coordinates refer to peak activations and are in mm. Increased
pain-related activation was observed in ACC [(–9, 6, 42); (0, 24, 33)], left insula [(–30, 9, 6);
(–39, 0, 18); (–48, 12, –3)], right anterior insula [(60, 15, 3); (39, 12, 3); (42, 27, 6)],
cerebellum [(–33, 66, –24); (27, 66, –18)] and brainstem (–6, –39, –30). (C) The time
courses of pain-related activation (pain–no pain) for peak activations in ACC and right anterior
insula cortex for self (green lines) and others (red lines). A complete list of activated areas is
in table S3. The anticipation cue was presented at time 0, and the pain was applied between
3 and 4 s into the trial (mean 3.5 s).
RESEARCH ARTICLES
www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 303 20 FEBRUARY 2004 1159
These findings underscore the crucial role
of rostral ACC and anterior insula cortices
for empathic experience related to pain.
They are also in line with a report based on
single-neuron recordings in a precingu-
lotomy patient that provided evidence that
neurons in the ACC can respond not only
when a person receives a painful stimulus
but also when a person observes or antici-
pates a potentially painful stimulus deliv-
ered to an experimenter (21). Note that these
regions are fundamentally different from sites
subserving mirror neurons that respond when
performing or watching a conspecific perform-
ing particular movements (22).
Numerous imaging studies have investi-
gated the neural correlates of pain experience
(14,15), but none have explored the empathic
experience of pain. Our results confirm pre-
vious findings of pain-related activation in
SI, SII, bilateral insula cortex, ACC, thala-
mus, brainstem, and cerebellum. In addition,
our data show that only part of the network
mediating pain experience is shared when
empathizing with pain in others. Empathizing
with someone elses pain elicited activity
principally in left and right AI, ACC, lateral
cerebellum, and brainstem. This activity was
elicited without an explicit focus on empathy
insofar as subjects were not aware of the
study aims, nor were they required to make
an explicit judgement of what their loved one
was feeling at the time of the scan. The
finding of empathy-related activation sug-
gests an automatic engagement of empathic
processes when perceiving pain in others.
Moreover, our analysis demonstrates that
pain-related activation in contralateral SI,
SII/posterior insula, and caudal ACC are spe-
cific to self-experienced pain, as opposed to
perceived pain in others.
Recent neuroimaging studies on pain
have demonstrated contralaterally biased
representations of painful stimulus in SI,
distinct parts of SII, and posterior insula, as
well as in lateral thalamus, which suggests
that these structures provide sensory-
discriminative information concerning no-
ciceptive stimuli such as location, quality,
and intensity (23,24 ). In contrast, AI and
ACC do not show such a contralateral bias,
supporting a role in coding the autonomic
and affective dimension of pain such as the
subjectively perceived unpleasantness (15,
2527 ). Using hypnosis as a tool to disas-
sociate sensory-discriminative from affec-
tive pain components, activation in ACC
(posterior rostral zone) was shown to be
modulated by perceived unpleasantness,
whereas activation in SI and SII was unaf-
fected (28). Similarly, attentional manipu-
lations modulate ACC (posterior rostral
zone) and right AI, but not posterior insular/
SII cortices (29). The role of ACC (poste-
rior rostral zone) and right AI in coding the
subjective affective dimension of pain is
also highlighted by findings showing that
activation of right AI was correlated with
subjective intensity ratings of thermal sen-
sation in a manner that is distinct from a
linear representation of stimulus temp-
erature in posterior insula (30). Indeed,
anticipation of pain activates more anterior
insular regions, whereas the actual experi-
ence of pain activates more posterior insu-
la, which suggests that the latter is associ-
ated with the actual sensory experience of
pain and the former with affective dimen-
sions such as the anticipatory arousal and
anxiety of pain (31). Furthermore, subjec-
tive reduction of pain associated with pla-
cebo and opioid analgesia is associated
with increased activity in rostral ACC and
right AI (32).
Conclusion. The strong anatomical con-
nections between regions constituting the
pain matrix suggest that these regions do not
function independently in encoding different
aspects of pain but are highly interactive.
Nevertheless, the results of the present study
and previous investigations suggest a segre-
gation of sensory-discriminative and auto-
nomic-affective attributes of the pain experi-
ence. Rostral ACC and AI appear to reflect
the emotional experience that evokes our re-
actions to pain and constitutes the neural
basis for our understanding of the feelings of
others and ourselves.
The above proposal is consistent with a
recent model by Craig based on detailed
anatomical observations that conceives of
pain as one of the homeostatic emotions
that reflects the internal (interoceptive)
Fig. 3. Brain activity specific to the experience of pain in oneself. (Ato C) illustrate results of
the interaction between the two factors (pain–no pain and self/other). Statistical maps
(threshold set at P0.001) are overlayed on axial (A), coronal (B), and sagittal (C) sections
from the mean structural scan. Coordinates refer to peak activations and are in mm. (A)
Increased pain-related activity [(–21, –33, 75); (–30, –24, 72); (–27, –36, 60)] in SI/MI. (B)
Activity in left posterior insula/SII [–39, –27, 24]. (C) Activity in a part of caudal ACC (6, 6, 42).
On the bottom, time courses are displayed for peak voxels of contralateral SI, SII, and caudal
ACC. Green lines reflect parameter estimates for pain-related activity (pain–no pain) in self;
red lines reflect pain-related activity in others.
RESEARCH ARTICLES
20 FEBRUARY 2004 VOL 303 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org1160
condition of the body, similar to tempera-
ture, sensual touch, itch, hunger, or thirst
(25,33). More specifically, it is assumed
that an image of the bodys internal state is
mapped to the brain by afferents that pro-
vide input by way of the ventromedial tha-
lamic nucleus to area 3a (sensorimotor cor-
tex), as well as to the mid/posterior dorsal
insula. In humans, this high-resolution,
modality-specific sensory representation of
the physiological condition of the body in
the posterior insula is initially re-represent-
ed in the AI on the same side of the brain,
and then, by way of a callosal pathway,
remapped to the other side of the brain in
the right AI. Such a second-order re-
representation in the right AI is assumed to
subserve subjective feelings and the aware-
ness of a physical self as a feeling entity. At
the same time, afferents also project by way
of the medial dorsal thalamic nucleus to
produce behavioral drive in ACC. Thus,
direct activation of both the insula (limbic
sensory cortex) and the ACC (the limbic
motor cortex) may correspond to a simul-
taneous generation of both a feeling and an
affective motivation with its attendant au-
tonomic effects (25,33). Indeed, imaging
studies focusing on the relation between
peripheral measures of arousal and brain
activity give robust evidence for the crucial
role of rostral ACC and AI cortices in the
representation of internal bodily states of
arousal, as well as emotional awareness
(26,27 ). Furthermore, activation in these
regions has been observed in a wide range
of imaging studies associated with positive
and negative subjective feelings expressed
by subjective ratings of facial trustworthi-
ness (34), musical enjoyment (35), sensual
touch (36 ), and distress resulting from
social exclusion (37 ). More generally,
these regions may play a critical role in
second-order representations of bodily ho-
meostatic states that underpin core repre-
sentations of self (38,39).
Our data suggest that empathizing with
the pain of others does not involve the acti-
vation of the whole pain matrix, but is based
on activation of those second-order re-
representations containing the subjective
affective dimension of pain. Accordingly, we
propose that these cortical re-representations
have a dual function. First, they form the
basis for our ability to form subjective
representation of feelings that allow us to
predict the effects of emotional stimuli with
respect to the self. Second, they serve as the
neural basis for our ability to understand
the emotional importance of a particular
stimulus for another person and to predict
its likely associated consequences. From a
functional and evolutionary perspective, a
detailed representation of the source and
nature of a noxious stimulus (i.e., intensity,
location) is of functional relevance when it
concerns our own body, allowing effective
engagement of executive action systems
(i.e., removing the noxious source). In
contrast, the understanding of someone
elses emotional reaction to pain does not
necessitate such a detailed sensory-
discriminative representation of the nox-
ious stimulus but rather a representation of
the subjective relevance of the stimulus as
reflected in the subjective unpleasantness
that the other person feels. Such decoupled
representationswhich are independent of
the sensory inputs of the outside world
have been postulated to be necessary for
our ability to mentalize, that is, to under-
stand the thoughts, beliefs, and intentions
of others (2). Our data suggest that we use
similar decoupled representations to under-
stand the feelings of others and that our
ability to empathize has evolved from a
system for representing our internal bodily
states and subjective feeling states.
References and Notes
1. D. Premack, G. Woodruff, Behav. Brain Sci. 1, 515
(1978).
2. U. Frith, C. D. Frith, Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser.
B358, 459 (2003).
3. V. Gallese, Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B 358,
517 (2003).
4. R. Adolphs, Trends Cognit. Sci. 3, 469 (1999).
5. R. Adolphs, Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4, 165 (2003).
6. L. Brothers, Concepts Neurosci. 1, 27 (1990).
7. R. W. Levenson, A. M. Ruef, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63,
234 (1992).
8. S. D. Preston, F. B. M. de-Waal, Behav. Brain Sci. 25,1
(2002).
9. W. Prinz, Eur. J. Cognit. Psychol. 9, 129 (1997).
10. S. K. Langer, An Essay on Human Feelings (John Hop-
kins Press, Baltimore, MD, 1988).
11. J. Decety, T. Chaminade, Neuropsychologia 41, 127
(2003).
12. L. Carr, M. Iacoboni, M. C. Dubeau, J. C. Mazziotta,
G. L. Lenzi, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 5497
(2003).
13. B. Wicker et al., Neuron 40, 655 (2003).
14. K. D. Davis, Neurol. Res. 22, 313 (2000).
15. R. Peyron, B. Laurent, L. Garcia-Larrea, Neurophysiol.
Clin. 30, 263 (2000).
16. Materials and methods are available as supporting
material on Science Online.
17. N. Picard, P. L. Strick, Cerebral Cortex 6, 342
(1996).
18. A. Mehrabian, N. Epstein, J. Pers. 40, 525 (1972).
19. A. Mehrabian, Aggress. Behav. 23, 433 (1997).
20. M. A. Davis, JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in
Psychology 10, 85 (1980) (American Psychological
Association, Journal Supplement Abstract Service).
21. W. D. Hutchison, K. D. Davis, A. M. Lozano, R. R.
Tasker, J. O. Dostrovsky, Nature Neurosci. 2, 403
(1999).
22. J. Grezes, J. Decety, Hum. Brain Mapp. 12, 1 (2001).
23. U. Bingel et al., Neuroimage 18, 740 (2003).
24. J. C. Brooks, T. J. Nurmikko, W. E. Bimson, K. D. Singh,
N. Roberts, Neuroimage 15, 293 (2002).
25. A. D. Craig, Nature Rev. Neurosci. 3, 655 (2002).
26. H. D. Critchley, S. Wiens, P. Rotshtein, A. O
¨hman, R. J.
Dolan, Nature Neurosci. 7, 189 (2004).
27. H. D. Critchley et al., Brain 126, 2139 (2003).
28. P. Rainville, G. H. Duncan, D. D. Price, B. Carrier, M. C.
Bushnell, Science 277, 968 (1997).
29. R. Peyron et al., Brain 122, 1765 (1999).
Fig. 4. Activation level (parameter estimates) observed within peaks of the ACC and the left insula
during empathy-related conditions (pain–no pain in other) are significantly correlated with
individual differences in empathy as measured by (A) the Empathic Concern Scale of Davis (20) and
(B) the Balanced Emotional Empathy Scale of Mehrabian (18,19). The lines represent the linear best
fit; rrefers to the correlation coefficient. All correlations are significant on the P0.05 level. Peak
activations lie within regions of ACC and left insula that were activated in the simple contrast
pain–no pain in others. Coordinates refer to peak activations and are in mm.
RESEARCH ARTICLES
www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 303 20 FEBRUARY 2004 1161
30. A. D. Craig, K. Chen, D. Bandy, E. M. Reiman, Nature
Neurosci. 3, 184 (2000).
31. A. Ploghaus et al., Science 284, 1979 (1999).
32. P. Petrovic, E. Kalso, K. M. Petersson, M. Ingvar, Science
295, 1737 (2002).
33. A. D. Craig, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 500 (2003).
34. J. S. Winston, B. A. Strange, J. O’Doherty, R. J. Dolan,
Nature Neurosci. 5, 277 (2002).
35. A. J. Blood, R. J. Zatorre, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
98, 11818 (2001).
36. H. Olausson et al., Nature Neurosci. 5, 900 (2002).
37. N. I. Eisenberger, M. D. Lieberman, K. D. Williams,
Science 302, 290 (2003).
38. P. S. Churchland, Science 296, 308 (2002).
39. A. R. Damasio, Descartes’ Error (Putnam, New York,
1994).
40. This work was supported by a personal grant for T.S.
from the German Academy of Natural Sciences
Leopoldina, Halle, with the grant BMBF-LPD 9901/8-
73 from the Ministry of Education and Science, by the
Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, and
by the University College of London. R.J.D. and C.D.F.
are in receipt of Wellcome Trust program grants. We
thank S. Kiebel, J. Schultz, K. Wiech, R. Kalisch, P.
Aston, E. Featherstone, and P. Allen for their help.
Supporting Online Material
www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/303/5661/1157/
DC1
Materials and Methods
Fig. S1
Tables S1 to S5
References
11 November 2003; accepted 23 December 2003
Placebo-Induced Changes in fMRI
in the Anticipation and
Experience of Pain
Tor D. Wager,
1
*James K. Rilling,
2
Edward E. Smith,
1
Alex Sokolik,
3
Kenneth L. Casey,
3
Richard J. Davidson,
4
Stephen M. Kosslyn,
5
Robert M. Rose,
6
Jonathan D. Cohen
2,7
The experience of pain arises from both physiological and psychological factors,
including one’s beliefs and expectations. Thus, placebo treatments that have no
intrinsic pharmacological effects may produce analgesia by altering expecta-
tions. However, controversy exists regarding whether placebos alter sensory
pain transmission, pain affect, or simply produce compliance with the sugges-
tions of investigators. In two functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
experiments, we found that placebo analgesia was related to decreased brain
activity in pain-sensitive brain regions, including the thalamus, insula, and
anterior cingulate cortex, and was associated with increased activity during
anticipation of pain in the prefrontal cortex, providing evidence that placebos
alter the experience of pain.
The idea that sensory experience is shaped
by ones attitudes and beliefs has gained
currency among psychologists, physicians,
and the general public. Perhaps nowhere is
this more apparent than in our ability to
modulate pain perception. A special case of
this phenomenon is placebo analgesia, in
which the mere belief that one is receiving
an effective analgesic treatment can reduce
pain (15). Recently, some researchers
have attributed placebo effects to response
bias and/or to publication biases (6), which
raises the issue of whether placebo treat-
ments actually influence the sensory, affec-
tive, and cognitive processes that mediate
the experience of pain.
One important piece of evidence that
placebo effects are not simply due to re-
sponse or publication bias is that such ef-
fects can be reversed by the mu-opioid
antagonist naloxone (2,3,7), suggesting
that some kinds of placebo effects may be
mediated by the opioid system. However,
naloxone has also been shown to produce
hyperalgesia independent of placebo, in
some cases offsetting rather than blocking
the effects of placebo analgesia (8). Al-
though pharmacological blockade provides
suggestive evidence regarding the neuro-
chemical mechanisms mediating placebo
effects, such data do not illuminate the
nature of the information-processing sys-
tem that gives rise to such effects. Neuro-
imaging data can provide complementary
evidence of how pain processing in the
brain is affected by placebos and about the
time course of pain processing. Identifying
placebo-induced changes in brain activity
in regions associated with sensory, affec-
tive, and cognitive pain processing (9) may
provide insight into which components of
pain processing are affected by placebo. In
addition, identifying changes that occur at par-
ticular timesin anticipation of pain, early or
late during pain processingmay shed light on
how cognitive systems mediating expectancy
interact with pain and opioid systems.
In two functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI) experiments (n24 and
n23), we examined two hypotheses re-
garding the psychological and neural mech-
anisms that underlie placebo analgesia. Our
first hypothesis was that if placebo manip-
ulations reduce the experience of pain,
pain-responsive regions of the brain should
show a reduced fMRI blood oxygen level
dependent (BOLD) signal (a measure relat-
ed to neural activity) during pain. [Pain-
responsive regions, or the pain matrix,
include thalamus, somatosensory cortex,
insula, and anterior cingulate cortex (10
14).] Our second hypothesis was that pla-
cebo modulates activity of the pain matrix
by creating expectations for pain relief,
which in turn inhibit activity in pain-
processing regions. Converging evidence
suggests that the prefrontal cortex (PFC),
the dorsolateral aspect (DLPFC) in partic-
ular, acts to maintain and appropriately up-
date internal representations of goals and
expectations, which modulate processing in
other brain areas (15,16). Thus, stronger
PFC activation during the anticipation of
pain should correlate with greater placebo-
induced pain relief as reported by participants
and greater placebo-induced reductions in neu-
ral activity within pain regions (17).
Placebo reduces reported pain and
brain activity in Study 1 (shock pain). The
design of Study 1 is illustrated in Fig. 1A
(see the figure legend for a description)
(18). First, to confirm that application of
shock elicited a neural response in pain-
related areas, we compared brain activity in
the intense shock versus no shock condi-
tions. This revealed activation of the classic
pain matrix (11,14,19,20), including thal-
amus, primary somatosensory cortex/
primary motor cortex (S1/M1), secondary
somatosensory cortex (SII), midbrain, an-
terior insula, anterior cingulate cortex
(ACC), ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, and
cerebellum (fig. S1). As expected, activa-
tions in thalamus, S1, SII, and M1 were
larger in the left hemisphere, contralateral
to the wrist where shocks were applied,
whereas cerebellar activation was ipsilater-
al, although some bilateral activation was
observed in each of these areas. We also
1
Department of Psychology, University of Michi-
gan, 525 East University, Ann Arbor, MI 48109
1109, USA.
2
Center for the Study of Brain, Mind and
Behavior, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ
08544, USA.
3
Department of Neurology, Veterans
Affairs Medical Center, University of Michigan, Ann
Arbor, MI 48109, USA.
4
Department of Psychology,
University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI 53706, USA.
5
Department of Psychology, Harvard University,
Cambridge, MA 02138, USA.
6
Mind Brain Body and
Health Initiative, University of Texas Medical
Branch, Galveston, TX 77555, USA.
7
Department of
Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ
08544, USA.
*To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail:
torw@umich.edu, tor@paradox.psych.columbia.edu
Present address: Department of Psychology, Colum-
bia University, 1190 Amsterdam Avenue, New York,
NY 10027, USA.
RESEARCH ARTICLES
20 FEBRUARY 2004 VOL 303 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org1162
... Specifically, IFG activity is responsible for action observation and extracting the painful meaning from these actions, as well as mimicry of others' emotions [13][14][15] . ACC and AI activities support emotional contagion ("experiencing the emotions of another person") and formation of shared affective representations between oneself and the other experiencing pain 12,16,17 . The AI and amygdala are engaged in differentiating others' emotional states and generating one's own negative affective responses triggered by others' emotional states [18][19][20] . ...
... Dots represent individual participants and gray lines show the conditional differences for individual participants. The correlations between the ratings of patients (n = 16) or healthy participants (n = 22) and the normative ratings for empathy strength (e), perceived pain intensity (f), and unpleasantness (g). Permutation tests (two-sided) showed that the ratings of patients and healthy participants were similar to the normative ratings across all painful stimuli (for patient-normative (P-N) similarity, empathy strength: p < 0.001, perceived pain intensity: p < 0.001, unpleasantness: p = 0.003; for healthy-normative (H-N) similarity, empathy strength: p < 0.001, perceived pain intensity: p < 0.001, unpleasantness: p < 0.001). ...
... We found that ACC alpha power was specifically associated with the intensity of perceived pain (β = −0. 16 Fig. 5f). These results showed that stronger suppression of AI low-frequency power was related to higher intensity of perceived pain in others but a lower level of self-related unpleasantness. ...
Article
Full-text available
Empathy enables understanding and sharing of others’ feelings. Human neuroimaging studies have identified critical brain regions supporting empathy for pain, including the anterior insula (AI), anterior cingulate (ACC), amygdala, and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). However, to date, the precise spatio-temporal profiles of empathic neural responses and inter-regional communications remain elusive. Here, using intracranial electroencephalography, we investigated electrophysiological signatures of vicarious pain perception. Others’ pain perception induced early increases in high-gamma activity in IFG, beta power increases in ACC, but decreased beta power in AI and amygdala. Vicarious pain perception also altered the beta-band-coordinated coupling between ACC, AI, and amygdala, as well as increased modulation of IFG high-gamma amplitudes by beta phases of amygdala/AI/ACC. We identified a necessary combination of neural features for decoding vicarious pain perception. These spatio-temporally specific regional activities and inter-regional interactions within the empathy network suggest a neurodynamic model of human pain empathy.
... We understand others partially by "copying" their behaviours and internal states in our own minds. There is ample evidence of such embodied vicarious representation of others motor, motivational and affective states (Katsyri et al., 2013;Mobbs et al., 2009;Nummenmaa, Glerean, et al., 2012;Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004;Singer et al., 2004). Together with the data on the tendency to overeat in the presence of others (Higgs & Thomas, 2016;Ruddock et al., 2021) these data suggest that the tendency to automatically remap others' feeding behaviour in the observers' brain could be a potent modulator of feeding and food-induced reward. ...
... Overall, our results suggest that the brain regions participating in voluntary movements, somatosensation and reward processing activate during vicarious eating, likely reflecting mental simulation of the actions and emotions associated with first-hand feeding similarly as has previously been established for emotions and various motor actions states (Katsyri et al., 2013;Nummenmaa, Glerean, et al., 2012;Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004;Singer et al., 2004). We propose that this tendency to internally mimic others feeding in social contexts might be a powerful cue for increasing appetite and initiating feeding. ...
Preprint
Eating is inherently social for humans. Yet, most neuroimaging studies of appetite and food-induced reward have focused on studying brain responses to food intake or viewing pictures of food alone. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure haemodynamic responses to "vicarious" feeding. The subjects (n=97) viewed a series of short videos representing naturalistic episodes of social eating intermixed with videos without feeding/appetite-related content. Viewing the vicarious feeding (versus control) videos activated motor and premotor cortices, thalamus, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices, consistent with somatomotor and affective engagement. Responses to the feeding videos were also downregulated as a function of the participants BMI. Taken together, seeing others eating engages the corresponding motor and affective programs in the viewers brains, potentially increasing appetite and promoting mutual feeding.
... increased negative affect when witnessing a friend or a romantic partner go through a laboratory stressor (Meyer et al., 2013;Singer et al., 2004). In the present studies, we focus on reactivity to partner stress, which we define as increases in negative affect in response to daily hassles experienced by one's romantic partner. ...
... Even in the dyadic analysis by , the focus was on one's reactivity to one's own experiences of discrimination. Given that individuals also experience negative affect when witnessing significant others undergoing stressors (e.g., Singer et al., 2004), a promising approach to further our understanding of how "affective reactivity operates uniquely within couples" (Ong et al., , p. 1189 is to assess reactivity to partner stress. We define this interpersonal construct as within-person increases in negative affect as a function of the partner's exposure to stressors (see Figure 1, Panel A). ...
Article
Full-text available
Affective reactivity, defined as within-person increases in negative affect triggered by daily stressors, has well-established links to personal well-being. Prior work conceptualized affective reactivity as an intrapersonal phenomenon, reflecting reactions to one’s own stressors. Here, we conceptualized reactivity interpersonally, examining one’s responses to a romantic partner’s daily stressors. Across four longitudinal dyadic studies, we investigated how reactivity to partner stress predicts relationship quality appraisals. In fledgling couples, reactivity to a partner’s stressors, assessed via weekly (Study 1; N = 152) and daily (Study 2; N = 144) diaries, positively predicted partner relationship quality. In both studies, the associations were mediated by the partner’s perceptions of responsiveness. Furthermore, reactivity to partner stress buffered against declines in partner relationship quality over 8 weeks in Study 1 and 13 months in Study 2. The relevance of reactivity to partner stress for relationship quality diminished in the later stages of relationships. Among samples of established couples (Studies 3 and 4, Ns = 164 and 208, respectively), reactivity to partner stress did not directly predict partner relationship quality or moderate its trajectory over time. Overall, the predominant pattern across four studies painted a portrait of relational well-being benefits specific to fledgling relationships. Through its novel framework of situating affective reactivity interpersonally between partners, the present research contributes to both affective science and relationship science.
... Many of these approaches have been taken from other fields (e.g., social and cognitive psychology), making assumptions about their validity and suitability for (a) studies in the autistic population and (b) studies within dynamic social interaction between autistic and neurotypical people. None have directly measured the "empathy" part of double empathy insofar as how empathy is currently measured in social cognitive science (e.g., Mackes et al., 2018;Singer et al., 2004;Zaki et al., 2009). This follows a general trend in autism research, and clinical science more widely, whereby measures developed in the general population are used without fully considering their conceptual and psychometric validity in clinical populations and for testing clinically relevant theories (e.g., see example in Z. J. Williams & Gotham, 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Work on the “double empathy problem” (DEP) is rapidly growing in academic and applied settings (e.g., clinical practice). It is most popular in research on conditions, like autism, which are characterized by social cognitive difficulties. Drawing from this literature, we propose that, while research on the DEP has the potential to improve understanding of both typical and atypical social processes, it represents a striking example of a weak derivation chain in psychological science. The DEP is poorly conceptualized, and we find that it is being conflated with many other constructs (i.e., reflecting the “jingle–jangle” fallacy). We provide examples to show how this underlies serious problems with translating theoretical claims into empirical predictions and evidence. To start tackling these problems, we propose that DEP research needs reconsideration, particularly through a better synthesis with the cognitive neuroscience literature on social interaction. Overall, we argue for a strengthening of the derivation chain pertaining to the DEP, toward more robust research on (a)typical social cognition. Until then, we caution against the translation of DEP research into applied settings.
Article
Placebo and nocebo effects have been well documented for nearly two centuries. However, research has only relatively recently begun to explicate the neurobiological underpinnings of these phenomena. Similarly, research on the broader social implications of placebo/nocebo effects, especially within healthcare delivery settings, is in a nascent stage. Biological and psychosocial outcomes of placebo/nocebo effects are of equal relevance. A common pathway for such outcomes is the endogenous opioid system. This chapter describes the history of placebo/nocebo in medicine; delineates the current state of the literature related to placebo/nocebo in relation to pain modulation; summarizes research findings related to human performance in sports and exercise; discusses the implications of placebo/nocebo effects among diverse patient populations; and describes placebo/nocebo influences in research related to psychopharmacology, including the relevance of endogenous opioids to new lines of research on antidepressant pharmacotherapies.
Article
The human tendency to help others in need has been subject to trans-, inter-, and multidisciplinary studies (e.g., anthropology, neurobiology, evolutionary psychology, economy), within the frame of studying the mechanisms and adaptive significance of human prosocial behavior. Volunteering directed to unrelated and unfamiliar individuals is one common form of such helping behavior. Helping others may be adaptive for a species at a macro-level, which in turn is mediated by neurobiological mechanisms. A key target for analysis of the neurobiological underpinnings of volunteering is the endogenous opioid system (EOS). This chapter discusses EOS activity as a potential mediator of volunteering behavior. Evidence of the congruence between EOS involvement in social group behavior and social bonding and the role of these phenomena in volunteerism is reviewed. Models and empirical evidence of the mechanisms and adaptive value of helping unrelated others are discussed and integrated, including the mammalian caregiving system, the neurobiological model of prosocial behavior, synchrony promoting social bonding, and stress-driven motivation of prosocial action in immediate needs.
Preprint
Full-text available
In daily life, we must recognize others' emotions so we can respond appropriately. This ability may rely, at least in part, on neural responses similar to those associated with our own emotions. We hypothesized that the insula, a cortical region near the junction of the temporal, parietal, and frontal lobes, may play a key role in this process. We recorded local field potential (LFP) activity in human neurosurgical patients performing two tasks, one focused on identifying their own emotional response and one on identifying facial emotional responses in others. We found matching patterns of gamma- and high-gamma band activity for the two tasks in the insula. Three other regions (MTL, ACC, and OFC) clearly encoded both self- and other- emotions, but used orthogonal activity patterns to do so. These results support the hypothesis that the insula plays a particularly important role in mediating between experienced vs. observed emotions.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: The intensity of pain experienced in sports or daily life injuries can be perceived differently in different social environments. The aim of this study is to compare the perceived intensity of pain in sports and daily life musculoskeletal injuries among university students with different social backgrounds. Methods: Pain beliefs of 521 university students (309 physical education and sport, 212 physical therapy and rehabilitation students) were evaluated using the Pain Beliefs Questionnaire. Pain fear was evaluated using the Fear of Pain Questionnaire-III Scale. Perceived pain intensity was measured using vignette scenarios of musculoskeletal injuries in sports and daily life. The students were grouped according to department, gender, and professional sports branch. Results: A significant difference was found in pain psychological beliefs between groups (p<0.01). Psychological pain beliefs of women were lower than men (p<0.01). Estimated pain intensity in vignettes containing sport injury scenarios was found to be higher in women than men (p=0.01). A strong positive correlation was found between fear of pain and pain intensity in both sport and daily life vignettes (p=0.001). Individuals who are professionally involved in sport activities reported higher pain intensity in daily life scenarios (p=0.01). Conclusion: The vignette method can be used to determine pain intensity in musculoskeletal injuries for different genders. Women are more sensitive in perceiving and determining pain intensity. Professional athletes did not yield high scores to sport vignettes, despite giving high scores to daily life vignettes. This may suggest that the sport social environment normalizes pain from sport injuries.
Chapter
Full-text available
Interventionswissenschaft hat das Potential, nachhaltige Veränderungen zu ermöglichen. Roland J. Schuster gibt einen umfassenden Einblick in Theorie, Praxis und Entstehungsgeschichte dieser besonderen Form der Wissenschaft. Aus trans- und interdisziplinärer Sicht widmet der Band sich der Bedeutung von Wissen und Bewusstsein in Bezug auf die menschliche Wahrnehmung und Kommunikation. Sieben Forschungsbeispiele zeigen auf, wie Veranstaltungsdesign, didaktische Interventionen oder Gruppenreflexionen neue Wege öffnen können - für ein Mehr an Kreativität in Praxis, Forschung und Lehre.
Article
Full-text available
The relation between empathy (defined as the ability to perceive accurately how another person is feeling) and physiology was studied in 31 Ss. Ss viewed 15-min marital interactions and used a rating dial to indicate continuously how they thought a designated spouse was feeling. Rating accuracy was determined by comparing Ss' ratings with identical self-ratings obtained previously from the target spouse. Physiological linkage between S and target was determined using bivariate time-series analyses applied to 5 autonomic and somatic measures obtained from the S during the rating task and from the target spouse during the original conversation. Accuracy of rating negative emotion was greatest when S and target evidenced high levels of physiological linkage across time. Accuracy of detecting positive emotion was related to a state of low cardiovascular arousal in the S, but not to physiological linkage between S and target.
Article
Full-text available
An individual has a theory of mind if he imputes mental states to himself and others. A system of inferences of this kind is properly viewed as a theory because such states are not directly observable, and the system can be used to make predictions about the behavior of others. As to the mental states the chimpanzee may infer, consider those inferred by our own species, for example, purpose or intention, as well as knowledge, belief, thinking, doubt, guessing, pretending, liking, and so forth. To determine whether or not the chimpanzee infers states of this kind, we showed an adult chimpanzee a series of videotaped scenes of a human actor struggling with a variety of problems. Some problems were simple, involving inaccessible food – bananas vertically or horizontally out of reach, behind a box, and so forth – as in the original Kohler problems; others were more complex, involving an actor unable to extricate himself from a locked cage, shivering because of a malfunctioning heater, or unable to play a phonograph because it was unplugged. With each videotape the chimpanzee was given several photographs, one a solution to the problem, such as a stick for the inaccessible bananas, a key for the locked up actor, a lit wick for the malfunctioning heater. The chimpanzee's consistent choice of the correct photographs can be understood by assuming that the animal recognized the videotape as representing a problem, understood the actor's purpose, and chose alternatives compatible with that purpose.
Article
Article
Six years of careful observations have revealed a gradual secular evolution in the pulse profile of binary pulsar PSR 1913 + 16. At frequencies near 1400 MHz, the flux density in the first component of the double-peaked profile is decreasing relative to the second component at a rate slightly greater than 1 percent per year. The changes are consistent with the expected rate of general relativistic geodetic precession of the pulsar spin axis. In addition to its relativistic implications, the precessional motion is providing unique two-dimensional information on the structure of the pulsar emission zone.
Article
Fourteen-year observations of the binary pulsar PSR 1913 + 16 provided data consistent with a straightforward model allowing for the motion of the earth, special and general relativistic effects within the solar system, dispersive propagation in the interstellar medium, relativistic motion of the pulsar in its orbit, and deterministic spin-down behavior of the pulsar itself. The results indicate that at the present level of precision, the PSR 1913 + 16 can be modeled dynamically as a pair of orbiting point masses. Five Keplerian and five post-Keplerian orbital parameters are therefore mostly determined with remarkably high precision. The masses of the pulsar and its companion are determined to be m1 = 1.442 + or - 0.003 and m2 = 1.386 + or - 0.003 times the mass of the sun, respectively, and the orbit is found to be decaying at a rate equal to 1.01 + or - 0.01 times the general relativistic prediction for gravitational damping. The results represent the first experimental tests of gravitation theory not restricted to the weak-field, slow-motion limit in which nonlinearities and radiation effects are negligible. Excellent agreement between observation and theory indicates conclusively that gravitational radiation exists, at the level predicted by general relativity.
Article
This study was designed to explore relations of emotional empathy (two scales) with aggression and violence (three scales). An initial study investigated validity of one of the violence scales, the Risk of Eruptive Violence Scale (REV), by comparing individual REV scores with individual histories of criminal violence for a sample of incarcerated juveniles. Validity of the REV was supported by a very strong correlation of .71 between REV scores and the amount of criminal violence in this homogeneous sample. The second study yielded positive intercorrelations among measures of aggression and violence, positive intercorrelations among measures of emotional empathy, and negative correlations (ranging from –.22 to –.50, P < .05) of measures of aggression and violence with measures of emotional empathy. Analyses of the five scales in terms of the Pleasure-Arousability-Dominance (PAD) Temperament Model helped explain similarities of the emotional empathy scales with other individual difference measures of prosocial orientation (e.g., affiliation). PAD analyses also explained some paradoxical effects of experimental "empathy arousal" on aggression toward victims. Aggr. Behav. 23:433–445, 1997. © 1997 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
Article
Brain activity was studied by fMRI in 18 healthy subjects during stimulation of the thenar eminence of the hand with either warm (non-painful, 40 degrees C) or hot (painful, 46-49 degrees C) stimuli using a contact thermode. Experiments were performed on the right and left hand independently and with two attentional contexts: subjects either attended to pain or attended to a visual global motion discrimination task (to distract them from pain). Group analysis demonstrated that attended warm stimulation of the right hand did not produce any significantly activated clusters. Painful thermal stimulation of either hand elicited significant activity over a large network of brain regions, including insula, inferior frontal gyrus, cingulate gyrus, secondary somatosensory cortex, cerebellum, and medial frontal gyrus (corrected P < 0.05). Insula activity was distributed along its anterior-posterior axis and depended on the hand stimulated and attentional context. In particular, activity within the posterior insula was contralateral to the site of stimulation, tested using regions of interest (ROI) analysis: significant side x site interaction (P = 0.001). With attention diverted from the painful stimulus bilateral anterior insula activity moved posteriorly to midinsula and decreased in extent (ROI analysis: significant main effect of attention (P = 0.03)). The role of the insula in thermosensation and attention is discussed.
Article
A new framework for the understanding of functional relationships between perception and action is discussed. According to this framework, perceived events and planned actions share a common representational domain (common-coding approach). Supporting evidence from two classes of experimental paradigms is presented: induction paradigms and interference paradigms. Induction paradigms study how certain stimuli induce certain actions by virtue of similarity. Evidence from two types of induction tasks is reviewed: sensorimotor synchronisation and spatial compatibility tasks. Interference paradigms study the mutual interference between the perception of ongoing events and the preparation and control of ongoing action. Again, evidence from two types of such tasks is reviewed, implying interference in either direction. It is concluded that the evidence available supports the common coding principle. A further general principle emerging from these studies is the action effect principle that is, the principle that cognitive representations of action effects play a critical role in the planning and control of these actions.