ArticlePDF Available

[ELLIOTT SLATER WAS WRONG] --------> Evolution of the human Fear Circuitry and ACUTE SOCIOGENIC PSEUDO-NEUROLOGICAL SYMPTOMS: The Neolithic balanced-polymorphism Hypothesis.

Authors:
  • US Department of Veterans Affairs Honolulu, Hawaii retired

Abstract

ABSTRACT. In light of the increasing threat of large-scale massacres, such as terrorism against non-combatants (civilians), more attention is warranted to acute sociogenic pseudo-neurological symptoms, especially epidemic sociogenic symptoms [MASS PSYCHOGENIC ILLNESS]. We propose that Conversion Disorders are etiologically related to specific evolutionary pressures (inescapable threats to life) in the late stage of the human environment of evolutionary adaptedness (the neolithic). [Bracha at al.] have recently argued that from the neuroevolutionary perspective, medically unexplained efferent vasovagal syncope and medically unexplained craniofacial musculoskeletal pain in young otherwise healthy individuals, may be taxonomized as stress and fear-circuitry disorders. In the present article, we extend neuroevolutionary perspectives to Acute Sociogenic Pseudo-Neurological symptoms: non-epileptic attacks ("pseudoseizures"), pseudo-paralysis, and psychogenic blindness. We hypothesize that these perplexing pseudoneurological stress-triggered symptoms, which constitute psychopathology in extant humans, are traceable to allele-variant polymorphisms which spread into a balanced polymorphism state during the Neolithic EEA. During Neolithic warfare, conversive symptoms may have increased the survival odds for some non-combatants by visually (i.e., "non-verbally") signaling to predatory conspecifics that one does not present a danger. This is consistent with the age and sex pattern of conversive disorders. Testable and falsifiable predictions are presented; e.g., at the genome-transcriptome interface, one of the major oligogenic loci involved in conversive spectrum disorders may carry a developmentally sensitive allele in a stable polymorphism (balanced polymorphism) in which the gene expression mechanism is gradually suppressed by pleiotropic androgens especially DHEA-S. Taxonomic implications for a much-needed rapprochement between the DSM and the ICD are discussed. PS. Also see comments.
Special review
Evolution of the human fear-circuitry and acute sociogenic
pseudoneurological symptoms: The Neolithic
balanced-polymorphism hypothesis
H. Stefan Bracha*, Dawn T. Yoshioka,
Nicole K. Masukawa, Deborah J.J. Stockman
National Center for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Department of Veterans Affairs,
Pacific Islands Health Care System Spark M. Matsunaga Medical Center 1132 Bishop St. #307, Honolulu, USA
Received 10 December 2004; accepted 16 May 2005
Available online 18 August 2005
Abstract
In light of the increasing threat of large-scale massacres such as terrorism against non-combatants (civilians), more attention
is warranted not only to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) but also to acute sociogenic pseudoneurological (bconversionQ)
symptoms, especially epidemic sociogenic symptoms. We posit that conversion disorders are etiologically related to specific
evolutionary pressures (inescapable threats to life) in the late stage of the human environment of evolutionary adaptedness
(EEA). Bracha et al. have recently argued that from the neuroevolutionary perspective, medically unexplained efferent
vasovagal syncope and medically unexplained craniofacial musculoskeletal pain in young otherwise healthy individuals,
may be taxonomized as stress and fear-circuitry disorders. In the present article, we extend neuroevolutionary perspectives
to acute pseudoneurological sociogenic (bconversiveQ) symptoms: psychogenic non-epileptic attacks (bpseudoseizuresQ),
epidemic sociogenic disorders (DSM-IV-TR Epidemic bHysteriaQ), conversive motor deficits (pseudo-paralysis and pseudo-
cerebellar symptoms), and psychogenic blindness. We hypothesize that these perplexing pseudoneurological stress-triggered
symptoms, which constitute psychopathology in extant humans, are traceable to allele-variant polymorphisms which spread
during the Neolithic EEA. During Neolithic warfare, conversive symptoms may have increased the survival odds for some non-
combatants by visually (i.e., bnon-verballyQ) signaling to predatory conspecifics that one does not present a danger. This is
consistent with the age and sex pattern of conversive disorders. Testable and falsifiable predictions are presented; e.g., at the
genome–transcriptome interface, one of the major oligogenic loci involved in conversive spectrum disorders may carry a
developmentally sensitive allele in a stable polymorphism (balanced polymorphism) in which the gene expression mechanism is
gradually suppressed by pleiotropic androgens especially dehydroxyepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEA-S). Taxonomic implica-
0165-0327/$ - see front matter. Published by Elsevier B.V.
doi:10.1016/j.jad.2005.05.013
* Corresponding author. National Center for PTSD, Department of Veterans Affairs, Pacific Islands Health Care System, Spark M. Matsunaga
Medical Center, 1132 Bishop St., Ste. #307, Honolulu, HI 96813-2830, USA. Tel.: +1 808 566 1652; fax: +1 808 566 1885.
E-mail address: h.bracha@med.va.gov (H.S. Bracha).
Journal of Affective Disorders 88 (2005) 119– 129
www.elsevier.com/locate/jad
tions for the much-needed rapprochement between the forthcoming Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders,
Fifth Edition (DSM-V) and the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) are discussed.
Published by Elsevier B.V.
Keywords: Allele-variant polymorphisms; Conversion disorder; DSM-V; Terrorism; Combat; War
Contents
1. Distal (evolutionary) etiological perspectives in psychiatry ............................ 120
2. Up to 95.3% of conversive symptoms remain unexplained ............................ 121
3. Pseudoneurological symptoms as fitness-enhancing Neolithic fear responses ................... 122
4. Neolithic inter-tribal warfare patterns and pseudoneurological conversive symptoms ............... 122
5. Subtypes within the sociogenic–conversive spectrum ............................... 123
5.1. Acute fear-induced motor deficits ..................................... 123
5.2. Sociogenic fear-induced motor (non-epileptic) attacks ........................... 123
5.3. Acute fear-induced epidemic faintness ................................... 123
6. Insights from recent bio-anthropological research ................................. 124
7. Implications for DSM-V and some empirically testable and falsifiable predictions ................ 124
7.1. Predictions for research across culture-bound genomes .......................... 124
7.2. Toward a rapprochement between DSM and the European ICD . . . ................... 125
7.3. Conversive (pseudoneurological) disorder may be a more robust construct than dissociation,
and thus a better target for biological research ............................... 125
7.4. Implications for the taxonomy of fear-induced faintness in DSM-V . ................... 125
7.5. Some possible implications for the DSM-V terminology ......................... 125
7.6. Predictions empirically testable in non-human simians........................... 126
7.7. Predictions for transcriptomic research into the sociogenic (conversive) spectrum ............. 126
7.8. Testable pharmacological treatment approaches .............................. 126
7.9. Toward stigma reduction in clinical settings ................................ 126
7.10. Toward a psychiatric taxonomy that is more etiologically based . . . ................... 126
8. Concluding remarks ................................................ 126
9. In summary .................................................... 127
Acknowledgements ................................................... 127
References ....................................................... 127
1. Distal (evolutionary) etiological perspectives in
psychiatry
This article’s focus on the question of bwhyQ(distal
or neuroevolutionary etiology) may be a useful com-
plement to the standard biological psychiatric research
focus on bhowQ(proximal/proximate or neurochem-
ical etiology). Experimental and evolutionary neuros-
cientists have cogently argued that the distal/ultimate
etiology of many phobias is a fear-circuitry adaptation
that served to enhance survival and reproduction dur-
ing mammalian or early primate evolution. For land-
mark empirical studies and comprehensive recent
reviews of the empirical research, see Cosmides and
Tooby (1992, 1999),Blanchard et al. (1993),Cook
and Mineka (1989, 1990),Menzies and Clarke
(1993),Ohman and Mineka (2001) and Poulton et
al. (1999).
The neuroevolutionary perspective is most clear
regarding specific phobias triggered by situations
that were potentially lethal throughout the human
EEA, such as high elevations, confined spaces, or
being under the simultaneous visual scrutiny of
many non-kin conspecifics. Nesse summarized most
of the literature prior to 1999 in a series of landmark
reviews (Nesse, 1999a,b). Nesse has pointed out that
H.S. Bracha et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 88 (2005) 119–129120
subsequent assortative mating may also play a role in
amplifying polygenic transmission resulting in shift-
ing the severity of these responses from the normative
range into the clinical range (a similar argument can
be made for oligogenic transmission). Similar per-
spectives were echoed by Bracha (in press, 2004),
Bracha et al. (2005a),Buss et al. (1998),Marks
(1987, 1988),Klein (1993),Marks and Nesse
(1997),Niculescu and Akiskal (2001),Perry and Pol-
lard (1998),Perry et al. (1995), and Stein and Bouwer
(1997).
More recently, we have argued that from a neuroe-
volutionary (distal etiology) perspective, medically
unexplained facial pain, clenching–grinding symp-
toms, as well as habitual efferent vasovagal neurocar-
diovascular syncope, are traceable to fear-circuits
wired during the early Paleolithic (early Pleistocene)
or earlier, and conserved in the human clade, and may
be taxonomizable in the fear-circuitry disorders spec-
trum (Bracha, 2004; Bracha et al., 2005a,b).
In light of the increasing threat of large-scale mas-
sacres, especially terrorist attacks against non-comba-
tants (civilians), more attention to sociogenic symptoms,
particularly epidemic sociogenic symptoms, may be
warranted (Bartholomew and Wessely, 2002; Wessely,
2000). Biological psychiatric researchers have paid
little attention to this perplexing spectrum of fear-
induced syndromes. While the evolutionary etiology
of many specific phobias is intuitively clear, certain
symptoms within the sociogenic–conversive spectrum
remain less obvious.
In a landmark article on dissociative symptoms,
Perry et al. have pointed out that:
bHumans evolved... in the presence of two major
predators: large cats (e.g., tigers, panthers) and, the
most dangerous predators, other hominids, including
humans... To the cats, all humans (males, females,
and children) were roughly equivalent prey, with
some preferences for the small, slow, and weak. To
other hominids, however, there was a dramatic differ-
ence between males, females, and young children...
One need not imagine long the response of a violent
human male when faced with one female who will
willingly comply with the commands to move to his
camp and with another who is screaming, yelling,
hitting, fighting, and trying to run away. Hyperarousal
(fighting or fleeing) would clearly reduce... the prob-
ability that her genes would be passed to another
generationQ(Perry et al., 1995).
In the present article, we extend a similar neuroevo-
lutionary perspective to acute pseudoneurological
symptoms. This neuroevolutionary hypothesis is com-
plementary to the clinical and basic research on the
neurophysiology (proximal/proximate etiology) of con-
versive spectrum disorders. Our primary focus is on
psychogenic, non-epileptic attacks (bpseudoseizuresQ),
and epidemic sociogenic syndromes (epidemic faint-
ness being the most common form) (Boss, 1997; Mof-
fatt, 1982; Van Ommeren et al., 2001).
Some pseudoneurological symptoms, which have
outlived their usefulness and thus are non-fitness-
enhancing in the modern environment, may have
been fitness-enhancing fear-circuitry responses to
war-related, inescapable stress in the late stage of
the human EEA. We posit that a firm-wired (prepo-
tentiated) limbic extreme-stress response resembling a
severe handicap, such as pseudo-paralysis (limping),
pseudo-cerebellar symptoms (staggering), psycho-
genic loss of eyesight (psychogenic blindness), or
psychogenic motor attacks (falling to the ground
with epileptic-like behavior) may have been selected
into some genomes specifically during the Neolithic.
We posit that during Neolithic periods of warfare,
acute pseudoneurological symptoms may have pro-
vided a clear non-verbal (visual) signal to predatory
conspecifics (often raiders from a tribe that spoke a
different language) (Keeley, 1996; LeBlanc and Reg-
ister, 2003) that one is severely injured or severely
diseased and thus unlikely to be an immediate threat.
Evolutionary pressures such as these have been shown
to result in balanced (stable) polymorphisms (Cavalli-
Sforza, 2000; Cavalli-Sforza et al., 1994).
2. Up to 95.3% of conversive symptoms remain
unexplained
In clinical settings, the importance of a comprehen-
sive neuropsychiatric work-up of the first episode of
acute conversive symptoms cannot be overemphasized
(Saygi et al., 1992). However, the wording regarding
conversive symptoms in the DSM-IV-TR reflects the
extreme caution with which academic psychiatry pre-
sently regards these symptoms. This caution was lar-
H.S. Bracha et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 88 (2005) 119–129 121
gely in response to the often cited 1965 study by Elliot
Slater (Slater, 1965; Slater and Glithero, 1965). Slater
found previously undetected localized neurological
disorders in 56% of patients originally diagnosed
with conversive disorders. However, newer, more
carefully conducted studies do not support Slater’s
conclusions published 40 years ago. The 1998 study
by Crimlisk et al. (bSlater RevisitedQ) done at the
British National Hospital for Nervous Disease found
that 95.3% (61 out of 64) of conversive disorder
patients referred to tertiary-care centers had no neuro-
logical explanation after a 6-year follow-up (Crimlisk
et al., 1998). Similarly, a study by Mace and Trimble
on conversive disorders found that the presenting
symptoms were still neurologically unexplained at
10-year follow-up in 86.0% (68 out of 79) of their
patients (Mace and Trimble, 1996). These two British
landmark studies on conversive disorders may not
have been considered by the DSM-IV-TR work group.
As the DSM-V publication deadline approaches, it
is noteworthy that since the 1994 publication of DSM-
IV, advances in neuroimaging and other neurological
diagnostic techniques have resulted in a decrease in
the percentage of localizable neurological lesions mis-
diagnosed as a conversive disorder. Thus, we predict
that the majority of patients diagnosed with conver-
sive disorders will remain in the realm of psychiatry at
least for the next decade.
3. Pseudoneurological symptoms as
fitness-enhancing Neolithic fear responses
There has been surprisingly little discussion of
neuroevolutionary etiologies as applied to conversive
symptoms. While pseudoneurological symptoms are
not part of the pan-mammalian hard-wired fear
response sequence (freeze, flight, fight, fright) (Bra-
cha, in press, 2004; Bracha et al., 2004a), we posit
below that due to long, nearly continuous warfare
during the Neolithic EEA, certain pseudoneurological
fear-circuitry responses were selected into the genome
of neurally modern humans through what Darwin
termed bsexual selectionQ(also known as mate selec-
tion or mate choice). Darwin dedicated 70 pages of his
book bDescent of Man, and Selection in Relation to
SexQto human sexual selection. However, it was not
until the mid-1990s that experimental psychologists
revived Darwin’s ideas regarding sexual selection of
behavioral traits. Reviews of recent research empha-
sized the importance of adaptations shaped by Dar-
win’s sexual selection. Miller concluded that b...sex
differences are highly diagnostic of sexual selectionQ
(Miller, 2001, p. 226). Similar arguments have been
made by Cosmides and Tooby (1999),Campbell
(1999), and Morgan (1990).
4. Neolithic inter-tribal warfare patterns and
pseudoneurological conversive symptoms
It could reasonably be argued that during the Neo-
lithic EEA, any antagonistic encounter with non-kin
conspecifics was consistently associated with threats to
life. As Perry has noted, dissociative symptoms may
have been one of the effective survival responses for
some individuals in settled Neolithic communities
when under attack by predatory conspecifics (Perry
et al., 1995). Similarly, we posit that inheriting non-
species typical (non-wild type) polymorphic alleles
predisposing (firm-wiring), an individual to manifest
acute symptoms resembling a neurological injury dur-
ing extreme fear may have increased the odds of sur-
viving Neolithic inter-group warfare and hence was
selected into some genomes. A wide range of such
Neolithic evolutionary pressures have been shown
to result in a balanced (stable) allele polymorphism
(Harvey et al., 1998; Holden and Mace, 1997).
Paleo-anthropological research has documented a
specific pattern of prehistoric inter-group warfare in
the Neolithic. In contrast to warfare in historical times,
Neolithic inter-group warfare almost exclusively
involved attacks against non-combatants in unsuspect-
ing settlements by raiding parties of mateless young,
post-pubertal males in search of material and especially
reproductive resources. Neolithic combat occurred
exclusively between young males, with females and
children serving as objects of competition. This has
been clearly documented by research on prehistoric
human remains. It has been estimated that the victors
killed 15%–50% of post-pubertal males and most
infants and toddlers, and took females and most
weaned pre-pubertal individuals captive (Lambert,
1997; Larsen, 1999; LeBlanc and Register, 2003;
Maschner and Reedy-Maschner, 1998). These Neo-
lithic patterns of intra-human killing have also been
H.S. Bracha et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 88 (2005) 119–129122
confirmed by studies of male lineages through the Y
chromosome (Underhill et al., 2001) and of female
lineages through mitochondrial DNA (Seielstad et al.,
1998).
Fear-induced acute pseudoneurological symptoms
were thus probably non-fitness-enhancing for Neo-
lithic post-pubertal males, resulting either in death,
severe injury, or, at minimum, a drop in mate avail-
ability or in mate choice hierarchy. In contrast, for all
reproductive-age women and for weaned pre-pubertal
individuals, fear-induced acute pseudoneurological
symptoms may have been fitness-enhancing since
they increased the likelihood of preserving life and
reproduction. Consistent with the above reasoning,
conversive symptoms are rare among post-pubertal
males (DSM-IV-TR) (Boss, 1997, p. 496). Conversive
symptoms are significantly less common among men
than women, with reported female-to-male ratios ran-
ging from 2:1 to 10:1 (Boss, 1997). Additionally,
epidemiological studies have universally documented
that conversive symptoms mostly affect young and
healthy individuals under age 20 and rarely occur after
age 35 (Boss, 1997).
5. Subtypes within the sociogenic–conversive
spectrum
5.1. Acute fear-induced motor deficits
Conversive motor deficits are typically marked by
unilateral pseudo-paralysis (limping) or pseudo-cere-
bellar symptoms (staggering). We posit that during the
Neolithic EEA some healthy individuals who reflex-
ively limped or staggered away rather than engaged in
combat were less likely to be attacked by predatory
con-specifics during inter-tribal warfare. As Perry et
al. have presciently noted, the reverse would be
expected if an unsuspecting human settlement was
attacked by non-human predators (typically large
cats, which prefer to prey on the small, the slow,
and the already-injured) (Perry et al., 1995).
5.2. Sociogenic fear-induced motor (non-epileptic)
attacks
Conversive non-epileptic attacks account for
approximately 20% of all patients discharged from
tertiary epilepsy centers (Benbadis, 2001; Krumholz,
1999). Prior to such referrals, patients with acute fear-
induced non-epileptic attacks often endure ineffective
and costly pharmacological treatment. These patients
are often prescribed multiple anti-convulsants due to
lack of response. Conversive non-epileptic attacks
have also been identified as a significant healthcare
cost issue, although exact figures are not available
(Goldstein, 2004). Consistent with the neuroevolution-
ary hypothesis presented here, young women account
for approximately 75% of conversive motor (non-epi-
leptic) attacks (Tojek et al., 2000). This age and sex
distribution is similar to fainting induced by blood-
letting-related stimuli (Bienvenu and Eaton, 1998).
5.3. Acute fear-induced epidemic faintness
There has been little attention paid to the fact that
epidemic sociogenic illness is also being reported in
developed countries (Brown and Trimble, 2000; Mof-
fatt, 1982; Wessely, 2000). However, a high percen-
tage of the reports on epidemic sociogenic illness are
published in languages other than English, making it
difficult to summarize all available data. Boss has
critically reviewed the 70 English-language reports
on epidemic sociogenic illness published between
1973 and 1993 (Boss, 1997). In her comprehensive
review, symptoms which we consider in the
bfaintness-spectrumQ(fainting, swooning, dizziness,
lightheadedness, pseudo-seizures, unconsciousness)
were reported by 84% of individuals (Boss, 1997).
In all published studies of epidemic sociogenic dis-
orders, symptoms were most often reported in healthy
individuals under age 20 (Bartholomew and Wessely,
2002; Boss, 1997; Jones et al., 2000; Wessely, 2000).
In her review, Boss noted that epidemic sociogenic
symptoms frequently occur in indoor settings such as
classrooms. Therefore, dehydration, overheating (sun-
stroke), physical fatigue, and/or prolonged standing
are unlikely causative agents of the symptoms. Boss
has also called attention to the fact that such epidemic
faintness occurs primarily in individuals who are in
the line of sight of the index case (Boss, 1997). This
is consistent with visual (non-verbal) signaling. There
has been little attention paid to the findings of Boss
and others that epidemic sociogenic illness most often
manifests as epidemic faintness (i.e., faintness in
response to the fainting of a peer group member)
H.S. Bracha et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 88 (2005) 119–129 123
(Boss, 1997; Moffatt, 1982; Van Ommeren et al.,
2001).
Epidemic sociogenic syndromes may be ripe for a
science-based theoretical conceptualization. In this
theoretical article, we posit that during the Neolithic
(a period of nearly continuous intertribal predation)
seeing a peer drop to the ground (thus appearing
severely injured) non-verbally signaled to others in
the group that there was a cause for alarm. We posit
that a predisposition to faint in response to fainting of
a member of one’s peer group may have increased the
odds of survival. Epidemic faintness in modern clin-
ical settings may be a manifestation of this hard-wired
or prepotentiated (firm-wired) neolithic alarm-signal-
ing response, now encoded in some genomes and
possibly amplified through assortative mating.
6. Insights from recent bio-anthropological
research
The high density of populations required for the
hypothesis we present in this paper came into exis-
tence only in the Neolithic. It may not be immedi-
ately obvious that the Neolithic period was of
sufficient time depth (i.e., sufficient duration) to
allow the spread of balanced polymorphisms. Lit-
tle-known evolutionary anthropological and allele
frequency studies may be instructive in this regard.
The Neolithic period lasted approximately 480 gen-
erations (12,000 years) in those parts of the world
that pioneered agriculture and in parts of the world
that pioneered pottery. This would have permitted
the spread of polymorphic traits with moderate selec-
tion coefficients. The best known example of an
adaptation, which is specifically of Neolithic time
depth (origin) and which entered the human genome
as a balanced (stable) polymorphism, is hereditary
persistence of intestinal lactase (the ability to digest
lactose past the age of four) (Harvey et al., 1998;
Holden and Mace, 1997). Cavalli-Sforza et al. have
calculated that a high selection coefficient (3%) will
result in a 100-fold increase in allele frequency in
3800 years. Furthermore, a moderate selection coef-
ficient of 1% will result in a 100-fold increase in
allele frequency in 11,500 years (e.g., from 0.05% to
5% of the population) (Cavalli-Sforza et al., 1994).
Thus, we posit that most acute pseudoneurological
syndromes are evolved fear-circuits of Neolithic time
depth (12,000 years).
Geographical differences are especially instructive.
Another example of a balanced polymorphism of
Neolithic time depth is the region-specific reduction
in the size of human dentition after the development
of pottery. Pottery technology was first developed in
East Asia in the early Neolithic. Pottery cooking
greatly decreased the survival advantage of large
teeth in the East Asian culture-bound genomes (Eur-
opean culture-bound genomes still retain tooth size
similar to that of the more archaic non-neurally mod-
ern humans). A directional selection resulting in a
reduction in tooth size, which progressed at 2% per
1000 years, has been proposed (review by Hill, 2004).
Sex differences are also instructive; e.g., reduction in
tooth size evolved more rapidly in females than in
males. This is attributed to the pioneering role of
females in developing both agriculture technologies
and pottery technologies and their lower consumption
of game meat during the Neolithic (review by Hill,
2004). Our hypothesis that pseudoneurological syn-
dromes are of recent (Neolithic) time depth is also
evinced by the observation that the predisposing
alleles were not driven to fixation but rather remained
species atypical.
7. Implications for DSM-V and some empirically
testable and falsifiable predictions
The purpose of theory-driven articles is to look at
conventional wisdom from a new angle, often from
the perspective of a seemingly distant scientific dis-
cipline. Hypotheses presented in theory-driven articles
are, by definition, conjectural. However, they serve
the purpose of generating testable and falsifiable pre-
dictions. As Nesse points out, neuroevolutionary
hypotheses are not untestable (Nesse, 1999b). Possi-
ble implications for the DSM-V research agenda and a
few testable and falsifiable predictions are listed
below.
7.1. Predictions for research across culture-bound
genomes
Based on the literature reviewed here, we predict
that the highest prevalence of sociogenic pseudoneur-
H.S. Bracha et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 88 (2005) 119–129124
ological syndromes would be found in culture-bound
genomes whose geographic ancestry is in ecological
locations of the globe where agriculture and pottery
were pioneered (and thus the Neolithic period lasted
the longest). These regions are the Fertile Crescent,
North Africa, Anatolia, and parts of South Asia and of
East Asia. We predict that pseudo-neurological dis-
orders will demonstrate high genotypic variance as
evinced by high h-squared, high phenotypic variance,
and lack of universality across cultures.
7.2. Toward a rapprochement between DSM and the
European ICD
The importance of rapprochement between the
forthcoming DSM-V and the ICD taxonomy has
been emphasized by Perugi et al. (1998). ICD-10
has re-categorized the conversive disorders and re-
conceptualized them as more akin to dissociative dis-
orders (Guggenheim, 2000; Kupfer et al., 2002).
Our neuroevolutionary perspective also suggests
that DSM-V should similarly delete bConversion
DisordersQfrom the somatization section and re-
conceptualize them as part of a new bDissociative–
Conversive Spectrum,Qwith conversive and disso-
ciative syndromes falling along a continuum, as in
the ICD-10 and in the forthcoming ICD-11.
7.3. Conversive (pseudoneurological) disorder may
be a more robust construct than dissociation, and
thus a better target for biological research
The nebulous construct of dissociation has been
recently widely criticized, primarily for being a hetero-
geneous syndrome consisting of unrelated etiologies.
Research on dissociation over the last three decades
has been a challenging enterprise, since neurological
phenocopies of dissociation, especially frontal lobe
seizures and other hyper-glutamatergic states (endo-
genous or exogenous), are difficult to rule out. Heur-
istically, it may be easier to focus early research on the
more extreme manifestations of a clinical spectrum.
We posit that conversion is a much more robust con-
struct than dissociation. Unlike dissociation, the diag-
nosis of conversion relies much less on self-report of
symptoms and much more on collateral history and
clinically observable signs. Clinical research toward
the development of the DSM-V and ICD-11 may
benefit by shifting some of the focus from dissociative
to conversive syndromes, which we posit, lie at the
extreme end of the Dissociative–Conversive Spectrum.
7.4. Implications for the taxonomy of fear-induced
faintness in DSM-V
As we have argued in two recent reviews (Bracha,
2004; Bracha et al., 2005a), blood-injection-injury
phobia is a pseudo-neurocardiovascular disorder,
which is physiologically puzzling and hemodynami-
cally paradoxical when compared with all other anxi-
ety disorders. We posit that blood-injection-injury
fears are better conceptualized as part of a new
DSM-V bDissociative–Conversive SpectrumQbecause
of shared features, which are all in the bfaintness-
spectrumQ(fear-induced fainting, swooning, bmedi-
cally unexplained dizziness,Qand lightheadedness).
This is consistent with reports in the neurological
literature that medically unexplained syncope is highly
comorbid with conversive motor (non-epileptic)
attacks (Brenner, 1997; Zaidi et al., 1998). More the-
ory-driven research in this inter-disciplinary area
between neurology and psychiatry is needed.
7.5. Some possible implications for the DSM-V
terminology
The current American terminology for the conver-
sive spectrum frequently includes terms that are
increasingly perceived as stigmatizing. The British
term bacute sociogenic illness,Qcommonly used in
Europe, has the advantage of emphasizing the brief
nature of these particular pseudoneurological attacks.
It was originally coined by Wessely (2000) and may
be preferable for this spectrum of conditions, ranging
from conversive motor deficits to pseudo-convulsive
(non-epileptic) attacks. Other terms such as bacute
pseudo-localized disorderQor acute pseudoneurologi-
cal disorder can also be considered. Sociogenic symp-
toms in epidemic form have been reported under a
variety of names since circa 400 BC. The DSM-IV-TR
continues to use the original 2400-year-old Herodo-
tian term epidemic bhysteria,Qwhich is stigmatizing
and should not be retained in DSM-V.
On the other hand, we argue that the prefix
bepidemic,Qused in the DSM-IV-TR (p. 494) should
be retained in DSM-V and is preferable to the lay term
H.S. Bracha et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 88 (2005) 119–129 125
bmassQbecause it may help minimize association with
terms such as bmass injuryQand bmass trauma.Q
Furthermore, bepidemicQemphasizes the acute and
benign nature of the disorder. The term bepidemic
sociogenic attacksQis increasingly used by epileptol-
ogists and should also be considered for epidemic
sociogenic disorders in DSM-V. An additional advan-
tage of the term bepidemic sociogenic attacksQmay be
its resemblance to the term bpanic attacksQand to
bataque de nervios.Q
7.6. Predictions empirically testable in non-human
simians
One testable prediction, based on the Neolithic
balanced polymorphism hypothesis, is that sociogenic
pseudoneurological symptoms would not be found in
extant non-human simians (i.e., that these symptoms
are species-specific and limited to neurally modern
humans).
7.7. Predictions for transcriptomic research into the
sociogenic (conversive) spectrum
In addition to anatomically identifying fear-circuits
(Bracha et al., in press), it is critical to identify the
epigenetic transcriptomic processes involved. One of
the major (oligogenic) loci involved in the conver-
sive/dissociative spectrum disorders may carry a
developmentally sensitive allele (existing in a
balanced polymorphism) in which the expression
mechanism is gradually suppressed by pleiotropic
androgens, such as dehydroxyepiandrosterone sulfate
(DHEA-S). This is likely to manifest as a decrease in
gene penetrance following puberty in males. The above
prediction is testable and may provide a plausible
genetic model for our explanation of the age and sex
pattern of sociogenic pseudoneurological disorders.
The above hypothesis may thus facilitate a candidate-
gene approach to research into these syndromes.
7.8. Testable pharmacological treatment approaches
The possible role of the pleiotropic adreno-cortical
androgen DHEA-S is especially intriguing because,
unlike testosterone (that decreases during combat-
training stress), DHEA-S increases during combat-
training stress in males (Charney, 2004; Morgan et
al., 2004). DHEA-S is also intriguing from a clinical
point of view because, unlike testosterone, it is an
over-the-counter compound and may thus be a pro-
mising treatment option for sociogenic pseudo-neuro-
logical syndromes.
7.9. Toward stigma reduction in clinical settings
Acute medically unexplained physical symptoms
in young healthy individuals after exposure to war
zones are a rapidly growing concern and includes a
sub-group with sociogenic etiology (Clauw et al.,
2003; Jones and Wessely, 2003). We have argued
elsewhere that stigma has been the major self-imposed
barrier to early identification and early treatment of
medically unexplained physical symptoms, especially
in active duty military personnel and veterans (Bracha
et al., 2004b, 2005b). We posit that stigma about this
spectrum of disorders may be reduced by a new non-
psychodynamic conceptualization and by shifting the
emphasis from ontogenetic etiologies to phylogenetic
neuroevolutionary etiologies. Diminished stigma may
increase acceptability of evidence-based treatments,
such as the ones already used for other neuroevolu-
tionarily based fear-circuitry syndromes such as social
phobia.
7.10. Toward a psychiatric taxonomy that is more
etiologically based
The discussion of etiologies has been judiciously
minimized in DSM-III, DSM-IV, and DSM-IV-TR.
However, the published research agenda for the forth-
coming DSM-V has specifically highlighted the need
for a more etiologically based classification (Charney
et al., 2002; Kupfer et al., 2002). The neuroevolution-
ary etiological perspective presented in this paper is
consistent with, and may serve a useful purpose in, the
DSM-V research agenda.
8. Concluding remarks
Evolutionary psychiatrists have primarily focused
on non-species specific (bbasicQ) fear-circuitry
mechanisms, which were wired much earlier than
the Neolithic, such as the fear of snakes (which, as
we have argued elsewhere, is of Cenozoic time depth
H.S. Bracha et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 88 (2005) 119–129126
and thus is a simian-wide fear) (Bracha, in press).
Similarly, we have recently articulated a neuroevolu-
tionary hypothesis for other fear-circuitry symptoms
which extant humans share with other extant pri-
mates—jaw-clenching and tooth-grinding (Bracha et
al., 2005b). In contrast, in the present theory-driven
article, we attempt to articulate a neuroevolutionary
hypothesis for a number of puzzling and difficult-to-
treat neuropsychiatric syndromes for which an evolu-
tionary hypothesis has not been previously articulated.
It is important to emphasize that:
!We are not proposing newly emerged genomic loci.
Rather, we posit that rare allele variants that had
little or no survival value throughout most of the
Paleolithic EEA (when large African cats were the
primary terrestrial predators) did spread widely
during the Neolithic EEA when non-kin conspecific
predators replaced large cats as the primary cause
of premature violent death among young, healthy
humans.
!We posit an oligogenic genetic transmission (not a
distinctive singular allele) in the etiology of the
syndromes addressed in this review. Nevertheless,
a candidate gene approach may also warrant
exploration.
!Non-protein-coding genome regions responsible
for complex modulation of existing genes (e.g., at
the transcriptome level), while less well understood
at present, are also hypothesized to undergo
changes as a result of evolutionary pressures. Sin-
gle nucleotide polymorphisms in non-protein-cod-
ing regions should also be explored as part of
future research into the post-genomic etiology of
acute sociogenic pseudoneurological syndromes.
9. In summary
More theory-driven research on, and a re-concep-
tualization of, acute pseudoneurological sociogenic
symptoms are warranted. In this theoretical article,
we endeavor to use creative lumping to obtain a
conceptual gain in the understanding of these com-
mon, often treatment-resistant symptoms. We present
a neuroevolutionary etiological perspective on the
acute pseudoneurological symptoms. We argue that
some presently hard to treat and perplexing pseudo-
neurological symptoms may be traceable to allele
variants which were fitness-enhancing during the
Neolithic, the latest period of the human EEA. We
posit that at the transcriptome level, one of the major
oligogenic loci involved may carry a developmentally
sensitive allele (in a balanced polymorphism) in
which the gene expression mechanism is gradually
suppressed in post-pubertal males by pleiotropic
androgens, such as DHEA-S. Possible taxonomic
implications for rapprochement between the forth-
coming DSM-V and the ICD are also discussed.
Acknowledgements
This material is based upon work supported in part
by the Office of Research and Development, Medical
Research Service, Department of Veterans Affairs, VA
Pacific Islands Health Care System, Spark M. Matsu-
naga Medical Center. Support was also provided by a
National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and
Depression (NARSAD) Independent Investigator
Award, and the VA National Center for Posttraumatic
Stress Disorders.
References
Bartholomew, R.E., Wessely, S., 2002. Protean nature of mass
sociogenic illness (from possessed nuns to chemical and biolo-
gical terrorism fears). The British Journal of Psychiatry 180,
300 – 306.
Benbadis, S.R., 2001. Provocative techniques should be used for the
diagnosis of psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Archives of
Neurology 58, 2063 – 2065.
Bienvenu, O.J., Eaton, W.W., 1998. The epidemiology of blood-
injection-injury phobia. Psychological Medicine 28, 1129 – 1136.
Blanchard, R.J., Yudko, E.B., Rodgers, R.J., Blanchard, D.C., 1993.
Defense system psychopharmacology: an ethological approach
to the pharmacology of fear and anxiety. Behavioural Brain
Research 58, 155 – 165.
Boss, L.P., 1997. Epidemic hysteria: a review of the published
literature. Epidemiologic Reviews 19, 233 – 243.
Bracha, H.S., 2004. Freeze-Flight-Fight-Fright-Faint: adaptationist
perspective on the acute stress response spectrum. CNS Spec-
trums—The International Journal of Neuropsychiatric Medicine
9, 679 – 685.
Bracha H.S. in press. Neuro-evolutionary factors in the etiology of
fear-circuitry-related traits: Current perspectives, falsifiable pre-
dictions and the bTime-Depth PrincipleQProgress in Neuro-
Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry.
H.S. Bracha et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 88 (2005) 119–129 127
Bracha, H.S., Williams, A.E., Ralston, T.C., Bracha, A.S., Matsu-
kawa, J.M., 2004a. Does bFight or flightQneed updating? Psy-
chosomatics 45, 5.
Bracha, H.S., Williams, A.E., Haynes, S.N., Kubany, E.S., Ralston,
T.C., Yamashita, J.M., 2004b. The STRS (shortness of breath,
tremulousness, racing heart, and sweating): a brief checklist for
acute distress, with panic-like sympathetic indicators; develop-
ment and factor structure. Annals of General Hospital Psychiatry
3, 8.
Bracha, H.S., Bracha, A.S., Williams, A.E., Ralston, T.C., Matsu-
kawa, J.M., 2005a. The human fear-circuitry and fear-induced
fainting in health individuals — The paleolithicQthreat hypo-
thesis. Clinical Autonomic Research 15, 238 – 241.
Bracha, H.S., Ralston, T.C., Yamashita, J.M., Williams,A.E., Bracha,
A.S., 2005b. The clenchingQgrinding spectrum and fear-
circuitry disorders: clinical insights from the neuroscien-
ceQpaleoanthropology interface. CNS Spectrums — The Interna-
tional Journal of Neuropsychiatric Medicine 10, 311– 318.
Bracha H.S, Garcia-Rill E, Mrak RE, Skinner R. in press. Post-
mortem locus coeruleus neuron count in three American veter-
ans with probable or possible war-related PTSD. Journal of
Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.
Brenner, R.P., 1997. Electroencephalography in syncope. Journal of
Clinical Neurophysiology 14, 197 – 209.
Brown, R.J., Trimble, M.R., 2000. Dissociative psychopathology,
non-epileptic seizures, and neurology. Journal of Neurology,
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 69, 285– 288.
Buss, D.M., Haselton, M.G., Shackelford, T.K., Bleske, A.L.,
Wakefield, J.C., 1998. Adaptations, exaptations, and spandrels.
American Psychologist 53 (5), 533 – 548.
Campbell, A., 1999. Staying alive: evolution, culture, and
women’s intrasexual aggression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences
22, 203 – 214.
Cavalli-Sforza, L.L., 2000. Genes, Peoples, and Languages, 1 ed.
North Point Press, New York.
Cavalli-Sforza, L.L., Menozzi, P., Piazza, A., 1994. The History and
Geography of Human Genes. Princeton University Press, Prin-
ceton, NJ.
Charney, D.S., 2004. Psychobiological mechanisms of resilience
and vulnerability: implications for successful adaptation to
extreme stress. American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 195– 216.
Charney, D.S., Barlow, D.H., Botteron, K.N., Cohen, J.D., Gold-
man, D., Gur, R.E., Lin, K.-M., Lopez, J.F., Meador-Woodruff,
J.H., Moldin, S.O., Nestler, E.J., Watson, S.J., Zalcman, S.J.,
2002. Neuroscience research agenda to guide development of a
pathophysiologically based classification system. In: Kupfer,
D.J., First, M.B., Regier, D.A. (Eds.), A Research Agenda for
DSM-V. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC,
pp. 31 – 83.
Clauw, D.J., Engel, C.C., Aronowitz, R., Jones, E., Kipen, H.M.,
Kroenke, K., Ratzan, S., Sharpe, M., Wessely, S., 2003. Unex-
plained symptoms after terrorism and war: an expert consensus
statement. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine
45, 1040 – 1048.
Cook, M., Mineka, S., 1989. Observational conditioning of fear to
fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology 98, 448 – 459.
Cook, M., Mineka, S., 1990. Selective associations in the observa-
tional conditioning of fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of
Experimental Psychology. Animal Behavior Processes 16,
372 – 389.
Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., 1992. The adapted mind: evolutionary
psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University
Press, New York.
Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., 1999. Toward an evolutionary taxonomy of
treatable conditions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 108,
453 – 464.
Crimlisk, H.L., Bhatia, K., Cope, H., David, A., Marsden, C.D.,
Ron, M.A., 1998. Slater revisited: 6 year follow up study of
patients with medically unexplained motor symptoms. British
Medical Journal 316, 582 – 586.
Goldstein, L.H., 2004. Assessment of patients with psychogenic
non-epileptic seizures. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and
Psychiatry 75, 667 – 668.
Guggenheim, F.G., 2000. Somatoform disorders. In: Sadock,
B.J., Sadock, V.A. (Eds.), Comprehensive Textbook of Psychia-
try, 7th ed. Lippincott Williams and Wilkins, Philadelphia,
pp. 1504 – 1532.
Harvey, C.B., Hollox, E.J., Poulter, M., Wang, Y., Rossi, M.,
Auricchio, S., Iqbal, T.H., Cooper, B.T., Barton, R., Sarner,
M., Korpela, R., Swallow, D.M., 1998. Lactase haplotype fre-
quencies in Caucasians: association with the lactase persistence/
non-persistence polymorphism. Annals of Human Genetics 62
(Pt. 3), 215 – 223.
Hill, M.K., 2004. Dental reduction and diet in the prehistoric Ohio
River Valley. Dental Anthropology 17, 34 – 44.
Holden, C., Mace, R., 1997. Phylogenetic analysis of the evolution
of lactose digestion in adults. Human Biology 69, 605 628.
Jones, E., Wessely, S., 2003. bForward psychiatryQin the military:
its origins and effectiveness. Journal of Traumatic Stress 16,
411– 419.
Jones, T.F., Craig, A.S., Hoy, D., Gunter, E.W., Ashley, D.L., Barr,
D.B., Brock, J.W., Schaffner, W., 2000. Mass psychogenic ill-
ness attributed to toxic exposure at a high school. The New
England Journal of Medicine 342, 96 – 100.
Keeley, L.H., 1996. War Before Civilization: The Myth of the
Peaceful Savage. Oxford University Press, New York.
Klein, D.F., 1993. False suffocation alarms, spontaneous panics,
and related conditions. Archives of General Psychiatry 50,
306 – 317.
Krumholz, A., 1999. Nonepileptic seizures: diagnosis and treat-
ment. Neurology 53, 76 83.
Kupfer, D.J., First, M.B., Regier, D.A., 2002. A Research Agenda for
DSM-V. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.
Lambert, P.M., 1997. Patterns of violence in prehistoric hun-
ter–gatherer societies of coastal Southern California. In:
Martin, D.L., Frayer, D.W. (Eds.), Troubled Times: Violence
and Warfare in the Past. Gordon and Breach Publishers, Aus-
tralia, pp. 77 – 109.
Larsen, C.S., 1999. Bioarchaeology: Interpreting Behavior from
the Human Skeleton, 1 ed. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
LeBlanc, S.A., Register, K.E., 2003. Constant Battles: The Myth of
the Peaceful, Noble Savage. St. Martin’s Press, New York.
H.S. Bracha et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 88 (2005) 119–129128
Mace, C.J., Trimble, M.R., 1996. Ten-year prognosis of conversion
disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 169, 282 – 288.
Marks, I.M., 1987. Fears, Phobias, and Rituals: Panic, Anxiety, and
their Disorders. Oxford University Press, New York.
Marks, I.M., 1988. Blood-injury phobia: a review. American Jour-
nal of Psychiatry 145, 1207 1213.
Marks, I.M., Nesse, R.M., 1997. Fear and fitness: an evolutionary
analysis of anxiety disorders. In: Baron-Cohen, S. (Ed.), The
Maladapted Mind: Classic Readings in Evolutionary Psycho-
pathology. Psychology Press, United Kingdom, pp. 57 – 72.
Maschner, H.D.G., Reedy-Maschner, K.L., 1998. Raid, retreat,
defend (repeat): the archaeology and ethnohistory of warfare
on the north Pacific rim. Journal of Anthropological Archaeol-
ogy 17, 19 – 51.
Menzies, R.G., Clarke, J.C., 1993. The etiology of fear of heights
and its relationship to severity and individual response patterns.
Behaviour Research and Therapy 31, 355 – 365.
Miller, G.F., 2001. The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped
the Evolution of Human Nature. Anchor Books, New York.
Moffatt, M.E., 1982. Epidemic hysteria in a Montreal train station.
Pediatrics 70, 308 – 310.
Morgan, E., 1990. The Scars of Evolution: What Our Bodies Tell Us
About Human Origins. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Morgan, C.A., Southwick, S., Hazlett, G., Rasmusson, A., Hoyt, G.,
Zimolo, Z., Charney, D., 2004. Relationships among plasma
dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate and cortisol levels, symptoms of
dissociation, and objective performance in humans exposed to
acute stress. Archives of General Psychiatry 61, 819– 825.
Nesse, R.M., 1999a. Proximate and evolutionary studies of anxiety,
stress and depression: synergy at the interface. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews 23, 895 – 903.
Nesse, R.M., 1999b. Testing evolutionary hypotheses about mental
disorders. In: Stearns, S.C. (Ed.), Evolution in Health and Dis-
ease. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 260 – 266.
Niculescu, A., Akiskal, H.S., 2001. Proposed endophenotypes of
dysthymia: evolutionary, clinical and pharmacogenomic consid-
erations. Molecular Psychiatry 6, 363 – 366.
Ohman, A., Mineka, S., 2001. Fears, phobias, and preparedness:
toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psycholo-
gical Review 108, 483 – 522.
Perry, B.D., Pollard, R., 1998. Homeostasis, stress, trauma,
and adaptation: a neurodevelopmental view of childhood
trauma. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North
America 7, 33.
Perry, B.D., Pollard, R.A., Blakley, T.L., Baker, W.L., Vigilante, D.,
1995. Childhood trauma, the neurobiology of adaptation, and
buse-dependentQdevelopment of the brain: how bstatesQbecome
btraitsQ. Infant Mental Health Journal 16, 271 – 291.
Perugi, G., Toni, C., Benedetti, A., Simonetti, B., Simoncini, M.,
Torti, C., Musetti, L., Akiskal, H.S., 1998. Delineating a puta-
tive phobic-anxious temperament in 126 panic–agoraphobic
patients: toward a rapprochement of European and US views.
Journal of Affective Disorders 47, 11 – 23.
Poulton, R., Menzies, R.G., Craske, M.G., Langley, J.D., Silva,
P.A., 1999. Water trauma and swimming experiences up to
age 9 and fear of water at age 18: a longitudinal study. Beha-
viour Research and Therapy 37, 39 – 48.
Saygi, S., Katz, A., Marks, D.A., Spencer, S.S., 1992. Frontal lobe
partial seizures and psychogenic seizures: comparison of clinical
and ictal characteristics. Neurology 42, 1274 – 1277.
Seielstad, M.T., Minch, E., Cavalli-Sforza, L.L., 1998. Genetic
evidence for a higher female migration rate in humans. Nature
Genetics 20, 278 – 280.
Slater, E., 1965. Diagnosis of bhysteriaQ. British Medical Journal
5447, 1395 – 1399.
Slater, E.T., Glithero, E., 1965. A follow-up of patients diagnosed as
suffering from bhysteriaQ. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 9,
9 – 13.
Stein, D.J., Bouwer, C., 1997. A neuro-evolutionary approach to the
anxiety disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 11, 409 – 429.
Tojek, T.M., Lumley, M., Barkley, G., Mahr, G., Thomas, A.,
2000. Stress and other psychosocial characteristics of patients
with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Psychosomatics 41,
221 – 228.
Underhill, P.A., Passarino, G., Lin, A.A., Marzuki, S., Oefner, P.J.,
Cavalli-Sforza, L.L., Chambers, G.K., 2001. Maori origins, Y-
chromosome haplotypes and implications for human history in
the Pacific. Human Mutation 17, 271 – 280.
Van Ommeren, M., Sharma, B., Komproe, I., Poudyal, B.N.,
Sharma, G.K., Cardena, E., De Jong, J.T.V.M., 2001. Trauma
and loss as determinants of medically unexplained epidemic
illness in a Bhutanese refugee camp. Psychological Medicine
31, 1259 – 1267.
Wessely, S., 2000. Responding to mass psychogenic illness. New
England Journal of Medicine 342, 129 – 130.
Zaidi, A., Clough, P., Scheepers, B., Fitzpatrick, A., 1998. Treat-
ment resistant epilepsy or convulsive syncope? BMJ 317,
869 – 870.
H.S. Bracha et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 88 (2005) 119–129 129
... The above hypothesis can also be tested in clinical populations. Elsewhere (also see below) we have argued that, for other evolutionary reasons, acute pseudoneurological manifestations of anxiety will have a higher prevalence in the above world regions in which hunting/gathering subsistence ended first (Bracha et al., 2005c). ...
... The above HPIL and ASPM studies demonstrating that the Neolithic EEA was of sufficient time-depth to allow geneculture co-evolution-based allele-variant polymorphism to be established by balancing/stabilizing selection, are highly relevant to pseudoneurological (a.k.a. conversive) disorders (Bracha et al., 2005c). There has been almost no previous discussion of neuroevolutionary factors in the etiology of the pseudoneurological (pseudo-localized) symptoms which appear during and in the immediate aftermath of inescapable stress. ...
... As others and we have pointed out elsewhere, in light of the increasing threat of terrorist attacks against civilians, more attention is warranted to acute pseudoneurological (pseudolocalized) symptoms. Additionally, more attention is warranted to epidemic sociogenic symptoms (previously known as mass hysteria) which emerge in the immediate aftermath of largescale disasters, especially intentionally caused disasters (Bracha et al., 2005c;Bartholomew and Wessely, 2002;Wessely, 2000). We have recently proposed that a firm-wired fear circuitry response resembling a severe handicap, such as pseudoparalysis (limping), pseudo-cerebellar symptoms (staggering, astasia abasia), psychogenic loss of sight, or falling to the ground with convulsion-like paroxysms (psychogenic nonepileptic attacks, pseudo-seizures), was selected into some genotypes, specifically during the Neolithic, resulting in a balanced (stable) allele variant polymorphism (Bracha et al., 2005c;Cavalli-Sforza et al., 1994). ...
Presentation
Full-text available
VERY rough notes of presentation. was published as part of the larger 2006 comprehensive review see reference below ------------> TWENTY FIRST CENTURY WARFARE IS NOT WELL MATCHED TO THE HARD-WIRED LIMBIC CIRCUITS WHICH EVOLVED FOR PALEOLITHIC WARFARE PATTERNS: ... . With a few notable exceptions, most deployed troops of modern armies are expected to endure deployments lasting 6 to 12 months. Such chronic activation of a warrior's locus coeruleus --amygdala circuit did not exist during the Paleolithic EEA. Actual combat during the EEA usually lasted only a single day or at most a few days. Furthermore, this brief operational-combat stress was followed imediately by several days of HIGH-VAGAL-TONE recuperative behaviors consisting of resting, sleeping, feasting, and the predominant other form of prehistoric male recreation-- sexual activity............ .Bioanthropological studies have documented this very specific pattern of prehistoric inter-group warfare. These patterns have been confirmed by several paleogenomic and gene-flow studies of male lineages through the Y-chromosome and of female lineages and female migration patterns through mitochondrial DNA ........... These studies have found that, in contrast to the total war patterns of the 20th century CE (and the urban warfare characterizing the early years of the 21st century), inter-group warfare in the upper Paleolithic EEA almost exclusively involved raiding parties of young, mateless post-pubertal males primarily in search of reproductive resources. THEREFORE ALMOST BY DEFINITION A WARRIOR IN THE PALEOLITHIC DID YET NOT HAVE A SPOUSE OR CHILDREN (LeBlanc and Register, 2003; Larsen, 1999; Maschner and Reedy-Maschner, 1998; Wilkinson, 1997). ........ We have argues that, when estimating the risk/ reward odds of initiating armed conflict, Extant political decision makers are firm-wired to misperceive warfare as it took place during the upper Paleolithic and Neolithic EEA (i.e., merely a raiding party for a low- risk attack against an unsuspecting settlement). . ........ The above insights should be more widely known among VA clinicians as well as among decision makers in the military in order to advance primary and secondary prevention of PTSD in service-members facing long war zone exposure.
... The above hypothesis can also be tested in clinical populations. Elsewhere (also see below) we have argued that, for other evolutionary reasons, acute pseudoneurological manifestations of anxiety will have a higher prevalence in the above world regions in which hunting/gathering subsistence ended first (Bracha et al., 2005c). ...
... The above HPIL and ASPM studies demonstrating that the Neolithic EEA was of sufficient time-depth to allow geneculture co-evolution-based allele-variant polymorphism to be established by balancing/stabilizing selection, are highly relevant to pseudoneurological (a.k.a. conversive) disorders (Bracha et al., 2005c). There has been almost no previous discussion of neuroevolutionary factors in the etiology of the pseudoneurological (pseudo-localized) symptoms which appear during and in the immediate aftermath of inescapable stress. ...
... As others and we have pointed out elsewhere, in light of the increasing threat of terrorist attacks against civilians, more attention is warranted to acute pseudoneurological (pseudolocalized) symptoms. Additionally, more attention is warranted to epidemic sociogenic symptoms (previously known as mass hysteria) which emerge in the immediate aftermath of largescale disasters, especially intentionally caused disasters (Bracha et al., 2005c;Bartholomew and Wessely, 2002;Wessely, 2000). We have recently proposed that a firm-wired fear circuitry response resembling a severe handicap, such as pseudoparalysis (limping), pseudo-cerebellar symptoms (staggering, astasia abasia), psychogenic loss of sight, or falling to the ground with convulsion-like paroxysms (psychogenic nonepileptic attacks, pseudo-seizures), was selected into some genotypes, specifically during the Neolithic, resulting in a balanced (stable) allele variant polymorphism (Bracha et al., 2005c;Cavalli-Sforza et al., 1994). ...
Presentation
Full-text available
Very rough notes of an oral presentation see our comprehensive review below for background and more. ............................... ...MALE HARDWIRED HOARDING PATTERNS. Following is more or less common since but has never been written up the best of my knowledge. ................... Evidence for hoarding of non-food items in the upper Paleolithic is extensive. In particular, inordinately large hoards of unused stone axes (which served as both tools and weapons) have been documented see LeBlanc and Register (2003) for a review. The paleo-anthropological data lead to our prediction that extant males with compulsive hoarding traits will primarily hoard tools and/or weapons. It is noteworthy that, unlike most fear-circuitry-driven behaviors, hoarding of clinical severity is more common in males than in females (the only other anxiety disorder with this unique sex ratio is severe clinical social phobia, as briefly discussed below). ... And by the way renaming social phobia social anxiety was an error. It meet the definition of a phobia. Think about it............. HOARDING DISORDER is highly hereditary and In the groundbreaking Johns- Hopkins study of 126 subjects with OCD, nearly 30% of the subjects had hoarding symptoms, and hoarding was twice as prevalent in males than females (Samuels et al., 2002). The heritability and unusual male to female ratio (2:1) reported by the Johns-Hopkins group is consistent with our Paleolithic evolutionary theory. Hoarding by upper Paleolithic males of tools and weapons was arguably more fitness-enhancing than hoarding food items. Food storages were vulnerable to rodents, insects, fungi and bacteria, or to theft by hostile conspecifics. Weapons, on the other hand, could have been used to protect food storages (as well as for obtaining food by raiding neighboring groups during periods of nutritional stress)(Keeley, 1997). ........ The above briefly reviewed and similar evolution related data, MAY EXPLAIN WHY FEDERAL EFFORTS TO ADDRESS FIREARMS HOARDING ("COLLECTING") HAVE BEEN SO DIFFICULT. .............................................................. FEMALE HARDWIRED HOARDING PATTERNS: A different pattern of non-food item hoarding are evolutionarily predicted in extant females. An important fitness-enhancing trait among upper Paleolithic females was hoarding of two survival-enhancing non-foods items: first, HIGH QUALITY LEATHER FOOT-COVERINGS FOR EXTENDED FOREST FORAGING (BOOTS/ SHOES), and second, LEATHER CONTAINERS FOR TRANSPORTING GATHERED FOODS (UNDER ARM LEATHER BAGS). It may be of interest that, during foraging as well as during long-distance migrations, Paleo- lithic females are believed to have carried TWO UNDER-ARM LEATHER BAGS: one containing fat/protein-rich foods and the second carrying the unweaned infant. This under-arm positioning of bags facilitated breastfeeding while mobile and easy access to the foods. Since leather deteriorates rapidly in the ground, at present, there are no paleo-archaeological data in support of this speculation. However, paleo-archaeological digs in previously ice covered northern regions are ongoing. . . STAGE FRIGHT. WHY IS IT MORE COMMON IN MALES? . Stage Fright, a manifestation of social anxiety is literally tonic immobility-like paralyzing helplessness, and a tachycardic diaphoretic tonic immobility reported and easily observed among conference speakers. Individual with stage fright report being frightened but not able to flee or fight. .....Neurobiological speaking stage fright is FEAR OF SIMULTANEOUS VISUAL SCRUTINY BY NUMEROUS NON- SMILING, NON-KIN CONSPECIFICS. ... ......................... Fear of SVS-NNNC ? . ..... FEAR OF SIMULTANEOUS VISUAL SCRUTINY BY NUMEROUS NON- SMILING, NON-KIN CONSPECIFICS. Heritability of social anxiety traits was demonstrated in the several studies (Kendler et al., 2001). A conceptually driven landmark genomic study has documented an association between social anxiety traits and a (balanced) allele-variant polymorphism in the Beta-1 adrenergic receptor (Stein et al., 2004). Evolutionary factors contributing to social anxiety traits are cogently articulated elsewhere (Stein, 1998; Stein and Bouwer, 1997; Stein et al., 1996). Several authorities have argued that, during parts of the human EEA, being stared at by a large group of strangers was frequently associated with potential threat to life. Stated differently, for upper Paleolithic H. sapiens, situations in which an individual especially an isolated male, was simultaneously scrutinized by a large group of non- smiling, non-kin conspecifics were more likely than not to be followed by the most severe form of negative consequences (death). The consequence for a female although very severe were usually not death but being raped or being kidnapped, of the type that will interfere with passing her genes to the next generation. Such evolutionary hypotheses can be tested empirically in patients with social- phobia-related traits (e.g., by comparing autonomic nervous system response and neuroendocrine response during visual scrutiny by a large group of non-kin versus a comparably sized group of blood relatives) . . Also the author has noted that previous studies, and the DSM-, cluster together two innate fears of arguably DIFFERENT TIME-DEPTH as “social-anxiety-related traits”: 1) a generalized fear of “new people” — an innate upper Paleolithic ∼70,000 y/o fear of non-blood-related conspecifics and 2) fear of public bathrooms — an innate Neolithic ∼12,000 y/o fear of fecal contamination. Arguably, this clustering is unhelpful in charting the genetic architecture of social-anxiety-related traits. . With regard to the sex ratio of anxiety disorders, men are generally believed to under-report symptoms of most phobic and anxiety disorders in both epidemiological and clinical studies. Male under-reporting may partly account for the typical sex ratio of anxiety disorders. In contrast, HOWEVER objectively severe social anxiety (severe enough to require hospitalization) presents primarily in males (DSM-IV-TR, 2000, p. 453). In this regard, the sex ratio of severe social phobia (as well as of severe OCD) stands out among anxiety disorders....... ... The high prevalence of clinically significant social anxiety among males is consistent with neuroevolutionary predictions since, in the mid–upper Paleolithic EEA, visual scrutiny by a large group of strangers was more likely to result in loss of life for a male than for a female. . Finally, Goodall's (1986) research on the patterns of behavior of the chimpanzees of Gombe leads us to predict that, especially in males, symptom severity during meeting “new people” will often manifest only when the “new people” encountered include three or more men. This prediction can be easily confirmed or disconfirmed in clinical samples. (The correct term should be social phobia because it is an innate fear disorder phobia not social anxiety.anxiety is a worry disorder not a fear disorderRead the definitions of both terms)
... The above hypothesis can also be tested in clinical populations. Elsewhere (also see below) we have argued that, for other evolutionary reasons, acute pseudoneurological manifestations of anxiety will have a higher prevalence in the above world regions in which hunting/gathering subsistence ended first (Bracha et al., 2005c). ...
... The above HPIL and ASPM studies demonstrating that the Neolithic EEA was of sufficient time-depth to allow gene– culture co-evolution-based allele-variant polymorphism to be established by balancing/stabilizing selection, are highly relevant to pseudoneurological (a.k.a. conversive) disorders (Bracha et al., 2005c). There has been almost no previous discussion of neuroevolutionary factors in the etiology of the pseudoneurological (pseudo-localized) symptoms which appear during and in the immediate aftermath of inescapable stress. ...
... As others and we have pointed out elsewhere, in light of the increasing threat of terrorist attacks against civilians, more attention is warranted to acute pseudoneurological (pseudolocalized ) symptoms. Additionally, more attention is warranted to epidemic sociogenic symptoms (previously known as mass hysteria) which emerge in the immediate aftermath of largescale disasters, especially intentionally caused disasters (Bracha et al., 2005c; Bartholomew and Wessely, 2002; Wessely, 2000). We have recently proposed that a firm-wired fear circuitry response resembling a severe handicap, such as pseudoparalysis (limping), pseudo-cerebellar symptoms (staggering, astasia abasia), psychogenic loss of sight, or falling to the ground with convulsion-like paroxysms (psychogenic nonepileptic attacks, pseudo-seizures), was selected into some genotypes, specifically during the Neolithic, resulting in a balanced (stable) allele variant polymorphism (Bracha et al., 2005c; Cavalli-Sforza et al., 1994). ...
... Há, por exemplo, a teoria de que a SVV pode, de forma semelhante à imobilidade tônica, ser uma adaptação para simular a morte (Bracha et al. 2005). Bracha (2004;2005a;2005b;2005c;2007) sugere a hipótese de que os combates pré -históricos que ocorriam entre humanos durante o paleolítico médio davam -se, quase exclusivamente, entre machos jovens, como forma de competir por fêmeas em idade reprodutiva. Estas lutas frequentes selecionaram machos que não fossem fóbicos a sangue ou a objetos pontiagudos/afiados empunhados por estranhos, dado este medo, provavelmente, poder resultar em morte ou menor disponibilidade de parceiros. ...
Book
Full-text available
A quantidade de variáveis implicadas na etiologia e manutenção de medos, fobias e ansiedades é vasta. Neste livro abordamos as respostas defensivas, o medo e a ansiedade nas suas dimensões social, política, antropológica e religiosa. A predisposição biológica para temermos certas configurações de estímulos, e a ativação fisiológica e neuroendócrina que suporta as respostas defensivas bem como a expressão genética necessária para a criação de uma memória a longo prazo do evento. Abordamos ainda o papel de acontecimentos traumáticos, evitamento, alarmes falsos, vieses cognitivos, insight e psicoterapia, bem como os subtipos de medo e propostas de alteração da sua nosologia. Abordamos ainda a dimensão histórica do conceito de medo, autores seminais, a revolução cognitiva e o papel importante dos estudos que usam a memória do medo para perceber como aprendemos coisas novas. Concluímos que o estudo dos elementos de um estímulo que podem ser geradores de medo (e.g., dentes num cão, a velocidade da sua aproximação ou o tamanho) pode ajudar a identificar sintomas anteriormente incompreendidos. Exploramos as formas de identificar estes elementos e o seu impacto no tratamento. Estes fatores etiológicos têm implicações na forma como aplicamos técnicas de exposição, que podem ser modificadas para atender a tendências específicas de resposta (e.g., “tremer os joelhos” em locais altos). Ao explorar os elementos específicos dos estímulos que causam medo, exploramos neste livro os seguintes: 1 Dor; 2 Repugnância, Contaminação e Nojo; 3 Resposta vasovagal; 4 Postura; 5 Movimento e velocidade; 6 Distância/tamanho; 7 Saliência, cor e visibilidade; 8 Configuração/forma; 9 Olfato e 10 Território. Esperamos que este livro vos encoraje a explorar o medo e ansiedade de forma detalhada e inovadora
... Lasting disruptions of peripheral autonomic and cardiovascular function after exposure to extreme traumas such as the stress of war are best thought of from larger developmental and evolutionary perspectives (Bracha et al., 2005). Behaviors such as hypervigilance, hyperarousal, and light sleep that had survival value for primitive hunter gatherers thousands of years ago are no longer useful for humans who live in buildings with security systems and no longer have natural predators (Teicher et al., 2003). ...
Article
Da Costa originally described Soldier's Heart in the 19th Century as a syndrome that occurred on the battlefield in soldiers of the American Civil War. Soldier's Heart involved symptoms similar to modern day posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) as well as exaggerated cardiovascular reactivity felt to be related to an abnormality of the heart. Interventions were appropriately focused on the cardiovascular system. With the advent of modern psychoanalysis, psychiatric symptoms became divorced from the body and were relegated to the unconscious. Later, the physiology of PTSD and other psychiatric disorders was conceived as solely residing in the brain. More recently, advances in psychosomatic medicine led to the recognition of mind-body relationships and the involvement of multiple physiological systems in the etiology of disorders, including stress, depression, PTSD, and cardiovascular disease, has moved to the fore, and has renewed interest in the validity of the original model of the Soldier's Heart syndrome.
Research
Full-text available
INTRODUCTION A reflection on the utopian nature of mental health and the highlighting of profound connections between functional neurological disorders and the placebo and nocebo effects have led me to propose a new definition of the term utopia in the medical field. I categorize as utopian any clinical situation to which medicine mistakenly attributes chimerical qualities. In other words, my definition applies to unexplained clinical situations that contradict the classical pathophysiological approach and thus challenge it. A second element of my definition pertains to the involvement of a dominant unconscious process. I ultimately classify clinical situations meeting the aforementioned definition into a new nosological class called "House of Utopia." METHOD To be included in the House of Utopia, a clinical situation must pass through the criteria of inclusion and exclusion. Inclusion criteria are: 1) Being an entity amenable to clinical observation; 2) Being unexplained (or having lacunar or contradictory explanations); 3) Involving an unconscious process; 4) Concerning human beings, whether on an individual or collective level; 5) Constituting a "healthy" or "toxic" process concerning the maintenance of Life [derived from "physiosophy," a system of thought merging philosophy and the physiology of living organisms]. Exclusion criteria are: 1) Simulation; 2) Being an established clinical entity with unambiguous pathophysiology; 3) Incompatibility with the discretization error hypothesis; 4) Situations related to a psychiatric disorder of psychotic lineage; 5) Situations linked to the consumption of psychoactive substances. RESULTS Admissible clinical situations in the House of Utopia are categorized into four groups, at the intersection of "Healthy Processes" and "Toxic Processes" on one hand, and "Individual Level" and "Collective Level" on the other. Healthy and individual processes: placebo effect, hypnosis. Healthy and collective processes: collective intelligence (also known as wisdom of crowds). Toxic and individual processes: nocebo effect, functional disorders, factitious disorders, ELIZA effect, voodoo death. Toxic and collective processes: mass psychogenic illness. DISCUSSION Unexplained clinical situations are discredited by the dominant explanatory model because it is unable to integrate them and because they challenge it. However, these situations, gathered in the House of Utopia, are regularly associated with each other in scientific literature, such as the placebo effect, nocebo effect, and functional disorders. The same applies to the association between nocebo and mass psychogenic illness, often linked to hypnosis and voodoo death. Voodoo death is associated with functional disorders, hypnosis, and nocebo; etc. This supports the creation of a common nosological class. Another argument arises from the presumed common pathophysiology of these clinical situations: expectation, conditioning, evolutionary component, role of anxiety, Bayesian theory, and the discretization error hypothesis. The latter is linked to a new concept: the unconscious working avatar. The unconscious working avatar is derived from a generalization of the reality/virtual reality model proposed by P. D. Wall. IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH The function of utopia in medicine is to prompt us to challenge dogmas. The study of the House of Utopia should thus lead to the establishment of new paradigms related to human pathophysiology. The seemingly chimeric nature of the conditions gathered in the House of Utopia is an error that advancing research must correct. Over time, these clinical situations will therefore exit the House of Utopia. The great complexity of the etiology of the clinical entities discussed here requires a transdisciplinary approach that goes beyond medicine. For this clinic of the utopian, our knowledge from other scientific domains must be added. The concept of the unconscious working avatar allows a new direction for research, focused on the mechanisms underlying this virtual reality specific to brain function. CONCLUSION The House of Utopia is a new nosological class that brings together unexplained, poorly understood, or neglected individual or collective clinical situations in medicine. The pathophysiology of these clinical situations is common and highly complex. The discretization error hypothesis is one of the lines of inquiry. This hypothesis fits into a model of global cognitive function assigning a dominant role to the unconscious: the unconscious working avatar. The House of Utopia also represents a practical application of physiosophy.
Article
Full-text available
The quality of the defensive response to threats depends on the elements that trigger the fear response. The current classification of phobias does not account for this. Here, we analyze the fear-eliciting elements and discern the different types of fears that originate from them. We propose Pain, Disgust, Vasovagal response, Visual-vestibular and postural interactions, Movement and Speed, Distance and Size, Low and mid-level visual features, Smell, Territory and social status as core elements. We subdivide phobias the elements most frequently triggered by them and their impact on behavior. We discuss the implications of a clinical conceptualization of phobias by reconsidering the current nosology. This conceptualization will facilitate finding etiological factors in defensive behavior expression, fine-tuning exposure techniques, and challenging preconceived notions of preparedness. This approach to phobias leads to surprising discoveries and shows how specific responses bear little relation to the interpretation we might later give to them. Dividing fears into their potentially fear-eliciting elements can also help in applying the research principles formulated by the Research Domain Criteria initiative.
Chapter
Somatoforme Störungen umfassen eine Gruppe heterogener Störungen, denen als führende klinische Beschwerden körperliche Symptome ohne eine hinreichende organmedizinische Erklärung gemeinsam sind. Dass Konzept der „Somatisierung“ liegt den somatoformen Störungen, aber auch anderen primären psychischen Störungen zugrunde, die wie z. B. Depression oder Angst vorrangig in ihren integralen körperlichen Symptomen dargestellt werden. Ursache, Entstehung und Aufrechterhaltung von somatoformen Störungen werden multifaktoriell vermittelt. Akute und chronische psychosoziale Stressoren spielen eine wichtige Rolle. Es kommen unterschiedliche Schweregrade und Verlaufstypen vor. Zu beachten sind eine bedeutsame psychiatrische Komorbidität, eine häufige psychosoziale Behinderung sowie eine intensive Inanspruchnahme vielfältiger medizinischer Ressourcen. Nicht selten gestaltet sich die Arzt-Patient-Beziehung emotional konfliktträchtig. Es existieren differenzierte störungsbezogene psychotherapeutische Ansätze. Pharmakotherapeutische Interventionen sind v. a. bei koexistenten psychischen Störungen indiziert, besitzen aber auch unabhängig davon eine gewisse Evidenzbasierung. Das diagnostische Konzept der somatoformen Störungen spielt bisher für die ärztliche Versorgung nur eine untergeordnete Rolle. Die Herausforderungen hier sind in erster Linie in der hohen Anzahl von Patienten zu sehen, die entweder nur vereinzelte oder nur wenige körperliche Symptome ohne mögliche somatisch-medizinische Zuordnung präsentieren und daher häufig nur „Symptom-Diagnosen“ erhalten, oder aber bei denen meist auf einer fachärztlichen Ebene ein fachspezifisch definiertes „funktionelles Körpersyndrom“ diagnostiziert wird. Beide eigenständigen diagnostischen Zugangsweisen sind häufig nicht spannungsfrei und klinisch sinnvoll in das psychiatrische Diagnosesystem übersetzbar. Das DSM-5 hat eine Reihe von formalen und inhaltlichen Kritikpunkten an der früheren Diagnosegruppe der „somatoformen Störungen“ aufgenommen und schlägt nunmehr die Gruppe der „somatischen Belastungsstörung und verwandter Störungen“ vor. Es ist derzeit noch unklar, zu welchen Positionen und Bezeichnungen die für die nächsten Jahre angekündigte Revision von ICD-11 kommen wird.
Article
Founded upon the psychoevolutionary theories of Darwin, Plutchik and Izard, a general socioevolutionary theory of the emotions - affect-spectrum theory - classifies a wide spectrum of the emotions and analyzes them on the sociological, psychological and neurobiological levels. This neurocognitive sociology of the emotions supersedes the major theoretical perspectives developed in the sociology of emotions by showing primary emotions to be adaptive reactions to fundamental problems of life which have evolved into elementary social relationships and which can predict occurrences of the entire spectrum of primary, complex secondary, and tertiary emotions. Written by leading social theorist Warren D. TenHouten, this book presents an encyclopaedic classification of the emotions, describing forty-six emotions in detail, and presenting a general multilevel theory of emotions and social life. The scope of coverage of this key work is highly topical and comprehensive, and includes the development of emotions in childhood, symbolic elaboration of complex emotions, emotions management, violence, and cultural and gender differences. While primary emotions have clearly defined valences, this theory shows that complex emotions obey no algebraic law and that all emotions have both creative and destructive potentialities.
Article
Full-text available
An evolved module for fear elicitation and fear learning with 4 characteristics is proposed. (a) The fear module is preferentially activated in aversive contexts by stimuli that are fear relevant in an evolutionary perspective. (b) Its activation to such stimuli is automatic. (c) It is relatively impenetrable to cognitive control. (d) It originates in a dedicated neural circuitry, centered on the amygdala. Evidence supporting these propositions is reviewed from conditioning studies, both in humans and in monkeys; illusory correlation studies; studies using unreportable stimuli; and studies from animal neuroscience. The fear module is assumed to mediate an emotional level of fear learning that is relatively independent and dissociable from cognitive learning of stimulus relationships.
Article
This review discusses the clenching-grinding spectrum from the neuropsychiatric/neuroevolutionary perspective. In neuropsychiatry, signs of jaw clenching may be a useful objective marker for detecting or substantiating a self-report of current subjective emotional distress. Similarly, accelerated tooth wear may be an objective clinical sign for detecting, or substantiating, long-lasting anxiety. Clenching-grinding behaviors affect at least 8% of the population. We argue that during the early paleolithic environment of evolutionary adaptedness, jaw clenching was an adaptive trait because it rapidly strengthened the masseter and temporalis muscles, enabling a stronger, deeper and therefore more lethal bite in expectation of conflict (warfare) with conspecifics. Similarly, sharper incisors produced by teeth grinding may have served as weaponry during early human combat. We posit that alleles predisposing to fear-induced clenching-grinding were evolutionarily conserved in the human clade (lineage) since they remained adaptive for anatomically and mitochondrially modern humans (Homo sapiens) well into the mid-paleolithic. Clenching-grinding, sleep bruxism, myofacial pain, craniomaxillofacial musculoskeletal pain, temporomandibular disorders, oro-facial pain, and the fibromyalgia/chronic fatigue spectrum disorders are linked. A 2003 Cochrane meta-analysis concluded that dental procedures for the above spectrum disorders are not evidence based. There is a need for early detection of clenching-grinding in anxiety disorder clinics and for research into science-based interventions. Finally, research needs to examine the possible utility of incorporating physical signs into Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition posttraumatic stress disorder diagnostic criteria. One of the diagnostic criterion that may need to undergo a revision in Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition is Criterion D (persistent fear-circuitry activation not present before the trauma). Grinding-induced incisor wear, and clenching induced palpable masseter tenderness may be examples Of such objective physical signs of persistent fear-circuitry activation (posttraumatic stress disorder Criterion D).
Article
A carbon dioxide hypersensitivity theory of panic has been posited. We hypothesize more broadly that a physiologic misinterpretation by a suffocation monitor rnisfires an evolved suffocation alarm system. This produces sudden respiratory distress followed swiftly by a brief hyperventilation, panic, and the urge to flee. Carbon dioxide hypersensitivity is seen as due to the deranged suffocation alarm monitor. If other indicators of potential suffocation provoke panic, this theoretical extension is supported. We broadly pursue this theory by examining Ondine's Curse ae the physiologic and phalmacologic converse of panic disorder, splitting panic in terms of symptomatology and challenge studies, reevaluating the role of hyperventilation, and reinterpreting the contagiousness of sighing and yawning, as well ae maes hysteria. Further, the phenomena of panic during relaxation and sleep, late luteal phaee dysphoric disorder, pregnancy, childbirth, pulmonary disease, separation anxiety, and treatment are used to test and illuminate the suffocation false alann theory. Recent advances with regard to ambulatory monitoring of paI~ic disorder, catbon monoxide prevention of carbon dioxide panicogenesis, and naloxone/lactate challenge in normals will be presented.
Article
Childhood trauma has profound impact on the emotional, behavioral, cognitive, social, and physical functioning of children. Developmental experiences determine the organizational and functional status of the mature brain. The impact of rruumufic experiences on the development and function of the brain are discussed in context of basic principles of neurodevelopment. There are various adaptive mental and physical responses to trauma, including physiological hyperarousal and dissociation. Because the developing brain organizes and internalizes new information in a use-dependent fashion, the more a child is in a state of hyperarousal or dissociation, the more likely they are to have neuropsychiatric symptoms following trauma. The acute adaptive states, when they persist, can become maladaptive traits. The clinical implications of this new neurodevelopmental conceptualization of childhood trauma are discussed. Le trauma de l'enfance a un impact profond sur le fonctionnement émotionnel, comportemental, cognitif, social et physique des enfants. Les expériences en matière de développement déterminent l'organisa-tion et le fonctionnement du cerveau arrivé à maturité. L'impact d'expériences traumatiques sur le développement et le fonctionnement du cerveau sont discutés dans le contexte de principes de bases de neurodéveloppe-ment. Il existe plusieurs résponses mentales et physiques d'adaptation au trauma, parmi lesquelles l'excitation physique intense et de la dissociation. Parce que le cerveau qui se développe organise et internalise les nouvelles “informations” d'une manière liée B l'utilisation et en dépendant, plus un enfant se trouve dans un état d'excitation ou de dissociation et plus il risque d'y avoir des symptǒmes neuropsychiatriques aprés le trauma. L'“état” adaptatif aigu peut devenir persistent et conduire à des “traits” d'inadaptation. Les implications cliniques de cette nouvelle conceptualisation de neurodéveloppement du trauma de l'enfance sont discutées.