ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Prosopagnosia is the inability to recognize someone by the face alone in the absence of sensory or intellectual impairment. In contrast to the acquired form of prosopagnosia we studied the congenital form. Since we could recently show that this form is inherited as a simple monogenic trait we called it hereditary form. To determine whether not only face recognition and neuronal processing but also the perceptual acquisition of facial information is specific to prosopagnosia, we studied the gaze behaviour of four hereditary prosopagnosics in comparison to matched control subjects. This rarely studied form of prosopagnosia ensures that deficits are limited to face recognition. Whereas the control participants focused their gaze on the central facial features, the hereditary prosopagnosics showed a significantly different gaze behaviour. They had a more dispersed gaze and also fixated external facial features. Thus, the face recognition impairment of the hereditary prosopagnosics is reflected in their gaze behaviour.
No caption available
… 
No caption available
… 
No caption available
… 
No caption available
… 
Content may be subject to copyright.
Abstract Prosopagnosia is the inability to recognize
someone by the face alone in the absence of sensory or
intellectual impairment. In contrast to the acquired
form of prosopagnosia we studied the congenital form.
Since we could recently show that this form is inherited
as a simple monogenic trait we called it hereditary
form. To determine whether not only face recognition
and neuronal processing but also the perceptual
acquisition of facial information is specific to proso-
pagnosia, we studied the gaze behaviour of four
hereditary prosopagnosics in comparison to matched
control subjects. This rarely studied form of proso-
pagnosia ensures that deficits are limited to face rec-
ognition. Whereas the control participants focused
their gaze on the central facial features, the hereditary
prosopagnosics showed a significantly different gaze
behaviour. They had a more dispersed gaze and also
fixated external facial features. Thus, the face recog-
nition impairment of the hereditary prosopagnosics is
reflected in their gaze behaviour.
Introduction
Human beings are extremely competent at recognizing
faces quickly and accurately (Bruce, 1988; Bruce &
Young, 1998; Carey, 1996). In the last 2 decades, re-
sults of electrophysiological and neuroimaging studies
suggest that the recognition of faces is a highly specific
process. This view is supported by the fact that a spe-
cific impairment such as prosopagnosia exists that
concerns the recognition of faces only. The most
important characteristic of prosopagnosia—a term that
is first introduced by Bodamer (1947)—is the difficulty
in recognizing famous or familiar faces without marked
impairment of the recognition of other visual stimuli
(Benton, 1990; de Gelder & Rouw, 2000; Jones &
Tranel, 2001; McConachie, 1976; Nunn, Postma, &
Pearson, 2001; Temple, 1992).
Almost all cases reported in the literature are of the
acquired form. The diagnosis in these cases is often
self-reported because of the sudden loss of a hitherto
unimpaired skill. The congenital or developmental
forms are apparent from early childhood and typically
no traumatic, toxic or other exogen affects are known.
Participants with this ‘‘lifelong’’ impairment are
thinking of having just low-functioning (face) recogni-
tion skills or even are not aware of the deficit at all.
This may be the reason that the congenital form might
have been mostly overlooked. A slowly growing
number of only a few congenital prosopagnosic cases
are reported (Ariel & Sadeh, 1996; Bentin, Deouell, &
Soroker, 1999; De Gelder & Rouw, 2000; Duchaine,
2000; Duchaine, Parker, & Nakayama, 2003; de Haan,
1999; de Haan & Campbell, 1991; Hasson, Avidan,
Deouell, Bentin, & Malach, 2003; Jones & Tranel,
2001; Kress & Daum, 2003; McConachie, 1976; Nunn
G. Schwarzer (&) Æ S. Huber Æ C. Groß Æ M. Hipfel
University of Giessen, Otto-Behaghel-Str. 10F,
35394 Giessen, Germany
e-mail: gudrun.schwarzer@psychol.uni-giessen.de
M. Gru
¨
ter Æ T. Gru
¨
ter Æ I. Kennerknecht
Institut fu
¨
r Humangenetik, Westfa
¨
lische
Wilhelms-Universita
¨
t, Mu
¨
nster, Germany
Psychological Research
DOI 10.1007/s00426-006-0068-0
123
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Gaze behaviour in hereditary prosopagnosia
Gudrun Schwarzer Æ Susanne Huber Æ
Martina Gru
¨
ter Æ Thomas Gru
¨
ter Æ Cornelia Groß Æ
Melanie Hipfel Æ Ingo Kennerknecht
Received: 11 November 2004 / Accepted: 25 April 2006
Springer-Verlag 2006
et al., 2001; Temple, 1992, for a review see Behrmann
& Avidan 2005). The terms ‘‘congenital’’ and ‘‘devel-
opmental’’ are often used synonymously. Sensu strictu
they say nothing to the aetiology but only to the time of
onset, ‘‘at birth’’ and somewhere ‘‘during develop-
ment’’. These descriptions leave open whether it is
congenital by hereditary or acquired by birth (e.g. by
perinatal asphyxia) or during development (e.g. by
encephalitis). There is a lack of data for a putative
acquired congenital form and until recently only some
reports about a hereditary form exist. McConachie
(1976) and de Haan (1999) were the only one reporting
familial transmission from mother to daughter and fa-
ther to two daughters (and probably to one son),
respectively. Recently we could demonstrate that the
congenital form is hereditary (ab ovo) and very com-
mon (Gru
¨
ter, 2004; Gru
¨
ter, Gru
¨
ter, Bell, Horst, &
Laskowskiet al., in press; Kennerknecht, Gru
¨
ter, Gru
¨
-
ter, Otte, & Neumann et al., 2002). In all cases where
we could evaluate the family history we found further
first-degree impaired probands fitting to a simple
autosomal dominant segregation pattern. We therefore
coined the term ‘‘hereditary prosopagnosia’’. Heredi-
tary prosopagnosia is certainly a qualitatively distinct
deficit from the acquired form. Whether it supports the
traditional concept of an associative/mnestic disorder
remains an open question. In our personal experience
with 38 hereditary prosopagnosics (Gru
¨
ter et al., in
press and unpublished data) the perception of facial
details is not grossly distorted. All hereditary prosop-
agnosics can tell a normal perception of attractiveness,
emotions, and easy identification of gender. In this
way, hereditary prosopagnosia might be rather an
associative/mnestic deficit.
Up to now studies on congenital prosopagnosia tried
to find out, whether it is associated with an unusual
neuronal or electrophysiological pattern (Ariel &
Sadeh, 1996; Bentin et al., 1999; Choisser & de Haan,
1999; Duchaine, 2000; Hadjikhani & de Gelder, 2002;
McConachie, 1976; Nunn et al., 2001). For example,
there is evidence that congenital prosopagno-
sics—although not suffering from a neurological
disease—do not show the normal pattern of brain
activity such as higher fMRI activity to faces than to
objects in the fusiform face area and inferior occipital
gyrus (Barton, Press, Keenan, & O’Connor, 2002;
Hadjikhani & de Gelder, 2002). However, a recent
study carried out by Hasson et al. (2003) showed that
the face-related activation pattern of a congenital
prosopagnosic subject in the ventral occipito-temporal
cortex was similar to that observed in control subjects.
Thus, the neuronal correlates of congenital prosopag-
nosia are still under investigation.
Little is known about the perceptual processes that
accompany congenital prosopagnosia. It is still un-
known whether it is not only specific with regard to
face recognition performance and neuronal processing
but possibly also with regard to processes of visual
perception such as the acquisition of facial information
in particular. As visual information acquisition is ex-
pressed by gaze behaviour (Viviani, 1990), in the
present study we examined the gaze pattern in hered-
itary prosopagnosics as opposed to control participants
when they were encoding and recognizing faces.
Concerning the acquired form of prosopagnosia
there are a few studies that have analysed the gaze
behaviour during face recognition (e.g., Le, Raufaste,
& Demonet,
2003; Rizzo, Hurtig, & Damasio, 1987).
These studies showed no significant differences on gaze
behaviour during face recognition between the pros-
opagnosics and control participants. However, this re-
sult is not very surprising because these prosopagnosic
probands had normal face recognition abilities before
they acquired prosopagnosia. Therefore, it is possible
that they retained their internal schema of visually
examining a face even after they acquired prosopag-
nosia.
In contrast to the acquired type, with a large variety
of brain damages and hence accompanying deficits, the
investigation of hereditary prosopagnosics allowed us
to study a homogeneous group of prosopagnosics, who
all have difficulties in recognizing faces from the very
beginning of life without any other neurologic or per-
ceptional impairments.
Method
Face recognition performance and gaze behaviour
were studied in two tasks, in the ‘‘famous face task’’
and in the ‘‘unfamiliar face task’’ which are described
below. Both tasks were constructed in such a way that
even the hereditary prosopagnostic participants were
able to recognize some of the faces. This in turns
should make it possible for us to analyse the gaze
behaviour of both groups in terms of faces that had
been recognized and those face that had not been
recognized.
Participants
This study was part of a broader study carried out with
ethical committee approval from the University of
Mu
¨
nster, protocol No 3XKenn2, ‘‘Genotype/phenotype
correlation of prosopagnosia (syn. face blindness)’’.
Psychological Research
123
Probands were recruited after informed consent. In a
pilot study we described recruitment and diagnostic
assessment of 38 prosopagnosic participants in detail
(Gru
¨
ter, 2004, Gru
¨
ter et al., in press). In short, impaired
participants were identified among personal acquain-
tances and further by providing an Internet portal. The
diagnostic approach of the hereditary prosopagnosics
consisted of (1) anamnestic data excluding any event of
brain lesion (perinatal asphyxia, epileptic attack, men-
ingitis, injuries, brain surgery) or neurological or psy-
chiatric disorders, and of (2) a semistructural interview
concentrating on cognitive features and including de-
tailed data about the individual family history.
All hereditary prosopagnosics reported that
throughout their entire life they had been unable to
recognize people by their faces alone. They reported
that context enables them to recognize others more
easily and that they identify people by their hair, voice
etc. Furthermore, they reported that they sometimes
do not even recognize very close relatives and that they
avoid participating in large events where they cannot
be sure who they will meet.
In order to assess the diagnoses more objectively 8
out of 38 hereditary prosopagnosics from 7 families
agreed with in-depth testing (Warrington Recognition
Memory Test for Faces, RMF, Warrington 1984, fa-
mous and family faces tests, learning tests for internal
and external facial features, and a measure of mental
imagery for face and non-face images, see Gru
¨
ter et al.,
in press). These findings were fully compatible with the
data of the individual interviews as was true with the
respective familial segregation data. Thus, since the
eight hereditary prosopagnosics with in-depth testing
were randomly chosen it is likely that the other
hereditary prosopagnosics show a similar pattern of
face recognition abilities.
The probands of the present study stemmed from
the pool of hereditary prosopagnisics described above.
They consisted of three persons of one large family LI,
HE (females, 71 years), and TH (male, 45 years) de-
scribed in Gru
¨
ter et al. (in press) and a further person
GM (female, 35 years) from this pool. All probands
had a positive family history of prosopagnosia with
several impaired first-degree relatives, respectively.
Moreover, none of them had a history of neurological
or other disorders. Their visual acuity was normal or
corrected to normal. Intellectual abilities were normal
or above average.
4 control subjects (control group 1) who were mat-
ched in gender and approximate age to the hereditary
prosopagnosics (3 women, 35, 72, 73 years of age; 1
man, 42 years of age) participated in the ‘‘famous face
task’’. In the ‘‘unfamiliar face task’’, another sample of
4 control subjects (control group 2: 2 women, 60,
41 years of age; 2 men, 32, 78 years of age) was studied.
Since we ran the control study of the ‘‘unfamiliar face
task’’ first and the participants were from the USA, we
assembled a further control group coming from Ger-
many for the ‘‘famous face task’ because this task
consisted of German famous people.
Stimuli
The stimuli of the ‘‘famous face task’ consisted of 25
faces of well-known German politicians, TV stars and
highly popular singers and scientists (see Appendix). In
a pilot study, ten students were asked to name the faces
and to indicate their professions. The results showed
92% correct answers. The stimuli of the ‘‘unfamiliar
face task’’ were chosen from the face database PICS
(Psychological Image Collection at Stirling, 2002).
They consisted of 20 target faces and 20 distractor fa-
ces. The stimuli of both tasks were presented at a visual
angle of 12.5·9.8 on a light grey background.
Apparatus
The SMI remote eye-tracker (refresh rate 50 Hz) was
used to track eye movements (spatial accuracy of gaze
direction 0.5–1). This operates by illuminating the eye
with infra-red light and monitoring its reflection off
components of the eye via a video camera. The stimuli
of both tasks were displayed via a PC (single Pentium
II processor, 512 MB RAM) on a monitor (21 in. with
a resolution of 1,024 · 768 pixels) that was mounted
together with the eye-tracking camera and the infra-
red light source inside a so-called gaze-catch box. Gaze
behaviour was recorded on a second computer, which
was connected to the eye-tracking computer. The two
computers communicated via serial connection to
provide synchronization of stimulus on/off-set and start
and stop of recording of the gaze behaviour. The gaze-
catch box had a 16 · 2 cm slit where the participants
could look inside. A chin-rest was mounted below the
slit so that the head movements of the participants
were reduced to a minimum.
Procedure
Participants were tested individually. Prior to the
experiment, participants were told that their gaze
behaviour would be recorded, and the eye-tracker was
calibrated once by a nine-point calibration technique.
Before the presentation of each face, a small white
fixation-cross appeared at the centre of the monitor.
The ‘‘famous face task’’ consisted of two blocks. In the
Psychological Research
123
first block (recognition), participants were asked to
indicate whether they knew any of the 25 faces. The
faces were presented for 7 s each. The participants
indicated that they know the face by lifting up their
right arm. This method was chosen in order to ensure
gaze recording was not affected by possible head
movements accompanying speech. Once the partici-
pant gave the answer, the experimenter pressed a
keyboard button to stop eye-movement recording. It
was made sure that the experimenter stopped the eye-
movement recording reliably. Upon pressing the but-
ton, the face disappeared and the fixation cross reap-
peared on the screen until the next trial was started by
the experimenter. Thus, gaze behaviour was recorded
from stimulus onset until the experimenter pressed a
button following the response of the participant. This
time interval also indicates the corresponding reaction
time. In a second block (naming), the same faces were
presented again and participants were asked to name
the faces and to indicate the profession of the person.
No reaction times and eye movements were measured.
In the ‘‘unfamiliar face task’’ the participants per-
formed a forced-choice recognition task on the unfa-
miliar faces. In the encoding phase, subjects were
shown a single target face for 7 s. After a short delay of
1.5 s—in the subsequent recognition phase—the target
face was presented as before but now paired with a
distractor face, e.g. each target face was shown side by
side with a distractor face. The sides (right, left) of the
target faces were balanced. The participants’ task was
to indicate whether the target face was on the right or
left side by lifting up the right or left arm. Eye move-
ments were recorded.
Results
To test the face recognition impairment of the hered-
itary prosopagnosics, their performance in the ‘‘famous
face task’’ was compared to that of the four participants
of control group 1. The recognition performance was
measured as the percentage of correct recognition, as
the percentage of correct naming and as the percentage
of a so-called combined score. In the combined-score
only those answers were counted as correct indicating
that the participant correctly recognized the face and
was able to correctly name the face or the profession of
the person. Concerning each of the three measures, the
hereditary prosopagnosics’ performance was lower
than that of the controls (recognition: F(1, 6) = 4.54,
P = .07, naming: F(1, 6) = 4.41, P = .08, combined-
score: F(1, 6) = 5.34. P = .06). The combined score of
the hereditary prosopagnosics was 60% correct and the
score of the control participants was 87% correct. Thus,
in face recognition and naming hereditary prosopag-
nosics’ performance was nearly significant lower than
that of the controls. The small number of participants
should be considered by interpreting this difference
that only slightly failed the level of significance. How-
ever, when comparing the hereditary prosopagnosics’
measure for correctly naming the faces to the scores of
the pilot group used for evaluating the famous faces
(naming the famous faces), the difference was highly
significant, F(1, 11) = 20.04, P < .001.
The reaction times of the four hereditary prosop-
agnosics (‘‘famous face task’’) were compared to those
of the four participants of control group 1. Here the
hereditary prosopagnosics’ reaction times were signif-
icantly longer than those of the controls, (F(1,
6) = 31.7, P < .01, PA: M = 2.50 s, SD = 0.385 s, non-
PA: M = 1.361 s, SD = 0.128).
In the ‘‘unfamiliar face task’ the hereditary pros-
opagnosics and the four subjects of control group 2
showed nearly perfect recognition performance. In
each group only one subject failed to recognize one of
the 20 faces.
In the ‘‘famous face task’’ and in the ‘‘unfamiliar
face task’ fixations were calculated with a dispersion
algorithm with a minimum fixation duration of 100 ms
and a dispersion area of 0.5 (Stark & Ellis, 1981). In
the ‘‘famous face task’’ fixations were computed from
the onset until the end of face presentation in the
‘‘recognition’’ condition (7 s), and in the ‘‘unfamiliar
face task’’ fixations were calculated during the encod-
ing phase when participants looked at the target faces
for 7 s. For each target face an ellipse-shaped area that
closely matched the fixations was calculated by prin-
cipal component analysis i.e., the area of the ellipse
that included 85.35% of the fixations was measured
(Oliveira, Simpson & Nadal, 1996). For each partici-
pant the average ellipse areas were computed in rela-
tion to the average size of all faces presented (relative
fixation area).
In both tasks the results were evident (see Fig. 1 left
panel, Fig. 2): In the ‘‘famous face task’’ and in the
‘‘unfamiliar face task’’ the relative fixation area used
by the hereditary prosopagnosics was significantly
greater than that of the participants of the corre-
sponding control groups (group 1 in the ‘‘famous face
task’’ and group 2 in the ‘‘unfamiliar face task’’) (Task
1: F(1, 6) = 15.80, P < .01; Task 2: F(1, 6) = 13.31,
P < .01). To rule out the possibility that hereditary
prosopagnosics in the ‘‘famous face task’’ used a
greater area of fixations than the controls because they
were significantly less able to recognize the famous
faces correctly, the relative fixation areas were com-
Psychological Research
123
pared between hereditary prosopagnosics and control
participants (group 1) for faces correctly recognized
and faces not recognized separately. As depicted in
Fig. 1 (middle and right panel), hereditary prosopag-
nosics used a significantly greater relative fixation area
than the control participants (faces recognized cor-
rectly: F(1, 6) = 8.89, P < .05; faces not recognized:
F(1, 6) = 7.23, P < .05), no matter whether faces were
recognized or not. In both tasks the average number of
fixations per second (Task 1: F(1, 8) = 0.53, P > .05;
Task 2: F(1, 8) = 1.13, P > .05) and the average fixation
length did not differ between hereditary prosopagno-
sics and controls (Task 1: F(1, 8) = 2.56, P > .05; Task
2: F(1, 8) = 2.13, P > .05).
The fixation areas of a hereditary prosopagnosic and
a control proband depicted in Figs. 3 and 4 illustrate
the differences in fixation patterns typical for each
group of participants. As can be seen, when recogniz-
ing a famous face or when encoding an unfamiliar face,
the controls focused their gaze predominately on the
central section of the face (eyes, nose, mouth) whereas
the hereditary prosopagnosics showed a more dis-
persed gaze in both tasks. They fixated not only the
central facial features but mostly also external features
such as the hair, neck, or chin.
Discussion
The results of the present study show that when faces
are viewed hereditary prosopagnosics use a signifi-
cantly different fixation pattern than control partici-
pants. Whereas the controls focused their gaze on the
central facial features, the hereditary prosopagnosics
had a more dispersed gaze and also fixated external
facial features.
With regard to face recognition performance our
results are in line with previous findings. The typical
difficulties hereditary prosopagnosics have when rec-
ognizing famous faces and their prolonged reaction
times were confirmed (Bentin et al., 1999; Duchaine,
2000; Nunn et al., 2001). Also, in agreement with pre-
vious findings hereditary prosopagnosics showed no
difficulty in recognizing unfamiliar faces when the
target faces to be recognized were presented exactly as
in the encoding phase (Ariel & Sadeh, 1996; Duchaine,
2000; Nunn et al., 2001). Thus, when faces have to be
matched within a short period of time and no access to
long-term memory is necessary, hereditary prosopag-
Fig. 1 Relative fixation area in hereditary prosopagnosics (PA)
and control subjects (control group 1) when recognising famous
faces. Relative fixation area is defined as the average ellipse that
covered 83.35% of the fixations (Stark & Ellis, 1981) in relation
to the average size of all faces presented. Error bars indicate
standard deviation. Left panel shows the relative fixation area in
all trials in which hereditary prosopagnosics (PA) and control
subjects responded to the faces. Middle panel shows the relative
fixation area in the trials in which hereditary prosopagnosics and
control subjects recognized the faces correctly. For each
participant, fixation behavior was analysed from stimulus onset
until participant’s response. Right panel shows relative fixation
area for the trials in which hereditary prosopagnosics and control
subjects did not recognize the faces. For each participant,
fixation behavior was analysed from stimulus onset until the
end of stimulus presentation (7 s)
Fig. 2 Relative fixation area in hereditary prosopagnosics (PA)
and control subjects (control group 2) when encoding unfamiliar
faces. Error bars indicate standard deviation. Fixation area was
analysed when unfamiliar faces were presented for 7 seconds
each and participants’ task was to look at the faces that had to be
identified in the subsequent recognition phase
Psychological Research
123
nosics show the same performance as control pro-
bands.
More importantly, our results show that the gaze of
hereditary prosopagnosics is more dispersed when
looking at faces as opposed to that of controls who
focus on the central features of the face. This holds
true for the encoding and recognition of famous faces
and the encoding of unfamiliar faces. Furthermore, in
both cases the dispersed gaze pattern of the heredi-
tary prosopagnosics and the focused gaze of the
control subjects were independent of their recognition
performance. Even if the hereditary prosopagnosics
recognized a face correctly, compared to the control
participants their gaze was significantly more dis-
persed. Thus, the decisive factor for the differences in
gaze behaviour is whether the participants belong to
the group of hereditary prosopagnosics or not.
This relationship between gaze behaviour and being
a hereditary prosopagnosic or not can be seen in
analogy to the relationship between fixation behaviour
and different states of expertise (Viviani, 1990). For
example, some technical images (Chest X-rays, sono-
grams etc.) contain details which are only meaningful
to experts. Skilled professionals immediately concen-
trate fixations on these details, whereas laymen tend to
search for other informative regions (Carmody, No-
dine, & Kundel, 1980; Kundel & Nodine, 1983). Ana-
logically, the control probands, as so-called face
experts, cluster their fixations around the central facial
features, i.e, around those places with high informative
value (Antes, 1974; Henderson, Falk, Minut, Dyer, &
Mahadevan, 2001; Janik, Wellens, Goldberg, & Del-
l’Osso, 1978; Viviani, 1990; Walker-Smith, Gale, &
Findlay, 1977). In general, places with high informative
value are associated with those parts of the stimulus
which help the viewer to understand the meaning of
the stimulus (Yarbus, 1967). Hereditary prosopagno-
sics, in contrast, also search visually for places with
high informative values; however these are not the
central facial features but other external features that
could also be informative for recognition.
Since gaze behaviour can be understood as a crite-
rium that differentiates between hereditary and control
participants, the question arises of whether gaze
behaviour of hereditary prosopagnosics differs from
other untypical gaze patterns as well. For example,
clinical studies of face processing show that gaze
behaviour differs between control participants and
patients with disorders such as schizophrenia and aut-
ism (Manor, Gordon, Williams, Rennie, & Bahrama-
liet al., 1999; Pelphrey, Sasson, Reznick, Paul, &
Goldman, et al., 2002). The studies showed that pa-
tients with autism and schizophrenia look unsystem-
atically at only one or two non-central facial features
such as the ears or chin and avoid looking at central
facial features. The main difference between the gaze
behaviour of patients with autism and schizophrenia
and hereditary prosopagnosics appears to be the fact
that hereditary prosopagnosics do not avoid looking at
central features. Rather, in addition to the central
features they fixate external features and cover a
greater area of fixation. The dispersed gaze behaviour
observed in our results seems to be specific to heredi-
tary prosopagnosics.
Fig. 3 Fixations of a prosopagnosic and a participant of control
group 1 during a trial of the famous face task in which both of
them recognized the target face correctly. Crosses indicate
fixations (size corresponds to fixation length) and ellipse
indicates area that covered 83.35% of the fixations (Stark &
Ellis, 1981)
Fig. 4 Fixations of a prosopagnosic and a participant of control
group 2 during a trial of the unfamiliar face task in which both of
them recognized the target face correctly. Crosses indicate
fixations (size corresponds to fixation length) and ellipse
indicates the area that covered 83.35% of fixations (Stark &
Ellis, 1981)
Psychological Research
123
The findings of the present study can be related to
the way children recognize a face. For example,
Campbell, Walker and Baron-Cohen (1995) have
shown that young children tend to recognize
faces—unfamiliar faces in particular—better from their
external than their internal sections. Since the heredi-
tary prosopagnosics of the present study also included
information on the external parts of the faces in their
gaze behaviour this fact can be understood as a parallel
between children’s and hereditary prosopagnosics’ way
to process faces. Another parallel refers to the obser-
vation that children’s gaze behaviour is less focused
than that of adults when they were viewing faces
(Schwarzer, Huber & Du
¨
mmler, 2005). Thus, there
seem to be slight parallels between children’s and
hereditary prosopagnosics’ way to examine a face
visually. However, our results leave the question open
of whether the dispersed gaze of the hereditary pros-
opagnosics is specific to face processing only. In prin-
ciple, it is possible that hereditary prosopagnosics
exhibit a similar gaze pattern when viewing non-face
objects. This assumption, however, is not very likely
since until now there has been no empirical evidence of
any kind of object recognition impairment in heredi-
tary prosopagnosics. Nevertheless, this question needs
to be addressed in a further study. For now, it can be
stated that hereditary prosopagnosics show a different
gaze-orienting pattern as compared to controls. It is
also a task for a future project to test a larger sample of
hereditary prosopagnosics and to study to what extent
the analysis of gaze behaviour can serve as a method of
confirming the diagnosis of hereditary prosopagnosia.
Thus, our results show that not only there is a neu-
ronal processing pattern and face recognition perfor-
mance specific to congenital prosopagnosia but also a
certain gaze behaviour is associated with this impair-
ment. Even early perceptual processes concerning the
acquisition of facial information are sensitive to the
various processes that support face recognition. The
dispersed gaze of the hereditary prosopagnosics is
associated with a low face recognition performance.
Appendix
Faces used in the famous face task
Boris Becker
Norbert Blu
¨
m
George Bush
Sabine Christiansen
Bill Clinton
Gerard Depardieu
Lady Diana
Albert Einstein
Joschka Fischer
Greta Garbo
Thomas Gottschalk
Gu
¨
nther Grass
Gregor Gysi
Gu
¨
nther Jauch
Helmut Kohl
Oskar Lafontaine
Elvis Pressly
Claudia Roth
Gerhard Schro
¨
der
Ralf Schumacher
Ju
¨
rgen Trittin
Theodor Waigel
Richard von Weiza
¨
cker
Ulrich Wickert
References
Antes, J. R. (1974). The time course of picture viewing. Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 103, 62–70.
Ariel, R., & Sadeh, M. (1996). Congenital visual agnosia and
prosopagnosia in a child: A case report. Cortex, 32, 221–240.
Barton, J. J. S., Press, D. Z., Keenan, J. P., & O’Connor, M. (2002).
Lesions of the fusiform face area impair perception of facial
configuration in prosopagnosia. Neurology, 58, 71–78.
Bentin, S., Deouell, L. Y., & Soroker, N. (1999). Selective visual
streaming in face recognition: Evidence from developmental
prosopagnosia. Neuroreport, 10, 823–827.
Behrmann, M., & Avidan, G. (2005). Congenital prosopagnosia:
Face-blind from birth. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 180–
187.
Benton, A. (1990). Facial recognition. Cortex, 26, 491–499.
Bodamer, J. (1947). Die Prosop-Agnosie. Archiv fu
¨
r Psychiatrie
und Nervenkrankheiten, 179, 6–53.
Bruce, V. (1988). Recognizing faces. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Bruce, V., & Young, A. (1998). In the eye of the beholder. Ox-
ford: Oxford University Press.
Campbell, R., Walker, J., & Baron, C. S. (1995). The develop-
ment of differential use of inner and outer face features in
familiar face identification. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 59, 196–210.
Carey, S. (1996). Perceptual classification and expertise. In R.
Gelman, & T. Kit-Fong Au (Eds.). Perceptual and cognitive
development (pp. 49–69). San Diego: Academic.
Carmody, D. P., Nodine, C. F., & Kundel, H. L. (1980). Global
and segmented search for lung nodules of different edge
gradients. Investigative Radiology, 15, 224–233.
Choisser, B. http://www.choisser.com/faceblind/.
De Gelder, B., & Rouw, R. (2000). Configural face processes in
acquired and developmental prosopagnosia: Evidence for
two seperate face systems? Neuroreport, 11, 3145–3150.
De Haan, E. H. F. (1999). A familial factor in the development
of face recognition deficits. Journal of Clinical and Experi-
mental Neuropsychology, 21, 312–315.
Psychological Research
123
De Haan, E. H., & Campbell, R. (1991). A fifteen year follow-up
of a case of developmental prosopagnosia. Cortex, 27(4),
489–509.
Duchaine, B. C. (2000). Developmental prosopagnosia with
normal configural processing. Neuroreport, 11, 79–83.
Duchaine, B. C., Parker, H., & Nakayama, K. (2003). Normal
recognition of emotion in a prosopagnosic. Perception, 32,
827–838.
Gru
¨
ter, M. (2004). Genetik der Prosopagnosie. Dissertation.
Rheinisch Westfa
¨
lische Wilhelms-Universita
¨
t, Mu
¨
nster,
Germany.
Gru
¨
ter, M., Gru
¨
ter, T., Bell, V., Horst, J., Laskowski, W., Sper-
ling, K., et al. (in press). Hereditary prosopagnosia: The first
case series. Cortex.
Hadjikhani, N., & De Gelder, B. (2002). Neural basis of proso-
pagnosia: An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 16, 176–
182.
Hasson, U., Avidan, G., Deouell, L., Bentin, S., & Malach, R.
(2003). Face-selective activation in a congenital prosopag-
nosic subject. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 419–
431.
Henderson, J. M., Falk, R., Minut, S., Dyer, F. C., & Mahadevan,
S. (2001). Gaze control for face learning and recognition in
humans, machines. In T. Shipley & P. Kellman (Eds.) From
fragments to objects: Segmentation processes in vision (pp.
463–481). New York: Elsevier.
Janik, S. W., Wellens, A. R., Goldberg, M. L., & Dell’Osso, L. F.
(1978). Eyes as the center of focus in the visual examination
of human faces. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 857–858.
Jones, R. D., & Tranel, D. (2001). Severe developmental pros-
opagnosia in a child with superior intellect. Journal of
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 265–273.
Kennerknecht, I., Gru
¨
ter, M., Gru
¨
ter, T., Otte, S., Neumann T.,
Meyer B., et al. (2002). First report on the genetics of
prosopagnosia. European Journal of Human Genetics,
10(Suppl. 1), 249.
Kress, T., & Daum, I. (2003). Event-related potentials reflect
impaired face recognition in patients with congenital pros-
opagnosia. Neuroscience Letters, 352, 133–136.
Kundel, H. L., & Nodine, C. F. (1983). A visual concept shapes
image perception. Radiology, 146, 363–368.
Le, S., Raufaste, E., & Demonet, J. (2003). Processing of normal,
inverted, and scrambled faces in a patient with prosopag-
nosia: behavioural and eye tracking data. Cognitive Brain
Research, 17, 26–35.
Manor, B. R., Gordon, E., Williams, L. M., Rennie, C. J., Bah-
ramali, H., Latimer C. R., et al. (1999). Eye movements
reflect impaired face processing in patients with schizo-
phrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 963–969.
McConachie, H. R. (1976). Developmental prosopagnosia: a
single case report. Cortex, 12, 76–82.
Nunn, J. A., Postma, P., & Pearson, R. (2001). Developmental
prosopagnosia: Should it be taken at face value? Neurocase,
7, 15–27.
Oliveira, L. F., Simpson, D. M., & Nadal, J. (1996). Calculation
of area of stabilometric signals using principal component
analysis. Physiological Measurement, 17, 305–312.
Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson N. J., Reznick J. S., Paul G., Goldman B.
D., & Piven J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 249–
261.
PICS. Psychological Image Collection at Stirling, University of
Stirling, Psychology Department, http://www.pics.psych.-
stir.ac.uk/ (access May 2002).
Rizzo, M., Hurtig, R., & Damasio, A. R. (1987). The role of
scanpaths in facial recognition and learning. Annals of
Neurology, 22, 41–45.
Schwarzer, G., Huber, S., & Du
¨
mmler, T. (2005). Gaze behav-
iour in analytical and holistic face processing. Memory &
Cognition, 33, 344–354.
Stark, L., & Ellis, S. R. (1981). Scanpath revisited: Cognitive
models of direct active looking. In D. F. Fisher, R. A.
Monty, & J. W. Senders (Eds.) Gaze behavior: Cognition
and visual perception (pp. 193–226). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Temple C. M. (1992). Developmental memory impairment: faces
and patterns. In R. Campbell (Ed.). Mental lives: case studies
in cognition (pp. 199–215). Oxford: Blackwell.
Viviani P. (1990). Gaze behavior into visual search. Cognitive,
perceptual and motor control aspects. In E. Kowler (Ed.).
Gaze behavior and their role in visual and cognitive pro-
cesses (pp. 353–393). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Walker-Smith, G. J., Gale, A. G., & Findlay, J. M. (1977). Eye
movement strategies involved in face perception. Percep-
tion, 6, 313–326.
Yarbus, A. L. (1967). Eye movements and vision. New York:
Plenum.
Psychological Research
123
... Notably, individuals with CP exhibit abnormal eye movement scanning patterns of faces (Malaspina et al. 2017). The exact deviation from the normal pattern described in this population somewhat varies across studies and tasks, and not all studies address both the temporal and spatial dynamics (Bobak et al. 2017, Malaspina et al. 2017, Schmalzl et al. 2008, Schwarzer et al. 2007). However, a common theme that emerges is that those with CP exhibit a more dispersed gaze profile that often includes fixations toward external facial features (for example, hairline) or toward internal features such as the mouth that are less explored by controls. ...
... However, a common theme that emerges is that those with CP exhibit a more dispersed gaze profile that often includes fixations toward external facial features (for example, hairline) or toward internal features such as the mouth that are less explored by controls. That is, individuals with CP show a clear deviation in the spatial distribution of their eye movements compared to controls, and these deviations are evident during the perception of both familiar and unfamiliar faces and during conditions of both encoding and recognition (Avidan et al. 2011, Bobak et al. 2017, Schmalzl et al. 2008, Schwarzer et al. 2007, Wilcockson et al. 2020. ...
... For example, in a group of individuals with CP, deviations from the normal pattern of face scanning resulted in both successful and unsuccessful face recognition. Likewise, in the same study, normal scanning patterns of internal features in controls were not related to recognition success (Schwarzer et al. 2007). Additionally, individuals with CP exhibited similar patterns of scanning behavior for both upright and inverted faces despite showing overall lower recognition performance for inverted faces, thus implying no obvious relation between recognition performance and scanning behavior in these individuals (Malaspina et al. 2017) (see Figure 4). ...
Article
Congenital prosopagnosia (CP), a life-long impairment in face processing that occurs in the absence of any apparent brain damage, provides a unique model in which to explore the psychological and neural bases of normal face processing. The goal of this review is to offer a theoretical and conceptual framework that may account for the underlying cognitive and neural deficits in CP. This framework may also provide a novel perspective in which to reconcile some conflicting results that permits the expansion of the research in this field in new directions. The crux of this framework lies in linking the known behavioral and neural underpinnings of face processing and their impairments in CP to a model incorporating grid cell–like activity in the entorhinal cortex. Moreover, it stresses the involvement of active, spatial scanning of the environment with eye movements and implicates their critical role in face encoding and recognition. To begin with, we describe the main behavioral and neural characteristics of CP, and then lay down the building blocks of our proposed model, referring to the existing literature supporting this new framework. We then propose testable predictions and conclude with open questions for future research stemming from this model. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Vision Science, Volume 7 is September 2021. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.
... Visual Scan Paths training involves establishing correct face scanning paths by changing the eye movement trajectory of patients during face perception through games or tasks. Schwarzer et al. analyzed the eye movements of patients with prosopagnosia during face perception and found significant differences in the way they perceive faces compared to the general population [44]. Schmalzl et al. suggested that abnormalities in the visual scan paths for faces are likely to be a common feature in children with prosopagnosia [39]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Children with prosopagnosia, also known as face blindness, struggle to recognize the faces of acquaintances, which can have a negative impact on their social interactions and overall functioning. This paper reviews existing research on interventions for children with prosopagnosia, including compensatory and remedial strategies, and provides a summary and comparison of their effectiveness. However, despite the availability of these interventions, their effectiveness remains limited and constrained by various factors. The lack of a widely accepted treatment for children with prosopagnosia emphasizes the need for further research to improve intervention strategies. Last, three future research directions were proposed to improve interventions for prosopagnosia, including ecological approaches, the social challenges faced by children, and new potential intervention methods.
... These results are somewhat unexpected, as some previous research has shown that some individuals with DP focus more on the mouth region (which is covered by face masks) when looking at faces (e.g., Bobak et al., 2017). However, individuals with DP also report using a variety of compensatory strategies when identifying faces (e.g., hairstyle), and eye-tracking studies have also shown a greater focus on external features of the face in DP compared to controls (Schmalzl et al., 2008;Schwarzer et al., 2007). Many of these cues remained available in the current stimuli, so it is possible that DPs were able to compensate for lack of information in the lower face by attending to external features (e.g., hairstyle). ...
Article
Full-text available
Some research indicates that face masks impair identification and other judgements such as trustworthiness. However, it is unclear whether those effects have abated over time as individuals adjust to widespread use of masks, or whether performance is related to individual differences in face recognition ability. This study examined the effect of masks and sunglasses on face matching and social judgements (trustworthiness, competence, attractiveness). In Experiment 1, 135 participants across three different time points (June 2020–July 2021) viewed unedited faces and faces with masks, sunglasses, or both. Both masks and sunglasses similarly decreased matching performance. The effect of masks on social judgements varied depending on the judgement and whether the face was depicted with sunglasses. There was no effect of timepoint on any measure, suggesting that the effects of masks have not diminished. In Experiment 2, 12 individuals with developmental prosopagnosia (DP) and 10 super-recognisers (SRs) completed the same tasks. The effect of masks on identity matching was reduced in SRs, whereas the effects of masks and sunglasses for the DP group did not differ from controls. These findings indicate that face masks significantly affect face perception, depending on the availability of other facial information, and are not modified by exposure.
... Individuals with identity recognition deficits seem to have a specific impairment in building a holistic face representation (Bukach et al. 2006;Ramon et al. 2010;DeGutis et al. 2014). Additionally, low identity recognition ability seems to be associated with more interference due to local information in a Navon test (Behrmann and Avidan 2005;Bentin et al. 2007), lower strength of the composite effect (Palermo et al. 2011a,b;Avidan et al. 2011), difficulty in holistic processing of non-face stimuli (Bentin et al. 2007;Tanzer et al. 2013), and face exploration strategy more focused on details (Schwarzer et al. 2007). All these phenomena point out to the possibility that normal identity recognition ability requires holistic processing of the face. ...
Article
Recognition of identity and of emotional facial expressions of individuals are both based on processing of the human face. While most studies show these abilities to be dissociated, some others find evidence of a connection. One possible explanation for these contradictory results comes from neurological evidence, which points to identity recognition being mostly based on holistic processing, while emotion recognition seems to be based on both an explicit, fine-grained process, and an implicit, mostly-holistic one. Our main hypothesis, that would explain the contradictory findings, is that holistic implicit emotion recognition, specifically, would be related to identity recognition, while explicit emotion recognition would be a process separate to identity recognition. To test this hypothesis, we employed an experimental paradigm in which spatial frequencies of visual stimuli are manipulated so that automatic, holistic-based, implicit emotion recognition influences perceived friendliness of unfamiliar faces. We predicted the effect to be related to identity recognition ability, since they both require holistic face processing. After a successful replication study, we employed the paradigm with 140 participants, measuring also identity recognition ability and explicit emotion recognition ability. Results showed that the effect is not moderated by these two variables (p = .807 and .373, respectively), suggesting that the independence of identity and emotion recognition holds even when considering, specifically, implicit emotion recognition.
... Deficient holistic processing of the eyes may explain the gaze behavior of DP individuals. Four individuals displayed a "more dispersed gaze" and fixated on external facial features in contrast to control subjects (255). ...
Book
Examination tools such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and magnetoencephalography (MEG) have enabled extensive research in neuroanatomy in normal subjects and in individuals with congenital or acquired prosopagnosia. Greater awareness is necessary for early diagnosis and treatment. Three different aspects of prosopagnosia are covered below. In part one, neuroanatomical findings responsible for face recognition and identification in healthy subjects are described, highlighting the core and extended neural network of the extrastriate visual cortex. The three main areas in the core system are: the fusiform face area (FFA), the occipital face area (OFA), and posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS). In part two, the essential clinical and diagnostic features of developmental (congenital) prosopagnosia (DP) are described. DP remains life-long during life. This prosopagnosia is often hereditary. Its prevalence is 2.5% among Caucasians. Of interest to ophthalmologists are functional changes in individuals with DP. Subjects with DP exhibit normal visual acuity and visual fields. Part three covers acquired prosopagnosia (AP), a defect in face recognition following focal brain damage, caused by various brain diseases: it is usually due to bilateral or unilateral occipitotemporal lesions or anterior temporal damage. A better functioning right hemisphere in patients with unilateral brain damage as the cause of AP should be considered. Neuro-ophthalmological deficits have often been diagnosed, i. e., visual field defects in 82%, color vision disturbances (achromatopsia or dyschromatopsia) in 23.3%. Of special interest to ophthalmologists are the findings in subjects’ gaze behavior. There is evidence that averted-gaze and mutual gaze-activated areas differ from those involved in face processing.
... Furthermore, researchers have suggested that atypical gaze behavior toward the face, with fewer fixations on the eye-region, during development, may reflect an important contributing factor to DP (e.g., Schwarzer et al., 2007; also see Bobak et al., 2016). This finding seems to be supported by other studies (e.g., DeGutis et al., 2012b), but more research is needed before firm conclusions can be drawn (e.g., see Geskin and Behrmann, 2018;Peterson et al., 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous research on the beneficial effect of motion has postulated that learning a face in motion provides additional cues to recognition. Surprisingly, however, few studies have examined the beneficial effect of motion in an incidental learning task and developmental prosopagnosia (DP) even though such studies could provide more valuable information about everyday face recognition compared to the perception of static faces. In the current study, 18 young adults (Experiment 1) and five DPs and 10 age-matched controls (Experiment 2) participated in an incidental learning task during which both static and elastically moving unfamiliar faces were sequentially presented and were to be recognized in a delayed visual search task during which the faces could either keep their original presentation or switch (from static to elastically moving or vice versa). In Experiment 1, performance in the elastic-elastic condition reached a significant improvement relative to the elastic-static and static-elastic condition, however, no significant difference could be detected relative to the static-static condition. Except for higher scores in the elastic-elastic compared to the static-elastic condition in the age-matched group, no other significant differences were detected between conditions for both the DPs and the age-matched controls. The current study could not provide compelling evidence for a general beneficial effect of motion. Age-matched controls performed generally worse than DPs, which may potentially be explained by their higher rates of false alarms. Factors that could have influenced the results are discussed.
... Bobak et al. (2017) found that adults with prosopagnosia spent less time than controls looking at the eye region. Further, acquired prosopagnosia cases also demonstrate a similar pattern of facial examination (e.g., Caldara et al., 2005;de Xivry et al., 2008), whilst developmental cases may also have a preference for more external features during facial examination (see Schwarzer et al., 2007). In general, it would appear that adults with prosopagnosia make fewer fixations and demonstrate reduced regional sampling for famous (known) compared to novel faces (Bate et al., 2008), spend more time examining the mouth and less time examining the eyes when compared to controls (e.g., Bate et al., 2015;cf. ...
Article
When people recognize faces, they normally move their eyes so that their first fixation is in the optimal location for efficient perceptual processing. This location is found just below the centre-point between the eyes. This type of attentional bias could be partly innate, but also an inevitable developmental process that aids our ability to recognize faces. We investigated whether a group of people with developmental prosopagnosia would also demonstrate neurotypical first fixation locations when recognizing faces during an eye-tracking task. We found evidence that adults with prosopagnosia had atypically heterogeneous first fixations in comparison to controls. However, differences were limited to the vertical, but not horizontal, plane of the face. We interpret these findings by suggesting that subtle changes to face-based eye movement patterns in developmental prosopagnosia may underpin their face recognition impairments, and suggest future work is still needed to address this possibility.
Article
Full-text available
Dyslexia is a neurodevelopmental difficulty affecting reading, but recent data in adults suggest that difficulties also extend to face processing. Here, we tested face processing in school children with and without dyslexia, using eye-tracking and neuropsychological tests. Children with dyslexia didn't differ significantly from controls in face gaze patterns, face memory, or face identification accuracy. However, they were slower and more heterogeneous, with larger within-group variance than controls. Increased gaze patterns toward the eyes were associated with better face memory in controls. We discuss the possible role of experiential factors in prior research linking dyslexia and face processing differences.
Article
Neuropsychological studies have shown that prosopagnosic individuals perceive face structure in an atypical way. This might preclude the formation of appropriate face representations and, consequently, hamper effective recognition. The present ERP study, in combination with Bayesian source reconstruction, investigates how information related to both external (E) and internal (I) features was processed by E.C. and I.P., suffering from acquired and developmental prosopagnosia, respectively. They carried out a face-feature matching task with new faces. E.C. showed poor performance and remarkable lack of early face-sensitive P1, N170 and P2 responses on right (damaged) posterior cortex. Although she presented the expected mismatch effect to target faces in the E-I sequence, it was of shorter duration than in Controls, and involved left parietal, right frontocentral and dorsofrontal regions, suggestive of reduced neural circuitry to process face configurations. In turn, I.P. performed efficiently but with a remarkable bias to give “match” responses. His face-sensitive potentials P1–N170 were comparable to those from Controls, however, he showed no subsequent P2 response and a mismatch effect only in the I-E sequence, reflecting activation confined to those regions that sustain typically the initial stages of face processing. Relevantly, neither of the prosopagnosics exhibited conspicuous P3 responses to features acting as primes, indicating that diagnostic information for constructing face representations could not be sufficiently attended nor deeply encoded. Our findings suggest a different locus for altered neurocognitive mechanisms in the face network in participants with different types of prosopagnosia, but common indicators of a deficient allocation of attentional resources for further recognition.
Article
Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) to faces have been shown to be altered in patients suffering from prosopagnosia. In this report we present ERP findings from two patients suffering from a congenital form of prosopagnosia, with other visual and cognitive functions being spared and without any structural abnormalities as assessed by anatomical brain imaging. Subjects were presented with photographs of faces and houses, and they had to respond to photographs of hands. Both patients did not show a difference in N170 amplitude to faces compared to houses, whereas there was a significant N170 difference of these two stimulus classes in healthy control subjects.
Article
Face recognition is an area where research has increased considerably in recent years, yet theoretical progress has been slow. Here it is argued that by considering the perception and recognition of familiar faces, as well as episodic memory for unfamiliar faces, a functional framework for face recognition can be developed. Experiments using faces, that include tasks analogous to 'visual search' and 'lexical decision' are described, and the processes in operation are compared with those occurring in word recognition. The results allow us to distinguish a number of possible subcomponents for a functional model of face recognition.
Article
intelligence and verbal fluency / normal models of visual perception and face recognition / visual perception / face processing / recognizing other pictures / visual memory / memory for faces and patterns (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Fat manzum Schlu die Merkmale, die das Wesen der Prosopagnosie ausmachen, zusammen, so lt sich sagen: Sie ist die Agnosie fr das Erkennen von Gesichtern und Ausdrucksphnomen berhaupt. Bei ungestrter Perzeption der Formteile von Physiognomien bleibt der Erkenntnisvorgang aus oder kommt, wie wir das auch von anderen Agnosien kennen, nur unvollkommen zustande. Wie es nun zum Wesen der Agnosie gehrt, sich auf eine optische Kategorie zu beschrnken, so die Prosopagnosie elektiv auf Gesichter. Nicht blo die Tatsache der Prosopagnosie selbst, sondern auch gewisse Beobachtungen (Flimmeranflle, cerebrale Metamorphopsien fr Gesichter) weisen darauf hin, da sie die Strung einer eigentmlichen optischen Kategorie ist, die sowohl das Physiognomiesehen, wie das Physiognomieerkennen umfat und der im Aufbau der Wahrnehmungswelt ein ganz bestimmter Platz zukommt. Es handelt sich hier prinzipiell um den gleichen Vorgang wie bei den brigen Kategorien fr Objekte, Sinnzusammenhnge, Farben, symbolische Zeichen usw., in deren Strungsformen die ihnen zugrunde liegenden optischen Sonderkategorien zum Ausdruck kommen. Da die Agnosie als klinisches Phnomen in solche kategorialen Einzelformen auseinanderfllt, wird gewhnlich einfach hingenommen, ist aber im Grunde tief rtselhaft, und mit der Annahme von gestrten Sonderapparaten im Gehirn so wenig erklrt, wie durch die Theorie der Gestaltspsychologie, da in aller Agnosie die Strung der Erfassung der Gestalt es sei, die die Agnosie bedinge.Die Bearbeitung der Frage, ob die in den Agnosien erscheinenden optischen Sonderkategorien nur durch Zufall vereint auftreten, oder eine innere Hierarchie erkennen lassen, steht noch in den Anfngen. Ein erster Anhaltspunkt lt sich gewinnen durch unseren Nachweis, da die Prosopagnosie Storungsfrm einer optischen Kategorie sein mu, in der die ursprnglichste genetisch frheste Wahrnehmungs- und Erkenntnisfunktion sich prsentiert. Im Strungstyp der Prosopagnosie sehen wir eine Regression auf diese frheste optische Umwelterfassungsstufe, eine Grund- und Urfunktion der Sehwelt berhaupt. In einzelnen Merkmalen der Agnosie, Radikalen gleichsam, vermgen wir noch die, diese Grundkategorie ursprnglich konstituierenden Elemente, wenn auch in verzerrter Form zu erkennen: In der Ocula das primre Wahrnehmungsfeld, in der Faszination durch das mitmenschliche Auge den frhesten optischen Erlebnisakt, in der Strung der eigenen Ausdrucksfhigkeit den durchgehenden Seinsbezug dieser optischen Kategorie und in dem konstanten Ausfall der optischen Merkfhigkeit fr Gesichter das zeitliche Vorausgehen des Ausdruckserkennens vor dem Objektsehen.Bezglich der Frage der chronogenen Engraphierung (v. Monakow) der optischen Kategorien im Laufe der Entwicklung hat Ptzl die Ansicht geuert, da die Simultanagnosie die Regression auf das Bilderbuchstadium der Kinder darstelle, auf die Phase der Und-Verbindungen (Pick). Demnach wre die Simultanagnosie die Strungsform der optischen Sinnkategorie. Zwischen beiden, der Ausdruckskategorie und der Simultankategorie, drfte die Kategorie der Objekt- und Farberfassung liegen, jenseits davon die Welt der symbolischen Zeichen. Diese, ihrer Qualitt nach ganz unterschiedlichen Kategorien, von denen wir hier der Einfachheit halber nur die Ausdrucksschicht, die Objekt-, Sinn- und Symbolschicht nennen, gehen nicht kontinuierlich auseinander hervor, sondern die Entwicklung geschieht in Sprngen. Jede Schicht ist von der anderen durch einen Hiatus irrationalis getrennt. Mit jeder der genannten Schichten beginnt etwas kategorial Neues. Da optisch gegebene Zusammenhnge simultan erkannt werden, ist nicht einfach Folge des vorausgegangenen optisch-gnostischen Erfassens von Objekten, so wenig wie das Erkennen von Symbolzeichen seinen Grund in der Erfassung ihrer Formen hat.Wir stoen hier von der klinischen Empirie her auf dasselbe Phnomen, das die moderne Ontologie, am strengsten verkrpert in Nicolai Hartmann, dazu gefhrt hat, der Welt den Charakter der Schichtung beizulegen. Im Schichtenbau der Welt hat jede Schicht ihre eigenen Gesetze, keine hat ein selbstndiges Sein, immer ruht die hhere auf der niederen, doch ohne Beeintrchtigung ihrer autonomen innerkategorialen Freiheit, denn mit jeder Schicht beginnt ein kategoriales Novum. Diese allgemeinsten Schichtgesetze treffen auch auf die optischen Kategorien zu, nur darf dabei nie bersehen werden, da wir in den optischen Kategorien keine Seinskategorien vor uns haben. Denn im Erkenntnisgebilde als der bloen, sich in Annherung vollziehenden Reprsentanz des Objekts im erkennenden Bewutsein erscheinen nur die Abbilder der eigentlichen Seinskategorien, eben die optischen Kategorien. Ihre Schichtung ist nur ein Hinweis auf die Schichtung der Welt, deren Objekte dem Menschen nie an sich, sondern immer nur als Bilder gegeben sind.So erhebt sich nach der Analyse des Phnomens unabweisbar die anthropologische Frage nach dem Wesen des Menschen und nach seiner Stellung in der Welt, denn in keinem anderen Problembereich, als in dem der Agnosie, Aphasie und Apraxie berhrt sich medizinische Tatsachenforschung so eng mit philosophischer Besinnung, als dem tragenden Grund aller Wissenschaft.