ArticlePDF Available

Not every pseudoword disrupts word recognition: An ERP study

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

If all available acoustic phonetic information of words is used during lexical access and consequently stored in the mental lexicon, then all pseudowords that deviate in a single acoustic feature from a word should hamper word recognition. By contrast, models assuming underspecification of redundant phonological information in the mental lexicon predict a differential disruption of word recognition dependent on the phonological structure of the pseudoword. Using neurophysiological measures, the present study tested the predicted asymmetric disruption by assuming that coronal place of articulation for consonants is redundant. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded during a lexical decision task. The focus of interest was on word medial consonants. The crucial pseudowords were created by replacing the place of articulation of the medial consonant in German disyllabic words. We analyzed the differential temporal characteristics of the N400 pseudoword effect. N400 amplitudes for pseudowords were enhanced compared to words. As the uniqueness and deviation points differ for coronal and non-coronal items, the ERPs had to be correspondingly adjusted. The adjusted ERPs revealed that the N400 pseudoword effect starts earlier for coronal than for non-coronal pseudoword variants. Thus, non-coronal variants are accepted as words longer than the coronal variants. Our results indicate that lexical representations of words containing medial coronal consonants are initially activated by their corresponding non-coronal pseudowords. The most plausible explanation for the asymmetric neuronal processing of coronal and non-coronal pseudoword variants is an underspecified coronal place of articulation in the mental lexicon.
Content may be subject to copyright.
BioMed Central
Page 1 of 10
(page number not for citation purposes)
Behavioral and Brain Functions
Open Access
Research
Not every pseudoword disrupts word recognition: an ERP study
Claudia K Friedrich*
1,2
, Carsten Eulitz
1
and Aditi Lahiri
1
Address:
1
Department of Linguistics, University of Konstanz, Germany and
2
Biological Psychology and Neuropsychology, University of Hamburg,
Germany
Email: Claudia K Friedrich* - Claudia.Friedrich@uni-hamburg.de; Carsten Eulitz - carsten.eulitz@uni-konstanz.de;
Aditi Lahiri - aditi.lahiri@uni-konstanz.de
* Corresponding author
Abstract
Background: If all available acoustic phonetic information of words is used during lexical access
and consequently stored in the mental lexicon, then all pseudowords that deviate in a single
acoustic feature from a word should hamper word recognition. By contrast, models assuming
underspecification of redundant phonological information in the mental lexicon predict a
differential disruption of word recognition dependent on the phonological structure of the
pseudoword. Using neurophysiological measures, the present study tested the predicted
asymmetric disruption by assuming that coronal place of articulation for consonants is redundant.
Methods: Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded during a lexical decision task. The focus
of interest was on word medial consonants. The crucial pseudowords were created by replacing
the place of articulation of the medial consonant in German disyllabic words. We analyzed the
differential temporal characteristics of the N400 pseudoword effect.
Results: N400 amplitudes for pseudowords were enhanced compared to words. As the
uniqueness and deviation points differ for coronal and non-coronal items, the ERPs had to be
correspondingly adjusted. The adjusted ERPs revealed that the N400 pseudoword effect starts
earlier for coronal than for non-coronal pseudoword variants. Thus, non-coronal variants are
accepted as words longer than the coronal variants.
Conclusion: Our results indicate that lexical representations of words containing medial coronal
consonants are initially activated by their corresponding non-coronal pseudowords. The most
plausible explanation for the asymmetric neuronal processing of coronal and non-coronal
pseudoword variants is an underspecified coronal place of articulation in the mental lexicon.
Background
Despite the fact that we perceive speech in an apparently
involuntary fashion, one requires several processing
stages to extract the meaning of an utterance. Acoustic-
phonetic features that build up the signal have to be
extracted and mapped onto knowledge stored in the lis-
teners' long-term memory in the form of lexical represen-
tations [1]. Mental representations must be capable of
successfully handling the tremendous amount of variabil-
ity in the speech signal with which the recognition system
is confronted. A frequent source of variation is assimila-
tion where segments take on the properties of other
sounds in close proximity. A common assimilation dis-
cussed frequently in the literature involves consonants
Published: 24 October 2006
Behavioral and Brain Functions 2006, 2:36 doi:10.1186/1744-9081-2-36
Received: 17 August 2006
Accepted: 24 October 2006
This article is available from: http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/content/2/1/36
© 2006 Friedrich et al; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0
),
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Behavioral and Brain Functions 2006, 2:36 http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/content/2/1/36
Page 2 of 10
(page number not for citation purposes)
with a CORONAL place of articulation (PLACE; e.g., /t, d,
n/), which appear to borrow the PLACE information of
the segment that immediately follows. The /n/ in rain, for
example, can take on the PLACE of the following LABIAL
/b/ in rainbow resulting in *raimbow (note that here and in
the following examples an asterisk marks pseudowords).
Different accounts have been proposed to explore the fact
that assimilated *raim does not disrupt recognition of rain
[2-16]. One of these approaches is the assumption of a
featurally underspecified lexicon (FUL) [2-4]. In an under-
specified lexicon feature value slots can be left empty par-
ticularly for feature values that frequently show variation.
Leaving the PLACE slot for coronal feature values empty
(underspecified representation) allows activation from
systematic variants that are specified for PLACE, as these
will not mismatch with the underspecified value in the
lexical representation. Thus, *raim can activate the word
rain. Invariant feature values, e.g. [LABIAL] and [DOR-
SAL] PLACE, are specified in the lexical representation
because they can reliably contribute to word identifica-
tion. Consider for example the dorsal /K/ in long days that
will not become a coronal /n/ even if it is followed by a
coronal /d/. The extraction of features from the signal, and
the presence or absence of those features in the represen-
tation of words predicts asymmetric activation patterns.
Words are activated if signal and representation contain
the same information, or if an extracted feature is not rep-
resented in the lexicon. In contrast, words are rejected if
signal and representation contain incompatible features.
Alternative approaches to the FUL model strengthen the
importance of context for the correct recognition of assim-
ilated forms. Gaskell and Marslen-Wilson assume that
mapping speech onto lexical representations involves on-
line phonological inference that detects systematic varia-
tion [5,6]. In this approach it is proposed that listeners
cope with assimilation by inverting phonological rules in
a given context. Accordingly, when presented with raim-
bow, listeners infer that a labial followed by another labial
may be an underlying coronal. Evidence for contextual
sensitivity in the analysis of feature changes has been pro-
vided in cross-modal priming experiments [5-7]. Further-
more, a combined impact of context and phonetic detail
has been proposed by Gow [8-10]. He argues that assimi-
lation does not change a feature completely, but provides
information about both the underlying form of the assim-
ilated segment and the surface form of the following seg-
ment. According to this point of view listeners detect
assimilated features, use this information to anticipate the
upcoming segment, and align the assimilated feature to
this subsequent element. Indeed, some empirical evi-
dence suggests that context in combination with phonetic
detail drives compensation for assimilation [8-12].
However, the representational-cum-mapping hypothesis
of FUL also received support from psycholinguistic as well
as from neurolinguistic studies. Behavioral priming exper-
iments showed that only non-coronal variants (e.g.,
*wickib) activate associates of coronal words (e.g., wicked),
whereas coronal variants (e.g., *sanctun) do not activate
associates of non-coronal words (e.g., sanctum) [4]. More-
over, recent cross-modal priming studies challenged the
impact of context by showing that lexical activation of
underlying coronal entries appears to be equally tolerant
to contextually appropriate and inappropriate changes of
coronal elements [13,14]. Finally, mismatch negativity
(MMN) observed in event related brain potentials (ERPs)
provides evidence for FUL. MMN is elicited by infrequent
deviant stimuli that are presented after a random number
of frequent standard stimuli (see [17] for an overview).
Earlier latency and higher amplitude MMN values were
found for coronal deviants among non-coronal standards,
as compared to non-coronal deviants among coronal
standards. This suggests that the representations activated
by the coronal standards do not have PLACE specified
[18].
Psycholinguistic research on underspecified entries largely
looked at assimilation contexts occurring in word final
position. Here one could argue that all assimilation vari-
ants are stored, since they occur in predictable environ-
ments and will be part of the listener's experience.
However, if lexical underspecification is part of the mental
representation, then asymmetric activation should also
occur for other positions within a word. Despite the fact
that variants in word medial position can never occur due
to the context of another word FUL expects asymmetric
acceptance: a labial or dorsal variant of an underlying
underspecified coronal would be tolerated but not vice
versa. For example, *wimmer will be accepted as a variant
of winner, but *tunny is an impossible variant of the word
tummy. Such an asymmetry also affects the rejection of
alternates as possible variants. The prediction would be
that variants like *tunny should be rejected as a word since
they conflict immediately with tummy. In contrast, it is
possible that *wimmer is more difficult to reject since it is
acceptable as a variant of the real word winner.
In the present experiment we tested the neurophysiologi-
cal validity of an underspecified lexicon with underspeci-
fied word medial coronal consonants, utilizing the high
temporal resolution of ERPs recorded in a lexical decision
task. Previous ERP research suggests that a specific nega-
tive ERP component, the N400, is sensitive to the time-
course of cognitive processes underlying word recogni-
tion. Since its first description [19], the N400 has not only
been correlated to aspects of semantic processing in sen-
tence and priming contexts, but also to single word
processing (see [20] for a review). The N400 pseudoword
Behavioral and Brain Functions 2006, 2:36 http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/content/2/1/36
Page 3 of 10
(page number not for citation purposes)
effect is characterized by larger and longer lasting negative
amplitudes for isolated spoken or written pseudowords as
compared to words, and is understood to reflect enhanced
lexico-semantic memory search for pseudowords that
have no lexical representation (e.g., [21-25]). Indeed,
using magnetoencephalograhpy (MEG) and magnetic res-
onance imaging, generators for the scalp-recorded N400
have been localized among others in the left temporal
lobe, comprising brain areas supporting long-term mem-
ory functions [26,27].
However, it appears as if ERP effects in the time course of
the N400 component reflect several temporally and cog-
nitively distinct processes [28]. In sentence context, pat-
terns of N400 amplitude change prior to and after the
minimum duration required to identify a word [29-32].
Even N400 amplitudes elicited by single words and pseu-
dowords diverge as a function of the amount of informa-
tion provided by the temporally unfolding speech signal
[33]. Words and pseudowords do not differ for the initial
N400 component, presumably because the speech signal
activates several alternatives in the listeners' mental lexi-
con. Different N400 patterns occur as soon as the signal
discriminates a pseudoword from all other entries in a
given language (deviation point). Therefore, only the ini-
tial part of the N400 pseudoword effect might be related
to normal fast word processing, whereas the later sus-
tained negativity for pseudowords might reflect subse-
quent top-down guided evidence checking.
In this study we presented spoken words and pseudoword
variants that differed only in their medial consonant,
which either had a coronal PLACE (/d/, /t/, /n/), or a non-
coronal PLACE (LABIAL: /b/, /p/, /m/; DORSAL: /g/, /k/;
see Figure 1 for an illustration of example stimuli).
According to the FUL model lexical representations of
words with word medial coronals like Horde ('horde')
have no word medial place represented in the lexicon. A
non-coronal variant like *Horbe cannot mismatch this
empty PLACE slot and therefore activates Horde. Although
CORONAL PLACE is not represented in the lexicon, this
feature can be perceived in the signal and can be used for
lexical mapping. For example, a coronal pseudoword var-
iant like *Prode mismatches the specified [LABIAL] PLACE
of the German word Probe 'test' and therefore cannot acti-
vate this word. If the assumption of underspecification
holds, lexico-semantic memory search processes would be
successful when a non-coronal variant is presented (first
activating the corresponding coronal target word), but not
when a coronal variant is presented (immediate correct
rejection as a non-existing lexical item). Thus, we expect
an asymmetry at least for the initial N400 pseudoword
effect, which most likely is related to lexico-semantic
processing. Related to the point in time where pseudow-
ords diverge from their respective words (deviation
points) initial N400 should be reduced for non-coronal
variants as compared to coronal variants.
Method
Participants
Sixteen (eight females, eight males) undergraduate stu-
dents from the University of Konstanz participated in the
experiment. All participants were native speakers of Ger-
man with no discernible uncorrected deficits in hearing.
Participants were paid for their participation or received
course credit points. Only right-handers were included, as
ascertained by the Edinburgh Handedness Questionnaire
[34].
Stimuli
Ninety familiar German disyllabic nouns, with a medial
stop or nasal consonant, and with stress on the first sylla-
ble were selected. Half of the words had a coronal conso-
Experimental ManipulationFigure 1
Experimental Manipulation. Examples of words (Above)
and pseudoword variants (Below) are illustrated by respec-
tive speech sounds (Upper) and spectropraphic displays
(Lower). Left: example for a presented coronal word (Horde
[Engl. horde]) and its non-coronal variant (*Horbe). Right:
Example for a presented non-coronal word (Probe [Engl.
test]) and its coronal variant (*Prode).
Behavioral and Brain Functions 2006, 2:36 http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/content/2/1/36
Page 4 of 10
(page number not for citation purposes)
nant and the other half a non-coronal consonant as onset
of the second syllable. A pseudoword with the opposite
medial PLACE feature was formed for each word. Fre-
quency of coronal and non-coronal words was matched
using an on-line dictionary of German [35].
As neighborhood density is known to modify N400
amplitudes [36], we determined phonological neighbors
of the words and pseudowords by using the CELEX data-
base [37]. A neighbor is any word which differs from the
stimulus by replacing, deleting or adding one phoneme in
any position (cf. [38]). Only monomorphemic, disyllabic
neighbors with stress on the first syllable were included. A
two-way ANOVA with repeated measures factors Lexical
Status (words vs. pseudoword variants), and Coronality
(coronal words, and their variants vs. non-coronal words
and their variants) only revealed a significant effect of Lex-
ical Status [F(1,44) = 32.41, p < .001]. Words had on aver-
age 3.4 (S [tandard] D [eviation] 2.6) neighbors,
pseudoword variants had 2.2 (SD = 1.8) neighbors.
Neighborhood density did not differ for coronal words
(M [ean] = 3.4, SD = 2.3) and non-coronal words (M =
3.4, SD = 2.8) [t(44) = 0.01, n.s.], nor for coronal pseu-
dowords (M = 2.4, SD = 2.0) and non-coronal pseudow-
ords (M = 2.0, SD = 1.6) [t(44) = 0.84, n.s.].
All words and pseudowords were spoken by a male native
speaker of German in a sound attenuating chamber. The
speaker was naïve with respect to the experimental manip-
ulation and the hypotheses of the study. He read the stim-
uli in a fluent style from lists in which a word was
immediately followed by the respective pseudoword vari-
ant. The speaker was instructed to pronounce the variants
in the same way as the words. Stimuli were recorded on a
DAT recorder at a sampling rate of 44.1 kHz using a high
quality microphone. The recordings were then transferred
to a computer, the volume equalized, and edited into
individual tokens using the Cooledit 2000 waveform
manipulation software package. Where possible, single
periods of voiced speech sounds were deleted from either
the word or the variant to equalize the duration of the
stimuli. This resulted in the same mean length of coronal
words and their non-coronal pseudoword variants (M =
678 ms, SD = 97 for both), and of the same mean length
for non-coronal words and their coronal pseudoword var-
iants (702 ms, SD = 102 for both).
Uniqueness points of the words and deviation points of
the pseudowords were established using the CELEX lexical
database [37]. Scanning from left to right, we determined
the first phoneme of the words that made them unique
with respect to all other entries in the CELEX database,
and also the first phoneme of the pseudowords that made
them distinguishable from any monomorphemic, disyl-
labic, first-syllable stressed word in the database. For 40
non-coronal words, 40 coronal words, 41 non-coronal
pseudowords, and 40 coronal pseudowords this unique-
ness or deviation point was the phoneme for which the
Neurophysiological results time-locked to word onsetFigure 2
Neurophysiological results time-locked to word onset. (A) plots the grand average ERPs for non-coronal words (blue
lines) and coronal pseudoword variants (magenta lines) for selected electrode sites. (B) shows the grand average ERPs for
coronal words (black lines) and non-coronal pseudoword variants (red lines) for selected electrode sites. For illustration pur-
poses only ERPs were low-pass filtered (20 Hz).
Behavioral and Brain Functions 2006, 2:36 http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/content/2/1/36
Page 5 of 10
(page number not for citation purposes)
place of articulation was manipulated. For the remaining
4 to 5 stimuli per group the uniqueness/deviation pho-
neme appeared later in the word. Next, the onset of the
deviating phonemes, which was the onset of the closure
period for plosives and the onset of the first period for
nasals, was determined in the acoustic signal. Mean
uniqueness points were 335 ms (SD = 107) for coronal
words and 366 ms (SD = 104) for non-coronal words.
Mean deviation points were 355 ms (SD = 98) for coronal
pseudoword variants and 322 ms (SD = 104) for non-
coronal pseudoword variants. These uniqueness and devi-
ation points were used to adjust the ERP and behavioral
data.
Procedure
Sintered silver-silver chloride electrodes were held in place
on the scalp with an elastic cap (EASY Cap). Scalp loca-
tions included 62 standard International 10–10 system
locations. Two additional electrodes to control for eye
movements were placed below both eyes. All electrodes
were online referenced to Cz. The data were re-referenced
offline to the algebraic average of the left and the right
mastoids. All electrode impedances were less than 5 K.
The electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded with a
sampling rate of 250 Hz.
Subjects sat in a sound attenuated booth and made
speeded lexical decisions to stimuli presented at a com-
fortable listening level (Sony loudspeakers). A trial started
with a fixation point presented in the center of a computer
screen. 200 ms after the onset of the fixation point a spo-
ken stimulus was presented. The fixation point remained
throughout the spoken stimulus and was terminated with
the subject's response. Subjects were told not to blink and
to look at the fixation point as long as it appeared on the
screen. Following the fixation point there was a 1500 ms
blank screen intertrial-interval. Subjects were told they
could blink during this interval. Half the subjects made
yes-responses with the thumb of their left hand and no-
responses with the thumb of their right hand. For the
remaining subjects, the response hands were reversed.
Speed and accuracy were stressed equally. Subjects were
given a break after half of the trials.
Data analysis
Error rates were calculated for all stimuli. Reaction times
and ERPs were only calculated for correctly responded tri-
als. Eye blinks and movements were systematically
recorded from each subject before the experimental task
started. Characteristic scalp topographies of eye artifacts
were corrected from the experimental data using Brain
Electrical Source Analysis (BESA;
®
MEGIS Software
GmbH). Word onset ERP data were quantified by calculat-
ing the mean amplitudes (relative to a 200 ms prestimulus
baseline) in a latency window between 500 and 1000 ms
(N400 pseudoword effect). ERPs time-locked to unique-
ness and deviation points were quantified by calculating
the mean amplitudes (relative to a 200 ms pre uniqueness
and deviation point baseline) in two latency windows
(early N400 pseudoword effect: 100–250 ms; late N400
pseudoword effect: 250–750 ms) according to 50 ms
time-step analyses (see 3C).
As there were virtually no interhemispheric effects, two
regions of interest (ROIs) each including 20 lateral and
midline electrode positions were defined. An anterior ROI
included electrodes AF7, AF3, AFz, AF4, AF8, F5, F1, Fz,
F2, F6, FC3, FC1, FCz, FC2, FC4, C5, C3, Cz, C4 and C6;
a posterior ROI included electrodes CP5, CP3, CP1, CPz,
CP2, CP4, CP6, P7, P3, P1, Pz, P2, P4, P8, T7, TP7, TP8,
T8, PO1 and PO2. Two factors entered the repeated meas-
ures ANOVAs for behavioral data: Lexical Status (words vs.
pseudoword variants), and Coronality (coronal words, and
their variants vs. non-coronal words and their variants).
An additional repeated measures factor Region (anterior
vs. posterior electrode leads) was included in the ANOVA
with the mean ERP amplitudes as the dependent variable.
Scalp topographies were generated using BESA.
Results
Behavioral results
Mean reaction times from stimulus onset and from
uniqueness/deviation points as well as error rates are
shown in Table 1. Starting from stimulus onset subjects
needed 976 ms to correctly identify words, and 1003 ms
to correctly reject pseudoword variants. As shown by the
main effect of Lexical Status [F(1,15) = 27.64, p < .001] this
difference was statistically significant. A main effect of
Coronality [F(1,15) = 17.84, p < .001] indicated that
responses were faster for coronal words and their respec-
tive pseudoword variants than for non-coronal words and
their pseudoword variants. This might well be caused by
the longer duration of the former as compared to the latter
stimuli (see Methods section). Factors Lexical Status and
Coronality did not interact [F(1,15) = 0. 05, n.s.].
In order to control for different word lengths across both
Coronality conditions, individual uniqueness points were
subtracted from individual reaction times to words and
deviation points were subtracted from reaction times to
pseudowords. After this correction, only the main effects
of Lexical Status remained significant [F(1,15) = 39.79, p <
.001]. Words were identified 615 ms after their unique-
ness point, pseudoword variants were identified 674 ms
after their deviation points. The interaction of Lexical Sta-
tus and Coronality remained to be not significant [F(1,15)
= 0.03]. It took subjects the same time to reject coronal or
non-coronal pseudowords after the appearance of their
respective deviation points.
Behavioral and Brain Functions 2006, 2:36 http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/content/2/1/36
Page 6 of 10
(page number not for citation purposes)
Neurophysiological results time-locked to uniqueness and deviation pointsFigure 3
Neurophysiological results time-locked to uniqueness and deviation points. (A) plots the grand average ERPs for
non-coronal words (blue lines) and coronal pseudoword variants (magenta lines) for selected electrode sites. (B) shows the
grand average ERPs for coronal words (black lines) and non-coronal pseudoword variants (red lines) for selected electrode
sites. (C) illustrates ERPs for all four experimental conditions for a representative electrode lead and summarizes outcomes of
50 ms time-steps analyses. Time windows yielding significant effects or trends (p < .10) of the factor Lexical Status (Lex.) or
interactions of Lexical Status and Coronality (Lex. X Cor.) are indicated by an asterisk. (D) shows subtraction waves (variant-
word) and respective scalp topography maps (180 ms after the uniqueness/deviation points). Across (A), (B) and (D) the part
of the N400 pseudoword effect that is sensitive to different PLACE mismatches, is highlighted in light grey. For illustration pur-
poses only ERPs were low-pass filtered (20 Hz).
AF3
AFz
AF4
F1
Fz
F2
CP1
CPz
CP2
P1
Pz
P2
AF3
AFz
AF4
F1
Fz
F2
CP1
CPz
CP2
P1
Pz
P2
-5 µV
5 µV
-200 400 800ms
non-coronal word
coronal variant
-200 400 800ms
-5 µV
5 µV
coronal word
non-coronal variant
FCz
-5 µV
-200 200 400 600 800 ms
Lex.
Lex. x Cor.
***
**
*
**
**
*
*
*
*
A
B
C
D
Fz
FCz
-200 400 800ms
2.5 -2.5 µV
coronal variant -
non-coronal word
-2 µV
2 µV
non-coronal variant
- coronal word
Table 1: Summarized in this table are mean reaction times (RT) in ms from stimulus onset and from uniqueness/deviation points (UP/
DP), and error rates in percent for all conditions (with standard deviations).
RT Error Rates
Stimulus Onset UP/DP
Noncoronal words 980 (98) 613 (97) 6.7 (4.3)
Coronal variant 1026 (125) 671 (125) 2.2 (2.1)
Coronal words 951 (96) 616 (95) 7.5 (4.8)
Non-coronal variant 1001 (107) 676 (107) 4.9 (3.1)
Behavioral and Brain Functions 2006, 2:36 http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/content/2/1/36
Page 7 of 10
(page number not for citation purposes)
Analysis of error rates revealed that speeded responses to
words were associated with lower accuracy. Subjects made
7% errors for words and 3.5% errors for pseudoword var-
iants [F(1,15) = 13.51, p < .01]. Furthermore, a main effect
of Coronality was observed for error rates [F(1,15) = 5.23 p
< .05]. Subjects made more errors for coronal words and
their variants (6.1%, SD = 4.2) than for non-coronal
words and their variants (4.4%, SD = 4.0). No significant
interactions of Lexical Status and Coronality were observed
for reaction times or error rates [F(1,15) = 1.41, n.s.].
ERPs time-locked to stimulus onset
The ERP grand mean waveforms time-locked to the onsets
of words and pseudoword variants for selected anterior
and posterior electrode sites are plotted in Figure 2. For all
the ERPs, the first visible component was a negative-going
deflection peaking at 130 ms after stimulus onset (N1).
This was followed by a positive deflection occurring at
approximately 200 ms (P2). Starting at 300 ms a broad
negativity was observed, which was enhanced for pseu-
doword variants as compared to words and is henceforth
referred to as the N400 pseudoword effect. Additionally, a
late positivity ranging between 600 and 1500 ms was
observed for posterior electrode leads. A time window
ranging from 500 to 1000 ms was used to further establish
the N400 pseudoword effect time-locked to stimulus
onset.
Time window 500 to 1000 ms
Significant main effects for the factors Region [F(1,15) =
46.46, p < .001] and Lexical Status [F(1,15) = 8.21, p = .01]
were observed in the time window of the N400 pseudow-
ord effect time-locked to stimulus onset. ERPs were more
negative for frontal electrode leads than for posterior elec-
trode leads, and more negative for pseudowords than for
words. An interaction of the factors Lexical Status and
Coronality did not reach significance [F(1,15) = 2.75, p =
.11].
Overall, an N400 pseudoword effect could be established
in the present experiment. However, temporal analysis of
this effect has to consider different uniqueness and devia-
tion points within and across conditions. Next we
adjusted the N400 pseudoword effect to the point in time
where differences between the words and their respective
pseudoword variants occurred in the acoustic signal.
ERPs time-locked to uniqueness and deviation points
The ERP grand mean waveforms time-locked to the
uniqueness points of the words and to the deviation
points of the pseudoword variants for selected anterior
and posterior electrode sites are plotted in Figure 3. The
ERPs zoom into the N400 effect and the posterior positiv-
ity. 50 ms time-steps analyses suggest an earlier differenti-
ation of N400 effects for coronal and non-coronal
pseudowords (see Figure 3C). A time window ranging
from 100 to 250 ms was analyzed to investigate the early
pseudoword N400 effect for ERPs adjusted to uniqueness
and deviation points. The later N400 effect was analyzed
in a time window ranging from 250 to 750 ms.
Time window 100 to 250 ms
Early N400 amplitudes showed a main effect of Region
[F(1,15) = 8.46, p = .02], which did not interact with any
other factor. ERPs were more negative over posterior than
over anterior electrode leads. Crucially, an interaction of
the factors Lexical Status and Coronality licensed differenti-
ation of coronal and non-coronal pseudoword effects
[F(1,15) = 6.22, p = .02]. Mean amplitudes for coronal
pseudoword variants were more negative than mean
amplitudes for their non-coronal base words [t(15) =
6.19, p = .02]. By contrast, ERPs for non-coronal variants
did not differ from their base words in this initial part of
the N400 pseudoword effect [t(15) = 0.26, n.s.]. Further-
more, a significant difference between both types of pseu-
doword variants [t(15) = 12.21, p < .01] but not between
both types of words [t(15) = 0.23, n.s.] relates this early
ERP deflection to mismatch detection in the case of coro-
nal pseudowords.
Time window 250 to 750 ms
Significant main effects for the factors Region [F(1,15) =
98.42, p < .001] and Lexical Status [F(1,15) = 11.66, p <
.01] were observed in the second time window of the ERPs
time locked to uniqueness and deviation points. ERPs
were more negative for frontal electrode leads and more
positive for posterior electrode leads. No interaction effect
reached significance revealing that both types of pseudow-
ord variants were differentiated in a same way from their
respective base words in the later time window of the
N400 pseudoword effect.
Discussion
This paper examined neurophysiological correlates of
spoken word identification, by means of ERPs recorded
during a lexical decision task. The question we asked was
how the recognition system copes with pseudowords that
differ only slightly from existing words, namely in word
medial place of articulation (PLACE). Assuming an under-
specified coronal PLACE, we predicted asymmetric effects
for pseudowords being either coronal or non-coronal var-
iants. In accordance with our hypotheses the N400 pseu-
doword effect starts earlier for coronal than for non-
coronal pseudowords.
Our results support assumptions of the FUL model of spo-
ken word recognition [2-4]. In this model coronal words
are proposed to have an empty feature value slot for
PLACE in the mental lexicon. Even though a LABIAL or
DORSAL PLACE is extracted from non-coronal pseudow-
Behavioral and Brain Functions 2006, 2:36 http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/content/2/1/36
Page 8 of 10
(page number not for citation purposes)
ords they can activate lexical representations of coronal
words (i.e. *Horbe can activate Horde). Hence, non-coro-
nal pseudowords ought to behave like words at the initial
activation period. The opposite hypothesis prevails for
non-coronal words. FUL assumes that non-coronal entries
have PLACE specified. Coronal variants, for which a
CORONAL PLACE is extracted from the signal, cannot
activate non-coronal words (i.e., *Prode cannot activate
Probe). That is, coronal variants should be regarded as
nonwords earlier than non-coronal variants. In line with
these assumptions, coronal variants elicited an earlier
N400 pseudoword effect than non-coronal variants.
The fact that it took subjects the same time to reject both
types of pseudoword variants, together with the finding
that pseudoword rejection took longer than word accept-
ance, point to a process that detects pseudowords and
guides responses in the lexical decision task independ-
ently of initially asymmetric lexical activation. The present
ERP results also provide evidence for an additional proc-
ess in the lexical decision task. Both types of pseudoword
variants elicited enhanced negativity in a later time win-
dow of the N400 pseudoword effect. The FUL model may
account for this later ERP effect and for the behavioral
results by assuming phonological parsing mechanisms
that are independent of initial lexical activation [4]. One
could surmise that a post-lexical mechanism rejects both
coronal and non-coronal pseudoword variants. Whether
this evidence checking operation is mandatory in spoken
word recognition or whether it is especially recruited to
solve the lexical decision task has to be clarified in future
research.
A separation of lexical and post-lexical processing in the
N400 time window has already been suggested in previ-
ous ERP research on speech recognition. Specifically it has
been argued that pseudowords undergo some additional
post-lexical processing that is reflected in the sustained
N400 pseudoword effect [33,39]. Similarly it has been
shown that negative going ERP effects in spoken sentence
comprehension are at least two-fold [29-32]. An early neg-
ativity starting 150 ms after word onset is equally reduced
for words that fit into the semantic context of a spoken
sentence (e.g., She illuminated the dark room with her can-
dle.), as well as for words that semantically do not fit into
a spoken sentence context, but match the initial pho-
nemes of the fitting word (e.g., She illuminated the dark
room with her candy.). Only later, starting at 300 ms,
semantically acceptable words diverge from those that
had initial phoneme overlap. Hagoort and coworkers
argue that the early interaction of word-form and content
information is related to ongoing lexical selection,
whereas the later N400 effect is associated to semantic
integration of selected candidates.
In sum, we separate an early from a later part of the N400
pseudoword effect. Integrating current and previous ERP
findings we conclude that the initial part of the N400
pseudoword effect closely relates to lexical activation pat-
tern in spoken word recognition. We argue that the asym-
metry of this effect reflects that pseudoword variants can
only activate underspecified cortical word form represen-
tations in the listeners' mental lexicon. The early onset of
the N400 pseudoword effect (100 ms after the deviation
point) might reflect that PLACE information is already
exploited at the vowel that precedes the deviating pho-
neme [40]. Crucially, however, the early onset of the
N400 pseudoword effect for coronal variants suggests that
the CORONAL PLACE is immediately used to prevent lex-
ical activation of non-coronal words.
Our findings add important arguments on theoretical
accounts to spoken word recognition. First, the early
asymmetric N400 pseudoword effect challenges context
based models of assimilated speech recognition [5,6,8-
10]. The fact that the pseudoword N400 effect for coronal
variants starts 100 ms after the onset of the coronal speech
sound reveals that initial lexical activation does not wait
until contextual information is available. Second, asym-
metric lexical activation is observed for the first time in
pseudowords created by replacing medial consonants in
monomorphemic words. Such pseudowords cannot be
the result of regular assimilation patterns with final con-
sonants like green bag > *gree [m] bag, and thus listeners
are never exposed to such variants. That is, there could be
no variant exemplars stored for medial stops and nasals,
and therefore an exemplar based model (e.g. [41]) cannot
account for the observed asymmetry. Third, asymmetric
pseudoword effects for coronal and non-coronal pseu-
dowords cannot be captured by models of spoken word
recognition that assume that all segments, usually pho-
nemes, equally contribute to lexical activation (e.g., [42-
44]). The fact that we have observed different ERP effects
for coronal and non-coronal pseudowords, which start
immediately after the deviation is present in the signal
and which are in the direction proposed by the FUL
model, supports our assumption that underspecification
is a basic principle of the functional organization of the
mental lexicon.
Conclusion
The FUL model of spoken word recognition [2-4] pro-
vides excellent theoretical underpinning for the discovery
of neurophysiological mechanisms underlying human
speech comprehension. Former studies showed that pho-
nological features are extracted early from the signal and
that cortical representations of speech are based on those
features [45-47]. Furthermore, the asymmetric mapping
of features onto specified and underspecified cortical
vowel representations has been established in a previous
Behavioral and Brain Functions 2006, 2:36 http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/content/2/1/36
Page 9 of 10
(page number not for citation purposes)
ERP study that used an MMN paradigm [18]. The present
findings are in accordance with this line of neurophysio-
logical evidence. Using a different ERP component and
more complex linguistic stimuli, the present study sup-
ports the notion of specified and underspecified cortical
word form representations and an asymmetric mapping
of features extracted from word medial positions onto
those lexical representations.
Authors' contributions
CF designed and conducted the study, analyzed the ERP
data, and wrote the initial drafts. CE contributed to the
ERP analysis. AL provided the theoretical background. All
authors contributed to the selection of the stimulus mate-
rials, experimental design and worked on the manuscript.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by the Leibniz science prize and a grant from
the Ministry of Science, Research, and the Arts of Baden-Württemberg
awarded to Aditi Lahiri, and by grants from the Deutsche Forschungsgemein-
schaft (SFB 471 and FOR 348) to Aditi Lahiri and Carsten Eulitz. We are
much obliged to Verena Felder and Barbara Awiszus for help in data acqui-
sition.
References
1. McQueen JM: Speech perception. In The handbook of cognition
Edited by: Lamberts K, Goldstone R. London: Sage Publications;
2004:255-275.
2. Lahiri A, Marslen-Wilson WD: The mental representation of
lexical form: A phonological approach to the recognition lex-
icon. Cognition 1991, 38:245-294.
3. Lahiri A, Marslen-Wilson WD: Lexical processing and phonolog-
ical representation. In Papers in Laboratory Phonology II Edited by:
Docherty GJ, Ladd DR. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press;
1992:229-254.
4. Lahiri A, Reetz H: Underspecified recognition. In Laboratory Pho-
nology VII Edited by: Gussenhoven C, Werner N. Berlin: Mouton;
2002:637-675.
5. Gaskell MG, Marslen-Wilson WD: Phonological variation and
inference in lexical access. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1996,
22:144-158.
6. Gaskell MG, Marslen-Wilson WD: Mechanisms of phonological
inference in speech perception. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform
1998, 24:380-396.
7. Coenen E, Zwitserlood P, Bölte J: Variation and assimilation in
German: Consequences for lexical access and representa-
tion. Lang Cognitive Proc 2001, 16:535-564.
8. Gow DW: Assimilation and anticipation in continuous spoken
word recognition. J Mem Lang 2001, 45:133-159.
9. Gow DW: Does English coronal place assimilation create lex-
ical ambiguity? J Exp Psychol Human 2002, 28:163-179.
10. Gow DW: Feature parsing: Feature cue mapping in spoken
word recognition. Percept Psychophys 2003, 65:575-590.
11. Mitterer H, Blomert L: Coping with phonological assimilation in
speech perception: Evidence for early compensation. Percept
Psychophys 2003, 65:956-969.
12. Mitterer H, Csepe V, Honbolygo F, Blomert L: The recognition of
phonologically assimilated words does not depend on spe-
cific language experience. Cogn Sci 2006, 30:451-479.
13. Wheeldon L, Waksler R: Phonological underspecification and
mapping mechanisms in the speech recognition lexicon.
Brain Lang 2004, 90:401-412.
14. Gumnior H, Zwitserlood P, Bölte J: Assimilation in existing and
novel German compounds. Lang Cognitive Proc 2005, 20:465-488.
15. Weber A: Help or hindrance: How violation of different
assimilation rules affects spoken-language processing. Lang
Speech 2001, 44:94-117.
16. Weber A: Assimilation violation in spoken-language process-
ing: A supplementary report. Lang Speech 2003, 45:37-46.
17. Näätänen R: The perception of speech sounds by the human
brain as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its
magnetic equivalent (MMNm). Psychophysiology 2001, 38:1-21.
18. Eulitz C, Lahiri A: Neurobiological evidence for abstract pho-
nological representations in the mental lexicon during
speech recognition. J Cogn Neurosci 2004, 16:577-583.
19. Kutas M, Hillyard SA: Reading senseless sentences: Brain poten-
tials reflect semantic incongruity. Science 1980, 207:203-205.
20. Kutas M, Federmeier K: Electrophysiology reveals semantic
memory use in language comprehension. Trends Cogn Sci 2000,
4:463-470.
21. Attias J, Pratt H: Auditory event related potentials during lexi-
cal categorization in the oddball paradigm. Brain Lang 1992,
4:230-239.
22. Bentin S: Event-related potentials, semantic processes, and
expectancy factors in word recognition. Brain Lang 1987,
31:308-327.
23. Picton TW, Hillyard SA: Endogenous event-related potentials.
In Human Event-related Potentials. EEG-Handbook Volume 3. Edited by:
Picton TW. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1988:361-426.
24. Soares I, Collet L, Duclaux R: Electrophysiological correlates of
auditory lexical decision: An attempt to test the 'Cohort
model'. Int J Neurosci 1991, 57:111-122.
25. Supp GG, Schlögl A, Gunter TC, Bernard M, Pfurtschneller G, Petsche
H: Lexical memory search during N400: Cortical couplings in
auditory comprehension. NeuroReport 2004, 15:1209-1213.
26. Helenius P, Salmelin R, Service E, Connolly JF: Semantic cortical
activation in dyslexic readers. J Cogn Neurosci 1999, 11:535-550.
27. Simos PG, Basile LF, Papnicolaou AC: Source localization of the
N400 response in a sentence-reading paradigm using evoked
magnetic fields and magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Res
1997, 762:29-39.
28. Nobre AC, McCarthy G: Language-related ERPs: Scalp distri-
butions and modulation by word type and semantic priming.
J Cogn Neurosci 1994, 6:233-255.
29. Hagoort P, Brown CM: ERP effects of listening to speech:
Semantic ERP effects. Neuropsychologia 2000, 38:1518-1530.
30. van den Brink D, Brown CM, Hagoort P: Electrophysiological evi-
dence for early contextual influences during spoken-word
recognition: N200 versus N400 effects. J Cogn Neurosci 2001,
13:967-985.
31. van den Brink D, Brown CM, Hagoort P: The cascaded nature of
lexical selection and integration in auditory sentence
processing. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2006, 32:364-372.
32. Van Petten C, Coulson S, Rubin S, Plante E, Parks M: Time course
of word identification and semantic integration in spoken
language. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 1999, 25:394-417.
33. O'Rourke TB, Holcomb PJ: Electrophysiological evidence for
the efficiency of spoken word processing. Biol Psychol 2002,
60:121-150.
34. Oldfield RC: The assessment and analysis of handedness: the
Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971, 9:97-113.
35. [http://wortschatz.uni-leipzig.de
].
36. Holcomb PJ, Grainger J, O'Rourke T: An electrophysiological
study of the effects of orthographic neighborhood size on
printed word perception. J Cogn Neurosci 2002, 14:938-950.
37. Baayen H, Piepenbrock R, Van Rijn H: The CELEX lexical data-
base (CD-ROM). Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania, Lin-
guistic Data Consortium; 1993.
38. Vitevitch MS, Luce PA: Probabilistic phonotactics and neighbor-
hood activation in spoken word recognition. J Mem Lang 1999,
40:374-408.
39. Holcomb PJ, Neville HJ: Auditory and visual semantic process-
ing: A comparison using event-related brain potentials. Lang
Cognitive Proc 1990, 5:281-312.
40. Marslen-Wilson W, Warren P: Levels of perceptual representa-
tion and process in lexical access: Words, phonemes and fea-
tures. Psychol Rev 1994, 101:653-675.
41. Goldinger SD: Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical
access. Psychol Rev 1998, 105:251-279.
42. McClelland JL, Elman JL: The TRACE model of speech percep-
tion. Cognit Psychol 1986, 18:1-86.
43. Norris D: Shortlist: A connectionist model of continuous
speech recognition. Cognition 1994, 52:189-234.
Publish with BioMed Central and every
scientist can read your work free of charge
"BioMed Central will be the most significant development for
disseminating the results of biomedical researc h in our lifetime."
Sir Paul Nurse, Cancer Research UK
Your research papers will be:
available free of charge to the entire biomedical community
peer reviewed and published immediately upon acceptance
cited in PubMed and archived on PubMed Central
yours — you keep the copyright
Submit your manuscript here:
http://www.biomedcentral.com/info/publishing_adv.asp
BioMedcentral
Behavioral and Brain Functions 2006, 2:36 http://www.behavioralandbrainfunctions.com/content/2/1/36
Page 10 of 10
(page number not for citation purposes)
44. Luce PA, Pisoni D: Recognizing spoken words: The neighbor-
hood activation model. Ear Hear 1998, 19:1-36.
45. Obleser J, Eulitz C, Lahiri A: Magnetic brain response mirrors
extraction of phonological features from spoken vowels. J
Cogn Neurosci 2004, 16:31-39.
46. Obleser J, Elbert T, Lahiri A, Eulitz C: Cortical representation of
vowels reflects acoustic dissimilarity determined by formant
frequencies. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2003, 15:207-213.
47. Obleser J, Lahiri A, Eulitz C: Auditory-evoked magnetic field
codes place of articulation in timing and topography around
100 milliseconds post syllable onset. Neuroimage 2003,
20:1839-1847.
... As reviewed by Pylkkänen and Marantz (2003), the N400 should be decomposed into several functionally distinct subcomponents. In accordance with this view, to better capture the dynamics of word recognition processes, especially in the spoken modality where speech signal unfolds over time, some studies proposed a more detailed analysis of this component by separating it into two phases (Friedrich et al., 2006;Helenius et al., 2009;Kocagoncu et al., 2016;O'Rourke & Holcomb, 2002). The initial phase is thought to reflect the access to lexical representations based on the parallel activation of the phonological and semantic representations of the activated candidates. ...
... The later phase of the N400m component (from 400 msec onward) was sensitive to stimulus lexicality, with long-lasting and more left-lateralized activation for pseudowords compared to words in the three populations. This observation replicated existing findings in skilled readers (Friedrich et al., 2006;O'Rourke & Holcomb, 2002). Although no behavioral task performance was reported, MEG signals showed that the magnitude of the lexicality effect tended to decrease linearly from skilled readers to participants with dyslexia and SLI. ...
... The prelexical stage was examined through the N100 and P200 components, which reflect analyses of acoustic and phonetic information (Wagner et al., 2016). Then, as either done or recommended by previous studies (Friedrich et al., 2006;Helenius et al., 2009;Kocagoncu et al., 2016;O'Rourke & Holcomb, 2002;Pylkkänen & Marantz, 2003;Winsler et al., 2018), the time-window that follows these early auditory ERPs, most likely presenting the N400 component, was segmented into different time-windows to capture the dynamics of the core stage of spoken word (and pseudoword) recognition processes that may differ in the two populations. As described below, time-window segmentation was conducted in a data-driven manner without being contaminated by the effects of interest, and it allowed us to separate the time-window from the onset of N400 to the end of the epoch into three parts. ...
Article
Purpose: In adults with dyslexia (DYS), the persistent influence of phonological deficits on spoken language processing has mainly been examined in either perceptual tasks or those tapping complex cognitive operations. Much less attention is devoted to spoken word recognition per se. Our study aimed to fill this gap. Method: Adults with and without dyslexia (for both groups: N = 30, mean age = 21 years, 50% female, 100% white European) performed an auditory lexical decision task. Performance and ERP were recorded. Results: Reaction times showed a lexicality effect in both groups although they differed in ERP responses to stimulus lexicality. Skilled readers showed the typical amplitude enhancement for pseudowords compared to words in a late phase of N400 (414-581msec) whereas DYS showed the opposite pattern in an earlier phase of N400 (246-413msec). Both groups showed a stronger negativity during pseudowords processing in the late post- lexical stage (582-800msec). Conclusions: ERP data showed subtle differences between the two populations during the lexical stage of word recognition despite their comparable behavioral outcomes. We hypothesized that a stronger reliance on intact semantic knowledge might contribute to the general enhanced and sustained ERP responses to words in DYS across different phases of lexical processing, although confirmation is needed.
... In this context, the degree and ease of lexical access are commonly assessed using accuracy metrics, reaction times or Event-Related Potentials (ERPs). Results of several studies converge on the notion that words are recognized with higher accuracy and shorter reaction times than pseudowords, irrespective of modality (e.g., Barber et al., 2013;Friedrich et al., 2006;Krause et al., 2006;Mosca & de Bot, 2017). Furthermore, most of the electroencephalographic (EEG) studies using lexical decision tasks reported a more negative deflection of the N400 component for pseudowords in comparison to words (i.e., Barber et al., 2013;Bien et al., 2014;Friedrich et al., 2006;L opez Zunini et al., 2020) and to lowfrequency compared to high-frequency words (i.e., Brysbaert et al., 2018;Monsell et al., 1989). ...
... Results of several studies converge on the notion that words are recognized with higher accuracy and shorter reaction times than pseudowords, irrespective of modality (e.g., Barber et al., 2013;Friedrich et al., 2006;Krause et al., 2006;Mosca & de Bot, 2017). Furthermore, most of the electroencephalographic (EEG) studies using lexical decision tasks reported a more negative deflection of the N400 component for pseudowords in comparison to words (i.e., Barber et al., 2013;Bien et al., 2014;Friedrich et al., 2006;L opez Zunini et al., 2020) and to lowfrequency compared to high-frequency words (i.e., Brysbaert et al., 2018;Monsell et al., 1989). The N400 component is commonly also more pronounced in switch compared to non-switch trials (Hut et al., 2017;van der Meij et al., 2011), especially at parietal electrodes (van der Meij et al., 2011). ...
... In the present study, we combined behavioural and EEG measurements to assess lexical access and code switching in multilinguals with different degrees of expertise. In line with previous studies, we were able to replicate the lexical status effect, which was reflected by more accurate and faster responses as well as smaller N400 amplitudes to words compared to pseudowords (Chwilla et al., 1995;Friedrich et al., 2006;Holcomb & Neville, 1990;Hut et al., 2017;L opez Zunini et al., 2020;Rodriguez-Fornells et al., 2002). These results corroborate previous findings and support the notion that words are processed easier than pseudowords because they can be directly mapped onto the mental lexicon, whereas the processing of pseudowords requires a deeper lexical search and a greater computational capacity at the neural level (Friedrich et al., 2006;Specht et al., 2003). ...
Article
Full-text available
With the worldwide increase in people speaking more than one language, a better understanding of the behavioral and neural mechanisms governing lexical selection, lexical access in multiple languages, and code-switching has attracted widespread interest from several disciplines. Previous studies documented higher costs when processing a non-native (L2) than a native (L1) language or when switching from L2 to L1. However, studies on auditory language reception are still scarce and did not take into account the degree of switching experience. Accordingly, in the present study, we combined behavioral and electrophysiological measurements to assess lexical access in L1 and L2 as well as code-switching in professional simultaneous interpreters, trainee interpreters, foreign language teachers, and Anglistics students while the participants performed a bilingual auditory lexical decision task. The purpose of this study was to expand the knowledge on code-switching in auditory language processing and examine whether the degree of simultaneous interpretation experience might reduce switching costs. As a main result, we revealed that L2 compared to L1 trials, as well as switch compared to non-switch trials generally resulted in lower accuracies, longer reaction times, and increased N400 amplitudes in all groups of participants. Otherwise, we did not reveal any influence of switching direction and interpretation expertise on N400 parameters. Taken together, these results suggest that a late age of L2 acquisition leads to switching costs, irrespective of proficiency level. Furthermore, we provided first evidence that simultaneous interpretation training does not diminish switching costs, at least when focusing on lexical access.
... Several researchers proposed that the smaller N400 reflected a greater degree of semantic activation. And integrating words into semantic context depends on the lexico-semantic memory activation of words stored in the mental lexicon (Friedrich et al., 2006;Federmeier, 2000, 2011;Lau et al., 2008). The present study provided ERP evidence to support this view, as reflected by the larger N400 amplitudes for the pseudowords than for the canonical and transposed words, which reflected that greater demands on a lexico-semantic memory were needed to search for pseudowords due to the lack of lexical representation. ...
Article
The present study investigated the electrophysiological correlates of morpheme transposition in two-character Chinese compound words (canonical words and transposed words) and pseudowords at a very short stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) of 83 ms, employing a dual-target rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task. Event-related potential (ERP) results showed that, relative to pseudowords, canonical words elicited increased positivity or decreased negativity in ERP amplitudes beginning with the 200–300 ms (P200) and continuing through the 300–450 ms (N400) into the late time window of 450–600 ms (late positive component, LPC). Critically, the morpheme transposition effects were found on the N400 component and LPC, with larger N400 and smaller LPC amplitudes in the transposed words than in the canonical words. Taken together, these results demonstrated that morpheme transposition hindered the semantic extraction and combinatorial processing of the whole word entities in very rapid succession, as reflected by the modulations of N400 and LPC.
... 13 September 2021 | Volume 12 | Article 679278 not all be processed in the same way. For example, studies suggest that the coronal place of articulation for consonants is not specified and that it can vary freely for coronal consonants (Friedrich et al., 2006;Lahiri and Reetz, 2010;Roberts et al., 2013). We used the coronal sounds /t/ and /d/ in our consonant minimal pairs, contrasted with other plosives differing in place of articulation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Language comprehension in noise can sometimes lead to mishearing, due to the noise disrupting the speech signal. Some of the difficulties in dealing with the noisy signal can be alleviated by drawing on the context – indeed, top-down predictability has shown to facilitate speech comprehension in noise. Previous studies have furthermore shown that strong reliance on the top-down predictions can lead to increased rates of mishearing, especially in older adults, which are attributed to general deficits in cognitive control in older adults. We here propose that the observed mishearing may be a simple consequence of rational language processing in noise. It should not be related to failure on the side of the older comprehenders, but instead would be predicted by rational processing accounts. To test this hypothesis, we extend earlier studies by running an online listening experiment with younger and older adults, carefully controlling the target and direct competitor in our stimuli. We show that mishearing is directly related to the perceptibility of the signal. We furthermore add an analysis of wrong responses, which shows that results are at odds with the idea that participants overly strongly rely on context in this task, as most false answers are indeed close to the speech signal, and not to the semantics of the context.
... Our research shows that differences can be found in earlier components as well. The earlier onset of the present effect may be due to the high level of psycholinguistic and physical matching between the stimuli which allowed registering earlier transient differences that may otherwise become smeared for more varied stimulus sets [55][56][57]. ...
Article
Full-text available
and concrete words differ in their cognitive and neuronal underpinnings, but the exact mechanisms underlying these distinctions are unclear. We investigated differences between these two semantic types by analysing brain responses to newly learnt words with fully controlled psycholinguistic properties. Experimental participants learned 20 novel abstract and concrete words in the context of short stories. After the learning session, event-related potentials (ERPs) to newly learned items were recorded, and acquisition outcomes were assessed behaviourally in a range of lexical and semantic tasks. Behavioural results showed better performance on newly learnt abstract words in lexical tasks, whereas semantic assessments showed a tendency for higher accuracy for concrete words. ERPs to novel abstract and concrete concepts differed early on, ~150 ms after the word onset. Moreover, differences between novel words and control untrained pseudowords were observed earlier for concrete (~150 ms) than for abstract (~200 ms) words. Distributed source analysis indicated bilateral temporo-parietal activation underpinning newly established memory traces, suggesting a crucial role of Wernicke’s area and its right-hemispheric homologue in word acquisition. In sum, we report behavioural and neurophysiological processing differences between concrete and abstract words evident immediately after their controlled acquisition, confirming distinct neurocognitive mechanisms underpinning these types of semantics.
... This was done to have an indication of whether the response-locked ERP waveform (reflecting error processing) overlapped with lexical processing. We repeated the analysis from the deviation point of the Pseudowords in the present paradigm (Friedrich et al., 2006). For a comparison with correct responses, the deviation point from the Pseudowords was then matched to an arbitrary deviation point 200 ms before the sound offset in Words. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates the error processing components in the EEG signal of Performers and Observers using an auditory lexical decision task, in which participants heard spoken items and decided for each item if it was a real word or not. Pairs of participants were tested in both the role of the Performer and the Observer. In the literature, an Error Related Negativity-Error Positivity complex has been identified for performed (ERN-Pe) and observed (oERN-oPe) errors. While these effects have been widely studied for performance errors in speeded decision tasks relying on visual input, relatively little is known about the performance monitoring signatures in observed language processing based on auditory input. In the lexical decision task, native Dutch speakers listened to real Dutch Words, Non-Words, and crucially, long Pseudowords that resembled words until the final syllable and were shown to be error-prone in a pilot study, because they were responded to too soon. We hypothesised that the errors in the task would result in a response locked ERN-Pe pattern both for the Performer and for the Observer. Our hypothesis regarding the ERN was not supported, however a Pe-like effect, as well as a P300 were present. Analyses to disentangle lexical and error processing similarly indicated a P300 for errors, and the results furthermore pointed to differences between responses before and after word offset. The findings are interpreted as marking attention during error processing during auditory word recognition.
Article
How does language background influence the neural correlates of visual word recognition in children? To address this question, we used an ERP lexical decision task to examine first-language (L1) and second-language (L2) visual word processing in monolingual and bilingual school-aged children and young adults (n = 123). In particular, we focused on the effects of word frequency (an index of lexical accessibility) on RTs and the N400 ERP component. Behaviorally, we found larger L1 versus L2 word frequency effects among bilingual children, driven by faster and more accurate responses to higher-frequency words (no other language or age group differences were observed). Neurophysiologically, we found larger L1 word frequency effects in bilinguals versus monolinguals (across both age groups), reflected in more negative ERP amplitudes to lower-frequency words. However, the bilingual groups processed L1 and L2 words similarly, despite lower levels of subjective and objective L2 proficiency. Taken together, our findings suggest that divided L1 experience (but not L2 experience) influences the neural correlates of visual word recognition across childhood and adulthood.
Article
The neuroanatomical correlates of basic semantic composition have been investigated in previous neuroimaging and lesion studies, but research on the electrophysiology of the involved processes is scarce. A large literature on sentence-level event-related potentials (ERPs) during semantic processing has identified at least two relevant components – the N400 and the P600. Other studies demonstrated that these components are reduced and/or delayed in people with aphasia (PWA). However, it remains to be shown if these findings generalize beyond the sentence level. Specifically, it is an open question if an alteration in ERP responses in PWA can also be observed during basic semantic composition, providing a potential future diagnostic tool. The present study aimed to elucidate the electrophysiological dynamics of basic semantic composition in a group of post-stroke PWA. We included 20 PWA and 20 age-matched controls (mean age 58 years) and measured ERP responses while they performed a plausibility judgment task on two-word phrases that were either meaningful (“anxious horse”), anomalous (“anxious wood”) or had the noun replaced by a pseudoword (“anxious gufel”). The N400 effect for anomalous versus meaningful phrases was similar in both groups. In contrast, unlike the control group, PWA did not show an N400 effect between pseudoword and meaningful phrases. Moreover, both groups exhibited a parietal P600 effect towards pseudoword phrases, while PWA showed an additional P600 over frontal electrodes. Finally, PWA showed an inverse correlation between the magnitude of the N400 and P600 effects: PWA exhibiting no or even reversed N400 effects towards anomalous and pseudoword phrases showed a stronger P600 effect. These results may reflect a compensatory mechanism which allows PWA to arrive at the correct interpretation of the phrase. When compositional processing capacities are impaired in the early N400 time-window, PWA may make use of a more elaborate re-analysis process reflected in the P600.
Article
The experimental study of artificial language learning has become a widely used means of investigating the predictions of theories of language learning and representation. Although much is now known about the generalizations that learners make from various kinds of data, relatively little is known about how those representations affect speech processing. This paper presents an event-related potential (ERP) study of brain responses to violations of lab-learned phonotactics. Novel words that violated a learned phonotactic constraint elicited a larger Late Positive Component (LPC) than novel words that satisfied it. Similar LPCs have been found for violations of natively acquired linguistic structure, as well as for violations of other types of abstract generalizations, such as musical structure. We argue that lab-learned phonotactic generalizations are represented abstractly and affect the evaluation of speech in a manner that is similar to natively acquired syntactic and phonological rules.
Article
Full-text available
The N400 ERP component is a direct neural index of word meaning. Studies show that the N400 component is already present in early infancy, albeit often delayed. Many researchers capitalize on this finding, using the N400 component to better understand how early language acquisition unfolds. However, variability in how researchers quantify the N400 makes it difficult to set clear predictions or build theory. Not much is known about how the N400 component develops in the first 2 years of life in terms of its latency and topographical distributions, nor do we know how task parameters affect its appearance. In the current paper we carry out a systematic review, comparing over 30 studies that report the N400 component as a proxy of semantic processing elicited in infants between 0 and 24 months old who listened to linguistic stimuli. Our main finding is that there is large heterogeneity across semantic-priming studies in reported characteristics of the N400, both with respect to latency and to distributions. With age, the onset of the N400 insignificantly decreases, while its offset slightly increases. We also examined whether the N400 appears different for recently-acquired novel words vs. existing words: both situations reveal heterogeneity across studies. Finally, we inspected whether the N400 was modulated differently with studies using a between-subject design. In infants with more proficient language skills the N400 was more often present or showed itself here with earlier latency, compared to their peers; but no consistent patterns were observed for distribution characteristics of the N400. One limitation of the current review is that we compared studies that widely differed in choice of EEG recordings, pre-processing steps and quantification of the N400, all of which could affect the characteristics of the infant N400. The field is still missing research that systematically tests development of the N400 using the same paradigm across infancy.
Chapter
Full-text available
Auditory event-related potentials are electrical changes recorded from the brain in association with auditory stimuli.
Article
Full-text available
Since its discovery by Naatanen and colleagues in 1978, the mismatch negativity (MMN) has been used as an index of auditory sensory memory. The present paper explicates various possibilities of how MMN can assess memory functions, it reveals possible traps when interpreting MMN as an index of auditory memory, and it reviews recent developments of paradigms showing that memory on a short time-scale, consolidation of memory traces, and even implicit memory can be probed with MMN. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to determine how potential lexical ambiguity produced by place assimilation is resolved. Four cross-modal form priming experiments using primes in sentential contexts were performed. In the first 2, prime items had underlyingly coronal offsets (e.g., right) with assimilated noncoronal place. The primes were judged to be perceptually ambiguous (between right and ripe) in Experiment 1 and noncoronal (ripe) in Experiment 2 in off-line testing. In Experiment 3, primes were replaced with corresponding underlyingly noncoronal items (ripe). In all 3 experiments, participants showed selective priming for the underlying form of the prime. A 4th form priming experiment using the gated tokens of priming stimuli used in Experiment 2 examined the role of postlexical context on this process. In this experiment, participants showed priming for both underlying and surface forms of the prime. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Recent work (Vitevitch & Luce, 1998) investigating the role of phonotactic information in spoken word recognition suggests the operation of two levels of representation, each having distinctly different consequences for processing. The lexical level is marked by competitive effects associated with similarity neighborhood activation, whereas increased probabilities of segments and sequences of segments facilitate processing at the sublexical level. We investigated the two proposed levels in six experiments using monosyllabic and specially constructed bisyllabic words and nonwords. The results of these studies provide further support for the hypothesis that the processing of spoken stimuli is a function of both facilitatory effects associated with increased phonotactic probabilities and competitive effects associated with the activation of similarity neighborhoods. We interpret these findings in the context of Grossberg, Boardman, and Cohen's (1997) adaptive resonance theory of speech perception.
Article
Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded from the scalp to investigate the processing of word stimuli. Three tasks were used: (1) a task comparing words that provided an anomalous or normal sentence ending, (2) a word-list task in which different word types were examined, and (3) a word-list task in which semantic priming was examined. ERPs were recorded from a 50-channel montage in an attempt to dissociate overlapping ERP features by their scalp distributions. The focus of these studies was the N400, an ERP previously associated with language processing (Kutas & Hillyard, 1980). The temporal interval typically associated with N400 (250500 msec) was found to contain overlapping ERP features. Two of these features were common to both sentence and word-list tasksbut one appeared different. Anomalous sentence endings and words with semantic content in lists both showed coincident negative left frontotemporal and midline-anterior ERP foci, peaking at 332 msec for sentences and 316 msec for word lists. The most negative voltage obtained in the sentence task peaked at 386 msec and had a midline-posterior focus. A right frontotemporal focus developed after the midline-posterior focus and outlasted its duration. The most negative voltage for content words in lists was reached at 364 msec. The distribution of this ERP was extensive over the midline and appeared to differ from that observed in the sentence task. Modulation of language-related ERPs by word type and semantic priming was investigated using the word-list tasks, which required category-detection responses. Two novel findings were obtained: (1) The ERP distributions for words serving grammatical function and content words differed substantially in word lists. Even when devoid of any sentence context, function words presented significantly attenuated measures of N400 compared to content words. These findings support hypotheses that suggest a differential processing of content and function words. (2) Semantic priming functionally dissociated two ERP features in the 250500 msec range. The later and most negative midline ERP feature (peaking at 364 msec) was attenuated by semantic priming. However, the earlier left frontotemporal feature (peaking at 316 msec) was enhanced by semantic priming. The isolation of this novel language-related ERF' that is sensitive to semantic manipulations has important consequences for temporal and mechanistic aspects of theories of language processing.
Article
Place assimilation is a phonologically regular variation that modifies the surface form of words: Coronal consonants can change their place of articulation (e.g., rainbow←raimbow). These variations do not seem to disturb lexical access as long as the change is licensed by the following phonemic context (Coenen, Zwitserlood, & Bölte, 2001; Gaskell & Marslen-Wilson, 1996). This study compares the listeners’ tolerance to contextually appropriate () and inappropriate changes (bahmgleis) in existing (bahngleis; railway track) and novel German compounds (bahngeist; railway ghost). The results from two cross-modal form priming experiments reveal that listeners showed equal tolerance for contextually appropriate changes that actually occur in language and for inappropriate changes that are never produced in the context presented. This pattern held for existing and novel compounds. Our findings suggest that word form representations for possible phonemic variants of morphologically complex words are not listed in the lexicon.
Article
This study compared and contrasted semantic priming in the visual and auditory modalities using event-related brain potentials (ERPs) and behavioural measures (errors and reaction time). Subjects participated in two runs (one visual, one auditory) of a lexical decision task where stimuli were word pairs consisting of “prime” words followed by equal numbers of words semantically related to the primes, words unrelated to the primes, pseudo-words, and nonwords. Subjects made slower responses, made more errors, and their ERPs had larger negative components (N400) to unrelated words than to related words in both modalities. However, the ERP priming effect began earlier, was larger in size, and lasted longer in the auditory modality than in the visual modality. In addition, the lateral distribution of N400 over the scalp differed in the two modalities. It is suggested that there may be overlap in the priming processes that occur in each modality but that these processes are not identical. The results also demonstrated that the N400 component may be specifically responsive to language or potential language events.
Article
The consequences of surface variation in speech on lexical access have recently received considerable attention. The lexical system is intolerant to mismatch between input and lexical representation, but an exception is phonologically regular variation. One example is assimilation of consonants that adopt the place of articulation of adjacent consonants in fast speech. Data are presented from crossmodal form priming experiments in German on regressive and progressive assimilation at word boundaries. The results show that some, but not all forms of lawful variation are tolerated by the lexical system. The consequences of these findings for psycholinguistic and linguistic models, some of which incorporate explanations for regular variation, are discussed.