ArticlePDF Available

The coordination patterns observed when two hands reach-to-grasp separate objects

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

What determines coordination patterns when both hands reach to grasp separate objects at the same time? It is known that synchronous timing is preferred as the most stable mode of bimanual coordination. Nonetheless, normal unimanual prehension behaviour predicts asynchrony when the two hands reach towards unequal targets, with synchrony restricted to targets equal in size and distance. Additionally, sufficiently separated targets require sequential looking. Does synchrony occur in all cases because it is preferred in bimanual coordination or does asynchrony occur because of unimanual task constraints and the need for sequential looking? We investigated coordinative timing when participants (n = 8) moved their right (preferred) hand to the same object at a fixed distance but the left hand to objects of different width (3, 5, and 7 cm) and grip surface size (1, 2, and 3 cm) placed at different distances (20, 30, and 40 cm) over 270 randomised trials. The hand movements consisted of two components: (1) an initial component (IC) during which the hand reached towards the target while forming an appropriate grip aperture, stopping at (but not touching) the object; (2) a completion component (CC) during which the finger and thumb closed on the target. The two limbs started the IC together but did not interact until the deceleration phase when evidence of synchronisation began to appear. Nonetheless, asynchronous timing was present at the end of the IC and preserved through the CC even with equidistant targets. Thus, there was synchrony but requirements for visual information ultimately yielded asynchronous coordinative timing.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Exp Brain Res (2008) 184:283–293
DOI 10.1007/s00221-007-1107-9
123
RESEARCH ARTICLE
The coordination patterns observed when two hands
reach-to-grasp separate objects
GeoVrey P. Bingham · Kirstie Hughes ·
Mark Mon-Williams
Received: 23 May 2007 / Accepted: 23 July 2007 / Published online: 29 August 2007
© Springer-Verlag 2007
Abstract What determines coordination patterns when
both hands reach to grasp separate objects at the same time?
It is known that synchronous timing is preferred as the most
stable mode of bimanual coordination. Nonetheless, normal
unimanual prehension behaviour predicts asynchrony when
the two hands reach towards unequal targets, with syn-
chrony restricted to targets equal in size and distance. Addi-
tionally, suYciently separated targets require sequential
looking. Does synchrony occur in all cases because it is
preferred in bimanual coordination or does asynchrony
occur because of unimanual task constraints and the need
for sequential looking? We investigated coordinative tim-
ing when participants (n = 8) moved their right (preferred)
hand to the same object at a Wxed distance but the left hand
to objects of diVerent width (3, 5, and 7 cm) and grip sur-
face size (1, 2, and 3 cm) placed at diVerent distances (20,
30, and 40 cm) over 270 randomised trials. The hand move-
ments consisted of two components: (1) an initial compo-
nent (IC) during which the hand reached towards the target
while forming an appropriate grip aperture, stopping at (but
not touching) the object; (2) a completion component (CC)
during which the Wnger and thumb closed on the target. The
two limbs started the IC together but did not interact until
the deceleration phase when evidence of synchronisation
began to appear. Nonetheless, asynchronous timing was
present at the end of the IC and preserved through the CC
even with equidistant targets. Thus, there was synchrony
but requirements for visual information ultimately yielded
asynchronous coordinative timing.
Keywords Bimanual · Prehension · Movement ·
Coordination · Attention
Introduction
It is a matter of common observation that adult humans are
skilled at coordinating the right and left hands when reach-
ing to grasp two separate objects at the same time. The
question is whether the two hands are synchronised in time
during such reaches? There has been little investigation into
how the nervous system coordinates the actions of the two
limbs in bimanual prehension. Those studies that explored
bimanual prehension tended to investigate the two hands
reaching to a single object (e.g. Castiello et al. 1993; Tresil-
ian and Stelmach 1997). Jackson et al. (1999) conducted
one of the few studies into bimanual prehension directed at
two separated objects. Jackson et al. (1999) found that
bimanual movements took longer but were otherwise
unaVected as to whether the actions required of each hand
were the same or diVerent. Jackson et al. suggested that
synchronising the limbs to a Wxed duration solves the prob-
lem of executing incongruent bimanual movements. Jack-
son et al.’s study has made an important contribution to our
understanding of bimanual prehension but it leaves some
issues unresolved. In particular, it remains unclear as to
how the nervous system produces coordinated movements
to targets of diVerent sizes at diVerent distances.
The results of previous bimanual coordination studies
involving tasks other than prehension suggest that the
G. P. Bingham
Department of Psychology, Indiana University,
Bloomington, IN 47405, USA
e-mail: gbingham@indiana.edu
K. Hughes · M. Mon-Williams (&)
College of Medicine and Life Sciences,
University of Aberdeen,
Aberdeen AB24 2UB, Scotland, UK
e-mail: mon@abdn.ac.uk
284 Exp Brain Res (2008) 184:283–293
123
hands often interact and show synchronous coordination,
that is, each hand doing a similar thing at the same time
(e.g. for rhythmic movement see Kelso et al. 1979a,b; for
discrete movements see Diedrichsen et al. 2001). The most
widely studied bimanual coordination tasks involve rhyth-
mic movement of diVerent limbs, for instance, the left and
right wrists. Such movements interact to produce character-
istic stable modes of coordination with synchronous move-
ment being the most stable (see, for instance, Kelso 1995).
The coupling between the two movements is often based on
visual information as veriWed in studies where two diVerent
people each move a joint rhythmically and coordinate
movement (e.g. Schmidt et al. 1990). Subsequently, a large
number of studies have established the role of perception in
coupling the limbs in bimanual coordination (Bingham
2001, 2004 a, b; Bingham et al. 1999; Bingham et al. 2000;
Liao and Jagacinski 2000; Mechsner et al. 2001; Schmidt
et al. 1990; Serrien et al. 2001; Wilson and Bingham 2005
a, b; Wilson et al. 2003; Wimmers et al. 1992). Indeed,
Bingham (2001, 2004a,b) has attributed the characteristic
stable modes of bimanual rhythmic movement to percep-
tual information variables hypothesized to couple such
movements (see also Wilson and Bingham 2005 a, b).
The study of rhythmic movement is informative but it is
also important to study how people move the two limbs
together when carrying out a wide range of everyday ‘dis-
crete’ bimanual tasks. Kelso et al. (1979 a, b) and Diedrich-
sen et al. (2001) both studied bimanual reaching (‘aiming’)
movements and found that the left and right limbs inter-
acted and showed synchronous timing. We wished to inves-
tigate as to whether the two limbs interact in the same way
when bimanual reach-to-grasp movements are made to two
spatially separate target objects. The work of Kelso (1979
a, b) and others led us to expect that the two hands would
start moving together (and end in reasonable temporal
proximity) but we predicted asynchronous coordinative
timing for two reasons. First, the timing of uni-manual
reach-to-grasp movements varies reliably with the distance
of the target as well as with its grasping surface area (i.e.,
reaches to targets that are more distant or smaller require a
longer time to complete, e.g. Jeannerod 1984, 1988).
Bimanual reaches to targets diVerent in size and/or distance
should thus be expected to yield asynchronous timing. The
second reason we expected asynchronous timing is because
precision prehension requires vision to guide reaches but it
is not possible to Wxate two objects simultaneously when
objects are suYciently separated. Asynchronous coordina-
tion would be expected to allow time to look from one
object to the other while guiding the hands. We will con-
sider these two reasons in some depth in the subsequent
paragraphs.
Prehension exhibits regularities that should be expected
to introduce asymmetries in the context of bimanual coordi-
nation. The pattern of behaviour observed in unimanual
prehension experiments is highly stereotypical across and
within participants (Haggard and Wing 1995; Paulignan
et al. 1991; Jackson et al. 1997). If a precision grip (Napier
1956) is used, as the hand starts forward, the thumb and
Wnger move apart to form a maximum grip aperture that is
scaled relative to the size of the target object (e.g. Jeann-
erod 1984, 1988; Smeets and Brenner 1999). The hand
accelerates to a peak speed and then decelerates as the hand
approaches the target. The movement of the hand exhibits a
smooth ‘bell-shaped’ speed proWle with a relatively longer
deceleration phase. The maximum grip aperture occurs dur-
ing the deceleration phase when closing of the aperture is
initiated (Jeannerod 1984, 1988; Smeets and Brenner 1999;
Mon-Williams and Tresilian 2001). The reach movement
ends at the object with the thumb and Wnger closing to
Wnally contact and acquire the target object (Wing and Fra-
ser 1983). A larger target distance yields a larger peak
speed, but not large enough to prevent longer movement
durations (i.e. movement duration increases with reach dis-
tance). Increasing the accuracy demands of the task by
decreasing the grip surface size also produces longer dura-
tion movements. A smaller sized target at a given distance
will yield a slower movement (e.g. Bootsma et al. 1994;
Tresilian and Stelmach 1997; Tresilian et al. 1997;
Mon-Williams and Tresilian 2001). Likewise, the grasp
component shows stereotypical patterns of behaviour. The
maximum grip aperture is sensitive to the size of the object
so that the aperture is a little wider than the width of the
target object and occurs later in the movement for larger
objects (Jeannerod 1984, 1988; Smeets and Brenner 1999;
Mon-Williams and Tresilian 2001). It can be seen that
synchronous prehensile coordination to targets of diVerent
sizes and at diVerent distances would entail changes in the
regular patterns of prehension.
Nevertheless, even if bimanual prehension were per-
formed to separate targets of equal distance and size, we
would expect to Wnd evidence of asynchronous behaviour.
The reason is that targeted prehension requires visual guid-
ance for accurate performance (Fisk and Goodale 1988).
This is why prehension movements to targets with a smaller
grip area have a longer duration. That is, prehension move-
ments require online correction of errors under visual guid-
ance and thus exhibit the speed-accuracy trade-oV typical
of visually guided actions (e.g. Bootsma et al. 1994; Fitts
1954; Wing et al. 1986). In bimanual reaching-to-grasp, the
two targets cannot be Wxated at the same time when they
are spatially separate (see Riek et al. 2003). Accurate per-
formance under these conditions requires a sequential orga-
nisation that allows the two targets to be Wxated in turn to
guide the corresponding prehension. Indeed, Riek et al.
(2003) found ‘hovering’ behaviour in bimanual targeted
reaching (aiming) movements where one hand would hover
Exp Brain Res (2008) 184:283–293 285
123
and wait while the other hand was targeted (in that study,
targets were contacted by a probe and not grasped). It fol-
lows that any noise in the execution of the organised
sequence will result in asynchronous coordination—and
noise is an intrinsic feature of such actions (hence the need
for visually guided corrections in the Wrst place).
In the current study, we investigated the organisation
and timing of bimanual reaching-to-grasp. First, we were
interested in investigating reaches to targets at identical dis-
tances. This condition was predicted to yield the same
arrival times for the two hands. Nevertheless, we expected
to see asynchrony reXecting the demands of visually guid-
ing two hands in the Wnal stages of the movements. All else
being equal, we could not predict which hand would close
Wrst in asynchronous trials. We also manipulated the diY-
culty of the task. With greater diYculty, we expected to see
greater asynchrony. We varied the diYculty of targeting in
two ways. We varied the grip area available for contacting
the object on its sides with Wnger and thumb (1, 2, and
3 cm). Smaller grip area is more diYcult (in Fitts’ 1954
sense of requiring more visually guided corrections). We
also varied the frontoparallel width of the targets (3, 5, and
7 cm). Wider targets present a larger collision hazard (Meu-
lenbroek et al. 2001; Rosenbaum et al. 1999) and thus are
more diYcult. With two targets of unequal diYculty, it was
unclear as to whether one should predict closure on the
more or less diYcult target Wrst. So, while we looked for
ordering eVects, we only predicted increasing asynchrony.
Second, we investigated reaches to targets at diVerent
distances. This condition was predicted to yield diVerent
arrival times for the two hands. Nevertheless, should we
expect to see synchronous arrival reXecting the tendency in
bimanual coordination for synchrony? We manipulated the
target distance for the left hand (20, 30, and 40 cm) while
the distance of the target for the right hand remained con-
stant at the middle distance (30 cm). When the variable tar-
get was either closer or farther than the constant target then
independent reaching behaviour should naturally introduce
asynchrony in arrival times. The question was whether this
asynchrony would occur, and then be maintained during
closure. If so, then the order in which grasping was
achieved between the hands should vary as a function of the
relative target distances (i.e. the nearer target should be
grasped Wrst). We also manipulated the diYculty of grasp-
ing at diVerent distances so that distance would interact
with grip area and object width to determine diYculty.
Once more, we expected greater asynchrony when the diY-
culty of grasping was greater.
The design of the study was such that one hand (called
the “constant hand”) always reached to the same target
object. The other hand (called the “variable hand”) reached
to targets that varied in width, grip surface area and dis-
tance. We studied timing measures of the constant hand in
terms of the levels of object width, grip surface area, and
distance encountered by the variable hand. Variations in the
timing of the constant hand would indicate that its behav-
iour was aVected by the action of the variable hand (i.e. that
there were interactions between the hands). The question
then would be as to whether the interactions (1) would yield
synchronous timing when targets were at diVerent distances
or (2) would yield asynchronous timing when the targets
were at the same distances?
Methods
Eight unpaid participants from the University of Aberdeen
were recruited for the study (Wve females and three males
aged between 20 and 30 years, mean age 24 years). An
additional ten unpaid volunteers participated in a control
experiment (eight females and two males aged between 21
and 26 years). All participants had normal or corrected to
normal vision and none had any history of neurological
deWcit. The participants all reported a right hand preference
and all wrote and threw a ball with their right hand. All par-
ticipants provided their informed consent prior to their
inclusion in the study. The study was approved by a univer-
sity ethics committee and was performed in accordance
with the ethical standards laid down in the Declaration of
Helsinki.
The experimental task required the participants to sit at a
table and reach for two objects (dimensions of objects:
length 9.5 cm; height 3.2 cm; width: varying sizes). Two
separate starting positions were located 10 cm from the
edge of the table closest to the participant. The starting
positions were 45 cm apart equally spaced to the left and
right of the participants’ midline. Participants started each
trial with the left hand on the left-side starting position and
the right hand on the right-side starting position. In the con-
stant (right) hand workspace an object was placed along the
sagittal plane at 30 cm from the starting position. The
block, with a width of 5 cm and with a grip surface of 2 cm
diameter, remained in this position for the entire session
(Fig. 1). In the variable (left) hand workspace, the target
object varied in distance, width and grip surface area. Three
object amplitudes were used: 20, 30, or 40 cm from the
starting position along the sagittal plane. Nine objects were
used. They were composed by crossing three object widths:
3, 5, and 7 cm and three grip surface sizes: 1, 2 or 3 cm
diameter. Each target consisted of a block of wood with a
wooden dowel Wxed in it so that the ends of the dowel
extended to either side of the block by 2 cm (like buttons).
See Fig. 1. The dowel was to be held pinched between
Wnger and thumb each in contact with an end of the dowel.
The block held the dowel suspended about 2 cm above a
table surface. We varied the width of target objects by altering
286 Exp Brain Res (2008) 184:283–293
123
the width of the block in which the dowel was inserted.
Altering the diameter of the dowel varied the grip surface
area. To be clear, the width determined the distance by
which the tips of the index Wnger and thumb were separated
whilst holding the object in the grip. The grip surface area
was the size of each of the surfaces contacted by the thumb
and Wngertip, respectively.
Participants performed ten test trials in each of the 27
diVerent conditions (9 objects £ 3 distances) but Wrst par-
ticipants performed ten practice trials in which the left and
right amplitudes and objects were the same. Following this,
the 270 test trials were completed with the trial order rando-
mised across and between the participants. The entire ses-
sion, including practice trials, lasted »1 h. Participants
were informed that they should grasp the objects as quickly
and accurately as possible between the pads of the foreWn-
ger and thumb and that they should not lift the object oV the
table.
In the control condition, the variable hand reached to all
three distances but only the medium width block was used
and only the narrow and wide grasp surface. This gave a
total of six conditions with participants reaching eight times
(randomised order) in each condition (total = 48 trials). In
the control condition, two IREDs were attached securely to
a head-mounted frame that allowed us to track head posi-
tion. The signal was calibrated by asking participants to
alter Wxation between points on the tabletop by moving
their head. We decided to use head movement to monitor
Wxation as previous research has shown that gaze shifts are
implemented by changes in head position when angular
separation exceeds 20° (Stahl 1999) as was the case in the
current experimental setup.
The acquisition of data was initiated approximately one
second before the experimenter’s verbal start command.
The objects were visible to the participants when the “go
signal was given. Infra red emitting diodes (IREDs) were
attached to the index Wnger (distal medial corner of the
Wnger) thumb (distal lateral corner of the thumb) and
the styloid process of the wrist for each reaching hand. The
positions of the IREDs were recorded by an Optotrak
movement recording system, factory precalibrated to a
static positional resolution of better than 0.2 mm at 250 Hz.
The data were collected for 3 s at 100 Hz and stored for
subsequent oVline analysis and Wltered using a dual-pass
Butterworth second-order Wlter with a cut-oV frequency of
16 Hz (equivalent to a fourth-order zero phase lag Wlter of
10 Hz). The distance between the thumb and index Wnger
IREDs was then computed (the aperture). Following this
operation, the tangential speed of the wrist IRED and the
aperture was computed and the onset and oVset of the
movement were estimated using a standard algorithm
(threshold for movement onset and oVset was 5 cm/s). Cus-
tom analysis routines were used to compute the dependent
variables of interest in this study.
The majority of hand movements were found to consist
naturally of two components: (1) an initial component (IC)
during which the wrist reached towards the target while
forming an appropriate grip aperture, stopping with the
Wngers surrounding (but not touching) the target object; (2)
a completion component (CC) during which the Wnger and
thumb closed on the target. That is, when the wrist had
stopped (ending the reach), the grasp remained to be com-
pleted, that is, the Wngers were not yet in contact with the
target object. A Wnal movement of the Wngers closed the
grasp with W
ngers in contact with the target object surfaces.
The two components were easily identiWed when the kine-
matic proWles were plotted for each trial (see Fig. 2). We
quantiWed these diVerent components and reported the sta-
tistics conducted on these data.
The criterion for onset of a reach was wrist speed exceed-
ing 5 cm/s. The criterion for termination of a reach, marking
the end of the IC (see below), was wrist speed falling below
5 cm/s. The criterion for termination of the grasp closure
and thus, of the CC and of the entire reach-to-grasp move-
ment was when Wnger speed dropped below 5 cm/s. We
measured and analysed nine diVerent dependent variables:
1. Onset Asynchrony (OA): the time between the onset of
the two hands (i.e., the diVerence in when each of the
two hands started moving).
2. Total Task Time (TTT): the total reach-to-grasp time,
deWned as the time taken from the reach onset until the
Fig. 1 Schematic showing layout of the experiment. The right hand
reached to grasp an object of constant width and grasp surface at a Wxed
distance. The left hand reached to grasp objects of three diVeren
t
widths and three diVerent grasp surfaces at one of three diVerent dis-
tances
Exp Brain Res (2008) 184:283–293 287
123
time at which both hands had secured a grasp, that is,
grasp termination.
3. Peak Speed: peak speed reached by the constant (PSc)
and variable (PSv) hand’s wrist.
4. Time to Peak Speed: time to peak speed for the con-
stant (TPSc) and variable (TPSv) hand’s wrist.
5. Normalised Time to Peak Speed: time to peak speed
normalised by Initial Component Duration (see 7
below) for the constant (NTPSc) and variable (NTPSv)
hand’s wrist.
6. Maximum Grip Aperture: the largest distance occur-
ring between the index Wnger and thumb tips during the
reach for the constant (MGAc) and variable (MGAv)
hand.
7. Initial Component Duration: the times from reach onset
(when the hand started moving) to reach termination
(when the wrist stopped moving) for the constant
(ICDc) and variable (ICDv) hand.
8. Initial Component Asynchrony (ICA): the diVerence
between the reach termination times (when each wrist
stopped moving) of the constant and variable hands
(we computed and analysed signed diVerences).
9. Grasp Termination Asynchrony (GTA): the diVerence
between when a grasp was achieved (i.e., when the
Wngers stopped moving) by the constant hand and
when it was achieved by the variable hand (we com-
puted and analysed signed diVerences).
We used the median value for the ten trials for each condi-
tion for each participant within our analyses for each
dependent variable. We selected the median as standard
practice as it is robust to outliers; mean values produced the
same pattern of results. We examined each of these mea-
sures in two ways. First, each was studied for evidence of
deviations from the characteristic form of unimanual reach-
to-grasp movements as determined by the target object
properties: distance, grip area size, and width. That is, we
looked for the appearance of interactions between the hands
causing them to be more synchronous as the reach-to-grasp
evolved through time. Second, each measure was examined
to see if asynchrony was present, especially in the Wnal
stages when the grasp is actually established. On the one
hand, a tendency towards synchrony was expected given
the results of the previous studies of bimanual coordination
described above. On the other hand, we also expected asyn-
chrony near the end of the movements as an unavoidable
consequence of vision being directed towards one hand and
then the other (with necessary corrections then made under
visual guidance).
Results
Control experiment
The data from all ten participants on all 48 trials were
inspected by eye and analysed using Labview routines.
The pattern of hand movements was consistent with the
Wndings reported for the main experiment. The control
experiment was conducted to explore the changes in Wxa-
tion that occur during the experimental task. It was found
that every participant shifted Wxation from one target to
the other after movement onset, on every trial. Fixation
shifted on average at 267 ms into the movement across
the participants (30.6% of movement duration on aver-
age). This result conWrms previous suggestions that large
angular gaze shifts are generated through head move-
ments (Stahl 1999). Moreover, this result shows that task
success rested upon Wxation of both reaching limbs as
they approached the target. The mode of the number of
head movements was one, with the normal pattern con-
sistent with the participant Wxating the constant target
and then shifting gaze to the variable hand’s target. Tri-
als where the participant started with Wxation on the vari-
able target generally occurred when the variable target
was at the close distance. The trials with more than one
head movement generally occurred with the smallest grip
surface.
Main experiment
All data were entered into a three way repeated measures
ANOVA with Greenhouse–Geisser corrections using
Fig. 2 Example of the kinematic proWles found in the experiment
(scaled and oVset) selected at random. The bottom two signals are the
wrist speed proWles. The top two signals show the formation of the grip
aperture. The constant hand’s signals are beneath the variable hand’s
signals (so the dashed and dotted signals belong to the constant hand).
The solid vertical line shows the cessation of the constant hand’s wrist
movement. The dotted vertical line shows when the constant hand se-
cured the object. The dashed vertical line showed when the variable
hand contacted the object
Time (ms)
100 300 500 700 900 1100
288 Exp Brain Res (2008) 184:283–293
123
distance, width and grip surface area as the three indepen-
dent variables. A separate ANOVA was conducted for each
of the eight dependant variables. For variables that entailed
a separate measure for each hand, separate ANOVAs were
conducted on the data for each hand or we did a four factor
ANOVA with hand as a fourth factor. In the majority of sit-
uations there were no reliable interactions in which case we
report only the statistically reliable main eVects. The uncor-
rected degrees of freedom are (2, 14) for main eVects and
(4, 28) for two-way interactions, the corrected values are
provided when we report the F scores.
Onset asynchrony
There was no reliable eVect of distance, width or grip sur-
face area on the onset asynchrony. The hands started their
movement at approximately the same time (average 5 ms
signed onset asynchrony, 14 ms unsigned asynchrony).
We had expected no signiWcant diVerences in this
analysis.
Total task time
The ANOVA yielded a reliable eVect of distance
[F
(1.05,7.35)
= 10.17, P < 0.05] and grip surface area
[F
(1.79,712.53)
=17.09, P < 0.05]. TTT increased (1) as the
average distance increased and (2) as the grip surface area
decreased in size (Fig. 3).
Peak speed, time to peak speed, and normalised
time to peak speed
Distance, width or grip surface area (of the variable hand)
aVected neither the peak speed nor the time to peak speed
of the constant hand. PS of the variable hand was aVected
reliably by distance alone [F
(1.02,7.03)
= 19.91, P <0.05] as
was the time to peak speed [F
(1.06,7.42)
= 19.03, P < 0.05].
These results can be seen in Fig. 4. There were no signiW-
cant eVects of distance, width or grip surface area on nor-
malised time to peak speed for either the constant or
variable hand. Peak speed occurred at about 40% of the
total reach duration (mean = 44.2%) as typically found in
prehension studies.
The lack of interaction between the hands during the Wrst
part of the reach movements has been reported in previous
studies on bimanual prehension that have analysed initial
reaction times, in particular (Diedrichsen et al. 2001;
Kunde and Weigelt 2005; see Ivry et al. 2004 for review
and discussion). This lack of interaction means that at least
up to the moment of peak speed, reaches to equivalent
objects at the same distances were approximately synchro-
nous while reaches to objects at diVerent distances were
asynchronous.
Maximum grip aperture
The maximum grip aperture of the variable hand was inXu-
enced by object width [F
(1.05,7.34)
= 52.47, P <0.05] and
grip surface area [F
(1.31,9.13)
=22.37, P < 0.05]. Notably,
MGA of the constant hand was also inXuenced by (variable
hand) object width [F
(1.18,8.23)
= 13.45, P<0.05]. This was
the Wrst sign of any interaction between the hands and,
given that MGA occurred at about 70% of the reach dura-
tion, it took place during the deceleration phase of the
reaches. These results can be seen in Fig. 5.
Initial component duration
We performed a four factor repeated measures ANOVA on
the time at which the wrist stopped moving (at the end of
the IC) with hand, distance, width, and grip surface area as
factors. Main eVects were obtained for hand [F
(1,7)
= 6.37,
P<0.05], distance [F
(1.08,7.54)
= 14.57, P<0.01], and grip
Fig. 3 Total task time (ms) was deWned as the time between the Wrst
hand starting to move forward and the time at which both hands had
secured a grasp on the target objects. The Wgure (a) shows that total
task time increases as distance (cm) increases. The Wgure (b) shows
that total task time increases as grasp surface size (cm) decreases. The
lines shown are least-squares linear regressions
Distance (cm)
45
Total task time (ms)Total task time (ms)
660
680
700
720
740
760
Grasp surface size (cm)
695
700
705
710
715
720
725
15 20 25 30
35 40
3.5
0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0
A
B
Exp Brain Res (2008) 184:283–293 289
123
surface area [F
(1.08,7.53)
= 7.73, P<0.05]. The constant
hand was faster than the variable hand (constant = 651 ms;
variable = 680 ms). Smaller grip surface area yielded
slower reaches as shown in Fig. 6b. Distance and width
both exhibited interactions with hand: distance by hand
[F
(1.22,8.52)
= 7.98, P<0.05], and width by hand
[F
(1.22,8.54)
= 5.25, P<0.05]. Given these interactions, we
performed a separate three-factor ANOVA on the data for
each hand. The time at which the wrist stopped moving was
aVected reliably by distance (of the variable hand target) for
both the constant [F
(1.75,12.26)
= 14.51, P<0.05] and vari-
able hand [F
(1.09,7.62)
= 11.99, P<0.05]. Likewise, the time
at which the hand stopped moving was aVected reliably by
grip surface area for both the constant [F
(1.09,7.64)
= 4.15,
P<0.05] and variable hand [F
(1.15,8.06)
= 12.37, P<0.05].
These results can be seen in Fig. 6. The eVect of distance
and grasp surface size on the timing of the constant hand
was the second result showing interaction between the
hands.
The changes in constant hand timing were not nearly
large enough to yield synchronous movements as can be
seen in Fig. 6. The asynchrony of timing was shown by the
statistically signiWcant interaction of hand with both dis-
tance and grip surface area in the four-factor ANOVA. The
main eVect found for the hand in the four-factor ANOVA
revealed that when all else was equal, the constant hand
would arrive Wrst. In the context of the relative phase mea-
sure used in rhythmic bimanual coordination studies, this
t80 ms asynchrony in movements of t640 ms duration
represented a phase diVerence of about 20°.
Initial component asynchrony
We next tested the asynchrony at the end of the IC directly.
We calculated the diVerence between when the constant
and variable wrist stopped moving (the end of the IC) in
each trial. The temporal diVerence between the two hands
was aVected reliably by distance [F
(1.05,7.34)
= 18.22,
Fig. 4 Peak tangential speed (mm/s) plotted against distance in cm
(upper, a) and time to peak tangential speed (ms) plotted against dis-
tance in cm (lower, b). The peak speed and the time to peak speed o
f
the variable hand were aVected by average distance. The target of the
variable hand did not aVect the peak speed of the constant hand. The
lines shown are least-squares linear regressions
Distance (cm)
45
800
900
1000
1100
1200
1300
Variable
Constant
Distance (cm)
260
270
280
290
300
310
320
Variable
Constant
Peak speed (mm/s)
Time to peak speed (ms)
15 20 25 30
35
40
45
15 20 25 30
35
40
A
B
Fig. 5 Maximum grip aperture (cm) of both the constant and variable
hand plotted against object width in cm (upper, a) and against grip sur-
face in cm (lower, b) for the variable hand. The inXuence of the vari-
able hand target’s width on the maximum grip aperture of the constant
hand was the Wrst sign of any interaction between the hands. The lines
shown are least-squares linear regressions
Object width (cm)
8
Maximum grip aperture (cm)
Maximum grip aperture (cm)
8.5
9.0
9.5
10.0
10.5
11.0
11.5
Variable
Constant
Grasp surface size (cm)
8.5
9.0
9.5
10.0
10.5
11.0
11.5
Variable
Constant
40
2
3
456
7
3.5
0.5
1.0
1.5
2.0
2.5 3.0
290 Exp Brain Res (2008) 184:283–293
123
P<0.05] and object width [F
(1.62,11.36)
= 5.56, P<0.05] as
shown in Fig. 7. (These were the factors that interacted
with hand in the previous analysis.) Figure 7 shows that the
order of the hands reversed as predicted (with the nearer
target reached Wrst and the further target reached last). An
examination of the frequency histograms of these asynchro-
nies at each distance revealed that for the 20 and 40 cm tar-
gets (i.e., diVerent target distances for the two hands), about
90% of the data were in the direction represented by the
means shown in Fig. 7 and the coeYcients of variation (i.e.,
SD/mean) were about 1.0. In contrast, the mean asynchrony
for the 30 cm distance (i.e., the equal distance condition)
was small (t¡20 ms). Examination of the frequency histo-
gram revealed that the data were distributed about both pos-
itive and negative asynchronies signiWcantly diVerent from
zero, but with distributions that overlapped at zero. We
computed mean (and SD) asynchronies for the positive
(variable hand Wrst) and negative (constant hand Wrst) dis-
tributions as object width varied from small to large. See
Table 1. In all cases, the mean absolute value of asyn-
chrony, whether positive or negative, was about 76 ms with
a coeYcient of variation of about 86% (i.e. SDs were about
65 ms). Both positive and negative asynchronies varied
with object width as shown by two-tailed t-tests,
F(98) = 756.4, P<0.01, and F(136) = 1458.4, P<0.01,
respectively. Thus, in the equal target distance condition,
which hand arrived Wrst appeared to vary randomly, but in
about half of the trials the constant hand arrived Wrst by
about 76 ms on average, and in the other half, the variable
hand arrived Wrst by about the same amount of time.
Grasp termination asynchrony
We calculated the temporal diVerences between when the
constant hand and variable hand achieved a Wnal grasp for
each individual trial and analysed these data. A reliable
interaction was found between the GTA and both reach dis-
tance and grip surface area [F
(2.81,19.64)
= 4.03, P<0.05].
Fig. 6 The initial component duration (ms) for the constant and vari-
able hand plotted against distance in cm (upper, a) and against grasp
surface size in cm (lower, b). The eVect of distance and grasp surface
size on the timing of the constant hand was the second result showing
interaction between the hands. The lines shown are least-squares linear
regressions
Distance (cm)
45
620
640
660
680
700
720
740
Variable
Constant
Grasp surface size (cm)
630
640
650
660
670
680
690
700
710
Variable
Constant
Initial component duration (ms)
Initial component duration (ms)
15 20
25
30 35
40
3.5
0.5 1.0
1.5
2.0 2.5
3.0
Fig. 7 The signed temporal diVerence (ms) between when the con-
stant and variable hand stopped moving plotted against distance in cm
in the upper plot (a) and against object width (cm) in the lower plot (b).
The lines shown are least-squares linear regressions. It can be seen that
the order of the hands reversed according to target distance (with the
nearer target reached Wrst by the variable hand and the further targe
t
reached last)
Distance (cm)
Reach termination asynchrony (ms)
Reach termination aynschrony (ms)
-80
-60
-40
-20
0
20
40
60
Variable hand
finishes before
constant
Object width (cm)
-35
-30
-25
-20
-15
-10
-5
0
Variable hand
finishes after
constant
A
B
45
15 20 25 30
35 40
8
2345
67
Exp Brain Res (2008) 184:283–293 291
123
There was also a main eVect of object width on GTA
[F
(1.73,12.71)
= 8.15, P<0.05]. The results shown in Fig. 8
are essentially the same as in Fig. 7, that is, the timing at the
end of the IC was preserved through to the end of the entire
reach-to-grasp movement.
Discussion
We investigated bimanual prehension to separate target
objects that varied in distance and size. We did not explic-
itly instruct the participants to move their hands together
but previous research (e.g. Diedrichsen et al. 2001; Kelso
et al. 1979a,b; Kunde and Weigelt 2005) led us to predict
that: (1) the participants would start to move the hands at
the same time and (2) the hands would move in reasonable
temporal proximity (i.e. the movement of the two hands
would occur in a common time window). We observed this
expected pattern in all participants. However, we also
expected target distance and size to aVect the reach-to-
grasp in the usual ways, which is to say that reaches should
take longer to targets that are more distant or smaller. As
shown in Fig. 3, these expectations were met. Furthermore,
the peak speeds and the times for acceleration to peak speed
for each hand were strictly a function of the targets
approached by each hand. Thus, the peak speed and the
time to peak speed were constant for the constant hand and
variable for the variable hand.
The central question was how would the two hands
coordinate in the terminal phase of the bimanual reaches?
Would the reaches-to-grasp be synchronous or asynchro-
nous? On the one hand, previous studies of reaching
(without grasping) have found relative synchrony through-
out (e.g. Kelso 1979a,b). On the other hand, the task
requirements for visual guidance led us to predict that asyn-
chrony would be observed in the movements (e.g. Fisk and
Goodale 1988; Winges et al. 2003). In fact, we did see a
tendency for synchronisation between the hands during the
deceleration portion of the reaches. The movement of one
hand started to inXuence the movement of the other hand so
the variable hand’s target began to inXuence the kinematics
recorded for the constant hand. Variations in the width of
the variable hand target aVected the maximum grasp aper-
ture of the constant hand. Furthermore, the deceleration
phase of the constant hand was aVected by the variable
hand target as indexed by the mean times at the end of the
reaches (i.e., the Initial Component Durations). The arrival
of the constant right hand lengthened as variable hand’s tar-
get became further and its surface became smaller. The
inXuence of grip surface area on maximum grasp aperture
(MGA) was probably mediated by this eVect. The maxi-
mum grip aperture of the variable hand altered in response
to grasp surface size as well as to object width. A small
grasp surface resulted in a decreased MGA whereas a large
grasp surface resulted in an increased MGA. The changes
in grasp aperture can be explained by the shorter decelera-
tion phase found in both hands when moving to a larger
grasp surface (a larger grasp surface decreases accuracy
demands allowing the system to reduce feedback correction
and thus produce faster movements). It is known that
Table 1 Mean (and SD) asynchronies (in ms) at the end of the IC for
the 30 cm variable hand target (i.e. equal target distances for the two
hands) for constant hand Wrst (negative) and variable hand Wrst (posi-
tive) trials by object width
Object width
Small Medium Large
Mean ¡84.71 ¡85.57 ¡80.95
SD 71.51 77.75 65.35
Mean 77.52 63.11 60.74
SD 76.89 46.41 58.66
Fig. 8 The upper plot (a) shows the signed temporal diVerences (ms)
between when the constant hand and variable hand achieved a stable
grasp plotted against distance (cm) for the three diVerent grasp sur-
faces. The lower plot (b) shows the temporal diVerences (ms) between
when the constant hand and variable hand achieved a stable grasp plot-
ted against object width (cm). The lines shown are least-squares linear
regressions. The pattern of results is similar to that shown in Fig. 6
indicating that the timing at the end of the IC was preserved through to
the end of the entire reach-to-grasp movement
-25
-20
-15
-10
-5
0
5
10
Variable hand
finishes before
constant
constant
B
A
Distance (cm)
Grasp termination asynchrony (ms)
Grasp termination asynchrony (ms)
-100
-80
-60
-40
-20
0
20
40
60
80
Narrow
Medium
Wide
Variable hand
finishes before
45
15 20 25 30
35 40
Object width (cm)
8
2
3
45
67
292 Exp Brain Res (2008) 184:283–293
123
alterations in movement time produce reliable changes in
grasp aperture formation (Wing et al. 1986; Smeets and
Brenner 1999; Mon-Williams and Tresilian 2001; Loftus
et al. 2004). Nonetheless, these interactions were not nearly
enough to yield synchrony between the hands when they
reached towards unequal target objects at diVerent dis-
tances. Moreover, when the hands reached towards equiva-
lent targets at equal distances, their arrival was
asynchronous. There was some tendency for the constant
hand to arrive sooner, but which hand arrived Wrst was
largely random. When the targets were diVerent distances,
then the order of arrival was determined by the relative
proximity of the targets as predicted (with the closer target
reached Wrst). It should be noted that the precise pattern we
observed might have altered if the non-preferred hand had
been reaching for the constant target (with the asynchrony
and interaction of the hands being either larger or smaller
during a particular movement phase). Nevertheless, this
study shows the generic constraints that aVect bimanual
reach-to-grasp behaviour.
In conclusion, diVerences in the distances of targets did
yield asynchronies in reaching as expected from normal uni-
manual behaviour. These asynchronies were used to deter-
mine the ordering of the hands in closing on the targets.
Increasing the diYculty of the target increased the magni-
tude of the asynchrony. Furthermore, when the targets dis-
tances were equal, the hands still exhibited asynchronies in
arrival times, but now the ordering of the hands was largely
random. The asynchronies that accrued during the IC were
preserved through the CC. The parsimonious explanation is
that the asynchronies were driven by the need for visual
information to guide each hand in turn. Riek et al.’s (2003)
eye movement study suggests that these asymmetries reXect
looking behaviours and the time taken to Wxate and guide
Wrst one hand and then the other. Consistent with this con-
clusion, the control experiment showed that every partici-
pant shifted Wxation at least once (sometimes more when
task diYculty increased) on every trial. The normal Wxation
shift pattern was consistent with the participant Wxating the
constant target and then shifting gaze to the variable hand’s
target. The strategy of Wxating on the variable target last by
the participants (as evidenced by the control experiment)
coincides well with the Wnding of increasing asynchrony
with increasing target diYculty. The eVect of this did not
appear until the stage at which visual feedback was actually
being used to guide the reach (i.e. during deceleration).
Until the deceleration phase, the two reaches did not interact
and each followed a trajectory determined stably by the task
and the distance of the given target.
In summary, this article has shown the lawful relation-
ships that are present in bimanual prehension movements.
The experimental task often produced a qualitatively diVer-
ent prehension pattern than that sometimes described in the
past studies. This result shows the Xexibility of the prehen-
sion pattern in response to task demands. We set out to
determine whether the bias for synchronicity would deter-
mine the coordinative timing of bimanual prehension
movements to separate objects. We found that the behav-
iour was biased towards synchrony but the need for visual
information also inXuenced the coordinative timing, pro-
ducing asynchrony in reliable ways.
Acknowledgment We are grateful to Jennifer Leaper (Charles) and
Silke Leifheit for conducting the control experiment and analysing the
data. A grant to Mark Mon-Williams from Action Medical Research
and Scottish Enterprise helped support this research work. The produc-
tion of the manuscript was facilitated by an award from the Royal
Society of Edinburgh/Lloyds TSB to Bingham and Mon-Williams.
References
Bingham GP (2001) A perceptually driven dynamical model of rhyth-
mic limb movement and bimanual coordination. In: Proceedings
of the 23rd annual conference of the cognitive science society,
LEA Publishers, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 75–79
Bingham GP (2004a) Another timing variable composed of state vari-
ables: phase perception and phase driven oscillators. In: Hecht H,
Savelsbergh GJP (eds) Theories of time-to-contact. MIT, Boston
Bingham GP (2004b) A perceptually driven dynamical model
of bimanual rhythmic movement (and phase perception). Ecol
Psychol 16:45–53
Bingham GP, Schmidt RC, Zaal FTJM (1999) Visual perception of rel-
ative phasing of human limb movements. Percept Psychophys
61:246–258
Bingham GP, Zaal FTJM, Shull JA, Collins D (2000) The eVect of fre-
quency on visual perception of relative phase and phase variabil-
ity of two oscillating objects. Exp Brain Res 136:543–552
Bootsma RJ, Marteniuk RG, MacKenzie CL, Zaal FTJM (1994) the
speed-accuracy trade-oV in manual prehension: eVects of move-
ment amplitude, object size, and object width on kinematic char-
acteristics. Exp Brain Res 98:535–541
Castiello U, Bennett KM, Stelmach GE (1993) The bilateral reach to
grasp movement. Behav Brain Res 56:43–57
Diedrichsen J, Hazeltine E, Kennerley S, Ivry RB (2001) Moving to di-
rectly cued locations abolishes spatial interference during biman-
ual actions. Psychol Sci 12:493–498
Fisk JD, Goodale MA (1988) The eVects of unilateral brain damage on
visually guided reaching: hemispheric diVerences in the nature of
the deWcit. Exp Brain Res 72:425–435
Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system
in controlling the amplitude of the movement. J Exp Psychol
47:381–391
Haggard P, Wing AM (1995) Coordinated responses following
mechanical perturbation of the arm during prehension. Exp Brain
Res 102:483–494
Ivry R, Diedrichsen J, Spencer R, Hazeltine E, Semjen A (2004) A
cognitive neuroscience perspective on bimanual coordination and
interference. In: Swinnen S, Duysens J (eds) Neuro-behavioral
determinants of interlimb coordination. Kluwer, Boston, pp 259–
295
Jackson GM, Jackson SR, Kritikos A (1999) Attention for action: coor-
dinating bimanual reach-to-grasp movements. Br J Psychol
90:247–270
Jackson SR, Jones CA, Newport R, Pritchard C (1997) A kinematic
analysis of goal-directed prehension movements executed under
Exp Brain Res (2008) 184:283–293 293
123
binocular, monocular and memory-guided viewing conditions.
Vis Cogn 4:113–142
Jeannerod M (1984) The timing of matural prehension movements.
J Mot Behav 16:235–254
Jeannerod M (1988) The neural and behavioural organisation of goal-
directed movements. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Kelso JAS (1995) Dynamic patterns: the self-organisation of brain and
behavior. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
Kelso JS, Southard DL, Goodman D (1979a) On the nature of human
interlimb coordination. Science 203:1029–1031
Kelso JS, Southard DL, Goodman D (1979b) On the coordination of
two-handed movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform
5:229–238
Kunde W, Weigelt M (2005) Goal-congruency in bimanual object
manipulation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31:145–156
Liao M, Jagacinski RJ (2000) A dynamical systems approach to man-
ual tracking performance. J Mot Behav 32:361–378
Loftus A, Goodale MG, Servos P, Mon-Williams M (2004) When two
eyes are better than one in prehension: prehension, end-point var-
iance and monocular viewing. Exp Brain Res 158:317–327
Mechsner F, Kerzel D, Knoblich G, Prinz W (2001) Perceptual basis
of bimanual coordination. Nature 414:69–73
Meulenbroek RGJ, Rosenbaum DA, Jansen C, Vaughan J, Vogt S
(2001) Multijoint grasping movements: simulated and observed
eVects of object location, object size, and initial aperture. Exp
Brain Res 138:219–234
Mon-Williams M, Tresilian (2001) A simple rule of thumb for elegant
prehension. Curr Biol 11:1058–1061
Napier JR (1956) The prehensile movements of the human hand.
J Bone Joint Surg 38B:902–913
Paulignan Y, MacKenzie C, Marteniuk R, Jeannerod M (1991) Selec-
tive perturbation of visual input during prehension movements. 1.
The eVects of changing object position. Exp Brain Res 83:502–512
Riek S, Tresilian JR, Mon-Williams M, Coppard V, Carson RC (2003)
Bimanual aiming and overt attention: one law for two hands. Exp
Brain Res 153:59–75
Rosenbaum DA, Meulenbroek RGJ, Vaughan J, Jansen C (1999)
Coordination of reaching and grasping by capitalizing on obstacle
avoidance and other constraints. Exp Brain Res 128:92–100
Schmidt RC, Carello C, Turvey MT (1990) Phase transitions and crit-
ical Xuctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic move-
ments between people. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform
16:227–247
Serrien DJ, Li Y, Steyvers M, Debaere F, Swinnen SP (2001) Proprio-
ceptive regulation of interlimb behavior: interference between
passive movement and active coordination dynamics. Exp Brain
Res 140:411–419
Smeets JBJ, Brenner E (1999) A new view on grasping. Motor Control
3:237–271
Stahl JS (1999) Amplitude of human head movements associated with
horizontal saccades. Exp Brain Res 126:41–54
Tresilian JR, Stelmach GE (1997) Common organisation for unimanu-
al and bimanual reach-to-grasp tasks. Exp Brain Res 115:283
299
Tresilian JR, Stelmach GE, Adler CH (1997) Stability of reach-to-
grasp movement patterns in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 120:2093–
2111
Wilson A, Bingham GP (2005a) Perceptual coupling in rhythmic
movement coordination—stable perception leads to stable action.
Exp Brain Res 164:517–528
Wilson A, Bingham GP (2005b) Human movement coordination
implicates relative direction as the information for relative phase.
Exp Brain Res 165:351–361
Wilson AD, Bingham GP, Craig JC (2003) Proprioceptive perception
of phase variability. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform
29:1179–1190
Wimmers RH, Beek PJ, van Wieringen PCW (1992) Phase transitions
in rhythmic tracking movements: a case of unilateral coupling.
Hum Mov Sci 11:217–226
Wing AM, Fraser C (1983) The contribution of the thumb to reaching
movements. Q J Exp Psychol 35:297–309
Wing AM, Turton A, Fraser C (1986) Grasp size and accuracy of
approach in reaching. J Mot Behav 3:245–260
Winges SA, Weber DJ, Santello M (2003) The role of vision on hand
preshaping during reach to grasp. Exp Brain Res 152:489–498
... A few works have focused on the coordination patterns underlying asymmetric and non-cyclic motions, such as asymmetric reaching motion toward distinct targets (Kelso et al. 1979;Bingham et al. 2008). It is in a way surprising that so little attention has been brought to this topic, since such bimanual movements are quite common in human everyday activities (e.g., picking-up large objects). ...
... For instance, reaching tasks are performed more easily when both hands reach the targets simultaneously. When the two hands are involved in asymmetric reaches, the movements of the hand reaching for the target farthest away influences the movement of the other hand, slowing it down to preserve temporal coordination (Bingham et al. 2008). ...
... For instance, when reaching for grasping a moving target, arm and hand motions are tightly coupled to the target's location and orientation (Jeannerod et al. 1995). Bingham et al. (2008) suggested that the coordinate timing of bimanual movements to distinct targets is biased toward synchrony and also influenced by visual information. In these studies, however, task demands (i.e., spatial position of the reaching target) are unaffected by human motions and "extrinsic" to the generated coordination patterns. ...
Article
Full-text available
Tasks that require the cooperation of both hands and arms are common in human everyday life. Coordination helps to synchronize in space and temporally motion of the upper limbs. In fine bimanual tasks, coordination enables also to achieve higher degrees of precision that could be obtained from a single hand. We studied the acquisition of bimanual fine manipulation skills in watchmaking tasks, which require assembly of pieces at millimeter scale. It demands years of training. We contrasted motion kinematics performed by novice apprentices to those of professionals. Fifteen subjects, ten novices and five experts, participated in the study. We recorded force applied on the watch face and kinematics of fingers and arms. Results indicate that expert subjects wisely place their fingers on the tools to achieve higher dexterity. Compared to novices, experts also tend to align task-demanded force application with the optimal force transmission direction of the dominant arm. To understand the cognitive processes underpinning the different coordination patterns across experts and novice subjects, we followed the optimal control theoretical framework and hypothesize that the difference in task performances is caused by changes in the central nervous system’s optimal criteria. We formulated kinematic metrics to evaluate the coordination patterns and exploit inverse optimization approach to infer the optimal criteria. We interpret the human acquisition of novel coordination patterns as an alteration in the composition structure of the central nervous system’s optimal criteria accompanied by the learning process.
... For bimanual movements, targets presented were either the same size (termed congruent) or were of different sizes (termed incongruent). The basic aiming task used shared numerous similarities with previous studies of bimanual aiming and prehension, where kinematic data were reported in the absence of any eye movement data (Marteniuk et al. 1984;Corcos 1984;Fowler et al. 1991;Castiello et al. 1993;Jackson et al. 1999;Bingham et al. 2008;Bruyn and Mason 2009;Miller and Smyth 2012;Hesse et al. 2010). Before considering the eye movement data presented here and relations with the limb movement data reported, we first discuss how our limb kinematic data compare with these previous studies. ...
... At movement onset and during the early stages of movement, tight synchrony between the limbs was apparent and was consistent with the preponderance of influential bimanual coordination literature emphasising coupling between the limbs (Kelso et al. 1979;Swinnen 2002). However the asynchrony that emerged in the latter stages of movement and was captured at movement end appears to be driven by the varying visuo-motor requirements of the task and was largely consistent with a number of more recent studies (Miller and Smyth 2012;Bruyn and Mason 2009;Bingham et al. 2008). ...
Article
Full-text available
Although synchrony between the limbs is an often-cited feature of bimanual coordination, recent studies have also highlighted the small asynchronies that can occur. The visuo-motor demands of any bimanual task are considered central to the emergence of asynchrony, but the relationship between the two remains largely unexplored. This study aimed to address this issue. Hand and eye movements were measured in 19 participants, while they made either unimanual or bimanual reach-to-point (aiming) movements to targets presented on a touchscreen. Bimanual movements were either congruent (same-sized targets) or incongruent (different-sized targets). Resulting hand data showed many of the typical patterns of movement previously reported. While temporal coupling between the limbs remained largely evident for bimanual movements, small between-limb asynchronies were apparent and demonstrated clear associations with the competing precision requirements of the targets and related visual attention. Participants mainly directed their gaze towards the more difficult target with corresponding reaching movements demonstrating greater precision than for the easier target. Additionally, there was a reliable tendency for the hand reaching towards the more difficult target to lead. Importantly, it was the competing visuo-motor demands of individual movements rather than overall difficulty that resulted in greater between-limb asynchrony; accordingly, where both targets were small (i.e., the most difficult condition), asynchrony was significantly less pronounced than for incongruent bimanual conditions. The results show how the visuo-motor system balances its apparent drive for synchrony in coordinating bimanual movements with the competing demands that characterise the constituent unimanual movements.
... The current study extends Piedimonte et al. (2018) by looking at kinematic performance in a discrete bimanual reach to grasp task. Bimanual reach-to-grasp also creates an interesting behavioral paradigm for studying motor development due to the challenges it places on the perceptual-motor control system (Bingham et al., 2008;Mason et al., 2010), as it is not possible to visually fixate both limbs and both targets at the same time when they are spatially separated. Therefore, it is necessary to sequentially divide visual attention between the reaching limbs and targets in order to successfully perform the task. ...
... However, both the young ASD and TD groups demonstrated MTs that were modulated as a function of task complexity, although the modulation was not as large in the ASD group. These results suggest that task complexity affects both motor planning and movement execution in typical development, replicating previous research on discrete bimanual tasks in both adults and typically developing youth (Jackson et al., 1999;Bingham et al., 2008;Mason et al., 2010Mason et al., , 2013. However, task complexity may affect movement execution but not motor planning in young children with ASD. ...
Article
Full-text available
Reaching and grasping (prehension) is one of the earliest developing motor skills in humans, but continued prehension development in childhood and adolescence enables the performance of increasingly complex manual tasks. In individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) atypical unimanual reaching and grasping has been reported, but to date, no studies have investigated discrete bimanual movements. We examined unimanual and bimanual reach to grasp tasks in youth with ASD to better understand how motor performance might change with increasing complexity. Twenty youth with ASD (10.1 ± 2.4 years) and 17 youth with typical development (TD) (9.6 ± 2.6 years) were instructed to reach and grasp cubes that became illuminated. Participants were asked to reach out with the right and/or left hands to grasp and lift targets located at near (18 cm) and/or far (28 cm) distances. For the unimanual (simplest) condition, participants grasped one illuminated cube (with either the left or right hand). For the bimanual conditions, participants grasped two illuminated cubes located at the same distance from the start position (bimanual symmetric condition) or two illuminated cubes located at different distances (bimanual asymmetric condition). Significant interactions among diagnostic group, task complexity, and age were found for initiation time (IT) and movement time (MT). Specifically, the older children in both groups initiated and performed their movements faster in the unimanual condition than in the bimanual conditions, although the older children with ASD produced slower ITs and MTs compared to typically developing peers across all three conditions. Surprisingly, the younger children with ASD had similar ITs and MTs as their peers for the unimanual condition but did not considerably slow these times to adjust for the complexity of the bimanual tasks. We hypothesize that they chose to re-use the motor plans that were generated for the unimanual trials rather than generate more appropriate motor plans for the bimanual tasks. An atypical spatiotemporal relationship between MT and peak aperture (PA) was also found in the ASD group. Together, our results suggest deficits in motor planning that result in subtle effects on performance in younger children with ASD that become more pronounced with age.
... This finding is consistent with a line of research demonstrating that the ability to produce stable bimanual coordination is dependent on the participant's ability to perceive the relative phase pattern (Bingham et al. 1999;Bingham 2004;Wilson et al. 2005a, b). Typically, participants only perceive in-phase and anti-phase coordination patterns without specific training to recognize other phase and frequency relationships (e.g., Bingham et al. 2008). However, the results of the current investigation demonstrated that both individual (bimanual) and pairs of participants (social) could effectively coordinate 1:1 in-phase and 1:2 patterns of isometric force within a few minutes of practice (see Fig. 4). ...
Article
Full-text available
The current investigation was designed to examine the influence of inherent and incidental constraints on the stability characteristics associated with bimanual and social coordination. Individual participants (N = 9) and pairs of participants (N = 18, 9 pairs) were required to rhythmically coordinate patterns of isometric forces in 1:1 in-phase and 1:2 multi-frequency patterns by exerting force with their right and left limbs. Lissajous information was provided to guide performance. Participants performed 13 practice trials and 1 test trial per pattern. On the test trial, muscle activity from the triceps brachii muscles of each arm was recorded. EMG–EMG coherence between the two EMG signals was calculated using wavelet coherence. The behavioral data indicated that individual participants performed the 1:1 in-phase pattern more accurately and with less variability than paired participants. The EMG coherence analysis indicated significantly higher coherence for individual participants than for the paired participants during the 1:1 in-phase pattern, whereas no differences were observed between groups for the 1:2 coordination pattern. The results of the current investigation support the notion that neural crosstalk can stabilize 1:1 in-phase coordination when contralateral and ipsilateral signals are integrated via the neuromuscular linkage between two effectors.
... Spatiotemporal coupling has also been shown for discrete bimanual movements, including bimanual pointing movement and bimanual reach-to-grasp movements. Temporal coupling is stronger than spatial coupling in both bimanual pointing movements (Kelso et al. 1979(Kelso et al. , 1983 and bimanual reach-to-grasp movements (Bingham et al. 2008;Blinch et al. 2018;Dohle et al. 2000;Jackson et al. 1999;Mason and Bruyn 2009). For bimanual reach-to-grasp movements, the spatial coupling of the grip apertures to targets with difference sizes is comparatively weak (c.f. ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has established that the left cerebral hemisphere is dominant for the control of continuous bimanual movements. The lateralisation of motor control for discrete bimanual movements, in contrast, is underexplored. The purpose of the current study was to investigate which (if either) hemisphere is dominant for discrete bimanual movements. Twenty-one participants made bimanual reach-to-grasp movements towards pieces of candy. Participants grasped the candy to either place it in their mouths (grasp-to-eat) or in a receptacle near their mouths (grasp-to-place). Research has shown smaller maximum grip apertures (MGAs) for unimanual grasp-to-eat movements than unimanual grasp-to-place movements when controlled by the left hemisphere. In Experiment 1, participants made bimanual symmetric movements where both hands made grasp-to-eat or grasp-to-place movements. We hypothesised that a left hemisphere dominance for bimanual movements would cause smaller MGAs in both hands during bimanual grasp-to-eat movements compared to those in bimanual grasp-to-place movements. The results revealed that MGAs were indeed smaller for bimanual grasp-to-eat movements than grasp-to-place movements. This supports that the left hemisphere may be dominant for the control of bimanual symmetric movements, which agrees with studies on continuous bimanual movements. In Experiment 2, participants made bimanual asymmetric movements where one hand made a grasp-to-eat movement while the other hand made a grasp-to-place movement. The results failed to support the potential predictions of left hemisphere dominance, right hemisphere dominance, or contralateral control.
... Les actions habiles semblent nécessiter des stratégies oculomotrices spatio-temporelles spécifiques pour l'échantillonnage des informations utiles pour la tâche en cours [156]. Quand il s'agit d'attraper un objet, il est naturel de regarder l'objet plutôt que la main [157] et lors d'une action bimanuelle, le regard alterne entre les objets à saisir [158]. Des similitudes peuvent être établies entre l'approche bimanuelle et le pilotage à travers une série de points de repère (par exemple lors d'un slalom) où la cible la plus immédiate est suivie avec 1 à 2 s d'avance, ensuite le regard est dirigé vers la cible suivante dans la séquence [137]. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Les fauteuils roulants électriques (FRE) ont permis à nombre de personnes handicapés moteurs de retrouver une mobilité satisfaisante, ce qui a amélioré leur qualité de vie, un vaste champ d’activités leur étant devenu accessible. Lors de la prescription d’un FRE ou d’une phase d’apprentissage à la conduite il est cependant nécessaire d’évaluer les capacités de ces personnes à piloter un fauteuil. Dans ce contexte, notre objectif consiste à déterminer des paramètres pertinents permettant de décrire la qualité de la conduite d’un FRE pour des personnes ayant des déficiences motrices sévères. Tout d’abord, nous avons modélisé le système pilote-FRE par le modèle OPCM (Optimal Preview Control Model) de Sharp. Ce modèle est basé sur la distance anticipée (Dp) par l’utilisateur durant sa conduite du FRE. Cette distance a été estimée grâce à un système de suivi du regard combiné avec le simulateur 3D ViEW du laboratoire LCOMS. Un panel de 15 sujets valides a été recruté pour faire des tests de conduite sur simulateur et valider la modélisation OPCM. La distance Dp a permis de décrire le comportement du pilote. Ensuite, une analyse de la distance anticipée Dp en fonction de différentes zones du parcours (slalom serré, virage, slalom large), a montré que ce paramètre peut être utilisé pour différencier les différents comportements de conduite liées aux différentes situations du parcours. Ensuite nous avons comparé cette distance anticipée entre deux groupes, familiarisés et novices vis-à-vis de la conduite en FRE. L’analyse a montré que le groupe familiarisé présente une distance Dp plus importante que le groupe novice. Enfin, une dernière expérience a eu lieu au Centre de Réadaptation de Flavigny sur Moselle (54, France). Nous avons estimé la distance anticipée (Dp) pour des sujets à déficiences motrices sévères, afin de valider le paramètre Dp comme indicateur de performance. Une étude de cas a été établie portant sur 5 enfants en situation de handicap, en comparant leurs résultats à ceux obtenus précédemment sur un panel de personnes valides
... Mon-Williams and Bingham (2011) investigated reachesto-grasp from an affordance perspective to identify the scaling of the spatial structure relative to the collision avoidance and targeting goals. They found that the maximum grasp aperture (MGA) (the widest opening between the fingers and thumb before they start to close down on the object) reflects the collision avoidance goal, whereas the terminal grasp aperture (TGA) (when the hand stops moving but prior to fingers closing on the object) reflected the targeting goal (see also Bingham et al. 2008;Coats et al. 2008;Lee and Bingham 2010). They also found that the relevant property of the object for collision avoidance was the maximum diagonal distance through the object across which the participants grasped, called the maximum object extent (MOE), while the relevant object property for the targeting goal is the object's width. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mon-Williams and Bingham (Exp Brain Res 211(1):145–160, 2011) developed a geometrical affordance model for reaches-to-grasp, and identified a constant scaling relationship, P, between safety margins (SM) and available apertures (SM) that are determined by the sizes of the objects and the individual hands. Bingham et al. (J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 40(4):1542–1550, 2014) extended the model by introducing a dynamical component that scales the geometrical relationship to the stability of the reaching-to-grasp. The goal of the current study was to explore whether and how quickly change in the relevant effectivity (functionally determined hand size = maximum grip) would affect the geometrical and dynamical scaling relationships. The maximum grip of large-handed males was progressively restricted. Participants responded to this restriction by using progressively smaller safety margins, but progressively larger P (= SM/AA) values that preserved an invariant dynamical scaling relationship. The recalibration was relatively fast, occurring over five trials or less, presumably a number required to detect the variability or stability of performance. The results supported the affordance model for reaches-to-grasp in which the invariance is determined by the dynamical component, because it serves the goal of not colliding with the object before successful grasping can be achieved. The findings were also consistent with those of Snapp-Childs and Bingham (Exp Brain Res 198(4):527–533, 2009) who found changes in age-specific geometric scaling for stepping affordances as a function of changes in effectivities over the life span where those changes preserved a dynamic scaling constant similar to that in the current study.
... During a bimanual task that requires simultaneous reaching and grasping from both hands, the visual system is only physically able to focus on one visually-guided action at a time (i.e., the task essentially becomes two separate unimanual tasks due to this sequential dependence) (Bingham et al., 2008). It was hypothesized that visuomotor coupling delays movement time when completing a two-handed task using a single visuomotor system (i.e., bimanually) when compared to completing the two-handed task using two separate visuomotor systems (i.e., intermanually). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated previously observed differences in speed when completing a two-handed task using either the bimanual coordination mode (i.e., an individual completing a two-handed task) or the intermanual coordination mode (i.e., two people completing a two-handed task). When comparing these coordination modes, various research domains have reported an intermanual “mode effect” of speed. Research suggests that the difference in performance may depend on fundamental characteristics of each coordination mode that facilitate or impede speed during two-handed tasks. To further investigate this intermanual speed advantage, a task was constructed to exploit a hypothesized bimanual limitation that may underlie this mode effect: bimanual visuomotor coupling. Results replicated the intermanual speed advantage and showed a higher degree of visuomotor coupling during bimanual performance. Subsequent analyses suggest that speed during two-handed tasks may be a function of visuomotor coupling, regardless of coordination mode.
Article
Full-text available
Проводилось сравнение графической деятельности (рисование окружностей) левой руки при бимануальном условии с двигательной активностью этой же руки, но при унимануальном условии. Исследование выполнялось на 20 здоровых студентах-психологах (ср. возраст ~20 лет). Сравнительный анализ унимануальных и бимануальных графомоторных движений левой руки показал различия по таким показателям, как средняя скорость, стандартное отклонение от средней скорости, время движения и размер нарисованной фигуры. Анализ динамики двигательной активности левой руки для бимануального и унимануального условий в серии последовательных проб показал значимые изменения для бимануального условия по основным кинематическим показателям, для унимануального условия такой динамики не выявлено. Предполагается статус бимануальной координации как не сводимой к сумме координаций двух унимануальных движений.
Chapter
Full-text available
We argue that bimanual coordination and interference depends critically on how these actions are represented on a cognitive level. We first review the literature on spatial interactions, focusing on the difference between movements directed at visual targets and movements cued symbolically. Interactions manifest during response planning are limited to the latter condition. These results suggest that interactions in the formation of the trajectories of the two hands are associated with processes involved in response selection, rather than interactions in the motor system. Neuropsychological studies involving callosotomy patients argue that these interactions arise from transcallosal interactions between cortically-based spatial codes. The second half of the chapter examines temporal constraints observed in bimanual movements. We propose that most bimanual movements are marked by a common event structure, an explicit representation that ensures temporal coordination of the movements. The translation of an abstract event structure into a movement with a particular timing pattern is associated with cerebellar function, although the resulting temporal coupling during bimanual movements may be due to the operation of other subcortical mechanisms. For rhythmic movements that do not entail an event structure, timing may be an emergent property. Under such conditions, both spatial and temporal coupling can be absent. The emphasis on abstract levels of constraint makes clear that limitations in bimanual coordination overlap to a considerable degree with those observed in other domains of cognition.
Article
Full-text available
Vision is critical for the efficient execution of prehension movements, providing information about: The location of a target object with respect to the viewer; its spatial relationship to other objects; as well as intrinsic properties of the object such as its size and orientation. This paper reports three experiments which examined the role played by binocular vision in the execution of prehension movements. Specifically, transport and grasp kinematics were examined for prehension movements executed under binocular, monocular, and no vision (memory-guided and open-loop) viewing conditions. The results demonstrated an overall advantage for reaches executed under binocular vision; movement duration and the length of the deceleration phase were longer, and movement velocity reduced, when movements were executed with monocular vision. Furthermore, the results indicated that binocular vision is particularly important during ''selective'' reaching, that is reaching for target objects which are accompanied by flanker objects. These results are related to recent neuro psychological investigations suggesting that stereopsis may be critical for the visual control of prehension.
Article
Full-text available
Studies of human prehension have revealed characteristic patterns of grasping kinematics. We sought to gain insight into the determinants of those patterns by means of a computer simulation and accompanying behavioral experiment concerning multijoint, planar grasping behavior. The simulation was based on a recent theory of posture-based motion planning which hypothesizes that movement preparation entails time-limited, multiple task-constraint satisfaction. Prehension was modeled with a stick-figure animation involving 12 series of 81 grasping movements. Circular objects to be grasped were located at three angles (45°, 90°, and 135°) and at three distances (20 cm, 30 cm, and 40 cm) relative to the initial location of the hand in the workplane. Additionally, three object sizes (2 cm, 4 cm, and 6 cm in diameter) and three initial aperture sizes (0.3 cm, 3.3 cm, and 7.0 cm) were used. Analyses of the simulated grasping movements focused on the time course of the hand opening, the tangential velocity of the wrist, and the rotations of the joints in the arm, hand, and fingers. The results showed that the model accurately mimicked detailed kinematics of prehension observed in earlier studies. With respect to the frequently reported relationship between object size and hand opening, the simulations further revealed an effect of initial aperture. This predicted effect was confirmed in an experiment in which four participants performed analogous planar grasping tasks. An analysis of the time course of the opening of the hand showed that maximum aperture covaried with initial aperture. A conclusion of this work is that a major determinant of grasping kinematics is avoidance of collisions with objects that are to be grasped.
Article
Full-text available
Reaching and grasping an object can be viewed as the solution of a multiple-constraint satisfaction problem. The constraints include contact with the object with the appropriate effectors in the correct positions as well as generation of a collision-free trajectory. We have developed a computational model that simulates reaching and grasping based on these notions. The model, rendered as an animation program, reproduces many basic features of the kinematics of human reaching and grasping behavior. The core assumptions of the model are: (1) tasks are defined by flexibly organized constraint hierarchies; (2) manual positioning acts, including prehension acts, are first specified with respect to goal postures and then are specified with respect to movements towards those goal postures; (3) goal postures are found by identifying the stored posture that is most promising for the task, as determined by the constraint hierarchy, and then by generating postures that are more and more dissimilar to the most-promising stored posture until a deadline is reached, at which time the best posture that was found during the search is defined as the goal posture; (4) depending on when the best posture was encountered in the search, the deadline for the search in the next trial is either increased or decreased; (5) specification of a movement to the goal posture begins with straight-line interpolation in joint space between the starting posture and goal posture; (6) if an internal simulation of this default movement suggests that it will result in collision with an obstacle, the movement can be reshaped until an acceptable movement is found or until time runs out; (7) movement reshaping occurs by identifying a via posture that serves as a body position to which the actor moves from the starting posture and then back to the starting posture, while simultaneously making the main movement from the starting posture to the goal posture; (8) the via posture is identified using the same posture-generating algorithm as used to identify the goal posture. These processes are used both for arm positioning and, with some elaboration, for prehension. The model solves a number of problems with an earlier model, although it leaves some other problems unresolved.
Article
Previous work has established that judgments of relative phase variability of 2 visually presented oscillators covary with mean relative phase. Ninety degrees is judged to be more variable than 0° or 180°, independently of the actual level of phase variability. Judged levels of variability also increase at 180°. This pattern of judgments matches the pattern of movement coordination results. Here, participants judged the phase variability of their own finger movements, which they generated by actively tracking a manipulandum moving at 0°, 90°, or 180°, and with 1 of 4 levels of Phase Variability. Judgments covaried as an inverted U-shaped function of mean relative phase. With an increase in frequency, 180° was judged more variable whereas 0° was not. Higher frequency also reduced discrimination of the levels of Phase Variability. This matching of the proprioceptive and visual results, and of both to movement results, supports the hypothesized role of online perception in the coupling of limb movements. Differences in the 2 cases are discussed as due primarily to the different sensitivities of the systems to the information.
Article
Prehension involves processing information in two hypothesized visuomotor channels: one for extrinsic object properties (e.g., the spatial location of objects) and one for intrinsic objects properties (e.g., shape and size). The present study asked how the two motor components that correspond to these channels (transport and grasp, respectively) are related. One way to address this question is to create a situation where unexpected changes occur at the input level of one of the visuomotor channels, and to observe how the movement reorganizes. If transport and grasp are independent components, then changing the object location, for example, should affect only the transport, not the grasp component. Subjects were requested to reach, grasp and lift as accurately as possible one of three dowels using the distal pads of the thumb and index finger. On certain trials, upon movement initiation towards the middle dowel, the dowel was made to instantaneously change its location to one of the two other positions, requiring the subject to reorient the hand to the new dowel location. Results consisted of comparing the movement characteristics of the transport and grasp components of these perturbed movements with appropriate control movements. Kinematics of the wrist trajectory showed fast adjustments, within 100 ms, to the change of dowel position. This duration seems to correspond to the minimum delay required within the visuomotor system for visual and/or proprioceptive reafferents to influence the ongoing movement. In addition, these delays are much shorter than has been found for conditions where object location changes before movement initiation (approximately 300 ms). The faster times may relate to the dynamic character of the deviant limb position signals, with the only constraint being the physiological delays for visual and kinaesthetic signals to influence the movement. A spatiotemporal variability analysis of the movement trajectories for non-perturbed trials showed variability to be greatest during the acceleration part of the movement, interpreted as due to control by a relatively inaccurate directional coding mechanism. Control during the deceleration phase, marked by low trajectory variability, was seen to be due to a sensorimotor process, using motor output signals, and resulting in an optimized trajectory supporting a successful grasp. Analysis of the grasp component of prehension showed that perturbing object location influenced the movement of the fingers suggesting a kinematic coupling of the two components. However, forthcoming work shows that, when object size changes, and location remains constant, there is a clear temporal dissociation of the two components of prehension. Collectively, these results suggest that the two visuomotor channels have different time constraints with the time-constant of the channel activated by the perturbation constraining the timing of the other.
Article
Rhythmic bimanual movement has been a paradigm system in the study of percep- tion-action. In perception-action, movement generates information that is then used in turn to guide the movement. The result is complex behavior emerging from relatively simple dynamical organization. Nonlinear dynamics has been used to de- scribe the organization, but the models thus far have not much featured the role of information so that the information has not been investigated. Therefore, we pro- pose a new perceptually driven dynamical model. We first review our perceptual judgment studies of coordinated rhythmic movements. We then describe a percep- tually driven model of single and coupled oscillators. Finally, we explore the impli- cations for investigation of information. The main variable in the study of coordination of human rhythmic move- ments is relative phase, that is, the relative position of two oscillating limbs within an oscillatory cycle. For people without special skills (e.g., jazz drum- ming), the cardinal phenomena in bimanual coordination are (a) at preferred frequency, only two relative phases are stably produced, at 0° and 180° (preferred frequency is about 1 Hz); (b) other relative phases can be produced on average when people follow metronomes, but the movements exhibit large amounts of phase variability; and (c) as frequency is increased beyond preferred, phase vari- ability of 180° increases and movement switches to 0° when frequency reaches about 3 to 4 Hz. With the switch, phase variability drops. These phenomena were produced by the Haken-Kelso-Bunz (HKB; Haken, Kelso, & Bunz, 1985) model shown in Figure 1. The potential function in this model represents the en- ergy required to maintain a given phase. The HKB simply assumes this function.
Article
We review the properties of coordinated rhythmic bimanual movements and previous models of those movements. Those models capture the phenomena but they fail to show how the behaviors arise from known components of the perception/ action system and in particular, they do not explicitly represent the known perceptual coupling of the limb movements. We review our own studies on the perception of relative phase and use the results to motivate a new perceptually driven model of bimanual coordination. The new model and its behaviors are described. The model captures both the phenomena of bimanual coordination found in motor studies and the pattern of judgments of mean relative phase and of phase variability found in perception studies.
Article
We have investigated how the control of hand transport and of hand aperture are coordinated in prehensile movements by delivering mechanical perturbations to the hand transport component and looking for coordinated adjustments in hand aperture. An electric actuator attached to the subject's right arm randomly pulled the subject backwards, away from the target, or pushed them towards it, during a quarter of the experimental trials. A compensatory adjustment of hand aperture followed the immediate, mechanical effects of the perturbation of hand transport. The adjustment appeared to return the subject towards a stereotyped spatial relation between hand aperture and hand transport. These spatial patterns suggest how the two components may be coordinated during prehension. A simple model of this coordination, based on coupled position feedback systems, is presented.