ArticlePDF Available

Short Term Meditation Training Improves Attention and Self-Regulation

Authors:
  • Dalian Blood Center,China

Abstract and Figures

Recent studies suggest that months to years of intensive and systematic meditation training can improve attention. However, the lengthy training required has made it difficult to use random assignment of participants to conditions to confirm these findings. This article shows that a group randomly assigned to 5 days of meditation practice with the integrative body-mind training method shows significantly better attention and control of stress than a similarly chosen control group given relaxation training. The training method comes from traditional Chinese medicine and incorporates aspects of other meditation and mindfulness training. Compared with the control group, the experimental group of 40 undergraduate Chinese students given 5 days of 20-min integrative training showed greater improvement in conflict scores on the Attention Network Test, lower anxiety, depression, anger, and fatigue, and higher vigor on the Profile of Mood States scale, a significant decrease in stress-related cortisol, and an increase in immunoreactivity. These results provide a convenient method for studying the influence of meditation training by using experimental and control methods similar to those used to test drugs or other interventions.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Short-term meditation training improves attention
and self-regulation
Yi-Yuan Tang*
†‡§¶
, Yinghua Ma*, Junhong Wang*, Yaxin Fan*, Shigang Feng*, Qilin Lu*, Qingbao Yu*, Danni Sui*,
Mary K. Rothbart
, Ming Fan
, and Michael I. Posner
†¶
*Institute of Neuroinformatics and Laboratory for Body and Mind, Dalian University of Technology, Dalian 116023, China; Department of Psychology,
University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403; Key Laboratory for Mental Health, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China; §Center for Social
and Organizational Behavior, Graduate University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100080, China; and Institute of Basic Medical Sciences,
Beijing 100850, China
Contributed by Michael I. Posner, August 16, 2007 (sent for review July 26, 2007)
Recent studies suggest that months to years of intensive and
systematic meditation training can improve attention. However,
the lengthy training required has made it difficult to use random
assignment of participants to conditions to confirm these findings.
This article shows that a group randomly assigned to 5 days of
meditation practice with the integrative body–mind training
method shows significantly better attention and control of stress
than a similarly chosen control group given relaxation training. The
training method comes from traditional Chinese medicine and
incorporates aspects of other meditation and mindfulness training.
Compared with the control group, the experimental group of 40
undergraduate Chinese students given 5 days of 20-min integrative
training showed greater improvement in conflict scores on the
Attention Network Test, lower anxiety, depression, anger, and
fatigue, and higher vigor on the Profile of Mood States scale, a
significant decrease in stress-related cortisol, and an increase in
immunoreactivity. These results provide a convenient method for
studying the influence of meditation training by using experimen-
tal and control methods similar to those used to test drugs or other
interventions.
anterior cingulate gyrus attention training control mental training
In recent years, a number of articles have demonstrated the
benefits of various forms of meditation and mindfulness
training (1–8). Many of them have tested practitioners of
different meditative practices and compared them with controls
without any training. Others have compared groups that were
equivalent in performance before training but had chosen
whether or not to undertake training (2, 4, 9, 10).
In a well done study (2), the experimental group received 3
months of training. The control group c onsisted of matched persons
wanting to learn about meditation. That study used a strong assay
of attention, the attentional blink (11); the two groups performed
equally before meditation. The experimental group was signifi-
cantly better after meditation, suggesting that meditation caused an
improvement in the executive attention network, most probably in
the form of attention used by this task (2).
Nevertheless, problems of subtle differences between control
and experimental groups remain in the study discussed above,
because the two groups were not completely randomly assigned,
and meditators differed greatly in styles of meditation previously
practiced (2). Slagter et al. (2) said that ‘‘the absence of an
association between the amount of prior meditation training and
our study results may be due to the fact that there was great
variation across the practitioners in the styles and traditions of
the previously learned meditation. Longitudinal research exam-
ining and comparing the effects of different styles of meditation
on brain and mental function and the duration of such effects is
needed.’’
Styles of meditation differ. Some techniques such as concentra-
tion meditation, mantra, mindfulness meditation, etc. rely on mind
control or thought work, including focus on an object, paying
attention to the present moment, etc. (2, 12–15). Mental training
methods also share several key components, such as body relax-
ation, breathing practice, mental imagery, and mindfulness, etc.,
which can help and accelerate practitioner access to meditative
states (3, 8, 16–19). This background raises the possibility that
combining several key components of body and mind techniques
with features of meditation and mindfulness traditions, while
reducing reliance on control of thoughts, may be easier to teach to
novices because they would not have to struggle so hard to control
their thoughts. Therefore, integrative body–mind training (IBMT;
or simply integrative meditation) was developed in the 1990s, and
its effects have been studied in China since 1995. Based on the
results from hundreds of adults and children ranging from 4 to 90
years old in China, IBMT practice improves emotional and cogni-
tive performance and social behavior (20, 21).
IBMT achieves the desired state by first giving a brief instruc-
tional period on the method (we call it initial mind setting and
its goal is to induce a cognitive or emotional set that will
influence the training). The method stresses no effort to control
thoughts, but instead a state of restful alertness that allows a high
degree of awareness of body, breathing, and external instructions
from a compact disc. It stresses a balanced state of relaxation
while focusing attention. Thought control is achieved gradually
through posture and relaxation, body–mind harmony, and bal-
ance with the help of the coach rather than by making the trainee
attempt an internal struggle to control thoughts in accordance
with instruction.
Training in this method is followed by 5 days of group practice,
during which a coach answers questions and observes facial and
body cues to identify those people who are struggling with the
method. The trainees concentrate on achieving a balanced state
of mind while being guided by the coach and the compact disc
that teaches them to relax, adjust their breathing, and use mental
imagery. Because this approach is suitable for novices, we
hypothesized that a short period of training and practice might
influence the efficiency of the executive attention network
related to self-regulation (22).
In the present study, we used a random assignment of 40
Chinese undergraduates to an experimental group and 40 to a
control group for 5 days of training 20 min per day. The
experimental group was given a short term of IMBT (module
one) (20, 21). Training was presented in a standardized way by
compact disc and guided by a skillful IBMT coach. Because of
Author contributions: J.W. and Y.F. contributed equally to this work; Y.-Y.T., M.F., and
M.I.P. designed research; Y.-Y.T., Y.M., J.W., Y.F., S.F., Q.L., Q.Y., and D.S. performed
research; Y.-Y.T., Y.M., J.W., Y.F., S.F., Q.L., and Q.Y. analyzed data; and Y.-Y.T., M.K.R., and
M.I.P. wrote the paper.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Abbreviations: IBMT, integrative body–mind training; ANT, Attention Network Test; POMS,
Profile of Mood States; sIgA, secretory IgA.
To whom correspondence may be addressed. E-mail: yiyuan@uoregon.edu or mposner@
uoregon.edu.
© 2007 by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA
17152–17156
PNAS
October 23, 2007
vol. 104
no. 43 www.pnas.orgcgidoi10.1073pnas.0707678104
their importance the coaches generally have several years of
experience with IBMT. The control group was given a form
of relaxation training very popular in the West (17, 23).
The two groups were given a battery of tests 1 week before
training and immediately after the final training session. A
standard computerized attention test measured orienting, alert-
ing, and the ability to resolve conf lict (executive attention). The
Attention Network Test (ANT) involves responding to an arrow
target that is surrounded by f lankers that point either in the same
or opposite direction. Cues provide information on when a trial
will occur and where the target will be (24). The Raven’s
Standard Progressive Matrix (25, 26), which is a standard culture
fair intelligence test; an assay of mood state, the Profile of Mood
States (POMS) (27, 28); and a stress challenge of a mental
arithmetic task were followed by measures of cortisol and
secretory IgA (sIgA) (29–34). All of these are standard assays
scored objectively by people blind to the experimental condition.
See details in Materials and Methods.
In prior work (24), ANT has been used to measure skill in the
resolution of mental conf lict induced by competing stimuli. It
activates a frontal brain network involving the anterior cingulate
gyrus and lateral prefrontal cortex (35, 36). Our underlying
theory was that IBMT should improve functioning of this
executive attention network, which has been linked to better
regulation of cognition and emotion (22, 37). Specifically, we
hypothesized that (i) because assignment was random, the
training and control group should not differ before training, (ii)
the training would improve executive attention (indexed by ANT
and POMS scales related to self-regulation and Raven’s Stan-
dard Progressive Matrices) in the experimental group more than
for controls, and (iii) if self-regulation ability improved in the
training group, members of the group should also show reduced
reaction to stress as measured by cortisol and immunoreactivity
assays. In short, the experimental group would show greater
improvement in the executive attention network related to
self-regulation; the Raven’s intelligence test, which is known to
differ with improved executive attention; mood scales related
to self-control; and cortisol and immunoreactivity measures of
stress to a mental arithmetic challenge.
Results
ANOVAs were conducted with group (trained and control) and
training session (before and after) as factors, using each of the
attention network scores as dependent variables. Before train-
ing, no differences were found for alerting, orienting, and
executive networks in two groups (P0.05). The main effect of
the training session was significant only for the executive net-
work [F(1,78) 9.859; P0.01]. More importantly, the group
session interaction was significant for the executive network
[F(1,78) 10.839; P0.01], indicating that the before vs. after
difference in the conf lict resolution score was significant only for
the trained group (see Fig. 1). The groups did not differ in
orienting or alerting after training (P0.05). The result
demonstrated that the short-term IBMT practice can influence
the efficiency of executive attention.
Because the efficiency of executive attention improved, we
expected better self-regulation of emotion. We used the POMS
to measure emotion in the same two groups. Before training,
none of the six scales of POMS showed differences between the
two groups (P0.05). The ANOVAs revealed a group session
effect for anger–hostility (A) [F(1,78) 5.558; P0.05],
depression–dejection (D) [F(1,78) 5.558; P0.05], tension–
anxiety (T) [F(1,78) 11.920; P0.01], and vigor–activity ( V)
[F(1,78) 7.479; P0.01]. After training, the ttest indicated
there were significant differences in the experimental group (but
not the control group) in A, D, T, V, and F (fatigue–inertia); in
general, P(positive mood)
average
0.01 (see Fig. 2). The result
indicated that short-term IBMT can enhance positive moods and
reduce negative ones.
In previous work, the network associated with executive
attention has been related to intelligence (38, 39). We also tested
the hypothesis that improvement of efficiency of executive
attention accompanies higher intelligence scores. Scores of
the Raven’s Matrices did not differ before training (P0.05).
The ANOVAs revealed the main effect of the training session;
the Raven’s improvement was significant [F(1,46) 10.171; P
0.01], but the group session interaction was not significant,
although there was a trend in that direction [F(1,46) 3.102; P
0.085]. The ttests showed a significant improvement in the
experimental group after training (P0.001), but no significant
improvement for the control group (P0.05). The result
revealed that short-term IBMT can improve the Raven’s score
in the experimental group, although only marginally more so
than in the control group.
Cortisol and sIgA are indexes of the amount of stress induced
by a cognitive challenge (29 –34). We applied 3 min of mental
arithmetic as an acute stress after 5 days of IBMT or relaxation.
Fig. 3 shows that at baseline before stress there was no significant
difference between the groups (P0.05). This finding indicated
no sensitivity for cortisol and sIgA reactions to stress under
normal states as found by others (32, 34). After the arithmetic
Fig. 1. Performance of the ANT after 5 days of IBMT or control. Error bars
indicate 1 SD. Vertical axis indicates the difference in mean reaction time
between the congruent and incongruent flankers. The higher scores show less
efficient resolution of conflict.
Fig. 2. Comparison of six scales of the POMS before and after training for the
experimental group. Blue bar, five negative moods and one positive mood
pretraining; red bar, five negative moods posttraining; green bar, one positive
mood posttraining. Significance was found in POMS scales of anger–hostility
(A), depression–dejection (D), fatigue–inertia (F), tension–anxiety (T), and
vigor–activity (V) posttraining in the experimental group. No significant dif-
ference was found in POMS scale C (confusion–bewilderment) posttraining.
**,Paverage 0.01. Error bars indicate 1 SD.
Tang et al. PNAS
October 23, 2007
vol. 104
no. 43
17153
PSYCHOLOGYNEUROSCIENCE
challenge, both groups increased in cortisol activity, indicating
that the mental arithmetic challenge was stressful (30). Then the
experimental group received an additional 20 min of IBMT and
the control 20 min of relaxation training. An ANOVA showed
that the group (experimental vs. control) session (baseline
before stress vs. after additional training) interaction was sig-
nificant [F(1,38) 6.281; P0.01]. The experimental group had
a significantly lowered cortisol response to the mental stress
after training than did the control group (see Fig. 3A).
Similarly, there was no significant difference between groups
in sIgA at baseline before stress (P0.05). However, the mental
arithmetic task resulted in a increase of sIgA relative to the
baseline as found by others (34). After training, ANOVA showed
the group session interaction was significant [F(1,38)
10.060; P0.001], with the arithmetic challenge resulting in
significantly greater sIgA for the experimental group than the
control group (see Fig. 3B).
Five days of training reduced the stress response to the mental
challenge especially after an additional 20 min of practice.
Discussion
In the ANT and POMS, the experimental group showed signif-
icantly greater improvement after 5 days of IBMT than the
relaxation control group. Because we randomly assigned subjects
to experimental and control groups and used objective tests with
researchers blind to the condition, we conclude that IBMT
improved attention and self-regulation more than the relaxation
control. The reaction to a mental stress was also significantly
improved in the experimental group, which showed less cortisol
and more immunoreactivity than the control group after the
additional training. These outcomes after only 5 days of training
open a door for simple and effective investigation of meditation
effects. The IBMT provides a convenient method for studying
the influence of meditation training by using appropriate ex-
perimental and control methods similar to those used to test
drugs or other interventions. Our findings further indicate the
potential of IBMT for stress management, body–mind health,
and improvement in cognitive performance and self-regulation
(20, 21).
Although no direct measures of brain changes were used in
this study, some previous studies suggest that changes in brain
networks can occur. Thomas et al. (40) showed that, in rats, one
short experience of acute exposure to psychosocial stress re-
duced both short- and long-term survival of newborn hippocam-
pal neurons. Similarly, the human brain is sensitive to short
experience. Naccache et al. (41) showed that the subliminal
presentation of emotional words (100 ms) modulates the
activity of the amygdala at a long latency and triggers long-lasting
cerebral processes (41). Brefczynski-Lewis et al. (42) compared
novices, who participated in meditation 1 h per day by using
three different techniques with expert meditators who had
10,000–54,000 h of practice. Both groups showed activation of a
large overlapping network of attention-related brain regions
(42). Another situation where a single session changed brain
processes is described by Raz et al. (43). Highly hypnotizable
persons when given an instruction to view a word as nonsense
showed elimination of the Stroop interference effect and also
eliminated activity in the anterior cingulate during conflict trials
(43). It was also demonstrated that 5 days of attention training
with a computer program improved the efficiency of the exec-
utive attention networks for children (39). Taken together, we
have reason to believe that 5 days of IBMT practice could change
brain networks, leading to improvements in attention, cognition,
and emotion and reaction to stress.
Why does IBMT work after only a few days of practice whereas
studies with other methods often require months? The following
are possible reasons.
First, IBMT integrates several key components of body and
mind techniques including body relaxation (17), breathing ad-
justment (18), mental imagery (44, 45), and mindfulness training
(12–15), which have shown broad positive effects in attention,
emotions, and social behaviors in previous studies (1– 8). This
combination may amplify the training effect over the use of only
one of these components.
Second, because everyone experiences mindfulness some-
times (12, 13, 19, 46, 47), a qualified coach can help each
participant increase the amount of this experience and thus
guarantee that each practice session achieves a good result (20,
21). For participants with months to years of meditation, there
has been the opportunity to make mistakes, correct them, and
gradually find the right way. For 5 days of training, quality
practice is needed for every session. Recent findings indicate that
the amount of time participants spent meditating each day,
rather than total number of hours of meditative practice over
their lifetime, affects performance on attentional tasks (10).
Third, in one study a selected comfortable music background
showed more effects than Mozart’s music (21). Having music on
the compact disc integrates the practice instruction and occupies
the novice’s wandering mind via continuous sensory input,
maintaining and facilitating the mindful state. Many meditation
Fig. 3. Physiological changes before and after stress. (A) Comparison of
cortisol concentration between the experimental group (red bars) and control
group (gray bars) in three different stages after 5 days of training. *,P0.05;
**,P0.01. Error bars indicate 1 SEM. More cortisol secretion indicates higher
levels of stress. (B) Comparison of sIgA concentration between experimental
group (red bars) and control group (blue bars) at three different stages after
5 days of training. **,P0.01. Error bars indicate 1 SEM. Higher immunore-
activity indicates a better response to stress.
17154
www.pnas.orgcgidoi10.1073pnas.0707678104 Tang et al.
training methods use audiotapes or compact discs to help
beginners (13, 46, 48).
We do not know which of these features is of greatest
importance in obtaining change, but we believe that the inte-
gration of the various methods into one easy-to-use training
package may explain why IBMT is effective at such a low dose.
We also regard the work of the trainer as critical. The trainer
needs to know how to interact with the trainees to obtain the
desired state. Although the trainers are not present during the
training sessions, they observe the trainees over television and
help them after the session with problems. The trainers could
well be a part of the effective ingredient of IBMT, and their role
requires additional research.
We have thus far assessed the utility of IBMT with random
assignment only for Chinese undergraduates. However, we do
not believe that any meditation method is appropriate for every
person. In other work, similar, but more preliminary, effects
have been observed for Chinese children and adults of many
ages. Preliminary data from studies in the United States suggest
wider utility of IBMT (20, 21). Some may argue that IBMT
effects require a prior belief in the benefits of meditation that
would be common in China. However, belief in meditation and
traditional medicine is not high among undergraduates in mod-
ern China (49). Also a relaxation group used as a control failed
to achieve significant improvement. One potentially important
difference between Eastern and Western meditation studies is
that in China we used group training at the same time of day for
each session over 5 days; most of the Western studies used
individual meditation at different times. Studies of group dy-
namics indicate that the group acts to facilitate outcomes (50),
and possible differences between individual and group admin-
istration of mediation needs further study. Studies of perception,
language, mathematics, and psychopathology suggest differences
between Americans and Chinese within each of these domains
(51–54). Thus culture-specific experiences may subtly shape
cognition and direct neural activity in precise ways. If differences
between Chinese and American studies of attention training
arise, further studies will be needed to determine the reasons.
In previous work, executive attention has been shown to be an
important mechanism for self-regulation of cognition and emo-
tion (22, 37, 39). The current results with the ANT indicate that
IBMT improves functioning of this executive attention network.
Studies designed to improve executive attention in young chil-
dren showed more adult-like scalp electrical recordings related
to an important node of the executive attention network in the
anterior cingulate gyrus (22, 37, 39). We expect that imaging
studies with adults would show changes in the activation or
connectivity of this network after IBMT. Such studies would help
to determine the mechanisms by which IBMT improves perfor-
mance. They may also provide a good objective basis for
comparison between training methods.
In summary, IBMT is an easy, effective way for improvement
in self-regulation in cognition, emotion, and social behavior. Our
study is consistent with the idea that attention, affective pro-
cesses, and the quality of moment-to-moment awareness are
flexible skills that can be trained (55, 56).
Materials and Methods
Participants. Eighty healthy undergraduates at the Dalian Uni-
versity of Technology [44 males, mean age (SD) 21.8 0.55]
without any training experiences participated in this study. They
were randomly assigned to an experimental or control group
(40:40). Forty experimental subjects continuously attended
IBMT for 5 days with 20 min of training per day. Forty control
subjects were given the same number and length of group
sessions but received information from the compact disc about
relaxation of each body part. The coach observed them from a
closed-circuit television and provided answers to any question
after each training session. The human experiment was approved
by a local Institutional Review Board, and informed consent was
obtained from each participant. ANOVA and ttests were
applied for analysis.
Measures. The ANT was administered before and after training
(24). Each person received 248 trials during each assessment
session. Subtraction of reaction times was used to obtain scores
for each attentional network, including alerting, orienting, and
conflict resolution (24).
The same two groups of 80 subjects took POMS (27, 28), and
48 subjects [28 males, mean age (SD) 21.7 0.53]
participated in the Raven’s Matrices (25, 26) separately before
and after 5 days of training.
Half of the experimental or control group [20:20, 26 males,
mean age (SD) 21.9 0.97] was chosen randomly to
participate in the physiological measures. Mental arithmetic was
used as an acute stressor after 5 days of IBMT and relaxation in
two groups (29 –34). Subjects were instructed to perform serial
subtraction of 47 from a four-digit number and respond verbally.
During the 3-min mental arithmetic task, participants were
prompted to be as fast and accurate as possible. If the partici-
pants did not finish the mental arithmetic in time and correctly,
the computer would produce a harsh sound to remind the
subjects, who were required to restart the task and do it again.
Cortisol and sIgA measures were taken during three periods:
baseline before stress, after mental stress, and after additional
20-min training. First, all subjects were given a 5-min rest to get
baseline. Second, all subjects were instructed to finish a 3-min
mental arithmetic task to test whether there were different stress
reactions between two groups. Third, the experimental group
practiced an additional 20-min IBMT, whereas the control group
relaxed for 20 min to test whether additional training could
improve the alterations based on 5 days of training.
To control for variations of cortisol levels over the circadian
rhythm, the math stress was performed from 2 p.m. to 6 p.m.
(32). Saliva samples were collected immediately after each
period by one-off injectors and were encased in test tubes in
succession, with the tubes placed into a refrigerator under 20°C
and then thawed 24 h later for analysis. The concentration of
cortisol and sIgA was analyzed by RIA at Dalian Medical
University (31–34).
IBMT Method. IBMT involves several body–mind techniques in-
cluding: (i) body relaxation, (ii) breath adjustment, (iii) mental
imagery, and (iv) mindfulness training, accompanied with se-
lected music background. In this study, IBMT module one was
used. A compact disc was developed for module one that
included background music. IBMT module one practice in-
cluded (i) presession, (ii) practice session, and (iii) postsession.
In the presession, usually 1 day before the experiment, the coach
gathered subjects to have a free question-and-answer meeting about
IBMT practice via coaching techniques to ensure the clear grasp of
IBMT for the novices. The coach also set up the exact time, training
room, and discipline for the group practice. The most important
thing for the coach was to create a harmonious and relaxed
atmosphere for effective practice (20, 21).
In the training session, subjects followed the compact disc with
body posture adjustment, breathing practice, guided imagery,
and mindfulness training accompanied by a music background.
Y.-Y.T. gave the practice instructions on the compact disc
himself. The practice time was 20 min for 5 days. During the
training session, the coach observed facial and body cues to
identify those who were struggling with the method and gave
proper feedback immediately in postsession.
In the postsession, every subject filled out a questionnaire and
evaluated the practice. The coach gave short responses to
subjects as required.
Tang et al. PNAS
October 23, 2007
vol. 104
no. 43
17155
PSYCHOLOGYNEUROSCIENCE
IBMT belongs to body–mind science in the ancient Eastern
tradition. Chinese tradition and culture is not only a theory of
being but also (most importantly) a life experience and practice.
The IBMT method comes from traditional Chinese medicine,
but also uses the idea of human in harmony with nature in
Taoism and Confucianism, etc. The goal of IBMT is to serve as
a self-regulation practice for body–mind health and balance and
well being and to promote body–mind science research.
IBMT has three levels of training: (i) body–mind health, (ii)
body–mind balance, and (iii) body–mind purification for adults
and one level of health and wisdom for children. In each level,
IBMT has theories and several core techniques packaged in
compact discs or audiotapes that are instructed and guided by a
qualified coach. A person who achieves the three levels of full
training after theoretical and practical tests can apply for in-
structor status.
IBMT involves learning that requires experience and explicit
instruction. To ensure appropriate experience, coaches (quali-
fied instructors) are trained to help novices practice IBMT
properly. Instructors received training on how to interact with
experimental and control groups to make sure they understand
the training program exactly. After each training session, the
instructors gave brief and immediate responses to questions
raised by the participants, helped those who were observed to be
having difficulties, and asked each participant to fill out a
questionnaire and make any comments. The most important
thing for coaches was to create a harmonious and relaxed
atmosphere and give proper feedback for effective practice. The
coach believes everyone has full potential and equality and that
his/her job is to find and enjoy a person’s inner beauty and
capacities to help them think better and unfold their potentials
rather than to teach them.
A qualified coach is very important for each level of teaching
and practice. Without coaching, it is impossible or very difficult
to practice IBMT with only compact discs.
We thank the Institute of Neuroinformatics staff for assist ance with data
collection and Kewei Chen and the RIA division at Dalian Medical
University for technical assistance. This work was supported in part by
National Natural Science Foundation of China Grant 30670699, Na-
tional 863 Plan Project Grant 2006AA02Z431, Ministr y of Education
Grant NCET-06-0277, and a Brain, Biology, and Machine Initiative
grant from the University of Oregon.
1. Lutz A, Dunne JD, Davidson RJ (2007) in The Cambridge Handbook of
Consciousness, eds Zelazo PD, Moscovitch M, Thompson E (Cambridge Univ
Press, Cambridge, UK), in press.
2. Slagter HA, Lutz A, Greischar LL, Francis AD, Nieuwenhuis S, Dav is JM,
Davidson RJ (2007) PLoS Biol 5:e138.
3. Walsh R, Shapiro SL (2006) Am Psychol 61:227–239.
4. Davidson RJ, Kabat-Zinn J, Schumacher J, Rosenkranz M, Muller D, San-
torelli SF, Urbanowski F, Harrington A, Bonus K, Sheridan JF (2003)
Psychosom Med 65:564–570.
5. Baer R (2003) Clin Psychol Sci Practice 10:125–143.
6. Grossman P, Niemann L, Schmidt S, Walach H (2004) J Psychosom Res
57:35–43.
7. Deshmukh VD (2006) Sci World J 6:2239 –2253.
8. Wallace BA, Shapiro SL (2006) Am Psychol 61:690 –701.
9. Jain S, Shapiro SL, Swanick S, Roesch SC, Mills PJ, Bell I, Schwartz GE (2007)
Ann Behav Med 33:11–21.
10. Chan D, Woollacott M (2007) J Alt Complement Med, in press.
11. Shapiro KL, Arnell KA, Raymond JE (1997) Trends Cognit Sci 1:291–296.
12. Hart W (1987) The Art of Living: Vipassana Meditation: As Taught by SN Goenka
(HarperOne, New York).
13. Kabat-Zinn J (1994) Wherever You Go, There You Are: Mindfulness Meditation
in Everyday Life (Hyperion, New York).
14. Langer EJ (1990) Mindfulness (Addison–Wesley, Reading, MA).
15. Wallace BA (2006) The Attention Revolution: Unlocking the Power of the
Focused Mind (Wisdom, Somerville, M A).
16. Liu Y, Vian K, Eckman P, Fang T, Chen L (1988) The Essential Book of
Traditional Chinese Medicine (Columbia Univ Press, New York).
17. Benson H (1975) The Relaxation Response (Avon, New York).
18. Lusk JT (2005) Yoga Meditations (Whole Person Associates, Duluth, MN).
19. Suzuki S (1973) Zen Mind, Beginner’s Mind (Weatherhill, Boston, MA).
20. Tang Y (2005) Health from Brain, Wisdom from Brain (Dalian University of
Technology Press, Dalian, China).
21. Tang Y (2007) Multi-Intelligence and Unfolding the Full Potentials of Brain
(Dalian University of Technology Press, Dalian, China).
22. Posner MI, Rothbart MK (2007) Educating the Human Brain (APA Books,
Washington, DC).
23. Bernstein DA, Borkovec TD (1973) Progressive Relaxation Training (Research
Press, Champaign, IL).
24. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI (2002) J Cognit Neurosci
14:340–347.
25. Raven JC (1938) Progressive Matrices: A Perceptual Test of Intelligence (HK
Lewis, London).
26. Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH (2003) Manual for Raven’s P rogressive Matrices
and Vocabulary Scales, Section 1: General Overview (Harcourt Assessment, San
Antonio, TX).
27. Shacham S (1983) J Personality Assessment 47:305–306.
28. Spinella M (2007) J Gen Psychol 134:101–111.
29. Kirschbaum C, Pirke KM, Hellhammer DH (1993) Neuropsychobiology 28:76
81.
30. Wang J, Rao H, Wetmore GS, Furlan PM, Korczykowski M, Dinges DF, Detre
JA (2005) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:17804–17809.
31. Polk DE, Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Skoner DP, Kirschbaum C (2005) Psychoneu-
roendocrinology 30:261–272.
32. Hammerfald K, Eberle C, Grau M, Kinsperger A, Zimmermann A, Ehlert U,
Gaab J (2006) Psychoneuroendocrinology 31:333–339.
33. Deinzer R, Kleineidam C, Stiller-Winkler R, Idel H, Bachg D (2000) Int
J Psychophysiol 37:219–232.
34. Ring C, Drayson M, Walkey DG, Dale S, Carroll D (2002) Biol Psychol 59:1–13.
35. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI (2005) NeuroImage
26:471–479.
36. Posner MI, Sheese BE, Odludas Y, Tang Y (2006) Neural Netw 19:1422–1429.
37. Posner MI, Rothbart MK, Sheese BE, Tang Y (2007) Cognit Affect Behav
Neurosci, in press.
38. Duncan J, Seitz RJ, Kolodny J, Bor D, Herzog H, Ahmed A, Newell FN, Emslie
H (2000) Science 289:457–460.
39. Rueda MR, Rothbart MK, McCandliss BD, Saccomanno L, Posner MI (2005)
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:14931–14936.
40. Thomas RM, Hotsenpiller G, Peterson DA (2007) J Neurosci 27:2734 –2743.
41. Naccache L, Gaillard RL, Adam C, Hasboun D, Clemenceau S, Baulac M,
Dehaene S, Cohen L (2005) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:7713–7717.
42. Brefczynski-Lewis JA, Lutz A, Schaefer HS, Lev inson DB, Davidson RJ (2007)
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:11483–11488.
43. Raz A, Fan J, Posner MI (2005) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:9978–9983.
44. McKinney CH, Antoni MH, Kumar M, Tims FC, McCabe PM (1997) Health
Psychol 16:390– 400.
45. Watanabe E, Fukuda S, Shirakawa T (2005) BMC Complementary Alt Med 5:21.
46. Goldstein J (2003) Insight Meditation: The Practice of Freedom (Shambhala,
Boston).
47. Lao T, Mitchell S (1988) Tao Te Ching (HarperCollins, New York).
48. Salzberg S, Goldstein J (2002) Insight Meditation: A Step-by Step Course on How
to Mediatate (Sounds True, New York).
49. Zhang G (2006) Med Philos (Humanistic Soc Med Ed) 27:14 –17.
50. Heathfield SM (2002) How to Build a Teamwork Culture: Do the Hard Stuff
(Human Resources About.com, New York).
51. Siok W T, Perfetti CA, Jin Z, Tan LH (2004) Nature 431:71–76.
52. Tang Y, Zhang W, Chen K, Feng S, Ji Y, Shen J, Reiman EM, Liu Y (2006)
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:10775–10780.
53. Nisbett R E, Masuda T (2003) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:11163–11170.
54. Draguns JG, Tanaka-Matsumi J (2003) Behav Res Ther 41:755–776.
55. Tononi G, Edelman GM (1998) Science 282:1846–1851.
56. Posner MI, DiGirolamo GJ, Fernandez-Duque D (1997) Consciousness Cognit
6:267–290.
17156
www.pnas.orgcgidoi10.1073pnas.0707678104 Tang et al.
... These skills are crucial in the learning context as they allow students to better understand the material, complete tasks more efficiently, and achieve better academic results. Research by Tang et al. (2007) shows that meditation can improve attention and self-control, which contributes to increased focus and concentration among students. ...
... The effectiveness of this meditation and relaxation application will be measured using various validated and reliable tools. The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) by Cohen et al. (1983) will be used to measure students' stress levels, while the Focus and Concentration Scale by Tang et al. (2007) will be used to assess changes in students' focus and concentration before and after using the application. Additionally, in-depth interviews and focused group discussions will be conducted to collect qualitative data on students' and teachers' experiences using this application. ...
... Additionally, the Focus and Concentration Scale by Tang et al. (2007) is used to assess changes in students' ability to focus and concentrate. This scale measures students' ability to maintain attention on specific tasks and manage distractions during the learning process. ...
Article
Full-text available
The meditation and relaxation application specifically designed to help students manage stress and improve focus during learning is a technological innovation aimed at supporting students' mental and academic well-being. This application offers a variety of features, including guided meditation sessions, breathing exercises, and relaxation techniques designed to reduce anxiety and stress commonly experienced by students. Additionally, the application provides tools to help students enhance concentration and focus during the learning process through specially designed programs based on scientific research on the effects of meditation and relaxation on cognitive performance. With a user-friendly interface and easy access via mobile devices, this application allows students to seamlessly integrate meditation and relaxation practices into their daily routines. Initial studies indicate that regular use of this application can yield significant benefits in terms of stress reduction, improved focus, and enhanced sleep quality. The implementation of this application in educational settings is expected to create a more positive and productive learning environment, thereby supporting academic achievement and overall student well-being. This initiative responds to the growing need for effective solutions to mental health challenges among students, especially amidst increasing academic pressures. Thus, this meditation and relaxation application has the potential to become a crucial tool in efforts to enhance the quality of education and students' mental health.
... Kedua kemampuan ini sangat penting dalam konteks pembelajaran, karena mereka memungkinkan siswa untuk memahami materi pelajaran dengan lebih baik, menyelesaikan tugas dengan lebih efisien, dan mencapai hasil akademis yang lebih baik. Penelitian oleh Tang et al. (2007) menunjukkan bahwa meditasi dapat meningkatkan perhatian dan kontrol diri, yang berkontribusi pada peningkatan fokus dan konsentrasi siswa. Praktik meditasi dan relaksasi juga dapat membantu siswa mengembangkan keterampilan pengelolaan emosi yang lebih baik. ...
... Dengan menggunakan metode penelitian yang komprehensif ini, diharapkan penelitian ini dapat memberikan kontribusi yang signifikan dalam upaya meningkatkan kualitas pendidikan melalui aplikasi meditasi dan relaksasi yang dirancang khusus untuk siswa. Cohen et al. (1983) dan Skala Fokus dan Konsentrasi oleh Tang et al. (2007). Survei PSS menunjukkan penurunan signifikan dalam tingkat stres siswa setelah penggunaan aplikasi. ...
... Peningkatan skor rata-rata fokus dan konsentrasi dari 3,2 sebelum intervensi menjadi 4,5 setelah intervensi menunjukkan bahwa aplikasi tidak hanya membantu dalam mengurangi stres, tetapi juga meningkatkan kemampuan kognitif siswa. Penelitian oleh Tang et al. (2007) juga menemukan bahwa meditasi dapat meningkatkan perhatian dan kontrol diri, yang sangat penting dalam konteks pembelajaran. Peningkatan fokus ini memungkinkan siswa untuk lebih efektif dalam memahami materi pelajaran dan menyelesaikan tugas-tugas akademik mereka. ...
Article
Full-text available
Aplikasi meditasi dan relaksasi yang dirancang khusus untuk membantu siswa mengatasi stres dan meningkatkan fokus siswa saat pembelajaran merupakan inovasi teknologi yang bertujuan untuk mendukung kesejahteraan mental dan akademik siswa. Aplikasi ini menawarkan berbagai fitur yang meliputi sesi meditasi terpandu, latihan pernapasan, dan teknik relaksasi yang dirancang untuk mengurangi kecemasan dan stres yang sering dialami oleh siswa. Selain itu, aplikasi ini menyediakan alat untuk membantu siswa meningkatkan konsentrasi dan fokus selama proses pembelajaran melalui program khusus yang dirancang berdasarkan penelitian ilmiah tentang efek meditasi dan relaksasi pada kinerja kognitif. Dengan tampilan yang user-friendly dan akses mudah melalui perangkat seluler, aplikasi ini memungkinkan siswa untuk dengan mudah mengintegrasikan praktik meditasi dan relaksasi ke dalam rutinitas harian mereka. Studi awal menunjukkan bahwa penggunaan aplikasi ini secara rutin dapat memberikan manfaat signifikan dalam hal pengurangan tingkat stres, peningkatan fokus, serta peningkatan kualitas tidur. Implementasi aplikasi ini di lingkungan pendidikan diharapkan dapat menciptakan iklim belajar yang lebih positif dan produktif, sehingga mendukung pencapaian akademik dan kesejahteraan keseluruhan siswa. Inisiatif ini merupakan respons terhadap meningkatnya kebutuhan akan solusi yang efektif dalam mengatasi tantangan kesehatan mental di kalangan pelajar, terutama di tengah tekanan akademik yang semakin tinggi. Dengan demikian, aplikasi meditasi dan relaksasi ini berpotensi menjadi alat penting dalam upaya meningkatkan kualitas pendidikan dan kesehatan mental siswa.
... However, in addition to its well-established benefits for mental health, human performance research indicates how mindfulness techniques can further improve cognitive functioning [5][6][7][8]. In this domain, engaging in mindfulness practices have been associated with enhancements in attentional processes [7,9,10], executive functions [11,12], working memory ability [12][13][14] and even creative thinking [15,16]. Despite demonstrating promising results, these studies are limited by the narrow nature of cognitive tasks employed (i.e., Attentional Network Task or simple motor sequence tasks [10,17,18]), which fail to explore how mindfulness may enhance performance in more naturalistic environments. ...
... In this domain, engaging in mindfulness practices have been associated with enhancements in attentional processes [7,9,10], executive functions [11,12], working memory ability [12][13][14] and even creative thinking [15,16]. Despite demonstrating promising results, these studies are limited by the narrow nature of cognitive tasks employed (i.e., Attentional Network Task or simple motor sequence tasks [10,17,18]), which fail to explore how mindfulness may enhance performance in more naturalistic environments. Secondly, mindfulness regimes are typically of considerable duration [5] and investigations largely employ a between-groups approach, ignoring the individual variability in intervention outcomes [19]. ...
... Although mindfulness interventions have predominantly been employed within the realm of well-being and mental health, recent research has started to reveal its broader effects on cognitive functioning and performance [5][6][7][8]. Notably, performance enhancements have been demonstrated in a range of traditional cognitive tests including the Attentional Network Test [10,17], the Symbol Digit Modalities Test [24], Stroop Test [25] and in sequenced motor tasks [18,26]. These mindfulness interventions also do not need to be extensive or lengthy to produce significant results, highlighting their propensity to be an easily accessible cognitive training tool. ...
Article
Full-text available
Human performance applications of mindfulness-based training have demonstrated its utility in enhancing cognitive functioning. Previous studies have illustrated how these interventions can improve performance on traditional cognitive tests, however, little investigation has explored the extent to which mindfulness-based training can optimise performance in more dynamic and complex contexts. Further, from a neuroscientific perspective, the underlying mechanisms responsible for performance enhancements remain largely undescribed. With this in mind, the following study aimed to investigate how a short-term mindfulness intervention (one week) augments performance on a dynamic and complex task (target motion analyst task; TMA) in young, healthy adults (n = 40, age range = 18–38). Linear mixed effect modelling revealed that increased adherence to the web-based mindfulness-based training regime (ranging from 0–21 sessions) was associated with improved performance in the second testing session of the TMA task, controlling for baseline performance. Analyses of resting-state electroencephalographic (EEG) metrics demonstrated no change across testing sessions. Investigations of additional individual factors demonstrated that enhancements associated with training adherence remained relatively consistent across varying levels of participants’ resting-state EEG metrics, personality measures (i.e., trait mindfulness, neuroticism, conscientiousness), self-reported enjoyment and timing of intervention adherence. Our results thus indicate that mindfulness-based cognitive training leads to performance enhancements in distantly related tasks, irrespective of several individual differences. We also revealed nuances in the magnitude of cognitive enhancements contingent on the timing of adherence, regardless of total volume of training. Overall, our findings suggest that mindfulness-based training could be used in a myriad of settings to elicit transferable performance enhancements.
... • Mindfulness: Mindfulness practices involve intentionally paying attention to the present moment without judgment. Research suggests that mindfulness meditation can improve attentional control and working memory capacity, leading to enhanced cognitive performance (Tang, 2007). By training individuals to be more present and focused, mindfulness can reduce distractions and enhance encoding and retrieval processes in memory. ...
Chapter
This paper delves into the intricate interplay between artificial intelligence (AI), human memory, and spiritual development, aiming to uncover their interconnectedness and potential synergies in enhancing cognitive understanding and personal growth. Firstly, it comprehensively explores the concept of human memory, including its definition, function, processes, and stages, while outlining a comprehensive design to elucidate its workings. Secondly, it examines the complex relationship between memory and intelligence, highlighting various cognitive processes and factors involved. Thirdly, it conducts a detailed analysis of the physiological mechanisms underlying human memory, with a focus on glucose metabolism, oxygen supply, and nutritional factors, supported by scientific evidence and examples. Furthermore, it explores the spiritual dimensions of human memory, investigating the source of the power that establishes spiritual connections within memory processes. Finally, it examines the interconnectedness between AI, human memory, and spiritual development, elucidating their interactions and potential synergies in fostering cognitive understanding and personal growth. The qualitative study used interviews and group discussions to explore human memory’s complexity and its links with intelligence, physiology, spirituality, and AI. Fifty participants shared diverse insights, guiding discussions on memory and spirituality. The comprehensive exploration of human memory uncovers intricate processes and mechanisms. Spiritual power, often overlooked, shapes memory function, influencing cognitive development and emotional resilience. Integrating spiritual aspects enriches understanding, fostering self-discovery and alignment with higher selves.
... Finally, mindfulness meditations train several executive functions and improve attention test scores (Chiesa et al., 2010;Tang et al., 2007), working memory (Chiesa et al., 2011) and cognitive control (Fountain-Zaragoza et al., 2016). Mindfulness meditation also develops socio-emotional abilities (Malboeuf-Hurtubise et al., 2017), creativity (Capurso et al., 2014;Colzato et al., 2012Colzato et al., , 2014Ding et al., 2015aDing et al., , 2015bLebuda et al., 2015;Lippelt et al., 2014), prosocial emotions (Luberto et al., 2018) and prosocial behaviours (Berry et al., 2020;Donald et al., 2019;Malin, 2023;Schindler & Friese, 2022), but this process is not necessarily automatic, hence the importance of including ethical instructions during mindfulness meditation (Berry et al., 2020;Chen & Jordan, 2020). ...
Article
Background Creativity and responsibility are enhanced by meditation among adults, but such effects have not been studied in adolescents. Moreover, the determinants of the ethical effect (such as responsibility) of meditation are unclear. Aims To address this gap by investigating the impact of digital in-class meditation programmes in middle school, focusing on intentions (self-centred vs. responsibility-centred), on adolescents' creative potential and sense of responsibility. These intentions are operationalized by different purpose-based meditations. Methods We conducted a cluster randomized controlled trial involving 107 year 7 adolescents from six classes, assigning them to two experimental groups and an active control group. Pre- and post-intervention assessments were conducted over an 11-week period, including a creativity (EPoC) test comprising four exercises (graphic and verbal, divergent and convergent thinking), a responsibility and a mindfulness scale. Results Our findings revealed no discernible effects on divergent thinking or self-reported mindfulness. However, we observed significant differences in graphic and verbal convergent creative thinking, as well as impacting responsibility scores, between a responsibility-centred meditation group and a self-centred meditation group. Moreover, distinctions were noted between control and self-centred meditation groups and between some classes. Effect sizes indicated that the interventions had a moderate but significant impact on the variables measured. Conclusion Our study reveals the effectiveness of digital meditation interventions in enhancing convergent creative thinking and responsibility among middle-school students. Notably, it shed new light on the importance of meditation intentions, which may be as significant as the form of meditation itself.
... The present research is not free from shortcomings. First, both studies had short mindfulness interventions as even short exercises have positive impacts on psychological functioning [93][94][95]. However, the outcomes could differ with longer mindfulness inductions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mindfulness is a popular technique that helps people to get closer to their self. However, recent findings indicate that mindfulness may not benefit everybody. In the present research, we hypothesized that mindfulness promotes alienation from the self among individuals with low abilities to self-regulate affect (state-oriented individuals) but not among individuals with high abilities to self-regulate affect (action-oriented individuals). In two studies with participants who were mostly naïve to mindfulness practices (70% indicated no experience; N1 = 126, 42 men, 84 women, 0 diverse, aged 17–86 years, Mage = 31.87; N2 = 108, 30 men, 75 women, 3 diverse, aged 17–69 years, Mage = 28.00), we tested a mindfulness group (five-minute mindfulness exercise) against a control group (five-minute text reading). We operationalized alienation as lower consistency in repeated preference judgments and a lower tendency to adopt intrinsic over extrinsic goal recommendations. Results showed that, among state-oriented participants, mindfulness led to significantly lower consistency of preference judgments (Study 1) and lower adoption of intrinsic over extrinsic goals (Study 2) compared to text reading. The alienating effect was absent among action-oriented participants. Thus, mindfulness practice may alienate psychologically vulnerable people from their self and hamper access to preferences and intrinsic goals. We discuss our findings within Personality-Systems-Interactions (PSI) theory.
Article
Importance The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, Text Revision ( DSM-5-TR ), recently identified internet gaming disorder (IGD) as a condition warranting more research, and few empirically validated treatments exist. Mindfulness meditation (MM) has multiple health benefits; however, its efficacy in treating IGD and potential neural mechanisms underlying MM treatment of the disorder remain largely unknown. Objective To explore the efficacy of MM used to treat adults with IGD and to identify neural mechanisms underlying MM. Design, Setting, and Participants This randomized clinical trial was performed from October 1 to November 30, 2023, at Hangzhou Normal University in Hangzhou, China. Adults (aged ≥18 years) who met at least 6 of the 9 DSM-5-TR proposed criteria for IGD were recruited to receive either MM or progressive muscle relaxation (PMR). Data analysis was performed on December 1, 2023. Intervention Participants underwent MM training (an 8-session meditation program that focuses on attention and acceptance) and PMR training (an 8-time program for body relaxation) delivered in groups that met 2 times each week for 4 weeks. Main Outcomes and Measures This per-protocol analysis included only participants who finished the pretest assessment, 8 training sessions, and posttest assessment. The main outcomes were addiction severity (measured with the DSM-5-TR proposed criteria for IGD and with Internet Addiction Test scores), gaming craving (measured with Questionnaire for Gaming Urges scores), and blood oxygen level–dependent signals assessed with cue-craving tasks on fMRI. Behavioral and brain measurements were compared using analysis of variance. Functional connectivity (FC) among identified brain regions was measured to test connectivity changes associated with MM. Results This study included 64 adults with IGD. A total of 32 participants received MM (mean [SD] age, 20.3 [1.9] years; 17 women [53%]) and 32 received PMR (mean [SD] age, 20.2 [1.5] years; 16 women [50%]). The severity of IGD decreased in the MM group (pretest vs posttest: mean [SD], 7.0 [1.1] vs 3.6 [0.8]; P < .001) and in the PMR group (mean [SD], 7.1 [0.9] vs 6.0 [0.9]; P = .04). The MM group had a greater decrease in IGD severity than the PMR group (mean [SD] score change for the MM group vs the PMR group, −3.6 [0.3] vs −1.1 [0.2]; P < .001). Mindfulness meditation was associated with decreased brain activation in the bilateral lentiform nuclei ( r = 0.40; 95% CI, 0.19 to 0.60; P = .02), insula ( r = 0.35; 95% CI, 0.09 to 0.60; P = .047), and medial frontal gyrus (MFG; r = 0.43; 95% CI, 0.16 to 0.70; P = .01). Increased MFG-lentiform FC and decreased craving (pretest vs posttest: mean [SD], 58.8 [15.7] vs 33.6 [12.0]; t = −8.66; ƞ ² = 0.30; P < .001) was observed after MM, and changes in MFG-lentiform FC mediated the relationship between increased mindfulness and decreased craving (mediate effect, −0.17; 95% CI, −0.32 to −0.08; P = .03). Conclusions and Relevance In this study, MM was more effective in decreasing addiction severity and gaming cravings compared with PMR. These findings indicate that MM may be an effective treatment for IGD and may exert its effects by altering frontopallidal pathways. Trial Registration Chinese Clinical Trial Registry Identifier: ChiCTR2300075869
Article
Full-text available
Loneliness significantly contributes to cognitive impairment and dementia in older adults. Loneliness is a distressing feeling resulting from a perceived lack of social connection (i.e., a discrepancy between desired and actual social relationships), while social isolation is a related term that can be defined by number and type of social relationships. Importantly, loneliness is distinct from social isolation in that it is associated with a distressing self-perception. The primary focus of this narrative review is the impact of chronic loneliness on cognitive impairment and dementia among older adults. Loneliness has a significant association with many factors that are related to worse cognition, and therefore we include discussion on health, mental health, as well as the physiological effects of loneliness, neuropathology, and potential treatments. Loneliness has been shown to be related to development of dementia with a hazard ratio (HR) risk comparable to having a single APOE4 gene. The relationship of dementia to loneliness appears to be at least partially independent of other known dementia risk factors that are possibly associated with loneliness, such as depression, educational status, social isolation, and physical activity. Episodic memory is not consistently impacted by loneliness, which would be more typically impaired if the mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or dementia was due to Alzheimer’s disease (AD) pathology. In addition, the several longitudinal studies that included neuropathology showed no evidence for a relationship between loneliness and AD neuropathology. Loneliness may decrease resilience, or produce greater cognitive change associated with the same level of AD neuropathology. Intervention strategies to decrease loneliness in older adults have been developed but need to consider key treatment targets beyond social isolation. Loneliness needs to be assessed in all studies of cognitive decline in elders, since it significantly contributes to the variance of cognitive function. It will be useful to better define the underlying mechanism of loneliness effects on cognition to determine if it is similar to other psychological factors related to excessive stress reactivity, such as neuroticism or even depression, which are also associated with cognitive decline. It is important from a health perspective to develop better strategies to decrease loneliness in older adults.
Chapter
Postmaterial spiritual psychology posits that consciousness can contribute to the unfolding of material events and that the human brain can detect broad, non-material communications. In this regard, this emerging field of postmaterial psychology marks a stark departure from psychology's traditional quantum measurements and tenets. The Oxford Handbook of Psychology and Spirituality codifies the leading empirical evidence in the support and application of postmaterial psychological science. Sections in this volume include: personality and social psychology factors and implications; spiritual development and culture; spiritual dialogue, prayer, and intention in Western mental health; Eastern traditions and psychology; physical health and spirituality; positive psychology; and scientific advances and applications related to spiritual psychology. With articles from leading scholars in psychology, medicine, physics, and biology, this text is an interdisciplinary reference for a rapidly emerging approach to contemporary science.
Article
The study of attention is central to psychology. This book presents the science of attention in a larger social context, which includes our ability voluntarily to choose and act upon an object of thought. The volume is based on fifty years of research involving behavioral, imaging, developmental, and genetic methods. It describes three brain networks of attention that carry out the functions of obtaining and maintaining the alert state, orienting to sensory events, and regulating responses. The book ties these brain networks to anatomy, connectivity, development, and socialization, and includes material on pathologies that involve attentional networks as well as their role in education and social interaction.
Book
Human emotions
Article
Inter-subject variations in white matter tract properties are known to correlate with individual differences in cognitive performance in domains such as attention, yet the specificity of such linkages is unknown, both at level of specific component operations and their association with anatomically distinct networks. This study examines individual performance variation within three specific functional components of attention-- alterting, orienting, and conflict processing – identified by the Attention Network Task (ANT), and relates each to inter-subject variation in a distinct set of white matter tract regions. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) data collected via 55 diffusion-encoding directions in a 3 Tesla MRI scan, and average fractional anisotropy (FA) was assessed within a set of individualized, a priori defined regions of interest, via the Reliable Objective Quantification Scheme (ROQS) (Niogi & McCandliss, 2006, Niogi et al., 2007). Results demonstrate three functionally distinct components of attention that each correlate specifically and distinctly with three white matter tract regions. Structure-function correlations were found between alerting and the posterior limb of the internal capsule (PLIC), orienting and the splenium of the corpus callosum, and, conflict and the anterior corona radiata (ACR). A multiple regression/dissociation analysis demonstrated a triple dissociation between these three structure-function relationships, providing evidence of three functional and anatomically separable networks. These results extend previous findings from functional imaging and lesion studies that suggest these three components of attention are sub-served by dissociable networks, and suggest that variations in white matter tract microstructure may modulate the efficacy of these cognitive processes in highly specific ways.