ArticlePDF Available

A Follow-Up Study of Girls With Gender Identity Disorder

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study provided information on the natural histories of 25 girls with gender identity disorder (GID). Standardized assessment data in childhood (mean age, 8.88 years; range, 3-12 years) and at follow-up (mean age, 23.24 years; range, 15-36 years) were used to evaluate gender identity and sexual orientation. At the assessment in childhood, 60% of the girls met the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders criteria for GID, and 40% were subthreshold for the diagnosis. At follow-up, 3 participants (12%) were judged to have GID or gender dysphoria. Regarding sexual orientation, 8 participants (32%) were classified as bisexual/homosexual in fantasy, and 6 (24%) were classified as bisexual/homosexual in behavior. The remaining participants were classified as either heterosexual or asexual. The rates of GID persistence and bisexual/homosexual sexual orientation were substantially higher than base rates in the general female population derived from epidemiological or survey studies. There was some evidence of a "dosage" effect, with girls who were more cross-sex typed in their childhood behavior more likely to be gender dysphoric at follow-up and more likely to have been classified as bisexual/homosexual in behavior (but not in fantasy).
Content may be subject to copyright.
A Follow-Up Study of Girls With Gender Identity Disorder
Kelley D. Drummond
Ontario Institute for Studies in Education of the University of
Toronto
Susan J. Bradley
Centre for Addiction and Mental Health
Michele Peterson-Badali
Ontario Institute for Studies in Education of the University of
Toronto
Kenneth J. Zucker
Centre for Addiction and Mental Health
This study provided information on the natural histories of 25 girls with gender identity disorder (GID).
Standardized assessment data in childhood (mean age, 8.88 years; range, 3–12 years) and at follow-up
(mean age, 23.24 years; range, 15–36 years) were used to evaluate gender identity and sexual orientation.
At the assessment in childhood, 60% of the girls met the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders criteria for GID, and 40% were subthreshold for the diagnosis. At follow-up, 3 participants
(12%) were judged to have GID or gender dysphoria. Regarding sexual orientation, 8 participants (32%)
were classified as bisexual/homosexual in fantasy, and 6 (24%) were classified as bisexual/homosexual
in behavior. The remaining participants were classified as either heterosexual or asexual. The rates of
GID persistence and bisexual/homosexual sexual orientation were substantially higher than base rates in
the general female population derived from epidemiological or survey studies. There was some evidence
of a “dosage” effect, with girls who were more cross-sex typed in their childhood behavior more likely
to be gender dysphoric at follow-up and more likely to have been classified as bisexual/homosexual in
behavior (but not in fantasy).
Keywords: gender identity disorder, gender identity, sexual orientation, girls, follow-up
Research on normative (or typical) gender development has
documented various behavioral domains in which children show,
on average, significant sex differences: gender identity self-
labeling, sex-of-playmate preference, toy and activity interests,
roles in fantasy play, parental rehearsal play, and so on (for a
review, see Ruble, Martin, & Berenbaum, 2006; Zucker, 2005c).
The determinants of this between-sex variation in sex-typed be-
havior have long been deemed by developmentalists to have im-
portant implications for other aspects of psychosocial develop-
ment, such as interpersonal relational styles (e.g., Maccoby, 1998),
cognitive skills (e.g., Liss, 1983), and vocational interests (e.g.,
Lippa, 1998), for which there are also significant sex differences.
As noted by Lippa (2002), determining within-sex individual
differences in gender-related behavior is another strategy used to
study variations with regard to other aspects of development (see,
e.g., Barrett & White, 2002; Khuri & Ruble, 2006). In the present
study, we used this approach to examine the relation, if any,
between sex-typed behavior patterns in childhood, including gen-
der identity, and subsequent gender identity and sexual orientation
in late adolescent girls and young adult women.
Several lines of evidence suggest that there are empirical rea-
sons to posit a link between sex-typed behavior in childhood and
later gender identity and sexual orientation. Like sex-typed behav-
ior in childhood, gender identity and sexual orientation in adult-
hood are also sex dimorphic: Most women have a “female” gender
identity (the subjective sense of self as a woman) and are sexually
attracted to men, whereas most men have a “male” gender identity
and are sexually attracted to women. Indeed, gender identity and
sexual orientation may be the two behavioral traits that most
strongly differentiate women from men (cf. Hyde, 2005). Using a
self-report questionnaire designed to measure gender identity di-
mensionally in adolescents and adults, for example, Deogracias et
al. (2007) obtained a between-sex effect size, using Cohen’s d,of
13.24.
Over the past several decades, the empirical literature has relied
on two methods, namely, retrospective and prospective designs
using targeted samples, to examine the relation between sex-typed
behavior in childhood and subsequent gender identity and sexual
orientation in adulthood. Retrospective designs have studied adults
with known variation in their gender identity and/or sexual orien-
tation. For example, adults who meet the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) criteria for gender identity dis-
order (GID; also known as transsexualism) recall engaging in more
cross-gender-typed behavior in childhood than do adults without GID
(e.g., Blanchard & Freund, 1983; Doorn, Poortinga, & Verschoor,
1994; Ehrhardt, Grisanti, & McCauley, 1979; Freund, Langevin,
Satterberg, & Steiner, 1977; see also Bartlett & Vasey, 2006).
Kelley D. Drummond and Michele Peterson-Badali, Department of
Human Development and Applied Psychology, Ontario Institute for Stud-
ies in Education of the University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada;
Susan J. Bradley and Kenneth J. Zucker, Gender Identity Service, Child,
Youth, and Family Program, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health,
Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kenneth
J. Zucker, Gender Identity Service, Child, Youth, and Family Program,
Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, 250 College Street, Toronto,
Ontario M5T 1R8, Canada. E-mail: ken_zucker@camh.net
Developmental Psychology Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association
2008, Vol. 44, No. 1, 3445 0012-1649/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.34
34
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
The largest body of retrospective research pertains to the within-
sex association between sex-typed behavior in childhood and
sexual orientation in adulthood. Bailey and Zucker (1995) per-
formed a meta-analysis of 41 retrospective studies that made a
quantitative comparison between heterosexual and homosexual
same-sex adults using some measure of childhood sex-typed be-
havior. These studies yielded 48 independent effect sizes: 32
compared heterosexual and homosexual men, and 16 compared
heterosexual and homosexual women. Using Cohen’s d, Bailey
and Zucker found that there were substantial differences in pat-
terns of recalled childhood sex-typed behavior between heterosex-
ual and homosexual adults. On average, both homosexual men and
women recalled more cross-sex-typed behavior in childhood than
did their heterosexual counterparts (respective ds were 1.31 and
0.96). Subsequent studies have, with no exception, continued to
replicate these findings (summarized in Zucker et al., 2006).
There are, of course, both methodological and interpretive prob-
lems with retrospective designs (for an overview, see Hardt &
Rutter, 2004). In a targeted sample of adults with GID (invariably
recruited from specialized gender identity clinics), it is possible
that the association with cross-sex-typed behavior is magnified
because not all individuals with pervasive cross-gender behavior in
childhood end up seeking out medically assisted gender change in
adulthood (e.g., because their earlier gender dysphoria had de-
sisted). In the studies comparing the recollections of heterosexual
and homosexual adults, in which there is less of a sampling bias
problem, the most common criticism has pertained to memory
distortion or selective recall. For example, it has been argued that
the greater recollection of cross-gender behavior during childhood
by homosexual than by heterosexual adults is linked to the wide-
spread “master narrative” in Western culture that presupposes that
“gender inversion” is linked to homosexual sexual orientation (see,
e.g., Cohler & Galatzer-Levy, 2000; Gottschalk, 2003; Hegarty,
1999; Kite & Deaux, 1987). As a result, it has been claimed that
the sex-typed behavior–sexual orientation association is nothing
more than participants recalling behaviors that adhere to cultural
stereotypes and expectations. Although there is evidence that
speaks against this retrospective distortion hypothesis (summa-
rized in Bailey & Zucker, 1995; Zucker, 2005a, in press; Zucker et
al., 2006), there is general agreement that the retrospective data
should be confirmed (or disconfirmed) with prospective designs.
One prospective approach has been to target a sample of chil-
dren presumed to have moderate-to-pervasive cross-gender behav-
ior. In one line of research, sampling consisted of ad-recruited girls
with parent-nominated “tomboyish” behavior, along with mea-
sures of sex-typed behavior administered to the girls themselves
(e.g., Bailey, Bechtold, & Berenbaum, 2002; Berenbaum & Bailey,
2003; Green, Williams, & Goodman, 1982), who were compared
to girls unselected for their gender behavior. Neither research team
has, as of yet, reported on longer term linkages.
A second strategy has been to study children referred to spe-
cialized gender identity clinics because there is concern about their
cross-gender behavior and gender identity status (e.g., on the part
of parents, mental health professionals, teachers, etc.). Over the
years, several research teams have studied such children, and
overviews may be found in the work of Green (1987), Zucker and
Bradley (1995), and Cohen-Kettenis and Pfa¨fflin (2003).
In one study, Green (1987) assessed the gender identity and
sexual orientation of 44 behaviorally feminine boys and 30 control
boys who were at a follow-up mean age of 18.9 years (range,
14–24 years) and who had initially been evaluated at a mean age
of 7.1 years (range, 4–12 years). Of the 44 behaviorally feminine
boys, only 1 youth, at the age of 18 years, was gender dysphoric
to the extent of considering sex-reassignment surgery. None of the
other boys were reported to have gender identity problems at
follow-up. Sexual orientation in fantasy and behavior was assessed
by means of a semistructured, face-to-face interview. Kinsey rat-
ings were made on a 7-point continuum, ranging from exclusive
heterosexuality (a Kinsey “0”) to exclusive homosexuality (a Kin-
sey “6”; Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948). Depending on the
measure (fantasy or behavior), 75%–80% of the previously be-
haviorally feminine boys were either bisexual or homosexual
(Kinsey ratings between 2 and 6) at follow-up versus 0%–4% of
the control boys.
Data from seven other follow-up reports on a total of 82 behav-
iorally feminine boys have been summarized in detail elsewhere
(Zucker, 2005b; Zucker & Bradley, 1995, pp. 285–286, 290–297).
Similar to Green’s (1987) case-control study, these studies also
identified an elevated rate of either a bisexual or homosexual
sexual orientation (52.4%). In contrast to Green’s (1987) study,
however, the other studies found the rate of GID persistence was
higher, with rates ranging from 12% to 20%.
From these prospective studies of behaviorally feminine boys,
two conclusions might be drawn: (a) The rate of persistent gender
dysphoria was modest but arguably higher than one estimated base
rate for gender dysphoria in the general population of biological
males: 1 in 11,000 men (Bakker, van Kesteren, Gooren, & Beze-
mer, 1993), and (b) the rate of a later bisexual or homosexual
sexual orientation was notably higher than the known base rates
for a bisexual or homosexual sexual orientation in the general
population of biological males (see, e.g., Laumann, Gagnon, Mi-
chael, & Michaels, 1994). Thus, for sexual orientation, there
appears to be a reasonable convergence between prospective and
retrospective studies but, for gender identity, there is more diver-
gence: Many boys with pervasive cross-gender behavior and co-
occurring gender dysphoria do not show persistent gender dyspho-
ria by late adolescence or young adulthood, which is at some
variance from the recollections of most gender-dysphoric adoles-
cent boys and adult men.
Over the years, it has been noted that little is known about the
longer term psychosexual outcome of girls referred to specialized
gender identity clinics (Peplau & Huppin, in press; Peplau, Spal-
ding, Conley, & Veniegas, 1999). In part, this has been a function
of the fact that boys are much more likely than girls to be referred
to gender identity clinics: 5.75:1 in one clinic and 3.07:1 in another
(see Cohen-Kettenis, Owen, Kaijser, Bradley, & Zucker, 2003;
Cohen-Kettenis et al., 2006). The present study attempted to fill
this gap by providing, to our knowledge, the first systematic
follow-up report of clinic-referred girls with GID with regard to
gender identity and sexual orientation.
Method
Participants
Between 1975 and 2004, 71 girls (age range, 3–12 years) were
referred for assessment to the Gender Identity Service, Child,
Youth, and Family Program at the Centre for Addiction and
35
SPECIAL SECTION: GENDER IDENTITY DISORDER IN GIRLS
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Mental Health in Toronto, Ontario, Canada. To participate in the
follow-up study, patients had to be at least 17 years of age. Using
this age cutoff, we identified 37 eligible girls, of whom 30 were
contacted for participation. Of the remaining 7 girls, 3 could not be
traced through previous addresses, registrars, and personal con-
tacts (e.g., the patient and/or their family had moved and a current
telephone number, mailing address, or e-mail address could not be
identified), and 4 were not available to participate within the time
requirements of the study.
Initial telephone contact was first made with the parents or legal
guardians because participants were minors at the time of assess-
ment and some may have had no recollection of their clinic
attendance.
1
Of the 30 clients contacted, 25 (83.3%) agreed to
participate; 24 came into the clinic for testing, and 1 participant
completed a telephone interview because she was too anxious to
travel to the clinic. Of the remaining 5 girls, 4 of the girls’ parents
or guardians (e.g., the Children’s Aid Society) were unwilling to
provide contact information for their children. One individual
declined to participate.
The demographic characteristics of the participants in childhood
and at follow-up are shown in Table 1. The GID diagnosis in
childhood was based on the DSM (3rd ed. [DSM–III]; 3rd ed., rev.
[DSM–III–R]; or 4th ed. [DSM–IV]; American Psychiatric Asso-
ciation [APA], 1980, 1987, and 1994, respectively) criteria appli-
cable at the time of assessment. Fifteen girls (60%) met complete
DSM criteria for GID in childhood. The remaining 40% were
subthreshold for a DSM diagnosis of GID, but all had some
indicators of GID, and some would have met the complete DSM
criteria at some point in their lives prior to their assessment in
childhood.
Four of the girls in the follow-up sample were born with a
disorder of sex development (DSD; 2 had cloacal exstrophy, 1 had
congenital micropenis syndrome of unknown etiology, and 1 had
mixed gonadal dysgenesis; Hughes, Houk, Ahmed, Lee, & Law-
son Wilkins Pediatric Endocrine Society/European Society for
Paediatric Endocrinology Consensus Group, 2006). Three of the
nonparticipants also had a DSD (partial androgen insensitivity
syndrome, congenital adrenal hyperplasia, or true hermaphrodit-
ism). There are arguments for and against the inclusion of the 4
girls with a DSD in this sample (see, e.g., Meyer-Bahlburg, 1994).
A female gender assignment was made for all 4 girls almost
immediately after birth. Also in early infancy, the 4 girls were
gonadectomized and had surgical feminization of their external
genitalia. Like the somatically intact girls, the 4 girls were referred
for concern about their gender development in relation to their
assigned gender. On the one hand, as noted by Meyer-Bahlburg
(2005), “there is every reason to assume that the processes and
psychosocial factors involved in normative gender development
also contribute to development of all aspects of gender. . .in per-
sons with intersexuality” (p. 434). On the other hand, as also noted
by Meyer-Bahlburg (2005), “additional factors. . .may come into
play in [such persons]. . .particularly the awareness of an atypical
biological condition and medical history” (pp. 434435). As noted
in Table 3, only 1 of these girls met the complete Point A and Point
B DSM criteria for GID, and the other 3 were subthreshold.
Procedure
All participants were evaluated on a single day. Below, we
provide information on the measures used in this report (for other
measures, including parent and self-ratings of behavior problems,
psychiatric diagnoses, and experiences of stigma, see Drummond,
2006). All of the participants provided written informed consent
prior to their involvement in the follow-up assessment and were
provided a stipend for their participation and reimbursement for
travel expenses. The study was approved by the Institutional
Review Boards at the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health and
the University of Toronto.
1
It is beyond the scope of this report to describe the types of therapies
(as well as their frequency and duration) that the girls and/or their parents
may have received between the assessment in childhood and the follow-up
(e.g., by a therapist within the Gender Identity Service at the Centre for
Addiction and Mental Health or in the community). From the participants’
clinic files, 13 of the 25 girls had at least some contact with our clinic
during the interval between assessment and follow-up (e.g., as therapy
clients or for a reassessment). Of the 25 girls and/or their parents, 18 had
been in some type of therapy or counseling during the interval between
assessment and follow-up; of these, 5 were patients of staff within the
Gender Identity Service, and the remainder were seen by a professional in
the community.
Table 1
Demographic Characteristics (N 25)
Characteristic MSD Range %
From childhood
Age (in years) 8.88 3.10 3.17–12.95
Year of assessment 1989.36 7.02 1977–2002
IQ
a, b
105.17 21.73 57–144
Social class
c
35.72 14.40 8–66
Marital status
d
Two-parent family 60.0
Other 40.0
Caucasian 80.0
At follow-up
Age (in years)
e
23.24 5.82 15.44–36.58
Year of birth 1980.52 6.06 1968–1989
Interval (in years)
f
14.34 7.03 2.99–27.12
IQ
b, g
10.20 2.71 5.00–15.75
a
Full-scale IQ was obtained with age-appropriate Wechsler intelligence
scales (the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence—Third
Edition [Wechsler, 2002], the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—
Revised [Wechsler, 1974], and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Chil-
dren—Third Edition [Wechsler, 1991]). One participant was administered
the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale (Thorndike, Hagen, & Sattler,
1986).
b
IQ scores at assessment and follow-up were not available for 1
participant.
c
For social class, Hollingshead’s (1975) Four Factor Index
of Social Status was used. The absolute range was 866.
d
For marital
status, the category “Other” included the following family constellations:
single parent, separated, divorced, living with relatives, or in the care of the
Children’s Aid Society.
e
One participant (who was 15.44 years of age)
was below the lower bound age cutoff of 17 years but was included in the
study because her guardian had contacted the clinic for issues unrelated to
gender identity status.
f
Interval denotes the time between childhood
assessment and follow-up assessment.
g
Composite IQ (Vocabulary
Comprehension Block Design Object Assembly subscale scores)/4.
The absolute range was 1–19.
36
DRUMMOND, BRADLEY, PETERSON-BADALI, AND ZUCKER
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Measures
Childhood Assessment
Cognitive functioning. IQ was assessed with the Wechsler
Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition (Wechsler, 1997) or the
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third Edition (Wech-
sler, 1991) and, for one participant, with the Stanford-Binet Intel-
ligence Scale (Thorndike, Hagen, & Sattler, 1986).
Sex-typed behavior. Five child informant and three parent
informant measures were used to assess the participants’ sex-typed
behavior in childhood: (a) the Draw-a-Person test (Zucker, Fin-
egan, Doering, & Bradley, 1983); (b) a free-play task (Zucker,
Doering, Bradley, & Finegan, 1982); (c) the Playmate and Play-
style Preferences Structured Interview (Fridell, Owen-Anderson,
Johnson, Bradley, & Zucker, 2006); (d) sex-typed responses on the
Rorschach test (Zucker, Lozinski, Bradley, & Doering, 1992); (e)
the Gender Identity Interview (Zucker et al., 1993); (f) the Gender
Identity Questionnaire for Children (Johnson et al., 2004); (g) a
measure of activity level/extraversion (Zucker & Bradley, 1995);
and (h) the Games Inventory (Bates & Bentler, 1973). These child
and parent informant measures all had established discriminant
validity, that is, they significantly differentiated the clinic girls
referred for gender identity concerns from control girls (for a
review, see Zucker, 2005c; Zucker & Bradley, 1995). A Childhood
Sex-Typed Behavior Composite was computed for each partici-
pant by averaging the z-scores for these measures (which yielded
a total of 11 indices), as well as the GID DSM diagnosis (1
threshold,2 subthreshold) in childhood. Data from the total
sample of participants and nonparticipants (N 37) were used.
Because of missing data, the mean number of indices/participant
was 9.16 (SD 2.30).
Follow-Up Assessment
Cognitive functioning. Four subtests (Vocabulary, Compre-
hension, Block Design, and Object Assembly) of the Wechsler
Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition or the Wechsler Intelli-
gence Scale for Children—Third Edition were administered. The
standard scores from the subtests were averaged to form an IQ
score for cognitive functioning.
Recalled childhood gender identity and gender role behavior.
Participants completed the Recalled Childhood Gender Identity/
Gender Role Questionnaire (RCGI; Zucker et al., 2006). This
questionnaire consists of 23 items pertaining to various aspects of
sex-typed behavior, as well as to the relative closeness to the
mother and father during childhood. Individual items were rated on
a 5-point response scale. Each participant was instructed to make
ratings for her behavior as a child (“between the years 0 to 12”).
Factor analysis identified two factors, accounting for 37.4% and
7.8% of the variance, respectively (all factor loadings .40).
Factor 1 consisted of 18 items that pertained to childhood gender
role and gender identity, and Factor 2 consisted of three items that
pertained to parent–child relations (relative closeness to one’s
mother versus one’s father). Information on normative sex differ-
ences and discriminant validity was reported in Zucker et al.
(2006). For the present study, the mean Factor 1 score was com-
puted for each participant.
Concurrent gender identity. During an audiotaped interview,
each participant was asked to describe her current feelings about
being female and then to describe positive and negative aspects
about her gender status. The examiner also asked semistructured
gender identity questions from the adolescent and adult GID
criteria outlined in the DSM–IV–TR (APA, 2000). The interviewer
asked four questions related to the Point A criteria (e.g., the stated
desire to be a man, the desire to live or to be treated as a man) and
six questions from the Point B criteria (e.g., a preoccupation with
getting rid of breasts or genitalia). Participants were asked to
respond according to the last 12 months with No, Yes,orSome-
times. Participants who answered Yes or Sometimes for one or
more of the questions from both Point A and B criteria were
classified as displaying persistent gender dysphoria.
The female version of the Gender Identity/Gender Dysphoria
Questionnaire for Adolescents and Adults (GIDQ-AA; Deogracias
et al., 2007) was also completed. This 27-item questionnaire mea-
sures gender identity and gender dysphoria in adolescents or
adults. Item content was based on prior measures, expert panels,
and clinical experience. Each item was rated on a 5-point response
scale ranging from Never to Always based on a time frame of the
past 12 months. Item examples include the following: “In the past
12 months, have you felt unhappy about being a woman?” and “In
the past 12 months, have you wished to have an operation to
change your body into a man’s (e.g., to have your breasts removed
or to have a penis made)?” Factor analysis identified a strong
one-factor solution that accounted for 61.3% of the variance. All
27 items had factor loadings .30 (median, .86; range, .34–.96).
Psychometric evidence for discriminant validity and clinical utility
can be found in Deogracias et al. (2007). Participants’ GIDQ-AA
total scores were calculated by summing scores on the completed
items and dividing by the number of marked responses.
Sexual orientation in fantasy. Each participant’s sexual orien-
tation in fantasy was assessed with specific questions during an
audiotaped face-to-face interview and the self-report Erotic Re-
sponse and Orientation Scale (EROS; Storms, 1980). Questions
posed in the interview addressed four types of sexual fantasy: (a)
crushes on other people, (b) sexual arousal to visual stimuli (e.g.,
to strangers, acquaintances, partners, and individuals presented in
the media [video, movies, magazines, the internet]), (c) sexual
content of night dreams, and (d) sexual content of masturbation
fantasies. Using the Kinsey scale criteria, the interviewer assigned
ratings that ranged from 0 (exclusively heterosexual)to6(exclu-
sively homosexual) for each parameter. A dummy score of 7
denoted that the participant did not experience or report any
fantasies. A global fantasy score was derived on the basis of
ratings from the four questions. In the present study, only ratings
for the last 12 months are reported.
During the interview, participants were not asked directly about
the gender of the person or persons who elicited sexual arousal,
thus allowing time for the participant to provide this information
spontaneously. Directed questions were asked only if the partici-
pant did not volunteer specific information about same-sex or
opposite-sex partners. This approach was used so that, by the end
of the interview, the participant provided information about sexual
arousal to both same-sex and opposite-sex individuals.
The EROS is a 16-item self-report measure assessing sexual
orientation in fantasy over the past 12 months. Half of the ques-
tions pertained to heterosexual fantasy (e.g., “How often have you
had any sexual feelings (even the slightest) while looking at a
man?”) and the other half pertained to homosexual fantasy (e.g.,
37
SPECIAL SECTION: GENDER IDENTITY DISORDER IN GIRLS
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
“How often have you had any sexual feelings (even the slightest)
while looking at a woman?”). Each item was rated on a 5-point
scale for frequency of occurrence, ranging from “none”to“almost
every day.” Mean homoerotic and heteroerotic fantasy scores were
derived for each participant. Previous use of the EROS has shown
good evidence of discriminant validity (Storms, 1980; Zucker et
al., 1996).
Sexual orientation in behavior. Each participant’s sexual ori-
entation in behavior was assessed with specific questions during
the face-to-face interview and with a modified version of the
Sexual History Questionnaire (SHQ; Langevin, 1985). In the in-
terview, questions asked about five types of sexual behavior: (a)
dating; (b) holding hands in a romantic manner; (c) kissing; (d)
genital fondling or being touched on the breasts (or, in cases of
same-sex sexual behavior, touching another woman’s breasts); and
(e) penile–vaginal intercourse, anal intercourse, or the use of
dildos. Kinsey ratings for behavior for the past 12 months were
made in the same manner as fantasy ratings.
The modified SHQ consisted of 20 questions. Ten questions
pertained to heterosexual experiences (e.g., “How many men have
you kissed on the lips in a romantic way?”), and 10 questions
pertained to homosexual experiences (e.g., “How many women
have you kissed on the lips in a romantic way?”). Each item was
rated, for the 12 month period prior to the follow-up assessment,
on a 5-point scale for frequency of occurrence, ranging from none
to 11 or more. Mean total scores for heterosexual and homosexual
experiences were derived.
Sexual identity self-labeling. Participants were asked to pro-
vide a label for their current sexual identity and were offered the
following options: (a) “straight” or “heterosexual”; (b) “lesbian,”
“homosexual,” or “queer”; (c) “bisexual”; (d) “asexual”; or (e)
“other.”
Social desirability. Social desirability can threaten the validity
of self-report scales when respondents seek social approval or try
to represent themselves in a favorable manner (King & Brunner,
2000). Participants 18 years of age completed the Marlowe–
Crowne Social Desirability Scale (M–C SDS; Crowne & Marlowe,
1960), which consists of 33 true–false items. The scale consists of
18 culturally acceptable but unlikely statements keyed in the true
direction and 15 socially undesirable but probable statements
keyed in the false direction for a maximum possible score of 33.
Participants under 18 years of age completed a shorter version of
the M–C SDS (Strahan & Gerbasi, 1972). This scale consists of 12
culturally acceptable but improbable statements keyed in the true
direction and 8 socially undesirable but probable statements keyed
in the false direction for a maximum possible score of 20. Several
studies have found that the M–C SDS is a reliable and valid
measure (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960; Holden & Fekken, 1989;
Silverthorn & Gekoski, 1995).
Results
Participants Versus Nonparticipants
A preliminary analysis compared the assessment information
from childhood of the 25 girls who participated in the study with
that of the 12 girls who did not participate. There were no signif-
icant differences between the participants and nonparticipants on
any of these variables (data not shown).
2
At least by these mea
-
sures, it appears that the participants were representative of the
total pool of available patients and thus did not constitute a
markedly biased sample at follow-up.
Sex-Typed Behavior in Childhood
Table 2 shows the mean RCGI Factor 1 score, which pertained
to the participants’ recollections of their sex-typed behavior from
childhood. This mean score can be compared with the scores of
several samples of women, unselected for their gender identity or
sexual orientation, reported on in Zucker et al. (2006) and also
shown in Table 2. By comparing the mean factor score with the
scores from the other samples (mean range, 3.43–3.80), we see it
is apparent that the women in this study recalled relatively more
cross-gender behavior in childhood (M 2.57, SD .67).
Table 2 also shows the mean RCGI Factor 1 score of the
participants as a function of DSM diagnostic status in childhood.
Although the threshold participants recalled, on average, more
cross-gender behavior in childhood than the subthreshold partici-
pants, the difference was not significant, t(18) 1; the effect size
(Cohen’s d) of .32 would be considered small. We also examined
the z-composite for childhood sex-typed behavior as a function of
diagnostic status (for this analysis, the DSM metric was removed
from the composite and served as the independent variable). With
age at assessment in childhood covaried, the threshold participants
had, on average, significantly more cross-sex-typed behavior in
childhood (M .15, SD .54) than did the subthreshold partic-
ipants (M ⫽⫺.31, SD .36), F(1, 21) 23.36, p .001, partial
2
.53.
Psychosexual Differentiation at Follow-Up
A summary of the psychosexual differentiation data, including
gender identity at follow-up, sexual orientation, and sexual identity
self-labeling for each participant, is shown in Table 3.
2
These data are available in the study by Drummond (2006).
Table 2
Mean Factor 1 Score on the Recalled Childhood Gender
Identity/Gender Role Questionnaire (Zucker et al., 2006)
Group MSDdn
Total sample 2.57 .67 20
(Female university students) (3.43) (.54) (100)
(Mothers of boys with GID) (3.80) (.54) (230)
(Mothers of control boys) (3.72) (.34) (13)
(Mothers of nonreferred boys) (3.77) (.39) (24)
(Sisters/female cousins of
women with CAH) (3.70) (.43) (15)
Childhood diagnosis
GID: Threshold 2.48 .66 .32 11
GID: Subthreshold 2.70 .69 9
Note. Absolute range is 1.00–5.00. A lower score indicates more recalled
atypical gender identity and gender role behavior. Groups and values in
parentheses are from Zucker et al. (2006). GID gender identity disorder;
CAH congenital adrenal hyperplasia.
38
DRUMMOND, BRADLEY, PETERSON-BADALI, AND ZUCKER
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Gender Identity at Follow-up
On the basis of their answers to the semistructured clinical
interview questions, participants were classified as either gender
dysphoric or not gender dysphoric. In answering these questions,
22 participants (88%) reported no distress with their female gender
identity at follow-up. None of the participants desired contrasex
hormones or sex reassignment surgery to masculinize their bodies,
nor did they express a desire to get rid of their female sex
characteristics.
The remaining 3 participants (12%) were classified as gender
dysphoric at follow-up (none of these 3 girls had a co-occurring
DSD). Among these 3 participants, 1 had been living as a boy
since early adolescence (i.e., was known to others as a boy) and
was in the process of legally changing his name on official doc-
uments. The other 2 participants were living as girls, although both
were often perceived of as boys by naı¨ve others (e.g., new ac-
quaintances, strangers, etc.), which they preferred. All 3 gender
dysphoric participants wished they had been born a boy and
wondered whether they would have been happier as a boy. Two of
these individuals indicated a desire to have surgery to masculinize
their bodies. The other participant classified as gender dysphoric
reported indifference with regard to altering her physical appear-
ance but felt that “it was better to be neutral.” On the basis of this
information, 2 of the participants met DSM–IV–TR criteria for
GID. Although the other participant did not meet full criteria for
GID, information from the clinical interview and semistruc-
tured GID interview indicated that she was gender dysphoric at
follow-up.
In the Deogracias et al. (2007) study, a cutoff score of 3.00
was used to indicate “caseness” for gender dysphoria on the
GIDQ-AA. The 2 participants classified as gender dysphoric (and
who completed the GIDQ-AA) had scores lower than 3.00 (means
of 2.19 and 2.26, respectively), whereas the 18 participants clas-
sified as not gender dysphoric (and who completed the GIDQ-AA)
all had scores 3.00 (M 4.78, SD .20; range, 4.30–5.00). There
was a significant difference between these two subgroups, t(18)
17.81, p .001, d 13.27, which supports the classification of
the participants on the basis of the clinical interview.
Bakker et al. (1993) estimated that 1 in 30,400 genetically
female adults in the general population have GID. Using this
baseline prevalence value, the odds of persistent gender dysphoria
(12%) in the present sample was 4,084 times the odds of gender
dysphoria in the general population of biological females.
Sexual Orientation
On the basis of the Kinsey interview ratings, participants were
classified into the following three sexual orientation groups for
fantasy and behavior: (a) heterosexual (Kinsey ratings of 0–1), (b)
bisexual/homosexual (Kinsey ratings of 46), and (c) no sexual
fantasy or behavior. For the fantasy ratings (see Table 3), 15
Table 3
Summary of Gender Identity and Sexual Orientation Results at Follow-Up
Participant
ID
Age at
assessment
(years)
Age at
follow-up
(years)
Global Kinsey ratings Sexual
identity
label Gender identity DSMFantasy Behavior
1 9.74 36.58 6 6 HS WNL
2 8.88 36.61 6 6 HS WNL
3 5.85 32.41 0 0 HT WNL
4 3.17 28.78 0 HT WNL
5 4.92 26.61 4 0 BS WNL
6
a
5.75 26.58 0 0 HT WNL
7 12.67 17.09 AS Dysphoric
8 12.95 28.72 6 HS WNL
9 8.41 23.34 6 6 HT Dysphoric
10 8.29 24.12 4 6 BS WNL
11 4.10 20.04 0 0 HT WNL
12 4.72 19.73 0 HT WNL
13 6.70 21.53 0 0 HT WNL
14 6.81 18.73 0 0 HT WNL
15 12.62 23.57 6 6 HS WNL
16 12.16 21.10 6 6 HT Dysphoric
17 7.32 17.51 0 0 HT WNL
18 8.51 17.34 0 HT WNL
19
a
12.88 21.58 0 HT WNL
20 9.20 17.81 0 0 HT WNL
21 11.26 19.27 0 0 HT WNL
22
a
12.18 17.35 HT WNL
23 12.45 15.44 0 HT WNL
24
a
11.89 27.74 0 0 HT WNL
25 8.79 23.12 0 0 HT WNL
Note. For Kinsey ratings (last 12 months), 0 exclusively heterosexual and 6 exclusively homosexual. In the DSM column, a plus sign indicates the
participant met complete DSM-III, DSM-III-R, or DSM-IV symptom criteria for gender identity disorder at initial assessment. Dashes indicate the participant
did not report fantasy or behavior. ID identification label; HS homosexual (lesbian); HT heterosexual or straight; BS bisexual; AS asexual;
WNL within normal limits (i.e., the participant did not report any distress about being a female).
a
Participant with a disorder of sex development.
39
SPECIAL SECTION: GENDER IDENTITY DISORDER IN GIRLS
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
participants (60%) were classified as exclusively heterosexual, 8
(32%) were classified as bisexual/homosexual, and the remaining
2 (8%) were classified as having no sexual fantasies. Of the 3
participants classified as gender dysphoric, 2 were exclusively
homosexual in fantasy (i.e., sexually attracted to members of their
own birth sex). The other gender dysphoric participant reported no
sexual fantasies and described herself as being “dead sexually.”
(Of the 4 participants with a DSD, 3 were classified as exclusively
heterosexual in fantasy, and 1 reported no sexual fantasies; 2 were
classified as exclusively heterosexual in behavior, and 2 reported
no sexual behavior.)
For the EROS, we compared the participants classified as ex-
clusively heterosexual with those classified as bisexual/
homosexual on the basis of their Kinsey ratings. With age at
follow-up covaried, a 2 (sexual orientation: heterosexual vs. bi-
sexual/homosexual) 2 (EROS: attraction to men vs. attraction to
women) analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) revealed a significant
Sexual Orientation EROS interaction, F(1, 20) 25.67, p
.001, partial
2
.56.
Independent t tests showed that participants classified as het-
erosexual in fantasy had, on average, a higher heteroerotic EROS
score (M 2.03, SD .87) than participants classified as bisex-
ual/homosexual in fantasy (M 1.84, SD 1.34), but the differ-
ence was not significant, t(20) 1, d .19; however, participants
classified as bisexual/homosexual reported, on average, a signifi-
cantly higher EROS homoerotic score (M 3.32, SD 1.25) than
participants classified as heterosexual (M 1.02, SD .07),
t(20) ⫽⫺7.28, p .001, d ⫽⫺3.33. A paired-samples t test was
conducted to evaluate whether participants classified as heterosex-
ual reported higher heteroerotic fantasies than homoerotic fanta-
sies. The results indicated that the mean heteroerotic score was
significantly greater than the mean homoerotic score, t(14) 4.75,
p .001, with a large effect size of 1.23. Conversely, participants
classified as bisexual/homosexual reported significantly higher
homoerotic fantasies then heteroerotic fantasies, t(6) ⫽⫺2.61,
p .04, with a large effect size of .99.
Regarding Kinsey ratings of sexual orientation in behavior (see
Table 3), 11 participants (44%) were classified as exclusively
heterosexual, 6 (24%) were classified as bisexual/homosexual, and
the remaining 8 (32%) were classified as having no sexual expe-
riences. Of the 3 participants classified as gender dysphoric, 2
were exclusively homosexual in behavior (i.e., had sexual experi-
ences with members of their own birth sex). The other gender
dysphoric participant reported no sexual behaviors.
For the SHQ ratings, we compared the participants classified as
exclusively heterosexual with those classified as bisexual/
homosexual on the basis of their Kinsey ratings. A 2 (sexual
orientation: heterosexual vs. bisexual/homosexual) 2 (SHQ:
with men vs. with women) analysis of variance (ANOVA) re-
vealed a significant Sexual Orientation SHQ interaction, F(1,
13) 70.41, p .001, partial
2
.84. Independent t tests for the
SHQ scores showed that participants classified as heterosexual in
behavior reported, on average, significantly more heterosexual
sexual experiences (M 2.15, SD .54) than participants clas-
sified as bisexual/homosexual (M 1.00, SD .00), t(13) 4.12,
p .001, d 2.42. In fact, participants classified as bisexual/
homosexual reported no sexual experiences with men over the past
12 months. Participants classified as bisexual/homosexual re-
ported, on average, significantly more homosexual sexual experi-
ences (M 2.48, SD .40) than did participants classified as
heterosexual (M 1.04, SD .12), t(13) ⫽⫺11.17, p .001,
d ⫽⫺6.56.
For participants classified as having a “typical” (i.e., non-
gender-dysphoric) gender identity at follow-up, there were no
substantive disjunctions between Kinsey ratings and sexual iden-
tity self-labeling (see Table 3). One exception was a participant
who self-labeled as heterosexual, although she did not report any
sexual fantasies or behaviors in the 12 months prior to the inter-
view. For the 3 participants classified as gender dysphoric at
follow-up, 2 self-labeled as heterosexual; however, it should be
noted that their sexual orientation in relation to their birth sex was
homosexual. As noted earlier, the remaining gender-dysphoric
participant felt that she was “dead sexually” and labeled herself as
asexual.
One participant (ID 5 in Table 3) was classified as bisexual/
homosexual in fantasy but heterosexual in behavior. Her self-
labeled sexual identity was bisexual. For the 17 participants who
could be assigned a Kinsey rating between 0 and 6 for both
behavior and fantasy (i.e., excluding the 8 individuals who did not
report any sexual behavior [n 6] or any sexual fantasy and
behavior [n 2]; see Table 3), the correlation between Kinsey
fantasy and behavior ratings was .93 (df 15), p .001.
Odds Ratios for Bisexual/Homosexual Sexual Orientation
in Fantasy and Behavior
Odds ratios were calculated for bisexual/homosexual sexual
orientation in fantasy and behavior using prevalence estimates
from several major survey studies of sexual orientation in adoles-
cent girls and young women (Dickson, Paul, & Herbison, 2003;
Fergusson, Horwood, Ridder, & Beautrais, 2005; McCabe,
Hughes, Bostwick, & Boyd, 2005; Narring, Stronski, & Michaud,
2003; Remafedi, Resnick, Blum, & Harris, 1992; Russell & Seif,
2002). From these studies, base rates for bisexual/homosexual
sexual orientation in fantasy and behavior were estimated to range
from 2.0% to 5.0% in the female general population. The odds of
reporting bisexual/homosexual sexual orientation in fantasy in the
present sample was 8.9–23.1 times higher, and the odds of report-
ing bisexual/homosexual sexual orientation in behavior in the
present sample was 6.0–15.5 times higher than it is in women in
the general population.
Relation Between Age and Sexual Orientation
Table 4 shows the means and standard deviations of ages at
assessment and at follow-up as a function of Kinsey groups in
fantasy and behavior, respectively. For the Kinsey fantasy ratings,
a one-way ANOVA for age at follow-up was significant, F(2,
22) 4.91, p .017, while the ANOVA for age at assessment in
childhood approached statistical significance, F(2, 22) 2.58, p
.098. At follow-up, participants classified as bisexual/homosexual
were, on average, significantly older than participants classified as
heterosexual or asexual, t(21) ⫽⫺2.54, p .019, and t(8)
2.37, p .046, respectively. There was no significant difference
in the mean age at follow-up between participants classified as
heterosexual and those classified as asexual, t(15) ⫽⫺1.30, p
.211. For the Kinsey behavior ratings, the one-way ANOVAs for
40
DRUMMOND, BRADLEY, PETERSON-BADALI, AND ZUCKER
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
age at assessment and follow-up were nonsignificant, F(2, 22)
2.14, p .142, and F(2, 22) 2.73, p .087, respectively.
Social Desirability
One-way ANCOVAs (age at follow-up covaried) were con-
ducted to evaluate the proportion of socially desirable responses on
the M–C SDS for participants classified as heterosexual, bisexual/
homosexual, and asexual in fantasy and behavior. There were no
significant differences in the proportion of socially desirable re-
sponses on the M–C SDS as a function of Kinsey ratings in either
fantasy or behavior, F(2, 20) 1.00, ns, and F(2, 20) 1,
respectively (data not shown; see footnote 2).
Relation Between Sex-Typed Child Behavior and Sexual
Orientation
To evaluate whether degree of cross-sex-typed behavior in
childhood was related to sexual orientation at follow-up, we used
the z-composite of sex-typed behavior as a function of Kinsey
classification in fantasy (heterosexual, bisexual/homosexual, asex-
ual). With age at follow-up covaried, there was no significant
difference in participants’ cross-sex-typed behavior in childhood
as a function of sexual orientation in fantasy, F(2, 21) 1.06,
partial
2
.09 (data not shown; see footnote 2). For Kinsey
ratings in behavior, however, a one-way ANCOVA was signifi-
cant, F(2, 21) 6.45, p .006, the strength of which was large,
as assessed by partial
2
, with the Kinsey ratings accounting for
37% of the variance of participants’ cross-sex-typed behavior in
childhood. Participants classified as bisexual/homosexual (M
.52, SD .49) had significantly more cross-sex-typed behavior in
childhood than participants classified as heterosexual (M ⫽⫺.04,
SD .45) or asexual (M ⫽⫺.33, SD .39), both ps .05. There
was no significant difference in the mean z-composite of sex-typed
child behavior between participants classified as heterosexual and
those classified as asexual (see footnote 2).
For the Kinsey ratings in behavior, we reran this analysis with
the 3 gender-dysphoric participants removed (2 were classified as
bisexual/homosexual and 1 was classified as asexual). For the
z-composite, the main effect for Kinsey ratings in behavior re-
mained statistically significant, F(2, 18) 3.58, p .05, partial
2
.29.
Relation Between Recalled Childhood Cross-Gender
Behavior and Gender Identity at Follow-Up
We conducted an evaluation of recalled cross-gender behavior
between gender-dysphoric and non-gender-dysphoric participants.
Table 5 shows the means and standard deviations of the RCGI
Factor 1 score. Participants classified as gender dysphoric at
follow-up (n 2; Ms 1.29 and 1.81, respectively) recalled
significantly more cross-gender identity and role behavior in child-
hood than participants classified as having no gender dysphoria
Table 4
Means and Standard Deviations of Age (in Years) as a Function of Kinsey Ratings in Fantasy
and Behavior
Age
None
Exclusively
heterosexual Bisexual/homosexual
pM SD M SD M SD
By Kinsey fantasy ratings
a
At assessment 12.42 .35 7.96 3.10 9.75 2.73 .098
At follow-up 17.22 .18 21.76 4.78 27.50 5.88
.017
By Kinsey behavior ratings
b
At assessment 9.94 3.99 7.51 2.51 10.02 1.91 .142
At follow-up 20.66 5.11 22.81 4.79 27.45 6.93 .087
a
For participants grouped by Kinsey fantasy ratings, n 2, n 15, and n 8 for participants with no fantasies,
exclusively heterosexual fantasies, and bisexual/homosexual fantasies, respectively.
b
For participants grouped
by Kinsey behavior ratings, n 8, n 11, and n 6 for participants with no behaviors, exclusively heterosexual
behaviors, and bisexual/homosexual behaviors, respectively.
Table 5
Mean Factor Scores and Standard Deviations on the Recalled
Childhood Gender Identity/Gender Role Questionnaire (Zucker
et al., 2006) for Gender Identity Status and Sexual Orientation
at Follow-Up
Group MSD d n
Gender identity status
Gender dysphoric 1.55 .36 1.96 2
No gender dysphoria 2.69 .59 18
(Adolescent girls with GID) (2.15) (.58) (25)
Sexual orientation
a
Heterosexual 2.82 .54 1.88 15
(Heterosexual comparison sample) (3.34) (.53) (30)
Bisexual/homosexual 1.84 .44 5
(Homosexual comparison sample) (2.68) (.72) (21)
Note. The absolute range was 1.00–5.00. A lower score indicates more
recalled atypical gender identity and gender role behavior. Twenty partic-
ipants completed the questionnaire because the RCGI was not yet part of
the follow-up protocol for 5 participants. Groups and values in parentheses
are from Zucker et al. (2006); the factor scores were from a sample of
heterosexual and homosexual female university students unselected for
gender identity. GID gender identity disorder.
a
Sexual orientation was determined on the basis of Kinsey ratings for
fantasy and behavior.
41
SPECIAL SECTION: GENDER IDENTITY DISORDER IN GIRLS
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
(n 18; M 2.69; range, 1.56–3.87), t(18) ⫽⫺2.62, p .017.
As shown in Table 5, the mean Factor 1 score on the RCGI for the
participants with persistent gender dysphoria was more extreme
than it was for a sample of clinic-referred adolescent girls (n 25)
with GID reported on by Zucker et al. (2006), whereas the mean
score of the participants without gender dysphoria was somewhat
less extreme.
Further analyses on the RCGI Factor 1 score by sexual orien-
tation revealed that participants classified as bisexual/homosexual
recalled significantly more cross-gender identity and gender role
behavior in childhood than did participants classified as hetero-
sexual or asexual, t(18) 3.65, p .002.
Discussion
The data reported in this article represent the first systematic
psychosexual follow-up into late adolescence and young adulthood
of clinic-referred girls with potential problems in their gender
identity development. The two key findings were as follows: (a)
the percentage of girls with persistent gender dysphoria was mod-
est but arguably higher than the base rate of GID in the general
population of biological females, and (b) the percentage of girls
who differentiated a later bisexual/homosexual sexual orientation
was moderate but clearly higher than the base rates of bisexual/
homosexual sexual orientation in general survey and epidemiolog-
ical studies of adolescent girls and young adult women in which
sexual orientation (in fantasy and/or behavior) was assessed with
at least some gradation in response options (as opposed to simple
dichotomous items).
Before providing an analysis of these findings, we note two
limitations of the study. First, the sample size was small, but this
is, at least in part, understandable because the number of referred
girls to specialized gender identity clinics is notably lower than
that of referred boys (e.g., Cohen-Kettenis et al., 2003, 2006).
Second, the present study did not have a concurrent control group
(e.g., a group of girls referred for other kinds of clinical concerns
or a group of nonreferred girls). Accordingly, some of our com-
parative analyses relied on epidemiological or survey data.
Regarding the persistence of gender dysphoria from the child-
hood assessment to the follow-up, the present study found that the
vast majority of the girls showed desistance: 88% of the girls did
not report distress about their gender identity at follow-up. The
high rate of desistance appears to differ quite markedly from the
findings of other follow-up studies of adolescent girls and adult
women with GID (in which the baseline assessment is in adoles-
cence or adulthood). In these studies, the rate of GID persistence
appears to be, at minimum, around 70% (Cohen-Kettenis & van
Goozen, 1997; Smith, van Goozen, Kuiper, & Cohen-Kettenis,
2005). In a comparative developmental perspective, then, there
appears to be important variation in GID persistence between
childhood and adolescence/young adulthood.
How might this disjunction be understood? One possibility
pertains to the differences in the DSM criteria for GID that are used
for children versus those that are used for adolescents/adults. The
criteria for GID in girlhood place relatively greater weight on
surface behaviors of cross-gender identification, whereas the cri-
teria in adolescence and adulthood rely more strongly on behaviors
and feelings pertaining to the disjunction between gender subjec-
tivity and somatic sex. Thus, it is conceivable that the childhood
criteria for GID may “scoop in” girls who are at relatively low risk
for adolescent/adult gender dysphoria, which revolves so much
around somatic indicators (e.g., distress regarding breast develop-
ment or other markers of physical femaleness, etc.).
It should, however, be noted that adolescent girls and adult
women with GID typically recall the same kinds of cross-gender
behavior patterns in girlhood that correspond to the DSM criteria
for GID in childhood (e.g., Blanchard & Freund, 1983; Pearlman,
2006; Zucker et al., 2006), which are then augmented and exac-
erbated by the external physical markers of biological femaleness
at puberty. Indeed, in the present study, the recalled sex-typed
behavior from childhood of our participants was reasonably sim-
ilar to the childhood recollections of girls with GID assessed for
the first time in adolescence (see Table 5).
In the present study, 40% of the girls were not judged to have
met the complete DSM criteria for GID at the time of childhood
assessment (although some of these girls likely had met the com-
plete criteria at some earlier point in their development). Thus, on
the one hand, it could be argued that if some of the girls were
subthreshold for GID in childhood, then one might assume that
they would not be at risk for GID in adolescence or adulthood. On
the other hand, it could be argued that cross-gender identification
in girlhood (including subthreshold GID) is a risk factor for later
GID; that is, under some conditions, there is an intensification of
cross-gender identification that results in the development of gen-
der dysphoria (see Green, 2003). Indeed, clinical experience with
adolescent girls with GID indicates that not all of them would have
met the complete criteria for GID in girlhood. Indeed, it is not
uncommon for the parents of these girls to recall that their daugh-
ters identified as “tomboys” during childhood and that they did not
remember them voicing the desire to want to become a boy, but
that their gender dysphoria emerged only around the time of
puberty (see, e.g., Pearlman, 2006; Zucker, 2006, Case 1).
If one accepts the argument that girlhood cross-gender identifi-
cation is a risk factor for gender dysphoria in adolescence and
adulthood, the relatively high rate of desistance in the current study
(in comparison with the relatively high rate of persistence seen in
gender-dysphoric girls and women assessed for the first time in
adolescence or adulthood) suggests that there is some type of
plasticity in gender identity differentiation that operates early in
development but then narrows considerably by adolescence. Thus,
at least among the girls in the present sample, some factor or set of
factors may have operated to lessen the likelihood that their gender
dysphoria or cross-gender identification would persist or intensify
in adolescence and adulthood. Of course, such factors could in-
clude both biological and psychosocial influences, but the system-
atic identification of such factors was beyond the scope of the
present investigation.
To our knowledge, the results of the present study represent the
first prospective data set that shows that girlhood cross-gender
identification is associated with a relatively high rate of bisexual/
homosexual sexual orientation in adolescence and adulthood. Us-
ing survey data on sexual orientation in young women as a com-
parative metric, we estimated that the odds of reporting a bisexual/
homosexual sexual orientation in fantasy was 8.9–23.1 times
higher in the present sample and that the odds of reporting a
bisexual/homosexual sexual orientation in behavior was 6.7–15.5
times higher. In this respect, the data show at least some conver-
42
DRUMMOND, BRADLEY, PETERSON-BADALI, AND ZUCKER
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
gence with data from retrospective studies (Bailey & Zucker,
1995).
A strength of the present study was that the assessment of sexual
orientation was based on a multiparameter, face-to-face interview
from which Kinsey global ratings were derived and that was
complemented by self-report on psychometrically sound question-
naires (cf. Savin-Williams, 2006). Although one has to be cautious
about the possibility that our participants underreported a minority
sexual orientation, it should be recalled that we found no signifi-
cant relation between our Kinsey classifications and the propensity
to give socially desirable responses on the M–C SDS.
Our classification of participants’ sexual orientation was based
on fantasy and behavior ratings for the 12-month period prior to
follow-up. In the literature on women’s sexual orientation, there
has been a lot of recent discussion regarding its stability versus its
fluidity (see, e.g., Baumeister, 2000; Peplau et al., 1999). Diamond
(2005b), for example, followed 79 self-labeled lesbian, bisexual,
and “unlabeled” sexual minority women over an 8-year period
(mean age at baseline, 19 years). At the 8-year follow-up, 92.4%
of the women continued to self-label as lesbian, bisexual, or
unlabeled, although there was considerable fluctuation within
these three categories over time (e.g., lesbian to bisexual or unla-
beled to lesbian). The remaining 7.5% of the women self-labeled
as heterosexual at the follow-up. In our view, Diamond’s (2005b)
data suggest considerable stability of a minority sexual orientation
despite the evidence of greater fluidity within the subcategories of
lesbian, bisexual, and unlabeled.
One limitation of Diamond’s study was that it did not include a
group of self-labeled heterosexual women at baseline; thus, com-
parative evidence on the stability or fluidity of a majority sexual
orientation was not available. Using data from the National Lon-
gitudinal Survey of Adolescent Health, however, Savin-Williams
and Ream (2007) provided data on the stability of a heterosexual
sexual orientation (attraction and behavior) of several thousand
girls and women between the ages of 15 and 26 years in a
three-wave assessment. In their study, there was considerable
evidence for a stable heterosexual sexual orientation. For example,
only 3.1% of girls who reported exclusive heterosexual attractions
at Wave 1 reported bisexual or lesbian attractions at Wave 3, and
only 3.5% of girls who reported exclusive heterosexual behavior at
Wave 1 reported bisexual or lesbian behavior at the Wave 3. Given
these findings, the case could be made that our participants’ sexual
orientations will remain relatively stable over time but, on this
point, only continued follow-up can test this conjecture empiri-
cally.
Because there was considerable variability in sexual orientation
at follow-up, we made some relatively crude efforts at predicting
such variation (compromised, of course, by the small sample size).
There were hints in the data that younger age at assessment in
childhood was associated with a later heterosexual sexual orien-
tation (Table 4), but the effects were weak. The composite index of
sex-typed behavior in childhood was not significantly associated
with sexual orientation in fantasy, but it was with sexual orienta-
tion in behavior, with those participants classified as bisexual/
homosexual exhibiting more cross-gender behavior). We also
found that participants classified as bisexual/homosexual recalled
having engaged in more cross-gender behavior during childhood
than those classified as heterosexual or asexual (Table 5). These
data are suggestive, therefore, of a “dosage” effect, that is, that
degree of girlhood cross-gender identification is associated with a
greater likelihood of a later minority sexual orientation. Of course,
these preliminary findings need to be confirmed in much larger
clinical samples; in addition, it would be desirable to examine
whether or not variation in degree of girlhood cross-sex-typed
behavior is related to sexual orientation in epidemiological sam-
ples drawn from nonclinical populations.
How do the results of the present study compare with those of
follow-up studies of boys with GID? In Zucker (2005b), a
follow-up on 40 boys with GID from the same clinic, using the
same methods as in the present study, showed a persistence rate of
20%, only modestly higher than the rate of 12% for the girls in the
present study. In Zucker (2005b), 42.5% of the boys were classi-
fied as bisexual/homosexual in fantasy, which is again only mod-
estly higher than the rate of 32% for the girls in the present study;
however, the rate of a bisexual/homosexual sexual orientation in
fantasy was considerably lower than the 75% found by Green
(1987) in his study of feminine boys. In comparison with the boys
followed up by Green (1987) and by Zucker (2005b), it is impor-
tant to note that the girls in the present study were, on average,
several years older at follow-up, which, if anything, would suggest
that the likelihood of underreporting a minority sexual orientation
would be lower for this sample than for the samples of boys.
If it proves to be the case that cross-sex-typed behavior is,
indeed, less closely linked to a later bisexual/homosexual sexual
orientation in girls than it is in boys, this would be consistent with
a prediction made by Bailey and Zucker (1995) in their meta
analytic retrospective study. It would also be consistent with recent
theorizing on the greater flexibility of sexual orientation in women,
in which it has been argued that relational factors during adoles-
cence and adulthood play a more important role in sexual partner
preference than it does in men (Diamond, 2003, 2005a; Peplau et
al., 1999). It is apparent from the present study that there is
considerable within-sex variation to be explained in the long-term
psychosexual differentiation of behaviorally masculine girls, with
regard to both gender identity and sexual orientation. These find-
ings suggest that any reductionist account of psychosexual differ-
entiation will likely be unable to capture this variation. Multivar-
iate models are clearly required in order to identify the best
predictors of such within-sex variation. On this point, the field will
hopefully move forward as larger samples are collated, including
prospective, epidemiologically based cohorts that incorporate the-
oretically based predictor variables.
References
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical man-
ual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical man-
ual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man-
ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical man-
ual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
Bailey, J. M., Bechtold, K. T., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2002). Who are
tomboys and why should we study them? Archives of Sexual Behavior,
31, 333–341.
Bailey, J. M., & Zucker, K. J. (1995). Childhood sex-typed behavior and
sexual orientation: A conceptual analysis and quantitative review. De-
velopmental Psychology, 31, 43–55.
43
SPECIAL SECTION: GENDER IDENTITY DISORDER IN GIRLS
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Bakker, A., van Kesteren, P. J. M., Gooren, L. J. G., & Bezemer, P. D.
(1993). The prevalence of transsexualism in the Netherlands. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 87, 237–238.
Barrett, A. E., & White, H. R. (2002). Trajectories of gender role orien-
tation in adolescence and early adulthood: A prospective study of the
mental health effects of masculinity and femininity. Journal of Health
and Social Behavior, 43, 451–468.
Bartlett, N. H., & Vasey, P. L. (2006). A retrospective study of childhood
gender-atypical behavior in Samoan Fa’afafine. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 35, 659666.
Bates, J. E., & Bentler, P. M. (1973). Play activities of normal and
effeminate boys. Developmental Psychology, 9, 20–27.
Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: The
female sex drive as socially flexible and responsive. Psychological
Bulletin, 126, 347–374.
Berenbaum, S. A., & Bailey, J. M. (2003). Effects on gender identity of
prenatal androgens and genital appearance: Evidence from girls with
congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology &
Metabolism: Clinical and Experimental, 88, 1101–1106.
Blanchard, R., & Freund, K. (1983). Measuring masculine gender identity
in females. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 205–214.
Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., Owen, A., Kaijser, V. G., Bradley, S. J., & Zucker,
K. J. (2003). Demographic characteristics, social competence, and be-
havior problems in children with gender identity disorder: A cross-
national, cross-clinic comparative analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 31, 41–53.
Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., & Pfa¨fflin, F. (2003). Transgenderism and inter-
sexuality in childhood and adolescence: Making choices. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., & van Goozen, S. H. M. (1997). Sex reassignment
of adolescent transsexuals: A follow-up study. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 263–271.
Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., Wallien, M., Johnson, L. L., Owen-Anderson,
A. F. H., Bradley, S. J., & Zucker, K. J. (2006). A parent-report Gender
Identity Questionnaire for Children: A cross-national, cross-clinic com-
parative analysis. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 11, 397–
405.
Cohler, B. J., & Galatzer-Levy, R. M. (2000). The course of gay and
lesbian lives: Social and psychoanalytic perspectives. Chicago: Univer-
sity of Chicago Press.
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability
independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24,
349–354.
Deogracias, J. J., Johnson, L. L., Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Kessler, S. J.,
Schober, J. M., & Zucker, K. J. (2007). The Gender Identity/Gender
Dysphoria Questionnaire for Adolescents and Adults. Journal of Sex
Research, 44, 370–379.
Diamond, L. M. (2003). What does sexual orientation orient? A biobehav-
ioral model distinguishing romantic love and sexual desire. Psycholog-
ical Review, 110, 173–192.
Diamond, L. M. (2005a). From the heart or the gut? Sexual-minority
women’s experiences of desire for same-sex and other-sex partners.
Feminism & Psychology, 15, 10–14.
Diamond, L. M. (2005b). A new view of lesbian subtypes: Stable versus
fluid identity trajectories over an 8-year period. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 29, 119–128.
Dickson, N., Paul, C., & Herbison, P. (2003). Same-sex attraction in a birth
cohort: Prevalence and persistence in early adulthood. Social Science &
Medicine, 56, 1607–1615.
Doorn, C. D., Poortinga, J., & Verschoor, A. M. (1994). Cross-gender
identity in transvestites and male transsexuals. Archives of Sexual Be-
havior, 23, 185–201.
Drummond, K. D. (2006). A follow-up study of girls with gender identity
disorder. Unpublished master’s thesis, Ontario Institute for Studies in
Education of the University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
Ehrhardt, A. A., Grisanti, G., & McCauley, E. A. (1979). Female-to-male
transsexuals compared to lesbians: Behavioral patterns of childhood and
adolescent development. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 8, 481–490.
Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., Ridder, E. M., & Beautrais, A. L.
(2005). Sexual orientation and mental health in a birth cohort of young
adults. Psychological Medicine, 35, 971–981.
Freund, K., Langevin, R., Satterberg, J., & Steiner, B. (1977). Extension of
the Gender Identity Scale for Males. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 6,
507–519.
Fridell, S. R., Owen-Anderson, A., Johnson, L. L., Bradley, S. J., &
Zucker, K. J. (2006). The Playmate and Play Style Preferences Struc-
tured Interview: A comparison of children with gender identity disorder
and controls. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35, 729–737.
Gottschalk, L. (2003). Same-sex sexuality and childhood gender non-
conformity: A spurious connection. Journal of Gender Studies, 12,
35–50.
Green, R. (1987). The “sissy boy syndrome” and the development of
homosexuality. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Green, R. (2003). The “T” word [Letter to the editor]. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 32, 1.
Green, R., Williams, K., & Goodman, M. (1982). Ninety-nine “tomboys”
and “non-tomboys”: Behavioral contrasts and demographic similarities.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 11, 247–266.
Hardt, J., & Rutter, M. (2004). Validity of adult retrospective reports of
adverse childhood experiences: Review of the evidence. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 260–273.
Hegarty, P. (1999). Recalling childhood, constructing identity: Norms and
stereotypes in theories of sexual orientation. Unpublished doctoral dis-
sertation, Stanford University, Stanford, CA.
Holden, R. R., & Fekken, G. C. (1989). Three common social desirability
scales: Friends, acquaintances, or strangers? Journal of Research in
Personality, 23, 180–191.
Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four factor index of social status. Unpublished
manuscript, Department of Sociology, Yale University, New Haven, CT.
Hughes, I. A., Houk, C., Ahmed, S. F., Lee, P. A., & Lawson Wilkins
Pediatric Endocrine Society/European Society for Paediatric Endocri-
nology Consensus Group. (2006). Consensus statement on management
of intersex disorders. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 91, 554–563.
Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychol-
ogist, 60, 581–592.
Johnson, L. L., Bradley, S. J., Birkenfeld-Adams, A. S., Radzins Kuksis,
M. A., Maing, D. M., Mitchell, J. N., & Zucker, K. J. (2004). A
parent-report Gender Identity Questionnaire for Children. Archives of
Sexual Behavior, 33, 105–116.
Khuri, J., & Ruble, D. N. (2006, April). The consequences of early
gender-typicality: Findings in a female sample. Poster session presented
at the Second Gender Development Conference, San Francisco, CA.
King, M. F., & Brunner, G. C. (2000). Social desirability bias: A neglected
aspect of validity testing. Psychology and Marketing, 17, 79–103.
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior
in the human male. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.
Kite, M. E., & Deaux, K. (1987). Gender belief systems: Homosexuality
and the implicit inversion theory. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11,
83–96.
Langevin, R. (1985). Sexual strands: Understanding and treating sexual
anomalies in men. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994). The
social organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lippa, R. (1998). Gender-related individual differences and the structure of
vocational interests: The importance of the people–things dimension.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 996–1009.
44
DRUMMOND, BRADLEY, PETERSON-BADALI, AND ZUCKER
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Lippa, R. A. (2002). Gender, nature, and nurture. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Liss, M. B. (Ed.). (1983). Social and cognitive skills: Sex roles and
children’s play. New York: Academic Press.
Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together.
Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.
McCabe, S. E., Hughes, T. L., Bostwick, W., & Boyd, C. J. (2005).
Assessment of difference in dimensions of sexual orientation: Implica-
tions for substance use research in a college-age population. Journal of
Studies on Alcohol, 66, 620629.
Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L. (1994). Intersexuality and the diagnosis of
gender identity disorder. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23, 21–40.
Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L. (2005). Gender identity outcome in female-
raised 46,XY persons with penile agenesis, cloacal exstrophy of the
bladder, or penile ablation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 423–438.
Narring, F., Stronski, H. S., & Michaud, P.-A. (2003). Prevalence and
dimensions of sexual orientation in Swiss adolescents: A cross-sectional
survey of 16- to 20-year-old students. Acta Paediatrica, 92, 233–239.
Pearlman, S. F. (2006). Terms of connection: Mother-talk about female-
to-male transgender children. Journal of GLBT Family Studies, 2(3/4),
93–122.
Peplau, L. A., & Huppin, M. (in press). Masculinity, femininity and the
development of sexual orientation. Journal of Gay and Lesbian Psycho-
therapy.
Peplau, L. A., Spalding, L. R., Conley, T. D., & Veniegas, R. C. (1999).
The development of sexual orientation in women. Annual Review of Sex
Research, 10, 70–99.
Remafedi, G., Resnick, M., Blum, R., & Harris, L. (1992). Demography of
sexual orientation in adolescents. Pediatrics, 89, 714–721.
Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Gender devel-
opment. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.) and N. Eisenberg
(Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed.), Vol. 3: Social,
emotional, and personality development (pp. 858–932). New York:
Wiley.
Russell, S. T., & Seif, H. (2002). Bisexual female adolescents: A critical
analysis of past research, and results from a national survey. Journal of
Bisexuality, 2(2–3), 73–94.
Savin-Williams, R. C. (2006). Who’s gay? Does it matter? Current Direc-
tions in Psychological Science, 15, 4044.
Savin-Williams, R. C., & Ream, G. L. (2007). Prevalence and stability of
sexual orientation components during adolescence and young adulthood.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 385–393.
Silverthorn, N. A., & Gekoski, W. L. (1995). Social desirability effects on
measures of adjustment to university, independence from parents, and
self-efficacy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 244–251.
Smith, Y. L. S., van Goozen, S. H. M., Kuiper, A. J., & Cohen-Kettenis,
P. T. (2005). Sex reassignment: Outcomes and predictors of treatment
for adolescent and adult transsexuals. Psychological Medicine, 35, 89
99.
Storms, M. D. (1980). Theories of sexual orientation. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 38, 783–792.
Strahan, R., & Gerbasi, K. C. (1972). Short, homogeneous versions of the
Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychol-
ogy, 28, 191–193.
Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). Stanford–Binet
Intelligence Scale (4th ed.). Chicago: Riverside.
Wechsler, D. (1974). Manual: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—
Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third
Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition.
San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelli-
gence—Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Zucker, K. J. (2005a). Commentary on Gottschalk’s (2003) “Same-sex
sexuality and childhood gender non-conformity: A spurious connec-
tion.” Journal of Gender Studies, 14, 55–60.
Zucker, K. J. (2005b). Gender identity disorder in children and adolescents.
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 467–492.
Zucker, K. J. (2005c). Measurement of psychosexual differentiation. Ar-
chives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 375–388.
Zucker, K. J. (2006). Gender identity disorder. In D. A. Wolfe & E. J. Mash
(Eds.), Behavioral and emotional disorders in adolescents: Nature,
assessment, and treatment (pp. 535–562). New York: Guilford Press.
Zucker, K. J. (in press). Reflections on the relation between sex-typed
behavior in childhood and sexual orientation in adulthood. Journal of
Gay & Lesbian Psychotherapy.
Zucker, K. J., & Bradley, S. J. (1995). Gender identity disorder and
psychosexual problems in children and adolescents. New York: Guilford
Press.
Zucker, K. J., Bradley, S. J., Lowry Sullivan, C. B., Kuksis, M.,
Birkenfeld-Adams, A., & Mitchell, J. N. (1993). A gender identity
interview for children. Journal of Personality Assessment, 61, 443–456.
Zucker, K. J., Bradley, S. J., Oliver, G., Blake, J., Fleming, S., & Hood, J.
(1996). Psychosexual development of women with congenital adrenal
hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior, 30, 300–318.
Zucker, K. J., Doering, R. W., Bradley, S. J., & Finegan, J. K. (1982).
Sex-typed play in gender-disturbed children: A comparison to sibling
and psychiatric controls. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 11, 309–321.
Zucker, K. J., Finegan, J. K., Doering, R. W., &. Bradley, S. J. (1983).
Human figure drawings of gender-problem children: A comparison to
siblings, psychiatric, and normal controls. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 11, 287–298.
Zucker, K. J., Lozinski, J. A., Bradley, S. J., & Doering, R. W. (1992).
Sex-typed responses in the Rorschach protocols of children with gender
identity disorder. Journal of Personality Assessment, 58, 295–310.
Zucker, K. J., Mitchell, J. N., Bradley, S. J., Tkachuk, J., Cantor, J. M., &
Allin, S. M. (2006). The Recalled Childhood Gender Identity/Gender
Role questionnaire: Psychometric properties. Sex Roles, 54, 469483.
Received September 27, 2006
Revision received July 24, 2007
Accepted July 31, 2007
45
SPECIAL SECTION: GENDER IDENTITY DISORDER IN GIRLS
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Studies indicate that about 50% of males who were diagnosed with childhood gender dysphoria were androphilic at the time of follow-up in adulthood (Singh et al., 2021;Wallien & Cohen-Kettenis, 2008;Zucker, 1990), far higher than what is observed in the general population (Gates, 2011). In contrast, between 0-16% of the females that were diagnosed with childhood gender dysphoria were gynephilic at the time of follow-up in adulthood (Drummond et al., 2008;Steensma, McGuire, et al., 2013;Wallien & Cohen-Kettenis, 2008). Two points deserve further comment with respect to the studies on females. ...
... First, the study reporting a complete absence of gynephilic sexual attraction at follow-up included only three cisgender female participants (Wallien & Cohen-Kettenis, 2008). Second, the rate of female gynephilia at follow-up reported by the other two studies (Drummond et al., 2008;Steensma, McGuire, et al., 2013), while much lower than that of male androphilia, was still well above the rate of female gynephilia in the general population (Gates, 2011). In undertaking any cross-cultural comparison, it is important to apply context-independent terminology as culturally-specific terminology may hold different intersubjective meaning across cultures and among diverse sample populations (Vasey, 2023). ...
... Among transgender females, gynephilic individuals report higher rates of early onset of sex-atypical behavior and cross-gender ideation compared to those who are non-gynephilic (Cerwenka et al., 2014;Nieder et al., 2011). Finally, follow-up studies of clinically diagnosed gender dysphoric children show that more than 90% of those whose gender dysphoria persisted into adulthood and thus, could be considered transgender, were either androphilic males or gynephilic females (Drummond et al., 2008;Singh et al., 2021;Steensma, McGuire, et al., 2013;Wallien & Cohen-Kettenis, 2008). ...
... B. folgender transsexueller Entwicklung im Erwachsenenalter und/oder auch begleitende psychische Belastungen bzw. Störungen (siehe für verschiedene Verläufe hierzu die Arbeiten von Drummond, Bradley, Peterson-Badali & Zucker, 2008;Englert & Haas, 2023;Mahfouda, Panos et al., 2019;Mahfouda et al., 2023;Steensma et al., 2013a;Wallien & Cohen-Kettenis, 2008). Die Daten zur Häufigkeit der Verlaufsformen variieren, auch in Abhängigkeit von den untersuchten Stichproben. ...
... B. ein nur unzureichendes Erreichen eines Wohlbefindens durch die begonnenen körperlichen Veränderungen, eine reduzierte Libido, unbefriedigende Sexualität oder ein nicht (mehr) erfüllbarer Kinderwunsch, auftreten und die damalige Entscheidung bereuen lassen (vgl. die Arbeiten von Drummond et al., 2008;Englert & Haas, 2023;Steensma et al., 2013a;Wallien & Cohen-Kettenis, 2008). Die hier genannten erheblichen Konsequenzen einer ggfs. ...
Article
Full-text available
Zusammenfassung: Fragestellung: Die Unterdrückung der physiologischen Pubertät mittels sog. pubertätsblockierender Präparate (PB) und die Gabe gegengeschlechtlicher Hormone (sog. Cross-Sex-Hormone [CSH]) bei Minderjährigen mit Geschlechtsdysphorie (GD) wird als Intervention für ebendiese Personen diskutiert und teilweise in der klinischen Praxis bei dieser Population auch angewendet. Zwei Übersichtsarbeiten (eine zur PB-, eine zur CSH-Gabe) des britischen National Institute for Clinical Excellence (NICE) aus dem Jahr 2020 zeigten keinen eindeutigen klinischen Nutzen in Bezug auf kritische Zielvariablen sowie andere wichtige Variablen, insbesondere keine eindeutige Verbesserung der GD-Symptomatik. Weiterhin wurde die klinisch-wissenschaftliche Qualität der bis dahin vorliegenden Befunde gemäß Modified-GRADE-Kriterien als „sehr gering“ eingestuft. Methodik: Die vorliegende systematische Übersichtsarbeit umfasst eine aktualisierte Literatursuche gemäß NICE-Vorgehensweise für die seit Erscheinen der zuvor genannten beiden NICE-Übersichtsarbeiten neu erschienenen Arbeiten bezüglich der PB- und der CSH-Gabe bei GD (Suchzeitraum Juli 2020 bis August 2023). Ergebnisse: Die neue Literatursuche ergab keinerlei neue Originalarbeiten zur PB-Gabe entsprechend den vordefinierten PICO-Kriterien mit Blick auf kritische oder wichtige Zielvariablen gemäß NICE. Für die CSH-Gabe fanden sich zwei neue Originalarbeiten mit geringer Teilnehmendenzahl, welche die PICO-Kriterien erfüllten, jedoch gemäß Modified-GRADE-Kriterien mit geringer klinisch-wissenschaftlicher Sicherheit bzw. Qualität eingestuft wurden und keine neuen belastbaren Befunde zeigten. Derzeit vorliegende Studien zur PB- und CSH-Gabe haben signifikante konzeptionelle und auch methodische Mängel. Schlussfolgerungen: Die Studienlage zur PB- und CSH-Gabe bei Minderjährigen mit GD ist weiterhin sehr begrenzt und basiert auf wenigen Studien mit unzureichender Methodik und Qualität. Aussagekräftige Langzeitstudien fehlen bisher. Die aktuelle Studienlage deutet derzeit nicht darauf hin, dass sich die GD im Speziellen und die psychische Gesundheit im Allgemeinen im Verlauf der weiteren Entwicklung nach Gabe von PB oder CSH bedeutsam verbessern. Begleitende psychotherapeutische Interventionen bei Kindern und Jugendlichen mit GD zur Unterstützung bzw. zur Minderung der erlebten Belastungen können je nach individueller Situation der Betroffenen ggfs. notwendig werden. Sofern PB- und CSH-Gaben bei Minderjährigen mit GD nach sorgfältiger Risiko-Nutzen-Analyse, abgeschlossener kinder- und jugendpsychiatrischer Diagnostik und ggfs. folgender Behandlung auch begleitender psychischer Symptome oder Störungen bzw. Belastungsfaktoren zum Einsatz kommen sollten, so kann ein solches Vorgehen im Rahmen von Forschungsprojekten bzw. klinischen Studien – wie aktuell in England praktiziert – zum weiteren Erkenntnisgewinn beitragen und wichtige Daten liefern. Das elektronische Supplement (ESM) 1 ist eine adaptierte und gekürzte englische Version dieser Arbeit.
... The boys surveyed were more likely to identify as gay than as trans in adulthood (Bradley, Zucker, 1990). Persistence of dysphoria into adulthood, at 12-27% is reported by Drummond et al. (2008) and Wallien and Cohen-Kettenis (2008). Data derived from 12 studies compiled by Marianowicz-Szczygieł (2021) showed that 82% of children who manifested gender identity disorder in childhood no longer manifested it in their teenage years and earlier adulthood. ...
Article
Full-text available
The review article presents the phenomenon of transsexualism through the prism of the etiology, scale and dynamics of the phenomenon as well as the most controversial social consequences concerning "sex change" and the acceptance or non-acceptance of voluntary personal forms adopted by children and adolescents. Review of contemporary literature, analysis of positions on the issues of the so-called gender changes (gender reassignment, gender matching). The text shows the evolution of understanding of gender identity disorders in the ICD-10 and DSM-5 classifications as well as the controversy related to the tendencies depathologizing gender inconsistency in the ICD-11. Looking at the etiological issues we present an attempt of in-depth psychological analyzes as opposed to the dominant, reductive medical approach. In the text, we also recall the basic developmental regularities of children and adolescents, often overlooked in discussions on transsexualism. The basic developmental regularities of children and adolescents, often overlooked in discussions on transsexualism, were also reminded. As the review of the data shows the rapid increase in gender identity disorders in recent years, their pronunciation leads to emphasizing the growing role of pop culture influences on young people. There is also a lack of research on the use of puberty blockers in children/adolescents - their introduction appears to be an experiment with, in fact, unknown consequences. The analysis of developmental regularities, the transience of dysphoric tendencies and the lack of reliable scientific data on the use of puberty blockers and the consequences of taking hormones of the opposite sex lead to the conclusion that accepting voluntary personal forms proposed by children/teenagers is premature.
Article
Die Debatte über trans* ist sehr aktuell und wird polarisiert geführt. In der Auseinandersetzung sollte auf eine gendersensible und genderinklusive Sprache geachtet werden, insbesondere im Umgang mit Menschen, welche sich als trans* oder nicht-binär definieren. Sprache drückt unsere Haltung in Bezug auf die Integration diverser Geschlechtsidentitäten aus und beeinflusst unsere Realität. Geschlechtsidentität entwickelt sich in der frühen Kindheit in einem Wechselspiel zwischen Kind und Bezugspersonen aufgrund von biologischer Veranlagung und Umweltreaktionen auf Eigenschaften und Verhalten des Individuums. Geschlechtsidentität kann in der Folge von Kongruenzoder Inkongruenzerleben begleitet sein, je nachdem ob das Kind mit den Erwartungen des Umfeldes korrespondiert und wie sein Erleben im Verhältnis zu der sich entwickelnden Körperlichkeit steht. Geschlechtsidentität bezieht sich auf die zugeordnete Geschlechtsrolle und die körperliche Geschlechtlichkeit und wird besonders stark erlebt als Unstimmigkeit oder Transidentität. Für Kinder und Jugendliche mit früher Manifestation einer dauerhaften Geschlechtsinkongruenz stehen medizinische Behandlungen zur Verfügung, deren Indikation sorgfältig unter Abwägen möglicher Risiken und Nutzen gestellt werden muss. Cis und trans* Jugendliche stehen heute in einem Spannungsfeld sich wandelnder geschlechtlicher Möglichkeiten. Trans Jugendliche benötigen sorgfältige Begleitung durch das Umfeld. Eine Versachlichung der Debatte und vertiefte Erörterung ist dringend notwendig, um voreilige Schlussfolgerungen zu vermeiden und geschlechtsvarianten und geschlechtsinkongruenten Kindern und Jugendlichen die notwendigen Hilfen für eine psychisch gesunde Entwicklung zu ermöglichen.
Article
Aufbauend auf einem bio-psycho-sozialen Modell von Geschlecht werden Vorschläge für die Verwendung und Abgrenzung der Begriffe „inter-geschlechtlich“, „trans-geschlechtlich“ und „cis-geschlechtlich“ gemacht. Mit Blick auf die Gesundheitsversorgung werden die Unterschiede, insbesondere mit Blick auf die Indikationsstellung für körperverändernde medizinische Maßnahmen, dargestellt und diskutiert.
Article
Importance Concerns about the mental health of youths going through gender identity transitions have received increased attention. There is a need for empirical evidence to understand how transitions in self-reported gender identity are associated with mental health. Objective To examine whether and how often youths changed self-reported gender identities in a longitudinal sample of sexual and gender minority (SGM) youths, and whether trajectories of gender identity were associated with depressive symptoms. Design, Setting, and Participants This cohort study used data from 4 waves (every 9 months) of a longitudinal community-based study collected in 2 large cities in the US (1 in the Northeast and 1 in the Southwest) between November 2011 and June 2015. Eligible participants included youths who self-identified as SGM from community-based agencies and college groups for SGM youths. Data analysis occurred from September 2022 to June 2023. Exposure Gender identity trajectories and gender identity variability. Main Outcomes and Measures The Beck Depression Inventory for Youth (BDI-Y) assessed depressive symptoms. Gender identity variability was measured as the number of times participants’ gender identity changed. Hierarchical linear models investigated gender identity trajectories and whether gender identity variability was associated with depressive symptoms over time. Results Among the 366 SGM youths included in the study (mean [SD] age, 18.61 [1.71] years; 181 [49.4%] assigned male at birth and 185 [50.6%] assigned female at birth), 4 gender identity trajectory groups were identified: (1) cisgender across all waves (274 participants ), (2) transgender or gender diverse (TGD) across all waves (32 participants), (3) initially cisgender but TGD by wave 4 (ie, cisgender to TGD [28 participants]), and (4) initially TGD but cisgender by wave 4 (ie, TGD to cisgender [32 participants]). One in 5 youths (18.3%) reported a different gender identity over a period of approximately 3.5 years; 28 youths varied gender identity more than twice. The cisgender to TGD group reported higher levels of depression compared with the cisgender group at baseline ( Β = 4.66; SE = 2.10; P = .03), but there was no statistical difference once exposure to lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender violence was taken into account ( Β = 3.31; SE = 2.36; P = .16). Gender identity variability was not associated with within-person change in depressive symptoms ( Β = 0.23; SE = 0.74; P = .75) or the level of depressive symptoms ( Β = 2.43; SE = 2.51; P = .33). Conclusions These findings suggest that gender identity can evolve among SGM youths across time and that changes in gender identity are not associated with changes in depressive symptoms. Further longitudinal work should explore gender identity variability and adolescent and adult health.
Article
Full-text available
The definition of gender dysphoria has been the subject of extensive scientific debate in various fields. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM-V) characterizes gender dysphoria as a psychological condition due to a discrepancy between perceived and assigned gender. The scientific community has engaged in an extensive debate over the years regarding the classification of gender dysphoria, initially characterizing it as a gender identity disorder and subsequently removing it from the category of mental disorder. This paper aims to use bibliometric techniques to analyze scientific productivity and study the evolution of content on gender dysphoria from 1991 to 2022. It provides to map the scientific research production in this field through the science mapping approach highlighting the changes that have taken place over the past three decades.
Article
Full-text available
Abstrakt: Celem tego przeglądowego tekstu jest przybliżenie zjawiska transseksualizmu przez pryzmat etiologii, skali i dynamiki zaburzenia oraz najbardziej kontrowersyjnych skutków społecznych dotyczących "zmiany płci" oraz akceptacji bądź nieakceptacji wolincjonalnych form osobowych przyjmowanych przez dzieci i młodzież. Dokonano przeglądu współczesnej literatury i analizy stanowisk dotyczących problematyki tzw. zmiany płci (korekty płci, dopa-sowania płci). Tekst pokazuje ewolucję rozumienia zaburzeń tożsamości płciowej w klasyfikacjach ICD-10 i DSM-5 oraz kontrowersje związane z ten-dencjami depatologizującymi niezgodność płciową w ICD-11. W spojrzeniu na problematykę etiologiczną zaprezentowano próbę pogłębionych analiz psychologicznych w odróżnieniu od dominującego podejścia medycznego. Przypomniano także podstawowe prawidłowości rozwojowe dzieci i młodzieży, nierzadko pomijane w dyskusjach nad transseksualizmem. Przegląd danych pokazuje lawinowe narastanie zaburzeń tożsamości płciowej w ostatnich latach, ich wymowa prowadzi do podkreślania wzrastającej roli oddziaływań popkulturowych na młodzież. Widoczny jest także brak badań nad stosowaniem blokerów dojrzewania płciowego u dzieci/młodzieży-ich wprowadzanie jawi się jako eksperyment o, w gruncie rzeczy, nieznanych konsekwencjach. Analiza prawidłowości rozwojowych, przemijalność tendencji dysforycznych oraz brak rzetelnych danych naukowych na temat stosowania blokerów dojrzewania płciowego i konsekwencji przyjmowania hormonów płci przeciwnej prowadzą do konkluzji, iż akceptowanie wolincjonalnych form osobowych propono-wanych przez dzieci/nastolatki jest przedwczesne. Słowa kluczowe: zaburzenia tożsamości płciowej, transseksualizm, dysforia płciowa, niezgodność płciowa Abstract: The review article presents the phenomenon of transsexualism through the prism of the etiology, scale and dynamics of the phenomenon as well as the most controversial social consequences concerning "sex change" and the acceptance or non-acceptance of voluntary personal forms adopted by children and adolescents. Review of contemporary literature, analysis of positions on the issues of the so-called gender changes (gender reassignment, gender matching). The text shows the evolution of understanding of gender identity disorders in the ICD-10 and DSM-5 classifications as well as the controversy related to the tendencies depathologizing gender inconsistency in the ICD-11. Looking at the etiological issues we present an attempt of in-depth psychological analyzes as opposed to the dominant, reductive medical approach. In the text, we also recall the basic developmental regularities of children and adolescents, often overlooked in discussions on transsexualism. The basic developmental regularities of children and adolescents, often overlooked in discussions on transsexualism, were also reminded. As the review of the data shows the rapid increase in gender identity disorders in recent years, their pronunciation leads to emphasizing the growing role of pop culture influences on young people. There is also a lack of research on the use of puberty blockers in children/adolescents-their introduction appears to be an experiment with, in fact, unknown consequences. The analysis of developmental regularities, the transience of dysphoric tendencies and the lack of reliable scientific data on the use of puberty blockers and the consequences of taking hormones of the opposite sex lead to the conclusion that accepting voluntary personal forms proposed by children/teenagers is premature.
Preprint
Deenz Gender Dysphoria Scale (DGDS) is designed to explore inclinations towards gender dysphoria in non-clinical populations. Developed with the aim of understanding individuals’ experiences and feelings regarding their gender, the scale provides valuable insights into this complex phenomenon. The DGDS was not intended for clinical diagnosis according to DSM-5 criteria but rather to assess tendencies towards gender dysphoria and discomfort with one’s assigned gender. In a case study involving 45 college students, randomly selected from diverse streams and ethnic backgrounds, the DGDS demonstrated its effectiveness in identifying individuals experiencing gender dysphoria-related discomfort or distress. The study sample comprised 20 male and 25 female students, ensuring representation across genders.
Article
Full-text available
Although it is typically presumed that heterosexual individuals only fall in love with other-gender partners and gay-lesbian individuals only fall in love with same-gender partners, this is not always so. The author develops a biobehavioral model of love and desire to explain why. The model specifies that (a) the evolved processes underlying sexual desire and affectional bonding are functionally independent; (b) the processes underlying affectional bonding are not intrinsically oriented toward other-gender or same-gender partners; (c) the biobehavioral links between love and desire are bidirectional, particularly among women. These claims are supported by social-psychological, historical, and cross-cultural research on human love and sexuality as well as by evidence regarding the evolved biobehavioral mechanisms underlying mammalian mating and social bonding.
Article
Full-text available
The present study reports on the construction of a dimensional measure of gender identity (gender dysphoria) for adolescents and adults. The 27-item gender identity/gender dysphoria questionnaire for adolescents and adults (GID YQ-AA) was administered to 389 university); students (heterosexual and nonheterosexual) and 73 clinic-referred patients with gender identity disorder. Principal axis factor analysis indicated that a one-factor solution, accounting for 61.3% of the total variance, best fits the data. Factor loadings were all >= 30 (median,.82; range,.34-96). A mean total score (Cronbach's alpha,.97) was computed, which showed strong evidence for discriminant validity in that the gender identity patients had significantly more gender dysphoria than both the heterosexual and nonheterosexual university students. Using a cut-point of 3.00, we found the sensitivity was 90.4% for the gender identity patients and specificity was 99.7% for the controls. The utility of the GIDYQ-AA is discussed.
Article
Females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) resulting from 21-hydroxylase deficiency are relatively common among those with intersex conditions. Prenatal androgen exposure is independent of rearing gender, and it is possible to assess variations in gender identity in relation to genital appearance and genital surgery. For these reasons, the authors determined gender identity, using a 9-item interview, in 43 girls with classic CAH who ranged in age from 3-18 years. Seven girls identified as tomboys and 29 control girls also were evaluated. The latter included 25 unaffected sisters and cousins of girls and boys with CAH as well as 4 sisters of tomboys. Each of the interview items was scored for sex-typicality. Girls with CAH, on average, had gender scores intermediate between those of tomboys and control girls. Only 5 of the 43, however, had scores outside the control range. The difference in scores between girls with CAH and control subjects was statistically significant and could not be ascribed to outliers. Tomboys also had significantly higher scores than control girls; 3 of 7 were outside the control range. There was significantly more variation in gender identity in tomboys than in girls with CAH or control subjects. Gender identity did not correlate significantly with age. The CAH and control groups were similar in some items of gender identify such as discomfort with being a girl and the wish to be a boy. In the girls with CAH, there was no evident connection between gender identity and either the degree of genital virilization or the age at which genital reconstructive surgery was carried out. Moderate androgen excess early in development, in the form of CAH, clearly increases the risk of atypical gender identity, but this risk could not be predicted from genital virilization in the present study. These females are sex-atypical in some respects but not in others. It does not seem likely that gender role behaviors can predict gender identity or sexual orientation in this population. In addition, behavior cannot be predicted from any single parameter such as the degree of androgen exposure, the appearance of the genitals, or the child-rearing environment.
Chapter
Gender identity refers to a person's basic sense of self as male or female. Gender dysphoria refers to the distress one experiences when one's gender identity does not match one's assigned sex at birth, which often leads to the strong desire to become a member of the other gender. Research suggests that gender identity differentiation is the result of a complex interplay among biological and psychosocial factors. There are various therapeutic approaches, including both psychosocial and biomedical interventions, designed to reduce gender dysphoria.
Book
Transgenderism and Intersexuality in Childhood and Adolescence: Making Choices presents an overview of the research, clinical insights, and ethical dilemmas relevant to clinicians who treat intersex youth and their families. Exploring gender development from a cross-cultural perspective, esteemed scholar Peggy T. Cohen-Kettenis and experienced practitioner Friedemann Pfäfflin focus on assessment, diagnosis, and treatment issues. To bridge research and practical application, they include numerous case studies, definitions of relevant terminology, and salient chapter summaries.