ArticlePDF Available

Investigating the Roles of Knowledge and Cognitive Abilities in Older Adult Information Seeking on the Web

Authors:
  • Weill Cornell Medicine, New York City, United States

Abstract and Figures

This study investigated the influences of knowledge, particularly Internet, Web browser, and search engine knowledge, as well as cognitive abilities on older adult information seeking on the Internet. The emphasis on aspects of cognition was informed by a modeling framework of search engine information-seeking behavior. Participants from two older age groups were recruited: twenty people in a younger-old group (ages 60-70) and twenty people in an older-old group (ages 71-85). Ten younger adults (ages 18-39) served as a comparison group. All participants had at least some Internet search experience. The experimental task consisted of six realistic search problems, all involving information related to health and well-being and which varied in degree of complexity. The results indicated that though necessary, Internet-related knowledge was not sufficient in explaining information-seeking performance, and suggested that a combination of both knowledge and key cognitive abilities is important for successful information seeking. In addition, the cognitive abilities that were found to be critical for task performance depended on the search problem's complexity. Also, significant differences in task performance between the younger and the two older age groups were found on complex, but not on simple problems. Overall, the results from this study have implications for instructing older adults on Internet information seeking and for the design of Web sites.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Investigating the Roles of Knowledge and Cognitive Abilities in
Older Adult Information Seeking on the Web
JOSEPH SHARIT,
Department of Industrial Engineering, University of Miami, P.O. Box 248294, Coral Gables, FL
33124
MARIO A. HERNÁNDEZ,
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Miami Miller School of Medicine,
1695 N.W. 9th Ave, Miami, FL 33136
SARA J. CZAJA, and
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Miami Miller School of Medicine,
1695 N.W. 9th Ave, Miami, FL 33136
PETER PIROLLI
Palo Alto Research Center, 3333 Coyote Hill Road, Palo Alto, CA 94304
JOSEPH SHARIT: ; MARIO A. HERNÁNDEZ: ; SARA J. CZAJA: sczaja@med-miami.edu; PETER PIROLLI:
Abstract
This study investigated the influences of knowledge, particularly Internet, Web browser, and search
engine knowledge, as well as cognitive abilities on older adult information seeking on the Internet.
The emphasis on aspects of cognition was informed by a modeling framework of search engine
information-seeking behavior. Participants from two older age groups were recruited: twenty people
in a younger-old group (ages 60–70) and twenty people in an older-old group (ages 71–85). Ten
younger adults (ages 18–39) served as a comparison group. All participants had at least some Internet
search experience. The experimental task consisted of six realistic search problems, all involving
information related to health and well-being and which varied in degree of complexity. The results
indicated that though necessary, Internet-related knowledge was not sufficient in explaining
information-seeking performance, and suggested that a combination of both knowledge and key
cognitive abilities is important for successful information seeking. In addition, the cognitive abilities
that were found to be critical for task performance depended on the search problem’s complexity.
Also, significant differences in task performance between the younger and the two older age groups
were found on complex, but not on simple problems. Overall, the results from this study have
implications for instructing older adults on Internet information seeking and for the design of Web
sites.
General Terms
Theory; Experimentation; Performance; Measurement; Human Factors
Permission to make digital or hard copies of part or all of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that
copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies show this notice on the first page or initial screen
of display along with the full citation. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting
with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, to republish, to post on servers, to redistribute to lists, or to use any component of this work
in other works requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Permissions may be requested from the Publications Dept., ACM, Inc., 2
Penn Plaza, Suite 701, New York, NY 10121-0701 USA, fax +1 (212) 869-0481, or permission@acm.org.
NIH Public Access
Author Manuscript
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
Published in final edited form as:
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. 2008 ; 15(1): 3–. doi:10.1145/1352782.1352785.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Additional Key Words and Phrases: Human-computer interaction
older adults; health information seeking; Internet; mental models; Pathfinder networks; search
engines
1. INTRODUCTION
The emergence of the Internet has given people access to an information environment with
several unique and outstanding qualities. First, the Internet contains vast amounts of
information—as of August 2005, the search engine Google contained an index of more than 8
billion Web pages [Google 2005]. Second, this information is interlinked, allowing a person
to jump from page to page. Third, the information can be accessed relatively rapidly, depending
on the speed of the connection. Lastly, the Internet provides search tools, services, indexes,
and directories that help the user find information. Yet despite these virtues, for many people
the activity of information seeking continues to be problematic [Pew Internet and American
Life Project 2004].
Many of the issues concerning information seeking on the Internet are similar to those that
continue to confront traditional information retrieval (IR) research [Baeza-Yates and Ribeiro-
Neto 1999]. Historically, IR systems were viewed as technological artifacts that required search
experts to perform search and retrieval activities. With the introduction of user-centered
perspectives to library and information science, the research agenda shifted away from studying
experts and toward examining the capabilities and limitations as well as the information needs
of end-users interacting with these IR systems [Nahl 1997; 2003]. It was not until the late 1990s
that we began to see an emphasis on user-centered Internet information seeking (e.g., Wang et
al. [2000]).
The goal of the study reported in this article was to investigate the influence of various domains
of Internet-related knowledge and cognitive abilities on Internet information-seeking
performance by older adults. The focus was on health-related information as older adults are
likely to search for this type of information. In this regard, the study contributes to the growing
body of research issues concerning e-health [Bass 2003]. As noted by Powell et al. [2003], the
Internet is altering the knowledge-based balance of power between healthcare professionals
and healthcare consumers. Essentially, it is spurring the public to become more involved in
healthcare decision making by enabling access to data and information that was previously
inaccessible. The Inter-net thus has the potential to empower older adults by providing
information that could help them deal more effectively with issues that could impact their
health, independence, and well-being.
Despite an increasing older adult population—by 2030, people aged 65 and older will make
up 20% of the US population [U.S. Department of Health and Human Services 2001]—research
on this group’s ability to use the Internet to find information has been limited, especially when
the information-seeking activities relate to problem solving and decision making. Of particular
concern for older adults is that the inherent complexity of the Internet may make it difficult
for them to negotiate Web sites and utilize available resources. In addition, information seeking
is an activity that places heavy demands on cognitive abilities such as working memory, spatial
ability, and reasoning. For many older adults who have limited experience and knowledge
concerning the Inter-net and exhibit declines in cognitive abilities, effective Web-based
information seeking can be a daunting task. The contention here is that understanding the
factors which hinder and influence older adults’ Internet information-seeking activity can lead
to the design of better Web sites, search engines, and instruction that takes into account the
capabilities and limitations of older adults.
SHARIT et al. Page 2
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
1.1 Modeling Information-Seeking Behavior
Information-seeking behavior has been the focus of much research and theoretical activity.
The methodology of this study was based on an Internet information-seeking model (Figure
1) that was influenced primarily by the work of Sutcliffe and Ennis [1998] and Marchionini
[1997], but also by several other researchers including Borgman [1986;1996] and Kulthau
[2003].
The theoretical framework proposed by Sutcliffe and Ennis [1998] consists of four cyclical
cognitive activities: problem identification, need articulation, query formulation, and results
evaluation. Problem identification involves identifying the information need from a problem
statement. During need articulation, the information seeker expresses the information need by
selecting low-level terms from long-term memory, which may lead to a refinement or
restatement of the information need. Query formulation is the process of generating queries
and depends on the information seeker’s skill level and on the capabilities and possibilities of
the IR system. Finally, results evaluation is a decision-making process whereby the information
seeker decides whether to accept the retrieved results or continue searching for more results.
The framework of Sutcliffe and Ennis also attributes four types of knowledge to each
information seeker: (1) domain knowledge (knowledge about the specific domain searched);
(2) device knowledge (knowledge regarding system support facilities for browsing, querying,
and evaluating results); (3) information resources knowledge (knowledge regarding the
specific database(s) that can be searched by a system); and (4) IR knowledge (knowledge of
search strategies).
Marchionini [1997] conceptualized information seeking as guided by analytical strategies at
one end of a continuum and browsing strategies at the other end. Analytically-guided search
occurs when querying a Web search engine and reviewing the results, whereas with browsing
the search has a serendipitous quality and the information-seeking goal is attained more
indirectly. He proposed a process model of information seeking consisting of several
subprocesses that can be run in parallel: recognize and accept the problem; define the problem;
select the source; formulate the query; execute the query; examine the results; extract
information; and reflect, iterate, or stop.
Overall, the model depicted in Figure 1 is consistent with the view of information seeking as
a problem-solving process [Mayer and Whittrock 1996;Marchionini 1997], whereby the
problem-solver’s knowledge and other mental representations are manipulated in order to
achieve a goal. The problem-solving process consists of three subprocesses: representation,
planning, and execution. During representation, the problem statement is internalized through
the creation of a mental representation of the facts. In Figure 1, this is represented by the
Problem Orientation component. The planning process consists of generating a method for
coming up with a solution, which often requires breaking the problem up into parts or subgoals;
in Figure 1, aspects of Problem Orientation as well as the Facets component represent the
planning process. Execution involves carrying out the operations that were developed during
the planning process. These processes are iterative in nature (depicted by the iterative loops in
Figure 1) as planning may spur further insights into the problem and thus promote modified
problem representations. Also represented in the model are the potential impacts of domain
and technical system knowledge and the potential role of cognitive abilities during navigation
and evaluation activities related to information seeking.
Taken together, knowledge and cognitive abilities are assumed to be critical in keeping the
user oriented, that is, in preventing the user from getting confused or lost. These factors are
expected to assume an even greater role, especially for older adults, as the problem-solving
(i.e., information-seeking) process becomes more complex. Problems can generally be
described as consisting of an initial state, a goal state, and a set of allowable operations and
SHARIT et al. Page 3
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
constraints that govern transitions between states. When problems are complex or ill-defined,
one or more of these components may not be clearly specified [Chi and Glaser 1985], and the
information-seeking process can become exceedingly challenging. Overall, the information-
seeking model depicted in Figure 1 provides the basis for the emphasis in this study on
knowledge, cognitive abilities, and problem complexity.
1.2 Knowledge and Mental Models
The relationship between knowledge and information-seeking performance is far from
unequivocal. In examining the effects of knowledge about hyper-text and search experience
on search performance using a hypertext-based bibliographic search system, Dimitroff and
Wolfram [1995] found that individuals with little or no search experience did as well as those
with knowledge of hypertext-searching techniques. In contrast, Bhavnani [2001] found that
search experts working within their domain of expertise were able to use domain-specific
declarative knowledge related to URLs (Web addresses) to directly type in the URL of a useful
Web site, which led to effective searches. However, when they searched for information outside
their domain of expertise, they relied on general-purpose search knowledge and on clicking
links, which led to ineffective and, at times, aborted searches.
Another important and related construct in Internet information seeking is that of mental
models. These models are essentially mental representations that embody information about
the structure, function, relationships, and other characteristics of objects in the world, and thus
can help people explain and predict the behavior of things in the world around them [Craik
1943; Brewer 2003]. The importance of mental models for successful information-seeking
behavior was noted by Borgman [1986], who found that subjects trained with an analogical
model of an online system (training manuals described the online system in terms of a card
catalog) performed better in a complex task condition, but not in a simple task condition, and
suggested that mental models might be useful in situations where problem-solving behavior
was required (i.e., when no method of solution is obvious). A number of other researchers,
including Liebscher and Marchionini [1988], Slone [2002], and Zhang and Chignell [2001],
have used the concept of mental models as a theoretical basis for their information retrieval
and Web studies.
1.3 Cognitive Abilities
Few if any studies on Internet information seeking have examined the role of the user’s
cognitive abilities in relation to search performance. Evidence from studies on database
information search and retrieval indicates that age-related declines in many cognitive abilities
may make this factor especially important when examining the Internet information-seeking
performance of older adults. For example, in a simulated health insurance customer service
task, Czaja et al. [2001] found that factors such as working memory and spatial skills influenced
search performance. In a simulated telecommuting task in which older adults were required to
respond to queries from fictitious customer emails by navigating through a database configured
in the form of a hyperlinked information environment similar to the Internet, measures of verbal
ability, memory span, attention/concentration, and perceptual speed were found to strongly
predict performance [Sharit et al. 2004].
Another aspect of cognition that can have important implications for Inter-net information-
seeking performance is as measured by the visual recognition Group Embedded Figures Test
that assesses susceptibility to visual interference. This measure can differentiate those
individuals whose perception is dependent on the organization of the surrounding perceptual
field (i.e., field dependent (FD)) from those whose perception is independent (i.e., field
independent (FI)) of such influences [Sternberg and Grigorenko 1997]. Its relevance to Internet
information-seeking performance derives from the results of a number of studies that have
SHARIT et al. Page 4
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
incorporated the FD/FI distinction. For example, Weller et al. [1994] found that FI participants
accessed more hypermedia nodes and achieved greater learning than their FD counterparts.
They proposed that the FD/FI distinction reflects how well a learner restructures the
information presented in a hypermedia-type information environment by relying on salient
cues and the arrangement of elements in the perceptual field. Palmquist and Kim [2000] found
that in comparison to FI novice users, FD novice individuals used more embedded links
(ordinary Web page clickable links), which reflected a more passive navigational style, and
became lost more frequently. These researchers concluded that FD novice users should receive
special attention from Web designers in the form of well-structured and visually simple Web
sites.
1.4 Study Objectives
An important objective of this study was to investigate the relationship between Internet-related
knowledge and information-seeking performance and whether this relationship differs
depending on whether the search problems are simple or complex. Toward this end, a structured
interview instrument was developed to assess four different types of declarative (factual) and
procedural (how one does something) knowledge: general Internet, Web browser, simple
search, and advanced search. In addition, the Pathfinder methodology [Dorsey et al. 1999] was
used as a means for evaluating the importance of Internet-related structural knowledge (how
key concepts regarding the Internet and Web-based information search are related) for search
performance.
Another objective was to evaluate the mental models people have concerning the Internet, Web
browsers, and search engines and how these mental models impact search performance on both
simple and complex problems. For this analysis, selected items from the structured interview
instrument were examined.
In light of findings regarding the importance of cognitive abilities for older adult information-
seeking performance, a key objective of this study was to determine if cognitive abilities
predicted performance after accounting for Internet-related knowledge. Also of interest was
determining how problem complexity might influence which cognitive ability measures are
predictive of performance and if age had an influence on performance after both knowledge
and cognitive abilities were taken into account.
Overall, the goal of this research was to provide a better understanding of the factors that
influence the ability of older adults to find information on the Internet. Knowledge of this type
is fundamental to the development of guidelines for interface designs and instructional
strategies that are tailored to the specific needs of older adults.
2. EXPERIMENTAL METHODS
2.1 Sample
A sample of 40 older adults was recruited in two age groups: a younger-old group (60–70 years
of age, M = 65.2, SD = 3.17, n = 20) and an older-old group (71–85 years of age, M = 76.7,
SD = 4.08, n = 20). A comparison group of 10 younger adults (18–39 years of age, M = 27.9,
SD = 6.39) was also included in the study to establish benchmark performance comparisons.
There were 17 males and 33 females in the sample; 2 males and 8 females in the younger group,
8 males and 12 females in the younger-old group, and 7 males and 13 females in the older-old
group. With respect to ethnicity, the sample consisted of 5 Black/African-Americans, 31 White/
Caucasians, 11 Hispanic/Latino, and 3 other ethnicities. The highest education attained for the
younger group was as follows: 8 with some college, 1 with a bachelors degree, and 1 with a
masters degree; for the younger-old adults: 1 with high school, 11 with some college, 1 with
SHARIT et al. Page 5
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
a bachelors degree, 4 with a masters degree, and 3 with an advanced degree; and for the older-
old adults: 1 with high school, 6 with some college, 5 with a bachelors degree, 1 with some
graduate school, 4 with masters degree, and 3 with an advanced degree. The reason for the
relatively large “some college” classification for the younger participants was because many
of these participants were still pursuing their undergraduate degrees. Grouping education level
into the following two categories—some college and below, and bachelors degree and above
—resulted in no significant differences in education level between the 20 younger-old adults
and the 20 older-old adults, X 2(df = 1, n = 40) = 0.11, p > .05. All participants had at least
minimal Internet and Web-based search engine experience and spoke fluent English.
2.2 Setting and Materials
Performance of the Internet search tasks, the Pathfinder task, and administration of the
structured interview instrument took place in an office equipped with the experimental
computer workstation. Participants performed their information search tasks on a Dell
computer system that had a 19” flat panel display, mouse, and high-sensitivity microphone,
and accessed the Internet through the University of Miami’s high-speed broadband connection.
The system was configured with Microsoft Internet Explorer 6.0, the Hypercam 2.10.00 screen
capture utility [Hyperionics Technology 2006], Weblogger software [Reeder et al. 2000],
Windows Media Player, and PCKNOT software for Pathfinder network analysis [Interlink
1998].
The Hypercam screen capture utility enabled each participant’s onscreen task to be recorded
in the form of a Windows-based digital movie. This movie was used primarily for review
purposes. Verbal protocol data was also captured using this software. The Weblogger
application, which creates a visual map in Microsoft Visio of the participants’ solution path
through the Internet, was used to collect time and URL information related to the participants’
information-seeking moves.
2.3 Procedure
The study was conducted over two days. On the first day, a battery of tests consisting of 21
standardized measures of component cognitive abilities was administered to each participant
in a group testing format. These measures are widely used in the literature and have
demonstrated reliability and validity. The measures were chosen to assess a broad range of
abilities including verbal fluency, psychomotor speed, perceptual speed, attention/
concentration, long-term memory, memory span, working memory, spatial-visualization
ability, reasoning, and lifelong knowledge. There were at least two markers for each of the
abilities. Details concerning this cognitive battery can be found in Czaja et al. [2006]. A list of
the component cognitive abilities and their corresponding markers is presented in Table I. For
this study, we also included the Group Embedded Figures Test as the distinction between FI
and FD styles has been shown to be related to information-seeking performance.
Additional instruments also completed on the first day included a demographic and health
questionnaire, a technology and computer experience questionnaire, and a Web experience
questionnaire. On the second day, participants completed the individualized testing protocol
that included tests of vision (Snellen Near and Far), psychomotor speed, attention, and the
Group Embedded Figures Test. Following a lunch break, they were given a brief introduction
to the experiment and a short introduction to the verbal protocol methodology and
accompanying exercise. The exercise consisted of having the participant imagine his or her
home and then count aloud all of the windows they encountered as they mentally traversed the
space.
SHARIT et al. Page 6
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The participants then proceeded to find information that would answer six information
problems (Table II) that were presented in a fixed sequence. The simplest problems, 1(a) and
1(b), were presented first. A randomized order was used for selecting the sequence for the
remaining four problems. The same order was used for all participants. Each question was
printed on a separate laminated card. To perform their searches, participants were free to use
any search engine and any approach to solving the problem as long as they found it somewhere
on the Internet (e.g., they could proceed directly to a Web site or Web page by using its URL).
The experimenter then launched the Web browser, cleared the history feature, and pointed the
browser to Google’s homepage. The Google search engine was selected because it is a general-
purpose search engine with a simple interface and powerful search features. Although its use
was not required, in almost all cases, the participants chose to conduct their searches using
Google.
Participants were handed each of the problems, one at a time, on laminated cards. After reading
the card, the subject placed it on a stand next to the computer. Participants had up to 15 minutes
to solve each problem. However, in a few cases where it was judged that the participant was
close to a solution, the time limit was exceeded by a few minutes. All of the problems dealt
with medical, health, and wellness-related issues. Problems classified as complex were
generally ill-structured, with the exception of problem 3 (concerning flu shots).
The experimenter monitored the participant’s search process. By examining the final screen
outputs and guided by a scoring sheet, the experimenter determined the correctness of the
answer offered as a solution to each of the problems. As discussed in the following section,
problems were scored as incorrect, partially correct, or correct.
After completing the search tasks participants were given a rest break, and then a brief
introduction to the Pathfinder task. This task required judging the relatedness of a pair of
concepts displayed on the Pathfinder PCKNOT software computer window, which were drawn
from a pool of Internet and search-related concepts. For this study, a total of 14 concepts were
used (Table III). Participants provided pairwise ratings by entering a number from one
(Unrelated) to nine (Related). Most of the participants completed the 90 pairwise ratings in
under 15 minutes. These data were then analyzed by software (PCKNOT) that can generate a
graphical network representation of the participant’s knowledge structure for these concepts
and that also computes a similarity rating that compares each participant’s Pathfinder network
to that of a referent (e.g., expert) network.
Finally, each participant took part in a structured interview that consisted of questions that
assessed declarative and procedural knowledge related to Internet information seeking. This
instrument contained 74 questions divided into four parts: a general Internet section, followed
by a section on Web browsers, and ending with a two-part section related to simple and
advanced Internet search knowledge. In addition to fundamental factual knowledge, the
questions also required demonstration of practical hands-on knowledge related to Internet
information seeking. Contained within this 74-item instrument1 were 10 questions that
specifically probed the participant’s mental models of the Internet, Web browser, and search
engine (Table IV). All questions were read aloud to the participant who was required to
verbalize a response. The participant’s response was immediately rated by the researcher. The
scoring of the structured interview questions varied according to the nature and complexity of
the question. Some questions were simply marked correct or incorrect. Other more complex
questions were scored correct, partially correct, or incorrect, and, for some questions, the score
reflected the number of correct items identified by the participant (e.g., all that apply). The
maximum possible score was 300. The structured interviews were audio-taped.
1The entire instrument is available upon request.
SHARIT et al. Page 7
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
All participants were compensated $75.00 for their participation in the study. The study
protocol was approved by the University’s Institutional Review Board.
2.4 Measures
The primary measures of this study were: (1) task performance scores for simple, complex,
and all problems; (2) knowledge subcategory domain scores and a total knowledge domain
score; (3) Pathfinder similarity scores; (4) cognitive ability test scores, including the Group
Embedded Figures Test; and (5) measures of education and Web experience.
For each participant, a task performance score (TPSij) was computed corresponding to the
performance of person i on problem j. This performance score was a function of problem
correctness, problem completion time, and problem complexity. It proved to be more sensitive
than either time or accuracy alone and had good (normal) distributional characteristics.
TPSij was computed as follows. First, for any given problem j, the participants were divided
into three groups according to how correctly they answered the problem: those that provided
incorrect answers or no answer to the problem, those that provided partially correct answers
to the problem, and those that answered the problem correctly. Let cij represent the category
of correctness for participant i on problem j. If the problem solution was incorrect, cij = 0; if
the problem solution was partially correct, cij = 1; and if the problem solution was correct,
cij = 2. All problems had to be answered from information found on the Internet (i.e., they
could not be answered based on the participant’s prior knowledge of the problem content).
Next, “time to completion” of the problem was considered. Let tcij represent the time to
completion value assigned to participant i on problem j. These values were only assigned to
participants whose problem solutions were either correct or partially correct (for those
participants whose solutions to problem j were incorrect, TPS was assigned a value of zero for
that problem). For those participants whose solutions to problem j were partially correct, tcij
= tmax j/tij, where tmax was the time taken by the slowest participant whose solution to problem
j was partially correct, and tij represented the time each participant within this group took to
complete that problem. Thus, for those participants whose completion of problem j was
partially correct, the participant who had the longest completion time, that is, when tij =
tmax j, had a tcij value of one. Faster completion times (i.e., tij < tmax j) indicated better
performance and led to tcij values greater than one.
For those participants who completed problem j correctly, their tcij values were computed as
follows. First, the highest tcij value from the next best group, the partially correct group, was
determined. This value was then added to the tmax j/tij value computed for each participant
who had completed problem j correctly. Thus, any participant who answered problem j
correctly would be assigned a higher tcij value than any participant in the partially correct
group.
Finally, problem difficulty weights, d j, were assigned to each problem j. Based on the
consensus of three of the study’s investigators, problem difficulty was rated on a four-point
scale with the easiest and hardest problems assigned weights of one and four, respectively.
Problem weights were derived according to the number of answers the problem required and
whether any of its parts, such as initial conditions or expected outcomes, were not well-defined.
The weights were assigned as follows: 1 for problems 1(a) and 2; 2 for problem 1(b); 4 for
problem 3; and 3 for problems 4 and 5.
For any given problem j, the task performance score of participant i was computed as TPSij =
cij× tcij× d j. As indicated before, if participant i completed problem j incorrectly, then TPSij
= 0 because cij = 0.
SHARIT et al. Page 8
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
A participant i’s overall task performance score was derived by summing TPSij across all j
problems. For each participant, task performance scores were also derived for two categories
of problems: simple and complex. The task performance score for simple problems was
computed by summing TPSij across problems 1(a), 1(b), and 2; the task performance score for
complex problems was computed by summing TPSij across problems 3, 4, and 5. Simple
problems generally dealt with common conditions, required few answers, and were relatively
well-defined, whereas the complex problems generally dealt with uncommon conditions,
required many answers, and were more ill-defined.
We included the measure of overall task performance in the analyses as it reflects a complexity-
free measure. That is, it provides a measure of task performance that is not based on subjective
judgment with respect to the classification of problems into simple and complex categories.
Also, by aggregating scores from more problems, the overall task performance measure is
expected to provide more reliable statistical results.
3. RESULTS
3.1 Task Performance, Internet-Related Domain Knowledge, and Age
As noted, measures of simple task performance, complex task performance, and overall task
performance measures (performance on all problems) were computed. In addition, four
measures of Internet-related domain knowledge as well as the total knowledge score (their
sum) was computed for each participant. The means and standard deviations for each of the
knowledge measures are presented in Table V. Means and standard deviations for each of the
three task performance measures are presented in Table VI (note that complex problem
performance scores were higher than those for simple problems because they were given more
weight). As previously described, we also categorized the sample of older participants into
younger-old and older-old adults.
Table VII presents the correlations between the three task performance measures and each of
the knowledge scores. These data indicate different patterns in relationships between the
knowledge and the task performance measures across the younger-old and older-old age
groups. Specifically, significant correlations between the various knowledge scores and the
task performance measures, especially for performance on all problems, were much more
evident for the younger-old participants. Also evident from this analysis was the importance
of Internet knowledge for the simple problems and advanced search knowledge for the complex
problems for both the younger-old and the older-old participants.
To further examine the relationship between knowledge and performance, total knowledge
scores were recoded into four quartile groups: 1st quartile (worst knowledge scores), n = 10;
2nd quartile, n = 10; 3rd quartile, n = 10; and 4th quartile (best knowledge scores), n = 10. Using
overall task performance as the dependent measure, the analysis of variance (ANOVA)
procedure was then used to test for differences in performance as a function of knowledge
quartile. A significant effect of quartiles was found, F[3,36] = 4.90, p < .01 (Figure 2). Post-
hoc tests using Tukey’s HSD revealed significant differences between the 1st and the 3rd
quartiles (p < .05) and between the 1st and the 4th quartiles (p < .01). Similar results were found
for the simple and complex problems.
Interestingly, although Figure 2 implies a monotonically increasing relationship between
overall performance and total knowledge, the top five performers had a mean performance
score of M = 293.36 (SD = 39.25) and a mean knowledge score of M = 141.6 (SD = 45.87),
while the individuals who had the top five total knowledge scores had a much higher mean
knowledge score (M = 176.4, SD = 9.10) but a much lower mean performance score (M =
219.89, SD = 72.24).
SHARIT et al. Page 9
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Differences in knowledge between the younger-old and the older-old participants (Table V)
were examined using t-tests. No significant differences were found for total knowledge or any
of the four knowledge domains. However, when the performance of the 40 older adults was
compared to that of the 10 younger adults (the comparison sample), a trend toward significance
was found for total knowledge, t(48) = 2.00, p = .051, and a significant effect was found for
the Web browser knowledge domain, t(48) = 2.80, p = .007, with the younger adults scoring
higher in both cases.
Finally, differences in performance between the younger-old and older-old participants (Table
VI) were examined on each of the three task performance measures: all problems, simple
problems, and complex problems (these two groups did not significantly differ in previous
Web experience). The results indicated that there were no significant differences on any of the
three task performance measures. It should be noted, however, that when the performance of
the 40 older adults was compared to that of the 10 younger adults, significant results were
found for all problems, t(48) = 2.15, p= .037,as well as for complex problems, t(48) =
2.48,p= .023, but not for simple problems, t(48) = 0.94. In both cases, the younger adults
achieved higher scores than the older adults (Table VI).
3.2 Task Performance and Structural Knowledge (Pathfinder)
Pathfinder, a psychological scaling technique, was used in this study to examine the importance
of Internet-related structural knowledge for task performance. Pathfinder’s measure of
structural knowledge, the similarity score, requires that each individual’s network of
relationships be compared to a referent network. In this study, the referent network consisted
of the top five performers, based on the overall task performance measure, in the sample (Figure
3(a)). An example of a single participant’s Pathfinder network is shown in Figure 3(b) where
the many links between concepts and the somewhat symmetrical nature of this network suggest
poor structural knowledge.
The correlations between the Pathfinder similarity scores and each of the domain knowledge
scores derived from the structured interview instrument were as follows: total knowledge, r
(40) = .504, p < .01; Internet, r(40) = .41, p < .01; Web browser, r(40) = .439, p < .01; simple
search, r(40) = .516, p < .01; and advanced search, r(40) = .316, p < .05. These data indicate
relatively strong relationships between structural knowledge and the other knowledge
measures. However, there were no significant correlations between the Pathfinder similarity
scores and any of the three task performance measures.
3.3 Predictors of Information-Seeking Performance
A hierarchical regression analysis was performed to examine the extent to which cognitive
abilities predicted task performance after accounting for the effects of knowledge and to
determine the influence of age on performance after accounting for differences in knowledge
and cognitive abilities. As noted earlier, a battery of tests consisting of 21 standardized
measures of a broad range of component cognitive abilities was administered to each
participant, with at least two markers for each of the abilities [Czaja et al. 2006]. From each
of these component cognitive abilities, one marker was selected, based on the test measure that
most highly correlated with the measure of overall task performance, to explore the role of
cognitive abilities on Internet information seeking. In addition, we included the Group
Embedded Figures Test score.
For these analyses, the total knowledge score was entered in the first step. We chose to use
total knowledge as it served as the best measure of overall Internet-related knowledge. All ten
cognitive ability measures as well as the Group Embedded Figures Test score were entered in
the second step using the stepwise procedure. Age was then entered in the last step.
SHARIT et al. Page 10
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Table VIII summarizes the results of each step of the procedure for each of the three task
performance measures. Note that the second step actually consists of several substeps where
each substep is the addition of a cognitive variable by the stepwise procedure. Given that we
forced age into step 3 to examine the impact of age over and beyond the model in step 2 and
that age was not a significant predictor of any of the performance measures after accounting
for knowledge and cognitive abilities, the final models are those specified in step 2.
From Table VIII, the changes in R2 obtained by adding a variable in step 2 can be derived by
subtracting the R2 value associated with the model without that variable (the preceding R2 value
in the column) from the R2 value associated with the model containing that variable. The beta
values that are provided are the standardized beta coefficients. The squares of these coefficients
can be used to obtain estimates of the relative contributions of the predictor variables to the
overall variance of the performance measure.
Not surprisingly, the highest R2 (.537) was found for performance on all problems as this
measure reflected the largest number of problems performed by the participant. In addition to
knowledge, three cognitive ability measures were found to be predictors of overall
performance: reasoning, working memory, and perceptual speed, with each measure
accounting for a reasonably large proportion of the variance in performance.
For performance on the simple problems, following the inclusion of total knowledge the only
cognitive ability measure found to be significant was reasoning, resulting in a final model with
R2 = .388. Interestingly, knowledge was no longer significant when reasoning was added to
the model.
Finally, for the performance of the complex problems, following the inclusion of total
knowledge, the only cognitive ability measure found to be significant was working memory,
resulting in a final model with R2 = .312. Also, in contrast to the final (step 2) model for
performance on the simple problems, the knowledge variable remained significant even after
the inclusion of working memory.
For the sample of 40 older adults, a weak correlation was found between age and overall task
performance, r(40)= .223, p = .083 and between age and complex task performance, r(40) =
.219, p = .087. Still, even for this restricted age range, there was a clear indication of a
relationship between age and performance and specifically for performance on the complex
problems. However, the results of the regression analysis clearly indicate that after accounting
for knowledge and cognitive ability factors, the influence of age is negligible.
3.4 Task Performance and Mental Models
To understand the relationship between the user’s mental model and performance, ten questions
were extracted from the structured interview instrument (Table IV) and grouped into three
categories according to the type of mental model they addressed, either Internet, Web browser,
or search engine (the latter comprised questions from both the simple search and advanced
search sections of the structured interview instrument). In each of these cases, having a better
mental model was operationally defined by composite scores of 4 or greater on the questions
corresponding to that mental model. Twenty-two of the 40 older participants had a better mental
model of the Internet; 14 had a better mental model of Web browsers; and 14 had a better
mental model of search engines.
To determine if goodness of mental model was reflected in performance, t-tests were
conducted. Specifically, for each of the three measures of task performance, the impact on
performance of each of the mental model categories was assessed where the two groups were
defined based on whether the participant was in the better or worse group for that mental model
SHARIT et al. Page 11
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
category. The results of these tests, which are summarized in Table IX, indicated that the mental
model of the Internet had the most impact on overall and simple task performance, while the
search engine mental model had the most impact on performance of the complex problems.
4. DISCUSSION
Older adults have the potential to benefit tremendously from the vast stores of information on
the Internet. With rising healthcare costs and an overloaded healthcare system, access to health-
related information can empower older adults by enabling them to become better informed
about their own health and healthcare choices. This, in turn, could impact their independence
and well-being and thus have a substantial impact on their lives and on society as a whole.
However, despite these potential benefits one should not lose sight of the fact that information
seeking is an activity that places demands on many cognitive abilities, and the dynamics and
processes of finding information are often obscure. For many older adults who possess limited
knowledge concerning the Internet and search engine processes and who may also exhibit
declines in cognitive abilities, Web-based information seeking can be an intimidating and
frustrating experience.
The goals in this study were to examine the influence of various types of Internet-domain
knowledge, cognitive abilities, and problem complexity on the Internet health information-
seeking capabilities of older adults. An important overall finding was that knowledge of various
aspects of the Internet, though necessary, was not sufficient in explaining information-seeking
performance. As a general illustration of this point, consider those older participants with the
five best total knowledge scores. Compared to these participants, the older participants with
the five best overall performance scores had, on average, a lower total knowledge score (141.6
vs. 176.4). However, these five best performers had, on average, a much higher overall
performance score (293.36 vs. 219.89). Thus, although having good knowledge was found to
be strongly related to effective information seeking (Figure 2), other factors besides knowledge
had an impact on information seeking.
In particular, a number of cognitive abilities were critical for task performance. Importantly,
the data also indicated that the relationship between abilities and performance depended on
problem complexity. For overall task performance, after accounting for individual differences
in knowledge, significant predictors of performance were reasoning, working memory, and
perceptual speed. For the complex problems, knowledge and working memory were significant
predictors of performance. However, for simple problems, only reasoning ability was found
to be a significant predictor of performance. Thus as problems become more complex,
information-seeking performance appears to become more dependent on a combination of
knowledge and abilities.
The importance of working memory in Web navigation has been noted in a number of studies
[Kubeck et al. 1999; Laberge and Scialfa 2005] where both recall of where one is, planning of
where one wants to go, and comprehension of information on Web pages need to be carried
out more or less concurrently. The coordination of the processing and storage of information
is a critical aspect of working memory activity [Salthouse and Babcock 1991] and, as implied
in Figure 1, can play an important role in keeping the user oriented during more complex
information-seeking activities. With simpler problems, the emphasis may be on identifying
relevant facets and ultimately search box terms that are more likely to map into immediately
recognizable solutions. Therefore, for simpler problems that do not generate complex
navigational behaviors, reasoning ability would likely be more important than working
memory. However, for more complex problems, in addition to working memory, knowledge
pertaining to search would also play an instrumental role with respect to the translation of facets
into search box terms as implied in Figure 1.
SHARIT et al. Page 12
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The finding that perceptual speed was predictive of the overall task performance measure, even
after accounting for knowledge, reasoning, and working memory, is also not surprising. First,
speed was one of the components of the task performance measure. Second, according to the
model depicted in Figure 1, a deficit in perceptual speed which is related to visual scanning
ability, could adversely affect information seekers during the Results stage by decreasing the
number of results and result pages that they would be able to process.
It should also be noted that intuitively the identification of relevant facets or search terms would
depend as well on verbal fluency and lifelong knowledge abilities. While these two latter
abilities did not emerge as significant predictors of performance in the regression models, they
were highly correlated with the performance measures as well as with reasoning ability. Thus
their shared variance with reasoning ability would likely require much higher sample sizes for
their effects to become manifest.
This study also incorporated the Group Embedded Figures Test to differentiate field dependent
(FD) from field independent (FI) individuals. The results from this study were not consistent
with findings that a field-independent cognitive style is associated with better search
performance on Internet-based information-seeking tasks [Palmquist and Kim 2000]. The lack
of sensitivity of this measure to performance in this study could be due to a number of factors,
including reduced variability in this measure for the restricted age range comprising this sample
of participants, or perhaps to the reduced influence of visual complexity when using powerful
search engines for problem solving.
What the analyses in this study do suggest is that certain cognitive abilities, in particular
reasoning, working memory, and perceptual speed, and to some extent verbal fluency, have a
large impact on the extent to which older adults can negotiate the demands inherent in Internet
information seeking. The importance of reasoning ability also suggests that with the powerful
search engines currently available (such as Google), the crucial information-seeking skills may
be shifting from navigation activities to translation of the problem into appropriate facets and
discriminating useful from unhelpful results (Figure 1).
With respect to knowledge, the data suggest that its role in information seeking varies with
problem complexity. Specifically, the findings indicate that for complex problems, the most
critical Internet domain knowledge is search knowledge, and in particular, advanced search
knowledge (Table VII). The mental model data are also consistent with this finding. As
indicated in Table IX, good mental models of the Internet appear to support performance on
simple problems, while there is a trend toward good search engine mental models supporting
complex task performance. The latter result is consistent with findings from Borgman
[1996], which indicated that participants trained on an analogical model of an online search
system showed better performance on complex problems compared to those who received
procedural training. The ability for older adults to successfully negotiate more complex Web-
based searches is crucial in light of the sheer quantity of health-related information on the
Internet and the complex nature of health issues. Importantly, these results imply the need to
incorporate knowledge related to search engines into instructional programs targeting older
adults.
Another consideration in the relationship between knowledge and Internet information-seeking
behavior is the role of structural knowledge. The findings of significant and positive
correlations between Pathfinder similarity scores, the total knowledge score, and the individual
knowledge domain scores imply that the more knowledge a participant had, the more organized
that knowledge was, and are in line with other findings from other task domains (e.g., Conner
et al. [2004]). However, despite the rather impressive visual demonstrations of differences in
knowledge organization provided by the Pathfinder network graphs (e.g., Figures 3(a) and 3
SHARIT et al. Page 13
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
(b)), there were no significant correlations between the Pathfinder similarity scores and task
performance by the older participants. Studies have found mixed relationships between
Pathfinder similarity scores and performance (e.g., Wyman and Randel [1998]; Dorsey et al.
[1999]). The absence in this study of stronger relationships to task performance may have been
due to the specific set of concepts used in the pairwise comparisons (Table III), which may not
have been as relevant to information-seeking performance as were the declarative and
procedural knowledge elements of the structured interview instrument.
Overall, it appears that while knowledge related to various aspects of the Internet is critical,
what may be most crucial to performance is attaining some threshold of this knowledge; beyond
this point the benefits in performance may derive from relatively undiminished functioning on
key cognitive abilities. Further studies are necessary for clarifying the relative roles that
knowledge and cognitive abilities play in the Internet information-seeking performance of
older adults. In any case, it is important to note that there is no implication from this study that
knowledge can compensate for widespread deficits in cognitive abilities or, conversely, that
having good cognitive abilities can compensate for very poor knowledge.
On the practical side, the results suggest that instruction directed at the four domains of
knowledge investigated in this study (with emphasis on search-related knowledge for more
complex problems) could significantly enhance the older adult’s ability to perform information
searches on the Internet. In addition, training aimed at improving cognitive abilities such as
reasoning, working memory, and perceptual speed, while more challenging, may also result in
performance improvements. Evidence exists demonstrating the effectiveness of training
interventions of this kind. Nyberg [2005], in a review of cognitive training studies, concludes
that there is “robust evidence for the potential for plasticity in older age” (p. 311). Working
memory declines in older adults can also be compensated for by good Web designs, for
example, designs that utilize dynamic navigational side-trees or breadcrumbs [Maldonado and
Resnick 2002].
A limitation of this study that should be noted was that measures of prior knowledge that
participants may have had about the problem domains was not measured. This aspect of domain
knowledge could encompass several areas, including general health literacy skills, knowledge
of specific Web sites and their related Web addresses (URLs), and specific prior knowledge
related to the content of each problem (e.g., flu shots). However, we intentionally designed the
questions so that it would be difficult to identify a particular relevant URL. In addition, the
participants were instructed that they could proceed directly to a Web site or Web page by
using its URL rather than a search engine. As it turned out, the participants usually initiated
their searches using the search engine. Also, having any specialist knowledge, for example, of
wheelchairs or flu shots, would not have benefited their performance scores as these scores
were based on their ability to find the correct information from the Internet. Thus, while the
lack of measuring problem domain knowledge was a limitation of this study, we did not feel
that the lack of inclusion of this type of measure detracts from the study’s results and the
important implications of the findings.
In conclusion, our overall goal is to ensure that any adult, regardless of his or her age, computer,
or information-seeking knowledge and expertise, is successful at finding useful information
on the Internet in reasonable time no matter how difficult the nature of the information problem.
However, Marchionini [2004] has voiced concern over precisely this kind of exalted goal and
cautions information science against harboring unreasonable expectations. He states: (1) “Our
expectations that people can find and understand information without thinking and investing
effort are unreasonable”; and (2) “Our hopes that we can create systems (solutions) that ‘do’
IR for us are unreasonable” (slide 32). Instead, he argues for IR systems that evolve using the
interactions of people with computers. Both entities would continuously learn and change by
SHARIT et al. Page 14
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
virtue of their symbiotic relationship, much as today’s Google search engine evolves month-
to-month by adding the intellectual investment of hundreds of employees who are dedicated
to finetuning the system [Marchionini 2004].
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health Grant P01
AG17211-0252.
We thank Sankaran Nair, Rod Wellens, and Timothy Goldsmith for their help.
References
Baeza-Yates, R.; Ribeiro-Neto, B. Modern Information Retrieval. ACM Press/Addison Wesley; New
York, NY: 1999.
Bass SB. How will Internet use affect the patient? A review of computer network and closed Internet-
based system studies and the implications in understanding how the use of the Internet affects patient
populations. J Health Psych 2003;8:25–38.
Bhavnani SK. Important cognitive components of domain-specific search knowledge. Proceedings of
TREC’01 2001:571–578.
Borgman CL. The user’s mental model of an information retrieval system: An experiment on a prototype
online catalog. Int J Man-Mach Stud 1986;24(1):47–64.
Borgman CL. Why are online catalogs still hard to use. J Amer Soc Inform Sci 1996;47(7):493–503.
Brown, JL.; Fischo, VV.; Hanna, G. The Nelson-Denney Reading Test. Riverside Publishing Co.;
Chicago, IL: 1993.
Brewer, WF. Mental models. In: Nadel, L., editor. Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Nature Publishing
Group / Macmillan Publishers Ltd.; London, UK: 2003.
Chi, MTH.; Glaser, R. Problem solving ability. In: Sternberg, RJ., editor. Human Abilities: An
Information Processing Approach. Freeman; New York, NY: 1985.
Craik, KJW. The Nature of Explanation. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 1943.
Czaja SJ, Charness N, Fisk AD, Hertzog C, Nair SN, Rogers WA, Sharit J. Factors predicting the use of
technology: Findings from the Center for Research and Education on Aging and Technology
Enhancement (CREATE). Psych Aging 2006;21:333–352.
Czaja SJ, Sharit J, Ownby R, Roth DL, Nair SN. Examining age differences in performance of a complex
information search and retrieval task. Psych Aging 2001;16:564–579.
Delis, DC.; Kramer, JH.; Kaplan, E.; Ober, BA. California Verbal Learning Test: Adult version. The
Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1987.
Dimitroff A, Wolfram D. Searcher response in a hypertext-based bibliographic information retrieval
system. J Amer Soc Inform Sci 1995;46(1):22–29.
Dorsey DW, Campbell GE, Foster LL, Miles DE. Assessing knowledge structures: Relations with
experience and posttraining performance. Human Perform 1999;12(1):31–57.
Ekstrom, RB.; French, JW.; Harman, HH.; Dermen, D. Manual for Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive
Tests. Educational Testing Services; Princeton, NJ: 1976.
Golden, CJ. Stroop Color and Word Test. A Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. Stoelting
Company; Wood Dale, IL: 1978.
Google 2005. www.google.com.
Heppner, PP. The Problem Solving Inventory (PSI) Manual. Consulting Psychologists Press; Palo Alto,
CA: 1988.
Hyperionics Technology 2006. HyperCam. Murrysville, PA. info@hyperionics.com.
Institute for Personality and Ability Testing, Inc. Manual for the Comprehensive Ability Battery.
Champaign, IL: 1982.
Interlink. 1998. PCKNOT Version 4.3 for Pathfinder Network Analysis. Gilbert, AZ.
http://interlinkinc.net.
SHARIT et al. Page 15
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Jackson, DN. MAB-II. Multidimensional Aptitude Battery. Sigma Assessment Systems; Port Huron, MI:
1998.
Johnson, K.; Magusin, E. Exploring the Digital Library: A Guide for Online Teaching and Learning.
Jossey-Bass/Wiley; San Francisco, CA: 2005.
Kubeck JE, Miller-Albrecht SA, Murphy MM. Finding information on the World Wide Web: Exploring
older adults’ exploration. Educ Geront 1999;25:167–183.
Kulthau, G. Seeking Meaning: A Process Approach to Library and Information Services. Vol. 2. Libraries
Unlimited; Westport, CT: 2003.
Laberge J, Scialfa CT. Predictors of Web navigation performance in a life span sample of adults. Human
Factors 2005;47(2):289–302. [PubMed: 16170939]
Liebscher P, Marchionini G. Browse and analytical search strategies in a full-text CD-ROM encyclopedia.
School Library Media Quart 1988 Summer;:223–233.
Marchionini, G. Information Seeking in Electronic Environments. Cambridge University Press;
Cambridge, UK: 1997.
Marchionini, G. Human-computer information retrieval. 2004.
http://www.ils.unc.edu/~march/HCIRMIT.pdf
Mayer, RE.; Whittrock, M. Problem-solving transfer. In: Berliner, DC.; Calfee, RC., editors. Handbook
of Educational Psychology. Simon & Schuster; New York, NY: 1996.
Maldonado CA, Resnick ML. Do common user interface design patters improve navigation? Proceedings
of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 46th Annual Meeting 2002:1315–1319.
Nahl, D. The user-centered revolution: 1970–1995. In: Kent, A.; Williams, J., editors. Encycolpedia of
Microcomputers. Vol. 19. Marcel Dekker; New York, NY: 1997.
Nahl, D. The user-centered revolution. In: Drake, M., editor. Encyclopedia of Library and Information
Science. Vol. 2. Marcel Dekker Inc.; New York, NY: 2003.
Nyberg, L. Cognitive training in healthy aging: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. In: Cabeza, R.;
Nyberg, L.; Park, D., editors. Cognitive Neuroscience of Aging: Linking Cognitive and Cerebral
Aging. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 2005.
Palmquist RA, Kim KS. Cognitive style and online database earch experience as predictors of Web search
performance. J Amer Soc Inform Sci 2000;51(6):558–566.
Pew Internet and American Life Project 2004. Older Americans and the Internet. Washington, D.C.
http://www.pewinternet.org.
Powell JA, Darvell M, Gray JAM. The doctor, the patient and the WorldWide Web: How the Internet is
changing healthcare. J Royal Soc Medicine 2003;96:74–76.
Reeder, R.; Pirolli, P.; Card, SK. Weblogger: A data collection tool for Web-use studies. 2000. UIR Tech.
Rep. UIR-R-2000-06, Xeorx PARC
Salthouse TA, Babcock RL. Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. Develop Psych
1991;47:433–440.
Sharit J, Czaja SJ, Hernández MA, Yang Y, Perdomo D, Lewis JE, Lee CC, Nair S. An evaluation of
performance by older persons on a simulated telecommuting task. J Geront: Psycho Sci 2004;59B
(6):305–316.
Slone DJ. The influence of mental models and goals on search patterns during Web interaction. J Amer
Soc Inform Sci Tech 2002;53(13):1152–1169.
Sternberg RJ, Grigorenko EL. Are cognitive styles still in style? Amer Psych 1997;52(7):700–712.
Sutcliffe A, Ennis M. Towards a cognitive theory of IR. Interact Comput 1998;10:321–351.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. A Profile of Older Americans: 2001. Administration
on Aging; Washington, D.C: 2001.
Wang P, Hawk WB, Tenopir C. Users’ interaction with World Wide Web Resources: An exploratory
study using a holistic approach. Inform Process Manag 2000;36:229–251.
Wechsler, D. Manual for Wechsler Memory Scaled Revised. The Psychological Corp.; New York, NY:
1981.
Weller HG, Repman J, Rooze GE. The relationship of learning, behavior, and cognitive styles in
hypermedia-based instruction: Implications for design of HBI. Comput Schools 1994;10:401–420.
SHARIT et al. Page 16
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Wilkie FK, Eisdorfer C, Morgan R, Lowenstein DA, Szapocznik J. Cognition in early HIV infection.
Archives Neurol 1990;47:433–440.
Wyman BG, Randel JM. The relation of knowledge organization to performance of a complex cognitive
task. J Appl Cognit Psych 1998;12:251–264.
Zhang X, Chignell M. Assessment of the effects of user characteristics on mental models of information
retrieval systems. J Amer Soc Inform Sci Tech 2001;52(6):445–459.
SHARIT et al. Page 17
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Fig. 1.
A model of search engine information-seeking behavior.
SHARIT et al. Page 18
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Fig. 2.
Mean performance on all problems across total knowledge expressed in terms of quartile
groups. Error bars indicate +/ one standard deviation.
SHARIT et al. Page 19
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Fig. 3.
(a) Pathfinder consensus referent structure corresponding to the top five performers. (b) A
single participant’s Pathfinder network. The multiple links between concepts suggest poor
structural knowledge.
SHARIT et al. Page 20
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
SHARIT et al. Page 21
Table I
Component Cognitive Abilities and their Test Measures
Cognitive Ability Test Measure Reference
Psychomotor Speed RT Both Mean Choice Reaction Time [Wilkie et al. 1990]
Perceptual Speed Digit Symbol Digit Symbol Substitution Test [Wechsler 1981]
Attention/ConcentrationStroop Test – Color Word Stroop Color Word Association Test [Golden 1978]
Long-Term Memory Meaningful Memory Meaningful Memory [Institute for Personality and Ability Testing, Inc. 1982]
Memory Span CVLT-Immediate California Verbal Learning Test – Immediate [Delis et al. 1987]
Verbal Fluency Reading Comprehension ScoreNelson-Denney Reading Comprehension [Brown et al. 1993]
Spatial-Visualization Paper Folding Correct Paper Folding [Ekstrom et al. 1976]
Working Memory Computation Span – Simple Computation Span [Salthouse and Babcock 1991]
Reasoning Inference Test Correct Inference Test [Ekstrom et al. 1976]
Life Knowledge MAB Information Multidimensional Aptitude Battery [Jackson 1998]
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
SHARIT et al. Page 22
Table II
Search Task Information Problems
Search Task IDSearch Task Text
1(a) The US Government has a department that deals with aging and issues which concern older adults. Find a Web site for one of these
departments, the Administration on Aging.
1(b) In the Administration on Aging Web site, find a Web page containing information on ways to remodel a home or apartment that make
it more senior-friendly or more comfortable for older adults.
2 Suppose you have a friend and suspect he or she is overweight.
You remember something called the BMI that might help determine whether your friend is overweight or not. You know 2 facts about
your friend:
a. his or her height is 5 feet 2 inches (or 62”)
b. his or her weight is 175 pounds
Use the Internet to find out whether your friend is overweight or not. Also, what does BMI mean?
3Flu season is coming around and you’re interested in getting a flu shot. However, you want to be sure you don’t belong to the group of
people who should not receive this shot. Find information on at least 3 types of people who should not get a flu shot.
4 You’ve decided that you want to get back in shape. Using the Internet, find information on 5 things you can do to get back into shape.
Remember that these recommendations must be appropriate for your age.
5A friend of yours uses a wheelchair. He wants to get a new one and has asked you to help him find information on the Internet regarding
new models and prices. Wheelchairs are also known as mobility solutions and you can recommend new designs that don’t look like
traditional wheelchairs but are still recommended for older adults. Find 3 mobility solutions and corresponding prices for your friend.
They cannot all be wheelchairs.
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
SHARIT et al. Page 23
Table III
The 14 Internet and Search-Related Concepts Used for the Pathfinder Task
NumberInternet or Search-Related Concept
1 Internet Browser - Software
2 Hits or Page of Results
3 Site Map
4 Stop Button
5 Back Button
6 Web Directory or Directory of Web Sites
7 Web Address or URL
8 Web Site
9 Web Page
10 Internet
11 Clickable Link
12 Home Page
13 Search Engine
14 Keywords or terms
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
SHARIT et al. Page 24
Table IV
The 10 Mental Model Questions from the 74-item Structured Interview Instrument
Question IDStructured Interview Question Text
1Ok, here’s a computer. If I asked you what a computer is, you could say something like “It’s a device that’s made up of a screen, a keyboard,
the computer, a printer, and it runs software.” Please describe the Internet in a similar way. What is the Internet? (Internet domain)
2 The Internet has millions of Web sites and more are being added every day. Where are Web sites stored and what features of the Internet
permit you to see these Web sites? (Internet domain)
3Suppose you’re sitting in front of a computer that’s connected to the Internet. You type an Internet address, that is, a Web site address, and
hit the enter key. Describe as best as you can how a Web page that’s stored on a computer somewhere on the Internet actually makes it into
your computer and onto your screen. I don’t want technical details; just give me the main events. (Internet domain)
4 Take a look at this map of an Internet network in the United States. Suppose you are in Chicago, Illinois, using a computer and you are
viewing a Web site that’s stored on a computer in Dallas, Texas. Look at the connection that ties these two computers together and tell me
what would happen if the connection suddenly stopped working or was broken or interrupted in some way? What would happen to your
experience viewing and interacting with the Web site? (Internet domain)
5 How does the Web browser work? In other words, how does it communicate with the computer that stores the Web site? (Web browser
domain)
6 Try to explain the sorts of things that might go wrong if a Web browser can’t access a particular Web site or Web page? (Web browser
domain)
7 Can you describe what a search engine does? (Simple and advanced search domains)
8 When a search engine tries to match your search query words, is it searching the Internet or something else? (Simple and advanced search
domains)
9 Can search engines look for information on the entire World Wide Web? Explain. (Simple and advanced search domains)
10 Is there a difference between the Internet and the World Wide Web or are they the same thing? Explain. (Simple and advanced search
domains)
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
SHARIT et al. Page 25
Table V
Means and Standard Deviations of the Total Knowledge Score and the Individual Knowledge Domain Scores
Groups
Younger Younger-OldOlder-Old
Knowledge Domain M SD M SD M SD
Internet 34.619.0 34.6 17.0 26.7 15.0
Web Browser 37.010.9 27.1 11.5 25.0 11.0
Simple Search 31.6 13.2 28.0 12.3 23.2 7.82
Advanced Search 30.624.7 21.6 18.2 16.1 9.34
Total Knowledge 133.8 62.3 111.2 47.8 89.9 35.2
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
SHARIT et al. Page 26
Table VI
Means and Standard Deviations of the Task Performance Measures
Task Performance
All Simple Complex
Problems ProblemsProblems
Groups M SD M SD M SD
Younger (n= 10) 228.3 107.9 90.0 64.6 138.3 67.0
Younger-Old (n= 20)180.9 67.6 77.2 34.9 103.7 54.6
Older-Old (n= 20) 158.0 69.8 74.9 35.5 83.1 46.2
All Older (n= 40) 169.4 68.8 76.1 34.8 93.4 51.0
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
SHARIT et al. Page 27
Table VII
Correlations between Measures of Performance and Measures of Knowledge for the Younger-Old and the Older-Old Participants
Younger-Old Older-Old
Knowledge DomainAll ProblemsSimple ProblemsComplex ProblemsAll ProblemsSimple ProblemsComplex Problems
Internet .408 .469*.205 .398 .485*.228
Web Browser .501*.384 .376 .161 .282 .026
Simple Search .484*.184 .482*.188 .181 .146
Advanced Search .603** .330 .537*.455*.303 .454*
Total Knowledge .619** .431 .491*.381 .415 .258
*p < .05,
**p < .01 (younger-old: n = 20; older-old: n = 20)
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
SHARIT et al. Page 28
Table VIII
Summary of Regression Analysis for the Three Performance Measures
All Problems
Model StepVariable R2βt-score p
1 Total Knowledge .292 .541 3.86 <.001
2 Total Knowledge .292 .313 2.18 .037
Reasoning .405 .305 2.07 .046
Working Memory.474 .268 2.19 .035
Perceptual Speed .537 .253 2.11 .042
3Age*.537 .021 0.16 .874
Simple Problems
Model Step Variable R2βt-score p
1 Total Knowledge .174 .417 2.75 .009
2 Total Knowledge .174 .102 .635 .530
Reasoning .388 .560 3.50 .001
3Age*.388.008 .059 .954
Complex Problems
Model Step Variable R2βt-score p
1 Total Knowledge .190 .435 2.90 .006
2 Total Knowledge .190 .373 2.62 .013
Working Memory.312 .355 2.49 .018
3Age*.312 .001 .003 .997
*Step 2 represents the final model as adding age in step 3 was not significant.
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
SHARIT et al. Page 29
Table IX
T-tests of Performance for Superior vs. Inferior Types of Mental Models
Performance
Mental ModelAll ProblemsSimple ProblemsComplex Problems
Internet 2.19** 3.24*** 0.84
Web Browser 0.76 0.56 0.64
Search Engine 1.88*0.93 1.89*
The values in the table are t-values used for comparison of mean performance for superior versus inferior types of mental models.
***p < .01,
**p < .05,
*p < .10 (N = 40).
ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 14.
... 2.1. Strategic exploration-exploitation trade-offs in information search activity IS is cyclical and requires users to make iterative decisions in three different phases (Sanchiz, Amadieu, and Chevalier 2020;Sharit et al. 2008;: ...
... These types of indicators primarily provide a general indication of the degree to which users access new or similar content during the IS activity, but do not indicate the extent to which users' mental representations changed during the search session. And yet queries and their content are regarded as the direct translation of users' mental representations at time T (Sanchiz, Amadieu, and Chevalier 2020;Sharit et al. 2008;. ...
Article
While a clear distinction is established between simple lookup tasks and complex exploratory tasks, few studies have documented the differences between complex lookup tasks and simple exploratory tasks. To address this empirical gap, which has implications for system adaptability, the present study focused on these differences and the impact of users’ prior domain knowledge (low vs. high) on exploration-exploitation search strategies (thematic and navigational). The effects of prior domain knowledge on information search performance were also investigated, as well as the extent to which the navigational and thematic search strategies predicted users’ search scores and learning outcomes. Sixty students (35 in psychology, 25 in computer science) were asked to perform two complex lookup tasks and two simple exploratory tasks within and outside their knowledge domain. Results showed that when users performed tasks in their knowledge domain, they formulated more specific queries, achieved higher search scores, and produced higher quality factual knowledge. Complex lookup tasks led to more thematic exploitation and more navigational exploration than simple exploratory tasks. The search strategies predicted output performances differently, depending on the search domain (in vs. out). In light of these results, solutions helping users to improve their search strategies are given.
... This was observed in knowledge scores obtained for both the post-test and the follow-up test conducted concerning modern high-quality knowledge among students, young scientists and interested specialists. Research findings confirm the effect of both the mindsets and learners' content and situational contexts on transforming information into knowledge in virtual interactive learning (Sharit et al., 2008). These suggest that the diversity of content of sessions in these virtual learning significantly influences the knowledge and cognitionbuilding processes. ...
Article
Full-text available
The paper mapped the webinar content to evaluate its influence on participants' progress in cognitive learning on synchronous online platforms that facilitated a multidimensional structural learning process. A quasi-experimental research design was followed in analyzing the transcripts recorded from the three-day academic webinar attended by 630 participants, which was coded into significant themes and the frequency of codes recorded to validate the content coverage using MAXQDA software. The cognitive learning of the participants was calculated as the difference between post-and pre-evaluation test scores. Pre-and post-evaluation knowledge test scores of participants conducted on selected themes showed webinars to be effective in enhancing the knowledge level of participants across age, professional status and gender, with the most significant improvement in students and young professionals related to hybrid breeding correlating the content coverage. It has aided pedagogical characterisation and progress in the effectiveness of online learning environments. Contributes to the metacognition domain of cognitive learning theory in the emerging context of online learning, specifically to the information process theory. Address the research gap on effective planning and design of e-learning content in agricultural education.
... Given that users with lower/declined cognitive ability (e.g. some of the seniors) may perform search tasks in visual interfaces more slowly (Boardman and McCormick 2023;Sharit et al. 2008), it is a reasonable assumption that they should not necessarily be held to the same rigid standard for how fast they need to form satisfactory first impressions. The investigated cognitive abilitiesvisual working memory (WM) and perceptual speed (PS) of participants have a profound impact on first impression feedback that participants provide during n-second testing. ...
Article
Full-text available
Five-second testing is a method commonly used by user research professionals to assess users' first impressions of user interfaces or product designs. Its rule of thumb, that five seconds is generally the right amount of time for users to report realistic and relevant first impressions, misrepresents the reality of human cognition. Users possess disparate levels of cognitive ability for processing stimuli that possess varied visual complexity. We conducted a complex experiment where participants complete an evaluation of their cognitive ability – working memory and perceptual speed. They are shown website stimuli of varied complexity over differing time periods (2/5/10 seconds) to answer a representative list of questions typical for evaluation of first impressions. We show that first impression feedback is rendered inconsistent by cognitive ability and visual complexity. Visually complex stimuli viewed for too short a time link to a problem with identifying the web page purpose. Participants with varied perceptual speed produce comparable results at 5 seconds, but at lower working memory, they provide less verbose answers and recall less information. These findings suggest that to receive relevant first impression feedback, the time for which participants are shown stimuli should be adapted to cognitive abilities and stimulus complexity.
... One of the most researched agerelated effects is a general slowing of cognitive processes. Compared to younger learners, older adults are considerably slower in terms of variables such as reaction time (e.g., Salthouse, 1996) and search tasks (Sharit, Hernandez, Czaja, & Pirolli, 2008). Meta-analytic evidence (Verhaegen & Salthouse, 1997) reveals a mean correlation of −0.52 between aging and speed of processing. ...
Article
Individuals are known to differ in cognitive abilities, affecting their behavior and information processing in digital environments. However, we have a limited understanding of which behaviors are affected, how, and whether some features extracted from digital behavior can predict cognitive abilities. Consequently, researchers may miss opportunities to design and support individuals with personalized experiences and detect those who may benefit from additional interventions. To characterize digital behaviors, we collected 24/7 screen recordings, input behavior, and operating system data from the laptops of 20 adults for two weeks. We use cognitive test results from the same individuals to characterize their cognitive abilities: psychomotor speed, processing speed, selective attention, working memory, and fluid intelligence. Our results from regression analysis, path modeling, and machine learning experiments show that cognitive abilities are associated with differences in digital behavior and that naturalistic behavioral data can predict the cognitive abilities of individuals with small error rates. Our findings suggest naturalistic interaction data as a novel source for modeling cognitive differences.
Article
Searching for information on online environments is a crucial part of today's academics' work. However, no published research has been identified that comprehensively focuses on academics' online information searching patterns. This study aimed to determine the predictor variables of academics' online information searching strategies. A structural model which seeks to explain the relationship between online information searching strategies and predictor variables was constructed. Data was collected with four different data collection instruments: “Personal Information Form”, “Online Information Searching Strategies Scales”, “Cognitive Absorption Scale”, and “Digital Literacy Scale”, which were collected from 501 academics from 18 different universities. SEM analysis techniques were used in the analysis of the data. Twenty-four hypotheses were tested through SEM analysis. The results of the analysis found that academics' online information search strategies can be predicted by digital literacy. Daily internet usage time has a significant effect on cognitive absorption. In addition, digital literacy has a mediating role between online information searching strategies and cognitive absorption.
Article
Full-text available
With the rapid accumulation of online information, efficient web navigation has grown vital yet challenging. To create an easily navigable cyberspace catering to diverse demographics, understanding how people navigate differently is paramount. While previous research has unveiled individual differences in spatial navigation, such differences in knowledge space navigation remain sparse. To bridge this gap, we conducted an online experiment where participants played a navigation game on Wikipedia and completed personal information questionnaires. Our analysis shows that age negatively affects knowledge space navigation performance, while multilingualism enhances it. Under time pressure, participants’ performance improves across trials and males outperform females, an effect not observed in games without time pressure. In our experiment, successful route-finding is usually not related to abilities of innovative exploration of routes. Our results underline the importance of age, multilingualism and time constraint in the knowledge space navigation.
Thesis
Full-text available
ABSTRACT Information searching and information management are an essential part of everyday life in today's information society. The skills to find, evaluate, and organize digital information are needed in both professional and personal contexts, such as work, education, and leisure time. Digital information skills are considered one of the three adult basic skills, alongside reading and numeracy skills. This study examines how various factors related to person (e.g., age, gender), position (e.g., education, employment position), and resources (e.g., digital activity, in-service training) explain digital information skills. The theoretical framework of the study is Jan van Dijk's Resources and Appropriation theory. In Finland, research on digital information skills has mainly focused on schools and students. This study provides new insights into the digital information skills of Finnish adults and workshop youth. The empirical data used in the study consisted of four different groups: primary and secondary school students (n = 3,222), workshop youth (n = 93), metal industry employees (n = 270), and teachers (n = 4,988). The data was collected using digital skills testing applications developed at the University of Turku. The research methods were correlation analysis, linear regression analysis, and cluster analysis. Regarding individual factors, age and gender were associated with better information skills only among adults. Teachers' digital activity, self-efficacy, and digital information skills decreased with age. Digital activity and age explained most of the variation in teachers’ digital information skills. Among metal industry employees, older age groups had weaker digital information skills than younger ones. Men performed slightly better than women and also had more confidence in their digital skills compared to women. Regarding positional factors, both education and position at work were associated with digital information skills. Among metal industry employees, education and using digital tools at work were the most important factors explaining digital information skills. The relationship between work position and digital information skills could only be studied among metal workers. Clerical employees performed better than the production workers. Production workers used digital tools more in their free time than at work, while clerical workers used digital tools at work more than during their free time. Routine factory work, where the use of digital tools is limited, may lead to a vicious circle that prevents skill development. The most significant resource factor was digital activity. Furthermore, selfefficacy and in-service training explained teachers' digital information skills. In the metal industry, production workers' use of digital tools related to their work was the most important factor explaining digital information skills. The use of digital tools in the workplace also strongly correlated with education and position. On the other hand, leisure-time digital use did not explain digital information skills. Using digital tools during leisure time is often simpler than using them during working hours. For workshop youth, digital activity or individual use of digital tools was not related to digital information skills. The only variables that explained digital information skills for them were skills in other areas of digital competence. The results of regression analyses indicate that age was the only person-related factor that explained digital information skills, and even then, only for teachers. Regarding positional factors, educational background explained digital information skills. However, the most significant predictors of information skills were resource factors. Therefore, available resources, such as digital activity and skills in other areas of digital competence, were more significant than personal or positional factors. For instance, merely the position at work does not explain digital information skills; the true determinant of skills is the experience of using digital tools. This is a positive result as it is easier to influence resource factors. KEYWORDS: digital information skills, information retrieval, digital inequality, digital divide
Article
Full-text available
A framework for constructing a cognitive model of users' information searching behaviour is described. The motivation for the framework is to create explanatory and predictive theories of information searching to improve the design of information retrieval (IR) systems. The framework proposes a taxonomy of components for process models of the information seeking task, information need types and knowledge sources necessary to support the task. The framework is developed into a preliminary version of a cognitive theory of information searching by the addition of strategies and correspondence rules which predict user behaviour in different task stages according to information need types, facilities provided by the IR system and knowledge held by the user. The theory is evaluated by using claims analysis based on empirical observations of users information retrieval and by a walkthrough of an IR session to investigate how well the theory can account for empirical evidence. Results show that the theory can indicate the expert strategies which should be followed in different task contexts but predictions of actual user behaviour are less accurate. The future possibilities for employing the theoretical model as a tutorial advisor for information retrieval and as an evaluation method for IR systems are reviewed. The role and potential of cognitive theories of user task-action in Information Retrieval and Human Computer Interaction are discussed.
Book
Significant amounts of our time and energy are devoted to creating, managing, and avoiding information. Computers and telecommunications technology have extended our regard for information and are driving changes in how we learn, work, and play. One result of these developments is that skills and strategies for storing and retrieving information have become more essential and more pervasive in our culture. This book considers how electronic technologies have changed these skills and strategies and augmented the fundamental human activity of information seeking. The author makes a case for creating new interface designs that allow the information seeker to choose what strategy to apply according to their immediate needs. Such systems may be designed by providing information seekers with alternative interface mechanisms for displaying and manipulating multiple levels of representation for information objects. Information Seeking in Electronic Environments is essential reading for researchers and graduate students in information science, human-computer interaction, and education, as well as for designers of information retrieval systems and interfaces for digital libraries and archives.
Article
This article examines searcher behavior and affective response to a hypertext-based bibliographic information retrieval system called HyperLynx for searchers with different search skills and backgrounds. Search times and number of nodes visited were recorded for five specified search queries, and views of the system were recorded for each searcher. No significant differences were found in search times our user satisfaction with the system, indicating that a hypertext-based approach to bibliographic retrieval could be approapriate for a variety of searcher experience levels. © 1995 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Article
An empirical study was performed to train naive subjects in the use of a prototype Boolean logic-based information retrieval system on a database of bibliographic records. The research was based on the mental models theory which proposes that people can be trained to develop a 'mental model' or a qualitative simulation of a system which will aid in generating methods for interacting with the system, debugging errors, and keeping track of one's place in the system. It follows that conceptual training based on a system model will be superior to procedural training based on the mechanics of the system. We performed a laboratory experiment with two training conditions ( model and procedural), and with each condition split by sex. Forty-three subjects participated in the experiment, but only 32 were able to reach the minimum competency level required to complete the experiment. The data analysis incorporated time-stamped monitoring data, personal characteristics variables, affective variables, and interview data in which subjects described how they thought the system worked (an articulation of the model). As predicted, the model-based training had no effect on the ability to perform simple, procedural tasks, but subjects trained with a model performed better on complex tasks that required extrapolation from the basic operations of the system. A stochastic process analysis of search-state transitions reinforced this conclusion. Subjects had difficulty articulating a model of the system, and we found no differences in articulation by condition. The high number of subjects (26%) who were unable to pass the benchmark test indicates that the retrieval tasks were inherently difficult. More interestingly, those who dropped out were significantly more likely to be humanities or social science majors than science or engineering majors, suggesting important individual differences and equity issues. The sex-related differences were slight, although significant, and suggest future research questions.
Article
Our aim was to investigate how older and younger adults find information using the World Wide Web (Web) in a naturalistic setting. After brief training in using a Web browser, 29 older (M=70.6 years) and 30 younger (M=21.8 years) adults conducted Web searches to answer two questions. Age was related to search performance on difficult problems; older adults were somewhat less efficient on the first search and were somewhat less likely to find a correct answer on the second. Age differences in computer attitudes were small; the effects of the training and search experience on attitudes were positive, large, and similar for young and old. With brief but well-designed training, novice older adults were successful in their Web searches and they had very positive reactions to the Web experience.
Article
La necessite d'acceder a de vastes collections de donnees non structurees sur le Web requiert un nouveau type de systeme d'information, dans lequel un controle humain permanent s'exerce sur le processus de recherche. Cette approche hybride reunit les deux domaines de la recherche d'information d'une part et de l'interaction homme-machine d'autre part. Cette nouvelle conception de recherche d'information homme-machine, qui doit permettre aux utilisateurs d'explorer des bases d'information a grande echelle, repose notamment sur l'elargissement du controle et de l'implication humaine, et reflete l'interdependance des chercheurs d'information et des systemes et leur nature evolutive.