ArticlePDF Available

Color-advertising strategies of invasive plants through the bee eye

Frontiers
Frontiers in Plant Science
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Invasive plants represent a significant global challenge as they compete with native plants for limited resources such as space, nutrients and pollinators. Here, we focused on four invasive species that are widely spread in the French Pyrenees, Buddleja davidii, Reynoutria japonica, Spiraea japonica and Impatiens glandulifera, and analyzed their visual advertisement signals with respect to those displayed by their surrounding native species using a perceptual approach based on the neural mechanisms of bee vision given that bees are regular pollinators of these plants. We collected 543 spectral reflections from the 4 invasive species, and 66 native species and estimated achromatic and chromatic similarities to the bee eye. R. japonica, S. japonica and B. davidii were inconspicuous against the foliage background and could be hardly discriminated in terms of color from their surrounding native plants. These characteristics promote generalization, potentially attracting pollinators foraging on similar native species. Two morphs of I. glandulifera were both highly salient in chromatic and achromatic terms and different from their surrounding native species. This distinctive identity facilitates detection and learning in association with rich nectar. While visual signals are not the only sensory cue accounting for invasive-plant success, our study reveals new elements for understanding biological invasion processes from the perspective of pollinator perceptual processes.
(A) Spectral reflectance curves of the four invasive floral species studied. Normalized average reflectance (%) of Buddleja davidii, Reynoutria japonica, Spiraea japonica and Impatiens glandulifera. Royle in the visible range of the honey bee Apis mellifera (from 300 to 650 nm). The average reflectance of green leaves used as background for further perceptual analyses of floral colors is also shown. The inset shows the normalized average reflectance (%) of the three morphs of I. glandulifera, IgP (pink), IgV (violet) and IgW (white). (B) Color loci of the flower species analyzed in the color hexagon. The colour hexagon is a generalized color opponent space with metrics applicable to numerous species of Hymenoptera. The average reflectance of green leaves was used as background and corresponds to the center of the hexagon. Photoreceptor excitations E(UV), E(B), E(G) are plotted at angles of 120°. The hexagon is divided in six segments corresponding to six categories, which refer to the ways in which the bees’ receptors are stimulated by given pure broad-band spectral stimuli, or with uv-green, mixed spectral stimuli: u (ultraviolet), ub (ultraviolet-blue), b (blue), bg (blue-green), g (green) and ug (ultraviolet-green). The loci within the space correspond to the 66 native species (black dots) and the 4 invasive species, one of which had three different morphs (white diamonds), i.e. 72 color loci represented. The closed line surrounding the floral color loci defines the boundaries of color perception at adaptation light. The loci along the spectral line are marked in 10 nm steps, and the mixtures of 300 and 550 nm (ultraviolet-green) in 10% steps. (C) Color loci of the flower species included in our analyses in the color opponent coding (COC) space. The COC space is a color opponent space in which floral color loci of invasive species, native species of Group1 and native species of Group 2 are plotted as a function of the responses of two types of colour opponent coding cells, A and B. The origin of the graph represents the green leaves used as background. The loci within the space correspond to the 66 native species (black dots) and the 4 invasive species, one of which had three different morphs (white diamonds), i.e. 72 color loci represented. The closed line surrounding the floral color loci defines the boundaries of color perception at adaptation light. The loci along the spectral line are marked in 10 nm steps, and the mixtures of 300 and 550 nm (ultraviolet-green) in 10% steps.
… 
This content is subject to copyright.
Color-advertising strategies
of invasive plants through
the bee eye
Martin Dessart
1
,
João Marcelo Robazzi Bignelli Valente Aguiar
2
,
Eric Tabacchi
3
, Sylvie Guillerme
4
*
and Martin Giurfa
5,6
*
1
Institut de Recherche sur la Biologie de l'Insecte, Centre National de la Recherche Scientique
(CNRS), University of Tours, Tours, France,
2
Programa de Po
´s-Graduac¸ão em Entomologia, Faculdade
de Filosoa, Ciências e Letras de Ribeirão Preto, Universidade de São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto, São
Paulo, Brazil,
3
Centre de Recherche sur la Biodiversité et l'Environnement (CRBE), UMR 53000, Centre
National de la Recherche Scientique (CNRS), Institut de Recherche pour le Développement (IRD),
Institut National Polytechnique, Université Paul Sabatier, Toulouse, France,
4
Laboratoire Ge
´ographie
de lEnvironnement (GEODE), Centre National de la Recherche Scientique (CNRS), University
Toulouse Jean-Jaure
s, Toulouse, France,
5
Institut Universitaire de France, Paris, France,
6
Neuroscience Paris-Seine - Institut de Biologie Paris-Seine, Centre National de la Recherche
Scientique (CNRS), Institut National de la Sante
´et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), Sorbonne
University, Paris, France
Invasive plants represent a signicant global challenge as they compete with
native plants for limited resources such as space, nutrients and pollinators. Here,
we focused on four invasive species that are widely spread in the French
Pyrenees, Buddleja davidii,Reynoutria japonica,Spiraea japonica and Impatiens
glandulifera, and analyzed their visual advertisement signals with respect to those
displayed by their surrounding native species using a perceptual approach based
on the neural mechanisms of bee vision given that bees are regular pollinators of
these plants. We collected 543 spectral reections from the 4 invasive species,
and 66 native species and estimated achromatic and chromatic similarities to the
bee eye. R. japonica, S. japonica and B. davidii were inconspicuous against the
foliage background and could be hardly discriminated in terms of color from their
surrounding native plants. These characteristics promote generalization,
potentially attracting pollinators foraging on similar native species. Two
morphs of I. glandulifera were both highly salient in chromatic and achromatic
terms and different from their surrounding native species. This distinctive identity
facilitates detection and learning in association with rich nectar. While visual
signals are not the only sensory cue accounting for invasive-plant success, our
study reveals new elements for understanding biological invasion processes from
the perspective of pollinator perceptual processes.
KEYWORDS
invasive plants, honey bee, vision, color detection, color discrimination
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org01
OPEN ACCESS
EDITED BY
Jair E. Garcia,
RMIT University, Australia
REVIEWED BY
Zong-Xin Ren,
Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS), China
Junpeng Mu,
Mianyang Normal University, China
*CORRESPONDENCE
Sylvie Guillerme
sylvie.guillerme@univ-tlse2.fr
Martin Giurfa
martin.giurfa@sorbonne-universite.fr
These authors share rst authorship
These authors share senior authorship
RECEIVED 28 February 2024
ACCEPTED 30 April 2024
PUBLISHED 22 May 2024
CITATION
Dessart M, Aguiar JMRBV, Tabacchi E,
Guillerme S and Giurfa M (2024)
Color-advertising strategies of
invasive plants through the bee eye.
Front. Plant Sci. 15:1393204.
doi: 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
COPYRIGHT
©2024Dessart,Aguiar,Tabacchi,Guillerme
andGiurfa.Thisisanopen-accessarticle
distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The
use, distribution or reproduction in other
forums is permitted, provided the original
author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are
credited and that the original publication in
this journal is cited, in accordance with
accepted academic practice. No use,
distribution or reproduction is permitted
which does not comply with these terms.
TYPE Original Research
PUBLISHED 22 May 2024
DOI 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
1 Introduction
Invasive plant species represent a signicant challenge in
globalization times as they compete with native plants and animals
for limited resources, modifying habitats and reducing biodiversity
(IUCN Council, 2000;Charles and Dukes, 2007;Hejda et al., 2009;
Pysek et al., 2012;Barney et al., 2013;Langmaier and Lapin, 2020).
Their success in certain biotopes indicates that some species rely on
highly competitive traits allowing themto conquer and thrive in newly
colonized ecosystems (Parker et al., 1999;Gioria and Osborne, 2014).
Invasive plant species compete with native species for space, nutrients
(Skurski et al., 2017) and pollinators (Traveset and Richardson, 2006;
Bjerknes et al., 2007;Kovacs-Hostyanszki et al., 2022). Several studies
have revealed the disruption of native plant-pollinator networks by
successful invasive plant species in detriment of native plant species,
highlighting thereby one of the key factors of their success (Ghazoul,
2004;Bjerknes et al., 2007;Bartomeus et al., 2008;Goodell and Parker,
2017;Stout and Tiedeken, 2017;Burns et al., 2019;Parra-Tabla and
Arceo-Gomez, 2021;Kovacs-Hostyanszki et al., 2022). Some invasive
plants may provide richer nectar, being therefore particularly
attractive for pollinators (Chittka and Schurkens, 2001), or more
conspicuous owers that outcompete those of native plants (Traveset
and Richardson, 2006;Sooraj et al., 2019). They are also usually
generalists with respect to pollinators, a strategy that allows them
beneting from the fertilization contribution of local pollinators
(Richardson et al., 2007;Parra-Tabla and Arceo-Gomez, 2021).
Abundant invasive plants can even dominate the plant-pollinator
network, to a point where the interaction between native plants and
their pollinators is modied (Albrecht et al., 2014).
Color is one of the main advertising cues displayed by owers to
pollinators (Kevan, 1983;Weiss, 1991;Menzel and Shmida, 1993;
Chittka et al., 1999). Among the main pollinators of owers, the
western honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) is the most frequent oral
visitor of crops worldwide (Hung et al., 2018;Khalifa et al., 2021).
Honey bees are also one of the insects best studied in terms of their
visual perception, from behavior to the neural underpinnings of
visual performances (Giurfa and Lehrer, 2001;Avarguès-Weber
et al., 2011;Avarguès-Weber et al., 2012;Mota et al., 2013;
Avarguès-Weber and Giurfa, 2014). Bees possess trichromatic
color vision based on three photoreceptors types with sensitivities
peaking at 344 nm in the short-wave (ultra violet) region of the
spectrum, 436 nm in the middle-wave (blue) region, and 544 nm in
the long-wave (green) region of the spectrum (L receptor),
respectively (Menzel and Blakers, 1976;Peitsch et al., 1992).
Photoreceptor signals are fed into color-opponent neurons
present in higher-order visual areas of the bee brain (Kien and
Menzel, 1977;Backhaus, 1991), giving origin to color sensations.
Color processing networks are therefore well-characterized in the
honey bee, which provided the basis for the conception of color
perceptual spaces that allow determining to what extent colors are
discriminable for a honey bee (Backhaus, 1991;Chittka, 1992). This
strategy has been used to assess ower color perception and
discrimination by bees in multiple studies e.g. (Kevan et al., 1996;
Reisenman and Giurfa, 2008;Arnold et al., 2009;Aguiar et al., 2020;
Shrestha et al., 2024).
However, few analyses have focused on the oral color of
invasive species and its role and contribution to their ecological
success from a pollinator vision perspective. Analyses performed on
an Indian community including 22 native and 8 invasive plant
species showed that invasive species with higher UV absorbance
tended to differ chromatically from native species, a strategy that
may increase their attractiveness and discriminability for
pollinators (Sooraj et al., 2019). Yet, the strategy adopted by
invasive plants may vary signicantly according to the
characteristics of their ecological niche and those of the
surrounding native species.
The region of the Pyrenees, delimiting the border between
France and Spain, has been subjected to intensive invasion by
various plant species (Guillerme et al., 2020;Claudel et al., 2022).
The area is also rich and reputed for the diversity of local ora
(Saule, 2002) and the presence of numerous bee pollinating species
so that it provides an attractive and relevant scenario to study the
color strategies used by invasive plant species. Understanding the
role of oral coloration through the bee eye is a powerful proxy for
understanding the success of invasive plantsreproduction. We thus
focused on four dominant invasive plant species that can be found
in the French Pyrenees, the butterybush(Buddleja davidii,
Scrophulariaceae), the Japanese Knotweed (Reynoutria japonica,
Polygonaceae), the Japanese Spirea (Spiraea japonica, Rosaceae)
and the Himalaya Balsam (Impatiens glandulifera, Balsaminaceae),
which are frequently visited by honey bees (Ellis and Ellis-Adam,
1993;Najberek et al., 2023). We studied how their color displays are
perceived by bee pollinators in comparison to those of surrounding
native species. To this end, we used perceptual neural modelling of
honey bee vision, which allowed us to analyze the color advertising
strategies of invasive species as seen by the bee eye. While an
integrative analysis of invasive-plant success should include
measures of plant visitation by pollinators and of plant tness,
our goal here was to rely on a neuroscience approach to provide
evidence on pollinator perception that could serve as guide for
future works by scholars working in ecology, plant science and
other related disciplines.
Our results show that the color advertising strategies of the four
species considered are heterogeneous as some of them display
colors that are similar to those of surrounding native species,
potentially promoting generalization, while others display highly
prominent color and achromatic cues different from those of the
surrounding native species to facilitate color detection and learning
by pollinators.
2 Materials and methods
2.1 Study area and invasive plant species
We investigated two sites located at the foothills of the Central
Pyrenees Mountains (SW France, Figure 1): the Pique Valley (42°45
N, 0°37E, 1200 m mean altitude) (Figure 1A) and the Oussouet
Valley (43°5N, 0°5E, 800 m mean altitude) (Figure 1B). Both have
very similar landscapes including agro-pastoral and forested units
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org02
hosting high levels of plant and animal biodiversity (Rhone, 2015),
including abundant invasive species (Tabacchi et al., 2010;Guillerme
et al., 2020;Jantzi et al., 2021). The climate of the sampled sites
corresponds to mountain-oceanic temperate conditions (Mean
temperature: 4,0 16,5°C; Monthly accumulated precipitation:
27,8mm 174,2mm).
The dominant invasive species found in the study areas were the
buttery bush (Buddleja davidii Franch.; Figure 1C), the Japanese
Knotweed (Reynoutria japonica Houtt.; Figure 1D), the Japanese
Spirea (Spiraea japonica L.;Figure 1E) and the Himalaya Balsam
(Impatiens glandulifera Royle; Figure 1F), which provided the basis
for our study. The latter included three different morphs, which
appeared violet (Figure 1G), white (Figure 1H) and pink (Figure 1I)
to the human eye.
We conducted our surveys at four distinct dates - 20
th
and 22
nd
of July 2020, 23
rd
and 28
th
of September 2020 - which corresponded
to the average maximum development of the vegetation.
For each sampling session, we chose 12 representative
permanent vegetation patches centered on a particular hotspot of
a specic invasive plants among the four species targeted. Each
patch corresponded to one invasive species; in total, we established
2 patches for B. davidii, 3 patches for R. japonica, 1 patch for S.
japonica and 2 patches for each morph of I. glandulifera (6 patches
for I glandulifera). Each patch corresponded to a particular site in
order to integrate spatial variability. Data were pooled as an analysis
per patch would require a higher number of replicates, which was
not easily accessible. At the regional scale, these sampling units were
selected based on an aerial photograph survey. The location of each
patch was conrmed by local preliminary eld surveys. Within each
identied patch, we delineated four concentric zones (Figure 1J).
The core zonewas densely and almost exclusively occupied by one
of the invasive species chosen for our study. The inner edge zone
was delineated as an inner ring within the core zone (~1,5 m wide
strip) using the average quasi-absence (< 5% cover) of the invader as
criterion. The outer edge zonewas dened as an external ring
surrounding the core zone (~1,5 m wide strip) in which the invader
was totally absent. Finally, the more eccentric surrounding zone
was dened as a broader area included within a 20m width strip
beyond the outer edge zone. By denition the more external zone
corresponds to areas where the invader was absent. This is a
consequence of the patchy pattern induced by the competition/
colonization processes, but probably also by the effect of
management (mowing) in the outer zone.
2.2 Reectance measurements and analysis
A minimum of three fresh owers of each species were
systematically collected at each patch along the four zones in order
to measure their spectral reectance. Reectance measurements in
the bee visible range (300 650 nm) (Menzel and Blakers, 1976) were
performed in situ and with a resolution of 1 nm using a UV-VIS
Ocean Optics USB400 spectrometer (Dunedin, Florida, USA) and a
pulsed Xenon Lamp PX-2 light source. A white standard (barium
FIGURE 1
Study areas, invasive species studied and sampling strategy. (A) Relief map of the Pique Valley (42°45N, 0°37E, 1200 m mean altitude), one of the
areas chosen for our study. (B) Relief map of the Oussouet Valley (43°5N, 0°5E, 800 m mean altitude), the other area chosen for our study.
(C) The invasive buttery bush (Buddleja davidii Franch). (D) The invasive Japanese Knotweed (Reynoutria japonica Houtt). (E) The invasive Japanese
Spirea (Spiraea japonica L.). (F) The invasive Himalaya Balsam (Impatiens glandulifera Royle). The latter included three different morphs, which
appeared violet (G), white (H) and pink (I) to the human eye. (J) Sampling strategy of permanent vegetation patches centred on a particular hotspot
of a specic invasive plants. Within each patch, four concentric zones were established. The core zonewas densely invaded by one of the invasive
species chosen for our study. The inner edge zonewas delineated as an inner ring within the core zone (~1,5 m wide strip) using the average quasi-
absence (< 5% cover) of the invader as criterion. The outer edge zonewas dened as an external ring surrounding the core zone (~1,5 m wide strip)
in which the invader was totally absent. Finally, the more eccentric surrounding zonewas dened as a broader area included within a 20m width
strip beyond the outer edge zone. For each plant species, we estimated visually the relative cover within each zone.
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org03
sulphate) was used to calibrate the spectrophotometer before each
session of measurements. The reectance spectra of 70 plant species
were included in our analyses (see Supplementary Table S2).
Reectance spectra were obtained from 50 native species (n = 333
spectra; 1 spectrum corresponding to 1 ower part) and from the 4
invasive species mentioned above (n = 102 spectra), all collected in
the eld, in the areas dened for our sampling. The FRED database
(Arnold et al., 2010) allowed us to include 16 additional native species
(n = 34 spectra) to complement our measurements (Supplementary
Table S2). These species were present in the eld and were counted
for abundancy measurements but were inaccessible for reectance
measurements. The same database was used to include
complementary measurements for 32 native species (n = 74
spectra) that were also sampled in the eld (see Supplementary
Table S2 for details). Overall, our analyses included 543 spectra
(435 from the eld and 108 from the FRED data), from 70 plant
species, including the 4 invasive species, which were the focus of our
study, and 66 native species (18 exclusively from the eld, 16
exclusively from the FRED data base, and 32 both from the eld
and the FRED data base). For all species, both spectral curves and
relative abundance measurements were available and used for our
analyses. We also measured the spectral reectance from leaves of 15
species (n = 25 spectra) to characterize the green foliage background
(see Supplementary Table S2).
2.3 Color perception models
We used the pavo package (Maia et al., 2013) to normalize
reectance spectra in the wavelength range comprised between 300-
650 nm. Curves were smoothed to remove noise. Flower reectance
curves were averaged within each species. All statistical analyses
were conducted using R software version 4.0.4 (https://www.r-
project.org/).
To test differences in ower colour according to pollinator vision,
oral reectance data were transformed into color loci within the
color hexagon, a perceptual space allowing the evaluation of ower
colors as seen by honey bees, which are dominant pollinators in the
regions of interest (Chittka, 1992). For calculations, we used the
standard illuminant function D65, the average function obtained
from green leaves sampled as the background and the spectral
sensitivity curves of the three photoreceptor types of the honey bee
Apis mellifera (Peitsch et al., 1992). We created the coldspace object
by passing the vismodel object and selected the hexagon to plot our
models. To calculate color distances, we passed the coldspace object
and obtained unweighted Euclidean distances. All the data and code
used are available in: https://github.com/martindessart/
Invasive_plants_through_bee-eye_Pyrenees.
2.4 Data clustering and PCA analysis
In order to evaluate the color similarity of invasive and native
ower species to the honey bee eye, we rst calculated indices to
determine the optimized number of clusters of ower loci within the
hexagon space. These clusters were used to describe our dataset into
predened groups that have similar properties.
We rst used the NbClust package from R software (Charrad
et al., 2014), which provides 30 indices and allows choosing the best
clustering size for our data set. We veried this choice using the
function fviz_nbclust associated with the silhouette method from
the FactoExtra package (Kassambara and Mundt, 2020). We ran a
PCA analysis based on the coordinates of the ower loci in the
hexagon space using k-means clustering. This method of vector
quantization partitioned our data into k groups that minimized the
sum of squares from points to the assigned cluster center (see
Supplementary Material). All statistical analyses were conducted
using R software version 4.0.4 (https://www.r-project.org/).
2.5 Question 1: do invasive oral species
have more salient visual cues to the
bee eye?
For each ower species, we calculated the chromatic contrast,
the achromatic contrast and the spectral purity. The chromatic
contrast refers to the perceptual difference between the color of the
ower and that of the background. Given that the color hexagon is a
bidimensional space that includes only the dimensions of hue and
saturation (i.e. no brightness dimension), the chromatic contrast
can be quantied as the distance between the locus of a ower
species in that space and that of the background. The latter occupies
the center of the space (0, 0 coordinates) as it acts as the adaptation
background against which colors are evaluated (Chittka, 1992).
Chromatic contrast involves true color vision and allows close-up
detection of color targets with larger visual angles, i.e. typically
larger than 15° (Giurfa et al., 1996b;Giurfa et al., 1997). Chromatic
differences between color loci are quantied in terms of the
Euclidian distance between two color loci (see below); in the
hexagon this distance is given in hexagon units HU (Chittka, 1992).
The achromatic contrast refers to the relative number of
absorbed quanta by the L-receptor type (longwave or green
photoreceptor type) upon stimulation with the ower color with
respect to the background. This receptor-specic contrast provides
an achromatic channel allowing long-distance detection of visual
targets (i.e. targets with a reduced visual angle, typically smaller
than 15°) (Giurfa et al., 1997;Giurfa and Vorobyev, 1998).
Finally, the spectral purity represents the saturation of a given
color, a variable that may affect oral preference in bees (Lunau,
1990;Lunau, 1992;Lunau et al., 2006;Rohde et al., 2013). It is
quantied by dividing the distance between the loci of the oral
color and the background (chromatic contrast, see above) by the
distance between the corresponding monochromatic light with the
same dominant wavelength as the color target and the background
(Lunau et al., 1996).
Two steps were designed for the analysis of ower salience. First,
group-level salience was assessed by comparing invasive and native
owers in terms of three variables dened above (i.e., chromatic
contrast, achromatic contrast and spectral purity) using a one-
sample Kruskal-Wallis H test with group as the xed factor. To
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org04
analyze the salience of one invasive species with respect to groups of
native species, a one-sample Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used
setting the invasive-species value of the variable considered as
reference for the comparison. This analysis was also performed
segregating groups and invasive species according to the sample
areas (core, inner and outer edges and surrounding).
2.6 Question 2: are invasive owers
species discriminable from native species
to the bee eye?
We followed the procedure from Maia and White (2018) to
determine if colors were discriminable from each other. The
method considers the statistical distribution, the within-group
variation and the discriminability of groups based on honey bee
visual abilities. A distance-based PERMANOVA using the
Euclidian distances between species in the hexagon was applied to
two groups of interest (e.g., groups determined by the clustering
analysis) to determine if they were signicantly distinct. A bootstrap
procedure was then applied to simulate new samples, in order to
obtain a distribution of the mean distance between each sample and
the group geometric mean, and thus a 95% condence interval for
each color comparison. We nally compared the values obtained for
each invasive species to the beescolor discrimination threshold of
0.1 hexagon units (HU), which has been reported for bees trained
under absolute conditioning (a single color rewarded) (Dyer and
Chittka, 2004). This kind of training corresponds to the ecological
scenario of a ower-constant pollinator visiting a single species
during its foraging bouts. If a color distance was lower than this
threshold, we concluded that the samples compared were
perceptually similar and thus indistinguishable in chromatic
terms by honey bees (Maia and White, 2018).
3 Results
3.1 Spectral reectance curves and ower
colors in perceptual color spaces
We measured the spectral reectance of the four invasive
species considered in our study, Reynoutria japonica, which was
whitish to the human eye, Buddleja davidii, which was violet-
pinkish to the human eye, Spiraea japonica L., which was pink to
the human eye (Figure 2A), and Impatiens glandulifera, which
presented three different colored morphs that appeared violet, white
and pink to the human eye (inset of Figure 2A). In addition, we
measured the average reectance of green leaves present in the
sampling areas (Figure 2A) in order to use it as the background for
representing ower colors in perceptual color spaces. Our
measurements allowed us to represent the loci of the invasive
species, together with those of all ower species included in our
study, in the perceptual spaces of the color hexagon (Figure 2B) and
the color opponent coding space (Figure 2C)dened for the honey
bee. Subsequent analyses on ower color loci were restricted to the
color hexagon given the higher generality of this perceptual space
for pollinators other than honey bees (Backhaus, 1991;
Chittka, 1992).
ABC
FIGURE 2
(A) Spectral reectance curves of the four invasive oral species studied. Normalized average reectance (%) of Buddleja davidii,Reynoutria japonica,
Spiraea japonica and Impatiens glandulifera. Royle in the visible range of the honey bee Apis mellifera (from 300 to 650 nm). The average
reectance of green leaves used as background for further perceptual analyses of oral colors is also shown. The inset shows the normalized
average reectance (%) of the three morphs of I. glandulifera,IgP (pink), IgV (violet) and IgW (white). (B) Color loci of the ower species analyzed in
the color hexagon. The colour hexagon is a generalized color opponent space with metrics applicable to numerous species of Hymenoptera. The
average reectance of green leaves was used as background and corresponds to the center of the hexagon. Photoreceptor excitations E(UV), E(B), E
(G) are plotted at angles of 120°. The hexagon is divided in six segments corresponding to six categories, which refer to the ways in which the bees
receptors are stimulated by given pure broad-band spectral stimuli, or with uv-green, mixed spectral stimuli: u (ultraviolet), ub (ultraviolet-blue), b
(blue), bg (blue-green), g (green) and ug (ultraviolet-green). The loci within the space correspond to the 66 native species (black dots) and the 4
invasive species, one of which had three different morphs (white diamonds), i.e. 72 color loci represented. The closed line surrounding the oral
color loci denes the boundaries of color perception at adaptation light. The loci along the spectral line are marked in 10 nm steps, and the mixtures
of 300 and 550 nm (ultraviolet-green) in 10% steps. (C) Color loci of the ower species included in our analyses in the color opponent coding (COC)
space. The COC space is a color opponent space in which oral color loci of invasive species, native species of Group1 and native species of Group
2 are plotted as a function of the responses of two types of colour opponent coding cells, A and B. The origin of the graph represents the green
leaves used as background. The loci within the space correspond to the 66 native species (black dots) and the 4 invasive species, one of which had
three different morphs (white diamonds), i.e. 72 color loci represented. The closed line surrounding the oral color loci denes the boundaries of
color perception at adaptation light. The loci along the spectral line are marked in 10 nm steps, and the mixtures of 300 and 550 nm (ultraviolet-
green) in 10% steps.
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org05
3.2 Data clustering and PCA analysis
Unsupervised hierarchical clustering was performed on our
ower data set to group species by color. First, we assessed the
clustering tendency by calculating the Hopkins statistic using the
get_clust_tendency function from the FactoExtra R package
(Kassambara and Mundt, 2020). The closer the Hopkins statistic is
to 1, the more the data are clustered (Jove et al., 2020). In our case, the
value of 0.726 indicated that the dataset contained suitable information
for clustering. We then performed two independent analyses to
validate the correct number of clusters using the NbClust package
and the FactoExtra package. The data clustering analysis separated two
distinct groups according to their color loci (Figure 3A).
The rst group (hereafter Group 1) included 52 species (48 native
species and the 4 invasive species), which were located in the bee UV-
blue and blue regions of the hexagon (human blue purple)
(Figure 2B). The second group (hereafter Group 2) included 18
native species, which were located in the bee ultraviolet-green and
green regions of the hexagon (Figure 2B). Invasive ower species were
located per denition in the core zone as sampling patches were
centered on a particular hotspot of an invasive plant species
(Figure 3B). They could also bee found in the inner edge zone.
Group-1 native species were mostly found close to patch core zones,
i.e. in the inner and the outer edge zones. Group-2 native species were
less frequent and usually located in the outer edge and the
surrounding zones of the designed plots in the eld (Figure 3B).
3.3 Question 1: do invasive oral species
have more salient visual cues to the
bee eye?
We calculated the average chromatic contrast, achromatic
contrast and spectral purity for the four invasive species and for
the native species of Group 1 and Group 2 (Figures 4AC; see
Supplementary Table S3 for complete set of values).
Chromatic contrast calculations based on the visual system of
honey bees yielded values ranging from 0.05 to 0.40 hexagon units
(HU) (Figure 4A; Group 1: 0.15 ± 0.05; Group 2: 0.18 ± 0.08; Invasive
Species: 0.18 ± 0.08; mean ± SD), which did not differ signicantly
between groups (Kruskal Wallis Test: c
2
= 1.69, df: 2 p = 0.43). The
calculation of achromatic contrast yielded values ranging from 0.57
to 1.21 (Figure 4B; Group 1: 0.68 ± 0.12; Group 2: 0.65 ± 0.03;
Invasive Species: 0.76 ± 0.23; mean ± SD), which did also not differ
between groups (c
2
= 0.86, df:2, p = 0.65). Finally, spectral purity
values, which ranged from 0.11 to 0.73 HU (Figure 4C; Group 1: 0.37
± 0.11; Group 2: 0.36 ± 0.15; Invasive Species: 0.40 ± 0.15; mean ±
SD; see Supplementary Table S3), did also not differ between groups
(c
2
= 1.78, df = 2; p = 0.41). These results indicate that when taken
together the four invasive species did not differ in chromatic and
achromatic salience to bee eye. Yet, as the four invasive species
differed in their visual display, averaging their values for a between-
category analysis may hide signicant trends at a single-species level.
We thus performed separate comparisons between each invasive
species and the two groups of native species dened in our
cluster analysis.
While no clear differentiation trend was observable for B.
davidii, R. japonica and S. japonica in terms of chromatic
contrast, spectral purity and achromatic contrast, highly
signicant differences were found between the I. glandulifera
morphs and Group 1 and 2 of native species (Table 1). While the
violet (Ig V) and the pink (Ig P) morphs presented signicantly
higher chromatic and achromatic contrasts, as well as a higher
spectral purity than native species in Groups 1 and 2 (p < 0.0001 for
10 of the 12 comparisons, and p < 0.03 for the remaining two
comparison; see Table 1), the white morph (Ig W) had signicantly
lower values for the three variables considered than native species in
Groups 1 and 2 (p < 0.0001 for all 6 comparisons; see Table 1). This
AB
FIGURE 3
(A) Clustering analysis of the oral data set used in this study. Unsupervised hierarchical clustering performed on our ower data set allowed to
distinguish two groups according to their color loci in the hexagon. The two axes indicate in parentheses the % of the data variance accounted for
by our analysis. Group 1 (violet dots) included 52 species: 48 native species and the 4 invasive species Bd (Buddleja davidii), Sj (Spiraea japonica), Rj
(Reynoutria japonica) and the three morphs of Ig (Impatiens glandulifera) IgV, IgW and IgP, all located in the bee ultraviolet-blue and blue regions of
the hexagon. Invasive species are shown by red dots. Group 2 (yellow dots) included 18 native species located in the ultraviolet-green and green
regions of the hexagon. (B) Relative frequency of the invasive species and the two groups of native species in each sampled zone. Per denition,
invasive species were mostly located in the core zone, as sampling areas were centered on a particular hotspot of an invasive plant species. They
could also be located in the inner edge zone among the four species targeted. Group 1 species were mostly found in the inner edge and in the
outer edge zone. Group 2 species were less frequent and usually located in the outer edge and the surrounding zones. Boxplots show the median
(horizontal line) and interquartile ranges. Bars indicate +/- interquartile ranges. Dark squares indicate mean value for the zone considered.
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org06
analysis thus shows that three of the invasive species, B. davidii, R.
japonica and S. japonica, which clustered with native species of
Group 1 (Figure 3), did not differ particularly from their
surrounding native species while I. glandulifera morphs, which
were outside that cluster (Figure 3), adopted a different displaying
strategy to the beeseyes: the violet and the pink morphs appeared
more salient and thus better detectable to pollinators than native
species, while the white morph was less salient than the
native species.
In order to rene this analysis, we repeated the previous analysis
of chromatic and achromatic contrasts and spectral purity but this
time segregating the data according to the sampling zones dened
in Figure 1J.Figures 5ACand Table 2 show the three variables
evaluated (chromatic contrast, achromatic contrast and spectral
purity, respectively) for Groups 1 and 2 of native species and for the
invasive species as a function of the sampling zone. Some signicant
differences were found for the three variables when invasive species
were compared to Group 2 of native species (see Table 2, right), in
particular in the case of the achromatic contrast. Yet, the interesting
comparisons are those between invasive species and Group 1 of
native plants as relative-abundance analyses (see Figure 3) showed
that Group 1 species tended to be more present around invasive
species in the central sampling areas (core, inner and outer edge)
than Group 2 species. Figure 5 and Table 2 show that while R.
japonica did neither differ from its surrounding native species in
chromatic contrast nor in achromatic contrast nor in spectral
purity, the other invasive species showed either partial (for one or
two variables) or total (for all three variables) signicant differences
with native species of Group 1. B. davidii, for instance, had a higher
achromatic contrast than native species of Group, 1 which favors
long-distance detection (Giurfa et al., 1997), while S. japonica was
more salient in terms of chromatic variables such as chromatic
contrast and spectral purity. I. glandulifera morphs presented
consistent signicant differences in all three variables when
compared to surrounding native species of Group 1. The violet
andthepinkmorphs(IgVandIgP)hadsignicantly higher
achromatic and chromatic contrasts and spectral purity than
surrounding native species, which provides advantages in terms
of farther visual detection and higher chromatic salience which may
favour better associative learning by pollinators. Interestingly the
opposite trend was found for the white morph (IgW), which had
signicantly lower values for all three variables.
Taken together, these results indicate that the four invasive
species considered in our study use different visual advertising
strategies when compared to their surrounding native species
(Group 1): in general terms, they tended to be more salient to the
bee eye either in one, two or in the three visual variables considered,
which may facilitate their visual detection from farther distances
and their learning based on chromatic cues associated with nectar
reward. Only the white morph of I. glandulifera seems to be in
disadvantage with respect to native species, thus raising the
question of the mechanisms used by this morph to compensate
via other (non-visual) advertising mechanisms for this decit.
Similar trends were found for the comparisons between invasive
species and native species of Group 2, although these were
less consistent.
3.4 Question 2: are invasive owers
species discriminable from native species
to the bee eye?
Invasive species had higher achromatic and chromatic contrasts
as well as higher spectral purity, which facilitate visual detection and
learning by bee pollinators; yet, to what extent bees perceive them as
chromatically similar to surrounding native species (i.e. Group 1)
needs to be addressed separately. We thus determined the color
similarity between invasive and native species, which can be
estimated based on the distance between color loci in the color
hexagon. We used the threshold value of 0.1 hexagon units (HU)
reported for colour discrimination of bees trained under absolute
conditioning (Dyer and Chittka, 2004) and determined if the color
distance between the invasive species considered in our work and
Groups 1 and 2 of native species was above or below this threshold.
AB C
FIGURE 4
(A) Chromatic contrast (hexagon Units, HU) of Group1, Group 2 and the invasive species analyzed in our study. No signicant differences between
groups were found for this parameter. (B) Achromatic contrast (L-receptor contrast with respect to the background) of Group1, Group 2 and the
invasive species analyzed in our study. No signicant differences between groups were found for this parameter. (C) Spectral purity (hexagon Units,
HU) of Group1, Group 2 and the invasive species analyzed in our study. No signicant differences between groups were found for this parameter.
Boxplots show the median values (thick line) and interquartile ranges; dots indicate values recorded for each species; black squares indicate mean
values of each group. Number in parentheses indicate sample sizes. NS, not signicant.
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org07
TABLE 1 Chromatic contrast, achromatic contrast and spectral purity of single invasive species compared to values of Group 1 and Group 2 of native species.
Comparisons with Group 1 Comparisons with Group 2
Chromatic contrast Achromatic contrast Spectral purity Chromatic contrast Achromatic contrast Spectral purity
V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p
Ig V 054< 0.0001 054< 0.0001 054< 0.0001 318< 0.0001 018< 0.0001 318< 0.0001
Ig P 83 54 < 0.0001 117 54 < 0.0001 244 54 < 0.0001 36 18 0.03 018< 0.0001 35 18 0.03
Ig W 1423 54 < 0.0001 1420 54 < 0.0001 1423 54 < 0.0001 171 18 < 0.0001 170 18 < 0.0001 171 18 < 0.0001
Bd 698 54 0.88 479 54 0.04 727 54 0.92 113 18 0.25 20 18 < 0.01 51 18 0.14
Rj 930 54 0.06 867 54 0.18 829 54 0.32 130 18 0.05 100 18 0.55 66 18 0.42
Sj 446 54 0.02 836 54 0.29 365 54 < 0.01 83 18 0.93 98 18 0.61 36 18 0.03
To analyze the salience of a single invasive species with respect to Groups 1 and 2 of native species, a one-sample Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used setting the invasive-species value of the variable considered as reference for the comparison. V statistic values, sample sizes
(n) of the Group and p values are provided for each comparison performed; df: 1 for all comparisons. Ig V, violet morph of Impatiens glandulifera; Ig P, pink morph of Impatiens glandulifera; Ig W, violet morph of Impatiens glandulifera; Bd, Buddleja davidii; Rj, Reynoutria
japonica; Sj, Spiraea japonica. Values in red indicate signicant P values.
TABLE 2 Chromatic contrast (A), achromatic contrast (B) and spectral purity (C) of single invasive species compared to values of Group 1 and Group 2 of native species according to the sample zone (Core, Inner
Edge, Outer Edge and Surrounding).
A)
Chromatic Contrast
Comparisons with Group 1 Comparisons with Group 2
Core Inner Edge Outer Edge Surrounding Core Inner Edge Outer Edge Surrounding
V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p
Ig V 033< 0.0001 054< 0.0001 082< 0.0001 086< 0.0001 6 6 0,2 10 14 < 0.01 15 30 < 0.0001 21 38 < 0.0001
Ig P 16 33 < 0.0001 63 54 < 0.0001 135 82 < 0.0001 121 86 < 0.0001 18 6 0,55 57 14 0,89 155 30 0,07 229 38 0,02
Ig
W595 33 < 0.0001 1540 54 < 0.0001 3482 82 < 0.0001 3819 86 < 0.0001 28 6 0,02 120 14 < 0.001 496 30 < 0.0001 780 38 < 0.0001
Bd 232 33 0,27 746 54 0,84 1622 82 0,58 1825 86 0,71 23 6 0,15 93 14 0,06 395 30 < 0.01 582 38 < 0.01
Rj 332 33 0,56 971 54 0,09 2090 82 0,12 2340 86 0,07 26 6 0,05 100 14 0,02 431 30 < 0.001 647 38 < 0.001
Sj 152 33 0,01 496 54 0,02 1093 82 < 0.01 1147 86 < 0.01 21 6 0,27 81 14 0,24 317 30 0,18 444 38 0,45
(Continued)
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org08
TABLE 2 Continued
B)
Achromatic Contrast
Comparisons with Group 1 Comparisons with Group 2
Core Inner Edge Outer Edge Surrounding Core Inner Edge Outer Edge Surrounding
V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p
Ig V 033< 0.0001 054< 0.0001 082< 0.0001 086< 0.0001 060,02 014< 0.001 030< 0.0001 038< 0.0001
Ig P 15 33 < 0.0001 88 54 < 0.0001 138 82 < 0.0001 135 86 < 0.0001 060,02 014< 0.001 030< 0.0001 038< 0.0001
Ig
W584 33 < 0.0001 1523 54 < 0.0001 3463 82 < 0.0001 3809 86 < 0.0001 27 6 0,03 119 14
<
0.001 495 30 < 0.0001 779 38 < 0.0001
Bd 142 33 < 0.01 466 54 0,01 933 82 < 0.001 1047 86 < 0.001 360,08 36 14 0,18 103 30 < 0.01 162 38 < 0.01
Rj 268 33 0,62 851 54 0,5 1845 82 0,65 2098 86 0,44 18 6 0,55 98 14 0,03 377 30 0,01 622 38 < 0.01
Sj 266 33 0,6 833 54 0,6 1793 82 0,82 2034 86 0,61 18 6 0,55 98 14 0,03 376 30 0,01 621 38 < 0.01
C)
Spectral Purity
Comparisons with Group 1 Comparisons with Group 2
Core Inner Edge Outer Edge Surrounding Core Inner Edge Outer Edge Surrounding
V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p V statistic n p
Ig V 033< 0.0001 054< 0.0001 082< 0.0001 086< 0.0001 6 6 0,2 10 14 < 0.01 15 30 < 0.0001 21 38 < 0.0001
Ig P 109 33 < 0.01 310 54 < 0.001 750 82 < 0.0001 711 86 < 0.0001 18 6 0,55 54 14 0,75 145 30 0,04 219 38 0,02
Ig
W595 33 < 0.0001 1540 54 < 0.0001 3482 82 < 0.0001 3819 86 < 0.0001 28 6 0,02 120 14 < 0.001 496 30 < 0.0001 780 38 < 0.0001
Bd 260 33 0,53 793 54 0,85 1740 82 0,99 1918 86 0,99 19 6 0,45 66 14 0,75 228 30 0,7 310 38 0,27
Rj 323 33 0,67 894 54 0,3 1939 82 0,37 2132 86 0,36 21 6 0,27 81 14 0,24 286 30 0,46 391 38 0,99
Sj 144 33 < 0.01 440 54 < 0.01 1024 82 < 0.01 1063 86 < 0.001 18 6 0,55 57 14 0,89 155 30 0,07 229 38 0,02
A one-sample Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used to compare Group 1 and Group 2 values to invasive-species values considered as reference for the comparison. V statistic values, sample sizes (n) of the Group and p values are provided for each comparison performed; df: 1 for all
comparisons. Ig V, violet morph of Impatiens glandulifera; Ig P, pink morph of Impatiens glandulifera; Ig W, violet morph of Impatiens glandulifera; Bd, Buddleja davidii; Rj, Reynoutria japonica; Sj, Spiraea japonica. Values in red indicate signicant P values.
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org09
The global analysis (i.e. taking all invasive species together) showed
no signicative differences of variance homogeneity within the three
groups (one-way ANOVA: F2,78 = 2.21, p = 0.11). The between-
group comparison showed signicant differences of color distance
between Groups 1 and 2 (PERMANOVA: F2,78 = 86.6, p < 0.01)
and between the Invasive group and Group 2 (PERMANOVA:
F2,78 = 13.8, p < 0.01). Thus, both Group 1 and 2 and Group 2 and
the Invasive group appeared clearly distinct to bees. No signicant
difference was found between the Invasive group and Group 1
(PERMANOVA: F2,78 = 0.04, p = 0.14), thus suggesting that taken
together, invasive species tended to be chromatically similar to plant
species in Group 1 and different from plant species in Group 2.
As invasive species differ in their color display strategies, we
rened the analysis of color similarity by comparing the color
distance (HU units) between each invasive species/morph and
Groups1and2ofnativespecies.Figure 6 conrms that honey
bees should be able to discriminate all four invasive species from
the native species of Group 2 as the color distances separating
them are well above threshold. However, the situation changes
when the comparison is between invasive species and Group 1 of
native species, which are precisely those surrounding principally
the invasive species considered. Indeed, B. davidii,R. japonica and
S. japonica are likely to be chromatically undistinguishable from
the average color of Group 1 species as their mean color distance,
lower and upper limits of the 95% condence interval were 0.06,
0.03, 0.08 (B. davidii), 0.04, 0.02, 0.06 (R. japonica) and 0.05, 0.03,
0.07 (S. japonica). The white morph of I. glandulifera exhibited a
similar trend although its values were at the threshold of
discrimination (0,12, 0.10, 0.14) so that it is unclear whether it
can be distinguished from Group 1 species in color terms. On the
contrary, the highly salient violet and pink morphs of I.
glandulifera are clearly distinguishable from Group 1 species in
color terms (Ig V: 0.31, 0.29, 0.34; Ig P: 0.20, 0.18, 0.23), thus
showing that the color-display strategies adopted by the four
invasive species are not homogeneous. While the colors of three
of them (B. davidii,R. japonica and S. japonica)favoured
generalization with respect to surrounding native species, the
highly salient I. glandulifera pink and violet morphs presented
differentiable color displays.
4 Discussion
4.1 Visual signals of owers as seen
through the bee eye
Communication between plants and pollinators is essential for
an effective pollination process (Proctor and Yeo, 1972;Barth, 1985).
Throughout evolution, owering plants have developed oral
features aimed at pollinators to advertize feeding resources such as
pollen and nectar. In exchange, ower-constant pollinators such as
bees transport pollen grains to other owers of the same species, thus
enabling ower fertilization. In this partnership, visual signals play a
fundamental role to guide pollinators to owers from the distance
(Kevan, 1978;Chittka and Menzel, 1992). Behavioral and
physiological studies on honey bee vision, an insect with a model
status for the analysis of perceptual phenomena (Giurfa and Menzel,
1997), have revealed that besides the fundamental role of color (i.e.
dominant wavelength) displayed by owers for advertisement, other
parameter such as the achromatic contrast (the contrast of the visual
target against the background evaluated through the L-receptor
channel) and the spectral purity (the amount of a single
wavelength component within a target reection) are also used for
visual orientation at different ranges. The achromatic contrast
provided by a visual target enables farthest detection, i.e. when the
targets subtend small visual angles between 5 and ca. 15° (Giurfa
et al., 1996b;Giurfa et al., 1997) so that highest achromatic contrasts
provide more visibility at the distance. At closer distances, i.e. at
larger visual angles subtended by the targets, color (wavelength)
information determines the choice of bees based on innate
preferences in the rst ights (Giurfa et al., 1995) and then on
experience through the association of color and food reward
(Menzel, 1985). In addition, spectral purity, which is the equivalent
of color saturation, also guides the bees at even closer ranges, during
the close-up recognition process. In this situation bees tend to follow
the increasing gradient of spectral purity from the petal periphery to
A
B
C
FIGURE 5
(A) Chromatic contrast (hexagon Units, HU), (B) Achromatic contrast
(L-receptor contrast with respect to the background) and (C)
Spectral purity (hexagon Units, HU) of Group1 (G.1), Group 2 (G.2)
and the invasive species (Inv.) analyzed according to the sampling
zones, core, inner and outer edges and surrounding zone. Bd,
Buddleja davidii; Sj, Spiraea japonica; Rj, Reynoutria japonica; Ig,
Impatiens glandulifera; IgV, IgW and IgP, violet, white and pink
morphs of Ig. Number in parentheses indicate sample sizes.
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org10
the center of the corolla and the enhanced spectral purity of nectar
guides (Lunau, 1990;Lunau, 1992;Rohde et al., 2013), thus providing
a direct orientation to the hidden location of reward within a ower.
Thus, given the importance of these visual signals for ower
detection and recognition by pollinators, it is relevant to study to
what extent the success of invasive plants relies on them in the
competition with native species for pollinators using insect vision as
an interpretational framework. In this study, we analyzed the color
signals of four successful invasive species in the French Pyrenees
and compared them with those of surrounding native species. Our
analyses were performed from the perspective of honey bee vision
in order to understand the strategies deployed by invasive plants to
compete for pollinators and ensure a higher reproductive success.
Clearly, honey bees are not the only pollinators of these plants, but
they are among the major ones, and other frequent visitors such as
bumble bees have a very similar set of color photoreceptors and a
similar color vision (Peitsch et al., 1992).
Importantly, in this analysis we focused on chromatic and
achromatic cues knowing that ower recognition may rely on
further visual cues such as shape or symmetry (Lehrer et al.,
1995;Giurfa et al., 1996a;Dafni and Kevan, 1997) and on non-
visual cues such as scent (Dobson and Bergstrom, 2000;Friberg
et al., 2014)orower texture (Kevan and Lane, 1985), among
others. Yet, in the case of bees, scent and spatial details contained in
shape, as well as texture, operate in shorter, close-up ranges when
compared with color cues, i.e. when the approach decision has been
already made, as shown by studies by Karl von Frisch (1967). Thus,
while these cues may divert a bee from inspecting an erroneous
target once it has been chosen, the primary decision of approaching
and choosing such a target is driven by cues such as the achromatic
contrast and the chromatic contrast of a oral target, which were
analyzed in our study.
4.2 The color similarity strategy: the case
of B. davidii, R. japonica and S. japonica
The invasive species B. davidii, R. japonica and S. japonica
shared a common chromatic identity with most native plant species,
which exhibited blue and blue-purple owers to the human eye
(UV-blue, blue and blue-green to the bee eye; Group 1). These three
invasive species clustered with Group 1 of native species (Figure 3),
thus highlighting commonalities in terms of color properties. They
were clearly different from other native species, which appeared
yellow to the human eye (blue-green and green to the bee eye;
Group 2). Interestingly, perceptual similarity correlated with
adjacency. Group 1 species, which were similar to B. davidii, R.
japonica and S. japonica, were more common in central sampling
zones that constituted the immediate surrounding of an invasive
species spot. On the contrary, Group 2 species, which differed from
the three invasive species, were more frequent at the outermost
zones of the sampling areas. Thus, the three invasive species shared
similarcolorswiththenativespeciesthatconstitutedtheir
immediate surroundings and differed chromatically from distant
native species. This suggests that invasive plants may prot from the
established communication between pollinators and native plants to
succeed in the invaded area, as reported for the invasive Acacia
saligna in South Africa (Gibson et al., 2012). By adopting similar
colors as their surrounding native plants, B. davidii, R. japonica and
S. japonica may benet from color generalization and thus attract
pollinators from the distance. This hypothesis was further
conrmed by the analysis of chromatic and achromatic contrasts,
and spectral purity. These variables tended to be similar to those of
native species (Table 1), thus indicating that the three invasive
species were not perceptually salient among their surrounding
native competitors. The analysis of color similarity, which
provides a direct assessment of the beescapacity to discriminate
between different color stimuli, showed that B. davidii, R. japonica
and S. japonica were below the discrimination threshold when
compared to the mean locus of Group 1 species (Figure 6). This
results thus conrms that the color of these three invasive species
could be confused or generalized with respect to that of the native
species immediately surrounding them, a strategy that may increase
their visitation rate and fertilization but also those of surrounding
native species as the clustering of owers with the same colors in the
same area could attract more pollinators, a phenomenon termed
the magnet effect(Moeller, 2004;Peter and Johnson, 2008;
Cuadra-Valdes et al., 2021).
Although the magnet effect can be advantageous for both the
invasive and the native species depending on their fertility success,
it could lead to more heterospecic (interspecic) pollen deposition,
which could represent a waste of pollen grains (Morales and
Traveset, 2008). This scenario could be detrimental for less
frequent species as they could lose more pollen via the deposit on
FIGURE 6
Color similarity (color distance in hexagon units, HU) between the
invasive species and Group 1 and Group 2 of native species. The
dashed line at 0.1 HU indicates the color discrimination threshold
reported for bees trained under absolute conditioning (Dyer and
Chittka, 2004), which corresponds to the ecological scenario of a
color-constant foraging bee. All invasive species differed in color
from the native species of Group 2. Three invasive species, Buddleia
davidii,Reynoutria japonica and Spiraea japonica, were
undistinguishable from the surrounding species of Group 1 in terms
of color. The white morph of Impatiens glandulifera was separated
from native species of Group 1 by a distance that was at the
threshold of discrimination. The two other morphs of I. glandulifera
(pink and violet) were clearly distinguishable from native species of
Group 1.
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org11
the wrong owers. Thus, less abundant native species could suffer
fromthepresenceofinvasivespeciesinthearea,giventhe
abundance of the latter in the Pyrenees region.
4.3 The color salience strategy: the case of
the pink and violet morphs of
I. glandulifera
One of the invasive species, Impatiens glandulifera, presented
oral color polymorphism, with white, pink and violet morphs,
which appeared uv-blue/blue to the bee eye (Figure 2). The pink and
the violet morphs did not cluster with the native species of Group 1
and even less with the native species of Group 2 (Figure 3). They
thus differed signicantly from all native species in their visual
properties. For instance, they had signicantly higher achromatic
contrasts against the background when compared to surrounding
native species of Group 1 (Table 2), which means that they were
more detectable from the distance, at smaller visual angles, in terms
of this achromatic variable, even before bees could perceive the
color of the targets they were aiming at (Giurfa et al., 1996b;Giurfa
et al., 1997). They also had signicantly higher chromatic contrast
and spectral purity than surrounding native species of Group 1
(Table 1), which means that their colors were more salient against
the green background and their higher spectral purity rendered
them more attractive for pollinators (Lunau, 1990;Rohde et al.,
2013). Besides, their colors were dissimilar from those of native
species of Group 1 and 2 (Figure 6), thus bestowing a unique, highly
detectable and salient identity.
These characteristics may confer these I. glandulifera morphs
with exceptional advantages in the context of bee pollination
activities, which are governed by associative learning. In fact,
ower constancy, the essential characteristic of bee foraging
(Grant, 1950;Waser, 1986;Chittka et al., 1999) relies on the bees
capacity to learn and memorize ower features such as the ones
evaluated in our work based on their association with food reward
(nectar or pollen) (Giurfa, 2007). Analyses of foraging activities
using a Pavlovian learning framework have led to successful and
valuable prediction of bee foraging activities (Greggers and Menzel,
1993;Montague et al., 1995). From these perspective, a basic tenet
of Pavlovian associative learning refers to the salience of the stimuli
to be learned and to the intensity of the reward delivered during
learning trials (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972): salient stimuli increase
the learning rate, leading to a faster reaching of a learning plateau.
Similarly, better rewards facilitate learning. Thus, the pink and the
violet morphs of I. glandulifera could eventually outcompete their
surrounding native owers via highly attractive and salient visual
cues, which could be better learned than those of their native
competitors, ensuring thereby efcient ower constancy.
Additionally, I. glandulifera owers have a particularly rich
nectar, which is more rewarding than that of any known native
plant in central Europe (Chittka and Schurkens, 2001), thus
fullling the reward-intensity criterion required for improved
associative learning.
In addition to nectar, pollen may also act as a reinforcing
resource for bee pollinators (Muth et al., 2016). Quantitative
information for pollen abundance and quality is not available to
the best of our knowledge for the invasive species considered in our
work. Yet, invasive pollen transport by Hymenoptera (honey bees
and bumblebees) in eld plots in which I. glandulifera coexisted
with native species was signicantly high as bees were found to
carry more pollen from I. glandulifera than from native species
(Lopezaraiza-Mikel et al., 2007). Thus, this invasive species may
reinforce pollinator visits not only via particularly rich nectar but
also via pollen.
Based on these features, the pink and violet morphs of I.
glandulifera are, in principle, well equipped to tempt bee
pollinators away from native owers, potentially reducing thereby
the tness of native ora. Indeed, studies on the effects of both
proximity and abundance of I. glandulifera on the reproductive
success of native plant species showed that abundance of the
invasive species led both honey bees and bumble bees to visit
more often the invasive species to the detriment of the native
ones (Cawoy et al., 2012). These arguments should, nevertheless be
considered with caution in the absence of tness studies focusing on
the native and the invasive plants in the study community and
because visual signals are not the only ones predicting the
reproductive success of insect-pollinated plants.
4.4 The case of the white morph of
I. glandulifera
The white morph of I. glandulifera represents an interesting
case in visual terms as it is neither well detectable against the
background (Table 2) nor well distinguishable from its surrounding
native plants as the color distance separating them is at the
threshold of discrimination (Figure 6). The owers of this morph
presented signicantly lower achromatic contrast and chromatic
contrast against the background and lower spectral purity
(Figure 5). Its owers appear white to human eyes but not to bees
as they do not reect evenly along all the visible spectrum of bees, as
shown in the inset of Figure 2A (Kevan et al., 1996); given the lack
of reection in the UV range, the owers of this morph appear blue-
green to bees. Less is known about these white morphs in terms of
their natural pollinators. Also, reports on their relative abundance
in different regions are scarce; however, when color morphs of I.
glandulifera were quantied, white morphs co-occurred with the
other morphs, yet being clearly less abundant (Valentine, 1971).
Given their visual characteristics, it is tempting to suggest that white
owers of I. glandulifera rely on other, non-visual cues (e.g. scent),
to attract pollinators. The lower chromatic distance to surrounding
owers (Figure 6) may promote approaches to the white owers by
pollinators foraging on surrounding native species, which would
give them the opportunity to sense these cues and associate them
with the rich nectar present in the white owers. As our analyses
revolved around honey bee vision, it could be possible that the main
recipients of the white-morph signals are not bees but other animals
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org12
endowed with a different visual system. The nature of non-visual
advertisement in the white morph of I. glandulifera remains to be
determined and constitutes a fascinating topic for future research.
5 Conclusions
Plant invasion relies not only on the competition for space,
nutrients and sunlight with native species but also on the
competition for local pollinators to enhance reproductive success
and spread. Thus, a fundamental component for understanding the
success of invasive plants is to evaluate their characteristics from the
perspective of the sensory and cognitive abilities of pollinators,
which are the main addressees of their signals. We adopted this
perspective to analyze the case of four successful invasive plants in
the French Pyrenees and evaluated their color signals using the
extensive knowledge gathered on honey bee vision. By recording the
spectral signals of these species as well as those of native species in
the same areas in which the invasive plants are located, we could
estimate the achromatic and chromatic salience of the invasive
species and their color similarity to surrounding native species. In
this way, we were able to distinguish the color advertising strategies
employed by the invasive species and evaluate their contribution to
their invasive success.
We showed that the four invasive species differ in their color
advertising strategy. Three species (R. japonica, S. japonica and B.
davidii) were generally inconspicuous against the background in
achromatic and chromatic terms and could be hardly discriminated
in terms of color from their immedialy surrounding native plants.
These characteristics may promote generalization and potentially
attract visits from a ower constant pollinator foraging on a similar
native species. The remaining species, I. glandulifera, presented three
morphs with different characteristics. The pink and the violet morph
were highly salient in chromatic and achromatic terms against the
background and were very different from their surrounding native
species. These features provide a distinctive identity, which may
facilitate their detection and learning in association with the rich
nectar they provide, thus potentially endowing the plants with
signicant advantages in the competition for pollinators. The white
morph, on the contrary, did not present salient visual features, thus
raising the double question of the sensory channels it may use to
advertise its presence and of the natural addressees of its signals.
Our study focused on honey bees as a main pollinator of the
four invasive oral species considered in our work in the Pyrenees
landscape. Yet, in the same environment, many other insect species
visit the invasive species analyzed, thus raising the question of the
generality of our ndings with respect to the perceptual capacities of
these alternative pollinators. Answering this question is difcult as
for some insect species that visit these invasive ower species,
photoreceptor types may not have been characterized by means
of electrophysiological recordings, thus precluding perceptual
modeling analyses and building of color spaces for which this
information is mandatory. In the case of other hymenopterans
such as bumble bees, which are also regular visitors of the four
invasive species considered, the spectral sensitivity curves of their
three photoreceptor types show high similarity with that of honey
bees (Peitsch et al., 1992). Modeling analyses on the optimality of
spectral sensitivity curves in Hymenoptera showed that the optimal
color receptors in terms of their capacity to nely code ower colors
invariably display peak sensitivities at wavelengths of 330, 430 and
530 nm (Chittka and Menzel, 1992). These values correspond to
those of l
max
found both in honey bees and bumble bees as well as
in many other hymenopterans for which photoreceptor sensitivities
have been characterized by means of electrophysiological methods.
Thus, the conclusions reported in our work may not differ
substantially in the case of bumble bees and other trichromatic
hymenopterans with similar photoreceptor sensitivities.
While the unveiling of the visual strategies used by invasive
plant species enlightens some crucial aspects of their success, it is,
however, obvious that such success does not rely exclusively on
visual cues but depends on multiple sensory dimensions,
physiological adaptations and reproductive specicities, among
others. It is thus important to stress that other cues [e.g., odors
(Suchet et al., 2011), taste (Bestea et al., 2021)] may be as relevant as
the visual cues analyzed in our study for the process of ower
attraction and recognition and that their analysis should be based
on the perceptual dimensions of the addressees. Overall, by
adopting the perspective of the signal receiver, studies on
biological invasion processes may uncover unknown aspects of
the biology of invasive plants and contribute to the development
of conservation strategies for native plants.
Data availability statement
The datasets presented in this study can be found in online
repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession
number(s) can be found below: https://github.com/martindessart/
Invasive_plants_through_bee-eye_Pyrenees.
Author contributions
MD: Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing review & editing.
JA: Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing review & editing. ET:
Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing review & editing. SG:
Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing
review & editing. MG: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition,
Supervision, Writing original draft.
Funding
The author(s) declare that nancial support was received for the
research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Acknowledgments
Funding was provided by the Région Occitanie - Appel à
projets Recherche et Société(s) 2019and by the Maison des
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org13
Sciences de l'Homme et de la Société de Toulouse (MSH-T) APEX
2020. This work is endorsed by the CNRS/INEE Zone Atelier
Pyrénées Garonne (ZA PYGAR). The Zones Ateliers network
(RZA) is recognized by ALLENVI as an ESFRI eLTER (European
Long-Term Ecological Research) Integrated European Long-Term
Ecosystem, Critical Zone & Socio-Ecological System Research
Infrastructure. We also thank the CNRS, the University Jean
Jaures Toulouse II and the University Paul Sabatier Toulouse III
for support. M. Giurfa thanks in addition the Institut Universitaire
de France for continuous support.
Conict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the
absence of any commercial or nancial relationships that could be
construed as a potential conict of interest.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board
member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no
impact on the peer review process and the nal decision.
Publishers note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the
authors and do not necessarily represent those of their afliated
organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the
reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or
claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or
endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online
at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204/
full#supplementary-material
SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE 1
Relative abundance of invasive owers. Patches labelled from A to L represent
the 12 sampling sites. Core,inner edge,outer edgeand surroundingrefer to
areas of each patch for sampling (see Figure 1J, main text). Numbers indicate the
percentage of relative abundance measured in the eld. See main text for details.
SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE 2
Flower and leave reectance spectra included in our analyses. Reectance
spectra were obtained from 50 native species (n = 333 spectra; 1 spectrum
corresponding to 1 ower part) and from the 4 invasive species mentioned
above (n = 102 spectra), all collected in the eld (left column, measured in the
eld). The FRED database (http://www.reectance.co.uk/) allowed us to
include 16 additional native species (n = 34 spectra) to complement our
measurements (middle column, measured in the eld). These species were
present in the eld and were counted for abundancy measurements but were
inaccessible for reectance measurements. The same database was used to
include complementary measurements for 32 native species (n = 74 spectra)
that were also sampled in the eld. Overall, our analyses included 543 spectra
(435 from theeld and 108 from the FRED data) from70 plant species, including
the 4 invasive species (bottom of left column), which were the focus of our
study, and 66 native species (18 exclusively from the eld, highlighted in green,
16 exclusively from the FRED data base, highlighted in yellow) and 32 both from
the eld and the FREDdata base; not highlighted). The spectralreectance from
leaves of 15 species (n = 25 spectra) was also measured to characterize the
green foliage background (right column, leave reectance spectra).
SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE 3
Chromatic contrast, achromatic contrast and spectral purity dataset. Group
refers to the three categories used for analyses: Invasive, which indicates an
invasive species, Group 1and Group 2, which refer to the two groups of
invasive plants dened through clustering analysis (see Figure 3, main text).
The three morphs of I. glandulifera (P = pink, V = violet and W = white) are
indicated in separate lines.
References
Aguiar, J. M. R. B. V., Giurfa, M., and Sazima, M. (2020). A cognitive analysis of
deceptive pollination: associative mechanisms underlying pollinatorschoices in non-
rewarding colour polymorphic scenarios. Sci. Rep. 10, 9476. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-
66356-4
Albrecht, M., Padron, B., Bartomeus, I., and Traveset, A. (2014). Consequences of
plant invasions on compartmentalization and species' roles in plant-pollinator
networks. Proc. Biol. Sci. 281, 20140773. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.0773
Arnold, S. E., Faruq, S., Savolainen, V., McOwan, P. W., and Chittka, L. (2010).
FReD: the oral reectance databasea web portal for analyses of ower colour. PLoS
One 5, e14287. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014287
Arnold, S. E. J., Savolainen, V., and Chittka, L. (2009). Flower colours along an alpine
altitude gradient, seen through the eyes of y and bee pollinators. Arthropod-Plant
Interact. 3, 2743. doi: 10.1007/s11829-009-9056-9
Avarguès-Weber, A., Deisig, N., and Giurfa, M. (2011). Visual cognition in social
insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol 56, 423443. doi: 10.1146/annurev-ento-120709-
144855
Avarguès-Weber, A., and Giurfa, M. (2014). Cognitive components of color vision in
honey bees: how conditioning variables modulate color learning and discrimination. J.
Comp. Physiol. A - Neuroethology Sensory Neural Behav. Phys iol. 200, 449461.
doi: 10.1007/s00359-014-0909-z
Avarguès-Weber, A., Mota, T., and Giurfa, M. (2012). New vistas on honey bee
vision. Apidologie 43, 244268. doi: 10.1007/s13592-012-0124-2
Backhaus, W. (1991). Color opponent coding in the visual system of the honeybee.
Vision Res. 31, 13811397. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(91)90059-E
Barth, F. G. (1985). Insects and owers: the biology of a partnership (Princeton, USA:
Princeton Univ Press).
Bartomeus, I., Vila, M., and Santamaria, L. (2008). Contrasting effects of invasive
plants in plant-pollinator networks. Oecologia 155, 761770. doi: 10.1007/s00442-007-
0946-1
Barney, J. N., Tekiela, D. R., Dollete, E. S., and Tomasek, B. J. (2013). What is the
realimpact of invasive plant species? Front. Ecol. Environ. 11 (6), 322-329.
doi: 10.1098/rsob.120120
Bestea, L., Rejaud, A., Sandoz, J.-C., Carcaud, J., Giurfa, M., and de Brito Sanchez, M.
G. (2021). Peripheral taste detection in honey bees: What do taste receptors respond to?
Eur. J. Neurosci. 54, 441744444. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15265
Bjerknes, A.-L., Totland, Ø., Hegland, S. J., and Nielsen, A. (2007). Do alien plant
invasions really affect pollination success in native plant species? Biol. Conserv. 138, 1
12. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2007.04.015
Burns, J. H., Bennett, J. M., Li, J., Xia, J., Arceo-Gomez, G., Burd, M., et al. (2019).
Plant traits moderate pollen limitation of introduced and native plants: a phylogenetic
meta-analysis of global scale. New Phytol. 223, 20632075. doi: 10.1111/nph.15935
Cawoy, V., Jonard, M., Mayer, C., and Jacquemart, A.-L. (2012). Do abundance and
proximity of the alien Impatiens glandulifera affect pollination and reproductive success
of two sympatric co-owering native species? J. Pollination Ecol. 10, 130139.
doi: 10.26786/1920-7603(2012)16
Charles, H., and Dukes, J. S. (2007). Biological Invasions Ecological Studies (ed
Wolfgang Nentwig) Vol. 13 (Berlin Heidelberg: Springer), 217237.
Charrad, M., Ghazzali, N., Boiteau, V., and Niknafs, A. (2014). Nbclust: an R package
for determining the relevant number of clusters in a data set. J. Stat. Softw 61, 136.
doi: 10.18637/jss.v061.i06
Chittka, L. (1992). The colour hexagon: a chromaticity diagram based on
photoreceptor excitations as a generalized representation of colour opponency. J.
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org14
Comp. Physiol. A - Neuroethology Sensory Neural Behav. Phys iol. 170, 533543.
doi: 10.1007/BF00199331
Chittka, L., and Menzel, R. (1992). The evolutionary adaptation of ower colors and
the insect pollinators color-vision. J. Comp. Physiol. A - Neuroethology Sensory Neural
Behav. Physiol. 171, 171181. doi: 10.1007/BF00188925
Chittka, L., and Schurkens, S. (2001). Successful invasion of a oral market. Nature
411, 653. doi: 10.1038/35079676
Chittka, L., Thomson, J. D., and Waser, N. M. (1999). Flower constancy, insect
psychology, and plant evo lution. Naturwissenschaften 86, 361377. doi: 10.1007/
s001140050636
Claudel, M., Lerigoleur, E., Brun, C., and Guillerme, S. (2022). Geohistorical dataset
of ten plant species introduced into Occitania (France). Biodiversity Data J. 10, e76283.
doi: 10.3897/BDJ.10.e76283
Cuadra-Valdes, J., Vizentin-Bugoni, J., and Fonturbel, F. E. (2021). An exotic magnet
plant alters pollinator abundance and behavior: a eld test with a native mistletoe. Biol.
Invasions 23, 25152525. doi: 10.1007/s10530-021-02519-2
Dafni, A., and Kevan, P. G. (1997). Flower size and shape: Implications in
pollination. Israel J. Plant Sci. 45, 201212. doi: 10.1080/07929978.1997.10676684
Dobson, H. E. M., and Bergstrom, G. (2000). The ecology and evolution of pollen
odors. Plant Systematics Evol. 222, 6387. doi: 10.1007/BF00984096
Dyer, A. G., and Chittka, L. (2004). Fine colour discrimination requires differential
conditioning in bumblebees. Naturwissenschaften 91, 224227. doi: 10.1007/s00114-
004-0508-x
Ellis, W. N., and Ellis-Adam, A. C. (1993). To make a meadow it takes a clover and a
bee: the entomophilous ora of N.W. Europe and its insects. Bijdragen tot Dierkunde
63, 193220. doi: 10.1163/26660644-06304001
Friberg, M., Schwind, C., Roark, L. C., Raguso, R. A., and Thompson, J. N. (2014).
Floral scent contributes to interaction specicity in coevolving plants and their insect
pollinators. J. Chem. Ecol. 40, 955965. doi: 10.1007/s10886-014-0497-y
Ghazoul, J. (2004). Alien abduction: disruption of native plant-pollinator
interactions by invasive species. Biotropica 36, 156164. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-
7429.2004.tb00308.x
Gibson, M. R., Richardson, D. M., Pauw, A., and Gibson, D. (2012). Can oral traits
predict an invasive plant's impact on native plant-pollinator communities? J. Ecol. 100,
12161223. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2745.2012.02004.x
Gioria, M., and Osborne, B. A. (2014). Resource competition in plant
invasions: emerging patterns and research needs. Front. Plant Sci. 5. doi: 10.3389/
fpls.2014.00501
Giurfa, M. (2007). Behavioral and neural analysis of associative learning in the
honeybee: a taste from the magic well. J. Comp. Physiol. A - Neuroethology Sensory
Neural Behav. Physiol. 193, 801824. doi: 10.1007/s00359-007-0235-9
Giurfa, M., Eichmann, B., and Menzel, R. (1996a). Symmetry perception in an insect.
Nature 382, 458461. doi: 10.1038/382458a0
Giurfa, M., and Lehrer, M. (2001). Cognitive Ecology of Pollination Vol. 4. Eds. L.
Chittka and J. Thomson (Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press), 6182.
Giurfa, M., and Menzel, R. (1997). Insect visual perception: complex abilities of
simple nervous systems. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7, 505513. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388
(97)80030-X
Giurfa, M., Nuñez, J., Chittka, L., and Menzel, R. (1995). Color preferences of ower-
naive honeybees. J. Comp. Physiol. A - Sensory Neural Behav. Physiol. 177, 247259.
doi: 10.1007/BF00192415
Giurfa, M., and Vorobyev, M. (1998). The angular range of achromatic target
detection by honey bees. J. Comp. Physiol. A - Sensory Neural Behav. Physiol. 183,
101110. doi: 10.1007/s003590050238
Giurfa, M., Vorobyev, M., Brandt, R., Posner, B., and Menzel, R. (1997).
Discrimination of coloured stimuli by honeybees: Alternative use of achromatic and
chromatic signals. J. Comp. Physiol. A - Sensory Neural Behav. Physiol. 180, 235243.
doi: 10.1007/s003590050044
Giurfa, M., Vorobyev, M., Kevan, P., and Menzel, R. (1996b). Detection of coloured
stimuli by honeybees: minimum visual angles and receptor specic contrasts. J. Comp.
Physiol. A - Neuroethology Sensory Neural Behav. Physiol. 178, 699709. doi: 10.1007/
BF00227381
Goodell, K., and Parker, I. M. (2017). Invasion of a dominant oral resource: effects
on the oral community and pollination of native plants. Ecology 98, 5769.
doi: 10.1002/ecy.1639
Grant, V. (1950). The ower constancy of bees. Bot. Rev. 16, 379398. doi: 10.1007/
BF02869992
Greggers, U., and Menzel, R. (1993). Memory dynamics and foraging strategies of
honeybees. Behav. Ecol. sociobiology 32, 1729. doi: 10.1007/BF00172219
Guillerme, S., Barcet, H., de Munnik, N., Maire, E., and Marais-Sicre, C. (2020).
Evolution of traditional agroforestry landscapes and development of invasive species:
lessons from the Pyrenees (France). Sustainability Sci. 15, 12851299. doi: 10.1007/
s11625-020-00847-1
Hejda, M., Pysek, P., and Jaros
ı
k, V. (2009). Impact of invasive plants on the species
richness, diversity and composition of invaded communities. J. Ecol. 97, 393403.
doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2745.2009.01480.x
Hung, K. J., Kingston, J. M., Albrecht, M., Holway, D. A., and Kohn, J. R. (2018). The
worldwide importance of honey bees as pollinators in natural habitats. Proc. Biol. Sci.
285, 20172140. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2017.2140
IUCN Council. (2000). Guidelines for the prevention of biodiversity loss caused
by alien invasive species,in Prepared by the IUCNSSC Invasive Species Specialist
Group (ISSG) and approved by the 51st Meeting of the IUCN Council.(Gland
Switzerland), 1-21.
Jantzi, H., Marais-Sicre, C., Maire, E., Barcet, H., and Guillerme, S. (2021).
Diachronic mapping of invasive plants using airborne rgb imagery in a Central
Pyrenees landscape (South-West France). Biol. Life Sci. Forum 3, 51. doi: 10.3390/
IECAG2021-10008
Jove, V., Gong, Z., Hol, F. J. H., Zhao, Z., Sorrells, T. R., Carroll, T. S., et al. (2020).
Sensory discrimination of blood and oral nectar by. Aedes aEgypti mosquitoes. Neuron
108, 11631180 e1112. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.09.019
Kassambara, A., and Mundt, F. (2020). Factoextra: extract and visualize the results of
multivariate data analyses. R package version 1.0.7. Available at: https://CRAN.R-
project.org/package=factoextra.
Kevan, P. G. (1978). The pollination of Flowers by Insects. Ed. A. J. Richards (London:
Linnean Soc Sympos Series), 5178.
Kevan, P. G. (1983). Handbook of experimental pollination biology. Eds. C. E. Jones
and R. S. Little (New York, USA: Van Nostrand Reinhold), 330.
Kevan, P. G., and Lane, M. A. (1985). Flower petal microtexture is a tactile cue for
bees. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 82, 47504752. doi: 10.1073/pnas.82.14.4750
Kevan, P., Giurfa, M., and Chittka, L. (1996). Why are there so many and so few
white owers? Trends Plant Sci. 1, 252. doi: 10.1016/1360-1385(96)20008-1
Khalifa, S. A. M., Elshaey, E. H., Shetaia, A. A., Abd El-Wahed, A. A., Algethami, A.
F., Musharraf, S. G., et al. (2021). Overview of bee pollination and its economic value
for crop production. Insects 12, 688. doi: 10.3390/insects12080688
Kien, J., and Menzel, R. (1977). Chromatic properties of interneurons in the
optic lobes of the bee. II. Narrow band and colour opponent neurons. J. Comp.
Physiol. A - Neuroethology Sensory Neural Behav. Physiol. 113, 3553. doi: 10.1007/
BF00610452
Kovacs-Hostyanszki, A., Szigeti, V., Miholcsa, Z., Sandor, D., Soltesz, Z., Török, E.,
et al. (2022). Threats and benets of invasive alien plant species on pollinators. Basic
Appl. Ecol. 64, 89102. doi: 10.1016/j.baae.2022.07.003
Langmaier, M., and Lapin, K. (2020). A systematic review of the impact of invasive
alien plants on forest regeneration in european temperate forests. Front. Plant Sci. 11.
doi: 10.3389/fpls.2020.524969
Lehrer, M., Horridge, G. A., Zhang, S. W., and Gadagkar, R. (1995). Shape vision in
bees - Innate preference for ower-like patterns. Philos. T R Soc. B 347, 123137.
doi: 10.1098/rstb.1995.0017
Lopezaraiza-Mikel,M.E.,Hayes,R.B.,Whalley,M.R.,andMemmott,J.
(2007). The impact of an alien plant on a native plant-pollinator network:
an experimental approach. Ecol. Lett. 10, 539550. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-
0248.2007.01055.x
Lunau, K. (1990). Color saturation triggers innate reactions to ower signals - Flower
Dummy Experiments with Bumblebees. J. Comp. Physiol. A - Sensory Neural Behav.
Physiol. 166, 827834. doi: 10.1007/BF00187329
Lunau, K. (1992). A new interpretation of ower guide coloration - Absorption of
ultraviolet-light enhances color saturation. Plant Systematics Evol. 183 (1-2), 51-65.
doi: 10.1007/Bf00937735
Lunau, K., Fieselmann, G., Heusche n, B., and van de Loo, A. (2006). Visual
targeting of components of oral colour patterns in ower-naive bumblebees
(Bombus terrestris; Apidae). Naturwissenschaften 93, 325328. doi: 10.1007/s00114-
006-0105-2
Lunau, K., Wacht, S., and Chittka, L. (1996). Colour choices of naive bumble bees
and their implications for colour perception. J. Comp. Physiol. A - Sensory Neural
Behav. Physiol. 178, 477489. doi: 10.1007/BF00190178
Maia, R., Eliason, C. M., Bitton, P.-P., Doucet, S. M., Shawkey, M.D., and Tatem, A.
(2013). pavo: an R package for the analysis, visualization and organization of spectral
data. Behav. Ecol. 29, 649-659. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.12069
Maia, R., and White, T. E. (2018). Comparing colors using visual models. Behav. Ecol.
29, 649659. doi: 10.1093/beheco/ary017
Menzel, R. (1985). Experimental behavioral ecology and sociobiology.Eds.B.
Holldobler and M. Lindauer (Stuttgart, Germany: Gustav Fischer Verlag), 5574.
Menzel, R., and Blakers, M. (1976). Colour receptors in the bee eye Morphology
and spectral sensitivity. J. Comp. Physiol. A - Neuroethology Sensory Neural Behav.
Physiol. 108, 1113. doi: 10.1007/BF00625437
Menzel, R., and Shmida, A. (1993). The ecology of ower colors and the natural
color-vision of insect pollinators - the Israeli ora as a study case. Biol. Rev. 68, 81120.
doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1993.tb00732.x
Moeller, D. A. (2004). Facilitative interactions among plants via shared pollinators.
Ecology 85, 32893301. doi: 10.1890/03-0810
Montague, P. R., Dayan, P., Person, C., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1995). Bee foraging in
uncertain environments using predictive hebbian learning. Nature 377, 725728.
doi: 10.1038/377725a0
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org15
Morales, C. L., and Traveset, A. (2008). Interspecic pollen transfer: Magnitude,
prevalence and consequences for plant tness. Crit. Rev. Plant Sci. 27, 221238.
doi: 10.1080/07352680802205631
Mota, T., Gronenberg, W., Giurfa, M., and Sandoz, J. C. (2013). Chromatic
processing in the anterior optic tubercle of the honey bee brain. J. Neurosci. 33, 4
16. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1412-12.2013
Muth, F., Papaj, D. R., and Leonard, A. S. (2016). Bees remember owers for more
than one reason: pollen mediates associative learning. Anim. Behav. 111, 93100.
doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.09.029
Najberek, K., Solarz, W., Wysoczanski, W., Węgrzyn, E., and Olejniczak, P. (2023).
Flowers of Impatiens glandulifera as hubs for both pollinators and pathogens. NeoBiota
87, 126. doi: 10.3897/neobiota.87.102576
Parker, I. M., Simberloff, D., Lonsdale, W. M., Goodell, K., Wonham, M., Kareiva, P.
M., et al. (1999). Impact: Toward a framework for understanding the ecological effects
of invaders. Biol. Invasions 1, 319. doi: 10.1023/A:1010034312781
Parra-Tabla, V., and Arceo-Gomez, G. (2021). Impacts of plant invasions in native
plant-pollinator networks. New Phytol. 230, 21172128. doi: 10.1111/nph.17339
Peitsch, D., Fietz, A., Hertel, H., Souza, J., Ventura, D. F., and Menzel, R. (1992). The
spectral input systems of hymenopteran insects and their receptor-based colour vision.
J. Comp. Physiol. A - Neuroethology Sensory Neural Behav. Physiol. 170, 2340.
doi: 10.1007/Bf00190398
Peter, C. I., and Johnson, S. D. (2008). Mimics and magnets: The importance of color
and ecological facilitation in oral deception. Ecology 89, 15831595. doi: 10.1890/07-
1098.1
Proctor, M. Y., and Yeo, P. (1972). The pollination of owers (New York, USA:
Taplinger).
Pysek, P., Jaros
ı
k, V., Hulme, P. E., Pergl, J., Hejda, M., Schaffner, U., et al. (2012). A
global assessment of invasive plant impacts on resident species, communities and
ecosystems: the interaction of impact measures, invading species' traits and
environment. Global Change Biol. 18, 17251737. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2486.2011.02636.x
Reisenman, C. E., and Giurfa, M. (2008). Chromatic and achromatic stimulus
discrimination of long wavelength (red) visual stimuli by the honeybee. Apis
mellifera. Arthropod-Plant Interact. 2, 137146. doi: 10.1007/s11829-008-9041-8
Rescorla, R. A., and Wagner, A. R. (1972). Classical conditioning II: Current research
and theory. Eds. A. H. Black and W. F. Prokasy (New York, USA: Appleton-Century-
Crofts), 6499.
Rhone,F.(2015).Honeybee across the elds : how do honeybees (Apis mellifera L.)
interact with agricultural landscapes (Gers French department, 32)? : the role of the woody
landscape structures in supplying trophic resources and their repercussions on colonieslife
history traits PhD thesis (Toulouse, France: Universitede Toulouse Jean Jaurès).
Richardson, D. M., Allsopp, N., D'Antonio, C. M., Milton, S. J., and RejmA
Nek, M.
(2007). Plant invasions - the role of mutualisms. Biol. Rev. 75, 6593. doi: 10.1111/
j.1469-185X.1999.tb00041.x
Rohde, K., Papiorek, S., and Lunau, K. (2013). Bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) and
honeybees (Apis mellifera) prefer similar colours of higher spectral purity over trained
colours. J. Comp. Physiol. A - Neuroethology Sensory Neural Behav. Physiol. 199, 197
210. doi: 10.1007/s00359-012-0783-5
Saule, M. (2002). La grande ore illustree des pyrenees. 730 p.
Shrestha, M., Tai, K.-C., Dyer, A. G., Garcia, J. E., Yang, E.-C., Jentsch, A., et al.
(2024). Flower colour and size-signals vary with altitude and resulting climate on the
tropical-subtropical islands of Taiwan. Front. Plant Sci. 15. doi: 10.3389/
fpls.2024.1304849
Skurski, T. C., Rew, L. J., and Maxwell, B. D. (2017). Mechanisms underlying
nonindigenous plant impacts: A review of recent experimental research. Invasive Plant
Sci. Manage. 7, 432444. doi: 10.1614/IPSM-D-13-00099.1
Sooraj, N. P., Jaishanker, R., Athira, K., Sajeev, C. R., Lijimol, D., Saroj, K. V., et al.
(2019). Comparative study on the oral spectral reectance of invasive and non-invasive
plants. Ecol. Inf. 53, 100990. doi: 10.1016/j.ecoinf.2019.100990
Stout, J. C., and Tiedeken, E. J. (2017). Direct interactions between invasive plants
and native pollinators: evidence, impacts and approaches. Funct. Ecol. 31, 3846.
doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12751
Suchet, C., Dormont, L., Schatz, B., Giurfa, M., Simon, V., Raynaud, C., et al. (2011).
Floral scent variation in two Antirrhinum majus subspecies inuences the choice of
naïve bumblebees. Behav. Ecol. sociobiology 65, 10151027. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-
1106-x
Tabacchi, E
., Planty-Tabacchi, A.-M., and Muller, S. (2010). Les invasions
biologiques, une question de natures et de societes(Versailles, France: E
ditions Quæ),
4563.
Traveset, A., and Richardson, D. M. (2006). Biological invasions as disruptors of
plant reproductive mutualisms. Trends Ecol. Evol. 21, 208216. doi: 10.1016/
j.tree.2006.01.006
Valentine, D. H. (1971). Flower-colour polymorphism in Impatiens glandulifera
Royle. Boissiera Mem Botanique Syst. 19, 339343. doi: 10.5169/seals-895481
von Frisch, K. (1967). The dance language and orientation of bees (Cambridge, MA,
USA: Harvard University Press).
Waser, N. M. (1986). Flower constancy: denition, cause and measurement. Am.
Nat. 127, 593603. doi: 10.1086/284507
Weiss, M. R. (1991). Floral color changes as cues for pollinators. Nature 354, 227
229. doi: 10.1038/354227a0
Dessart et al. 10.3389/fpls.2024.1393204
Frontiers in Plant Science frontiersin.org16
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
The diversity of flower colours in nature provides quantifiable evidence for how visitations by colour sensing insect pollinators can drive the evolution of angiosperm visual signalling. Recent research shows that both biotic and abiotic factors may influence flower signalling, and that harsher climate conditions may also promote salient signalling to entice scarcer pollinators to visit. In parallel, a more sophisticated appreciation of the visual task foragers face reveals that bees have a complex visual system that uses achromatic vision when moving fast, whilst colour vision requires slower, more careful inspection of targets. Spectra of 714 native flowering species across Taiwan from sea level to mountainous regions 3,300 m above sea level (a.s.l.) were measured. We modelled how the visual system of key bee pollinators process signals, including flower size. By using phylogenetically informed analyses, we observed that at lower altitudes including foothills and submontane landscapes, there is a significant relationship between colour contrast and achromatic signals. Overall, the frequency of flowers with high colour contrast increases with altitude, whilst flower size decreases. The evidence that flower colour signaling becomes increasingly salient in higher altitude conditions supports that abiotic factors influence pollinator foraging in a way that directly influences how flowering plants need to advertise.
Article
Full-text available
Flower infestation by pathogens may influence pollination effectiveness. At the same time, by sharing infested flowers, pollinators increase transmission of pathogens. In the presented study we identified fungi that colonised flowers of the invasive alien Himalayan balsam Impatiens glandulifera , one of the most nectar rewarding plants in Europe, as well as its pollinators. We determined factors (e.g., plant size, length of flower lower sepal and the width of its entry, air temperature and sun illuminance) that affect pathogen species presence and pollinators numbers. The study was conducted in three regions in Poland differing in time from the I. glandulifera invasion onset. It allowed embedding our results in the context of the evolution of increased competitive ability (EICA) hypothesis. With reference to this hypothesis we tested whether I. glandulifera from the two younger populations are more frequently pollinated than individuals from the old one, which may be a result of the higher infection prevalence in the flowers of individuals from the latter population. Harmful primary pathogens of I. glandulifera (e.g., Botrytis cinerea and Fusarium graminearum ) were identified from its flowers. Although the knowledge of the impact of the recorded pathogen species on the pollinators that transmit them is still limited, these pathogens are known to cause devastating diseases of native plant species and to incur significant economic losses in crops. Therefore, the facilitation of their transmission by I. glandulifera in the invaded communities may pose a serious threat both to native biodiversity and nearby crop production. We did not find support for the EICA hypothesis that flower release from pathogens may increase the pollinator’s activity. Bombus hortorum was the most frequent visitor in the youngest surveyed population, while B. pascuorum was most frequent in the two others. So far the dominance of B. hortorum as a pollinator of I. glandulifera has not been recorded. A possible explanation is that flowers in the youngest population, with significantly wider entries than in the two older ones, were more accessible for this large bumblebee. We suggest that the shifts in flower dimensions may result from the evolutionary processes and/or phenotypic plasticity; however, this suggestion needs to be confirmed in further studies. At the same time, it can be expected that exceptionally frequent visits of B. hortorum in flowers of I. glandulifera in the youngest population may contribute to increasing transmission rate of pathogen species to the new native host plants that are particularly associated with this pollinator.
Article
Full-text available
The original dataset presented here is the result of the first near-exhaustive analysis performed on historical data concerning ten plant species introduced in and around Occitania (south-western France) since 1651. Research was carried out on the following species: Alnus incana , Buddleja davidii , Castanea sativa , Helianthus tuberosus , Impatiens glandulifera , Prunus cerasifera , Prunus laurocerasus , Reynoutria japonica , Robinia pseudoacacia and Spiraea japonica . The data file contains 199 occurrence data exclusively based on historical observations and records made between 1651 and 2004 that were retrieved from 111 of the 640 literary sources consulted. All the records are associated with a year and 61% of them have associated spatial coordinates. Initially, the EI2P-VALEEBEE research project focused on the introduction of these species into Occitania (95 occurrences, 47.7%), but mentions found of introductions beyond this territory - mainly in metropolitan France - are also reported. The creation of this dataset involved five stages: (1) selection of species, (2) consultation of historical sources, (3) recording of occurrences in the dataset, (4) dataset standardisation/enrichment and Darwin core mapping and (5) data publication. Quality controls were conducted at each step. The dataset is available on the platform of the Global Biodiversity Information Facility (GBIF) at https://doi.org/10.15468/3kvaeh. It respects the internationally recognised FAIR Data Principles (Findable, Accessible, Interoperable and Reusable). The dataset will be progressively enriched by new data during the EI2P-VALEEBEE research project and future projects on invasive plant species conducted by the team.
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary: There is a rising demand for food security in the face of threats posed by a growing human population. Bees as an insect play a crucial role in crop pollination alongside other animal pollinators such as bats, birds, beetles, moths, hoverflies, wasps, thrips, and butterflies and other vectors such as wind and water. Bees contribute to the global food supply via pollinating a wide range of crops, including fruits, vegetables, oilseeds, legumes, etc. The economic benefit of bees to food production per year was reported including the cash crops, i.e., coffee, cocoa, almond and soybean, compared to self-pollination. Bee pollination improves the quality and quantity of fruits, nuts, and oils. Bee colonies are faced with many challenges that influence their growth, reproduction , and sustainability, particularly climate change, pesticides, land use, and management strength, so it is important to highlight these factors for the sake of gainful pollination. Citation: Khalifa, S.A.M.; Elshafiey, E.H.; Shetaia, A.A.; El-Wahed, A.A.A.; Algethami, A.F.; Musharraf, S.G.; AlAjmi, M.F.; Zhao, C.; Masry, S.H.D.; Abdel-Daim, M.M.; et al.
Article
Full-text available
Understanding the neural principles governing taste perception in species that bear economic importance or serve as research models for other sensory modalities constitutes a strategic goal. Such is the case of the honey bee (Apis mellifera), which is environmentally and socioeconomically important, given its crucial role as pollinator agent in agricultural landscapes and which has served as a traditional model for visual and olfactory neurosciences and for research on communication, navigation and learning and memory. Here we review the current knowledge on honey bee gustatory receptors to provide an integrative view of peripheral taste detection in this insect, highlighting specificities and commonalities with other insect species. We describe behavioral and electrophysiological responses to several tastant categories and relate these responses, whenever possible, to known molecular receptor mechanisms. Overall, we adopted an evolutionary and comparative perspective to understand the neural principles of honey bee taste, and define key questions that should be answered in future gustatory research centered on this insect.
Article
Full-text available
Exotic species can threaten biodiversity by disrupting ecological interactions among native species. Highly-attractive exotic species can exert a ‘magnet effect’ by attracting native pollinators, which may have either competitive or facilitative effects on co-flowering native plants. However, those effects may be context-dependent. We used a mistletoe-hummingbird pollination system in the Valdivian rainforest (southern Chile) to test whether the exotic tree Eucalyptus globulus (a highly attractive species to pollinators) acts as a magnet species, affecting the co-flowering native mistletoe. We compared hummingbird abundance, visitation rates, and activity patterns between native forest and abandoned E. globulus plantations. We found that hummingbirds were more abundant and visited more flowers at the plantation irrespective of E. globulus flowering. We observed a significant change of pollinator activity at the native habitat during E. globulus flowering, as hummingbirds visited mistletoe flowers more frequently early in the morning at the plantations and in the afternoon at the native forests. Our results showed that E. globulus acts as an exotic magnet species and can alter pollinator abundance and behavior. Our findings demonstrate the importance of considering local- and landscape-scale processes to understand the effects of magnet species on native plants and suggest that magnet species may influence even highly-attractive plants.
Article
Full-text available
The disruption of mutualisms by invasive species has consequences for biodiversity loss and ecosystem function. Although invasive plant effects on the pollination of individual native species has been the subject of much study, their impacts on entire plant–pollinator communities are less understood. Community‐level studies on plant invasion have mainly focused on two fronts: understanding the mechanisms that mediate their integration; and their effects on plant–pollinator network structure. Here we briefly review current knowledge and propose a more unified framework for evaluating invasive species integration and their effects on plant–pollinator communities. We further outline gaps in our understanding and propose ways to advance knowledge in this field. Specifically, modeling approaches have so far yielded important predictions regarding the outcome and drivers of invasive species effects on plant communities. However, experimental studies that test these predictions in the field are lacking. We further emphasize the need to understand the link between invasive plant effects on pollination network structure and their consequences for native plant population dynamics (population growth). Integrating demographic studies with those on pollination networks is thus key in order to achieve a more predictive understanding of pollinator‐mediated effects of invasive species on the persistence of native plant biodiversity.
Article
Full-text available
The term agroforestry covers practices that are promoted to maintain or even enhance biodiversity. However, the relationship between agroforestry and invasive species is rarely addressed, even though the spread of such species is an important issue, not only ecologically but also economically and socially. Over the past few decades, humans have greatly accelerated the process of biological invasions, to such an extent that they are now recognized as the second cause of rapid decline in biodiversity. In France (as in other parts in Europe), abandonment of agricultural land in remote areas is a major problem having socio-economic, landscape and environmental implications. The objective of the research presented here was to find a method for studying the relationship between traditional agroforestry systems and invasive species, despite a lack of available data. We investigated the evolution of a traditional agroforestry system in the Pyrenean foothills, where invasive species are abundant, by implementing two complementary methods: (1) interviews highlighting the local stakeholders’ perception of landscape evolution, invasive species and the challenges they represent for the local traditional agroforestry landscapes, and (2) detailed mapping of an area occupied by invasive plants, by means of very high-resolution spatial technologies using UAV’s and aerial photography. The results show that invasive species have spread in relation with the abandonment of agricultural land, which has also led to “landscape closure” by the encroachment of natural afforestation. They also underline the difficulty of assessing the spread of invasive species. This situation is of major importance in terms of land-use planning, as the various stakeholders have different perceptions of the problem, and it raises questions about the sustainability of practices and territories.
Article
Invasive alien plant species are usually disliked due to their high pressure on native communities. However, their ecological effects on pollinators are complex: some species provide abundant floral resources, boosting the number of pollinators, while they often disrupt plant-pollinator interactions by outcompeting native plants. Our direct knowledge is mainly based on single-species studies, while understanding the mechanism of these complex ecological interactions needs multi-species field-based approaches. It is also imperative to clarify the pros and cons of invasive plants and drivers of invasion from the perspective of pollinators. We conducted a standard protocol-driven regional study in Central and Eastern Europe, comparing 6-7 invaded and non-invaded sites of 12 herbaceous invasive plant species. We sampled floral resources, bees, and hoverflies before and during the flowering of the invasive plants. We analysed the effects of plant invasion at the invasive plant species level and in combined analyses, and tested whether the life span (perennial vs. annual) and flowering time (early-, middle-, and late-flowering) of invasive plants affect the abundance, species richness, diversity and species composition of native plants and pollinators. The combined analyses showed lower abundance and species richness of flowering plants and pollinators before, and higher abundance of both during the flowering of invasive plants in invaded sites. However, invasive plants had significant species-specific effects. Perennial invasive plants had a stronger negative impact on floral resources and pollinators already before their flowering compared to annuals. Flowering time of invasive plants affected the pollinator guilds differently. We suggest that in certain critical time periods of the year, invasive plants might provide the dominant foraging resources for pollinators in an invaded ecosystem. But, they also often cause significant losses in native floral resources over the year. Instead of simple eradication, careful preparation and consideration might be needed during removal of invasive plants.
Article
Blood-feeding mosquitoes survive by feeding on nectar for metabolic energy but require a blood meal to develop eggs. Aedes aegypti females must accurately discriminate blood and nectar because each meal promotes mutually exclusive feeding programs with distinct sensory appendages, meal sizes, digestive tract targets, and metabolic fates. We investigated the syringe-like blood-feeding appendage, the stylet, and discovered that sexually dimorphic stylet neurons taste blood. Using pan-neuronal calcium imaging, we found that blood is detected by four functionally distinct stylet neuron classes, each tuned to specific blood components associated with diverse taste qualities. Stylet neurons are insensitive to nectar-specific sugars and respond to glucose only in the presence of additional blood components. The distinction between blood and nectar is therefore encoded in specialized neurons at the very first level of sensory detection in mosquitoes. This innate ability to recognize blood is the basis of vector-borne disease transmission to millions of people worldwide.