ArticlePDF Available

Determinants of behaviour and their efficacy as targets of behavioural change interventions

Authors:
Nature Reviews Psychology
nature reviews psychology https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-024-00305-0
Review article
Determinants of behaviour
and their efficacy as targets of
behavioural change interventions
Dolores Albarracín 1,2,3,4,5 , Bita Fayaz-Farkhad2 & Javier A. Granados Samayoa1,3
Abstract
Unprecedented social, environmental, political and economic
challenges — such as pandemics and epidemics, environmental
degradation and community violence — require taking stock of
how to promote behaviours that benet individuals and society at
large. In this Review, we synthesize multidisciplinary meta-analyses
of the individual and social-structural determinants of behaviour
(for example, beliefs and norms, respectively) and the ecacy of
behavioural change interventions that target them. We nd that, across
domains, interventions designed to change individual determinants
can be ordered by increasing impact as those targeting knowledge,
general skills, general attitudes, beliefs, emotions, behavioural skills,
behavioural attitudes and habits. Interventions designed to change
social-structural determinants can be ordered by increasing impact
as legal and administrative sanctions; programmes that increase
institutional trustworthiness; interventions to change injunctive
norms; monitors and reminders; descriptive norm interventions;
material incentives; social support provision; and policies that increase
access to a particular behaviour. We nd similar patterns for health and
environmental behavioural change specically. Thus, policymakers
should focus on interventions that enable individuals to circumvent
obstacles to enacting desirable behaviours rather than targeting salient
but ineective determinants of behaviour such as knowledge and
beliefs.
Sections
Introduction
Behavioural determinants
Individual determinants and
interventions
Social-structural determinants
and interventions
Summary and future
directions
1Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA. 2Annenberg School of
Communication, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA. 3Annenberg Public Policy Center, University of
Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA. 4Department of Community and Family Health, School of Nursing, University
of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA. 5Department of Health Care Management, Wharton School of Business,
University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA. e-mail: dalba@upenn.edu
Check for updates
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
an empirical model of behavioural change based on their efficacy to
provide a picture of general principles that can inform intervention
decisions for new or understudied behaviours.
Our Review includes all identified meta-analyses of behaviour
prediction or intervention efficacy across domains (Supplementary
Note1) based on clearly classifiable determinants, targets of change
and behavioural outcomes. However, although interventions designed
to change a particular target are assumed to change that specific tar-
get
16
, they might exert an array of effec ts. For example, an inter vention
that communicates that neighbours use less energy might influence
both descriptive norms and positive attitudes towards conserving
energy
19
. Verifying all possible mechanisms of effects is outside the
scope of this Review.
We concentrated on what targets might be most effective, which
is the first critical question when designing a programme to change
behaviour. For example, deciding whether to instil pro-vaccination
norms, combat conspiracy theories about vaccination or add vac-
cination sites is essential to the public health management of a pan
-
demic. However, implementing interventions once a target of change
is selected brings up a different set of questions that are outside the
scope of this Review. Although we briefly describe what interven-
tions often do, readers should review the primary research literature
to determine what the most successful interventions within a given
target look like. After all, reviewing intervention manuals is critical to
a faithful programme implementation2022.
Behavioural determinants
Individual factors are at the centre of behavioural prediction and change
models such as the reasoned action approach
2025
, the information–
motivation–behavioural-skills model
16,20,23,2629
and social cognitive
theory25,3032. These models collectively suggest that knowledge
(a collection of facts about an object of behaviour, typically held with
certainty even though they might be factually incorrect32), beliefs (prob-
ability judgements about an object in connection with an attribute or an
outcome
32
), general and behavioural attitudes (evaluations of objects
or behaviours, respectively, along a positive–negative dimension
33
),
emotions (visceral feelings associated with an objec t or behaviour33),
general and specific skills (cognitive skills involved in self-control30 or
domain-specific cognitive or motor skills, respectively
30
) and habits
(repeated, automated behaviours that continue even in the absence of
rewards
34
) are important determinants of behaviour and/or potential
targets for behaviour change.
For example, according to the reasoned ac tion approach, beliefs
that performing a behaviour will lead to various outcomes and the
evaluations of those outcomes influence attitudes and subsequent
intentions to execute a behaviour20. According to the information–
motivation–behavioural-skills model, information entails knowledge
about the behaviour in question, motivation comprises attitudes,
norms and intentions, and behavioural skills encompass routines
that facilitate a behaviour and associated feelings of self-efficacy or
perceived behavioural control24,27,30,35. Emotions, habits, general atti-
tudes and general skills are part of the integrative model of behavioural
prediction and change
21
and have been shown to be important for self-
regulation
29
. They are also incorporated as external variables within
the reasoned action approach29.
One problem with existing models of behavioural prediction
and change is a relative neglect of social and structural factors2,36. For
example, although the reasoned action approach posits that social
norms influence intentions, intentions are still an individual factor.
Introduction
During the past 5 years, humanity has been confronted with extraordi
-
nary social, environmental, political and economic challenges, includ-
ing pandemics and epidemics, threats to natural habitats and climate,
and community, state and police violence. The science of behaviour
change can identify efficacious interventions to change behaviours
that might be central to solving these crises. Thus, it is important to
understand the degree to which correcting misinformation, modifying
cultural beliefs, or changing norms or legal sanctions will, for example,
increase vaccination or decrease energy usage.
Previous work has provided taxonomies of the tools available to
change behaviour13. For example, a review and expert judgements were
used to classify behavioural change interventions, determine whether
they were based on behavioural change principles and, then, organ-
ize them into displays that enable practitioners to visualize possible
tools at their disposal
4,5
. However, despite its descriptive value, this
taxonomy is not informative about the relative inter vention efficacy
of different approaches. An intervention based on ‘behavioural change
principles’ does not guarantee success, therefore leaving the question
of efficacy unaddressed.
Other relevant work has produced estimates of specific strategies
across behaviours, but these estimates are typically obtained by com-
parison with a control group rather than other strategies
69
. For exam-
ple, past reviews of the efficacy of implementation intentions (forming
if–then plans to execute a behaviour) or normative appeals1013 are not
informative about whether implementation intentions are more or less
efficacious than behavioural skills training, or whether interventions
that make group norms more apparent are more or less efficacious than
programmes that aim to increase the trustworthiness of institutions.
Existing reviews that do compare the efficacy of interventions
across different targets have been circumscribed to specific domains,
such as health
14
, climate change mitigation
15
and human immunode-
ficiency virus (HIV) prevention and care1618. This traditional focus on
single behavioural domains might arise because research funding
is often allocated by problem (as illustrated by the disease-specific
organization of the National Institutes of Health (NIH), the main health
research funding agency in the USA) or because researchers are often
trained in siloes and assume that each issue is unique.
From a theoretical standpoint, understanding a broad spectrum of
behavioural domains is critical to a generalizable behavioural change
model. From a practical standpoint, new behavioural change challenges
will continue to surface. For example, before the COVID-19 pandemic,
no research had examined how to promote widespread masking, social
distancing or adherence to lockdown measures. Thus, reviewing targets
of behavioural change across domains is essential for well-informed
public health decisions in unprecedented situations.
In this Review, we synthesize disparate bodies of research to
facilitate decisions about what behavioural change targets to choose
when designing an intervention. First, we define a parsimonious set of
individual and social-structural determinants of behaviour based on
existing theories, supplemented by an extensive review of the literature
and author verification that the final groupings were meaningful, par-
simonious and relatively homogeneous. Next, we summarize the meta-
analytic evidence for correlations between each naturally occurring
determinant (for example, knowledge) and behaviour, as well as meta-
analytic effect sizes for experimental and quasi-experimental tests
of the efficacy of behavioural interventions that target that determi-
nant (for example, interventions that provide information to increase
knowledge). We conclude by organizing intervention approaches into
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
Similarly, even though social cognitive theor y emphasizes the impact of
others as models of behaviour
30
, the theory also includes self-efficacy
and personal agency, which are individual factors.
Nevertheless, several theories suggest important social-structural
determinants of behaviour that could be targets of behavioural change.
For example, there are theoretical distinctions between injunctive
norms (perceptions of the degree to which others support a person’s
behaviour20,37) and descriptive norms (subjective estimates of the
frequency of a behaviour in a particular population
3841
)
37
, and these
two norms do not always correlate with each other (r= 0.1–0.4)4244.
There are also theoretical distinctions between regulatory and distrib-
uted policies45, which led to our decision to separate formal legal and
administrative sanctions (legal and administrative instruments to ban
or punish a behaviour) from institutional trustworthiness (justice or
fairness within an organization or government entity, which increases
trust and reduces vigilance4648), which can often be achieved in infor-
mal ways such as demonstrating benevolence(ref.49 and A. H. Jung
et al., unpublished data). Moreover, material incentives (provision of
financial or nonfinancial rewards) can affect the motivation to perform
a behaviour and are theoretically important drivers of behaviour50,51.
The literature also suggests other social-structural factors that
might be particularly relevant for determining behaviour and driving
behaviour change. For example, a large literature suggests that social
support influences human behaviour52, and increasing the feasibility
of behaviour such as through access and defaults
2
(material or logistic
resources to facilitate the performance of a behaviour) or monitors and
reminders53,54 (physical or digital instruments that track behavioural
performance and alert users of the need to execute a behaviour) is an
important aspect of intervention design53,54.
In sum, the classification of individual and social-structural deter-
minants of behaviour we use in subsequent sections based on the
above considerations is more comprehensive and theor y-based than
classifications of nudges
1
and considerably more parsimonious and
theory-driven than classifications of behavioural change techniques
55
.
Individual determinants and interventions
Individual determinants of behaviour include knowledge, beliefs, atti-
tudes, emotions, skills and habits (Table1). In this section, we synthesize
results from meta-analyses of correlational studies that measure the
determinant along with the behaviour in question (Supplementary
Table1) and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials, quasi-
experimental studies and laboratory research of behavioural change
interventions based on these determinants (Supplementary Table2).
Determinants are discussed in order from least to most effective when
targeted by interventions.
In comparing effect sizes across studies, readers should keep in
mind their meaning (Table2) and interpretational limitations. For
example, in a correlational study, an odds ratio of 2 between knowl-
edge and behaviour implies that for each increasing unit in the meas-
ure of knowledge, the probability of behaviour doubles. However,
correlational studies do not inform the degree to which changing
knowledge will produce a change in behaviour. Similarly, in an inter-
vention context, an odds ratio of 2 implies that the behaviour is twice
as likely following exposure to a knowledge-based intervention relative
to the control group. However, in both cases, the ultimate meaning of
the effect size depends on the baseline probability of executing the
behaviour. An odds ratio of 2 implies much greater savings in energy
if 30% of the control group saves energy than if only 3% of the control
does so.
Knowledge
Knowledge links an object or behaviour to an attribute or event with
absolute certainty and is often formally imparted through educational
efforts. For example, knowledge that a COVID-19 vaccine exists or that
human activity contributes to climate change is accepted by many
individuals and endorsed by governments. The associations bet ween
knowledge and behaviour are often studied under the umbrella of
‘literacy’, which involves a body of facts and mental models in a par-
ticular domain. For example, financial literacy (a person’s f inancial
knowledge
56,57
) correlates with desirable financial behaviours at r= 0.29
(ref. 57). However, the association between financial literacy and behav-
iour is extremely small (r= 0.09) when the behaviour is measured after
the measure of literacy was obtained instead of before (ref. 57).
There is also extensive research on the relation between literacy
and behaviour in the health and environment domains, but effects
are small (Supplementary Table1). For example, there is a negligi
-
ble association between oral health literacy and visiting the dentist
(OR = 1.25)58 and between HIV knowledge and actual condom use
(r= 0.06)44, and small associations between recycling literacy and recy-
cling (r= 0.20)59 and between climate change knowledge and climate
change-adaptation behaviours such as supporting environmentally
friendly policies or relocating in response to climate change (r= 0.14)60.
One potential explanation for the lack of a sizable correlation between
knowledge and behaviour overall (Fig.1a) is that the knowledge is
only tenuously related to the behaviours being studied. For example,
knowledge related to alcohol and its effec ts might be inconsequential
if drinking is related to normative or other beliefs20.
Interventions that target knowledge involve education (for exam-
ple, systematic instruction to individuals or groups) and other didactic
approaches intended to reduce a knowledge deficit. Meta-analyses of
behavioural effects suggest that these interventions produce negligible
effects (Fig.1b). For example, educational approaches have a negligi-
ble effect on climate change mitigation (d= 0.09)15. Similarly, a meta-
analysis of vaccination interventions showed that neither providing
information in general nor attempting to correct misinformation
increases vaccination uptake (OR = 1.04 and OR = 0.94, respectively)
(S. Liu et al., unpublished).
Importantly, some of the effect sizes derived from the correlational
evidence are larger than the largest effects obtained from intervention
studies. Thus, using correlational evidence to make inferences about
interventions might lead to the selection of ineffective programmes.
Even more critical is the fact that the efficacy of knowledge as a target
of change is negligible. From this standpoint, building a campaign or
programmes to increase knowledge is likely to leave policymakers and
constituents disappointed.
General skills
Broad behavioural and cognitive skills (for example, the ability to con-
trol attention during tasks or inhibit temptations when behaviours
require high levels of self-control) are small predictors of behaviour
(Fig.1a). For example, prosocial skills are not signif icantly correlated
with obtaining employment during adolescence (overall OR = 1.03)61
and executive functioning skills (which comprise inhibitory control
and cognitive flexibility) correlate only at r= –0.14 with disinhibited
eating62.
Many behavioural change programmes have emphasized the need
to train general skills that might help individuals tocontrol undesirable
behaviours
62
. Other interventions are based on mindfulness principles,
with the rationale that mindfulness can reduce aggression and other
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
impulsive behaviours. A meta-analysis of mindfulness interventions
for children and adolescents found a small effect on reducing negative
behaviours (d= 0.21)
63
. Overall, the effect of general skills inter ventions
is negligible (Fig.1b).
General attitudes
Psychologists have long considered whether general attitudes towards
objects (for example, attitudes towards recycling) predic t behaviour
(for example, actual recycling). A narrative review from the late 1970s
found that of 54 studies of the relation between general attitudes and
behaviour, 25 showed null results and those that showed significant
results rarely exceeded an effect size of r = 0.40 (ref. 64). More recent
meta-analyses suggest that the relation between general attitudes
and behaviour is quite small (d = 0.22 (ref. 65) and r = 0.14 (ref. 66)),
whereas others suggest that the relation is much stronger (r = 0.39)67.
An interesting wrinkle in the study of general attitudes is the pro-
posal that researchers measure implicit attitudes in addition to the
traditional measures of attitudes used in the meta-analyses described
above. Implicit attitude measures are designed to capture relatively
automatic evaluative responses through spontaneous participants’
judgements or timed responses to a task
6871
. In the implicit association
test, for example, implicit attitudes are measured by comparing the
time required to pair an object with the concept ‘good’ with the time
required to pair an object with the concept ‘bad’
72
. However, these
measures have produced negligible to medium associations with
behaviour as well. For example, in the area of substance use, there is a
medium association between implicit attitudes towards legal and illegal
psychoactive substances and substance use (r= 0.27)73.
Whereas the overall association between general attitudes
and specific behaviours is medium in size (Fig.1a), the effect size
Table 1 | Individual determinants of behaviour and associated measures and interventions
Determinant Deinition Example measures Example interventions
Knowledge Collection of facts about an object or behaviour,
which can include information about the
properties and consequences of a particular
object or event, such as a virus or pollution;
knowledge links an object or behaviour to an
attribute or event with absolute certainty
Measure of literacy: “Contact with a dirty
toilet is a common cause of venereal
disease or sexually transmitted disease”
(participant responds ‘true’ or ‘false’)213
Health education
Didactic instruction about climate
change in schools
General skills Cognitive or overt routines that enable individuals
to carry out various speciic behaviours; they
involve broad capacities such as controlling
attention during tasks and being able to inhibit
temptations when behaviours require high levels
of self-control
Self-report measures of self-control, which
include statements about a person’s ability
to make a plan or avoid temptations214
Behavioural change programmes
emphasizing the need to train general
skills that might help individualsto
control undesirable behaviours137
General attitudes Evaluations of objects, persons and events; for
example, prejudice is a negative judgement of
a group as the attitude object, and an attitude
towards cars is a positive or negative evaluation
of cars as the attitude object; this type of attitude
is often termed ‘attitude towards the target’23,215
Likert-scale measure of attitudes towards
environmental protections: “Humans are
severely abusing the environment”216
Implicit attitude test concerning alcohol217
Mass-media health-promotion
campaigns about a behaviour79
Interventions aimed at weakening
associations by instilling goals and
threat80
Beliefs Subjective assignments of probability that an
object or behaviour has a given attribute or
outcome32,218
Self-report measure of conspiracy beliefs:
“To what extent do you think the virus is
part of a biological warfare program?”219
Messages that explicitly introduce
expectations about a behaviour
Growth mindset interventions in
academic settings
Emotions Visceral feelings (for example, happiness or fear)
associated with a particular object, person or
event; experiencing fear of climate change or
disgust about a particular group of individuals are
examples of emotion
Likert-scale measure of emotions
towards COVID-19: “I feel fearful about
COVID-19”220
Emotional appeals that sensitize
audiences to risks and include discussion
of the threat posed by a problem or the
audience’s susceptibility to it
Behavioural skills Routines that enable people to execute a target
behaviour, often relected in higher levels of
perceived control or eficacy concerning the
behaviour30,134,218
Self-report measure of behavioural control
and conidence to perform or abstain from
a behaviour: “If I wanted to, it would be
easy for me to exercise for at least twenty
minutes, three times a week for the next
fortnight”221
Practising and receiving feedback on the
behaviour and performing homework
related to the behaviour27,129
Asking individuals to formulate
implementation intentions222,223
Behavioural
attitudes Evaluations of a behaviour as good or bad; for
example, whereas an attitude towards cars is a
general attitude, an attitude towards driving a car
for transportation is a behavioural attitude; this
type of attitude is often referred to as ‘attitude
towards the behaviour’23
Semantic differential measures of attitudes
linking recycling to adjectives such as
good or bad: “Recycling household waste
for me is something …” (participant selects
from ive-point response scale anchored
by adjectives ‘good’ and ‘bad’)224
Mode of questioning designed to
uncover and reduce attitudinal
ambivalence towards a particular
behaviour144,145
Habits Behavioural routines that have acquired features
of automaticity225, meaning that they occur
eficiently, without awareness, or continue even
without intention and after they are no longer
adaptive151,226
Measure of handwashing habit: “Washing
my hands would require effort not to do”2 27 Training to stop a behaviour when faced
with temptations157,158
Introducing environmental regularity
to promote habit formation150
Distracting oneself from behavioural
cues159
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
corresponding to intervention efficacy is negligible (Fig.1b). For
example, a meta-analysis of mass-media health-promotion campaigns
revealed a negligible effect on behaviour change (r=0.05)74. Moreover,
a meta-analysis found that although various techniques led to shifts
in implicit attitudes, these trainings had little effect on behaviour. For
example, interventions that aimed to weaken associations between
an object and a particular evaluation had a negligible influence on
behaviour (g = –0.10)75.
Clearly, people report general attitudes that correlate with their
behaviours even though attempts at changing these attitudes have a
much lower efficacy potential than the correlational evidence suggests.
It might be that people rationalize their behaviour when they report
general attitudes (consistent with research on cognitive dissonance
and self-perception
7678
), even though those attitudes did not have a
causal role in producing behaviour. Regardless, general attitudes are
relatively inconsequential targets of change.
Beliefs
Similar to knowledge, specific beliefs about an object or behaviour
have positive relations to behavioural performance (Fig.1a). However,
there is a range of effect sizes across domains, with larger effect sizes for
environmental versus health behaviours. For example, a meta-analysis
of the determinants of recycling found medium correlations between
expectations of positive feelings if one recycles or negative feelings if
one does not recycle correlate with actual recycling (r = 0.26)59 (note
that expectations of feelings are beliefs in the probability of experienc-
ing particular emotions and not emotions themselves). By contrast, the
correlations between condom use and the perceived attractiveness
of condoms (r = 0.14) and the belief that condom use protec ts people
from HIV infection (r= 0.10) are small, and the correlation between
condom use and the belief that purchasing condoms is embarrassing
is negligible (r = –0.05)44.
Specific beliefs have also been investigated in the context of con-
spiracy theories. Intuitively, endorsing COVID-19 conspiracy theories
might seem quite consequential for the likelihood of engaging in activi-
ties such as wearing a mask or social distancing. However, the effects
are not unlike those of knowledge and other beliefs (Supplementary
Table1). In fact, a meta-analysis of crossed-lagged correlations from
17 samples estimated the impact of conspiracy beliefs on risky COVID-
19-related behaviour to be β = 0.09 (ref. 79) with a reciprocal effect from
behaviour to beliefs of similar magnitude. Thus, even these dramatic
beliefs exert negligible effects on behaviour.
Other commonly studied beliefs are cultural. These beliefs entail
judgements related to religiosity, spirituality, fashion, food consump-
tion, interpersonal relationships and the relative standing of different
social groups, including interactions among group members and with
other groups80. Cultural beliefs can act as barriers to action when the
recommended behaviour is incongruent with cultural beliefs. For
instance, cultural beliefs can constitute roadblocks to participation
in community-based health insurance when a culture views prepara-
tion for illness as a magnet for illness itself
81
. Similarly, cultural beliefs
about food consumption, which designate which foods are healthy
or unhealthy, can act as a barrier to the management of diabetes
when they conflict with recommendations provided by health-care
professionals82.
Quantitative reviews have estimated the relation between differ-
ent kinds of cultural beliefs and behaviour. For instance, hostile sexism
(a collection of negative beliefs about the role of women in society and
their relation to men) has a medium correlation with male-to-female
violence (z= 0.26), whereas the relation between benevolent sexism
(a collection of beliefs that women have positive qualities but need to be
protected) and male-to-female violence is negligible (z= 0.05)83. As for
religious beliefs, greater religiosity correlates with lower engagement
in criminal behaviour (r= –0.12)84 and a combination of religiosity and
spirituality correlates with less physical aggression (r= –0.12) and less
sexual aggression and domestic violence (r= –0.05), albeit weakly
85
.
More generally, greater religious involvement is associated with less
engagement in destructive behaviour (z= –0.17) and more engage-
ment in constructive behaviour (z = 0.20)86. However, some Christian
groups are philosophically opposed to what they consider unnecessary
medical intervention, resulting in disparities in vaccination coverage
across religions87.
Cultural beliefs have important implications for many behav-
iours
8891
. For example, in the USA, Hispanic people have the lowest
rates of smoking among all racial and ethnic groups
92
, probably owing
to less acculturation (the degree to which people from minority groups
retain their native cultural language and values relative to those of the
new, dominant culture93) than other groups94. Furthermore, the preva-
lence of risky behaviours, including smoking, obesity and unhealthy
eating and drinking habits, is higher among second-generation Ameri-
cans born in the USA than first-generation immigrants to the USA
(r= 0.01–0.28). It seems that individuals living in the USA but born in
other countries (for example, Mexico and China) have closer ties to
their traditional cultures, which promote healthier lifestyle choices
95
.
This ‘immigrant paradox’ characterizes the situation of immigrants
who practised healthy dietary behaviours in their home countries but
abandon them as they acculturate to their new country of residence
90
.
When existing interventions fail to meet the needs of racial and
ethnic minority groups, culturally tailored programmes can be devel-
oped by modifying the content, language, mode of delivery or other
intervention components in existing interventions or new programmes
that consider cultural context can be developed based on the group’s
concerns
96
. However, the impac t of culturally tailored interventions on
health behaviours is seemingly negligible (g = 0.1–0.20)
97
. For exam-
ple, interventions designed to address hypermasculinity (machismo)
beliefs among Hispanic adolescents are successful at reducing the like-
lihood of engaging in HIV risk behaviour by 32% relative to participants
in the control groups98, whereas a cultural adaptation of a substance use
intervention for Latinx adolescents had a negligible effect (g = 0.06)
99
.
In fact, five out of seven of the effects of such cultural adaptations were
negligible (Supplementary Table2).
As with knowledge and general attitudes, the effect sizes for beliefs
derived from the correlational evidence (Fig.1a) are larger than the
largest effects obtained from intervention studies (Fig.1b). For exam-
ple, confidence in one’s ability to grow in a particular domain (growth
mindset) is associated with improved performance in academic
settings100102. Accordingly, interventions have been developed to
Table 2 | Interpretation of effect sizes
d or g r or zOdds ratio or risk ratio
Negligible <0.2 <0.1 <1.44
Small 0.2–0.49 0.1–0.23 1.44–2.47
Medium 0.5–0.79 0.24–0.36 2.48–4.26
Large ≥0.8 ≥0.37 ≥4.27
d or g, standardized mean difference; r, Pearson correlation coeficient; z, standardized r
coeficient.
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
change mindsets in the hope of also improving academic performance.
However, a meta-analysis of these interventions found a negligible
effect on behaviour both in experiments that successfully altered
mindset (d= 0.04) and when considering all experiments (d= 0.05)8.
Emotions
Experiencing fear of climate change or disgust about a particular group
of individuals present examples of emotion. Emotional appeals are
commonly used to sensitize audiences to the risks of an object or event
and include discussion of the threat posed by a problem or the audi-
ence’s susceptibility to it
103
. Emotions feature prominently in behav-
ioural change models (for example, the health belief model104106).
However, the correlations between negative emotions and/or risk and
behaviour tend to be small or negligible. For example, there is a small
correlation (r= 0.12) between anxiety about COVID-19 and COVID-19
protective behaviours (although the correlation with fear is medium in
size, r= 0.24)107. Similarly, the association between perceived climate
change risk and past adaptation behaviour is only r= 0.10 (ref. 60), and
the association between perceived HIV risk and condom use is only
r= 0.06 (ref. 44). The results tend to be similar for other forms of per-
ceived threat (Supplementary Table1). For example, in the domain of
condom use, the associations between worry or concern and perceived
HIV severity are r= 0.09 and r= 0.02, respectively44.
Social emotions (emotions that serve primarily social functions
and involve reactions to how the self is perceived by others, such as
pride, gratitude, guilt, anger and envy108,109) have garnered attention
from behavioural scientists studying interpersonal behaviours. For
example, people’s tendency to experience anger while driving has
a small association with speeding behaviour (r= 0.12)110 and is more
strongly associated with a composite of high-risk driving behaviours
(r= 0.39)111. As other examples, envy has a weak negative relation with
positive workplace behaviours such as help-seeking (r= –0.21 to 0.05;
median r= –0.03) and a stronger relation with negative workplace
behaviours such as incivility (r= 0.27–0.33; median r= 0.29)112. Likewise,
guilt is associated with greater engagement in pro-environmental
behaviours (r= 0.30)113; gratitude is associated with prosocial behav-
iour (r= 0.26)
114
; the affective experience of interpersonal attraction
is correlated with a behavioural composite of amount of talking, head
Legal and administrative sanctions
Trustworthiness
Injunctive norms
Monitors and reminders
Descriptive norms
Material incentives
Social support
Access
General skills
Beliefs
Knowledge
Emotions
General attitudes
Behavioural skills
Behavioural attitudes
Habits
Material incentives
Other structural factors
Access
Injunctive norms
Descriptive norms
Trustworthiness
Knowledge
General skills
General attitudes
Beliefs
Emotions
Behavioural skills
Behavioural attitudes
Habits
0 1 2
1.81
1.89
2.1 5
2.52
2.58
3.43
3.70
1.24
1.71
2.03
2.48
3.01
3.19
6.17
1.11
1.23
1.35
1.43
1.60
1.99
2.09
2.65
0.80
1.27
1.53
1.90
2.20
2.47
2.53
4.89
3 4 5 6 7 8
0 3 6 9 12 15
0 1 2 43 5 6
0 3 6 12915
Meta-analyses of determinantsaMeta-analyses of interventions
Odds ratio Odds ratio
Odds ratio
Individual factorsSocial-structural factors
Social-structural factors Individual factors
Odds ratio
b
Mean
Maximum
Minimum
Fig. 1 | Effect size range in meta-analyses of behaviour change. a,b, Range
(minimum, red; maximum, yellow) and mean (line) of effect sizes (odds ratios)
for meta-analyses of individual (Supplementary Table1) and social-structural
(Supplementary Table3) determinants of change (panel a) and for meta-analyses
of intervention studies that targeted individual (Supplementary Table2) and
social-structural (Supplementary Table4) determinants (panel b). Only meta-
analyses that excluded extreme publication bias are included (Supplementar y
Note 1). Mean odds ratio values are presented above the mean line. Odds ratios
<1.44 are negligible, those ≥1.44 but <2.48 are small, those ≥2.48 but <4.27 are
medium and those ≥4.27 are considered large.
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
nodding and sitting distance (r= 0.20)
115
; and emotional prejudice is
more strongly associated with discriminatory behaviour (r
median
 = 0.35)
than stereotypes and other beliefs
116
. Finally, even though social emo-
tions are not consistently associated with purchasing behaviour
(rgratitude = 0.50; rpride = 0.07; rguilt = –0.01; ranger = –0.19), they have medium
to large correlations with sharing behaviour (r
gratitude
 = 0.74; r
pride
 = 0.32;
r
guilt
 = 0.54; r
anger
 = –0.38)
117
. However, these strong associations with
sharing behaviour might partly be a function of the lower cost of this
behaviour (operationalized as complaining and word of mouth in the
source meta-analysis) compared with purchasing behaviour.
Generally, inducing emotions influences behaviour (g= 0.31)
118
.
Although negative emotions have been found to have no overall effect
on food consumption (g= 0.02), positive emotions increase food intake
(g= 0.24)
119
. Likewise, communicating to induce fear tends to have
small effects (Supplementary Table2). For example, communicating
the level of genetic cardiometabolic risk to patients has no effect on
dietary changes or weight loss
120
. Moreover, despite occasional claims
of backfire effects
121
, a comprehensive meta-analysis of fear-appeal
experiments found that the effects of risk information and fear were
positive but negligible in size (d= 0.20 (ref. 122) and d = 0.14 (ref. 103),
respectively). Furthermore, inductions of both anticipatory emo-
tions (for example, fear and worry; d= 0.21) and anticipated emotions
(for example, regret, guilt and shame; d= 0.30) produce positive but
small changes in the enactment of behaviour
123
. All in all, the effects of
emotions are small.
Many interventions have targeted social emotions to bring about
behavioural change
124
. In particular, gratitude interventions are popu-
lar in the positive psychology literature
125
. However, the overall effects
of gratitude interventions are small. For example, meta-analyses have
found negligible effects of gratitude interventions on exercise (d= 0.10)
and prosocial behaviour (d= 0 and d= 0.12)114, and a stronger but still
small effect on behaviours that express gratitude (for example, writing
a thank-you note; d= 0.40)125.
As with the other individual determinants reviewed thus far, the
effect sizes for emotions are stronger in correlational than intervention
studies. Although the available evidence suggests medium correlations
between emotions and behaviour (Fig.1a), concluding that they might
be a desirable avenue for intervention could lead to underwhelming
results as the efficacy of emotion-based interventions is small (Fig.1b).
Behavioural skills
Specific behavioural skills show a medium-sized correlation with
actual behaviour (Fig.1a). For example, mothers who have the skills
to discuss birth control methods with their daughters are 5.69 times
more likely to have their daughters vaccinated against human papil-
loma virus (HPV) than mothers who lack such communication skills
126
.
In addition, specific behavioural skills are often reflected in people’s
sense of the controllability of a particular behaviour (perceived
behavioural control)
22,127
. For example, according to meta-analyses,
perceived behavioural control has a strong association with actual recy-
cling (r= 0.39)
59
, and confidence that one can refuse alcohol (refusal
self-efficacy) has medium associations with the frequency of drinking
(r= –0.35), the quantity of alcohol consumed (r= –0.29) and binge
drinking (r= –0.32)128.
Behavioural skills interventions involve receiving arguments
about the execution of a set of skills, as well as observing a role model
execute a behaviour, practising and receiving feedback on the behav-
iour, and performing homework related to that behaviour
27,129
. For
example, verbal arguments might be used to encourage individuals
to secure resources for and overcome obstacles to wearing a condom
during sex130 and more practical behavioural skill training interven-
tions involve role-playing the application of condoms16. Teenag-
ers might practise refusing invitations to smoke cigarettes or drink
alcohol131133, and adults might be taught to avoid drinking before or
during sex or to monitor their emotional states to avoid risky sexual
situations27,134.
Meta-analyses of these types of interventions have shown that
training behavioural skills provides benefits for behavioural change.
For example, communication skills training effectively increases
both safer-sex discussions with partners (d= 0.35) and condom use
(d= 0.39)135. Organizational training across various such as inter-
personal communication also produces sizable improvements in
work behaviour (d= 0.62), particularly for programmed instruction
(d= 0.94), which is given in small, specific steps that require a correct
response before the learner moves to the next step
136
. Although the
overall effect size for inter vention efficacy is small (Fig.1b), behavioural
skills are among the more promising targets to achieve behavioural
change and have more sizable effects than general skills (d = 0.62
(ref. 136) versus d= 0.30 (ref. 137)).
Behavioural attitudes
Studies of attitudinal determinants involve analyses of associations
with behavioural attitudes as well as indirect measures of behavioural
attitudes (beliefs about behavioural outcomes weighted by evaluations
of those outcomes20,138). A general meta-analysis of newly formed atti-
tudes estimated that the link between attitudes towards behaviours
and actual behaviour is large (r= 0.58)
139
. These findings are supported
by meta-analyses in other domains. For instance, there is a medium
correlation between attitudes towards sun-protection behaviour
and actual sun-protection behaviour (r= 0.31)
140
, and large correla-
tions between attitudes towards car use and actual car use (r= 0.41)
141
,
attitudes towards consuming organic vegetables and organic veg-
etable consumption (r= 0.44)
142
, and attitudes towards condom use
and actual condom use (r= 0.38)138. Similarly, indirect attitude meas-
ures show a medium correlation with condom use (r= 0.31)
138
. Thus,
behavioural attitudes are generally better predictors of behaviour than
general attitudes, knowledge and specific beliefs (Fig.1a).
Interventions targeting behavioural attitudes include media mes-
sages or in-person discussions of the benefits of changing a behav-
iour130,143, as well as motivational interviewing designed to reduce
attitudinal ambivalence towards a particular behaviour144,145. However,
interventions to change attitudes towards behaviours are generally
comprehensive and include other strategies such as targeting norms
and perceived behavioural control20,127. Consequently, many interven-
tion studies provide little information on the specific impact of targeting
behavioural attitudes. Laboratory experiments designed to impact
behavioural attitudes as a way of influencing behaviour have found large
effects on behaviours (d = 1.10 and d = 0.79)146, but the effects of actual
interventions are typically small (Supplementary Table2). Overall, the
effects of behavioural attitude interventions are small (Fig.1b).
As with the other individual determinants, the differences in effect
sizes between correlational and intervention studies are considerable.
Importantly, the correlational studies that find the strongest associa-
tions measured behaviour in the laboratory
139
and involve behaviours
that exist only in those contexts (for example, voting in support of a
fictitious policy as part of an experiment
139,147
). Consequently, these
experiments are poor representatives of the complex decisions people
make when attitudes coexist with other factors.
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
Habits
Past behaviour is an important precursor of future behaviour. For exam-
ple, past condom use has a medium correlation with current condom
use (r= 0.36)
44
, and past recycling behaviour has large correlations
with future recycling (r = 0.41)
59
and seeing oneself as a person who
recycles (r= 0.48)59.
Habits have been equated with past behaviour in many analyses
148
.
However, contemporar y theories define habits as repeated behaviours
that exhibit automaticity, occur without awareness and are difficult to
stop even when they no longer provide benefits to the individual1 49154.
A meta-analysis of associations between health-provider habits (for
example, handwashing) measured with habit scales that tap into auto-
maticity showed a medium association with the execution of those
behaviours (r= 0.33)
155
, and another meta-analysis found a large asso
-
ciation between car habits and car use (r= 0.50)156. In sum, habits have
large associations with behaviours (Fig.1a).
Habit-promoting interventions involve157,158 training to stop
behaviour in the face of temptations157,158, introducing environmental
regularity to promote habit formation150 and distracting people from
behavioural cues159. For example, laboratory cognitive training to
inhibit approach to food cues, promote distraction, reappraise food
cravings and use other cognitive control techniques has a small effect
on food intake (g= 0.27), with reappraisal (g= 0.45), attentional bias
modification (g = 0.44) and distraction (g=0.31) having the strong-
est effects
159
. Similarly, a meta-analysis found that stop signal training
(d= –0.39) and attentional bias modification (d= –0.51) showed small
and medium effects on eating behaviour, respectively160.
Habit reversal training has also been used to reduce tics158. In this
treatment, patients are trained to identify occurrences of the tic and
the events that trigger it and implement a competing, incompatible
response. For example, if stress or hunger increases tics, activation
of antagonist muscles when a tic is expected can eliminate the tic
158
(d= 0.94)157. This treatment changes motor associations with external
stimuli and therefore reduces behaviours that are executed despite
undesirable consequences.
Interventions to curb habits are impressive because they are
fighting against chronic, automated tendencies that are difficult to
eliminate. As with many of the individual factors we considered, the
effects obtained from correlational studies are markedly stronger
than the corresponding effects from intervention studies. Neverthe-
less, interventions to train habits are clearly promising and, among all
individual targets, demonstrate the strongest impact on behavioural
change (Fig.1b). However, they face the challenge of needing to elicit
behaviour before that behaviour can become automated.
Social-structural determinants and interventions
Social-structural determinants of behaviour include legal and admin-
istrative sanctions, trustworthiness, injunctive norms, monitors and
reminders, descriptive norms, material incentives, social support
and access (Table3). Although these determinants reflect social and
environmental conditions, the measures of determinants often rely on
self-report. For example, descriptive norms tap into how much others
perform a behaviour, but measures in correlational studies reflect a
respondent’s perception of what others do. In this section, we synthe-
size results from meta-analyses of correlational studies that measure
the determinant along with the behaviour in question (Supplementary
Table3) and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials, quasi-
experimental studies and laboratory research of behavioural change
interventions based on these determinants (Supplementary Table4).
Determinants are discussed in order from least to most effective when
targeted by interventions. As noted above, readers should keep in mind
their meaning (Table2) and interpretational limitations when compar-
ing effect sizes across studies.
Legal and administrative sanctions
We identified no meta-analyses of correlations between behaviour
and legal and administrative sanctions. In terms of interventions, poli-
cies that attempt to ban negative behaviour and link it to sanctions
(for example, restricting one’s ability to work or travel if one chooses
not to get vaccinated)161 have been criticized for their potential for
psychological reactance (a negative emotional response caused by
threats to or actual losses of freedom)
162,163
. Specifically, people gen-
erally believe that they possess a certain level of freedom and wish to
have control over their actions. When they encounter events restricting
their perceived freedom, they might become motivated to restore it
by acting against the threatening events. Accordingly, although deter-
rence theory has remained a cornerstone of criminal justice policy,
deterrence-based initiatives have only small to medium effects on
behaviour (r= 0.22–0.33)164 and mandates can sometimes work, as
shown by the success of COVID-19 vaccination mandates in many
places165167. Collectively, however, legal and administrative sanctions
have a negligible effect on behaviour (Fig.1b).
Trustworthiness
Interpersonal trust is a combination of attitudes, affective reactions and
beliefs about others (for example, health-care providers or politicians)
that reduces interpersonal vigilance and increases vulnerability
4648
.
For instance, the trustworthiness of an individual delivering a message
has been found to influence its persuasiveness
168174
. Trust has been
frequently studied in the context of cooperation games, where trust in
one’s game partner strongly predicts altruistic behaviour (r= 0.58)175 .
Trust has also been examined in organizational research, where intra-
team trust is associated with better team performance (r = 0.30)
176
, and
trust in leaders is associated with better task performance (r= 0.26)
and better organizational citizenship behaviour (r= 0.30)177.
Behavioural scientists are also interested in institutional forms of
trust, such as trust in scientists and government institutions. One meta-
analysis found that climate-friendly behaviours correlated with trust
in governmental institutions (r= 0.17), trust in environmental groups
(r= 0.38), trust in industry (r= 0.14) and trust in scientists (r= 0.33)
178
.
However, these associations tend to be stronger for public behaviours
(for example, support of public environmental policies) than for private
behaviours (for example, obtaining health insurance)178.
Notably, specific factors can change the strength and direction of
these associations. For example, there were small correlations between
trust in government institutions and compliance with COVID-19 behav-
ioural guidelines (r= 0.11) and COVID-19 vaccination (r= 0.10)179. How-
ever, trust in former President Donald Trump correlated negatively with
all COVID-19 prevention behaviours
179
. Overall, there is a medium-sized
association between all forms of trustworthiness and behaviour (Fig.1a).
Interventions to increase institutional trustworthiness focus
on increasing the perceived fairness and goodwill of authorities or
organizations, in addition to programmes to increase distributed and
procedural justice. Interventions aimed at improving the perceived
trustworthiness of health-care authorities lead to negligible increases
in behavioural outcomes (g= 0.13)180. Inter ventions to increase distrib-
uted justice at work have produced negligible effects on work perfor-
mance (OR = 1.20)
181
, whereas interventions to increase procedural
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
justice have small positive effects on work behaviour (OR = 1.49)
182
.
Overall, however, interventions that aim to increase institutional
trustworthiness have a negligible effect (Fig.1b).
Injunctive norms
Several health behaviour theories (for example, the theories of rea-
soned action and planned behaviour
20,23
, as well as the theory of norma-
tive focus37,183) converge on the hypothesis that social norms influence
behaviour. Injunctive norms (perceptions of the degree to which others
support one’s behaviour
20,37
) have small associations with behaviours
such as blood donation (r = 0.17)
42
, recycling (r = 0.21)
59
and adolescent
sexual behaviour (r = 0.22)
13
. Overall, however, the correlation between
injunctive norms and behaviour is medium in size (Fig.1a).
Over the past five decades, social normative interventions (for
example, messages that communicate that others approve of specific
behaviours) have been used to change environmental behaviours
124
,
child-rearing practices12, health184 and other risky behaviours by making
people feel that others approve of the course of action recommended
in the intervention. A synthesis of these interventions across numerous
domains revealed a small effect on behaviour (d= 0.34)185. The impact of
injunctive norm interventions has also been synthesized in the domain
of environmental behaviour, revealing a negligible effect (d= 0.10)
186
.
Notably, these interventions can have effects because people are una-
ware of the true injunctive norms
187
. For example, if most students drink
heavily because they assume their peers approve of drinking, reporting
disapproving injunctive norms can curb drinking
188
. Overall, however,
interventions that target injunctive norms have small effects (Fig.1b).
Monitors and reminders
We identified no meta-analyses of correlations between behav-
iour and monitors and reminders. In terms of interventions, moni-
tors and reminder interventions can, potentially, delegate monitoring
and reminder functions to the environment and consequently decrease
self-control failures
189
. Manual reminders (for example, tracking sheets
and paper planners) can promote various health screenings, includ-
ing for breast, cervical and colorectal cancer (OR = 1.63, OR = 1.10 and
OR = 1.85, respectively)
190
. However, they fail to influence preventive
care more generally (OR = 0.99)190 and have negligible effects on vac-
cination (OR = 0.95)(S. Liu et al., unpublished). Often, the use of both
manual and computer-generated reminders is most effective (colo-
rectal cancer screening OR = 2.57; all preventative care OR = 2.23)
190
.
Overall monitors and reminders have a small effect (Fig.1b). Thus, they
might be a useful intervention strategy, particularly in combination
with interventions for other targets.
Descriptive norms
Descriptive norms contribute to the social processes that shape a wide
range of behaviours. Although descriptive norms do not correlate with
blood donation behaviour (r = 0.03)42, they do correlate with recy-
cling (r = 0.33)59, adolescent sexual behaviour (r = 0.40)13, consumer
Table 3 | Social-structural determinants of behaviour and associated measures and interventions
Determinant Deinition Sample measures Sample interventions
Legal and
administrative
sanctions
Legal and administrative instruments to
prescribe, ban or sanction a behaviour State and county records of laws
coded through a policy review228 Banning smoking in public establishments229
Mandating vaccination230
Mandating sick pay231
Taxing pollution232
Trustworthiness Justice or fairness within an
organization or government entity,
which leads constituents to follow
recommendations49
Self-report measure of procedural
justice: “How fair were the
procedures used to handle
the problem?”233
Providing channels for Latinx voters to voice their
concerns
Community-oriented policing that fosters
non-enforcement interactions234
Injunctive norms Perceptions of the degree to which
others support a person’s behaviour20,37 Self-report measure of injunctive
norms: “People who are important
to me think I should use condoms”21
Messages that communicate that others approve
of condom use235
Posting signs stating that taking the stairs is a good way
to get some exercise236
Monitors and
reminders Physical or digital instrument to
track behavioural performance and
remind users of the need to execute
a behaviour
Self-reported use of pill boxes,
diaries and planners237 Clinical reminder system for promoting preventive care238
Digital watches and phone apps that promote physical
activity
Descriptive norms Frequency of a behaviour in a particular
population3841 Self-reported perceptions of what
others do: “Most residents would
vaccinate their child against
COVID-19”228
Comparative feedback such as a chart tracking one’s
energy consumption in relation to one’s neighbours239
Using role models to promote a target behaviour30,31
Posting signs stating that most people used the stairs236
Material incentives Providing inancial or noninancial
rewards in exchange for a behaviour Introduction of state lottery for
vaccinated residents as a reward
for vaccinations240
Paying people US$24 to receive the COVID-19 vaccine241
Social support Informational, instrumental or
inancial help to facilitate a particular
behaviour201
Self-reported lists of individuals
who can perform instrumental,
informational and emotional
support functions242
Leveraging family or adhoc groups to assist individuals
to meet their physical activity goals
Groups of Latina mothers led by ‘promotoras’ who support
and accompany each other during health-promoting
activities243
Access Material or logistic resources
to facilitate the performance
of a behaviour
Census demographics and
self-report of health insurance244
Self-reported health insurance245
Reducing co-payments for medication246
Providing health insurance247
Providing basic income248
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
behaviour (r = 0.31)
9
and smoking initiation (OR = 1.88–2.53)
43
. Overall,
there is a medium-sized association between descriptive norms and
behaviour (Fig.1a).
Most normative interventions try to persuade recipients that
others already behave in the recommended ways. In fact, simply com-
municating descriptive social norms changes behaviour in various
settings, especially when the desired behaviour is highly prevalent191.
For example, college students tend to overestimate the amount of
alcohol consumed by their peers187 and normative interventions that
revise this misperception reduce drinking
182
. Indeed, meta-analyses of
approaches to modify descriptive norms have shown small effects for
alcohol use (d= 0.29)192 and condom use (d= 0.36)11.
Other normative interventions include providing comparative
feedback such as a chart tracking one’s energy consumption in rela-
tion to one’s neighbours
186
. Although people often dislike comparative
feedback193,194, exercise apps that provide comparative feedback are
highly effective (d= 0.96)195.
Having role models to look up to and learn from
30
is a particularly
influential normative intervention (d= 0.51)
186
. This finding is consist-
ent with evidence that interventions delivered by facilitators who
resemble recipients demographically are more successful at increasing
condom use than interventions delivered by demographically dissimi-
lar facilitators
173
. Overall, interventions that aim to change descriptive
norms have small effects on behaviour (Fig.1b).
Material incentives
Correlational evidence about the effects of material incentives suggests
that offering incentives for biochemically validated samples produces
medium increases in smoking cessation (risk ratio = 2.58)
196
. Other
effects, however, are negligible. For example, receiving state subsidies
is minimally correlated with environmentally friendly application of
pesticides (g = 0.12)197.
Many policies designed to promote human behaviour adopt
behaviourist principles
198
by pairing positive behaviour with incen-
tives (for example, providing financial incentives for choosing to get
vaccinated). However, the overall efficacy of incentives is small (Fig.1b).
For example, financial incentives were offered by many countries to
encourage COVID-19 vaccination but, according to a meta-analysis, the
effects were negligible (OR = 1.44)(S. Liu et al., unpublished). Financial
incentives have also been used to decrease energy consumption, where
the effects are small (d= 0.36)
199
, and to curb substance use, where the
effects are medium (d = 0.70)200.
Social support
Social support (the provision of informational, instrumental or finan-
cial help to facilitate a particular behaviour
201
) has been examined in
relation to stress and health, as well as particularly difficult behaviours
that benefit from external advice and assistance, such as weight loss,
medication adherence and resource conservation. Social support dif-
fers from norms in that, as studied in relation to behaviour, the support
concerns a particular behavioural goal. Whereas social norms might
concern others’ approval for maintaining a healthy diet, social support
implies that others are willing to provide advice or other forms of help
around a particular dietary goal.
There are variable associations between social support and behav-
iour. For example, adherence to medical treatments is 1.74 times higher
among patients with cohesive families and 1.53 times lower among
patients with high-conflict families
152
. Moreover, exercise is facilitated
by support from family and important others (d = 0.36 and d = 0.44) as
well as exercise-class leaders and classmates (d = 0.31 and d = 0.32)153.
In addition, there are large associations between emotional, material
and informational support and the quality of childcare behaviours
executed by mothers (r= 0.31, r = 0.27 and r = 0.31, respectively)154.
The effect of social support interventions is medium (Fig.1b).
These interventions often take the form of support groups that facili-
tate a behaviour such as the dietary or physical activity modifications
required to lose weight. Such social support interventions are associ-
ated with small positive effects on adherence to antiretroviral medi-
cation (OR = 1.66)
202
and a reduction in suicide (OR = 0.48)
203
. Social
support interventions based on public commitments to a behaviour
204
(which can increase a person’s motivation to execute a behaviour
but also social support for it) are associated with small (d= 0.26)15 to
medium (g = 0.58)186 increases in conservation behaviour.
Access
According to social cognitive theory24, environmental attributes
can constrain behaviour and thereby act as critical determinants of
behaviour. For example, increases in the price of pesticides decrease
environmentally friendly pesticide application (d= –0.36)197. Likewise,
demographic variables related to a person’s position within the social
hierarchy have a range of associations with behaviour. For example,
healthy behaviours during pregnancy correlate with income (r = 0.26)205
and having a recycling bin and owning a home both correlate with
recycling (r = 0.16 and r = 0.24, respectively)
59
. Overall, the association
between access and behaviour is small (Fig.1a).
Some access interventions are designed to impact the system at
large. Interventions to decrease inequality are attrac tive, given large
disparities in behaviours that benefit individuals and society at large.
Accordingly, researchers have tested structural and community inter
-
ventions, such as microfinancing, which involves small loans to develop
a business as a source of income. However, randomized controlled trials
testing the impact of microloans showed a negligible effect on women’s
control over household expenses (d=–0.01)206. In the area of health,
broader structural and community interventions have small effects as
well (risk ratio = 1.20 and risk ratio = 0.90 for condom use and number
of partners, respectively)207.
Other policy instruments increase access by changing the environ-
ment to offer more specific opportunities for behavioural change. For
example, interventions that ensure access to vaccines by providing
transportation or sites close to potential users double vaccination cov-
erage (S. Liu et al., unpublished). Other policies design situations that
channel behaviour, such as making the desired behaviour the default
on organ-donation forms (d= 0.68)208,209. Yet others decrease access
by taxing alcohol to reduce use (OR = 5.92)
210
. Overall interventions
that aim to increase access have large effects on behaviour (Fig.1b).
Summary and future directions
Our Review suggests that across domains, knowledge, general skills,
general attitudes, beliefs, legal and administrative sanctions, and trust-
worthiness have negligible effects as targets of intervention; emotions,
behavioural skills, behavioural attitudes, injunctive norms, monitors
and reminders, descriptive norms and material incentives have small
effects; habits and social support have medium effects; and access
has large effects (Fig.2a). Of course, some behaviours, populations
and contexts might be unique. Thus, no review or meta-analysis can
predict the result of an intervention across all contexts. Nevertheless,
our Review suggests that certain variables, although highly salient,
might not change behaviour and should not be the primary focus of a
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
behavioural intervention. Moreover, the discrepancies in effect sizes
between correlational studies and intervention studies suggest that
correlational studies are often ill-suited as a basis for deciding what
determinants to address in interventions.
A key aim of our Review was to offer a synthesis across all behav-
iours. To determine the extent to which these conclusions are gen-
eralizable, we examined determinants for health behaviour (Fig.2b)
and environmental behaviour (Fig.2c) specifically. These domains
a
c
b
Knowledge
General attitudes
Behavioural attitudes
Behavioural skills
Beliefs
Emotions
General skills
Habits
Knowledge
General attitudes
Behavioural skills
Beliefs
Emotions
General skills
Habits
Knowledge
Behavioural attitudes
Behavioural skills
Beliefs
sanctions
All
behaviours
Health
behaviours
Environmental
behaviours
Legal and administrative
sanctions
Legal and administrative
Descriptive norms
Injunctive norms
Social support
Access
Material incentives
N
e
g
l
i
g
i
b
l
e
e
e
c
t
s
S
m
a
l
l
e
e
c
t
s
M
e
d
i
u
m
e
e
c
t
L
a
r
g
e
e
e
c
t
M
e
d
i
u
m
e
e
c
t
S
m
a
l
l
e
e
c
t
s
Access
Trustworthiness
Monitors and reminders
Descriptive norms
Injunctive norms
Social support
Material incentives
N
e
g
l
i
g
i
b
l
e
e
e
c
t
s
S
m
a
l
l
e
e
c
t
s
S
m
a
l
l
e
e
c
t
s
N
e
g
l
i
g
i
b
l
e
e
e
c
t
s
S
m
a
l
l
e
e
c
t
s
M
e
d
i
u
m
e
e
c
t
s
M
e
d
i
u
m
e
e
c
t
S
m
a
l
l
e
e
c
t
s
Access
Trustworthiness
Monitors and reminders
Descriptive norms
Social support
Material incentives
Medium
eect
Large
eect
Large
eect
Medium
eect
I
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l
i
n
t
e
r
v
e
n
t
i
o
n
s
S
o
c
i
a
l
-
s
t
r
u
c
t
u
a
l
i
n
t
e
r
v
e
n
t
i
o
n
s
I
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l
i
n
t
e
r
v
e
n
t
i
o
n
s
S
o
c
i
a
l
-
s
t
r
u
c
t
u
a
l
i
n
t
e
r
v
e
n
t
i
o
n
s
I
n
d
i
v
i
d
u
a
l
i
n
t
e
r
v
e
n
t
i
o
n
s
S
o
c
i
a
l
-
s
t
r
u
c
t
u
a
l
i
n
t
e
r
v
e
n
t
i
o
n
s
Fig. 2 | Models of behavioural change intervention efficacy. a–c, Conclusions
of our synthesis of meta-analyses of behaviour change interventions for all
behaviours (panel a), health behaviours (panel b) and environmental behaviours
(panel c). In all panels, individual targets of change are presented on the left
and social-structural targets of change are presented on the right. Vertically,
targets of change are organized from least to most effective based on the
average effect sizes for each behavioural target (Fig.1b;Supplementary Figs.1
and2), and grouped based on whether effects are negligible, small, medium or
large (Table1). Only meta-analyses that excluded extreme publication bias are
included (Supplementary Note1).
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
were chosen because they have been assessed in most meta-analyses
(Supplementary Note2). The distribution of individual determinants for
health behaviour is the same as that for all behaviours. The distribution
is similar for environmental behaviour, except that the data are less com-
plete. The efficacy data for social-structural factors related to health
and environmental behaviours are sparser but still revealing. In both
cases, interventions that target descriptive norms, material incentives,
social support and access are promising, whereas interventions that
emphasize institutional trustworthiness in the health domain and legal
and administrative sanctions or injunctive norms in the environmental
domain might be insufficient to move populations to change.
Thus, the next pandemic and current climate change crisis will
require not knowledge but, rather, active approaches that enable
individuals to circumvent obstacles and gain support, and that ensure
access to resources in ways that promote positive behaviour in all
groups. For example, the US campaign for COVID-19 vaccination tar-
geted vaccine confidence (general attitudes). However, our Review
suggests that it would have been more appropriate to increase access
to vaccination, in addition to training behavioural skills, strengthening
norms, leveraging social support and using material incentives. Our
Review also suggests that behavioural skills training should be effective
to induce behaviours to curb climate change. However, in that case,
actual beliefs also have a small effect, suggesting that the dominant
intervention emphasis of increasing perceptions of climate change
and its outcomes, albeit insufficient, is not misguided.
The next step for intervention researchers is to link these con-
clusions to specific intervention contents and policies. For exam-
ple, randomized controlled trials should test different methods to
change descriptive norms or specific implementations of interventions
designed to increase access to a behaviour. Impor tantly, researchers
and policymakers need to stop repeating programmes that are typically
unsuccessful. For example, although some boilerplate information
about a behaviour should routinely be introduced to an audience not
familiar with a behaviour or the goal of a behaviour, launching large
efforts to test the efficacy of interventions to increase institutional
trust or corrections for misinformation seems futile if the motivation
is behavioural change. Finally, more trials that test different interven-
tion targets are needed so that future research reviews can draw on
more data that better control for populations and contexts. Such
controls are not possible when different experiments test different
targets of change.
Researchers should also study naive theories about behavioural
change among policymakers and their constituents. If policymakers
believe that knowledge is fundamental to behavioural change, they
will continue to implement well-intended but unsuccessful interven-
tions. Likewise, if policymakers consider all targets of change as equally
attractive possibilities without considering their relative efficacy, their
choices are also likely to be misguided. Understanding these naive
conceptualizations and how they translate into behavioural change
initiatives is critical to ensuring that evidence-based findings similar
to those provided here shape the practise of behavioural change.
Any literature review has limitations. First, our review did not
specifically consider that different channels might be used to impart
knowledge or modify beliefs or injunctive norms. For instance, indi-
vidualized knowledge might be imparted to a person who visits with
a dietician, delivered to schools or broadcast on mass media. In these
situations, even when the beliefs exist within the minds of individu-
als, interventions might operate at the individual, school or commu-
nity level. Similarly, policies to increase access to services might be
implemented at the level of an organization, county, state, nation or
group of nations that enter international agreements. Which level or
combination of levels produces the most effective interventions is an
important question for future research.
Second, the choice to synthesize meta-analyses might have biased
our conclusions because some areas have been meta-analysed more
than others. However, meta-analysis remains the only method that
allows for comparisons across research that uses different metrics
211,212
.
A first-order meta-analysis of this large intervention literature might be
an aspirational goal for the field that might be feasible with newer forms
of automation. Until then, our review of meta-analyses is informative
and actionable. Behavioural change is likely to remain one of the most
important solutions to humanity’s challenges, and we must be armed
with more and better guidelines to promote it.
Published online: xx xx xxxx
References
1. Last, B. S., Buttenheim, A. M., Timon, C. E., Mitra, N. & Beidas, R. S. Systematic review
of clinician-directed nudges in healthcare contexts. BMJ Open. 11, e048801 (2021).
2. Thaler, R. & Sunstein, C. Nudge: The Gentle Power of Choice Architecture (Yale Univ.
Press, 2008).
3. Michie, S., van Stralen, M. M. & West, R. The behaviour change wheel: a new method for
characterising and designing behaviour change interventions. Implementation Sci. 6, 42
(2011).
4. Crane, D., Garnett, C., Brown, J., West, R. & Michie, S. Behavior change techniques in
popular alcohol reduction apps: content analysis. J. Med. Internet Res. 17, e118
(2015).
5. Michie, S. et al. The Human Behaviour-Change Project: harnessing the power of ar tiicial
intelligence and machine learning for evidence synthesis and interpretation. Implement.
Sci. 12, 121 (2017).
6. Gollwitzer, P. M. & Sheeran, P. in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology Vol. 38
(ed. Zanna, M. P.) 69–119 (Elsevier Academic, 2006).
7. Malaguti, A. et al. Eectiveness of the use of implementation intentions on reduction of
substance use: a meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 214, 108120 (2020).
8. Macnamara, B. & Burgoyne, A. P. Do growth mindset interventions impact students’
academic achievement? A systematic review and meta-analysis with recommendations
for best practices. Psychol. Bull. 149, 133–173 (2023).
9. Melnyk, V., Carrillat, F. A. & Melnyk, V. The inluence of social norms on consumer
behavior: a meta-analysis. J. Mark. 86, 98–120 (2021).
10. Niemiec, R. M., Champine, V., Vaske, J. J. & Mertens, A. Does the impact of norms vary by
type of norm and type of conservation behavior? A meta-analysis. Soc. Nat. Resour. 33,
1024–1040 (2020).
11. Sheeran, P. et al. The impact of changing attitudes, norms, and self-eicacy on
health-related intentions and behavior: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 35, 1178–1188
(2016).
12. Russell, P. S., Smith, D. M., Birtel, M. D., Hart, K. H. & Golding, S. E. The role of emotions
and injunctive norms in breastfeeding: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Health
Psychol. Rev. 16, 257–279 (2022).
13. van de Bongardt, D., Reitz, E., Sandfort, T. & Deković, M. A meta-analysis of the relations
between three types of peer norms and adolescent sexual behavior. Personality Soc.
Psychol. Rev. 19, 203–234 (2015).
14. Wilson, K. et al. When it comes to lifestyle recommendations, more is sometimes less:
a meta-analysis of theoretical assumptions underlying the eectiveness of interventions
promoting multiple behavior domain change. Psychol. Bull. 141, 709–725 (2015).
15. Bergquist, M., Thiel, M., Goldberg, M. H. & van der Linden, S. Field interventions for
climate change mitigation behaviors: a second-order meta-analysis. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci.
USA 120, e2214851120 (2023).
16. Albarracín, D. et al. A test of major assumptions about behavior change: a
comprehensive look at the eects of passive and active HIV-prevention interventions
since the beginning of the epidemic. Psychol. Bull. 131, 856–897 (2005).
17. Albarracin, D. & Durantini, M. R. Are we going to close social gaps in HIV? Likely eects
of behavioral HIV-prevention interventions on health disparities. Psychol . Health Med 15,
694–719 (2010).
18. Johnson, B. T., Michie, S. & Snyder, L. B. Eects of behavioral intervention content on HIV
prevention outcomes: a meta-review of meta-analyses. J. Acquir. Immune Deic . Syndr.
66, S259–S270 (2014).
19. Deutsch, M. & Gerard, H. B. A study of normative and informational social inluences
upon individual judgment. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 51, 629–636 (1955).
20. Fishbein, M. & Ajzen, I. Predicting and Changing Behavior: The Reasoned Action Approach
(Psychology Press, 2011).
21. Fisher, W. A., Fisher, J. D. & Rye, B. J. Understanding and promoting AIDS-preventive
behavior: insights from the theory of reasoned action. Health Psychol. 14, 255–264
(1995).
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
22. Madden, T. J., Ellen, P. S. & Ajzen, I. A comparison of the theory of planned behavior
and the theory of reasoned action. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 18, 3–9 (1992).
23. Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior
(Martin Fishbein Prentice-Hall, 1980).
24. Bandura, A. & National Institute of Mental Health. Social Foundations of Thought and
Action: A Social Cognitive Theory Vol. 1 (Prentice-Hall, 1986).
25. Bandura, A. & Wood, R. Eect of perceived controllability and performance standards on
self-regulation of complex decision making. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 56, 805–814 (1989).
26. Fisher, J. D., Fisher, W. A., Amico, K. R. & Harman, J. J. An information–motivation–
behavioral skills model of adherence to antiretroviral therapy. Health Psychol. 25,
462–473 (2006).
27. Fisher, J. D., Fisher, W. A., Bryan, A. D. & Misovich, S. J. Information–motivation–behavioral
skills model-based HIV risk behavior change intervention for inner-city high school
youth. Health Psychol. 21, 177–186 (2002).
28. Rivet Amico, K. A situated-information motivation behavioral skills model of care
initiation and maintenance (sIMB-CIM): an IMB model based approach to understanding
and intervening in engagement in care for chronic medical conditions. J. Health Psychol.
16, 1071–1081 (2011).
29. Ajzen, I., Albarracin, D. & Hornik, R. (eds) Prediction and Change of Health Behavior:
Applying the Reasoned Action Approach (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2007).
30. Bandura, A. Self-Eicacy: The Exercise of Control (Macmillan, 1997).
31. Bandura, A. Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process
50, 248–287 (1991).
32. Wyer, R. S. & Albarracín, D. in The Handbook of Attitudes (eds Albarracin, D., Johnson, B. T.
& Zanna, M. P.) 273–322 (Lawrence Erlbaum, 2005).
33. Albarracín, D., Zanna, M. P., Johnson, B. T. & Kumkale, G. T. in The Handbook of Attitudes
(eds Albarracín, D. et al.) 3–19 (Lawrence Erlbaum, 2005).
34. Neal, D. T., Wood, W. & Quinn, J. M. Habits — a repeat performance. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci.
15, 198–202 (2006).
35. Fisher, J. D. & Fisher, W. A . Changing AIDS-risk behavior. Psychol. Bull. 111, 455–474 (1992).
36. Cialdini, R. B. Inluence: The Psychology of Persuasion (Morrow, 1993).
37. Cialdini, R. B. & Trost, M. in The Handbook of Social Psychology 4th ed. (eds Gilbert, D. T.,
Fiske, S. T. & Lindzey, G.) 151–192 (McGraw Hill, 1998).
38. Jacobson, R. P., Mortensen, C. R. & Cialdini, R. B. Bodies obliged and unbound:
dierentiated response tendencies for injunctive and descriptive social norms. J. Pers.
Soc. Psychol. 100, 433–448 (2011).
39. Reid, A. E., Cialdini, R. B. & Aiken, L. S. in Handbook of Behavioral Medicine: Methods
and Applications (eds. Steptoe, A. et al.) 263–274 (Springer Science+Business Media,
2011).
40. Cialdini, R. B., Kallgren, C. A. & Reno, R. R. A focus theory of normative conduct:
a theoretical reinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behavior.
Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 24, 201–234 (1991).
41. Bicchieri, C. The Grammar of Society: The Nature and Dynamics of Social Norms
(Cambridge Univ. Press, 2006).
42. Bednall, T. C., Bove, L. L., Cheetham, A. & Murray, A. L . A systematic review and meta-
analysis of antecedents of blood donation behavior and intentions. Soc. Sci. Med. 96,
86–94 (2013).
43. East, K., McNeill, A., Thrasher, J. F. & Hitchman, S. C. Social norms as a predictor of
smoking uptake among youth: a systematic review, met a-analysis and meta-regression
of prospective cohort studies. Addiction 116, 2953–2967 (2021).
44. Sheeran, P., Abraham, C. & Orbell, S. Psychosocial correlates of heterosexual condom
use: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 125, 90–132 (1999).
45. Lowi, T. J. Four systems of policy, politics, and choice. Public. Adm. Rev. 32, 298–310
(1972).
46. Nadelson, L. & Jorcyk, C. I just don’t trust them: the development and validation of an
assessment instrument to measure trust in science and scientists. Sch. Sci. Math. 114,
76–86 (2014).
47. Lee, T. T. Why they don’t trust the media: an examination of factors predicting trust.
Am. Behav. Scientist 54, 8–21 (2010).
48. Mayo, R. Cognition is a matter of trust: distrust tunes cognitive processes. Eur. Rev. Soc.
Psychol. 26, 283–327 (2015).
49. Tyler, T. Procedural justice and policing: a rush to judgment? Ann. Rev. Law Soc. Sci. 13,
29–53 (2017).
50. Galbiati, R. & Vertova, P. How laws aect behavior: obligations, incentives and
cooperative behavior. Int. Rev. Law Econ. 38, 48–57 (2014).
51. Ariely, D., Bracha, A. & Meier, S. Doing good or doing well? Image motivation and
monetary incentives in behaving prosocially. Am. Economic Rev. 99, 544–555 (2009).
52. Anderson, E. S., Winett, R. A. & Wojcik, J. R. Self-regulation, self-eicacy, outcome
expectations, and social support: social cognitive theory and nutrition behavior.
Ann. Behav. Med. 34, 304–312 (2007).
53. Cohen, J. B. & Andrade, E. B. The ADF framework: a parsimonious model for developing
successful behavior change interventions. J. Mark. Behav. 3, 81–119 (2018).
54. Latkin, C., Weeks, M. R., Glasman, L., Galletly, C. & Albarracin, D. A dynamic social
systems model for considering structural factors in HIV prevention and detection.
AIDS Behav 14, 222–238 (2010).
55. Michie, S., West, R., Sheals, K. & Godinho, C. A. Evaluating the eectiveness of behavior
change techniques in health-related behavior: a scoping review of methods used.
Transl. Behav. Med. 8, 212–224 (2018).
56. Huston, S. J. Measuring inancial literacy. J. C onsum. A. 44, 296–316 (2010).
57. Hwang, H. & In Park, H. The relationships of inancial literacy with both inancial
behavior and inancial well-being: meta-analyses based on the selective literature
review. J. Consum. A. https://doi.org/10.1111/joca.12497 (2022).
58. Firmino, R. T. et al. Association of oral health literacy with oral health behaviors,
perception, knowledge, and dental treatment related outcomes: a systematic review
and meta-analysis. J. Public. Health Dent. 78, 231–245 (2018).
59. Geiger, J. L., Steg, L., van der Wer, E. & Unal, A. B. A meta-analysis of factors related to
recycling. J. Env. Psychol. 64, 78–97 (2019).
60. van Valkengoed, A. M. & Steg, L. Meta-analyses of factors motivating climate change
adaptation behaviour. Nat. Clim. Change 9, 158 (2019).
61. Tayfur, S. N., Prior, S., Roy, A. S., Fitzpatrick, L. I. & Forsyth, K. Adolescent psychosocial
factors and participation in education and employment in young adulthood: a systematic
review and meta-analyses. Educ Res Rev 34, 100404 (2021).
62. Shields, C. V., Hultstrand, K. V., West, C. E., Gunstad, J. J. & Sato, A. F. Disinhibited eating
and executive functioning in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-
analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19, 13384 (2022).
63. Dunning, D. et al. Do mindfulness-based programmes improve the cognitive skills,
behaviour and mental health of children and adolescents? An updated meta-analysis
of randomised controlled trials. Evid. Based Ment. Health 25, 135–142 (2022).
64. Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. Attitude–behavior relations: a theoretical analysis and review
of empirical research. Psychol Bull. 84, 888–918 (1977).
65. Helmus, L., Hanson, R. K., Babchishin, K. M. & Mann, R. E. Attitudes supportive of sexual
oending predict recidivism: a meta-analysis. Trauma. Violence Abuse 14, 34–53 (2013).
66. Kraus, S. J. Attitudes and the prediction of behavior: a meta-analysis of the empirical
literature. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 21, 58–75 (1995).
67. Wallace, D. S., Paulson, R. M., Lord, C. G. & Bond, C. F. Which behaviors do attitudes
predict? Meta-analyzing the eects of social pressure and perceived diiculty. Rev. Gen.
Psychol. 9, 214–227 (2005).
68. Petty, R. E., Fazio, R. H. & Briñol, P. in Attitudes: Insights from the New Implicit Measures
(eds Petty, R. E., Fazio, R. H. & Briñol, P.) 3–18 (Psychology Press, 2009).
69. Fazio, R. H. & Olson, M. A. in Dual-Process Theories of the Social Mind (eds Sherman, J. W.,
Gawronski, B. & Trope, Y.) 155–171 (Psychology Press, 2014).
70. Samayoa, J. A. G. & Fazio, R. H. Who starts the wave? Let’s not forget the role of the
individual. Psychol. Inq. 28, 273–277 (2017 ).
71. Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A. & Lundberg, K. B. The bias of crowds: how implicit bias
bridges personal and systemic prejudice. Psychol. Inq. 28, 233–248 (2017).
72. Greenwald, A. G. & Farnham, S. D. Using the implicit association test to measure self-
esteem and self-concept. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 1022–1038 (2000).
73. Rooke, S. E., Hine, D. W. & Thorsteinsson, E. B. Implicit cognition and substance use:
a meta-analysis. Addictive Behav. 33, 1314–1328 (2008).
74. Anker, A. E., Feeley, T. H., McCracken, B. & Lagoe, C. A. Measuring the eectiveness
of mass-mediated health campaigns through meta-analysis. J. Health Commun. 21,
439–456 (2016).
75. Forscher, P. S. et al. A meta-analysis of procedures to change implicit me asures. J. Pers.
Soc. Psychol. 117, 522–559 (2019).
76. Bem, D. J. Self-perception theory. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 6, 1–62 (1972).
77. Festinger, L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance (Stanford Univ. Pre ss, 1957 ).
78. Albarracin, D. & Wyer, R. S. Jr The cognitive impact of past behavior: inluences on
beliefs, attitudes, and future behavioral decisions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 5 (2000).
79. Stasielowicz, L. A continuous time meta-analysis of the relationship between conspiracy
beliefs and individual preventive behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci. Rep. 12,
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-15769-4 (2022).
80. Bond, M. H. et al. in Understanding Culture: Theory, Rese arch, & Application
(eds Wyer, R. S. et al.) 469–506 (Psychology Press, 2009).
81. Dror, D. M. et al. What factors aect voluntary uptake of community-based health
insurance schemes in low- and middle-income countries? A systematic review and
meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 11, e016479 (2016).
82. Li-Geng, T., Kilham, J. & McLeod, K. M. Cultural inluences on dietary self-management
of type 2 diabetes in East Asian Americans: a mixed-methods systematic review. Health
Equity 4, 31–42 (2020).
83. Agadullina, E., Lovakov, A., Balezina, M. & Gulevich, O. A. Ambivalent sexism and
violence toward women: a meta-analysis. Eur. J. Soc . Psychol. 52, 819–859 ( 2022).
84. Baier, C. J. & Wright, B. R. E. “If you love me, keep my commandments”: a meta-analysis
of the eect of religion on crime. J. Res. Crime. Delinquency 38, 3–21 ( 2001).
85. Gonçalves, J. P. et al. The role of religiosity and spirituality in interpersonal violence:
a systematic review and meta-analysis. Braz. J. Psychiatry 45, 162–181 ( 2022).
86. Cheung, C. K. & Yeung, J. W. K. Meta-analysis of relationships between religiosity and
constructive and destructive behaviors among adolescents. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 33,
376–385 (2011).
87. Martens, J. P. & Rutjens, B. T. Spirituality and religiosity contribute to ongoing COVID-19
vaccination rates: comparing 195 regions around the world. Vaccine X. 12, 100241
(2022).
88. Bicchieri, C. Norms in the Wild: How to Diagnose, Measure, and Change Social Norms
1–246 (Psychology Press, 2017).
89. Wilhite, H., Nakagami, H., Masuda, T., Yamaga, Y. & Haneda, H. A cross-cultural analysis of
household energy use behaviour in Japan and Norway. Energy Policy 24, 795–803 (1996).
90. Weisberg-Shapiro, P. & Devine, C. M. “Because we missed the way that we eat at the
middle of the day”: dietary acculturation and food routines among Dominican women.
Appetite 95, 293–302 (2015).
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
91. Backett, K. C. & Davison, C. Lifecourse and lifestyle: the social and cultural location of
health behaviours. Soc. Sci. Med. 40, 629–638 (1995).
92. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Tobacco statistics. cdc.gov, https://www.
cdc.gov/tobacco/data_statistics/index.htm (accessed 9 April 2024).
93. Padilla A. M. Acculturation: Theory, Models, and Some New Findings (We stview,
1980).
94. Anderson, N. B., Bulatao, R. A. & Cohen, B., National Research Council (US) Panel
on Race, Ethnicity, and Health in Later Life. Critical Perspectives on Racial and Ethnic
Dierences in Health in Late Life (National Academies Press, 2004).
95. Winkleby, M. A. & Cubbin, C. Inluence of individual and neighbourhood socioeconomic
status on mortality among black, Mexican-American, and white women and men in the
United States. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 57, 444–452 (2003).
96. Hall, G. C., Yip, T. & Zárate, M. A. On becoming multicultural in a monocultural research
world: a conceptual approach to studying ethnocultural diversity. Am. Psychol. 71, 40–51
(2016).
97. Balci, S., Spanhel, K., Sander, L. B. & Baumeister, H. Culturally adapting internet- and
mobile-based health promotion interventions might not be worth the eort: a systematic
review and meta-analysis. NPJ Digit. Med 5, 34 (2022).
98. Herbst, J. H. et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of behavioral interventions to
reduce HIV risk behaviors of hispanics in the United States and Puerto Rico. AIDS Behav.
11, 25–47 (2007).
99. Hernandez Robles, E., Maynard, B. R., Salas-Wright, C. P. & Todic, J. Culturally adapted
substance use interventions for Latino adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
Res. Soc. Work. Pract. 28, 789–801 (2018).
100. Dweck, C. S. & Yeager, D. S. Mindsets: a view from two eras. Perspect. Psychol . Sci. 14,
481–496 (2019).
101. Yeager, D. S. & Dweck, C. S. Mindsets that promote resilience: when students
believe that personal characteristics can be developed. Educ. Psychol. 47, 302–314
(2012).
102. Dweck, C. S. Mindsets: how to motivate students (and yourself). Educ. Horiz. 91, 16–21
(2016).
103. Tannenbaum, M. B. et al. Appealing to fear: a meta-analysis of fear appeal eectiveness
and theories. Psychol. Bull. 141, 1178–1204 (2015).
104. Rosenstock, I. M. The health belief model and personal health behavior. Health Educ.
Monographs 2, 324–473 (1974).
105. Rosenstock, I. M. The health belief model and preventive health behavior. Health Educ.
Behav. https://doi.org/10.1177/109019817400200405 (1977).
106. Patterson, N. M., Bates, B. R., Chadwick, A. E., Nieto-Sanchez, C. & Grijalva, M. J. Using the
health belief model to identify communication opportunities to prevent Chagas disease
in southern Ecuador. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 12, e0006841 (2018).
107. Zhao, Y., Jiang, Y., Zhang, W. & Zhu, Y. Relationship between risk perception, emotion,
and coping behavior during public health emergencies: a systematic review and
meta-analysis. Systems 11, 181 (2023).
108. Hareli, S. & Parkinson, B. What’s social about social emotions? J. Theory Soc. Behav. 38,
131–156 (2008).
109. Sznycer, D., Sell, A. & Lieberman, D. Forms and functions of the social emotions. Curr. Dir.
Psychol. Sci. 30, 292–299 (2021).
110. Sârbescu, P. & Rusu, A. Personality predictors of speeding: anger-aggression and
impulsive-sensation seeking. A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Saf. Res. 77,
86–98 (2021).
111. Akbari, M. et al. Meta-analysis of the correlation between personality characteristics and
risky driving behaviors. J. Inj. Violence Res. 11, 107–122 (2019).
112. Li, M., Xu, X. & Kwan, H. K. The antecedents and consequences of workplace envy:
a meta-analytic review. Asia Pac. J. Manag. 40, 1–35 (2023).
113. Bamberg, S. & Möser, G. Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: a new meta-
analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. J. Env. Psychol.
27, 14–25 (2007).
114. Renshaw, T. L . & Steeves, R. M. O. What good is gratitude in youth and schools?
A systematic review and meta-analysis of correlates and intervention outcomes.
Psychol. Sch. 53, 286–305 (2016).
115. Montoya, R. M., Kershaw, C. & Prosser, J. L. A meta-analytic investigation of the relation
between interpersonal attraction and enacted behavior. Psychol. Bull. 144, 673–709
(2018).
116. Talaska, C. A., Fiske, S. T. & Chaiken, S. Legitimating racial discrimination: emotions, not
beliefs, best predict discrimination in a meta-analysis. Soc. Justice Res. 21, 263–296
(2008).
117. Kranzbühler, A . M., Zerres, A., Kleijnen, M. H. P. & Verlegh, P. W. J. Beyond valence: a
meta-analysis of discrete emotions in irm–customer encounters. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 48,
478–498 (2020).
118. Lench, H. C., Flores, S. A. & Bench, S. W. Discrete emotions predict change s in cognition,
judgment, experience, behavior, and physiology: a meta-analysis of experimental
emotion elicitations. Psychol. Bull. 37, 834–855 (2011).
119. Evers, C., Dingemans, A., Junghans, A. F. & Boevé, A. Feeling bad or feeling good, does
emotion aect your consumption of food? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 92, 195–208 (2018).
120. Li, S. X., Ye, Z., Whelan, K. & Truby, H. The eect of communicating the genetic risk of
cardiometabolic disorders on motivation and actual engagement in preventative lifestyle
modiication and clinical outcome: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised
controlled trials. Br. J. Nutr. 116, 924–934 (2016).
121. White, B. X. & Albarracín, D. Investigating belief falsehood. Fear appeals do change
behaviour in experimental laboratory studies. A commentary on Kok et al. (2018).
Health Psychol. Rev. 12, 147–150 (2018).
122. Peters, G. J. Y., Ruiter, R. A. C. & Kok, G. Threatening communication: a critical re-analysis
and a revised meta-analytic test of fear appeal theory. Health Psychol. Rev. 7, S8–S31
(2013).
123. Sheeran, P., Harris, P. R. & Epton, T. Does heightening risk appraisals change people’s
intentions and behavior? A meta-analysis of experimental studies. Psychol. Bull. 140,
511–543 (2014).
124. Constantino, S. M., Pianta, S., Rinscheid, A., Frey, R. & Weber, E. U. The source is the
message: the impact of institutional signals on climate change-related norm perceptions
and behaviors. Clim. Change 166, 1–20 (2021).
125. Dickens, L. R. Using gratitude to promote positive change: a series of meta-analyses
investigating the eectiveness of gratitude interventions. Basic. Appl. Soc. Psych. 39,
193–208 (2017).
126. Lechuga, J., Prieto, C., Mata, H., Belknap, R. A. & Varela, I. Culture and sexuality-related
communication as sociocultural precursors of HPV vaccination among mother-daughter
dyads of Mexican descent. Prev. Med. Rep. 9, 101105 (2020).
127. Ajzen, I. & Madden, T. J. Prediction of goal directed behavior: attitudes, intentions, and
perceived behavioral control. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 22, 453–474 (1986).
128. Plata, M. G., Laghi, F., Zammuto, M. & Pastorelli, C. Refusal self-eicacy and alcohol-
related behaviours in community samples: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
Curr. Psychol. 42 https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03954-7 (2023).
129. Yang, X. Y., Li, Z. J. & Sun, J. Eects of cognitive behavioral therapy-based intervention on
improving glycaemic, psychological, and physiological outcomes in adult patients with
diabetes mellitus: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Front. Psychiatry 11,
711 (2020).
130. Albarracin, D. et al. Persuasive communications to change actions: an analysis of
behavioral and cognitive impact in HIV prevention. Health Psychol. 22, 166–177 (2003).
131. Duncan, T. E., Duncan, S. C., Beauchamp, N., Wells, J. & Ary, D. V. Development and
evaluation of an interactive CD-ROM refusal skills program to prevent youth substance
use: “refuse to use”. J. Behav. Med. 23, 59–72 (2000).
132. Scheier, L . M., Botvin, G. J., Diaz, T. & Griin, K. W. Social skills, competence, and drug
refusal eicacy as predictors of adolescent alcohol use. J. Drug. Educ. 29, 251–278
(1999).
133. Wynn, S. R., Schulenberg, J., Maggs, J. L. & Zucker, R. A. Preventing alcohol misuse:
the impact of refusal skills and norms. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 14, 36–47 (2000).
134. St Kelly, J. A. et al. Community AIDS/HIV risk reduction: the eects of endorsements
by popular people in three cities. Am. J. Public. Health 82, 1483–1489 (1992).
135. Gause, N. K., Brown, J. L., Welge, J. & Northern, N. Meta-analyses of HIV prevention
interventions targeting improved partner communication: eects on partner
communication and condom use frequency outcomes. J. Behav. Med. 41, 423–440
(2018).
136. Arthur, W., Bennett, W., Edens, P. S. & Bell, S. T. Eectiveness of training in organizations:
a meta-analysis of design and evaluation features. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 234–245 ( 2003).
137. Takacs, Z. K. & Kassai, R. The eicacy of dierent interventions to foster children’s
executive function skills: a series of meta-analyses. Psychol. Bull. 145, 653–697 (2019).
138. Albarracin, D., Fishbein, M., Johnson, B. T. & Muellerleile, P. A. Theories of reasoned action
and planned behavior as models of condom use: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 127,
142–161 (2001).
139. Glasman, L. R. & Albarracín, D. Forming attitudes that predict future behavior:
a meta-analysis of the attitude–behavior relation. Psychol. Bull. 132, 778–822 (2006).
140. Starfelt Sutton, L. C. et al. Predicting sun-protective intentions and behaviours using the
theory of planned behaviour: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Health 31,
1272–1292 (2016).
141. Lanzini, P. & Khan, S. A. Shedding light on the psychological and behavioral determinants
of travel mode choice: a meta-analysis. Transp. Res. Part. F. Traic Psychol. Behav. 48,
13–27 (2017).
142. Scalco, A., Novent a, S., Sartori, R. & Ceschi, A. Predicting organic food consumption:
a meta-analytic structural equation model based on the theory of planned behavior.
Appetite 112, 235–248 (2017).
143. Albarracín, D. et al. When communications collide with recipients’ actions: eects of
post-message behavior on intentions to follow the message recommendation. Pers. Soc.
Psychol. Bull. 29, 834–845 (2003).
144. Magill, M. et al. The technical hypothesis of motivational interviewing: a meta-analysis
of MI’s key causal model. J. C onsult. Clin. Psychol. 82, 973–983 (2014).
145. Miller, W. R. & Rose, G. S. Toward a theory of motivational interviewing. Am. Psychol 64,
527–537 (2009).
146. Albarracín, D. Cognition in persuasion: an analysis of information processing in response
to persuasive communications. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 34, 61–130 (2002).
147. Albarracin, D. & Wyer, R. S. Jr Elaborative and nonelaborative processing of a behavior-related
communication. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 27, 691–705 (2001).
148. Ouellette, J. A. & Wood, W. Habit and intention in everyday life: the multiple processe s
by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychol. Bull. 124, 54–74 (1998).
149. Wood, W. & Neal, D. T. A new look at habits and the habit–goal interface. Psychol. Rev.
114, 843–863 (2007).
150. Wood, W. & Rünger, D. Psychology of habit. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 289–314 (2016).
151. Orbell, S. & Verplanken, B. The strength of habit. Health Psychol. Rev. 9, 311–317
(2015).
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
152. Dimatteo, M. R. Social support and patient adherence to medical treatment: a meta-analysis.
Health Psychol. 23, 207–218 (2004).
153. C arron, A. V., Hausenblas, H. A. & Mack, D. Social inluence and exercise: a meta-analysis.
J. Sport. Exerc. Psychol. 18, 1–16 (1996).
154. Andresen, P. A. & Telleen, S. L. The relationship between social support and maternal
behaviors and attitudes: a meta-analytic review. Am. J. Community Psychol. 20, 753–774
(1992).
155. Pottho, S. et al. The relationship between habit and healthcare professional behaviour
in clinical practice: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychol. Rev. 13, 73–90
(2019).
156. Gardner, B. & Abraham, C. Psychological correlates of car use: a meta-analysis.
Trans. Res. Part. F—Traic Psychol. Behav. 11, 300–311 (2008).
157. McGuire, J. F. et al. A meta-analysis of behavior therapy for Tourette syndrome.
J. Psychiatr. Re s. 50, 106–112 (2014).
158. Wile, D. J. & Pringsheim, T. M. Behavior therapy for Tourette syndrome: a systematic
review and meta-analysis. Curr. Treat. Options Neurol. 15, 385–395 (2013).
159. Wolz, I., Nannt, J. & Svaldi, J. Laboratory-based interventions targeting food craving:
a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes. Rev. 21, e12996 (2020).
160. Turton, R., Bruidegom, K., Cardi, V., Hirsch, C. R. & Treasure, J. Novel methods to help
develop healthier eating habits for eating and weight disorders: a systematic review
and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 61, 132–155 (2016).
161. Asch, D. A. & Rosin, R. Engineering social incentives for health. N. Engl. J. Med. 375,
2511–2513 (2016).
162. Brehm, J. W. A Theory of Psychological Reactance (Academic, 1966).
163. Brehm, J. Responses to Loss of Freedom: A Theory of Psychological Reactance (General
Learning, 1972).
164. Trang, S. & Brendel, B. A meta-analysis of deterrence theory in information security
policy compliance research. Inf. Syst. Front. 21, 1265–1284 (2019).
165. Navin, M. C. et al. Recent vaccine mandates in the United States, Europe and Australia:
a comparative study. Vaccine 36, 7377–7384 (2018).
166. Largent, E. A. et al. US public attitudes toward COVID-19 vaccine mandates. JAMA Netw.
Open. 3, 2019–2022 (2020).
167. Albarracin, D., Jung, H., Song, W., Tan, A. & Fishman, J. Rather than inducing
psychological reactance, requiring vaccination strengthens intentions to vaccinate
in US populations. Sci. Rep. 11, 1–9 (2021).
168. Hovland, C. I. & Weiss, W. The inluence of source credibility on communication
eectiveness. Public. Opin. Q. 15, 635–650 (1951).
169. Kumkale, G. T. & Albarracín, D. The sleeper eect in persuasion: a meta-analytic review.
Psychol. Bull. 130, 143–172 (2004).
170. Kumkale, G. T., Albarracín, D. & Seignourel, P. J. The eects of source credibility in
the presence or absence of prior attitudes: implications for the design of persuasive
communication campaigns. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1325–1356 (2010).
171. Albarracín, D., Kumkale, G. T. & Johnson, B. T. Inluences of social power and normative
support on condom use decisions: a research synthesis. AIDS Care 16, 700–723 ( 2004).
172. Albarracín, D., Kumkale, G. T. T. & Vento, P. P. D. How people can become persuaded
by weak messages presented by credible communicators: not all sleeper eects are
created equal. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 68, 171–180 (2017).
173. Durantini, M. R., Albarracin, D., Mitchell, A. L., Earl, A. N. & Gillette, J. C. Conceptualizing
the inluence of social agents of behavior change: a meta-analysis of the eectiveness
of HIV-prevention interventionists for dierent groups. Psychol. Bull. 132, 212–248 (2006).
174. Albarracín, D., Durantini, M. R. & Earl, A. N. E. Empirical and theoretical concludions of an
analysis of outcomes of HIV-prevention interventions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 73–78
(2006).
175. Balliet, D., Wu, J. & De Dreu, C. K. W. Ingroup favoritism in cooperation: a meta-analysis.
Psychol. Bull. 140, 1556–1581 (2014).
176. De Jong, B. A., Dirks, K. T. & Gillespie, N. Trust and team performance: a meta-analysis
of main eects, moderators, and covariates. J. Appl. Psychol. 101, 1134–1150 (2016).
177. Legood, A., van der Wer, L., Lee, A. & Den Hartog, D. A meta-analysis of the role of trust
in the leadership–performance relationship. Eur. J. Work. Organ. Psychol. 30, 1–22 (2021).
178. Cologna, V. & Siegrist, M. The role of trust for climate change mitigation and adaptation
behaviour: a meta-analysis. J. Env. Psychol . 69, 101428 (2020).
179. Devine, D. et al. Political trust in the irst year of the COVID-19 pandemic: a meta-analysis
of 67 studies. J. Eur. Public Policy https://doi.org/10.1080/13501763.2023.2169741
(2023).
180. Mosley, M. The Malleability of Trust in the Backdrop of Disparities: A Meta-Analysis of
Experimental Interventions Building Trust in Healthcare Settings. Bachelor’s thesis, Univ.
Illinois (2020).
181. Cohen-Charash, Y. & Spector, P. E. The role of justice in organizations: a meta-analysis.
Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 86, 278–321 (2001).
182. Lee, C. M., Geisner, I. M., Lewis, M. A., Neighbors, C. & Larimer, M. E. Social motives and
the interaction between descriptive and injunctive norms in college student drinking.
J. Stud. Alcohol. Drugs 68, 714–721 (2007).
183. Cialdini, R. B. & Goldstein, N. J. Social inluence: compliance and conformity. Annu. Rev.
Psychol. 55, 591–621 (2004).
184. Sunguya, B. F., Munisamy, M., Pongpanich, S., Yasuoka, J. & Jimba, M. Ability of HIV
advocacy to modify behavioral norms and treatment impact: a systematic review.
Am. J. Public. Health 106, E1–E8 (2016).
185. Rhodes, N., Shulman, H. C. & McClaran, N. Changing norms: a meta-analytic integration
of research on social norms appeals. Hum. Commun. Res. 46, 161–191 (2020).
186. Abrahamse, W. & Steg, L. Social inluence approaches to encourage resource
conservation: a meta-analysis. Glob. Environ. Change 23, 1773–1785 (2013).
187. Prentice, D. A. & Miller, D. T. Pluralistic ignorance and the perpetuation of social norms
by unwitting actors. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 28, 161–209 (1996).
188. Schroeder, C. M. & Prentice, D. A. Exposing pluralistic ignorance to reduce alcohol use
among college students. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 28, 2150–2180 (1998).
189. Fishbach, A. & Trope, Y. The substitutability of external control and self-control. J. Exp.
Soc. Psychol. 41, 256–270 (2005).
190. Shea, S., DuMouchel, W. & Bahamonde, L. A meta-analysis of 16 randomized controlled
trials to evaluate computer-based clinical reminder systems for preventive care in the
ambulatory setting. J. Am. Med. Inform. Assoc. 3, 399–409 (1996).
191. Yamin, P., Fei, M., Lahlou, S. & Levy, S. Using social norms to change behavior
and increase sustainability in the real world: a systematic review of the literature.
Sustainability 11, 5847 (2019).
192. Dotson, K. B., Dunn, M. E. & Bowers, C. A. Stand-alone personalized normative feedback
for college student drinkers: a meta-analytic review, 2004 to 2014. PLoS ONE 10, 1–17
(2015).
193. Chun, J. S., Brockner, J. & De Cremer, D. How temporal and social comparisons in
performance evaluation aect fairness perceptions. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process.
145, 1–15 (2018).
194. Nolan, J. M. Social norm interventions as a tool for pro-climate change. Curr. Opin.
Psychol. 42, 120–125 (2021).
195. Tong, H. L. & Laranjo, L. The use of social features in mobile health interventions to
promote physical activity: a systematic review. NPJ Digit. Med. 1 https://doi.org/10.1038/
s41746-018-0051-3 (2018).
196. Morgan, H. et al. Beneits of incentives for breastfeeding and smoking cessation in
pregnancy (BIBS): a mixed-methods study to inform trial design. Health Technol. Assess
19 https://doi.org/10.3310/hta19300 (2015).
197. Li, H., Wang, C., Chang, W. Y. & Liu, H. Factors aecting Chinese farmers’ environment-
friendly pesticide application behavior: a meta-analysis. J Clean Prod. 409, 137277
(2023).
198. Baum, W. M. Understanding Behaviorism: Science, Behavior, and Culture (Harpercollins
College Division, 1994).
199. Greene, D., Demeter, C. & Dolnicar, S. The comparative eectiveness of interventions aimed
at making tourists behave in more environmentally sustainable ways: a meta-analysis.
J. Travel Res. https://doi.org/10.1177/00472875231183701 (2023).
200. Bolívar, H. A. et al. Contingency management for patients receiving medication
for opioid use disorder a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 78,
1092–1102 (2021).
201. Turner, R. J., Frankel, B. G. & Levin, D. M. Social support: conceptualization, measurement,
and implications for mental health. Res. Community Ment. Health 3, 67–111 (1983).
202. Shushtari, Z. J., Salimi, Y., Sajjadi, H. & Paykani, T. Eect of social support inter ventions
on adherence to antiretroviral therapy among people living with HIV: a systematic review
and meta-analysis. AIDS Behav. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10461-022-03894-0 (2022).
203. Hou, X. et al. Methods and eicacy of social support interventions in preventing suicide:
a systematic review and meta-analysis. Evid. Based Ment. Health 25, 29–35 (2022).
204. Kiesler, C. A. The Psychology of Commitment (Academic, 1971).
205. Cannella, B. L ., Yarcheski, A. & Mahon, N. E . Meta-analyses of predictors of health
practices in pregnant women. West. J. Nurs. Res. 40, 425–446 (2018).
206. Vaessen, J. et al. The eects of microcredit on women’s control over household spending
in developing countries: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Campbell Syst. Rev. 10,
1–205 (2014).
207. Moreno, R. et al. Structural and community-level interventions for increasing condom
use to prevent the transmission of HIV and other sexually transmitted infections.
Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD003363.pub3 (2014).
208. Jachimowicz, J. M., Duncan, S., Weber, E. U. & Johnson, E. J. When and why defaults
inluence decisions: a meta-analysis of default eects. Behav. Public. Policy 3, 159–186
(2019).
209. White, B. X., Jiang, D. & Albarracín, D. The limits of defaults: the inluence of decision time
on default eects. Soc. Cogn. 39, 543–569 (2021).
210. Wagenaar, A. C. & Toomey, T. L. Eects of minimum drinking age laws: review and
analyses of the literature from 1960 to 2000. J. Stud. Alcohol. 63, 206–225 (2002).
211. Hedges, L. & Olkin, I. Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis (Academic Pre ss, 1985).
212. Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T. & Rothstein, H. R. Introduction to
Meta-Analysis (Wiley, 2009).
213. Jemmott, L. S. & Jemmot, J. D. Sexual knowledge attitudes and risky sexual behavior
among inner city black male adolescents. J. Adolesc. Res. 5, 346–369 (1990).
214. Rhoades, B. L., Greenberg, M. T. & Domitrovich, C. E. The contribution of inhibitory
control to preschoolers’ social-emotional competence. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol . 30,
310–320 (2009).
215. Ajzen, I., Fishbein, M., Lohmann, S. & Albarracín, D. in The Handbook of Attitudes, Volume 1:
Basic Principles 2nd ed. (eds Albarracín, D. & Johnson, B. T.) 197–225 (Routledge, 2018).
216. Schultz, P. W. & Oskamp, S. Eort as a moderator of the attitude–behavior relationship:
general environmental concern and recycling. Quarterly 59, 375–383 (1996).
217. Ostain, B. D., Marlatt, G. A. & Greenwald, A. G. Drinking without thinking: an implicit
measure of alcohol motivation predicts failure to control alcohol use. Behav. Res. Ther.
46, 1210–1219 (2008).
218. Albarracin, D. Action and Inaction in a Social World: Predicting and Changing Attitudes
and Behaviors (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2021).
Nature Reviews Psychology
Review article
219. Bierwiaczonek, K., Kunst, J. R. & Pich, O. Belief in COVID-19 conspiracy theories
reduces social distancing over time. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 12, 1270–1285
(2020).
220. Feng, Y. & Tong, Q. Exploring the mediating role of situation awarene ss and crisis
emotions between social media use and COVID-19 protective behaviors: cross-sectional
study. Front. Public Health 10, 793033 (2022).
221. Terry, D. J. & Hogg, M. A. Group norms and the attitude behavior relationship: a role for
group identiication. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 22, 776–793 (1996).
222. Gollwitzer, P. M. Implementation intentions: strong eects of simple plans. Am. Psychol.
54, 493–503 (1999).
223. Wieber, F., Thuermer, J. L. & Gollwitzer, P. M. Promoting the translation of intentions into
action by implementation intentions: behavioral eects and physiological correlates.
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 395 (2015).
224. Fornara, F., Carrus, G., Passafaro, P. & Bonnes, M. Distinguishing the sources of normative
inluence on proenvironmental behaviors: the role of local norms in household waste
recycling. Group. Process. Intergroup Relat. 14, 623–635 (2011).
225. Bargh, J. A. in Handbook of Social Cognition (ed. Wyer R. S.) 1–40 (Lawrence Erlbaum,
1994).
226. Neal, D. T., Wood, W., Labrecque, J. S. & Lally, P. How do habits guide behavior? Perceived
and actual triggers of habits in daily life. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.jesp.2011.10.011 (2012).
227. Gillebaart, M., Ybema, J. F. & de Ridder, D. T. D. Make it a habit: how habit strength, goal
importance and self-control predict hand washing behaviour over time during the
COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol. Health 37, 1528–1546 ( 2022).
228. Fayaz-Farkhad, B., Jung, H. A., Calabrese, C. J. & Albarracin, D. A culture of vaccination:
how state policies produce social norms. Sci Rep. 13, 21227 (2023).
229. Lemstra, M., Neudorf, C. & Opondo, J. Implications of a public smoking ban. Can. J.
Public. Health 99, 62–65 (2008).
230. Shaw, J. et al. Immunization mandates, vaccination coverage, and exemption rates in the
United States. Open. Forum Infect. Dis. 5, 1–9 (2018).
231. Maclean, J. C., Pichler, S. & Ziebarth, N. R. Mandated Sick Pay: Coverage, Utilization,
and Welfare Eects (Working Paper 2832) (National Bureau of Economic Research,
2020).
232. Fairbrother, M. When will people pay to pollute? Environmental taxes, political trust and
experimental evidence from britain. Br. J. Polit. Sci. 49, 661–682 (2019).
233. De Cremer, D. & Tyler, T. R. The eects of trust in authority and procedural fairness on
cooperation. J. Appl. Psychol. 92, 639–649 ( 2007).
234. Peyton, K., Sierra-Arévalo, M. & Rand, D. G. A ield experiment on community policing
and police legitimacy. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116, 19894–19898 (2019).
235. Fishbein, M., Higgins, D., Wolitski, R., Guenther-Grey, C. & Johnson, W. Community-
level HIV intervention in 5 cities: inal outcome data from the CDC AIDS community
demonstration projects. Am. J. Public. Health 89, 336–345 (1999).
236. Burger, J. M. & Shelton, M. Changing everyday health behaviors through descriptive
norm manipulations. Soc. Inlu. 6, 69–77 (2011).
237. Bruera, S., Barbo, A. G. & Lopez-Olivo, M. A . Use of medication reminders in patients with
rheumatoid arthritis. Rheumatol. Int. 36, 1543–1548 (2016).
238. Szilagyi, P. G. et al. Text message reminders for child inluenza vaccination in the setting
of school-located inluenza vaccination: a randomized clinical trial. Clin. Pediatr. 58,
428–436 (2019).
239. Schultz, P. W., Nolan, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J. & Griskevicius, V. The
constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychol. Sci. 18,
429–434 (2007).
240. Kearney, M. S. State lotteries and consumer behavior. J. Public. Econ. 89, 2269–2299
(2005).
241. Campos-Mercade, P. et al. Monetary incentives increase COVID-19 vaccinations. Sci. 374,
879–882 (2021).
242. Veiel, H. O. F. The Mannheim interview on social support. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr.
Epidemiol. 25, 250–259 (1990).
243. Keller, C. et al. A comparison of a social support physical activity intervention in weight
management among post-partum Latinas. BMC Public. Health 14, 971 (2014).
244. Owens, J., Dickerson, S. & Macintosh, D. L. Demographic covariates of residential
recycling eiciency. Env. Behav. 32, 637–650 (2000).
245. Fayaz Farkhad, B., Karan, A. & Albarracín, D. Longitudinal pathways to inluenza
vaccination vary with socio-structural disadvantages. Ann. Behav. Med. https://doi.org/
10.1093/abm/kaab087 (2021).
246. Maciejewski, M. L., Farley, J. F., Parker, J. & Wansink, D. Copayment reductions generate
greater medication adherence in targeted patients. Health A. 29, 2002–2008 (2010).
247. Ganey, A. & McCormick, D. The aordable care act: implications for health-care equity.
Lancet. 389, 1442–1452 (2017).
248. Fouksman, E. & Klein, E. Radical transformation or technological intervention? Two paths
for universal basic income. World Dev. 122, 492–500 (2019).
Acknowledgements
The authors thank M. Leung for assistance in checking eect sizes and references. The
research was funded by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grants R01MH132415, R01 AI147487,
DP1 DA048570, R01 MH114847 and NSF 2031972 to D.A., and by the Annenberg Foundation
Endowment to the Division of Communication Science at the Annenberg Public Policy Center.
Author contributions
All authors researched data for the article and extracted eect sizes. All authors contributed
substantially to discussion of the content. D.A. wrote the main sections of the article and all
authors contributed to the irst draft and all revisions. All authors reviewed and/or edited the
manuscript before submission.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Supplementary information The online version contains supplementary material available at
https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-024-00305-0.
Peer review information Nature Reviews Psychology thanks Taciano Milfont, Richard Petty and
the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional ailiations.
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this
article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-
archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms
of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
© Springer Nature America, Inc. 2024
Article
Why do we act on habit even when we intend to do something else? The answer lies in habit memories, or context-response associations, that form when people repeat rewarding actions in stable contexts. Although habits can form as people pursue goals, once habits develop, the perception of the context directly activates the response in mind. Because habit activation does not depend strongly on motivation, changing intentions has limited impact on habit memory. Instead, successful habit-change interventions directly impact the behavior itself: Along with classic behavior therapy interventions, habits change with (a) reward systems that form new habits, (b) disruption of context cues to forestall activation of the habit in mind, and (c) friction that makes the habitual response difficult and alternatives easier. Despite the strong evidence that habits are activated by contexts, people tend to believe that their own habits are a product of goal pursuit. This subjective reality might also explain why some researchers continue to maintain that habit performance depends on goals.
Article
Full-text available
To examine how well the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior predict condom use, the authors synthesized 96 data sets (N = 22,594) containing associations between the models' key variables. Consistent with the theory of reasoned action's predictions, (a) condom use was related to intentions (weighted mean r. = .45), (b) intentions were based on attitudes (r. = .58) and subjective norms (r. = .39), and (c) attitudes were associated with behavioral beliefs (r. = .56) and norms were associated with normative beliefs (r. = .46). Consistent with the theory of planned behavior's predictions, perceived behavioral control was related to condom use intentions (r. = .45) and condom use (r. = .25), but in contrast to the theory, it did not contribute significantly to condom use. The strength of these associations, however, was influenced by the consideration of past behavior. Implications of these results for HIV prevention efforts are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
This study assessed the effects of 3 theoretically grounded, school-based HIV prevention interventions on inner-city minority high school students’ levels of HIV prevention information, motivation, behavioral skills, and behavior. It involved a quasi-experimental controlled trial comparing classroom-based, peer-based, and combined classroom- and peer-based HIV prevention interventions with a standard-of-care control condition in 4 urban high schools (N = 1,532, primarily 9th-grade students). At 12 months postintervention, the classroom-based intervention resulted in sustained changes in HIV prevention behavior. This article discusses why both of the interventions involving peers were less effective than the classroom-based intervention at the 12-month follow-up and, more generally, suggests a set of possible limiting conditions for the efficacy of peer-based interventions.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to determine the extent to which refusal skills and norm setting mediated the impact of a school-based prevention program from the Alcohol Misuse Prevention Study (AMPS) on adolescent alcohol overindulgence. The AMPS is a randomized, pre-post, experimental-control study. Respondents in the present study included 6th through 10th graders (ns ranged from 232 to 371). Structural equation modeling analyses using EQS indicated that norm setting mediated the effect of the intervention on alcohol overindulgence at the 7th through the 8th grade and at the 8th through the 10th grade. In contrast, although the prevention program served to increase refusal skills, refusal skills did not mediate the effect of the program on alcohol misuse.
Article
Full-text available
To study the processes by which past behavior influences future behavior, participants were led to believe that without being aware of it, they had expressed either support for or opposition to the institution of comprehensive exams. Judgment and response time data suggested that participants' perceptions of their past behavior often influenced their decisions to repeat the behavior. This influence was partly the result of cognitive activity that influenced participants' cognitions about specific behavioral consequences and the attitude they based on these cognitions. More generally, however, feedback about past behavior had a direct effect on participants' attitudes and ultimate behavioral decisions that was independent of the outcome-specific cognitions. Results are discussed in terms of their implications for biased scanning of memory, dissonance reduction, self-perception, and the use of behavior as a heuristic.
Article
Full-text available
Despite increasing incidence of HIV/AIDS, there has been no systematic review of correlates of condom use among heterosexual samples. To rectify this, the present study used meta-analysis to quantify the relationship between psychosocial variables and self-reported condom use. Six hundred sixty correlations distributed across 44 variables were derived from 121 empirical studies. Variables were organized in terms of the labeling, commitment, and enactment stages of the AIDS Risk Reduction Model (Catania, Kegeles, & Coates, 1990). Findings showed that demographic, personality, and labeling stage variables had small average correlations with condom use. Commitment and enactment stage variables fared better, with attitudes toward condoms, behavioral intentions, and communication about condoms being the most important predictors. Overall, findings support a social psychological model of condom use highlighting the importance of behavior-specific cognitions, social interaction, and preparatory behaviors rather than knowledge and beliefs about the threat of infection.
Article
Full-text available
Psychological determinants of AIDS-preventive behaviors were examined from the perspective of the theory of reasoned action in prospective studies of gay men, heterosexual university students, and heterosexual high school students. Across samples, preventive behaviors, and prospective intervals of 1 and 2 months' duration, AIDS-preventive behaviors were predicted by behavioral intentions; behavioral intentions were a function of attitudes and norms; and attitudes and norms were a function of their theorized basic underpinnings. Discussion focuses on the development of AIDS-prevention interventions that modify intentions, attitudes, and norms concerning performance of AIDS-preventive behaviors by targeting the empirically identified underpinnings of attitudes and norms related to specific preventive behaviors in specific populations of interest.
Article
Full-text available
Complex mechanisms exist between public risk perception, emotions, and coping behaviors during health emergencies. To unravel the relationship between these three phenomena, a meta-analytic approach was employed in this study. Using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis 3.0, 81 papers were analyzed after selection. The results of the meta-analysis showed that (1) risk perception (perceived severity, perceived susceptibility) and negative emotions (especially fear) are both correlated with coping behaviors; (2) risk perception is strongly correlated with fear and moderately correlated with anxiety; and (3) anxiety predicts the adoption of coping behaviors. The existing research provided an empirical basis for implementing effective coping behavior interventions and implied that management decisionmakers need to consider reasonable interventions through multiple channels to maintain the public’s risk perception and emotions within appropriate levels. Finally, future research directions are suggested.
Article
Full-text available
Behavioral change is essential to mitigate climate change. To advance current knowledge, we synthesize research on interventions aiming to promote climate change mitigation behaviors in field settings. In a preregistered second-order meta-analysis, we assess the overall effect of 10 meta-analyses, incorporating a total of 430 primary studies. In addition, we assess subgroup analyses for six types of interventions, five behaviors, and three publication bias adjustments. Results showed that climate change mitigation interventions were generally effective (dunadjusted = 0.31, 95% CI [0.30, 0.32]). A follow-up analysis using only unique primary studies, adjusted for publication bias, provides a more conservative overall estimate (d = 0.18, 95% CI [0.13, 0.24]). This translates into a mean treatment effect of 7 percentage points. Furthermore, in a subsample of adequately powered large-scale interventions (n > 9,000, k = 32), the effect was adjusted downward to approximately 2 percentage points. This discrepancy might be because large-scale interventions often target nonvoluntary participants by less direct techniques (e.g., “home energy reports”) while small-scale interventions often target voluntary participants by more direct techniques (e.g., face-to-face interactions). Subgroup analyses showed that interventions based on social comparisons or financial incentives were the most effective, while education or feedback was the least effective. These results provide a comprehensive state-of-the-art summary of climate change mitigation interventions, guiding both future research and practice.
Article
Tourism generates 8% of all greenhouse gas emissions. One way of reducing emissions is to deploy behavioral change interventions that entice tourists to behave in more sustainable ways. In search of the most effective approaches, we conducted a meta-analysis of 118 interventions tested in field experiments in the tourism context. Most studies targeted beliefs and focused on towel reuse, food waste, or resource use. Changing choice architecture ( d = 1.40) and increasing pleasure ( d = 0.66) emerge as the most effective approaches. Imposing penalties for unsustainable behavior ( d = −0.12) and leveraging social norms to trigger sustainable behavior ( d = 0.18) have limited effectiveness. Future work should re-direct attention from designing interventions that modify beliefs toward interventions that change choice architecture or increase the pleasure associated with the desired behavior, and aim at changing a wider range of behaviors, including green transportation and the avoidance of single use plastics.