ArticlePublisher preview available

The Influence of Age and Gender on Affect, Physiology, and Their Interrelations: A Study of Long-Term Marriages

Authors:
  • THE GOTTMAN INSTITUTE
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

Self-reported affect and autonomic and somatic physiology were studied during three 15-min conversations (events of the day, problem area, pleasant topic) in a sample of 151 couples in long-term marriages. Couples differed in age (40–50 or 60–70) and marital satisfaction (satisfied or dissatisfied). Marital interaction in older couples was associated with more affective positivity and lower physiological arousal (even when controlling for affective differences) than in middle-age couples. As has previously been found with younger couples, marital dissatisfaction was associated with less positive affect, greater negative affect, and greater negative affect reciprocity. In terms of the relation between physiological arousal and affective experience, husbands reported feeling more negative the more they were physiologically aroused; for wives, affect and arousal were not correlated. These findings are related to theories of socioemotional change with age and of gender differences in marital behavior and health.
INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS AND GROUP
PROCESSES
The Influence of Age and Gender on Affect, Physiology, and Their
Interrelations: A Study of Long-Term Marriages
Robert
W.
Levenson, Laura
L.
Carstensen, and John M. Gottman
Self-reported affect and autonomic and somatic physiology were studied during three 15-min con-
versations (events of the day, problem area, pleasant topic) in a sample of
151
couples in long-term
marriages. Couples differed in age (40-50 or 60-70) and marital satisfaction (satisfied or dissatis-
fied). Marital interaction in older couples was associated with more affective positivity and lower
physiological arousal (even when controlling for affective differences) than in middle-age
couples.
As
has previously been found with younger couples, marital dissatisfaction was associated with less
positive affect, greater negative affect, and greater negative affect reciprocity. In terms of the relation
between physiological arousal and affective experience, husbands reported feeling more negative the
more they were physiologically aroused; for wives, affect and arousal were not correlated. These
findings are related
to
theories of socioemotional
change
with
age
and of gender differences in marital
behavior and health.
In the experiment described in this article,
we
applied obser-
vational methods to study affect and physiology in the interac-
tions in a sample of middle-aged and older couples who have
been married for most of their adult lives. This article focuses
on four
issues:
(a) age-related differences in the affective quality
of marital interaction; (b) age-related differences in the physio-
logical activation produced by marital interaction; (c) continu-
ity in later life of affective markers of marital distress; and (d)
gender differences in the covariation between subjective emo-
tional experience and physiological activation. We begin by
briefly reviewing the literatures relevant to each issue.
Affective Quality of Marriage in Late Life
Most research on marriage has studied relatively young cou-
ples in the early stages of
marriage.
Much less is known about
the nature of long-term marriages in later life.
Marital
Satisfaction
Early cross-sectional studies of marriage indicated that mar-
ital satisfaction steadily declined over time (Blood & Wolfe,
Robert
W.
Levenson, Department of
Psychology,
University of Cali-
fornia, Berkeley; Laura
L.
Carstensen, Department of Psychology, Stan-
ford University; John M. Gottman, Department of Psychology, Univer-
sity of Washington.
This research was supported by National Institute on Aging (NIA)
Grant AG07476 to Robert W. Levenson, NIA Grant AGO8816 to
Laura L. Carstensen, and National Institute of Mental Health Grant
MH42722 and Research Scientist Development
Award
1KO2MHOO257
to John M. Gottman.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Rob-
ert W. Levenson, Department of Psychology, University of California,
Berkeley, California 94720.
1960;
Dentler & Pineo, 1960; Pineo, 1961, 1969). Subsequent
studies that more fully sampled marriages across the entire life
span revealed a much less monotonic and much more complex
pattern, with marital satisfaction reaching low points after the
birth of the first child and when children are adolescents, and
reaching high points at the beginning of marriage and when
children leave home (Anderson, Russell, & Schumm, 1983;
Burr,
1970;
Cowan
&
Pape-Cowan,
1988;
Doherty
&
Jacobson,
1982;
Rollins & Cannon, 1974; Thurnher, 1976). According to
popular lore, retirement is thought to have a major impact on
marital
quality.
Although
some
data suggest that retirement im-
proves marital quality
(e.g.,
Atchley,
1976;
Dorfman
&
Heckert,
1988;
Gilford, 1984), a number of studies have found it to have
little effect (e.g., Ekerdt & Vinick, 1991; Matthews & Brown,
1987;
Vinick & Ekerdt, 1991) or slightly negative effects (e.g.,
Lee & Shehan, 1989). In the present study, to avoid possible
confounds, we limited the sample to couples who had not yet
retired.
Affective Characteristics
On the basis of questionnaire and interview data, older cou-
ples have been described as largely happy, affectionate, and
emotionally
close
(e.g.,
Erikson, Erikson,
&
Kivnick,
1986;
Par-
ron, 1982; Stinnett, Carter, & Montgomery, 1972). Guilford
and Bengtson's (1979) multigenerational study of families indi-
cated that positive interaction followed a path somewhat like
that of marital satisfaction
(i.e.,
highest in the youngest couples,
lowest in
the middle-aged
couples,
and intermediate in the older
couples); an orthogonal quality, negative sentiment, decreased
linearly with
age.
Thus, in their older
couples,
positive interac-
tion would
be
moderately high and negative sentiment would be
Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology,
1994,
Vol. 67, No.
1,56-68
Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association,
Inc.
0022-3514/94/S3.00
56
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Negative interactions between partners are associated with instant decreases in marital satisfaction, increases in unhappiness, and a higher likelihood of divorce (Gottman & Krokoff, 1989; for a review, see Fincham & Beach, 1999). Strong evidence suggests that individuals experience more negative emotions, such as anger and fear, and perceive more negative emotions from their partners during marital conflicts (Driver & Gottman, 2004;Levenson et al., 1994). The more negative emotions are experienced and perceived, the worse their marital satisfaction is . ...
... Previous research has shown that older adults reported better marital satisfaction, more positive and fewer negative emotions, more desirable interactions, and fewer undesirable interactions in a conflict than did younger adults (Seider et al., 2009). Older adults also exhibit more affectionate behaviors (Levenson et al., 1994), engage in more positive listening and communication (Pasupathi et al., 1999), demonstrate better conflict resolution skills (Levenson et al., 1994), perceive their partners more positively (Story et al., 2007) and engage more in conflict discussion (Srivastava et al., 2006) than do their younger counterparts. ...
... Previous research has shown that older adults reported better marital satisfaction, more positive and fewer negative emotions, more desirable interactions, and fewer undesirable interactions in a conflict than did younger adults (Seider et al., 2009). Older adults also exhibit more affectionate behaviors (Levenson et al., 1994), engage in more positive listening and communication (Pasupathi et al., 1999), demonstrate better conflict resolution skills (Levenson et al., 1994), perceive their partners more positively (Story et al., 2007) and engage more in conflict discussion (Srivastava et al., 2006) than do their younger counterparts. ...
Article
Full-text available
Older adults report higher marital satisfaction than younger adults even after negative interactions. The current study examined affect valuation as a potential moderator to explain age‐related differences in the relationship between negative interactions and marital satisfaction. We conducted a 14‐day daily diary study among 66 heterosexual couples (132 participants) aged from 21 to 80 years. At both person and daily levels, we found that valuing negative affect weakened the negative association between negative interactions and marital satisfaction in husbands. The moderating effect of negative affect valuation was stronger in older than younger husbands at the person but not daily level. Valuing positive affect more was associated with a stronger negative association between negative interactions and marital satisfaction in husbands at the person but not daily level. Such effect was again stronger in older than younger husbands. No significant results were found in wives. These findings shed light on the importance of affect valuation in understanding age‐related differences in marital dynamics.
... ; Levensen,Carstensen, & Gottman, 1994). And starting from puberty, girls tend to experience more episodes of depression as well as some types of fear compared to boys. ...
Article
This research paper investigates the correlation between individuals’ reactions to frustration and their values, with an emphasis on sex differences. Frustration is an arbitrary emotional response to obstacles or unmet expectations, although social values carry beliefs and principles that guide behavior within a societal context. Getting a gist of how sex influences these aspects can shed light on societal dynamics and inform interventions aimed at promoting psychological well-being and social harmony. The study undergoes a quantitative approach to the data, through different scales using SPSS to analyze data and gather comprehensive insights. Results reveal new patterns in how individuals of different genders perceive and respond to frustration in relation to their value system, Enlightening the need for gender-sensitive works in addressing emotional regulation and socialization processes.
... When dealing with interpersonal conflict, older adults are more likely to use avoidant and less confrontational strategies relative to younger adults (Birditt et al., 2005;Birditt & Fingerman, 2003;Lefkowitz & Fingerman, 2003). When avoidant strategies cannot be deployed, older adults appear to actively infuse the situation with positive affect (Carstensen et al., 1995;Levenson et al., 1994). A greater focus on emotionally fulfilling relationships may be associated with a deep desire to maintain social harmony and to keep the peace within one's social environment, allowing for more positive and fewer negative emotions and social experiences. ...
Article
Full-text available
Moral judgments and emotional reactions to sociomoral violations are heavily impacted by a perpetrator’s intentions and desires, which pose a threat to social harmony. Given that older adults are more motivated to maintain interpersonal harmony relative to younger adults, older adults may be more reactive to malicious desires. In three studies, we investigated adult age differences in moral judgments and emotional reactions to sociomoral violations. In all studies, participants read scenarios in which a perpetrator either (a) desired to harm another but nothing happened, or (b) harmed another accidentally without malicious desire. Study 2 incorporated additional scenarios designed to evoke anger and disgust without explicitly implicating another person to evaluate whether age differences emerge only when sociomoral violations against another are salient. In Study 3, we examined the combined effects of malicious desires and harmful outcomes by including scenarios in which (a) harmful desires were coupled with harmful outcomes, and (b) benign desires were coupled with benign outcomes. Predominantly across the studies, older adults judged perpetrators who desired to harm another more harshly but judged perpetrators who accidentally harmed another more leniently than younger adults. Emotional reactions generally corresponded with the differences in judgments. Taken together, this work suggests that desires more strongly impact older relative to younger adults’ judgments and emotional reactions in sociomoral contexts.
... When stressors are perceived as controllable and addressed with approach coping strategies (rather than passive/avoidance coping), the deleterious effects on cognitive flexibility tend to be mitigated (Palm & Follette, 2011;Quinn & Shields, 2023). Theoretical and empirical works suggest that in response to IPC, women adaptively push toward resolution of conflicts (i.e., approach coping), while men are more prone to maladaptively engage in passive coping strategies, such as escape, avoidance, and withdrawal (Cummings et al., 2010;Gottman & Levenson, 1988;Levenson et al., 1994;Story & Repetti, 2006). Thus, if fathers are more likely to perceive IPC as uncontrollable and passively cope by escaping or avoiding, they may be more vulnerable to stress-induced impairments in their cognitive flexibility. ...
Article
Full-text available
There is a well-documented interdependency between destructive interparental conflict (IPC) and parenting difficulties (i.e., spillover effect), yet little is known about the mechanisms that “carry” spillover between IPC and parenting. Guided by a cascade model framework, the current study used a longitudinal, multimethod, multi-informant design to examine a process model of spillover that tested whether parental executive functioning (working memory, cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control) served as a mediator of the prospective associations between IPC and subsequent changes in parenting over a 2-year period. Mothers and fathers were separated into differentiated models and multiple domains of parenting were examined (i.e., authoritarian discipline and scaffolding behavior). Participants included 231 families (both mothers and fathers of preschoolers). Race was reported as White (62%), Black (21%), Mixed (8%), Asian (3%), or Other (6%) and 14% considered their ethnicity to be Hispanic/Latino. Median household income was $65,000. Results indicated that for fathers, IPC indirectly predicted domain-general parenting difficulties (increased authoritarian parenting and decreased scaffolding) via deficits in paternal cognitive flexibility (but not inhibitory control or working memory). In mothers, IPC directly predicted domain-specific parenting difficulties (decreased scaffolding only) that did not operate via maternal executive functions. Notably, these effects occurred over and above the influence of parental socioeconomic status. This study constitutes a first step toward documenting parental executive functioning as a mechanism underlying the spillover of IPC to the parent–child relationship. Family interventions intended to interrupt IPC spillover should emphasize father involvement and consider targeting parental executive functions as change mechanisms.
... The first included a difference in cortisol production in all males between music and verbal conditions, where cortisol decreased after the music condition and increased following the verbal condition. Inferences cannot be drawn from such a small sample, but in a sufficiently powered study, one potential interpretation might indicate that compared to interacting musically, males experienced greater stress as a result of interacting verbally (Brugués, 2011;Buck, 1977Buck, , 1984Chaplin, 2015;Levenson et al., 1994). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction To date, studies focusing on the connection between psychological functioning and autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity usually adopted the one-dimensional model of autonomic balance, according to which activation of one branch of the ANS is accompanied by an inhibition of the other. However, the sympathetic and parasympathetic branches also activate independently; thus, co-activation and co-inhibition may occur, which is demonstrated by a two-dimensional model of ANS activity. Here, we apply such models to assess how markers of the autonomic space relate to several critical psychological constructs: emotional contagion (EC), general anxiety, and positive and negative affect (PA and NA). We also examined gender differences in those psychophysiological relations. Methods In the present study, we analyzed data from 408 healthy students, who underwent a 5-min group baseline period as part of their participation in several experiments and completed self-reported questionnaires. Electrocardiogram (ECG), electrodermal activity (EDA), and respiration were recorded. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), pre-ejection period (PEP), as well as cardiac autonomic balance (CAB) and regulation (CAR) and cross-system autonomic balance (CSAB) and regulation (CSAR), were calculated. Results Notably, two-dimensional models were more suitable for predicting and describing most psychological constructs. Gender differences were found in psychological and physiological aspects as well as in psychophysiological relations. Women's EC scores were negatively correlated with sympathetic activity and positively linked to parasympathetic dominance. Men's PA and NA scores were positively associated with sympathetic activity. PA in men also had a positive link to an overall activation of the ANS, and a negative link to parasympathetic dominance. Discussion The current results expand our understanding of the psychological aspects of the autonomic space model and psychophysiological associations. Gender differences and strengths and weaknesses of alternative physiological models are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Older adults report greater affective well-being in solitude than younger adults, but prior findings are based on correlational designs. We aim to examine age differences in affective well-being in solitude using an experimental design and to examine conflict de-escalation as a potential mechanism. In Study 1, 207 participants were randomly assigned to either a solitude or a social interaction condition. In Study 2, 128 participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions: solitude-escalation, solitude-de-escalation, interaction-escalation, and interaction de-escalation. After a 15-min solitude or social interaction period, they reported their affective experiences. In Study 1, older (vs. younger) adults reported more positive affect overall. This age-related difference was greater in the solitude (vs. social interaction) condition; older adults reported less negative affect than younger adults in the solitude, but not the social interaction, condition. In Study 2, older (vs. younger) participants reported more high-arousal positive affect in the solitude-escalation condition. This difference was not significant in conflict de-escalation conditions. Our studies provide causal evidence of the relationship between solitude and affective well-being and advance our understanding of motivations that explain why older adults maintain better affective well-being in solitude.
Article
Full-text available
In a study designed to examine how intimate partners' coping processes with regard to infertility predicted depressive symptoms across the course of a treatment cycle, 43 couples completed assessments in the week prior to and the week after receiving a negative pregnancy result from an alternate insemination attempt by the partner. Depressive symptoms in both partners increased significantly after the pregnancy result receipt. As hypothesized, avoidant coping predicted increased distress over time, and approach-oriented coping (e.g., problem-focused coping, emotional processing, and expression) predicted decreased distress. Coping strategies engaged in by both individuals and partners predicted depressive symptoms, and for women, interactions also emerged between their own and their partners' coping.
Article
Full-text available
The dimensions by which adults of differing ages experience emotion were studied by self-administering questionnaires administered to older adults (n = 828) recruited from Elderhostel programs, middle-aged (ages 30–59) children of Elderhostel attenders (n = 231), and young adult (ages 18–29) subjects recruited from college classes or through Elderhostel participants (n = 207). Elders were higher in emotional control, mood stability, and emotional maturity through moderation and leveling of positive affect and lower in surgency, psychophysiological responsiveness, and sensation seeking. These findings are consistent with the hypothesized increase in self-regulatory capacity with age. These cross-sectional differences cannot, however, be distinguished from cohort-related explanations; they require considerable replication across different types of subjects and further characterization of the dimensions in terms of their functions for self-regulation.
Article
Full-text available
Two longitudinal studies of marital interaction were conducted using observational coding of couples attempting to resolve a high-conflict issue. We found that a different pattern of results predicts concurrent marital satisfaction than predicts change in marital satisfaction over 3 years. Results suggest that some marital interaction patterns, such as disagreement and anger exchanges, which have usually been considered harmful to a marriage, may not be harmful in the long run. These patterns were found to relate to unhappiness and negative interaction at home concurrently, but they were predictive of improvement in marital satisfaction longitudinally. However, three interaction patterns: were identified as dysfunctional in terms of longitudinal deterioration: defensiveness (which includes: whining), stubborness, and withdrawal from interaction. Hypotheses about gender differences in roles for the maintenance of marital satisfaction are presented.
Article
Full-text available
Recent behavioral and biomedical research has suggested that psychophysiologic responsiveness (reactivity) to emotional stress may be a marker of processes involved in the development of cardiovascular disorders. The assessment of reactivity focuses on acute changes in functioning as opposed to the sole assessment of resting levels of variables. This article reviews evidence linking behaviorally induced cardiovascular and endocrine changes to coronary heart disease and essential hypertension. Particular attention is given to methodologic issues involving measurement. It is concluded that reactivity to stress is a construct with multiple dimensions: Different tasks and situations appear to elicit different patterns of physiologic responses. Further, an evaluation of evidence reveals that reactivity per se should currently not be regarded as a proven risk factor.Promising evidence does, however, justify continued laboratory and naturalistic, hypothesis-testing research. Recommended are methodologic studies to identify(a) the psychologic dimensions of stimuli that elicit reactivity in different subject groups, and (b) the patterns of physiologic responses produced. Such research would set the stage for epidemiologic studies to further examine relations between behavior and disease processes.