ArticlePublisher preview available

Effects of Anxiety on Eating: Does Palatability Moderate Distress-Induced Overeating in Dieters?

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

When confronted with an anxiety-producing threat to self-esteem, restrained eaters (dieters) increase their food consumption. The functional explanation suggests that increased eating temporarily counteracts or masks dysphoria for the restrained eater; externality or stimulus sensitivity theories propose that distress shifts the dieter's attention to external stimulus properties (e.g., taste) and to activities stimulated by such external cues. In an attempt to distinguish between these two explanations, anxious and nonanxious restrained and unrestrained eaters were given palatable and unpalatable foods, and consumption was measured. Results support the functional explanations: Distressed dieters increased their intake of food regardless of taste properties. Theoretical and practical implications for both restrained eating and the behavior of eating disorder patients are discussed.
Journal
of
Abnormal Psychology
1994, Vol.
103,
No.
3,
505-510
Copyright
1994
by the
American
Psychological
Association,
Inc.
0021-843X/94/S3.00
Effects
of
Anxiety
on
Eating:
Does
Palatability
Moderate
Distress-Induced
Overeating
in
Dieters?
Janet
Polivy,
C.
Peter
Herman,
and
Traci
McFarlane
When confronted with
an
anxiety-producing
threat
to
self-esteem,
restrained
eaters
(dieters)
in-
crease
their food consumption.
The
functional explanation
suggests
that
increased
eating
temporar-
ily
counteracts
or
masks
dysphoria
for the
restrained
eater; externality
or
stimulus sensitivity
theories
propose
that distress
shifts
the
dieter's
attention
to
external stimulus
properties
(e.g.,
taste)
and to
activities stimulated
by
such external cues.
In an
attempt
to
distinguish between
these
two
explana-
tions, anxious
and
nonanxious
restrained
and
unrestrained
eaters
were given
palatable
and
unpalat-
able foods,
and
consumption
was
measured.
Results
support
the
functional explanations:
Distressed
dieters
increased
their intake
of
food
regardless
of
taste
properties.
Theoretical
and
practical
im-
plications
for
both restrained eating
and the
behavior
of
eating
disorder
patients
are
discussed.
Although
distress suppresses eating
in
nondieters (un-
restrained eaters), chronic dieters (restrained eaters)
eat
more
when
they
are
upset (e.g., Baucom
&
Aiken,
1981;
Heatherton,
Herman,
&
Polivy,
1991;
Herman
&
Polivy, 1975; Herman, Pol-
ivy,
Lank,
&
Heatherton, 1987; Ruderman, 1985).
The in-
creased consumption
of
dysphoric dieters
is
especially pro-
nounced when
the
source
of
distress
is a
threat
to the ego or
self-image
(e.g.,
failure
or
performance anxiety)
as
opposed
to
a
physical threat (e.g., anticipated electric shock; Heatherton,
Herman,
&
Polivy,
1991).
Numerous
explanations
of the
enhanced eating
of
distressed
dieters have been
offered.
One of the
earliest,
and
among
the
most
widely
accepted (Orbach, 1978),
was
that eating provides
comfort
to
distressed dieters
(or
obese individuals,
who are
usu-
ally
dieters;
see, e.g., Herman
&
Polivy,
1988b),
as the
psycho-
somatic
hypothesis
of
obesity posited (Kaplan
&
Kaplan,
1957;
McKenna,
1972;
Slochower, 1983). This explanation,
of
course,
does
not
consider eating
to be
comforting
to
nondieters,
or at
least
not
comforting enough
to
offset
the
appetite-suppressing
effects
of the
autonomic activation attendant
on
distress.
A re-
lated
alternative suggests that eating
may
serve
as a
distraction
from
one's worries (Herman
&
Polivy,
1975);
like
the
comfort
hypothesis,
this explanation applies more obviously
to
dieters
(and
to the
obese,
who
also
eat
more when distressed) than
to
normal-weight
nondieters,
who
apparently
do not
derive
enough benefits from eating
to use it as a
"defense"
against
dis-
tress.
A
similar
proposal
by
Herman
and
Polivy (1988a) sug-
gested that overeating serves
a
"masking"
function:
Rather than
dwell
on the
direct threat
to
self-image,
the
dieter,
by
overeating,
Janet
Polivy,
C.
Peter
Herman,
and
Traci McFarlane,
Department
of
Psychology, University
of
Toronto, Erindale
Campus,
Mississauga,
Ontario,
Canada.
This
research
was
supported
by a
grant
to
Janet Polivy
and C.
Peter
Herman
from
the
Social Sciences
and
Humanities Research Council
of
Canada.
Correspondence
concerning this article should
be
addressed
to
Janet
Polivy,
Department
of
Psychology, University
of
Toronto,
Erindale
Campus, Mississauga,
Ontario,
Canada
L5L
1C6.
may
put
him-
or
herself
in a
position
to
misattribute distress
to
the
overeating itself;
the
distress
of
overeating
is
usually easier
to
deal
with
than
is
distress arising
from
more serious threats
to
one's
emotional well-being.
Despite their diversity,
the
foregoing explanations
all
share
a
view
of
distress-induced eating
(or
overeating)
as
purposive:
In
each explanation, eating serves
to
counteract, even
if
only tem-
porarily,
the
original distress.
It
should
be
noted that this theory
does
not
demand that such
efforts
at
self-soothing
be
effective
in
any
long-term sense.
The
distraction hypothesis implicitly
suggests
that
the
reduction
in
anxiety
can
last only
as
long
as the
distraction; once
one
thinks about
one's
anxiety
(as
when
one is
assessing
it on a
rating scale),
it is
likely
to
return
in
full
force.
The
masking theory posits that
the
distress
of a
threat
to
self-
esteem
becomes
misattributed
or
reinterpreted
as
distress
over
having
broken one's diet.
The
distress
is
still present,
but is
sim-
ply
relabeled
in a
preferable manner.
At
best,
the
psychosomatic
hypothesis theorizes that anxiety reduction
will
occur during
eating;
after
eating,
the
individual
may
well
return
to the
origi-
nal
anxiety level.
The
functional
explanations thus
do not
nec-
essarily
predict
a
lasting reduction
in
anxiety
as a
result
of
eat-
ing;
empirically,
a
reduction
in
anxiety
has
proved
to be
elusive
(see,
e.g., Herman
&
Polivy, 1975).
Another
set of
explanations does
not
share this emphasis
on
the
(temporary)
anxiolytic
effects
of
eating; rather, these other
explanations
view
distress-induced eating
in
dieters
or the
obese
not as
purposive,
but as an
unintended consequence
of a
dis-
tress-induced
shift
toward exaggerated control
of
eating
by ex-
ternal
stimuli. Specifically, distress creates
a
more stimulus-
bound
or
external orientation;
in the
presence
of
palatable food
cues, enhanced external
control
of
eating
will generally mean
more eating.
Schachter
and his
colleagues
(e.g.,
Schachter,
1971;
Schachter
&
Rodin, 1974)
offered
an
explanation
for the
overeating
of a
group
of
overweight subjects
(a
population
of
college students
that substantially overlapped
the
population
of
college-student
chronic dieters used
in
most restraint studies; see, e.g., Herman
&
Polivy, 1988b) premised
on
enhanced external stimulus sen-
sitivity.
Schachter
(1971)
saw
this
as a
general trait
of his
obese
505
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... First, the presence of these products in advertisements can aid individuals in shifting away from negative self-perceptions. Research indicates that consumers display a higher inclination for products that serve as distractions from thoughts related to self-discrepancies (Polivy, Herman, and McFarlane 1994;Troisi and Gabriel 2011). Similarly, when consumers experience feelings of shame and come across advertisements that do not exacerbate this shame, they tend to perceive such advertisements positively. ...
Article
This study investigates the effect of shame on subsequent advertisement processing. Consumers often experience shame in various advertising contexts, such as digital media contexts, TV advertisements , or even daily lives. However, it remains unclear how feeling shame affects consumers' processing of subsequent advertisements that are unrelated to the shame-inducing situations. In three experiments, we find that consumers who feel shame show increased cognitive processing (e.g. greater recall of advertisements) compared with those who do not feel shame or those who feel guilt. We further show that this increased cognitive processing leads to a more positive response toward the product. Furthermore, we find that the effect of shame on subsequent advertisement processing is stronger for high self-monitors than for low self-monitors.
... For example, in their investigation, found that to rejuvenate and restore her self-sufficiency, a respondent reported going for a camping trip shortly after her husband had died, considering its therapeutic and coping values. On the other hand, escapism may manifest self-gifting behavior by eating more chocolates and cookies under a high level of anxiety to divert one's attention (Polivy et al., 1994). Among many personal failures, romantic breakups (either marital or nonmarital) are considered one of life's most distressing psychological events (Kendler et al., 2003;Tashiro et al., 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
When do consumers indulge in self‐gifting? While extant research predominantly explores self‐gifting as a coping mechanism in postchallenging phases, this research suggests an additional dimension: self‐gifting as a form of self‐reward. Drawing from the idea of stress‐related growth, this paper focuses on consumer behavior following a romantic breakup, investigating self‐gifting as a reward mechanism. Results from five studies consistently demonstrate that the severity of a breakup significant influences consumers' self‐gifting propensity in both intrinsic‐ and extrinsic‐rewarding consumption, driven by their heightened perceived deservingness. These effects are attenuated when individuals engage in ruminative thoughts about their tumultuous (vs. harmonious) breakups. These findings explain the nuances of self‐gifting behavior following a romantic breakup by establishing a theoretical link between personal failures (e.g., romantic breakups) and a sense of deservingness, which influences consumer's self‐gifting propensity as a self‐reward.
... The primary end point was change in body weight (kilograms) from baseline to week 8 (end of treatment) following administration of oxytocin versus placebo. Key secondary end points were changes from baseline to end of treatment in body composition ( 20 baseline to week 4), and quality of life. Details of the specific scales used to assess these parameters are described below. ...
... The healthcare leader is not one to control the healthcare workers group, instead expected to take the responsibility to create an environment for guidance and coordination the process by which the group decides upon and carries out actions to accomplish its goals (Herman, 1994). Healthcare workers generally feel comfortable in an environment of collaboration healthcare leadership approach is especially handy in certain scenarios where issues are of an extremely complex nature and that no one person or entity has either the information or the power to change those (Müller & Van, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: This study intends to investigate the influence of strategic alignment capability (SAC) and volatile, uncertain, complex, and ambiguous (VUCA) as moderator on navigation leadership in private hospitals of healthcare sector in Pakistan. Utilizing ventakraman’s model of strategic alignment capability and volatility-ambiguity theory by Warren Bennis and Burt Nanus, the research hopes to identify the impact of SAC model and VUCA moderator on navigation leadership in healthcare sector in Pakistan. Design/Methodology/Approach: A total of 400 healthcare executives from the healthcare industry will participate in this study by completing an online self-filled questionnaire. The collected data will undergo various statistical analyses, including normality tests, reliability assessments, correlation analyses, and descriptive examinations. These analyses shall be employed to determine the connection between independent variables (strategic alignment capability model) and the dependent variable of navigation leadership along with volatile, uncertain, complex, and ambiguous context as moderator variable. Findings: The research paper serves as a conceptual framework of influence of strategic alignment capability and volatile, uncertain, complex, and ambiguous context as moderator on navigation leadership. Healthcare leaders have the potential to significantly enhance their productivity, making valuable contributions to their respective fields in the healthcare profession. The study underlines the significance of addressing these sensitive elements of healthcare sector to enhance the performance in the Pakistan healthcare sector. Practical Implications: This study offers valuable insights for healthcare leadership within the developing countries like Pakistan healthcare industry. By recognizing and fostering influence of strategic alignment capability and vuca moderator on navigation leadership in healthcare sector can create an environment where healthcare leaders are more inclined to increase their productivity and positively impact overall healthcare sector success. Originality/Value: The originality of this study lies in its application of venkatraman’s model of strategic alignment capability and volatility-ambiguity theory by Warren Bennis and Burt Nanus within the specific context of the Pakistan healthcare industry. By identifying the unique factors influencing strategic alignment capability model in navigation leadership along with vuca moderator in to evaluate and enhance the performance in this industry, this research contributes to a deeper understanding of the relationship between strategic alignment capability and navigation leadership moderating vuca context.
... Although we successfully manipulated mood, we did not find a significant three-way interaction, which suggests that the effects of commercials and restraint on food intake did not depend on the type of movie. Previous studies repeatedly found that restrained eaters showed disinhibited eating after induction of negative emotional states (Heatherton, Polivy, Herman, & Baumeister, 1993;Cools, Schotte, & McNally, 1992;Heatherton, Herman, & Polivy, 1991;Heatherton, Striepe, & Wittenberg, 1998;Herman, Polivy, Lank, & Heatherton, 1987;McFarlane, Polivy, & Herman, 1998;Mitchell & Epstein, 1996;Polivy & Herman, 1999;Polivy, Herman, & McFarlane, 1994;Schotte et al., 1990). However, all of these studies used the Restraint Scale (Herman et al., 1978), which contains items concerning disinhibited eating and therefore also tends to select restrained eaters with a tendency to over-eat. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: The aim of the present study was to explore the effects of exposure to slim images and diet-related products in commercials on actual food intake in relation to dietary restraint. Design: An experimental design was used, in which food intake was measured in 124 female students who watched either a sad or a neutral movie on television, which was interrupted by either commercials featuring slim models and diet products, or neutral commercials. Subsequently, participants filled out questionnaires on dietary restraint and any tendency toward overeating. Main outcome variable: Intake of snack food while watching television. Results: It was found that highly restrained students exposed to commercials with slim models and diet-related products ate less food, whereas less restrained eaters ate slightly more after seeing these commercials. Conclusion: The findings suggest that restrained eaters confronted with diet products and slim images when watching television will be reminded of their restricted eating behavior and eat less. The present study provides support for the reinhibition theory of slim media images.
... Escapism can also involve less pleasurable experiences, but which also facilitate escape from self-awareness. For example, Polivy et al. (1994) found that participants who were anxious about preparing and delivering an upcoming speech consumed a higher amount of both good-tasting and bad-tasting cookies, compared with participants who were not preparing for an upcoming speech. ...
... Anxiety, depression, and other mental illnesses have increased food cravings, particularly for sweets and snacks, leading to overconsumption and potentially resulting in BED [31]. Changes in taste preferences may also play a role for individuals with mental disorders as a way of coping with their mental state [32]. Furthermore, anxiety and eating disorders often have common etiologies, which can increase an individual's susceptibility to both disorders, with overlapping symptoms and risk factors [33,34]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Anxiety and depression are common worldwide and often occur together. Anxiety can alter appetite, causing women to consume more food than usual, leading to changes in eating behavior and obesity. This comparative cross-sectional study compared the severity of depression, anxiety, stress, and eating disorders among working and nonworking women visiting community pharmacies. Data were collected through face-to-face interviews with 244 working women and 267 nonworking women. The data were analyzed using descriptive statistics, and a chi-square test was conducted to evaluate differences in the severity of depression, anxiety, stress, and eating disorders between the two groups of participants. Most participants were literate; approximately half of the working (50.82%) and nonworking women (55.06%) had a normal body mass index. There were significant differences between the working and nonworking women in terms of their education and physical activity levels (p < 0.05). Both groups were identified as being at risk of developing bulimia, but their ability to control eating habits differed significantly (p = 0.008). Most participants reported never using laxatives, diet pills, or diuretics to control their weight. Moreover, they reported that they had never received treatment for eating disorders or experienced thoughts of suicide. The study found no significant difference in depression (p = 0.085) and anxiety levels (p = 0.207) between working and nonworking women. However, a significant difference was found in stress levels between working and nonworking women (p = 0.001). Our study highlights a significant prevalence of psychological issues among working and nonworking women in Pakistan, impacting their eating habits and contributing to developing eating disorders and obesity. Interestingly, working women display higher physical activity levels, while nonworking women demonstrate better oral control to prevent eating disorders. To improve women's mental and physical well-being, we recommend prioritizing mental health interventions for all women, promoting healthy eating habits, supporting physical activity, and investigating underlying factors influencing psychological well-being.
Article
The aim of this study was to investigate the relationship among sleep quality, impulsivity, anxiety, and depression in individuals with emotional eating behavior. The study was designed as a cross-sectional study. The study included 92 individuals (age 31.29 ± 9.17; female, 67.4% [ n = 62]; male, 32.6% [ n = 30]) with symptoms of emotional eating but no previous psychiatric diagnosis or treatment. Participants were administered a Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 Disorders interview form, a sociodemographic data form, the Emotional Eating Scale, the Beck Depression Scale, the Barratt Impulsivity Scale, the Beck Anxiety Scale, and the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index Scale. Emotional eating is positively correlated with anxiety ( r = 0.377, p = 0.001), depression ( r = 0.375, p = 0.001), impulsivity ( r = 0.250, p = 0.016), and poor sleep quality ( r = 0.478, p = 0.001). Obese individuals (defined as having a body mass index of 30 or higher) showed higher emotional eating ( z = −2.552, p = 0.016) and poorer sleep quality ( z = −2.089, p = 0.044) than nonobese individuals, and women showed higher emotional eating ( t = 2116, p = 0.037) and poorer sleep quality ( z = −2537, p = 0.010) than men. Poor sleep quality was associated with emotional eating. In this relationship, poor sleep quality influenced emotional eating through all mediators, including anxiety and depression ( B = 3.491; standardized effect, 0.485; p = 0.001). Poor sleep quality directly influenced emotional eating ( B = 2.806; standardized effect, 0.390; p = 0.001). The findings of the study suggest that emotional eating is associated with higher levels of anxiety, depression, impulsivity, and sleep problems, especially in women. It suggests that the interrelationships of psychological factors associated with emotional eating should be investigated.
Article
This paper aims to examine how emotions affect consumers' food choices and food preparation activities during stressful periods, using the context of the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) outbreak in China. This study used an online survey, with a sample of 1,050 individuals from 32 regions in China. Multi-regression and mediation models were used to test the relationships among perceived knowledge, emotions and food behaviors. The results show that positive emotions positively affect healthy food consumption and engagement in food preparations. In contrast, negative emotions contribute to an increase in indulgent food consumption and quick-and-easy meal preparations. Increased knowledge of the current situation can enhance positive emotions and thus promote healthy food behaviors. Lacking knowledge may result in unhealthy food behaviors through negative emotions. This study contributes to the understanding of emotions and food behaviors by examining the effects of both negative and positive emotions in the general population, exploring a wider constellation of food behaviors and identifying perceived knowledge as an important antecedent to emotions' effects on food behaviors. Implications for consumers and public policy are offered.
Article
Full-text available
The mediating influence of self-esteem on disinhibited eating was assessed in restrained and unrestrained subjects. Self-esteem was unrelated to the eating behavior of unrestrained subjects following a preload, but did moderate the disinhibitory effects of a preload on restrained subjects. Specifically, only those restrained subjects with low self-esteem displayed disinhibited eating after a preload. Theoretical and practical implications of this finding were discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Experimentally examined 2 theoretical conceptions of obesity using 56 obese and 69 normal male undergraduates. Food preloaded Ss, believing themselves participants in a market research study of a new product which they were to taste and evaluate, provided food consumption data relevant to 3 independent variables: S weight, food cue valence, and anxiety level. Analyses of the amount of test food eaten revealed a significant interaction between weight status and anxiety level; overweight Ss ate more under high than low anxiety, whereas normal weight Ss ate less under high- than low-anxiety conditions. This finding is consistent with the traditional psychosomatic account of the role of internal cues (anxiety) in the eating behavior of the overweight individual. No significant interaction between weight status and test food valence was observed, as would have been expected according to S. Schachter, R. Goldman, and A. Gordon's recent formulation of the role of external cues in the eating of the obese. (16 ref.)
Article
Full-text available
This study considered the role of perceived control over anxiety in the overeating patterns of obese and non-obese individuals. Obese and normal weight participants' eating was measured unobtrusively in a four-factorial design which covaried subjects' weight, arousal level, degree of perceived control over arousal, and the presence or absence of a label for arousal. Obese participants were found to be highly responsive to all experimental variables, and ate most when they felt anxious and could neither control nor explain this state. Their overeating response was sharply reduced when a sense of control and label for arousal were present. Aroused obese subjects also showed significant affect reduction following eating. In contrast to obese participants, normal weight subjects' eating was essentially unaffected by the label and control variables, but was positively related to their hunger level, and negatively correlated with their anxiety level. The implications of our findings for the psychosomatic hypothesis are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
In this study we examined the effects of anxiety and food deprivation on the amount of food consumed ad lib by dieters and nondieters. Eighty female college students served as subjects in an ostensible market research study in which an anxiety manipulation was embedded. Reassignment of the subjects to anxiety condition on the basis of self-reported anxiety produced a significant (p < .02) three-way interaction among level of anxiety, food deprivation, and dieting status. The results suggest that (a) for nondieters, anxiety suppresses hunger but has no effect when subjects are not initially hungry, and (b) for dieters, anxiety increases eating only when the subject is initially hungry. These results are interpreted in terms of Herman and Polivy's (1984) boundary model of eating.
Chapter
Full-text available
appetite as a disposition / objective measurement of mental states / drive and incentive structure of appetite / two-process model of appetite / palatability and post-ingestional satiation / externality / appetite stimulus configurations / conditioning or cognition / dimensional analyses of appetite / psychophysical identification of dimensions of appetite / extension to non-psychophysical designs / statistical importance of different influences on appetite / multidimensional cognitive algebra / cognitive networks in appetite / non-cognitive networks / dieting / quantitative and qualitative investigation / personal theory of energy (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Book
Originally published in 1974, this volume examines the behavioural similarities of obese humans and animals whose so-called feeding centre (the ventro-medial hypothalamic nuclei) has been lesioned. Both the obese human and the VMH-lesioned animal seem to share a hyposensitivity to the internal (physiological) cues to eating and hypersensitivity to external cues associated with food. Beginning with a review, these obese animals and the human obese are compared point by point on experimental results reported in the literature. Then, new findings are presented that specifically tested humans for relationships that are well-established for lesioned animals. Next, a theoretical framework integrates the human and animal data to postulate that the relationship of cue prominence and probability of response is stronger for the obese than for normal. The causes for this, and the extension of the basis for the obese’s eating behaviour to other areas, are discussed in light of further experiments that will make this invaluable reading for all concerned with the history of obesity and the issues of regulatory behaviour.
Article
Age, gender, and gender role differences on a set of variables including concern with eating, body weight, and physical appearance, global self-esteem, and appearance self-esteem were examined in a sample of subjects consisting of 639 visitors to a participatory science museum. Their ages ranged from 10 to 79 years. Results showed that females are more concerned than males about eating, body weight, and physical appearance and have lower appearance self-esteem. More important, these gender differences are generally apparent at all ages. The importance of gender differences across the life span in appearance concern and appearance self-esteem is discussed.
Article
several phases of binge eating can be identified, each of which can be analyzed in terms of determining influences, including psychological influences / divide the binge episode into five phases / examine the impact of psychological mechanisms at each of these stages / evaluate the various models that have been proposed to explain binge eating in terms of how they recognize and explain the operation of these psychological mechanisms at each phase addictions model / conditioning model / affective disorders/regulation model / escape model / dieting model / biopsychosocial models (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)