ArticlePDF Available

A Study on the Effect of Color on Human Food Perception

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The purpose of this research was to determine whether the color of food has any ability to influence and stimulate appetite. We divided the color spectrum into two broad representative groups: warm and cold colors. Based on anecdotal experiences, I hypothesized that warm-colored food would appear more appetizing than cold-colored food. To test this hypothesis, I created a survey that included images of staple food and desserts and applied different warm and cold-color filters, and asked respondents to choose their preference. Age, gender, and respondent hunger levels were also collected. The results show that staple food and desserts with warm filters attracted more overall interest, though cold-filtered desserts were notably more frequently selected than cold-filtered staple meals. The results of the research may be useful for cooking, decorating, and graphic design, and may have separate therapeutic applications for children with eating disorders.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Education, Humanities and Social Sciences
PSHE 2023
Volume 10 (2023)
225
A Study on the Effect of Color on Human Food Perception
Jingyi Huang
School of Shenzhen Foreign Languages, Shenzhen 518000, China
Abstract. The purpose of this research was to determine whether the color of food has any ability
to influence and stimulate appetite. We divided the color spectrum into two broad representative
groups: warm and cold colors. Based on anecdotal experiences, I hypothesized that warm-colored
food would appear more appetizing than cold-colored food. To test this hypothesis, I created a survey
that included images of staple food and desserts and applied different warm and cold-color filters,
and asked respondents to choose their preference. Age, gender, and respondent hunger levels were
also collected. The results show that staple food and desserts with warm filters attracted more overall
interest, though cold-filtered desserts were notably more frequently selected than cold-filtered staple
meals. The results of the research may be useful for cooking, decorating, and graphic design, and
may have separate therapeutic applications for children with eating disorders.
Keywords: Color; Appetite; Experimentation; Warm colors.
1. Introduction
Given its importance to perception, scientists have researched color and its influence on behavior,
frequently separating the color spectrum into two sides: warm colors, including red, orange, and
yellow, and cold, including green, blue, and purple. Warm and cold colors are different in terms of
their wavelengths: warm colors are long-wavelength, while cold colors have shorter wavelengths
(Sokolik et al., 2014). Humans perceive these two categories of (warm and cold) differently for
biological reasons (Elliot and Niesta, 2008). There are multiple ways warm and cold colors can
influence perception. A previous study shows that warm-color advertisements receive higher click
rates than cold-color advertisements (Sokolik et al., 2014). For the use of color in gastronomy, studies
show that the color of food will affect people’s judgment of how the food tastes, with one study
showing that red can influence perceptions towards spicy or sweet, while orange or yellow can help
create sour tastes (Spence, 2010).
Despite the number of color experiments in the literature, there are few experiments directly
examining the difference between warm and cold colors in affecting our perception and appetite. In
this research, I conducted a survey and asked participants to rate how likely they are to eat a food
based on an image of food with a colored filter applied. Based on past anecdotal observations, I
hypothesized foods that have warm-color filters will be more attractive than those with cold-color
filters.
2. Method
To assess whether perceptions of appetite could be influenced by color, I designed a survey and
conducted it online. I created a QR code to make the questionnaire more convenient to send to
participants, and after designing the questionnaire, I posted the QR code on social media sites. I also
asked friends and family to forward it to others.
The questionnaire first asked respondents about their gender and age and separated the ages into
four groups. The first age group was composed of respondents ages 0-18 years, the second was 19-
30 years, the third was 31-50 years, and the fourth included those older than 50. The following
question asked about the respondents’ appetite, with the degree of appetite divided into four levels:
“Hungry,” “A Little Bit Hungry,” “A Little Bit Full,” and “Full.” This question aimed to control an
individual’s level of hunger at the time of an experiment.
After the demographic data, the survey asks four main questions that required participants to
compare two images of a food item and select their preference. The survey provided two savory items
Journal of Education, Humanities and Social Sciences
PSHE 2023
Volume 10 (2023)
226
in hamburgers and pizza along with two pictures items in ice cream and cupcakes, with the inclusion
of both savory and sweet options intended to control for variations in respondents’ food preferences
that might bias the results. Each image set displayed the same image of a food item twice with
different filters applied using the “Snapseed” and “Procreate” apps. The first image in the set had a
warm-color filter, and the second had a cold-color filter. Red and orange filters comprised the warm-
color group while green and blue filters as the cold-color group. Then I asked the participants to look
at the picture side by side to choose which one they felt was more appetizing.
For each picture, I randomized the choice of the specific color from warm and cold groups. For
the hamburger, I used an orange filter image to compare it with the green filtered image; for pizza, I
used red and blue filters; for the cupcake, orange and blue; and for the ice cream, red and green. I
ordered the questions so that savory foods and desserts appeared alternately. Finally, to ensure the
usability of the results, the survey required participants to answer each question with no options for
neutral or ambiguous choices.
3. Results
A total of 105 participants filled out the questionnaire. 39% were male and 61% were female.
Respondents' age varied. Most respondents were in the under 18 group (44%) or the 30-50 age group
(37%), with minority numbers in the 19-30 group (13%) and the over 50 years group (6%). 40% of
respondents reported feeling “A Little Bit Hungry” while 42% of people said they were “A Little Bit
Full” full. Only 6% responded as feeling “Hungry” and 12% responded as feeling “Full.”
Our primary data concerned the filtered image selection, and for each food item, the majority chose
the warm-colored filtered versions. 87% of respondents chose the orange-filtered hamburger over
green filters, 62% chose the orange-filtered cupcakes over the blue filtered, 70% preferred the red-
filtered pizza over the blue filtered, and 55% preferred the red-filtered ice cream over the green
filtered.
Fig 1. Percentage of respondents choosing the color-filtered hamburgers.
Fig 2. Percentage of respondents choosing the color-filtered cupcakes.
Journal of Education, Humanities and Social Sciences
PSHE 2023
Volume 10 (2023)
227
Fig 3. Percentage of respondents choosing the color-filtered pizza.
Fig 4. Percentage of respondents choosing the color-filtered ice cream.
To determine whether respondent appetite had effects on respondents' bias for color filters, I built
cross-tabulations comparing respondent appetite levels against their indicated filter preference.
Fig 5. Respondent preferences for warm or cold-filtered hamburgers by appetite level.
In Figure 5, we can see that no matter which level of hunger, the percentage of choosing the warm-
color filtered hamburger is higher than 80%.
Journal of Education, Humanities and Social Sciences
PSHE 2023
Volume 10 (2023)
228
Fig 6. Respondent preferences for warm or cold-filtered cupcakes by appetite level.
Figure 6 shows that although the warm-color filter was more popular for cupcakes, the percentage
of cold-color filters notably increases. In the A Little Bit of Hungry” group, the percentage of
choosing a warm-color filter decreases from the hamburger’s 81% to the cupcake’s 55%, a difference
of 26%.
Fig 7. Respondent preferences for warm or cold-filtered pizza by appetite level.
For pizzas, people who are “A Little Bit Hungry” offers other diverges in the data, where compared
to the cupcakes, the preference for the warm-colored filter rises from 55% to 73%. For other groups
of people, the percentage doesn’t significantly vary.
Journal of Education, Humanities and Social Sciences
PSHE 2023
Volume 10 (2023)
229
Fig 8. Respondent preferences for warm or cold-filtered ice cream by appetite level.
Figure 8 demonstrates that for ice cream, warm colors are generally preferred except for the “A
Little Bit Hungry” group, where the green cold-color filter was preferred. None of the four groups
have a percentage above 80% for either choice.
The four graphs show that overall, there is a strong tendency among respondents to favor the warm-
color filtered images, and except in the case of respondents who were “A Little Bit Hungry”
responding to desserts. The preference for warm-colored filters among staple foods is striking, with
the warm-color preference ranging between 67% and 93%.
4. Discussion
The results demonstrate that for all four scenarios, individuals preferred foods with warm-color
filters over cool-color filters. From these results, we can conclude there is a general tendency in
individuals to prefer eating food with warm-color themes compared to cold-color themes. In other
words, foods with warm colors are more appetizing in appearance. As colors were varied and
represented across all the options, individual preferences did not owe to individual preference for a
specific color.
The main caveat to this conclusion to the results is that for desserts, the difference in color-filter
preferences wasn’t as significant as it was for staple foods. This is particularly notable in the results
for filtered ice cream preferences, where the preference for warm colors holds a relatively slim
majority, and in one appetite level group, the cold filter was even preferred, albeit by a small majority.
There are possible ice-cream-centric explanations: some individuals may have found the red-filtered
ice cream to be less appetizing because ice cream is less likely to be red, while green-filtered ice
cream is not uncommon as pistachio and green tea flavors.
These results are significant in looking at how colors can influence food choice or appetite, and
potential applications are broad. Chefs can choose to use different colors during cooking to make
their productions seem more flavored and appealing to appetites. Cold colors may have some unseen
benefits, most likely for desserts like ice cream, but our study does not conclusively address this, and
any ideas for their uses would be speculation. Beyond the food itself, culinary artists can use
decorations to change the atmosphere of the dish, helping beautify foods that are generally cold-color
in appearance due to their materials with warmer contexts.
Journal of Education, Humanities and Social Sciences
PSHE 2023
Volume 10 (2023)
230
Beyond cooking, the results of this survey offer insights and inform therapeutic practices in helping
children with picky eating habits and those with intake disorders like ARFID. Previous research
indicates that ARFID is a common problem in children, with between 14% and 50% of preschool
children having picky eating habits and between 7% and 27% of older children (Norris, M. L.,
Spettigue, W. J., & Katzman, D. K., 2016). ARFID is of particular concern as it can pose multiple
problems to children’s growth, especially for those under three years old, where a picky eating habit
increases the risk of being underweight (Ekstein, S., Laniado, D., & Glick, B., 2010). Research shows
that the pleasure of eating is the most important aspect in averting such issues, and remedies that
don’t focus on pleasurable eating, like applying too much pressure on children to force them to eat
vegetables, may result in worse eating habits and a negative relationship with food (Van der Horst,
K., 2012). To change children’s diet habits positively, parents can mix vegetables, which usually have
cold colors, with warm-colored food, like meats, or by adding healthy sauces to change the
vegetable’s color.
Future opportunities for research exist. This study, with few respondents, could be expanded with
more respondents, which would make the results available for analysis with statistically significant
results, and specific color options could be used. With enough respondents, more varieties of
comparisons could be drawn, like red versus green, red versus blue, etc. This research proposes a
foundation upon which future color research can continue.
References
[1] Ekstein, S., Laniado, D., & Glick, B. (2010). Does picky eating affect weight-for-length measurements in
young children? Clinical pediatrics, 49(3), 217-220.
[2] Elliot, Andrew J., and Daniela Niesta, (2008), Romantic Red: Red Enhances Men’s Attraction to
Women,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 11501164
[3] Norris, M. L., Spettigue, W. J., & Katzman, D. K. (2016). Update on eating disorders: current perspectives
on avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder in children and youth. Neuropsychiatric disease and
treatment, 12, 213.
[4] Sokolik, K., Magee, R. G., & Ivory, J. D. (2014). Red-hot and ice-cold web ads: The influence of web
ads’ warm and cool colors on click-through rates. Journal of Interactive Advertising, 14(1), 31-37.
[5] Spence, C. (2010, March 9). Does Food Color Influence Taste and Flavor Perception in Human
s? SpringerLink. Https: // link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12078-010-9067-z?error=cookies_not_s
upported&code=fe5aae0e-2354-4419-8c17-094d0809e779.
[6] Van der Horst, K. (2012). Overcoming picky eating. Eating enjoyment as a central aspect of children’s
eating behaviors. Appetite, 58(2), 567-574.
... Given that, colour is an attribute that can draw customers towards any product and this explains that the visual evaluation of colour usually takes place first out of all the other categories (Winarmo, 1997). According to (Huang, 2023) it is found that individuals preferred foods with warm-colour filters (red and orange) over coolcolour filters (green and blue). From the study's findings, it can be inferred that there is a general tendency of individuals to prefer eating food with warm-colour themes compared to cold-colour themes. ...
Article
Full-text available
Since, the COVID-19 outbreak, people are encouraged to eat healthy food including fruits. Eating fruit has many health benefits, for instance, lowering the risk of a certain disease. However, nowadays, not all people like to eat fruits which are very crucial for nutrients needed to maintain body health. Fruits can be consumed in fresh, frozen, canned, dried, or dehydrated forms. Fruits can also be consumed whole, sliced into pieces, pureed, mashed, or cooked. Therefore, halwa candy was introduced as an alternative, natural sweet for edible fruit. Halwa candy is produced for all ages group to enjoy the fruits in the form of candy and at the same time obtain its health benefits. It is convenient for the consumers to bring the Halwa candy around and enjoy it at any time of the day since the size of the candy is small-sized candy and portable. The ingredients are mainly halal gelatin, fruit extracts and honey. Thus, all the ingredients selected are halal and organic in order to make sure that the candy is healthy to consume. These products can find good marketable value in the future.
Article
Full-text available
Avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder (ARFID) is a new eating disorder diagnosis that was introduced in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) fifth edition. The fourth edition of the DSM had failed to adequately capture a cohort of children, adolescents, and adults who are unable to meet appropriate nutritional and/or energy needs, for reasons other than drive for thinness, leading to significant medical and/or psychological sequelae. With the introduction of ARFID, researchers are now starting to better understand the presentation, clinical characteristics, and complexities of this disorder. This article outlines the diagnostic criteria for ARFID with specific focus on children and youth. A case example of a patient with ARFID, factors that differentiate ARFID from picky eating, and the estimated prevalence in pediatric populations are discussed, as well as clinical and treatment challenges that impact health care providers providing treatment for patients.
Article
Full-text available
In many nonhuman primates, the color red enhances males' attraction to females. In 5 experiments, the authors demonstrate a parallel effect in humans: Red, relative to other achromatic and chromatic colors, leads men to view women as more attractive and more sexually desirable. Men seem unaware of this red effect, and red does not influence women's perceptions of the attractiveness of other women, nor men's perceptions of women's overall likeability, kindness, or intelligence. The findings have clear practical implications for men and women in the mating game and, perhaps, for fashion consultants, product designers, and marketers. Furthermore, the findings document the value of extending research on signal coloration to humans and of considering color as something of a common language, both within and across species.
Article
A field study compared the click-through rates of box and banner web ads with red and blue color schemes using data from more than 1.5 million ad impressions from 12 months of traffic on a popular news website. For box ads, a red (versus blue) color scheme generated substantially higher click-through rates, but for banner ads the effect of color was less pronounced. Consistent with research on color and attention, ads with a red color scheme elicited a greater number of responses. This finding also suggests, though, that habituation to certain types of ads, such as banner ads, can attenuate the effect of ad attributes.
Article
In this paper, we review the empirical literature concerning the important question of whether or not food color influences taste and flavor perception in humans. Although a superficial reading of the literature on this topic would appear to give a somewhat mixed answer, we argue that this is, at least in part, due to the fact that many researchers have failed to distinguish between two qualitatively distinct research questions. The first concerns the role that food coloring plays in the perception of the intensity of a particular flavor (e.g., strawberry, banana, etc.) or taste attribute (e.g., sweetness, saltiness, etc.). The second concerns the role that food coloring plays in the perception of flavor identity. The empirical evidence regarding the first question is currently rather ambiguous. While some researchers have reported a significant crossmodal effect of changing the intensity of a food or drink’s coloring on people’s judgments of taste or flavor intensity, many others have failed to demonstrate any such effect. By contrast, the research findings concerning the second question clearly support the view that people’s judgments of flavor identity are often affected by the changing of a food or drink’s color (be it appropriate, inappropriate, or absent). We discuss the possible mechanisms underlying these crossmodal effects and suggest some of the key directions for future research in order to move our understanding in this area forward. KeywordsFlavor-Taste-Color-Perception-Crossmodal-Multisensory-Expectancy-Attention
Article
Picky eating is a major source of parental concern, and children with picky eating habits are potentially at risk for nutritional deficits. This research aimed to determine whether picky eating is related to being underweight in young children. Participants included 34 children with picky eating behavior who were referred to the Pediatric Feeding and Nutrition Clinic for evaluation and 136 healthy controls.Weight and height measurements were obtained, and weight-to-length data were calculated for each child. In all, 7 of 34 children (20.6%) in the picky eaters group and 9 of 136 children (6.6%) in the control group were underweight (P = .02). Underweight was found in 15 children (14.2%) at or under 36 months and in 1 child (1.6%) older than 36 months (P = .002). Children with picky eating habits, especially those younger than 3 years of age, are at increased risk of being underweight.
Does Food Color Influence Taste and Flavor Perception in Human s? SpringerLink
  • C Spence
Spence, C. (2010, March 9). Does Food Color Influence Taste and Flavor Perception in Human s? SpringerLink. Https: // link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12078-010-9067-z?error=cookies_not_s upported&code=fe5aae0e-2354-4419-8c17-094d0809e779.