ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

In this article, we critically review the changes made to the DSM-5 Text Revision published in 2022 regarding the diagnostic entity of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). We structure our critique around three points. The first discusses the acknowledgment of ADHD as a neurodevelopmental disorder. The second examines the definition of ADHD provided in the updated edition of the manual. The third scrutinizes the changes in the diagnostic criteria for ADHD and assesses whether these changes make the diagnosis more accurate. We conclude that DSM's latest edition does not escape the logical and scientific pitfalls of its predecessor. DSM-5-TR keeps the faith in the neo-Kraepelinian paradigm by explicitly and implicitly cultivating the essentialist medical scientific metaphor of disorder, creating the illusion that it represents scientific progress that validates ADHD as a neurodevelopmental disorder.
TYPE Perspective
PUBLISHED 10 January 2023
DOI 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1064141
OPEN ACCESS
EDITED BY
Martin Whitely,
Curtin University, Australia
REVIEWED BY
Carolyn Quadrio,
University of New South
Wales, Australia
Melissa K. Raven,
University of Adelaide, Australia
*CORRESPONDENCE
Juho Honkasilta
juho.honkasilta@helsinki.fi
These authors have contributed
equally to this work
SPECIALTY SECTION
This article was submitted to
Public Mental Health,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychiatry
RECEIVED 07 October 2022
ACCEPTED 07 December 2022
PUBLISHED 10 January 2023
CITATION
Koutsoklenis A and Honkasilta J (2023)
ADHD in the DSM-5-TR: What has
changed and what has not.
Front. Psychiatry 13:1064141.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1064141
COPYRIGHT
©2023 Koutsoklenis and Honkasilta.
This is an open-access article
distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License
(CC BY). The use, distribution or
reproduction in other forums is
permitted, provided the original
author(s) and the copyright owner(s)
are credited and that the original
publication in this journal is cited, in
accordance with accepted academic
practice. No use, distribution or
reproduction is permitted which does
not comply with these terms.
ADHD in the DSM-5-TR: What
has changed and what has not
Athanasios Koutsoklenis1† and Juho Honkasilta2*
1Department of Primary Education, Democritus University of Thrace, Alexandroupolis, Greece,
2Faculty of Educational Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
In this article, we critically review the changes made to the DSM-5 Text
Revision published in 2022 regarding the diagnostic entity of Attention
Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). We structure our critique around
three points. The first discusses the acknowledgment of ADHD as a
neurodevelopmental disorder. The second examines the definition of ADHD
provided in the updated edition of the manual. The third scrutinizes the
changes in the diagnostic criteria for ADHD and assesses whether these
changes make the diagnosis more accurate. We conclude that DSM’s latest
edition does not escape the logical and scientific pitfalls of its predecessor.
DSM-5-TR keeps the faith in the neo-Kraepelinian paradigm by explicitly and
implicitly cultivating the essentialist medical scientific metaphor of disorder,
creating the illusion that it represents scientific progress that validates ADHD
as a neurodevelopmental disorder.
KEYWORDS
ADHD, DSM-5-TR, revisions, American Psychiatric Association, diagnosis, diagnostic
manual
1. Introduction
From the publication of the third edition in 1980 and on, the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) has embraced psychiatry as a branch of
medicine by committing to a “neo-Kraepelinian” cause-effect biomedical framework
with the assumption that biological discoveries will eventually establish the somatic
etiology of separate and independent mental diseases (1). This paradigm shift was
not based on promising scientific discoveries but on pragmatic consensus [see, for
a discussion (2,3)]. By the time of publishing DSM-5 in 2013, the continuous
medicalization of natural human responses led by APA became increasingly critiqued
within psychiatry [e.g., (3)], mental health sector [e.g., (4)], practitioners, and academia
in general. Debates surrounding the critical reception of DSM-5 primarily relate to the
pseudo-scientific nature of the manual and its normalizing power (5).
Frontiers in Psychiatry 01 frontiersin.org
Koutsoklenis and Honkasilta 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1064141
In this opinion paper, we critically review the changes
made to the DSM-5 Text Revision published in 2022 regarding
the diagnostic entity of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder (ADHD). We structure our critique around three
points. The first discusses the acknowledgment of ADHD as
a neurodevelopmental disorder. The second examines the
definition of ADHD provided in the updated edition of the
manual. The third scrutinizes the changes in the diagnostic
criteria for ADHD and assesses whether these changes make the
diagnosis more accurate. We point out how DSM-5-TR keeps
the faith in the neo-Kraepelinian paradigm by explicitly and
implicitly cultivating the essentialist medical scientific metaphor
of disorder.
2. Placement within
“neurodevelopmental disorders”
As in its predecessor, ADHD is placed within the manual’s
chapter “Neurodevelopmental Disorders”. According to the
DSM-5-TR neurodevelopmental disorders “are characterized
by developmental deficits or differences in brain processes
that produce impairments of personal, social, academic, or
occupational functioning” (p. 36). The authors of the manual
assert that issues relevant to the placement of ADHD have
been resolved by the available data “with the preponderance
of evidence supporting placement in the “Neurodevelopmental
Disorders” chapter” [(6), p. 13]. This assertion is strengthened
in section “Risk and Prognostic Factors”, which is more
detailed than in DSM-5. The authors state that heritability is
approximately 74% and that genome-wide association studies
(GWAS) “have identified a number of loci enriched in
evolutionarily constrained genomic regions and loss-of-function
genes as well as around brain-expressed regulatory regions.”
(p. 72).
However, as the authors of DSM-5-TR themselves
explicitly admit, the discoveries that could confirm ADHD
as a neurodevelopmental disorder have not yet materialized.
Specifically, DSM-5-TR authors state that “no biological
marker is diagnostic for ADHD” and that “meta-analysis of
all neuroimaging studies do not show differences between
individuals with ADHD and control subjects”, thus “no form
of neuroimaging can be used for diagnosis of ADHD” [(6), p.
73]. Apart from what is already stated in DSM, there is no hard
evidence available in the literature which proves that ADHD is
a brain disorder—something that denotes a deficit in people’s
brains [for a discussion, see American Psychiatric Association
(7), Batstra et al. (8), Schleim (9)].
The authors of DSM also leave unmentioned that the 74%
heritability estimate stems from twin-studies, which as a method
cannot reliably disentangle genetic from environmental factors
for psychiatric presentations [see, Joseph (10)]. GWAS on the
other hand yield a heritability estimate of 22%, and their
suggestive findings mentioned in the manual are yet lacking
convincing replication [e.g., (11)]. This challenges research to
account for the 50% gap in the assumed familial transmission
of ADHD, however, the authors of DSM have remained silent
about this.
The DSM-5-TR retains the same comment on the role
of social context as its predecessor. More specifically, it
is stated that “signs of the disorder may be minimal or
absent when the individual is receiving frequent rewards
for appropriate behavior, is under close supervision, is in a
novel setting, is engaged in especially interesting activities, has
consistent external stimulation (e.g., via electronic screens),
or is interacting in one-on-one situations (e.g., the clinician’s
office)” [(6), p. 71]. It is apparent that this statement
contradicts the conceptualization of ADHD provided in the
manual by undermining its existence as neurodevelopmental
disorder; how can frequent rewards, close supervision, novel
settings and interesting activities make a neurodevelopmental
disorder disappear?
Moreover, the DSM-5-TR includes a new and very
interesting statement in the “Prevalence” section: “prevalence
is higher in special populations such as foster children or
correctional settings” [(6), p. 72]. By merely stating the fact
without further discussion about psychosocial factors related
to ADHD diagnoses among population living in such settings,
the DSM-5-TR implies the role the alleged neurodevelopmental
disorder plays in these adverse life trajectories. The likelihood
that these children and young people have experienced trauma
and abuse at homes [see, for example, (12,13)] is not mentioned.
Other social factors that correlate with the manifestation of
inattentive, compulsive and/or hyperkinetic behaviors are not
discussed, such as poverty and socioeconomic hardship (14,15),
childhood trauma (16,17), child maltreatment (18), death in
the family (19), low family cohesion (20), parental psychiatric
disorder (19), parental separation (19), parental criminality (19),
household dysfunction (21), familial incarceration (14) and
parental long-term unemployment (22).
Leaving these factors out of the manual does not weaken the
neurodevelopmental hypothesis of behaviors and functioning
associated with the diagnostic category, as brain is a plastic
organ which development is affected by adverse life experiences.
However, silence about the complexity of the role of psychosocial
factors for the development of inattentive, compulsive and
hyperkinetic behaviors throughout the manual, and particularly
in connection with ADHD and foster or correctional setting,
implies essentialism. This bias toward biopsychological factors
is strengthened in section “Risk and Prognostic Factors”, where
psychosocial factors are vaguely referred to by noting that
“[f]amily interaction patterns in early childhood are unlikely
to cause ADHD but may influence its course or contribute
to secondary development of conduct problems” [(6), p. 73].
Thus, ADHD is assumed to expose affected individuals to being
vulnerable to adversities.
Frontiers in Psychiatry 02 frontiersin.org
Koutsoklenis and Honkasilta 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1064141
Essentialist neuropathological premise are also assumed
in the section “Diagnostic Features”, in which it is stated
that the “essential feature of attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD) is a persistent pattern of inattention and/or
hyperactivity-impulsivity that interferes with functioning or
development” [(6), p. 70]. It is difficult to comprehend how
children’s” own behaviors interfere with development without
explicating what kinds of development trajectories are in
question: neurodevelopment, school success, employment,
health or what? The non-biological developmental trajectories
are listed as functional consequences, thus, implying that
it is neurological development that is interfered here.
Understandably the logic here is that inattentive and/or
hyperactive-impulsive behaviors affect ability to function,
which in turn affects development, including that of the
brain. However, these behaviors let alone their potential
interferences with functioning imply psychosocial, societal,
and sociocultural aspects of development that in turn can
have biopsychological effects—not the other way around. And
again, this implied assertion of neuropathology—or whatever is
assumed to cause inattention and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity
that interferes development—is overtly invalidated by the
authors themselves by stating the lack of evidence supporting
brain disorder hypotheses.
3. Definition
The definition of ADHD remains the same in DSM-5-TR in
comparison to DSM-5. More specifically, it is stated that “ADHD
is a neurodevelopmental disorder defined by impairing levels of
inattention, disorganization, and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity.
Inattention and disorganization entail inability to stay on
task, seeming not to listen, and losing materials necessary for
tasks, at levels that are inconsistent with age or developmental
level. Hyperactivity-impulsivity entails overactivity, fidgeting,
inability to stay seated, intruding into other people’s activities,
and inability to wait— symptoms that are excessive for age
or developmental level” [6, p. 37]. This definition retains
the circular logic of the previous edition, that is “if A
then B, and if B then A.” (23). For the case of ADHD
specifically, this is translated to: “if an individual has attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder it is because he is inattentive,
disorganized and hyperactive-impulsive, and if an individual is
inattentive, disorganized and hyperactive-impulsive it is because
he has ADHD.
Without concrete and objective evidence of an identifiable
brain disorder there is nothing that explains behaviors
associated with ADHD diagnosis. ADHD as a diagnostic
entity remains a descriptive classification of behaviors, not an
explanation for them. When behaviors are explained by using
a descriptive classification, adhering to circular reasoning is
inescapable. Tautology is thus inevitably disguised as scientific
explanation (24). A characteristic example of this tautology is
evident in the section “Functional Consequences of Attention-
Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder”. The section states that “ADHD
is associated with reduced school performance and academic
attainment” [(6), p. 73] which are already entailed in the
diagnostic criteria in the first place. Thus, with “nothing for
ADHD to be actually tied to, all that remains are observations
about behavior [sic], which then act as both an indicator of,
and the defining criteria for, that initial disorder” [(23), p. 251].
In circular reasoning the argument refers to nothing outside
of itself.
4. Diagnostic criteria
The diagnostic criteria for ADHD in DSM-5-TR remained
identical with those that appear in the previous edition. In a
recently published paper [i.e., (25)] we have provided a thorough
critique of the accuracy of DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for ADHD.
To do that we have used as a blueprint the criticism for
descriptive diagnoses articulated by Kirk et al. (24). In our paper
we concluded that DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for ADHD are
ambiguous, redundant, and arbitrary (25). We also concluded
that they are ableist in the sense that they fortify normality and
that they pay inadequate attention to context and agency (25).
Since no changes were made in the diagnostic criteria for ADHD
in the DSM-5-TR our critique can be applied as such. Therefore,
we assert that no “precision” and subsequently no “enhanced
precision” can be claimed for the diagnostic criteria of ADHD
in the revised edition.
At this point we would also like to refer to the
categories of “Other Specified Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder” and “Unspecified Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder”1that were also present in the previous edition.
Both categories apply when “symptoms characteristic of
attention deficit/ hyperactivity disorder that cause clinically
significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or
other important areas of functioning predominate but do
not meet the full criteria for attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder or any of the disorders in the neurodevelopmental
disorders diagnostic class” [(6), p. 77]. That is, an individual is
1 The category “Other Specified Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder” is used in situations in which the clinician chooses to
communicate the specific reason that the presentation does not
meet the criteria for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder or any
specific neurodevelopmental disorder. The category ‘Unspecified
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder’ is used ‘in situations in which
the clinician chooses not to specify the reason that the criteria are
not met for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder or for a specific
neurodevelopmental disorder, and includes presentations in which there
is insucient information to make a more specific diagnosis [(6), p. 77].
Frontiers in Psychiatry 03 frontiersin.org
Koutsoklenis and Honkasilta 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1064141
diagnosed regardless of not meeting the diagnostic criteria for
the disorder.
In DSM-5, differences between males and females in
the frequency of ADHD (more frequent in males) and the
presentation of primarily inattentive features (females more
likely) were briefly discussed under section “Gender-related
Diagnostic Issues”. No explicit or implicit reference to potential
causes or factors leading to these differences were made. DSM-
5-TR presents two changes in this section. The tittle is changed
to “Sex- and Gender- Related Diagnostic Issues”, making a
conceptual distinction between biological notion of sex and
psychological, social, historical, and cultural aspects related to
biological sex (i.e., gender). Also, one sentence is added, stating
that differences in “ADHD symptom severity may be due to
differing genetic and cognitive liabilities between sexes” [(6),
p. 73].
Thus, in contrast to the previous edition, the authors of
DSM-5-TR explicitly imply the connection between inherent
features and the manifestation of so-called symptoms according
to sex. This attribution is strengthened by making a distinction
between sex and gender yet saying nothing about gender-
related factors (there is also silence about gender in “Culture-
Related Diagnostic Issues” section). Instead, diagnostic issues
related to sex and gender are reduced to biopsychological
aspects and assumptions related to female/male binary (i.e., sex)
(26). This is an example of essentialism. ADHD is portrayed
as having a fixed essence (i.e., genetic, neurodevelopmental
dysfunction) attributable to differences in binary sexes. This
completely disregards the socially constructed roles, behaviors,
expressions, and identities related to gender pluralism let alone
how sociocultural aspects (e.g., gender roles, cis normativity)
are intertwined with psychosocial aspects that may manifest as
behaviors deemed “symptoms” [see, for example, (27)].
Culture-related normative assumptions regarding behaviors
are discussed in the section about “Culture-Related Diagnostic
Issues” in similar fashion to the previous version of the manual,
that is, cultural bias in diagnostic practices is acknowledged.
Some apparent improvements have also taken place, suggesting
the need for “culturally competent diagnostic practices [. . . ]
in assessing ADHD” [(6), p. 73]. In addition to the previous
version, the interconnectedness of social class, race, and
ethnicity in both seeking for the diagnosis for schooling (namely
“non-Latinx White” parents) and affecting informant symptom
rating are mentioned. Also, DSM-5-TR has been reviewed and
revised by a Work Group on Ethnoracial Equity and Inclusion,
which can be seen in replacement of “Latino” with “Latinx”,
and in acknowledging of social oppression and racialization and
their interconnectedness with diagnosing.
In line with the previous version, ADHD is portrayed
as a neurodevelopmental condition within an individual
caused by natural development processes over which etiology
psychosocial, societal or cultural factors have no power. Instead,
these factors seem to be portrayed as hindering the adequate
detection and diagnosis of the condition, as evidenced in
the following statement: “Underdetection may result from
mislabeling of ADHD symptoms as oppositional or disruptive
in socially oppressed ethnic or racialized groups because of
explicit or implicit clinician bias)” [(6), p. 73]. Bluntly put,
it is of importance to apply culturally competent practices in
diagnosing members of socially oppressed ethnic or racialized
groups to ensure correct diagnosis.
This essentialist framework of Western psychiatry guided by
the DSM has long been criticized by feminist scholars within
and outside psychiatry, emphasizing the intersecting links
between psychological hardships and the broad social, economic
and political context [see, for example, the special issue by
Marecek and Gavey (28)]. While DSM-5-TR acknowledges the
intersecting axes of class and racial/ethnic categorization with
diagnostic judgments, it chooses to be oblivious and silent of
how various social categories (e.g., gender, class, racial/ethnic)
and broader contexts intersect with how behaviors and
functioning develop (i.e., biopsychosocial perspective) let alone
why they are diagnosed as neurodevelopmental disorders (i.e.,
sociocultural, and political perspectives) regardless of continues
incongruence of scientific rationale and clinical practices.
5. Discussion
As expected, DSM’s latest edition does not escape the
logical and scientific pitfalls of its predecessor (e.g., circular
reasoning, lack of explanatory power, accuracy related issues
of diagnostic criteria etc.). What is also pervasive in the
DSM-5-TR is an attempt to further solidify ADHD as a
neurodevelopmental disorder. Explicitly and implicitly, DSM-5-
TR creates the illusion that it represents real scientific progress
that validates ADHD as a neurodevelopmental disorder. In
contrast, scientific research on etiology and pathophysiology
of people diagnosed with ADHD rather questions the current
operationalization of ADHD as a categorical diagnosis in
line with the “neo-Kraepelinian” view of discrete boundaries
between health and disorder [e.g., 11]. Given DSM’s multifaceted
influence in organizing institutional (e.g., insurance eligibility,
disability payments, educational services, legal decisions),
academic (e.g., direction of research, fund allocations, course
and textbook contents), and social and psychological lives (e.g.,
identity recognition, stigma, empathy), [e.g., (25,2931)], it
seems unlikely that DSM would recategorize its classifications
according to science it purports to adhere to.
Finally, we would like to underline the importance of the
influence of the DSM since an ADHD diagnosis can expose
those diagnosed to potential harm. We will briefly illustrate
two examples here through the lens of the relative age effect
phenomenon. First, research clearly shows an international,
cross-cultural pattern of a relative-age effect in the diagnosis
of ADHD [see, for a review (32)]. Children with medicalized
Frontiers in Psychiatry 04 frontiersin.org
Koutsoklenis and Honkasilta 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1064141
behaviors are “railed” into certain ways of responses to those
behaviors. Findings from a recent cohort study suggest that
an ADHD diagnosis in childhood may not result in any
improvements in quality-of-life measures in adolescents and
may even negatively impact some outcomes, including the risk
of self-harm (33). Second, the relative age effect phenomenon
also concerns the pharmacological treatment for ADHD [see, for
a review, (34)]. Children are thus exposed to the adverse effects
of ADHD drugs which span from death, cardiac problems,
psychotic disorders (35) to reduced appetite, difficulty sleeping,
and abdominal pain (36).
On top of that, Panther et al. (37) found that most
ADHD drugs prescribed for very young children were off-
label, and raised concerns to lack of safety and efficacy
data. The United Nations (38) has expressed concerns about
the significant global increase in consumption of stimulants
such as Methylphenidate (common brand names include
Ritalin, Equasym, and Concerta). The report attributes this to
various causes such as an increase in the number of ADHD
diagnoses, misdiagnosis of ADHD, influential commercial
and/or aggressive pharmaceutical marketing practices, and
public pressure, such as parents’ associations lobbying for their
children’s right to access ADHD medication [see also (39)]. In
this regard, a recent meta-analysis of pediatric psychotropic drug
prevalence of ADHD in the Global North reports a lack of
systematic monitoring in most of the studied 23 countries (40).
DSM-5-TR is likely to contribute rather than avert these trends.
Author contributions
All authors listed have made a substantial, direct,
and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it
for publication.
Funding
The University of Helsinki has paid the fee for the
publication of this manuscript.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the
absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the
authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated
organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the
reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or
claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed
or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Jacobs DH, Cohen D. The end of neo-kraepelinism. Ethical
Hum Psychol Psychiatry. (2012) 14:87–90. doi: 10.1891/1559-4343.
14.2.87
2. Davies J. Cracked.The Unhappy Truth About Psychiatry. (2013). New York:
Pegasus Books.
3. Frances A. Saving Normal. An Insiders Revolt Against Out-of-Control
Psychiatric Diagnosis, DSM-5, Big Pharma, and theMedicalization of Ordinary Life.
(2013). New York: HarperCollins Publishers.
4. The British Psychological Society. Response to the American Psychiatric
Association: DSM-5 Development. (2011). Available at: https://dxrevisionwatch.
files.wordpress.com/2012/02/dsm-5- 2011-bps- response.pdf (accessed on 8
November 2022).
5. Roy M, Rivest MP, Namian D, Moreau N. The critical reception of the
DSM-5: towards a typology of audiences. Public Understand Sci. (2019) 28:932–
948. doi: 10.1177/0963662519868969
6. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders, 5th edn, Text Revision. (2022). Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Association.
7. Batstra L, Nieweg E, Hadders-Algra M. Exploring five common assumptions
on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Acta Pediatr. (2014) 103:696–
700. doi: 10.1111/apa.12642
8. Schleim S. Why mental disorders are brain disorders and why they are not:
ADHD and the challenges of heterogeneity and reification. Front Psychiatry. (2022)
13:943049. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.943049
9. Timimi S. Insane Medicine: How the Mental Health Industry CreatesD amaging
Treatment Traps and How You Can Escape Them. (2021). Available online at:
from: https://www.madinamerica.com/insane-medicine/ (retrieved on September
5, 2022).
10. Joseph J. The Trouble with Twin Studies: A Reassessment of Twin Research in
the Social and Behavioral Sciences. New York: Routledge (2015).
11. Posner J, Polanczyk GV, Sonuga-Barke E. Attention-deficit hyperactivity
disorder. Lancet. (2020) 395:450–62. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(19)33004-1
12. Dorsey S, Burns BJ, Southerland DG, Cox JR, Wagner HR, Farmer EM.
Prior trauma exposure for youth in treatment foster care. J Child Fam Stud. (2012)
21:816–24. doi: 10.1007/s10826-011-9542-4
13. Wilson C, Pence DM, Conradi L. Trauma-informed care. Encycl of Social
Work. (2013). doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780199975839.013.1063
14. Brown NM, Brown SN, Briggs RD, Germán M, Belamarich PF, Oyeku SO,
et al. Associations between adverse childhood experiences and ADHD diagnosis
and severity. Acad Pediatr. (2017) 17:349–55. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2016.08.013
15. Russell G, Ford T, Rosenberg R, Kelly S. The association of attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder with socioeconomic disadvantage: alternative explanations
and evidence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2014) 55:436–45. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.
12170
16. Erlandsson SI, Hornborg C, Sorbring E, Dauman ND. Is ADHD a way of
conceptualizing long-term emotional stress and social disadvantage? Front Public
Health. (2022) 10:966900. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.966900
17. Gul H, Gurkan KC. Child maltreatment and associated parental factors
among children with ADHD: A comparative study. J Atten Disord. (2018) 22:1278.
doi: 10.1177/1087054716658123
18. Ouyang L, Fang X, Mercy J, Perou R, Grosse SD. Attention-
Deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and child maltreatment: a population-
based study. J Pediatr. (2008) 153:851–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.06.002
19. Björkenstam E, Björkenstam C, Jablonska B, Kosidou K. Cumulative
exposure to childhood adversity and treated attention deficit/hyperactivity
Frontiers in Psychiatry 05 frontiersin.org
Koutsoklenis and Honkasilta 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1064141
disorder: a cohort study of 543 650 adolescents and young adults in Sweden.
Psychol Med. (2018) 48:498–507. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001933
20. Duh-Leong C, Fuller A, Brown NM. Associations between family
and community protective factors and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder outcomes among US children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2020)
41:1–8. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000720
21. Jimenez ME, Wade RJ, Schwartz-Soicher O, Lin Y, Reichman NE. Adverse
childhood experiences and ADHD diagnosis at age 9 years in a national urban
sample. Acad Pediatr. (2017) 17:356–61. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2016.12.009
22. Christoffersen MN. Sexual crime against schoolchildren with disabilities:
a Nationwide Prospective Birth Cohort Study. J Interpers Viol. (2020)
088626052093444. doi: 10.23889/ijpds.v4i3.1198
23. Tait G. The logic of ADHD: a brief review of fallacious reasoning. Stud Philo
Educ. (2009) 28:239–54. doi: 10.1007/s11217-008-9114-2
24. Kirk SA, Gomory T, Cohen D. Mad Science: Psychiatric Coercion, Diagnosis,
and Drugs. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction (2013).
25. Honkasilta J, Koutsoklenis A. The (un)real existence of ADHD—
Criteria, functions, and forms of the diagnostic entity. Front Sociol. (2022)
7:814763. doi: 10.3389/fsoc.2022.814763
26. Amoretti MC. The notion of gender in psychiatry: a focus on dsm-5. Not
Polit V36 N139. (2020) 139:70–82. Available online at: https://philpapers.org/rec/
AMOTNO
27. Goicoechea J. Invoking and inscribing mental illness: A discursive analysis
of diagnostic terminology in inpatient treatment planning meetings. Fem Psychol.
(2013) 23:107–18. doi: 10.1177/0959353512467973
28. Marecek J, Gavey N. DSM-5 and beyond: A critical feminist engagement with
psychodiagnosis. Fem Psych. (2013) 23:3–9. doi: 10.1177/0959353512467962
29. Koutsoklenis A. Functions of the ADHD diagnosis in educational contexts.
Metalogos. (2020) 36:1–10. Available online at: https://metalogos-systemic-
therapy-journal.eu/gr/issue/article/36- 13
30. Koutsoklenis A, Gaitanidis A. Interrogating the effectiveness of educational
practices: A critique of evidence-based psychosocial treatments for children
diagnosed with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Front Educ. (2017)
2:11. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2017.00011
31. Timimi S. Non-diagnostic based approaches to helping children who could
be labelled ADHD and their families. Int J Qual Stud Health Well-being. (2017)
12:1298270. doi: 10.1080/17482631.2017.1298270
32. Koutsoklenis A, Honkasilta J, Brunila K. Reviewing and
reframing the influence of relative age on ADHD diagnosis: beyond
individual psycho(patho)logy. Pedagogy Cult Soc. (2020) 28:165–
81. doi: 10.1080/14681366.2019.1624599
33. Kazda L, McGeechan K, Bell K, Thomas R, Barratt A. Association of
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder diagnosis with adolescent quality of life.
JAMA Netw Open. (2022) 5:e2236364. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.36364
34. Whitely M, Raven M, Timimi S, Jureidini J, Phillimore J, Leo J, et al. Annual
research review: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder late birthdate effect
common in both high and low prescribing international jurisdictions: a systematic
review. J Child Psychol Psychiatr. (2019) 60:380–91. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12991
35. Storebø OJ, Pedersen N, Ramstad E, Kielsholm ML, Nielsen SS,
Krogh HB, et al. Methylphenidate for attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD) in children and adolescents assessment of adverse
events in non-randomised studies. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2018)
5:CD012069. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD012069.pub2
36. Santos GM, Santos EM, Mendes GD, Fragoso YD, Souza MR, Martimbianco
ALC, et al. A review of Cochrane reviews on pharmacological treatment for
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Dement Neuropsychol. (2021) 15:421–
7. doi: 10.1590/1980-57642021dn15-040001
37. Panther SG, Knotts AM, Odom-Maryon T, Daratha K, Woo T, Klein TA,
et al. Off-label prescribing trends for ADHD medications in very young children. J
Pediatr Pharmacol Ther. (2017) 22:423–9. doi: 10.5863/1551-6776-22.6.423
38. United Nations. International Narcotics Control Board.Vienna: Report
2014. (2014). Available online at: https://www.incb.org/documents/Publications/
AnnualReports/AR2014/English/AR_2014.pdf (accessed on December 22, 2021)
39. Conrad P, Bergey MR. The impending globalization of ADHD: Notes on
the expansion and growth of a medicalized disorder. Soc Sci Med. (2014) 122:31–
43. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.10.019
40. Piovani D, Clavenna A, Bonati M. Prescription prevalence of psychotropic
drugs in children and adolescents: an analysis of international data. Eur J Clin
Pharmacol. (2019) 75:1333–46. doi: 10.1007/s00228-019-02711-3
Frontiers in Psychiatry 06 frontiersin.org
... Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by patterns of inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity that are pervasive, impairing, and inconsistent with the developmental level of a person [1]. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) states that for a diagnosis, symptoms should appear before age 12, be noticeable in multiple settings like home and school, and significantly affect daily functioning [2], [3]. ...
... ChatGPT-4 Turbo is a highly optimized version of the ChatGPT-4 model, designed for faster response times and improved efficiency in generating text[9].2 Claude-3 is an AI model designed for conversational understanding, focusing on generating coherent and contextually relevant responses. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity, which can significantly impact an individual's daily functioning and quality of life. Occupational therapy plays a crucial role in managing ADHD by fostering the development of skills needed for daily living and enhancing an individual's ability to participate fully in school, home, and social situations. Recent studies highlight the potential of integrating Large Language Models (LLMs) like ChatGPT and Socially Assistive Robots (SAR) to improve psychological treatments. This integration aims to overcome existing limitations in mental health therapy by providing tailored support and adapting to the unique needs of this sensitive group. However, there remains a significant gap in research exploring the combined use of these advanced technologies in ADHD therapy, suggesting an opportunity for novel therapeutic approaches. Thus, we integrated two advanced language models, ChatGPT-4 Turbo and Claude-3 Opus, into a robotic assistant to explore how well each model performs in robot-assisted interactions. Additionally, we have compared their performance in a simulated therapy scenario to gauge their effectiveness against a clinically validated customized model. The results of this study show that ChatGPT-4 Turbo excelled in performance and responsiveness, making it suitable for time-sensitive applications. Claude-3 Opus, on the other hand, showed strengths in understanding, coherence, and ethical considerations, prioritizing safe and engaging interactions. Both models demonstrated innovation and adaptability, but ChatGPT-4 Turbo offered greater ease of integration and broader language support. The selection between them hinges on the specific demands of ADHD therapy.
... Affected children are exposed to all kinds of injuries, including academic problems, behavioral disorders, and the risk of other disorders increasing. Therefore, in order to reduce the damage caused by the above problems, early intervention in the home and school environment becomes necessary [1,2]. A review of the prevalence of ADHD shows that there is a significant percentage of school children with ADHD among relatively diverse populations (geographically, racially, socially, and economically). ...
... The learning problems questionnaire consists of 20 items, which are completed by the students' parents. Each statement is answered on a 5-point Likert scale from not at all [1] to always [5]: the higher the score, the more learning problems the child has. The validity of the learning problems questionnaire and its components have been checked by the creators of the questionnaire with internal consistency and retesting methods and acceptable values have been obtained. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common disorders in school-aged children. Learning disorder (LD) is also one of the most important psychiatric disorders in children, which can often be associated with ADHD. In this study, we sought to compare self-esteem and quality of life in 8 to 12-year-old children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder with and without co-occurring learning disorders in order to emphasize the importance of attention and diagnosis in children with ADHD. Method Among the 8- to 12-year-old outpatients referred to the child and adolescent psychiatry clinic of Omid Babol Clinic, 120 children aged 8 to 12 years with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder whose disease was diagnosed by a child and adolescent psychiatry subspecialist. Among the tools used to collect information was the Colorado Learning Difficulties Questionnaire by Wilcott et al. (CLDQ), five-scale self-esteem test of Pepe et al. (1989) for children and quality-of-life questionnaire for 8–12-year-old children (PedsQL). Results This study investigated self-esteem and quality of life in children with ADHD (n = 120, 51.7% boys). Children with ADHD and learning disabilities reported significantly lower self-esteem and quality of life compared to those with ADHD alone. Conclusion Considering the relatively high probability of co-occurrence of ADHD and learning disorders, if one of them is diagnosed in a child, it is possible to look for other disorders in the child in order to avoid the more severe negative effects that this co-occurrence can have on the child by diagnosing it as soon as possible.
... The etiology of ADHD is still poorly recognized. However, there are indications of genetic and environmental factors contributing to this disorder (Koutsoklenis et al. 2023). On the other hand, epigenetic changes, especially microRNAs, have been suggested to be involved in factors that govern the progress of ADHD. ...
Article
Full-text available
ADHD has huge knowledge gaps concerning its etiology. MicroRNAs (miRNAs) provide promising diagnostic biomarkers of human pathophysiology and may be a novel therapeutic option. The aim was to investigate the levels of miR-34c-3p, miR-155, miR-138-1, miR-296-5p, and plasma brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in a group of children with ADHD compared to neurotypicals and to explore correlations between these measures and some clinical data. The participants were children with ADHD in Group I (N = 41; age: 8.2 ± 2) and neurotypical ones in Group II (N = 40; age: 8.6 ± 2.5). Group I was subjected to clinical examination, the Stanford Binet intelligence scale-5, the preschool language scale, and Conner’s parent rating scale-R. Measuring the expression levels of the miRNAs was performed by qRT-PCR for all participants. The BDNF level was measured by ELISA. The lowest scores on the IQ subtest were knowledge and working memory. No discrepancies were noticed between the receptive and expressive language ages. The highest scores on the Conner’s scale were those for cognitive problems. Participants with ADHD exhibited higher plasma BDNF levels compared to controls (p = 0.0003). Expression patterns of only miR-34c-3p and miR-138-1 were downregulated with significant statistical differences (p˂0.01). However, expression levels of miR-296-5p showed negative correlation with the total scores of IQ (p = 0.03). MiR-34c-3p, miR-138-1, while BDNF showed good diagnostic potential. The downregulated levels of miR-34c-3p and miR-138-1, together with high BDNF levels, are suggested to be involved in the etiology of ADHD in Egyptian children. Gender differences influenced the expression patterns of miRNAs only in children with ADHD.
... Some experts do feel the change in diagnostic criteria did not have a sound scientific base [16]. The concern was not just about influence from the pharma lobby, but from parents who felt their children had ADHD and needed easier access to medication [17]. Not every child with attentional difficulty has ADHD. ...
Article
Full-text available
The relationship between the pharmaceutical companies and the healthcare profession, especially doctors, has always been fraught with conflicts of interest (COI). The publication of the influential The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders, Fifth edition, Text Revision (DSM-5-TR), by the American Psychiatric Society (APA) raised concerns that the financial relationships between pharma and members responsible for DSM could result in bias. This resulted in calls for stricter enforcement of controls on financial conflict of interest (FCOI) [1, 2], which could influence the formulation of diagnostic criteria (resulting in more people being "diagnosable as mentally ill"), creating a larger pool of "patients" who "need" pharmaceutical drugs. Knowingly or unknowingly, they would end up serving the pharmaceutical companies' agenda to sell more drugs and drive up profits [2] .
... ADHD is a neurodevelopmental disorder that is defined by consistently impaired levels of attention, disorganization, hyperactivity, and impulsivity that substantially interfere with social, academic, and occupational functioning (DSM-5-TR, 2022;Koutsoklenis & Honkasilta, 2023). ADHD has three presentations: inattentive, hyperactive-impulsive, and combination (both inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive); it also has three severity levels: mild, moderate, and severe. ...
Research Proposal
Full-text available
In current times, where attention is a precious resource, worry runs rampant, and emotions are at an all-time high, this study aims to investigate the relationships between Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) symptoms, Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD) symptoms, executive functioning, and emotion regulation. ADHD, characterized by persistent inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity, and GAD, marked by excessive worry, are prevalent mental health conditions (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition-TR, 2022; National Comorbidity Survey, 2007). Executive functions, comprising higher-order cognitive processes like planning, inhibition, initiation, and monitoring, includes emotion regulation (Otero & Barker, 2013). Emotion regulation involves monitoring, evaluating, and managing emotions' intensity, duration, and expression (Thompson, 1994). Prior literature establishes connections between emotion regulation, ADHD symptoms, and GAD symptoms, (Christiansen et al., 2019; Groves et al., 2020; Reimherr et al., 2017); however, previous research has not examined which specific facets of emotion regulation are most relevant to ADHD. The proposed study aims to replicate previous research by examining the relationships among emotion regulation, ADHD, and GAD, extending the literature by exploring the specific facets of emotion regulation implicated, particularly for ADHD. The implications of this research extend to psychologists, researchers, and other social science professionals. By shedding light on the intricate relationships between ADHD, GAD, executive functions, and emotion regulation, the study provides insights that can guide tailored, individualized interventions. Researchers gain a different perspective on understanding ADHD-GAD comorbidity, while the findings contribute to enhancing therapeutic strategies and mental healthcare. By advancing psychological research and clinical practice, it offers transformative potential for treatments of both disorders.
... paper critiquing the text revision indicated that only a little has changed but was considered insignificant (Koutsoklenis and Honkasilta 2022). So, there appear to be age influences on ADHD, but they were not thoroughly presented and discussed. ...
Article
Full-text available
Even though the number of studies aiming to improve comprehension of ADHD pathology has increased in recent years, there still is an urgent need for more effective studies, particularly in understanding adult ADHD, both at preclinical and clinical levels, due to the increasing evidence that adult ADHD is highly distinct and a different entity from childhood ADHD. This review paper outlines the symptoms, diagnostics, and neurobiological mechanisms of ADHD, with emphasis on how adult ADHD could be different from childhood-onset. Data show a difference in the environmental, genetic, epigenetic, and brain structural changes, when combined, could greatly impact the behavioral presentations and the severity of ADHD in adults. Furthermore, a crucial aspect in the quest to fully understand this disorder could be through longitudinal analysis. In this way, we will determine if and how the pathology and pharmacology of ADHD change with age. This goal could revolutionize our understanding of the disorder and address the weaknesses in the current clinical classification systems, improving the characterization and validity of ADHD diagnosis, specifically those in adults.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter critically assesses the analytical framework on universal human needs developed by Doyal and Gough (1991). This critical assessment focuses on the limitations associated with the endorsement of the biomedical model when addressing the basic need of health. We illustrate these limitations with particular reference to the case of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) which blurs the boundaries between physical and mental health. We move along three interrelated levels. First, we expose the deficiencies associated with the biomedical model. Second, we illustrate how the conceptualization of mental health within this model affects the form and content of appropriate health care and education. Third, we question the dual strategy based on the expert codified and user's experiential knowledge. The main intention is to defy allegations of scientific neutrality in social policy formation and open up new research directions which allow for alternative systems of social provisioning.
Article
Full-text available
Background. ADHD is a neurodevelopmental disorder that is accompanied by executive challenges. Objectives. To obtain evidence of the usefulness of the BRIEF-P and to analyze the possible ceiling and floor effect of its scores in the assessment of executive function in preschoolers with signs compatible with a possible diagnosis of ADHD. Method. A search was performed in Science Direct, NCBI (PubMed), and ProQuest Education Journals during the period 2012–2022. We included studies that evaluated samples of individuals with symptomatology compatible with ADHD, with an age range between 2 and 6 years, published in English or Spanish. Of a total of 2538 articles, only seven met the inclusion criteria. The risk of bias was assessed using the QUADAS-2 questionnaire. The main variables were age and executive functioning. Conclusions. Executive deficits in early-age individuals with symptoms compatible with ADHD are more extensive than just deficits in working memory. A floor effect has been found in tests associated with hot executive functions and a ceiling effect in cold executive functions. This makes it necessary to use different tests to assess executive performance in preschoolers with ADHD-compatible symptomatology and to design intervention proposals accordingly. The BRIEF-P is an instrument that facilitates obtaining a sensitive and discriminative executive profile, although it should be used in combination with other neuropsychological performance tests.
Article
Full-text available
Dopamine regulates several functions, such as voluntary movements, spatial memory, motivation, sleep, arousal, feeding, immune function, maternal behaviors, and lactation. Less clear is the role of dopamine in the pathophysiology of type 2 diabetes mellitus (T2D) and chronic complications and conditions frequently associated with it. This review summarizes recent evidence on the role of dopamine in regulating insular metabolism and activity, the pathophysiology of traditional chronic complications associated with T2D, the pathophysiological interconnection between T2D and chronic neurological and psychiatric disorders characterized by impaired dopamine activity/metabolism, and therapeutic implications. Reinforcing dopamine signaling is therapeutic in T2D, especially in patients with dopamine-related disorders, such as Parkinson’s and Huntington’s diseases, addictions, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. On the other hand, although specific trials are probably needed, certain medications approved for T2D (e.g., metformin, pioglitazone, incretin-based therapy, and gliflozins) may have a therapeutic role in such dopamine-related disorders due to anti-inflammatory and anti-oxidative effects, improvement in insulin signaling, neuroinflammation, mitochondrial dysfunction, autophagy, and apoptosis, restoration of striatal dopamine synthesis, and modulation of dopamine signaling associated with reward and hedonic eating. Last, targeting dopamine metabolism could have the potential for diagnostic and therapeutic purposes in chronic diabetes-related complications, such as diabetic retinopathy.
Article
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder has increasingly been conceptualized as a disorder of abnormal brain connectivity. However, far less is known about the structural covariance in different subtypes of this disorder and how those differences may contribute to the symptomology of these subtypes. In this study, we used a combined volumetric-based methodology and structural covariance approach to investigate structural covariance of subcortical brain volume in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-combined and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-inattentive patients. In addition, a linear support vector machine was used to predict patient’s attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Results showed that compared with TD children, those with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-combined exhibited decreased volume of both the left and right pallidum. Moreover, we found increased right hippocampal volume in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-inattentive children. Furthermore and when compared with the TD group, both attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-combined and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-inattentive groups showed greater nonhomologous inter-regional correlations. The abnormal structural covariance network in the attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-combined group was located in the left amygdala–left putamen/left pallidum/right pallidum and right pallidum–left pallidum; in the attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-inattentive group, this difference was noted in the left hippocampus–left amygdala/left putamen/right putamen and right hippocampus–left amygdala. Additionally, different combinations of abnormalities in subcortical structural covariance were predictive of symptom severity in different attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes. Collectively, our findings demonstrated that structural covariance provided valuable diagnostic markers for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes.
Article
Full-text available
Background The prevalent, neuropsychiatric, deficit perspective on children and youth diagnosed with ADHD prohibits a multidimensional approach where socio-economic status, family stress and relationships within the families are relevant factors to examine. Assessments of ADHD through the use of rating scales and short-term interventions may lead not only to overdiagnosis but also to a reductionistic approach in the psychiatric field. This literature review aims to address research outside the prevailing discourse on ADHD as an organic brain dysfunction and broaden the perspectives on children's behavioral difficulties. Methods The articles included in this applied, mixed-method, systematic review includes 26 peer-reviewed articles, both English and French, with a search focus on ADHD in children and youth related to Attachment styles and relationships. Results In the studies reported, researchers approached correlations between ADHD and attachment in different ways, and in most cases, there was a caution to address causality. The role of parents was found to be both buffering and aggravating for the appearance of ADHD. In the French case studies, the diagnosis was conceptualized as a relational phenomenon where the child's behavior was inseparable from family member's suffering. Discussion This review article illustrates how children's difficulties in terms of ADHD symptoms can be addressed through a paradigm where emotional and cognitive dysregulation is understood through psychosocial factors rather than as a neurological condition. In our view, to avoid an overly reductionistic and medicalized approach to children's behavioral difficulties, it is time to reiterate the value of the biopsychosocial perspective. Conclusion Professionals and researchers need to acknowledge that becoming diagnosed with ADHD has a strong connection to economic disadvantage, social status, and familial care. The academic discourse of addressing brain dysfunctions might serve the unintended purpose of masking emotional stress and social disadvantage that manifests across generations. A biopsychosocial approach to ADHD including family, emotional history, and socio-economic issues could imply a lesser focus on medical treatment as a first choice.
Article
Full-text available
Importance: Appropriate diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) can improve some short-term outcomes in children and adolescents, but little is known about the association of a diagnosis with their quality of life (QOL). Objective: To compare QOL in adolescents with and without an ADHD diagnosis. Design, setting, and participants: This cohort study followed an emulated target trial design using prospective, observational data from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children, a representative, population-based prospective cohort study with biennial data collection from 2006 to 2018 with 8 years of follow-up (ages 6-7 to 14-15 years). Propensity score matching was used to ensure children with and without ADHD diagnosis were well matched on a wide range of variables, including hyperactive/inattentive (H/I) behaviors. Eligible children were born in 1999 to 2000 or 2003 to 2004 and did not have a previous ADHD diagnosis. All incident ADHD cases were matched with controls. Data were analyzed from July 2021 to January 2022. Exposures: Incident parent-reported ADHD diagnosis at age 6 to 7, 8 to 9, 10 to 11, 12 to 13, or 14 to 15. Main outcomes and measures: Quality of life at age 14 to 15 was measured with Child Health Utility 9D (CHU9D) and 8 other prespecified, self-reported measures mapped to the World Health Organization's QOL domains. Pooled regression models were fitted for each outcome, with 95% CIs and P values calculated using bootstrapping to account for matching and repeat observations. Results: Of 8643 eligible children, a total of 393 adolescents had an ADHD diagnosis (284 [72.2%] boys; mean [SD] age, 10.03 [0.30] years; mean [SD] H/I Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire score, 5.05 [2.29]) and were age-, sex-, and H/I score-matched with 393 adolescents without ADHD diagnosis at time zero. Compared with adolescents without diagnosis, those with an ADHD diagnosis reported similar QOL on CHU9D (mean difference, -0.03; 95% CI, -0.07 to 0.01; P = .10), general health (mean difference, 0.11; 95% CI, -0.04 to 0.27; P = .15), happiness (mean difference, -0.18; 95% CI, -0.37 to 0.00; P = .05), and peer trust (mean difference, 0.65; 95% CI, 0.00 to 1.30; P = .05). Diagnosed adolescents had worse psychological sense of school membership (mean difference, -2.58; 95% CI, -1.13 to -4.06; P < .001), academic self-concept (mean difference, -0.14; 95% CI, -0.02 to -0.26; P = .02), and self-efficacy (mean difference, -0.20; 95% CI, -0.05 to -0.33; P = .007); displayed more negative social behaviors (mean difference, 1.56; 95% CI, 0.55 to 2.66; P = .002); and were more likely to harm themselves (odds ratio, 2.53; 95% CI, 1.49 to 4.37; P < .001) than adolescents without diagnosis. Conclusions and relevance: In this cohort study, ADHD diagnosis was not associated with any self-reported improvements in adolescents' QOL compared with adolescents with similar levels of H/I behaviors but no ADHD diagnosis. ADHD diagnosis was associated with worse scores in some outcomes, including significantly increased risk of self-harm. A large, randomized clinical trial with long-term follow-up is needed.
Article
Full-text available
Scientific attempts to identify biomarkers to reliably diagnose mental disorders have thus far been unsuccessful. This has inspired the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) approach which decomposes mental disorders into behavioral, emotional, and cognitive domains. This perspective article argues that the search for biomarkers in psychiatry presupposes that the present mental health categories reflect certain (neuro-) biological features, that is, that these categories are reified as biological states or processes. I present two arguments to show that this assumption is very unlikely: First, the heterogeneity (both within and between subjects) of mental disorders is grossly underestimated, which is particularly salient for an example like Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Second, even the search for the biological basis of psychologically more basic categories (cognitive and emotional processes) than the symptom descriptions commonly used in mental disorder classifications has thus far been inconclusive. While philosophers have discussed this as the problem of mind-body-reductionism for ages, Turkheimer presented a theoretical framework comparing weak and strong biologism which is more useful for empirical research. This perspective article concludes that mental disorders are brain disorders in the sense of weak, but not strong biologism. This has important implications for psychiatric research: The search for reliable biomarkers for mental disorder categories we know is unlikely to ever be successful. This implies that biology is not the suitable taxonomic basis for psychiatry, but also psychology at large.
Article
Full-text available
The contemporary conceptualization of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) as a complex, multifactorial neurodevelopmental disorder cannot be understood as such without a complex assemblage of political, economic, and cultural processes that deem the conceptualization to be valuable and useful. In this article we use the notion of psychiatrization as a lens through which to see parts of these processes that make up ADHD what it is. In the first part of the article, we critically assess the scientific basis of the ADHD diagnosis via examining its diagnostic criteria as presented in the current fifth edition of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM), the so called “Bible” of modern psychiatry. The second part of the article asks what is done with the ADHD diagnostic entity and with the idea that it represents a natural neurodevelopmental state within an individual—something an individual has—as represented in the DSM-5. Drawn from our previous research, we analyze how ADHD becomes real in discourse practice as a powerful semiotic mediator through analysis of the various functions and forms in which it takes shape in institutional, social, and individual levels. We conclude that the frequent changes in the diagnostic criteria of ADHD do not reflect any real scientific progress. Among other reasons, they change to match better the maneuvers of individuals when navigating an increasingly psychiatrized society in the search for recognition, support, category membership, immunity, sympathy, and sense of belonging.
Article
Full-text available
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most frequent childhood psychiatric problems. Objective: The objective of this study was to identify, synthesize the results, and critically evaluate all Cochrane systematic reviews (SRs) on the pharmacological interventions for children and adolescents (up to age 18) diagnosed with ADHD. Methods: The search was performed in the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (via Wiley) in July 2020. Results: The search strategy resulted in four SRs of high methodological quality, analyzing 51 randomized clinical trials (9,013 participants). Compared to placebo, treatment with tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs) (desipramine), amphetamine, and methylphenidate showed improvement in symptoms such as difficulty concentrating, impulsivity, and hyperactivity in the short term (up to 6 months). There was an increase in the occurrence of adverse events, such as reduced appetite, difficulty sleeping, and abdominal pain. Insufficient evidence was found to support the effects of supplementation with polyunsaturated fatty acids. Conclusions: The use of TCAs, amphetamine, and methylphenidate in children and adolescents with ADHD seems to present positive effects and higher rates of minor adverse events when compared to placebo.
Article
Full-text available
Background The rate of violent and sexual victimization against children with disabilities is thought to be lower than the rate for children without disabilities but several studies shows otherwise. Aims The study focuses on examining sexual crime against children with disabilities and explaining differences in victimization in order to elucidate to what extent types of disability, family disadvantages, gender, high-risk behavior, location influences adolescents’ risk of sexual victimization. Previous population studies lack scientifically sound research methodology and results are weak or inconclusive. Method data is based on a national study of reported sexual crime against children in Denmark aged between 7 and 18 years of age using total birth cohorts (N=678,000). The statistical analysis is a discrete time Cox-model. An extended list of potential risk factors was included in the analysis in order to adjust for confounding. The potentially confounding risk-factors were collected independently from various population-based registers, e.g. employment statistics, housing statistics, education statistics, income compensation benefits, and population statistics (e.g. gender, age, location). Hospital records with information on types of disability based on the national inpatient register and national psychiatric register were collected independently of the collection of law enforcements records about reported sexual offences under the Danish Central Crime Register. Results Children with disabilities are more likely to be victimized of a reported sexual crime than non-disabled children: ADHD odds ratio: 3.7 (3.5-3.9), mental retardation: 3.8 (3.6-4.0), autism 3.8 (3.6-4.0). Conclusion The present study finds that family disadvantage e.g.parental substance abuse, parental violence, family separation, the child in care, and parental unemployment indicate an increased risk of being a victim of a sexual crime. Assessment of risk factors may permit professionals to facilitate prevention and treatment interventions. The study underreports the size of the problem because adolescents with disabilities face barriers when reporting victimization.
Article
In this paper I review how the notion of gender is understood in psychiatry, specifically in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5). First, I examine the contraposition between sex and gender, and argue that it is still retained by DSM-5, even though with some caveats. Second, I claim that, even if genderqueer people are not pathologized and gender pluralism is the background assumption, some diagnostic criteria still conceal a residue of gender dualism and essentialism. Third, I consider gender dysphoria, which is characterized by an incongruence between one’s experienced or expressed gender and one’s assigned gender; since this condition pertains to distress and disability, not to the incongruence per se, it does not pathologize transgender people. Still, I contend that it should be removed from DSM-5 for theoretical reasons.
Article
Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), like other psychiatric disorders, represents an evolving construct that has been refined and developed over the past several decades in response to research into its clinical nature and structure. The clinical presentation and course of the disorder have been extensively characterised. Efficacious medication-based treatments are available and widely used, often alongside complementary psychosocial approaches. However, their effectiveness has been questioned because they might not address the broader clinical needs of many individuals with ADHD, especially over the longer term. Non-pharmacological approaches to treatment have proven less effective than previously thought, whereas scientific and clinical studies are starting to fundamentally challenge current conceptions of the causes of ADHD in ways that might have the potential to alter clinical approaches in the future. In view of this, we first provide an account of the diagnosis, epidemiology, and treatment of ADHD from the perspective of both the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders and the eleventh edition of the International Classification of Diseases. Second, we review the progress in our understanding of the causes and pathophysiology of ADHD on the basis of science over the past decade or so. Finally, using these discoveries, we explore some of the key challenges to both the current models and the treatment of ADHD, and the ways in which these findings can promote new perspectives.