ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Abstract

Background: Neurophysiological models link dissociation (e.g. feeling detached during or after a traumatic event) to hypoarousal. It is currently assumed that the initial passive reaction to a threat may coincide with a blunted autonomic response, which constitutes the dissociative subtype of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Objective: Within this systematic review we summarize research which evaluates autonomic nervous system activation (e.g. heart rate, blood pressure) and dissociation in PTSD patients to discern the validity of current neurophysiological models of trauma-related hypoarousal. Method: Of 553 screened articles, 28 studies (N = 1300 subjects) investigating the physiological response to stress provocation or trauma-related interventions were included in the final analysis. Results: No clear trend exists across all measured physiological markers in trauma-related dissociation. Extracted results are inconsistent, in part due to high heterogeneity in experimental methodology. Conclusion: The current review is unable to provide robust evidence that peri- and posttraumatic dissociation are associated with hypoarousal, questioning the validity of distinct psychophysiological profiles in PTSD.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=zept20
European Journal of Psychotraumatology
ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/zept20
Trauma-related dissociation and the autonomic
nervous system: a systematic literature review
of psychophysiological correlates of dissociative
experiencing in PTSD patients
Sarah Beutler, Yoki L. Mertens, Liliana Ladner, Julia Schellong, Ilona Croy &
Judith K. Daniels
To cite this article: Sarah Beutler, Yoki L. Mertens, Liliana Ladner, Julia Schellong, Ilona
Croy & Judith K. Daniels (2022) Trauma-related dissociation and the autonomic nervous
system: a systematic literature review of psychophysiological correlates of dissociative
experiencing in PTSD patients, European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 13:2, 2132599, DOI:
10.1080/20008066.2022.2132599
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/20008066.2022.2132599
© 2022 The Author(s). Published by Informa
UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis
Group
View supplementary material
Published online: 02 Nov 2022. Submit your article to this journal
View related articles View Crossmark data
REVIEW ARTICLE
Trauma-related dissociation and the autonomic nervous system: a systematic
literature review of psychophysiological correlates of dissociative
experiencing in PTSD patients
Sarah Beutler
a
*, Yoki L. Mertens
b
*, Liliana Ladner
a,c
, Julia Schellong
a
, Ilona Croy
a,d
and Judith K. Daniels
b
a
Department of Psychotherapy and Psychosomatic Medicine, Medical Faculty, Technical University of Dresden, Dresden, Germany;
b
Department of Clinical Psychology and Experimental Psychopathology, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands;
c
Virginia Tech
Carilion School of Medicine, Roanoke, VA, USA;
d
Department of Clinical Psychology, Friedrich-Schiller University Jena, Jena, Germany
ABSTRACT
Background: Neurophysiological models link dissociation (e.g. feeling detached during or
after a traumatic event) to hypoarousal. It is currently assumed that the initial passive
reaction to a threat may coincide with a blunted autonomic response, which constitutes the
dissociative subtype of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).
Objective: Within this systematic review we summarize research which evaluates autonomic
nervous system activation (e.g. heart rate, blood pressure) and dissociation in PTSD patients
to discern the validity of current neurophysiological models of trauma-related hypoarousal.
Method: Of 553 screened articles, 28 studies (N= 1300 subjects) investigating the physiological
response to stress provocation or trauma-related interventions were included in the nal
analysis.
Results: No clear trend exists across all measured physiological markers in trauma-related
dissociation. Extracted results are inconsistent, in part due to high heterogeneity in
experimental methodology.
Conclusion: The current review is unable to provide robust evidence that peri- and post-
traumatic dissociation are associated with hypoarousal, questioning the validity of distinct
psychophysiological proles in PTSD.
La disociación relacionada con el trauma y el sistema nervioso
autónomo: Una revisión bibliográca sistemática de los correlatos
psicosiológicos de la experiencia disociativa en pacientes con TEPT
Antecedentes: Los modelos neurosiológicos vinculan la disociación (por ejemplo, la
sensación de desapego durante o después de un evento traumático) con la hipoactivación.
Actualmente se asume que la reacción pasiva inicial ante una amenaza puede coincidir con
una respuesta autonómica embotada, lo que constituye el subtipo disociativo del trastorno
de estrés postraumático (TEPT).
Objetivo: En esta revisión sistemática resumimos las investigaciones que evalúan la activación
del sistema nervioso autónomo (por ejemplo, la frecuencia cardíaca, la presión arterial) y la
disociación en pacientes con TEPT para discernir la validez de los modelos neurosiológicos
actuales de la hipoactivación relacionada con el trauma.
Método: De 553 artículos seleccionados, se incluyeron en el análisis nal 28 estudios (N=1300
sujetos) que investigaban la respuesta siológica a la provocación del estrés o a las
intervenciones relacionadas con el trauma.
Resultados: No existe una tendencia clara en todos los marcadores siológicos medidos en la
disociación relacionada con el trauma. Los resultados extraídos son inconsistentes, en parte
debido a la alta heterogeneidad en la metodología experimental.
Conclusión: La presente revisión no puede aportar pruebas sólidas de que la disociación peri y
postraumática esté asociada a la hipoactivación, lo que cuestiona la validez de los distintos
perles psicosiológicos en el TEPT
PTSD
事件
(PTSD)
ARTICLE HISTORY
Received 3 June 2022
Revised 29 August 2022
Accepted 13 September
2022
KEYWORDS
Biomarkers; autonomic
nervous system; dissociative
experiencing; threat
responding; defense
cascade; freeze; immobility;
heart rate; blood pressure;
skin conductance response
PALABRAS CLAVE
Biomarcadores; Sistema
Nervioso Autónomo;
Experiencia disociativa;
Respuesta a la amenaza;
Cascada de defensa;
Congelación; Inmovilidad;
Frecuencia cardíaca; Presión
arterial; Respuesta de
conductancia de la piel
;
;;;
;;
;;;;
;;
HIGHLIGHTS
There is no consensus on
physiological biomarkers
of trauma-related
dissociation.
Peri- and post-traumatic
dissociation are
physiologically distinct
from stress reactions in
chronic states.
Standardized
methodologies may
increase the
reproducibility and
specicity of
psychophysiological
biomarkers of dissociation.
© 2022 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which
permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
CONTACT Judith K. Daniels j.k.daniels@rug.nl Department of Clinical Psychology and Experimental Psychopathology; University of Groningen,
Grote Kruisstraat 2, 9712 TS Groningen, Netherlands
*These authors contributed equally to this work.
Supplemental data for this article can be accessed online at https://doi.org/10.1080/20008066.2022.2132599.
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTRAUMATOLOGY
2022, VOL. 13, 2132599
https://doi.org/10.1080/20008066.2022.2132599
目的统综PTSD
前创
553 篇筛28 N= 1300
明显不一
了不
PTSD
1. Introduction
Does anyone know of research documenting large
heart-rate decrease during episodes of psychological
dissociation?. This question, posted in ResearchGate
(2016), incited an ongoing debate in the psychotrau-
matology community by calling into question the
accepted notion that dissociation is related to psycho-
physiological hypoarousal (Iand et al., 2020; Pole,
2007). This article thus seeks to provide a systematic
and comprehensive overview on autonomic markers
of trauma-related dissociation. In the following, we
will (1) briey summarize key theoretical models
related to the psychophysiology of peri- and post-trau-
matic dissociation, and (2) systematically review exist-
ing ndings on the relationship between dissociation
and the autonomic nervous system (ANS) in PTSD.
1.1. Psychophysiology of post-traumatic stress
disorder and dissociation
PTSD is diagnosed when a person experiences auto-
nomic hyperarousal and symptoms of vivid re-experi-
encing, avoidance, and negative changes in mood and
thought within one month of the traumatic incident.
Hyperarousal refers to an active stress response to a per-
ceived threat characterized by hypervigilance, exagger-
ated startle response, irritability, disturbed sleeping
patterns, and the inability to concentrate (American
Psychiatric Association, 2013). Early studies (Orr,
1997;Orretal.,2002; Orr & Roth, 2000) demonstrated
that most PTSD patients experience increased heart rate
under stressful conditions. Yet, a subset of traumatized
individuals (approximately one third) do not show such
hyperreactivity to threat cues. Other studies found auto-
nomic responses in PTSD patients to be either physio-
logically similar to controls or reduced from baseline
(Cuthbert et al., 2003;DAndrea et al., 2013;Limberg
et al., 2011; McTeague & Lang, 2012). This divergence
from the standard hyperarousal phenotype of PTSD
towards a more blunted psychophysiological response
was later termed hyporeactivity or hypoarousal (DAn-
drea et al., 2013; Frewen & Lanius, 2006b; Terpou et al.,
2019). Further research by Lanius and colleagues
(Lanius et al., 2002; Lanius et al., 2005; Lanius et al.,
2006) employing trauma memory recall supports the
idea of two dierent PTSD proles and paved the way
for the inclusion of a new diagnosis by the American
Psychiatric Association (2013) (might need to update
citation here). PTSD patients with decreased aective
and physiological responses, concomitant with altered
brain activity in fronto-limbic circuits were labeled
physiological non-respondersand would later fall
into the dissociative subtype of PTSD (American Psy-
chiatric Association, 2013; Lanius et al., 2010; Lanius
et al., 2012; Spiegel et al., 2011; Terpou et al., 2019).
1.2. Peri- and post-traumatic dissociation
Previous metanalyses demonstrate a robust link
between dissociative experiencing and exposure to
trauma (Raq et al., 2018; Vonderlin et al., 2018).
Trauma-related dissociation may be an innate defense
mechanism to cope with and detach from trauma or
trauma-related cues (Dalenberg & Carlson, 2012;
Lanius et al., 2012). Dissociation is characterized by a
sense of psychological detachment (Holmes et al.,
2005) which can manifest as depersonalization (i.e. feel-
ing disconnected from the surroundings), derealization
(i.e. feeling disconnected from the surroundings),
emotional numbing (Frewen & Lanius, 2006a), analge-
sia (Ludascher et al., 2010; Mickleborough et al., 2011;
Strigo et al., 2010), and immobility (Volchan et al.,
2011; Volchan et al., 2017). One can distinguish
between peritraumatic dissociation and post-traumatic
dissociation by the timing of dissociation relative to
the traumatic event (e.g. during or after). Post-trau-
matic dissociation includes dissociative reactions
towards a real or experimental threat within a short
time scale (e.g. within the hour) (Carlson et al., 2018).
If these acute reactions occur repeatedly over time,
they may blend into a chronic state where dissociation
is experienced daily (i.e. trait dissociation). Between 6
and 45% of dissociative PTSD patients (Hansen et al.,
2017) exhibit more severe psychological symptoms
(more re-experiencing, higher suicidality, and impaired
role functioning) than non-dissociative PTSD patients
(e.g. Stein et al., 2013). It remains unclear which psy-
chobiological mechanisms underlie or illicit peri- and
post-traumatic dissociation. We will subsequently high-
light several theories which implicate acute defensive
reactions and altered ANS activity as the drivers of dis-
sociation in PTSD.
2S. BEUTLER ET AL.
1.3. The autonomic nervous system
The ANS, consisting of the sympathetic nervous sys-
tem (SNS) and parasympathetic nervous system
(PNS), regulates heart rate, respiratory rate, blood
pressure, and galvanic skin response. Broadly speak-
ing, an increase in SNS activity corresponds with phys-
iological arousal, as measured by elevated heart rate,
respiratory rate, blood pressure and skin conductance
(Bandler et al., 2000). Meanwhile, a decrease in SNS
activity and/or an increase in PNS activity corre-
sponds with the relative slowing of heart rate and low-
ering of blood pressure and skin conductance. In
addition, reduced SNS activity is associated with
increased heart rate variability parameters like the
high-frequency band or the root mean square succes-
sive, which can represent autonomic hypoarousal (see
Table 1). It should be noted, though, that sympathetic
and parasympathetic activity are not reciprocal, but
interact in a loosely coupled fashion (e.g. Billman,
2013; de Geus et al., 2019). Thus, blunted physiological
responses may reect parasympathetic hyperactivity,
sympathetic hypoactivity or both (Bandler et al.,
2000), warranting a detailed assessment of quantiable
biomarkers which underlie altered autonomic
responses in dissociation (see Table 1). In the follow-
ing, we will summarize key theoretical models that
have shaped the current scientic and clinical dialogue
on the psychophysiology of threat processing and
trauma-related dissociation.
1.4. Psychophysiological models
1.4.1. Defense cascade model
Incorporating animal research (for an overview see
https://fanselowlab.psych.ucla.edu/predatory-immine
nce-continuum/) and Mobbsthreat processing model
(Mobbs et al., 2009), the defense cascade model by
Lanius and colleagues (2018) posits that humans
under severe threat initially respond with an active
defense response (ght-and-ight, characterized by
sympathetic dominance and endocannabinoid release)
and then exhibit SNS/PNS co-activation (tonic immo-
bility). With a limited chance of escape, humans exhi-
bit a passive defensive response, characterized by
depersonalization/derealization symptoms (unre-
sponsive immobility) and opioid release. Simul-
taneously the heart rate, blood pressure and skeletal
muscle tone go down (tonic immobility) (McKinnon
et al., 2016), which may be mediated by increased
top-down modulation of limbic brain regions includ-
ing the amygdala and periaqueductal gray (Lanius
et al., 2018; Nicholson et al., 2017).
Table 1. Abbreviations, full names, and short denitions of included physiological parameters.
Abbreviation Full name Denition
Cardiovascular parameters
BP Blood pressure Force of circulating blood on the walls of the arteries reported as a ratio: systolic/diastolic;
hypertension is related to increased sympathetic and decreased parasympathetic tone
SBP Systolic blood pressure Pressure when heart beats (BP is at its highest)
DBP Diastolic blood pressure Pressure when heart is at rest (BP at its lowest)
CRD Cardiovascular response discordance Measure for discrepancy; Low BP while HR is high, calculation = subtracting each
standardized BP score from the corresponding standardized HR score
FPV Finger pulse volume Via infrared plethysmography measured pulse wave obtaining information about HR and
peripheral volume blood ow; changes in FPV serve as indicator of sympathetic arousal
HR Heart rate How many times the heart beats in a given time period (typically one minute); sympathetic
and parasympathetic innervation
HRV Heart rate variability Variation in the time interval between consecutive heartbeats; reects the regulation of
autonomic balance, blood pressure, gut, heart, vascular tone
HF-HRV High frequency heart rate variability Power of the high-frequency band (0.150.4 Hz); frequency domain; higher units are
associated with parasympathetic drive; magnitude of RSA
LF-HRV Low frequency heart rate variability Power of the low-frequency band (0.040.15 Hz); frequency domain; sympathetic and
parasympathetic involvement; but increased when dominant sympathetic drive
LF/HF-ratio Ratio of low frequency to high frequency
heart rate variability
Ratio of LF-to-HF power; frequency domain; value of interaction of parasympathetic (HF) and
sympathetic (LF) drive; increased ratio = increased sympathetic activity; decreased ratio =
increased parasympathetic and decreased sympathetic activity
RMSSD Root mean square successive dierence Root mean square of successive interbeat intervals; time domain; is associated with the
parasympathetic drive
RSA Respiratory sinus arrhythmia Heat rate variability synchronized with breathing, increases with inhalation, and decreases
with exhalation; proposed indirect measure of vagal tone
IBI Interbeat interval Time between two heart beats; Conversion between HR and IBI: 60,000 / beats per minute =
one beat in milliseconds; same drive as HR
PEP Pre-ejection period Length of time between the contraction of the heart (onset of systole) and the ejection of
blood out of the heart and into the aorta (onset of left ventricular ejection); shorter PEP is
associated with an increase in sympathetic drive
Electrodermal parameters
SC Skin conductance Measurement of the skins ability to conduct electricity; based on activity of eccrine sweat
glands controlled by the sudomotor nerves; Synonym = electrodermal activity; sympathetic
drive
NSCF Number of skin conductance uctuations Number of spontaneous uctuations in skin conductance
SCL Skin conductance level Measure of the basal level of sweat gland activity; tonic, continuous, and slowly changing
SCR Skin conductance response Measure of changes in sweat gland activity as discrete responses to stimuli; highly correlated
with sympathetic activity; indicator of emotional arousal/distress
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTRAUMATOLOGY 3
The proposed psychophysiological defense stages
correspond to the severity of dissociation. Following
a threat, the type of defensive response in PTSD
(hyper- vs. hypoarousal) depends on the severity of
dissociative symptomatology (Terpou et al., 2019). In
sum, this model conceptualizes dissociation in terms
of depersonalization, derealization and emotional/
physical numbing and suggests that the initial peri-
traumatic reaction (i.e. shock) can become a con-
ditioned response, triggered by trauma cues or
stressors (Lanius et al., 2018) in accordance with the
inescapable shock model (van der Kolk, 1988). Follow-
ing this rationale, the initial psychophysiological
response to a trauma (peritraumatic) and to a trauma
cue/stressor (acute post-traumatic dissociation) may
over time result in a chronic dissociative state. To
date, few neurophysiological studies have assessed
peritraumatic, acute, and trait dissociation within
one cohort (e.g. Bichescu-Burian et al., 2017;
Hauschildt et al., 2011), and longitudinal research is
warranted to better understand the hypothesized etio-
logical continuity of passive defensive responding in
humans.
1.4.2. Shutdown model
The shutdown model by Schauer and Elbert (2010) clo-
sely resembles the defense cascade model, but classies
six distinct stages of threat response. After an initial
orienting response (1. freeze), the body under threat
prepares to escape (2. ight), then attack (3. ght). If
both options fail to eliminate the threat, the body sub-
verts to a state of unresponsive immobility (4. tonic
immobility), and shut-down (5. ag) which may pro-
gress to fainting mediated by disgust (6. faint) as the
body surrenders to the threat (i.e. the attacker). Simi-
lar to the defense cascade model (Lanius et al., 2010;
Lanius et al., 2012), the shutdown model describes
two distinct response proles: the Type 1 uproar-
PTSD, characterized by dominant sympathetic acti-
vation, and Type 2 shutdown-PTSD, characterized
by dominant parasympathetic activation and includ-
ing patients who follow the cascade at least until
stage 4. The authors propose that the initial threat
response informs subsequent physiological reactivity,
where the hypersensitized trauma-related fear net-
work is easily reactivated, eliciting the initial psycho-
physiological defense response. Thus, an individual
who exhibits a parasympathetic-dominant reaction
(e.g. ag or faint) to the trauma is more likely to
experience an acute post-traumatic dissociative
response, accompanied by inhibition of the SNS, acti-
vation of the PNS, and reduction in heart rate and
blood pressure. For instance, a study of PTSD patients
demonstrated that experiencing acute dissociation
during trauma recall (via personalized trauma audio-
scripts) is associated with reduced heart rate (Sack
et al., 2012). It is unknown whether these individuals
experienced a similar initial peritraumatic response.
The model does not explicitly state that the (repeated)
PNS-dominant physiological reactions to trauma cues
share similar features to chronic post-traumatic dis-
sociation. However, the characterization of the shut-
down model of PTSD suggests that the stable
dissociative response pattern may be a conditioned
automatic response to stressors and perceived threat
(Schauer & Elbert, 2010).
1.4.3. Freeze and tonic immobility
Another model of threat-responding is one in which
two alternative pathways lead to tonic immobility
(Hagenaars et al., 2014; Roelofs, 2017). In this mode,
the initial orienting response to a threat is followed
by either the ght/ight response or the freeze
response. The latter is characterized by immobility,
bradycardia and increased muscle tonus accompanied
by concomitant SNS-PNS activation. The authors
describe that rapid shifting between the two defensive
behaviors of ght/ight and freeze is possible. If both
options fail, the body enters a state of tonic immobility
(death feigningor playing dead), characterized by
motor inhibition, muscle rigidity and the inability to
express oneself vocally (Kuiling et al., 2019). Tonic
immobility (i.e. hypotension or unresponsiveness) is
conceptualized as a more passive defensive response
as opposed to freezing. In this model freezing is associ-
ated with, and is thus measurable via, reduced body
sway (see also Volchan et al., 2017;2011). Roelofs
(2017) emphasizes the lack of evidence on the physi-
ology of tonic immobility in humans but suggests
that freezing may be linked to a maladaptive
emotional response which precedes the development
of PTSD, necessitating further longitudinal research
in this eld. Importantly, this model does not associate
defensive behavior with dissociation, but portrays dis-
sociation and tonic immobility as related but dierent
constructs (Abrams et al., 2009; Hagenaars, 2016;
Lima et al., 2010).
1.4.4. Polyvagal theory
The polyvagal theory, developed by Stephen Porges
(2003,2007,2011), has received substantial attention
in the psychotraumatology eld. Porges emphasizes
the evolutionary relevance of the vagal nerve in facil-
itating arousal states, mobilization and immobiliz-
ation, and social engagement. According to Porges,
the vagus nerve can be dierentiated into three dier-
ent branches with evolutionarily selective advantages
in regulating autonomic functioning (2003). First,
the unmyelinated vagus branch (e.g. the dorsal vagal
complex) is associated with immobilization (e.g.
fainting, shutdown, dissociation), bradycardia and
decreased muscle tone. The second branch, the sym-
pathetic-adrenal branch, is active during mobiliz-
ation behaviors (e.g. ght/ight) when under attack.
4S. BEUTLER ET AL.
The nal branch, themyelinatedbranch(e.g.theven-
tral vagal complex), regulates cardiac vagal activity
and withdrawal via inhibition of the sympathetic
branch, which corresponds with decreased arousal,
social engagement and (self-)soothing behaviors
(Porges, 2007). Through this framework, dissociative
responding in humans is purely a defensive immobil-
ization response modulated by the dorsal vagal com-
plex and associated with large heart rate decreases.
This immobilization response occurs when behaviors
initiated by the ventral vagal complex (e.g. social
engagement) or the sympathetic-adrenal pathway
(e.g. active defensive behavior) are not possible.
Indirect support for this model is seen in studies
which have proven that trait dissociation is nega-
tively correlated with heart rate during experimen-
tally-induced social stress (Powers et al., 2021;
Simeon et al., 2008).
1.4.5. Window of tolerance
The window of tolerance model (Corrigan et al.,
2011; Ogden et al., 2006;Siegel,1999) states that
trauma, particularly prolonged childhood trauma,
can result in high autonomic sensitivity towards
stressors and an imbalance in the dopaminergic sys-
tem associated with rapid, uncontrolled shifting
between two distress dipoles (also termed biphasic
rollercoaster, Corrigan et al., 2011, p. 20). When
exposed to trauma cues, patients switch between
an SNS-dominated high-arousal state of active
defensive responding (e.g. fear, anger, impulsivity,
vigilance) and a PNS-dominated low-arousal pas-
sive defensivestate of emotional coping (e.g. sub-
mission, depression, and numbing). To manage
these extremes, the window of tolerance represents
a zone of psychological and autonomic exibility: a
balanced psychophysiological state allowing for
exible shifting, regulation, and integration of
emotions, so they become tolerable.
In regards to dissociation, this model hypothesizes
a noradrenergic lateral tegmental limbic forebrain
midbrain circuit (Bergmann, 2008), which activates
the PNS and downregulates aect (both positive and
negative), leading to emotional numbing and cogni-
tive dissociation. Indirect support for this hypothesis
was found in a study by Krause-Utz and colleagues
(2018). This study found a strong positive association
between acute dissociation and PNS-mediated heart
rate variability when patients (with comorbid PTSD
and borderline personality disorder) were instructed
to downregulate emotion, but passively viewing aec-
tive pictures. Although not explicitly stated, the
authors draw a clear link between the initial physio-
logical eects of trauma and subsequent SNS and
PNS-mediated responses, with the latter including
numbing, collapse, and dissociation.
1.5. Summary and study aim
Taken together, current theories of ANS-mediated
defensive behaviors share a common framework,
mainly in that dissociation is (a) a defensive reaction
to traumatic stressors or reminders, (b) related to
parasympathetic dominance, and (c) associated with
a muted psychophysiological response (i.e. hypoarou-
sal; Pole, 2007). However, these theories dier in their
use of terminology, such as the meaning of freezing
and its corresponding physiological correlates. In the
shutdown model, freezing is part of the orienting
response, but Hagenaars and colleagues (2014) dis-
tinguish these two physiological states as unique
experiences. Additionally, this model does not men-
tion other defensive stages following tonic immobility,
as suggested by the shutdown model (e.g. ag) and
defense cascade model (e.g. collapsed freeze). On the
other hand, congruity across models can be found in
their neurophysiological correlates of threat response.
Importantly, some of these theories either do not
address dissociation at all or fail to specify how they
conceptualize dissociation. It remains unclear whether
acute dissociation as a defensive response shares simi-
lar psychophysiological characteristics with peritrau-
matic (i.e. during a trauma) or post-traumatic
dissociation (e.g. a stable behavioral pattern following
the trauma). Another general criticism of the afore-
mentioned models, particularly the polyvagal theory
(see Grossman & Taylor, 2007), concerns the oversim-
plication of biological processes and the assumption
of similarity between human and animal psychophy-
siology, due to its basis on partially outdated theoreti-
cal conceptualizations of human evolutionary biology
(see Barrett, 2020, for critical reection; for instance,
the triune brain theory by MacLean, 1990)
The current study aims to provide more clarity on
the question of whether dissociation in PTSD patients
is related to autonomic hypoarousal. To this end, a
systematic literature search was conducted on research
in human subjects evaluating both psychophysiologi-
cal markers, (e.g. heart rate, blood pressure, and skin
conductance) and dissociative experiences in PTSD
patients.
2. Methods
2.1. Search strategy
This systematic review was conducted with studies pub-
lished until 21 March 2022, following the Preferred
Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta
Analysis for Individual Patient Data (PRISMA-IPD;
Stewartetal.,2015) procedures. 801 articles were
identied through searches in Web of Science,
PubMed, PsycArticles, and PTSDpubs, using keywords
which combined dissociative symptoms according to
the dissociative subtype (dissociat*, dissociative,
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTRAUMATOLOGY 5
depersonali#ation, dereali#ation, DP/DR), PTSD (PTSD,
post-traumatic stress disorder, posttraumatic stress dis-
order, post-traumatic stress disorder) and ANS-related
physiological markers (heart rate, HR, HRV, ECG, elec-
trocardiogram, cardiac, cardiovascular, skin conduc-
tance, SC, SCR, electrodermal blood pressure, BP,
systolic,diastolic,temperature,eye,pupilsize,pupillary
dilatation, pupil* diameter, startle response, respiratory
sinus arrhythmia, respira*, breath*, ANS, autonomic
nervous system, parasympath*, sympath*, PNS, SNS,
visceral nervous system). Keywords within one concept
were combined with the ORcommand, while the con-
cepts (dissociation, PTSD, and physiological markers)
were combined with the ANDcommand. Search
terms on muscle tension (muscle tone, muscle tension,
body sway, electromyography, EMG) were also included.
Although skeletal muscles are not part of the ANS, they
are of interest as part of the physiologic stress response.
However, this search term revealed no studies worthy of
inclusion.
Of all 801 articles, 204 were duplicates and 44 were
not peer reviewed articles. Once removed, this left 553
relevant peer reviewed articles for further evaluation.
All 553 articles were screened by two independent
reviewers using the title, abstract, and methods (inter-
rater reliability: 0.83). Inclusion criteria were: (a)
study participant age equal to or greater than 18, (b)
at least 5 patients with a PTSD diagnosis or meeting cri-
teria for PTSD diagnosis included, (c) experimental
studies with trauma-related interventions (e.g. recall,
script, lm) or stress provocation (e.g. Trier Social
Stress Test, acoustic startle), (d) with measurable and
specic autonomic outcomes, and (e) designed in either
a correlative (e.g. dissociation and physiological mar-
ker) or comparative (e.g. physiological marker across
low dissociation vs. high dissociation subgroups) man-
ner. Exclusion criteria were (a) animal studies, (b) dis-
sociation not assessed, (c) assessment of treatment
outcome or baseline/resting state only, and (d) PTSD
assessment after experimental intervention (prospective
design). In the case of diverging ratings, which did
occur for 21 articles, they were assigned by mutual con-
sensus. After the initial screening process, 65 relevant
studies remained to be analyzed further. Four studies
were excluded in this step for other reasons (see
Figure 1). For 49 of the remaining 61 studies, authors
were contacted to acquire missing patient data or ana-
lyses which could support the current review. We con-
tacted authors at least twice and 65% replied. A total of
28 studies were included in the nal analysis (Table 2 &
Figure 1; for a list of non-included studies see Sup-
plemental Table S1).
2.2. Data extraction
A study coding spreadsheet was utilized to collect data
from each included study. Within the coding
spreadsheet was data on basic study information
(e.g. authors, year, title), sample characteristics (e.g.
sample size, mean age, gender, trauma history, PTSD
diagnosis or meeting criteria), experimental condition
(e.g. traumatic content, stress evoking, acoustic star-
tle), on which value scores were based (e.g. change
score trauma condition minus neutral condition,
change score trauma condition minus baseline,
specic trials), as well as indices (e.g. kind and score
of eect sizes, signicances) of the physiological par-
ameters (see Table 1 for an overview of all parameters
included). It should be noted that, although we
focused on physiological activity during an interven-
tion, we also reviewed baseline scores (see Table S2
in the supplement). The spreadsheet also included
comments to further dene specic trials, as well as
notes on our statistical procedures if eect sizes were
not directly taken from the original publication.
2.3. Publication bias
Publication bias remains an important issue in
psychological and medical research (Head et al.,
2015; Thornton & Lee, 2000). However, Pole (2007),
who conducted the most comprehensive meta-analy-
sis on psychophysiological markers and PTSD, argued
that studies measuring psychophysiological activity
may be less aected by publication bias because they
often report multiple physiological measures, increas-
ing the likelihood of non-signicant results or results
contrary to the hypothesis. Although an earlier
meta-analysis on heart rate variability and anxiety dis-
orders in general (Chalmers et al., 2014) and PTSD
(Nagpal et al., 2013) supported this assumption,
newer publications reveal publication bias in studies
on heart rate variability and PTSD (Ge et al., 2020;
Schneider & Schwerdtfeger, 2020). The inuence of
publication bias still needs to be carefully considered.
Therefore, we did not only screen titles and abstracts
for physiological and dissociative measurements, but
also the methods section of each of the 553 peer
reviewed articles to overcome possible biases in data
collection and interpretation. Frequently, dissociation
was reported as a descriptive sample characteristic, but
not further included into a studys analysis. If a disso-
ciative physiological measurement was reported in the
methods but not results of a study, we asked the
authors for additional data or calculations on this
parameter.
2.4. Data structuring and presentation
Our examination revealed great heterogeneity
between included studies on several characteristics,
including experimental condition, physiological par-
ameters, score calculation of physiological parameters,
and sample characteristics. Therefore, we have
6S. BEUTLER ET AL.
structured this systematic review as follows: articles
included are grouped according to the temporal
classication of dissociation as trait, peritraumatic,
or acute dissociation. Studies that report multiple tem-
poral classications are included in more than one cat-
egory (see Table 2). Subsections for each dissociative
classication are clustered along with physiological
parameters. Within these subsections we also dieren-
tiate between stimuli in experimental trauma stimuli
and more general stress (See Supplemental materials
4ae for an additional graphical overview).
We aim to be as precise and accurate as possible in
the presentation of its results. Therefore, we have
obtained all available eect sizes (ror Cohens d)to
elucidate the magnitude of reported dierences in dis-
sociation and physiological reactivity. If eect sizes
were not available, they were calculated by means
and standard deviation via the Psychometrica website
Figure 1. PRISMA Flowchart of literature search and study identication.
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTRAUMATOLOGY 7
(Lenhard & Lenhard, 2016). In a few cases, the calcu-
lation approach diered from this procedure. This is
indicated in Supplemental Table S3. For the study of
Kaufman et al. (2002), we were not able to match an
appropriate sample size with each physiologic variable
and have therefore conservatively based our calcu-
lation on the smallest reported sample sizes. Signi-
cance levels were taken from the original studies and
may therefore dier in their accuracy.
3. Results
3.1. Trait dissociation
3.1.1. General study characteristics
In total, 13 articles were identied that examined car-
diovascular or electrodermal reactions to trauma- or
stress-related interventions in the context of trait dis-
sociation. Sample sizes varied (n=14ton= 134) and a
combined 508 participants were studied. Mean age
varied from 23.86 years to 46.95 years. Five studies
examined female participants only, one study males
only, and the percentage of female participants in
other studies varied (8% to 95%). In eight studies, all
participants had been diagnosed with PTSD (for
detailed information see Supplemental Table S3).
The remaining articles included some patients with
PTSD, non-PTSD trauma-exposed participants and
non-exposed controls (one study) (compare Table 3
for more detailed information).
To assess trait dissociation, the majority of the
studies used the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES;
Bernstein & Putnam, 1986), the most commonly
used dissociation self-report questionnaire (Lyssenko
et al., 2018). Other questionnaires included the Trait
Dissociation Questionnaire (TDQ; Murray et al.,
2002), the Shutdown Dissociation Scale (Shut-D;
Schalinski et al., 2015), the Multidimensional Inven-
tory of Dissociation (MID; Dell, 2006), and the Multi-
scale Dissociation Inventory (MDI; Briere et al., 2005).
To provoke physiologic reactions, seven studies
used personalized trauma-related content and one
study used videos with trauma-associated content of
varying emotional valence. General stress stimuli
were used in ve studies, including mirror confronta-
tion, the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST), white noise,
and acoustic startle probes.
All but two studies examined heart rate (HR). Par-
ameters of heart rate variability (HRV) were assessed
in four studies, including root mean square successive
dierence (RMSSD), high frequency HRV (HF-HRV),
low frequency HRV (LF-HRV), the ratio of low fre-
quency to high frequency (LF/HF-ratio) and respirat-
ory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), which can be interpreted
as an HRV marker (Berntson et al., 1997). Two studies
measured systolic (SBP) and diastolic blood pressure
(DBP), and one the pre-ejection period (PEP). Five
studies measured skin conductance (SC) parameters,
which include skin conductance level (SCL), skin con-
ductance response (SCR), and numbers of skin con-
ductance uctuations (NSCF).
Six studies examined physiological reactions during
the experimental condition only. Seven studies either
additionally or solely reported a change score, which
Table 2. Included studies.
Study specication Trait Dissociation Peritraumatic Dissociation Acute Dissociation
Barnow et al. (2012) X
Bichescu-Burian, Steyer, Steinert, Grieb, & Tschoke (2017)X X X
Castro-Chapman et al. (2018)X
Chou, La Marca, Steptoe, & Brewin (2018) X
DAndrea, Pole, DePierro, Freed, & Wallace (2013) X
Duesenberg et al. (2019) X
Ebner-Priemer et al. (2009) X
Halligan, Michael, Wilhelm, Clark, & Ehlers (2006)X X
Hauschildt, Peters, Moritz, & Jelinek (2011)XXX
Hetzel-Riggin & Wilber (2010)X
Hyer, Albrecht, Boudewyns, Woods, & Brandsma (1993)X
Kaufman et al. (2002) X
Krause-Utz, Walther, Lis, Schmahl, & Bohus (2018) X
Ladwig et al. (2002) X
Lanius et al. (2002) X
Malta et al. (2021) X
Metz et al. (2020) X
Pole et al. (2006) X
Powers et al. (2021)X
Rhudy, Davis, Williams, McCabe, & Byrd (2008 X
Sack, Cillien, & Hopper (2012) X
Schäein, Sattel, Schmidt, & Sack (2018)X X
Schalinski, Elbert, & Schauer (2013)X
Schmahl et al. (2004)X X
Seligowski et al. (2019)X
Simeon, Yehuda, Knutelska, & Schmeidler (2008)X
Sledjeski & Delahanty (2012)X
Vermes et al. (2020)X X
Note: X = Study assessed this dissociation measure.
8S. BEUTLER ET AL.
Table 3. Trait dissociation Study characteristics and results.
HR HRV BP SC
Study NPatients
Mean
age
Sex
distribution
(% women)
Dissociation
measurement
Physiological
parameter Symptom provocation
Comparison
Specication HR PEP RMSSD HF LF
LF/
HF-
ratio RSA
BP
(s/d) SCL SCR NSCF
Personalized trauma stimuli
Bichescu-
Burian
et al. (2017)
21 BPD patients with
comorbid PTSD
23.86 100 DES HR, SCRa, SCRf Personalized trauma-
content
Correlation *
Castro-
Chapman
et al. (2018)
134 Veterans with combat
exposure (on
average moderate
PTSD symptom
severity)
37.64 8 DES HR Personalized trauma-
content
Correlation *
Halligan et al.
(2006)
61
a
Assault survivors (41%
met criteria for PTSD)
39.0 46 TDQ HR Personalized trauma-
content
Correlation
full sample
(n= 54)
*
Correlation
females
(n= 25)
*
Correlation
males
(n= 29)
*
Hauschildt
et al. (2011)
70 n= 26 trauma-exposed
with PTSD,
n= 26 trauma-
exposed without
PTSD,
n= 18 non-exposed
controls
32.7
b
71 DES RMSSD, HF-
HRV, LF-HRV
Video with emotional
valence (neutral,
negative, positive,
trauma-related)
Correlation ••
Hyer et al.
(1993)
57
(n=19
low diss.,
n=17
medium
diss.,
n=21
high diss.)
Veterans with chronic
PTSD
40.2 0 DES HR Personalized trauma-
content
High vs.
medium
dissociation
*
High vs. low
dissociation
*
Rhudy et al.
(2008)
28 Trauma-exposed with
chronic nightmares
(32% current PTSD,
46% lifetime PTSD)
39.0 68 DES HR, SCL Personal nightmare
scripts
Correlation **
Schmahl
et al. (2004)
14 PTSD patients with
history of sexual &
/or physical abuse
36.2 100 DES HR, BP, NSCF Personalized trauma-
content
Correlation **/**
Vermes et al.
(2020)
21 PTSD patients 34.7 95 DES SCR Personalized trauma-
content
Correlation
(Continued)
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTRAUMATOLOGY 9
Table 3. Continued.
HR HRV BP SC
Study NPatients
Mean
age
Sex
distribution
(% women)
Dissociation
measurement
Physiological
parameter Symptom provocation
Comparison
Specication HR PEP RMSSD HF LF
LF/
HF-
ratio RSA
BP
(s/d) SCL SCR NSCF
General stress stimuli
Powers et al.
(2021)
19 Trauma-exposed with
diabetes
46.95 100 MDI HR, LF/HF-
ratio, RSA
Trier Social Stress Test Correlation ••
Schäein
et al. (2018)
18 DDNOS patients with
comorbid PTSD
41.7 94 DES HR, lnRMSSD,
PEP
mirror confrontation
(only, or combined
with positive or
negative cognitions)
Correlation ••
Schalinski
et al. (2013)
31 Refugees with multiple
traumatic
experiences & PTSD
(6% did not meet the
avoidance criterion)
36.45 100 Shut-D HR White noise Correlation
Seligowski
et al. (2019)
19 General medical
hospital in-patients
with PTSD
37.68 100 MDI HR, RSA, SCR Fear-potentiated
startle paradigm
Correlation *
c
*
d
20 Psychiatric hospital in-
patients with PTSD
39.75 100 MID Correlation *
c
*
e
Simeon et al.
(2008)
21 New York City
residents highly
exposed to 9/11
(33% met criteria for
PTSD)
40.4 38 DES HR, BP Trier Social Stress Test Correlation /
Note: Arrow directions indicate increase or decrease during provocation condition, parentheses indicate non-signicant results, * = results indicated by change score, =noeect. If studies reported results during condition and change
scores, both are presented in the table; BP (s/d): (systolic or diastolic) blood pressure, BPD: Borderline Personality Disorder, DDNOS: Dissociative Disorder Not Otherwise Specied, DES: Dissociative Experience Scale, HF-HRV: high frequency
heart rate variability, HR: heart rate, HRV: heart rate variability, LF-HRV: low frequency heart rate variability, LF/HF-ratio: ratio of low frequency to high frequency heart rate variability, lnRMSSD: natural logarithm of root mean square
successive dierence; MDI: Multiscale Dissociation Inventory, MID: Multidimensional Inventory of Dissociation, NSCF: number of skin conductance uctuations, PEP: pre-ejection period, PTSD: Posttraumatic-Stress Disorder, RMSSD: root
mean square successive dierence, RSA: respiratory sinus arrhythmia, SC: skin conductance, SCL: skin conductance level, SCR: skin conductance response, SCRa: amplitude of skin conductance response, SCRf: frequency of skin conductance
response, Shut-D: Shutdown Dissociation Scale, TDQ: Trait Dissociation Questionnaire.
a
Correlation with trait dissociation is available for 54 participants.
b
N-adjusted mean value weighted by size of subgroups.
c
Signicant positive correlations were only found in trait dissociation subscales of depersonalization in the general hospital sample and derealization in the psychiatric sample.
d
For danger signal.
e
For safety signal.
10 S. BEUTLER ET AL.
was corrected by physiological activity during a base-
line or neutral condition. In these studies, the baseline
or neutral physiological response was subtracted from
the experiment response, reducing the amount of
interference from the baseline or default activity.
3.1.2. Eects on heart rate
With respect to heart rate, no trend across studies was
observed. Of the 11 studies investigating the relation-
ship between trait dissociation severity and HR, only
three reported signicant results. Using personalized
trauma stimuli, only Hyer et al. (1993) found a higher
HR in a high dissociative group than in a medium dis-
sociative group under a trauma condition (p< .050, d
= 0.34), which was no longer present when the calcu-
lation was based on the change score (p> .050, d=
0.10). Neither under the trauma condition (p> .050,
d= 0.27) nor based on the change score (p> .050, d
= 0.37) were there any dierences between the high
vs. low dissociation group. Under baseline conditions,
HR in the high dissociation group was increased as
compared to the medium (p< .010, d= 0.44) and
low dissociation groups (p< .050, d= 0.26; see Sup-
plemental Table S2). None of the other studies using
personalized trauma reminders (Bichescu-Burian
et al., 2017; Castro-Chapman et al., 2018; Halligan
et al., 2006; Schmahl et al., 2004) or nightmare scripts
(Rhudy et al., 2008) found a relationship between HR
and trait dissociation.
Using general stress stimuli, Simeon et al. (2008)
found that trait dissociation correlated negatively
and with moderate eect to HR (r=.48, p< .050)
measured during the TSST but not at baseline (r=
.08, p> .050; for baseline see Supplemental Table
S2). This negative correlation (r=.65, p= .009)
between trait dissociation and HR during the TSST
was conrmed by Powers et al. (2021), who observed
this relationship even at baseline (r=.52, p= .047).
The remaining studies reported no signicant change
in HR to mirror confrontation (Schäein et al., 2018),
white noise (Schalinski et al., 2013), or fear-poten-
tiated startle (Seligowski et al., 2019).
3.1.3. Eects on heart rate variability
Seligowski et al. (2019) demonstrated a positive corre-
lation between RSA and depersonalization/derealiza-
tion subscales (with MDI depersonalization subscale
in the general hospital sample: r= .35, p< .010; with
MID depersonalization/derealization mean in the psy-
chiatric sample: r= .33, p< .010) during fear-poten-
tiated startle, but not with overall trait dissociation
(in the general hospital sample: r= .16, p> .050; in
the psychiatric sample: r= .10, p> .050).
None of the other studies found correlations
between HRV and trait dissociation after exposure
to trauma related material (Hauschildt et al., 2011)
or general stress stimuli (Powers et al., 2021; Schäein
et al., 2018).
3.1.4. Eects on blood pressure
Only one study examined blood pressure during
trauma exposure (Schmahl et al., 2004), demonstrat-
ing a positive correlation between trait dissociation
and SBP (change score: r= .65, p= .032), but not
DBP (r= .48, p= .137).
One study examining blood pressure under a more
general stress stimulus (Simeon et al., 2008) showed
neither a correlation for SBP (r=.24, p> .050) nor
for DBP (r=.37, p> .050) during TSST. However,
this study demonstrated a correlation between trait
dissociation and SBP at baseline (r=.54, p< .010),
but not with DBP (r=.24, p> .050; for baseline see
Supplemental Table S2).
3.1.5. Eects on skin conductance
No study utilizing personalized trauma stimuli found
a relationship between SC and trait dissociation
(Bichescu-Burian et al., 2017; Rhudy et al., 2008;
Schmahl et al., 2004; Vermes et al., 2020).
Under non-personalized stress stimuli, Seligowski
et al. (2019) showed a moderate correlation between
SCR and trait dissociation during a fear-potentiated
startle paradigm. These results varied according to
the sample population and presented stimuli. In the
general hospital sample, trait dissociation correlated
negatively with the SCR response to the danger signal
(r=.39, p< .010), but not the SCR response to the
safety signal (r=.28, p> .050). On the contrary, in
the psychiatric hospital sample, trait dissociation cor-
related positively with the SCR to the safety signal (r
= .38, p< .010), but not the SCR to the danger signal
(r= .20, p> .050).
3.2. Peritraumatic dissociation
3.2.1. General study characteristics
In total, 8 articles were identied that examined cardi-
ovascular or electrodermal reactions to trauma- or
stress-related interventions in the context of peritrau-
matic dissociation. Sample sizes varied (n=19 to n=
200) and 581 participants were studied. Mean age var-
ied from 18.74 years to 61.0 years. Three studies exam-
ined female participants only, one study males only,
and the percentage of female participants in the
other studies varied (12% to 69%). In two studies, all
participants had been diagnosed with PTSD (for
detailed information see Supplemental Table S3).
The remaining articles included some patients with
PTSD and non-PTSD trauma-exposed participants
(compare Table 4 for more detailed information).
To assess peritraumatic dissociation, almost all
included studies used the Peritraumatic Dissociative
Experience Questionnaire (PDEQ; Marmar et al.,
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTRAUMATOLOGY 11
Table 4. Peritraumatic dissociation Study characteristics and results.
HR HRV BP SC
Study NPatients
Mean
age
Sex distribution
(% women)
Dissociation
measurement
Physiological
parameter Symptom provocation
Comparison
Specication HR PEP RMSSD HF LF RSA
BP
(s/d) CRD SCL SCR
Personalized trauma stimuli
Bichescu-Burian
et al. (2017)
21 BPD patients with comorbid
PTSD
23.86 100 PDEQ-SRV HR, SCRa, SCRf Personalized trauma-content Correlation *
Halligan et al.,
(2006)
61
a
Assault survivors (41% met
criteria for PTSD)
39.0 46 SDQ HR Personalized trauma-content Correlation full
sample (n= 60)
*
Correlation females
(n= 33)
*
Correlation males
(n= 27)
*
Hauschildt et al.
(2011)
52 n= 26 trauma-exposed with
PTSD,
n= 26 trauma-exposed
without PTSD
33.1
b
69 PDEQ-SRV RMSSD, HF-HRV,
LF-HRV
Video with emotional valence
(neutral, negative, positive,
trauma-related)
Correlation ••
Hetzel-Riggin &
Wilber (2010)
86 Sexual assault victims (22%
met criteria for PTSD)
18.8 100 PDEQ-SRV HR, SCR, SCL Personalized trauma-content Correlation (full
sample)
***
Correlation (n=19
PTSD subsample)
*
*
Kaufman et al.
(2002)
187200
(n=5156
low diss.,
n= 136
144 high
diss.)
Vietnam veterans with PTSD 42.8
b
0 PDEQ-RA
4
HR, SCL, BP Standardized combat scenes &
personally relevant combat
situations
High vs. low
dissociation
••
Pole et al. (2006)19
(n= 10 with
low diss.,
n= 9 with
high diss.)
Trauma-exposed (53% met
criteria for PTSD)
44.3
b
47 PDEQ-RA
10
HR, BP, CRD Personalized trauma-content High vs. low
dissociation
c
/
Sledjeski &
Delahanty
(2012)
39 Trauma-exposed
undergraduates (51% met
criteria for PTSD)
18.74
b
100 PDEQ-SRV HR, RSA, PEP Personalized trauma-content Correlation ***
General stimuli
Ladwig et al.
(2002)
103
(n= 71 no/
low diss.,
n= 32 high
diss.,
n= 19 high
diss. - PTSD,
n= 11 high
diss. + PTSD)
Survivors of life-threatening
cardiac event
61.0 12 PDEQ-RA
8
SCR Acoustic startle High vs. no/low
dissociation
High dissociation
without vs. with
PTSD
Note: Arrow directions indicate increase or decrease during provocation condition, * = results indicated by change score, =noeect. If studies reported results during condition and change scores, both are presented in the table; BP (s/d): (systolic or
diastolic) blood pressure, BPD: Borderline Personality Disorder, CRD: cardiovascular response discordance, HF-HRV: high frequency heart rate variability, HR: heart rate, HRV: heart rate variability, LF-HRV: low frequency heart rate variability, PDEQ-SRV:
Peritraumatic Dissociative Experience Questionnaire as self-report version, PDEQ-RA
4, 8, 10
: Peritraumatic Dissociative Experience Questionnaire as rater-administered version with 4, 8, or 10 items length, PEP: pre-ejection period, PTSD: Posttraumatic-
Stress Disorder, RMSSD: root mean square successive dierence, RSA: respiratory sinus arrhythmia, SC: skin conductance, SCL: skin conductance level, SCR: skin conductance response, SCRa: amplitude of skin conductance response, SCRf: frequency of skin
conductance response, SDQ: Sate Dissociation Questionnaire
a
Correlation with state dissociation during the traumatic event available for 60 participants.
b
N-adjusted mean value weighted by size of subgroups.
c
Only in thinking about trauma condition.
12 S. BEUTLER ET AL.
1994; Marmar et al., 1997). Several modications to
this instrument were made: four studies used the 10-
item self-report version, and three studies used a
rater-administrated version with varied item length.
One study used the State Dissociation Questionnaire
(SDQ; Murray et al., 2002) to assess peritraumatic
experiences like derealization, depersonalization,
detachment, altered time sense, and emotional numb-
ing during the traumatic experience (Halligan et al.,
2003).
To provoke physiological reactions, ve studies
used personalized intrusive content. One study of
combat veterans used standardized audiovisual com-
bat scenes as well as scripts describing combat situ-
ations. One study used videos with trauma-
associated content of varying emotional valence.
Only one study used more general stressors in the
form of acoustic startle probes.
Six articles examined HR. HRV was assessed in two
studies as RMSSD, HF-HRV, LF-HRV and RSA. Two
studies measured SBP and DBP, of which one also
introduced the cardiovascular response discordance
(CRD), a subtraction of standardized BP scores from
standardized HR scores. One study measured PEP
and four examined SC via SCL and SCR.
Four studies examined physiological reaction
during experimental conditions, while four reported
baseline or neutral condition-corrected change scores
(either alone or in combination).
3.2.2. Eects on heart rate
The relationships between peritraumatic dissociation
and HR during trauma exposure were mixed, although
there was a tendency towards a positive correlation.
Sledjeski and Delahanty (2012) report a positive corre-
lation between peritraumatic dissociation and HR (r
= .34, p= .034), and a negative correlation between
peritraumatic dissociation and the PEP score (r=
.42, p= .008), which corresponds to a faster HR.
Additionally, Hetzel-Riggin and Wilber (2010)
demonstrate a large positive correlation between peri-
traumatic dissociation and HR in a PTSD subsample
during the trauma condition (r= .52, p= .023). How-
ever, with a change score this correlation is non-sig-
nicant (r= .11, p= .661). On the other hand,
Bichescu-Burian et al. (2017) used a change score to
demonstrate a large negative correlation between peri-
traumatic dissociation and HR during an intervention
(r=.54, p= .010). No eects of experimentally-
induced peritraumatic dissociation on HR were
observed by Halligan et al. (2006), Pole et al. (2006),
and Kaufman et al. (2002).
3.2.3. Eects on heart rate variability
There was no correlation with peritraumatic dis-
sociation and the assessed HRV parameters in the
study by Hauschildt et al. (2011) or with RSA in the
study by Sledjeski and Delahanty (2012).
3.2.4. Eects on blood pressure
At baseline, Pole et al. (2006) found lower SBP (p
< .050, d=1.09), and DBP (p< .050, d=1.15) for
the high dissociation group compared to the low dis-
sociation group (for baseline see Supplemental Table
S2). This eect persisted while participants thought
about their trauma for SBP (p< .050, d=1.19), but
not for DBP in the same condition (p> .050, d=
0.68). Neither SBP (p> .050, d=0.75) nor DBP
(p> .050, d=0.39) diered while talking about the
trauma condition. The authors of this study addition-
ally calculated the CRD. In this regard, they found that
in the high dissociation group HR was elevated rela-
tive to their associated trauma SBP, which was not
observed in the low dissociation group during both
the trauma and baseline conditions (for baseline see
Supplemental Table S2). Kaufman et al. (2002)
found no eects of peritraumatic dissociation on BP.
3.2.5. Eects on skin conductance
No study found associations between SCL and peri-
traumatic dissociation when participants were
exposed to personalized trauma stimuli (Hetzel-Rig-
gin & Wilber, 2010; Kaufman et al., 2002). However,
there is some evidence of elevated SCR in relation to
peritraumatic dissociation. Hetzel-Riggin and Wilber
(2010) found a positive correlation between SCR and
peritraumatic dissociation in a sample of female
assault victims (r= .24, p< .050) but not in the subset
who had a diagnosis of PTSD (r= .19, p= .434). Con-
trarily, during an intervention Bichescu-Burian et al.
(2017) found no eects of peritraumatic dissociation
on SCR amplitude or frequency.
Using general stress stimuli, Ladwig et al. (2002)
found increased SCR in participants with high levels
of dissociation as compared to participants with no/
low dissociation during an acoustic startle probe (p
= .017, d= 0.42). Furthermore, when dissociation
was high, the presence or absence of PTSD did not
aect SCR (p= .878, d= 0.17).
3.3. Acute dissociation
3.3.1. General study characteristics
In total, 15 articles examined cardiovascular or elec-
trodermal reactions to trauma- or stress-related inter-
ventions in the context of acute state dissociation.
Sample sizes varied (n=14 to n= 61). A combined
482 participants were studied. Mean age varied from
18.8 years to 42.4 years. One study examined predomi-
nantly male participants, seven studies examined
female participants only, and the percentage of female
participants in the other studies was high (65% to
95%).
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTRAUMATOLOGY 13
In ve studies all participants were diagnosed with
PTSD. Another ve studies included trauma-exposed
participants of which some met criteria for PTSD
(for detailed information see Supplemental Table
S3). In contrast to trait and peritraumatic dissociation,
there are more studies on acute dissociative reactions
during symptom provocation in BPD patients. This is
expected, as dissociation is classied as a pathological
reaction in BPD (American Psychiatric Association,
2013). However, most of the BPD patients in the
included studies report a history of trauma and/or
PTSD symptoms. This is to be expected due to the
high comorbidity of the two disorders (Jowett et al.,
2020). Accordingly, four studies assessed acute dis-
sociation in patients with BPD, where all or a relevant
proportion of the participants reported comorbid
PTSD. However, one study reported correlations in
an overall sample of BPD-patients with and without
comorbid PTSD, as well as healthy controls (compare
Table 5 for more detailed information).
To evaluate acute dissociation, four studies used the
Dissociation-Tension Scale-acute (DSS-acute; Stigl-
mayr et al., 2003; Stiglmayr et al., 2010), consisting
of dissociation-related items from the DES (Bernstein
& Putnam, 1986), somatoform dissociation from the
Somatoform Dissociation Questionnaire (SDQ-20;
Nijenhuis et al., 1996), and 13 items of inner tension.
Two studies used the related short version, the DSS-4
(Stiglmayr et al., 2009). Two studies used the dis-
sociation subscale of the Responses to Script-Driven
Imagery Scale (RSDI-SSD; Hopper, Frewen, Sack
et al., 2007). This self-report rating was developed to
assess post-traumatic reactions during experimental
trauma confrontation on the subscales of re-experien-
cing, avoidance, and dissociation. Three studies used
the Clinician-Administered Dissociative States Scale
(CADSS; Bremner et al., 1998), which is rated by the
subject and by an observer, either on a 4-point scale
or as a dichotomy (present vs. absent). One study
used the Dissociation State Scale (DSS), a self-rating
adaptation of the CADSS (Bremner et al., 1998), and
one study modied the PDEQ (Marmar et al., 1997)
into an 8-item version they termed the State Dis-
sociation Scale (SDS). Malta et al. (2021) designed a
study-specic Dissociation Scale, including 3 items
assessing dissociation related experiences (e.g. losing
sense of time, losing track of events, and feeling
emotionally numb) on a 7-point scale.
To provoke physiological reactions to trauma
stimuli, six studies used personalized intrusive content
and one study used videos with trauma-associated
content of varying emotional valence. Malta et al.
(2021) used a novel approach, incorporating virtual
reality simulations of combat stressors. Seven studies
used more general stress stimuli, including the TSST,
white noise, acoustic startle probes, mirror confronta-
tion, idiographic aversive scripts with startle probes,
emotion regulation (with pictures of varying
emotional valence) and aversive dierential delay con-
ditioning. Regarding the results of the latter, we
focused on stress-eliciting interventions and therefore
only report the psychophysiology for the initial reac-
tivity within this conditioning paradigm.
Eight studies examined HR, one of which focused
on HR acceleration and HR deceleration (dened as
peak and lowest HR) following the experimental
stimulus. Five studies assessed HRV parameters,
including RMSSD, HF-HRV, LF-HRV and LF/HF-
ratio. Three studies examined BP, one PEP and one
nger pulse volume (FPV). Six studies examined SC
parameters, including SCL, SCR and NSCF.
3.3.2. Eects on heart rate
Regarding personalized trauma stimuli, only one
study (Sack et al., 2012) showed a moderate negative
correlation between maximum HR in the trauma con-
dition and acute dissociation (r=.30, p< .050), also
reected by the baseline corrected change scores (r=
.23, p< .050). Additionally, they used median split-
ting of the RSDI-SSD to divide the full sample into
high and low acute dissociation groups and, sub-
sequently, into high re-experiencing and low re-
experiencing groups. Focusing here on the high re-
experiencing group, those with high dissociation
exhibited signicantly lower maximum HR than
those with low dissociation (p= .002, d=1.11) a
nding conrmed by signicantly dierent HR change
scores across groups (p= .002, d=1.11). Other
studies (Bichescu-Burian et al., 2017; Chou et al.,
2018; Lanius et al., 2002; Malta et al., 2021; Schmahl
et al., 2004) found no correlation between HR and
alterations in acute dissociative states. Lanius et al.
(2002) found no dierences in HR between trauma-
exposed patients without PTSD and PTSD patients
with comorbid Dissociative Disorder Not Otherwise
Specied (DDNOS), but the authors interpreted this
as the absence of an increase in HR that would be
expected in PTSD patients.
For general stress stimuli, no correlation was found
between HR and acute dissociation (DAndrea et al.,
2013; Schäein et al., 2018).
3.3.3. Eects on heart rate variability
Studies using personalized trauma stimuli demon-
strate inconsistent and contradictory ndings on
HRV changes in acute dissociation. Sack et al. (2012)
found no correlation between the RMSSD acute dis-
sociation in the overall sample (r=.21, p< .050),
but did nd dierences in RMSSD across participants
with high vs low dissociation (p= .029, d=0.80).
More specically, the high dissociation group exhib-
ited a smaller decrease in RMSSD from the neutral
to the trauma condition, reecting less PNS adapta-
bility. Chou et al. (2018) found a negative correlation
14 S. BEUTLER ET AL.
Table 5. Acute dissociation Study characteristics and results.
Study NPatients
Mean
age
Sex
distribution
(% women) Dissociation
Physiological
parameter
Symptom
provocation
Comparison
Specication HR PEP RMSSD HF LF
LF/
HF-
ratio
BP
(s/
d) FPV SCL SCR NSCF
Personalized trauma stimuli
Bichescu-
Burian et al.
(2017)
21 BPD patients with
comorbid PTSD
23.86 100 DSS-4 HR, SCRa, SCRf Personalized
trauma-content
Correlation *
Chou et al.
(2018)
22 Volunteers with
current PTSD
42.36 68.2% DSS HR, HF-HRV,
LF-HRV, LF/
HF-ratio
Personalized
trauma-content
Correlation ****
Hauschildt
et al. (2011)
52 n= 26 trauma-
exposed with
PTSD,
n= 26 trauma-
exposed
without PTSD
33.1
c
69 DSS-acute RMSSD, HF-
HRV, LF-HRV
Video with
emotional
valence (neutral,
negative, positive,
trauma-related)
Correlation ••
Lanius et al.
(2002)
17
(n= 7 with
PTSD &
comorbid
DDNOS,
n=10
without
PTSD)
Trauma-exposed 35.4
a
94 CADSS
dich
HR Personalized
trauma-content
Patients vs.
controls
*
Malta et al.
(2021)
32 Combat trauma-
exposed
veterans, 34%
with PTSD
30.28 16 Dissociation
Scale
HR VR simulation
including
standardized
combat segments
Correlation
Sack et al.
(2012)
61
(n= 16 high
re-exp. with
high.;
n= 15 high
re-exp. with
low diss.)
Trauma-exposed,
69% met criteria
for current PTSD
34.5 77 RSDI-SSD HR, lnRMSSD,
FPV
Personalized
trauma-content
Correlation (full
sample)
***
High re-exp.
with high vs.
with low
dissociation
*
*
*
Schmahl et al.
(2004)
14 PTSD patients
with history of
sexual & /or
physical abuse
36.2 100 CADSS HR, BP, NSCF Personalized
trauma-content
Correlation **/
*
*
Vermes et al.
(2020)
21 PTSD patients 34.7 95 CADSS SCR Personalized
trauma-content
Correlation
General stress stimuli
Barnow et al.
(2012)
59 n= 21 BPD
patients with
comorbid PTSD
n= 12 BPD
patients without
comorbid PTSD
23.66
a
100 DSS-acute SCL Idiographic aversive
scripts including
startle probe
Correlation *
(Continued)
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTRAUMATOLOGY 15
Table 5. Continued.
Study NPatients
Mean
age
Sex
distribution
(% women) Dissociation
Physiological
parameter
Symptom
provocation
Comparison
Specication HR PEP RMSSD HF LF
LF/
HF-
ratio
BP
(s/
d) FPV SCL SCR NSCF
n= 26 healthy
controls
DAndrea
et al. (2013)
54
(n= 27 high
diss.,
n= 27 low
diss.)
Trauma-exposed
African
American
college
students, 37%
with lifetime
PTSD
18.8 65 SDS HRaccel,
HRdecel, SCR
Acoustic startle High vs. low
dissociation
d
e
*
d
*
e
*
Duesenberg
et al. (2019)
49 BPD patients, 45%
with comorbid
PTSD
28.7 100 DSS-4 BP Trier Social Stress
Test
Correlation */
*
Ebner-
Priemer
et al. (2009)
21
(n= 10 diss.,
n= 11 non-
diss.)
BPD patients, 27%
of the diss., 50%
of the non-diss.
subgroup with
current PTSD
27.8
b
100 DSS-acute SCR Aversive dierential
delay
conditioning
Dissociatives vs.
non-
dissociatives
Krause-Utz
et al. (2019)
20 BPD patients with
comorbid PTSD
34.16 100 DSS-4 HF-HRV Emotional
regulation task
with neutral,
positive, &
negative pictures
Correlation
f
g
h
Metz et al.
(2020)
21 PTSD patients 39.7 100 DSS-acute BP Trier Social Stress
Test
Correlation /
Schäein et al.
(2018)
18 DDNOS patients
with comorbid
PTSD
41.7 94 RSDI-SSD HR, lnRMSSD,
PEP
mirror
confrontation
(only, or
combined with
positive or
negative
cognitions)
Correlation ••
Note: Arrow directions indicate increase or decrease during provocation condition, * = results indicated by change score, =noeect. If studies reported results during condition and change scores, both are presented in the table; BP (s/
d): (systolic or diastolic) blood pressure, BPD: Borderline Personality Disorder, CADSS: Clinician-Administered Dissociative States Scale, CADSS
dich
: dichotomous version of Clinician-Administered Dissociative States Scale, DDNOS: Disso-
ciative Disorder Not Otherwise Specied, DSS: Dissociation State Scale, DSS-acute: Dissociation Tension scale acute, DSS-4: Dissociation Tension Scale acute as short version, FPV: nger pulse volume, HF-HRV: high frequency heart rate
variability, HR: heart rate, HRaccel: heart rate acceleration, HRdecel: heart rate deceleration, HRV: heart rate variability, LF-HRV: low frequency heart rate variability, LF/HF-ratio: ratio of low frequency to high frequency heart rate variability,
lnRMSSD: natural logarithm of root mean square successive dierence, NSCF: number of skin conductance uctuations, PEP: pre-ejection period, PTSD: Posttraumatic-Stress Disorder, RMSSD: root mean square successive dierence, RSDI-
SSD: subscale dissociation of the Responses to Script-Driven Imagery Scale, SC: skin conductance, SCL: skin conductance level, SCR: skin conductance response, SCRa: amplitude of skin conductance response, SCRf: frequency of skin
conductance response, SDS: State Dissociation Scale, VR: virtual reality.
a
N-adjusted mean value weighted by size of subgroups.
bMean age of all 33 BPD patients, while medium dissociative subgroup (n= 12) was not further reported.
cN-adjusted mean value weighted by size of subgroups.
d
HR acceleration.
e
HR deceleration.
f
Non-regulation condition.
g
Regulation of negative pictures.
h
Regulation of positive pictures.
16 S. BEUTLER ET AL.
between dissociation and LF/HF-ratio (r=.53, p
< .050), reecting increased parasympathetic activity,
but no correlation with HF-HRV (r= .16, p> .050)
or LF-HRV (r=.28, p> .050). Hauschildt et al.
(2011) found no correlation between HRV and acute
dissociation.
Regarding general stress stimuli, Schäein et al.
(2018) found no correlation between acute dis-
sociation and RMSSD in the mirror confrontation
task. Krause-Utz et al. (2018) also demonstrated no
eects of acute dissociation on HF-HRV while partici-
pants watched pictures with varying emotional
valence. However, when participants were asked to
downregulate their emotions while viewing, there
was a robust correlation between acute dissociation
and HF-HRV for negative pictures (r= .66, p= .025)
but not for positive pictures (r= .32, p= .614).
3.3.4. Eects on blood pressure and nger pulse
volume
There was no correlation between acute dissociation
and BP during trauma confrontation (Schmahl et al.,
2004) or stress provocation (Duesenberg et al., 2019;
Metz et al., 2020).
However, Sack et al. (2012) found a positive corre-
lation between acute dissociation and FPV (r= .33, p
< .010). This eect was not observed in participants
with high re-experiencing when comparing high vs.
low dissociation (p> .050, d=0.79).
3.3.5. Eects on skin conductance
With respect to studies using personalized trauma
stimuli, no eects of acute dissociation on SC were
observed (Bichescu-Burian et al., 2017; Schmahl
et al., 2004; Vermes et al., 2020).
When general stress stimuli were utilized, ndings
were inconclusive. In a mixed sample including BPD
patients with and without comorbid PTSD as well as
healthy controls, Barnow et al. (2012) demonstrated
a positive correlation between acute dissociation and
SCL change score (r= .14, p= .001). Neither DAndrea
et al. (2013) nor Ebner-Priemer et al. (2009) found
group dierences in SCL at baseline (for baseline see
Supplemental Table S2). Regarding SCR, DAndrea
et al. (2013) demonstrated that SCR was moderately
decreased in the high dissociation group (p= .040, d
=0.57).
4. Discussion
We systematically reviewed 28 articles (n= 1300 sub-
jects) with the hope of identifying the psychophysiolo-
gical correlates of trait, peritraumatic, and acute
dissociation. Due to conicting results within and
between studies, we found no clear pattern or trend
for physiological markers of trauma-related dis-
sociation. Studies on trait dissociation demonstrated
contradicting physiological reactions. Two studies
using TSST (Powers et al., 2021; Simeon et al., 2008)
found a decrease in heart rate during stress induction
which was associated with trait dissociation, whereas
two other studies (Hyer et al., 1993; Schmahl et al.,
2004) demonstrated hyperactive autonomic responses
to traumatic content. These inconsistencies allude that
the type of intervention and stress arousal (social
stress vs. traumatic stress) may drive specic physio-
logical responses. However, this divergence was only
observed in relation to chronic trait dissociation. For
peritraumatic dissociation, one study depicted a sig-
nicantly decreased heart rate in comorbid PTSD-
BPD patients (Bichescu-Burian et al., 2017). Other
studies showed a slight trend towards hyperarousal
associated with the retrospectively assessed dissocia-
tive reactions experienced during the initial traumatic
event (Hetzel-Riggin & Wilber, 2010; Ladwig et al.,
2002; Sledjeski & Delahanty, 2012). Contrary to this,
several studies investigating acute post-traumatic dis-
sociation depicted a tendency towards a hypoaroused
autonomic reactivity (Chou et al., 2018;DAndrea
et al., 2013; Krause-Utz et al., 2018; Sack et al.,
2012), suggesting that peri- and post-traumatic dis-
sociation may be physiologically distinct from each
other.
According to the defense cascade (Lanius et al.,
2018)andshutdown models (Schauer & Elbert,
2010), individuals who experience dissociation
during the initial traumatic event should bypass the
tonic immobility state and enter a hypoaroused
state. Additionally, an individuals initial physiologic
response to a traumatic event will determine their
subsequent responses to trauma cues and stressors
(see Introduction). Current research assessing peri-
traumatic and/or acute dissociation has not yet pro-
vided sucient support for this idea. As previously
stated, only a single study on peritraumatic dis-
sociation demonstrated a signicantly decreased
heart rate, reecting a hypoaroused autonomic state
(Bichescu-Burian et al., 2017). Contrarily, several
other studies on peritraumatic dissociation found a
tendency towards increased arousal. With regards
to acute post-traumatic dissociation, ndings were
similarly inconsistent, although there was some evi-
dence of an attenuated response (based on heart
rate, heart rate variability and skin conductance)
(Chou et al., 2018;DAndrea et al., 2013;Krause-
Utz et al., 2018;Sacketal.,2012). Based on our
assessment of the reviewed studies, we conclude
that peritraumatic dissociation and acute posttrau-
matic dissociation are neither associated with PNS-
mediated hypoarousal, nor follow the proposed
physiologic trajectory in which the initial passive
response to a trauma predicts onesresponsestosub-
sequent stressors or trauma reminders. At this point,
there is insucient evidence to prove a consistent
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTRAUMATOLOGY 17
association beyond spurious ndings, raising the
question: why do well-prepared experimental studies
fail to extract psychophysiological markers of
dissociation?
One possible answer might lie in the inconsistent
assessment of psychophysiological parameters across
studies. Outcomes may be inuenced by varying adap-
tation times, measurement durations, baseline con-
ditions, artifact editing, and post-experiment
processing. For instance, a review (Beauchaine et al.,
2019) reported high inconsistencies across studies
which used the RSA as a measure of transpathological
dimensions, which they attribute to a lack of methodo-
logical standardization. Contrarily, psychophysiologi-
cal markers of PTSD have been extensively
investigated and respected for their easy and aord-
able implementation into clinical, ambulatory, and
laboratory research (see Pineles & Orr, 2018).
Additionally, if PTSD is examined without consider-
ation of dissociative symptoms, there is consistent
support for increased autonomic activity while resting
and across experimental paradigms, as measured by
HR, HRV parameters, and SC (Ge et al., 2020; Nagpal
et al., 2013; Schneider & Schwerdtfeger, 2020). How-
ever, when the dissociative subtype of PTSD is
extracted, the psychophysiological ngerprint remains
elusive.
A recent extensive systematic review focused on the
psychophysiological markers (among others) of
pathological dissociation in a broader, transdiagnostic
view (Roydeva & Reinders, 2021). This review
conrmed our ndings, citing inconclusive results
due to most studies reporting no eects. The authors
further concluded that none of the reviewed physio-
logical measures could be recommended as a bio-
marker of pathological dissociation (Roydeva &
Reinders, 2021). The same is true for markers of
post-traumatic dissociation at the level of brain acti-
vation or anatomy. A review on this topic failed to
establish a clear trend across studies (Lotnia et al.,
2020). Although analyses on resting-state brain acti-
vation tentatively support the idea of an overmodu-
lated fear network (see defense cascade and shutdown
models) through reports of altered functional connec-
tivity between prefrontal and subcortical areas in dis-
sociative PTSD patients (Nicholson et al., 2017;
Nicholson et al., 2019; Rabellino et al., 2018), exper-
imental studies are lacking. The few experimental neu-
roimaging studies to date (Daniels et al., 2012;
Felmingham et al., 2008; Hopper, Frewen, van der
Kolk et al., 2007; Mertens et al., 2022) either report
inconsistent brain regions associated with trauma-
related dissociation or fail to present any associated
neural regions at all. Ultimately, this undermines the
proposed neurobiological basis (e.g. Lanius et al.,
2010; Terpou et al., 2019) of the defense cascade
model and the dissociative subtype as a whole.
Other factors which may inuence the assessment
of valid objective markers of dissociation are the
inconsistent conceptualization and measurement of
dissociation. Within this review alone, we include
studies with 14 unique measures of dissociation,
which each emphasize dierent aspects of dis-
sociation. The resulting sum scores are therefore dri-
ven by a spectrum of symptoms, from those
considered less pathological (e.g. absorption) to
those considered more pathological (e.g. identity frag-
mentation). When correlating such a sum score with
psychophysiological measures, any true eect of
depersonalization and derealization could be masked
by the inuence of these other symptoms. Hence, het-
erogeneity in our results may be driven by the diver-
sity of dissociation assessment techniques.
Additionally, the meaning of hypoarousal is not
consistent in the dissociation literature. Most of the
aforementioned models dened hypoarousal as equiv-
alent to parasympathetic dominance; however physio-
logical research shows that alterations in heart rate or
heart rate variability may be steered by both SNS and
PNS activity (de Geus et al., 2019). Multimodal assess-
ments incorporating psychophysiological measures
reective of both SNS and PNS activation are thus
highly recommended to disentangle these complex
interactions (e.g. as in Schmahl et al., 2004; Seligowski
et al., 2019).
The initial pilot studies describing alternating phys-
iological reactions to individualized trauma scripts in
chronic PTSD patients (Lanius et al., 2001; Lanius
et al., 2002) interpreted the data as supportive for
two distinct subtypes each representing unique path-
ways to chronic stress- related psychopathology
(Lanius et al., 2006, p. 710). In these studies, patients
with elevated heart rates (70%) were characterized as
intrusive subtypes whereas the 30% without elevated
heart rates were characterized as dissociative subtypes,
reective of derealization or depersonalization. Here,
dissociative hypoarousal was characterized as the
absence of the PTSD-pathognomonic hyperarousal
state. It should be noted that these ndings were
based on small samples (N10). Another study
from the same research group found that although
some dissociative PTSD patients had unchanged (or
not increased) HR during trauma exposure, there
was no clinical dierence between these patients and
those who exhibited decreased or increased HR
(Lanius et al., 2005). Meanwhile, other studies
employed the term active suppressionto reect sig-
nicant heart rate deceleration (DAndrea et al.,
2013) or reduced heart rate reactivity (Sack et al.,
2012) concurrent with parasympathetic dominance
(Lanius et al., 2018; Schauer & Elbert, 2010). Due to
these inconsistencies in language, we implore future
researchers in this eld to clearly state whether they
dene hypoarousal as the absence of an increase in
18 S. BEUTLER ET AL.
HR, a relative decrease in HR within-subjects (e.g.
from baseline or within tasks), or a decrease in HR
between-groups (e.g. compared to trauma-exposed
controls).
4.1. Limitations and strengths
Several limitations are present in the current review.
First, this qualitative review did not include a quanti-
tative meta-analysis of the reviewed studiesndings.
Although the search resulted in sucient data to be
quantitatively reviewed, it was not deemed sensible
due to the high heterogeneity in sample characteristics
(e.g. PTSD severity, gender, comorbidity), experimen-
tal interventions, dissociation assessment tools, and
physiological measures. With this variation, a meta-
analysis would run the risk of hastily simplifying a
complex phenomenon. Thus, a systematic overview
of these heterogenetic studies seemed to be more
appropriate.
This review specically focused on post-traumatic
dissociation and therefore concentrated on search
terms according to derealization/depersonalization.
However, except for the RSDI-SSD, none of the
reported instruments solely measured derealization/
depersonalization. Therefore, no clear distinction
could be made between physiological reactions
specic to derealization/depersonalization vs. those
associated with compartmentalization(Holmes
et al., 2005). Hence, these results can only be inter-
preted to a limited extent. Additionally, we have lim-
ited information on the dierences between
dissociators vs non-dissociators with PTSD to verify
the postulated opposing subtypes. Only one study on
these opposing groups was identied within in each
included dissociative modality (e.g. acute, trait, and
peritraumatic dissociation). We observed lower auto-
nomic activity in people with high acute dissociation
(Sack et al., 2012), higher HR in those with high
trait dissociation (Hyer et al., 1993), and no physio-
logic eect in those with high peritraumatic dis-
sociation (Kaufman et al., 2002).
One of the main limitations to the reviewed studies
is the low sample sizes and thus limited statistical
power to detect subtle changes in sensitive measures
of psychophysiological activity. Additionally, the het-
erogeneity of physiological indices and dissociation
measures employed across studies impedes our ability
to make any direct comparisons or generalizations.
Specically, only ve studies included at least two
types of dissociation modalities (e.g. assessed both
peritraumatic and acute dissociation). Most studies
related dissociation to mean scores of psychophysiolo-
gical measurements. In future studies, temporal
changes in physiology should be included to highlight
any uctuations or trends that may support current
models of defense response (e.g. progression of an
initial increased HR followed by reduced HR/hypoten-
sion as suggested by the defense cascade model).
This review has several strengths and provides novel
insight into the psychophysiology of dissociation in
PTSD. One strength is that we included only exper-
imental studies that employed symptom provocation
or stress-induction paradigms. This is of signicance
as all defense response models underscore the impor-
tance of dissociation as a defensive reaction to a clear
stressor or trauma reminder. To our knowledge, this
is the rst review to summarize the relationship
between dissociation and experimentally induced
acute physiological responses. Furthermore, we cat-
egorize ndings according to distinct dissociation mod-
alities, thus permitting a clear comparison of
physiological responses across peri-, acute, and chronic
post-traumatic dissociation. In doing so, we begin to
disentangle dissociation-related psychophysiological
reactions from the moment the trauma occurs, gets
reactivated, and develops into a chronic pattern.
4.2. Conclusion and future outlook
In sum, the current review investigates psychophysio-
logical correlates of dissociation in PTSD through a
well-dened and narrow scope. From the extracted lit-
erature, we nd (a) an association between a hyperac-
tive defensive response and peritraumatic dissociation,
(b) an association between a hypoactive defensive
response and acute post-traumatic dissociation, and
(c) a possible impact of the type of stressor on physio-
logic responses in chronic trait dissociation. However,
there is no evidence for a clear link between a hypoar-
oused ANS and peri- and post-traumatic dissociation
across studies. Furthermore, it should be noted that
none of the studied physiological parameters are
pathognomonic predictors of trauma-related dis-
sociation as similar reactive patterns have been
observed across various psychiatric disorders (see
Chalmers et al., 2014 for anxiety; see Koenig et al.,
2016 for BPD). As recommended by Roydeva and
Reinders (2021), future research should aim to
increase the specicity of biomarkers to PTSD-related
dissociation, for instance by testing the robustness of
these psychophysiological correlates across other psy-
chopathologies (e.g. depression or anxiety).
To overcome the gap between theoretical models
and empirical evidence, studies are needed which
have clear inclusion criteria (e.g. PTSD or post-trau-
matic stress symptoms), control for PTSD severity,
and use longitudinal designs to assess symptom vari-
ation. Particularly, studies which assess peritraumatic
dissociation as early as possible after a trauma will
reduce the risk of retrospective bias. Ambulatory
assessment strategies like ecological sampling (e.g.
wireless HR monitoring) may overcome the necessity
of provoking PTSD and dissociation symptoms. These
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTRAUMATOLOGY 19
ndings would be far more applicable to the PTSD
patient population.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Christina Schulte for taking
part in the rating process as independent rater and Margaret
Engstrom for language proofreading. The authors express
our gratitude to all authors of the original publications for
supporting this work by providing additional information,
calculations, and their interest in this research topic.
Disclosure statement
No potential conict of interest was reported by the author(s).
Funding
This research was funded by the German Research Foun-
dation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, DFG), Grant
number: CR 479/91 (http://www.dfg.de/). This work was
supported by the EU Rosalind-Franklin Fellowship granted
to J. K. Daniels. The work by Sarah Beutler was supported by
Fellowship of the Heinrich Böll Foundation. Liliana
R. Ladner was supported by the Deutscher Akademischer
Austauschdienst (DAAD) Research Internships in Science
and Engineering Program.
Data availability
Supplementary material, including a data le with the
eect sizes and detailed descriptions of the studies
reported (see Supplementary Table 3), can be found in
the online version, at https://doi.org/10.1080/20008066.
2022.2132599.
ORCID
Judith K. Daniels http://orcid.org/0000-0001-6304-2310
References
Abrams, M. P., Carleton, R. N., Taylor, S., & Asmundson, G.
J. (2009). Human tonic immobility: Measurement and
correlates. Depress and Anxiety,26(6), 550556. https://
doi.org/10.1002/da.20462
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and
statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
American Psychiatric Publishing. https://doi.org/10.
1176/appi.books.9780890425596
Bandler, R., Keay, K. A., Floyd, N., & Price, J. (2000). Central
circuits mediating patterned autonomic activity during
active vs. Passive emotional coping. Brain Research
Bulletin,53(1), 95104. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0361-
9230(00)00313-0
Barnow, S., Limberg, A., Stopsack, M., Spitzer, C., Grabe, H.
J., Freyberger, H. J., & Hamm, A. (2012, Apr).
Dissociation and emotion regulation in borderline per-
sonality disorder. Psychological Medicine,42(4), 783
794. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711001917
Barrett, L. F. (2020). Seven and a half lessons about the brain
(1st ed.). Houghton Miin Harcourt.
Beauchaine, T. P., Bell, Z., Knapton, E., McDonough-
Caplan, H., Shader, T., & Zisner, A. (2019). Respiratory
sinus arrhythmia reactivity across empirically based
structural dimensions of psychopathology: A meta-analy-
sis. Psychophysiology,56(5), e13329. https://doi.org/10.
1111/psyp.13329
Bergmann, U. (2008). Shes come undone: A neurobiologi-
cal exploration of dissociative disorders. In C. Forgash &
M. Copeley (Eds.), Healing the heart of trauma and dis-
sociation: with EMDR and ego state therapy (pp. 6190).
Springer.
Bernstein, E. M., & Putnam, F. W. (1986, Dec). Development,
reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. The Journal
of Nervous and Mental Disease,174(12), 727735. https://
doi.org/10.1097/00005053-198612000-00004
Berntson, G. G., Bigger, J. T., Jr., Eckberg, D. L., Grossman,
P., Kaufmann, P. G., Malik, M., Nagaraja, H. N., Porges,
S. W., Saul, J. P., Stone, P. H., & van der Molen, M. W.
(1997). Heart rate variability: Origins, methods, and
interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology,34(6), 623648.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02140.x
Bichescu-Burian, D., Steyer, J., Steinert, T., Grieb, B., &
Tschoke, S. (2017, Mar). Trauma-related dissociation:
Psychological features and psychophysiological responses
to script-driven imagery in borderline personality dis-
order. Psychophysiology,54(3), 452461. https://doi.org/
10.1111/psyp.12795
Billman, G. E. (2013). The LF/HF ratio does not accurately
measure cardiac sympatho-vagal balance. Frontiers in
Physiology,4,15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2013.
00026
Bremner, J. D., Krystal, J. H., Putnam, F. W., Southwick, S.
M., Marmar, C., Charney, D. S., & Mazure, C. M. (1998,
Jan). Measurement of dissociative states with the clini-
cian-administered dissociative states scale (CADSS).
Journal of Traumatic Stress,11(1), 125136. https://doi.
org/10.1023/A:1024465317902
Briere, J., Weathers, F. W., & Runtz, M. (2005, Jun). Is
dissociation a multidimensional construct? Data from
the multiscale dissociation inventory. Journal of
Traumatic Stress,18(3), 221231. https://doi.org/10.
1002/jts.20024
Carlson, E. B., Waelde, L. C., Palmieri, P. A., Macia, K. S.,
Smith, S. R., & McDade-Montez, E. (2018, Jan).
Development and validation of the dissociative symp-
toms scale. Assessment,25(1), 8498. https://doi.org/10.
1177/1073191116645904
Castro-Chapman, P. L., Orr, S. P., Berg, J., Pineles, S. L.,
Yanson, J., & Salomon, K. (2018, Mar). Heart rate reactiv-
ity to trauma-related imagery as a measure of PTSD
symptom severity: Examining a new cohort of veterans.
Psychiatry Research,261, 574580. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.psychres.2018.01.024
Chalmers, J. A., Quintana, D. S., Abbott, M. J., & Kemp, A.
H. (2014). Anxiety disorders are associated with reduced
heart rate variability: A meta-analysis. Frontiers in
Psychiatry,5,15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.
00080
Chou, C. Y., La Marca, R., Steptoe, A., & Brewin, C. R.
(2018). Cardiovascular and psychological responses to
voluntary recall of trauma in posttraumatic stress dis-
order. European Journal of Psychotraumatology,9(1),
Article 1472988. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2018.
1472988
Corrigan, F. M., Fisher, J. J., & Nutt, D. J. (2011). Autonomic
dysregulation and the window of tolerance model of the
eects of complex emotional trauma. Journal of
20 S. BEUTLER ET AL.
Psychopharmacology,25(1), 1725. https://doi.org/10.
1177/0269881109354930
Cuthbert, B. N., Lang, P. J., Strauss, C., Drobes, D., Patrick, C.
J., & Bradley, M. M. (2003). The psychophysiology of
anxiety disorder: Fear memory imagery. Psychophysiology,
40(3), 407422. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.00043
DAndrea, W., Pole, N., DePierro, J., Freed, S., & Wallace, D.
B. (2013, Oct). Heterogeneity of defensive responses after
exposure to trauma: Blunted autonomic reactivity in
response to startling sounds. International Journal of
Psychophysiology,90(1), 8089. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
ijpsycho.2013.07.008
Dalenberg, C., & Carlson, E. B. (2012). Dissociation in
posttraumatic stress disorder part II: How theoretical
models t the empirical evidence and recommen-
dations for modifying the diagnostic criteria for
PTSD. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research,
Practice, and Policy,4(6), 551559. https://doi.org/10.
1037/a0027900
Daniels, J. K., Coupland, N. J., Hegadoren, K. M., Rowe, B.
H., Densmore, M., Neufeld, R. W., & Lanius, R. A. (2012).
Neural and behavioral correlates of peritraumatic dis-
sociation in an acutely traumatized sample. The Journal
of Clinical Psychiatry,73(4), 420426. https://doi.org/10.
4088/JCP.10m06642
de Geus, E. J. C., Gianaros, P. J., Brindle, R. C., Jennings, J. R.,
& Berntson, G. G. ((2019, Feb). Should heart rate variabil-
ity be correctedfor heart rate? Biological, quantitative,
and interpretive considerations. Psychophysiology,56(2),
e13287. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13287
Dell, P. F. (2006). The multidimensional inventory of dis-
sociation (MID): A comprehensive measure of pathologi-
cal dissociation. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation,7(2),
77106. https://doi.org/10.1300/J229v07n02_06
Duesenberg, M., Wolf, O. T., Metz, S., Roepke, S., Fleischer,
J., Elias, V., Renneberg, B., Otte, C., & Wingenfeld, K.
(2019). Psychophysiological stress response and memory
in borderline personality disorder. European Journal of
Psychotraumatology,10(1), Article 1568134. https://doi.
org/10.1080/20008198.2019.1568134
Ebner-Priemer, U. W., Mauchnik, J., Kleindienst, N.,
Schmahl, C., Peper, M., Rosenthal, M. Z., Flor, H., &
Bohus, M. (2009). Emotional learning during dissociative
states in borderline personality disorder. Journal of
Psychiatry & Neuroscience: JPN,34(3), 214222. https://
www.jpn.ca/content/34/3/214.full
Felmingham, K., Kemp, A. H., Williams, L., Falconer, E.,
Olivieri, G., Peduto, A., & Bryant, R. (2008).
Dissociative responses to conscious and non-conscious
fear impact underlying brain function in post-traumatic
stress disorder. Psychological Medicine,38(12), 1771
1780. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291708002742
Frewen, P. A., & Lanius, R. A. (2006a). Neurobiology of dis-
sociation: Unity and disunity in mind-body-brain.
Psychiatric Clinics of North America,29(1), 113128.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2005.10.016
Frewen, P. A., & Lanius, R. A. (2006b). Toward a psychobiol-
ogy of posttraumatic self-dysregulation: Reexperiencing,
hyperarousal, dissociation, and emotional numbing.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,1071(1),
110124. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1364.010
Ge, F., Yuan, M., Li, Y., & Zhang, W. (2020, Jan).
Posttraumatic stress disorder and alterations in resting
heart rate variability: A systematic review and meta-
analysis. Psychiatry Investigation,17(1), 920. https://
doi.org/10.30773/pi.2019.0112 [Record #149 is using a
reference type undened in this output style.]
Grossman, P., & Taylor, E. W. (2007). Toward understand-
ing respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Relations to cardiac
vagal tone, evolution and biobehavioral functions.
Biological Psychology,74(2), 263285. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.014
Hagenaars, M. A. (2016). Tonic immobility and PTSD in a
large community sample. Journal of Experimental
Psychopathology,7(2), 246260. https://doi.org/10.5127/
jep.051915
Hagenaars, M. A., Oitzl, M., & Roelofs, K. (2014). Updating
freeze: Aligning animal and human research.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,47, 165176.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.07.021
Halligan, S. L., Michael, T., Clark, D. M., & Ehlers, A. (2003,
Jun). Posttraumatic stress disorder following assault: The
role of cognitive processing, trauma memory, and apprai-
sals. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,71(3),
419431. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006x.71.3.419
Halligan, S. L., Michael, T., Wilhelm, F. H., Clark, D. M., &
Ehlers, A. (2006, Oct). Reduced heart rate responding to
trauma reliving in trauma survivors with PTSD:
Correlates and consequences. Journal of Traumatic
Stress,19(5), 721734. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20167
Hansen, M., Ross, J., & Armour, C. (2017, Apr 15). Evidence
of the dissociative PTSD subtype: A systematic literature
review of latent class and prole analytic studies of PTSD.
Journal of Aective Disorders,213,5969. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jad.2017.02.004
Hauschildt, M., Peters, M. J., Moritz, S., & Jelinek, L. (2011,
Dec). Heart rate variability in response to aective scenes
in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychology,88
(2-3), 215222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.
08.004
Head, M. L., Holman, L., Lanfear, R., Kahn, A. T., &
Jennions, M. D. (2015, Mar). The extent and conse-
quences of p-hacking in science. PLOS Biology,13(3),
e1002106. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002106
Hetzel-Riggin, M. D., & Wilber, E. L. (2010). To dissociate
or suppress? Predicting automatic vs. conscious cognitive
avoidance. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation,11(4), 444
457. https://doi.org/10.1080/15299732.2010.495376
Holmes, E. A., Brown, R. J., Mansell, W., Fearon, R. P.,
Hunter, E. C., Frasquilho, F., & Oakley, D. A. (2005).
Are there two qualitatively distinct forms of dissociation?
A review and some clinical implications. Clinical
Psychology Review,25(1), 123. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.cpr.2004.08.006
Hopper, J. W., Frewen, P. A., Sack, M., Lanius, R. A., & van
der Kolk, B. A. (2007). The responses to script-driven
imagery scale (RSDI): Assessment of state posttraumatic
symptoms for psychobiological and treatment research.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,
29(4), 249268. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-007-
9046-0
Hopper, J. W., Frewen, P. A., van der Kolk, B. A., & Lanius,
R. A. (2007). Neural correlates of reexperiencing, avoid-
ance, and dissociation in PTSD: Symptom dimensions
and emotion dysregulation in responses to script-driven
trauma imagery. Journal of Traumatic Stress,20(5),
713725. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20284
Hyer, L. A., Albrecht, J. W., Boudewyns, P. A., Woods, M.
G., & Brandsma, J. (1993). Dissociative experiences of
Vietnam veterans with chronic posttraumatic stress dis-
order. Psychological Reports,73(2), 519530. https://doi.
org/10.2466/pr0.1993.73.2.519
Iand, B., Klein, F., Rosner, R., Renneberg, B., Steil, R., &
Neuner, F. (2020, Jan). Cardiac reactions to emotional
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTRAUMATOLOGY 21
words in adolescents and young adults with PTSD after
child abuse. Psychophysiology,57(1), e13470. https://doi.
org/10.1111/psyp.13470
Jowett, S., Karatzias, T., & Albert, I. (2020, Sep). Multiple
and interpersonal trauma are risk factors for both post-
traumatic stress disorder and borderline personality dis-
order: A systematic review on the traumatic backgrounds
and clinical characteristics of comorbid post-traumatic
stress disorder/borderline personality disorder groups
versus single-disorder groups. Psychology and
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice,93(3),
621638. https://doi.org/10.1111/papt.12248
Kaufman, M. L., Kimble, M. O., Kaloupek, D. G., McTeague,
L. M., Bachrach, P., Forti, A. M., & Keane, T. M. (2002).
Peritraumatic dissociation and physiological response to
trauma-relevant stimuli in Vietnam combat veterans
with posttraumatic stress disorder. The Journal of
Nervous and Mental Disease,190(3), 167174. https://
doi.org/10.1097/00005053-200203000-00005
Koenig, J., Kemp, A. H., Feeling, N. R., Thayer, J. F., &
Kaess, M. (2016). Resting state vagal tone in borderline
personality disorder: A meta-analysis. Progress in
Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry,
64,1826. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.07.002
Krause-Utz, A., Walther, J.-C., Lis, S., Schmahl, C., & Bohus,
M. (2018). Heart rate variability during a cognitive reap-
praisal task in female patients with borderline personality
disorder: The role of comorbid posttraumatic stress dis-
order and dissociation. Psychological Medicine,49(11),
18101821. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291718002489
Kuiling, J. M. E., Klaassen, F., & Hagenaars, M. A. (2019).
The role of tonic immobility and control in the develop-
ment of intrusive memories after experimental trauma.
Memory,27(6), 772779. https://doi.org/10.1080/
09658211.2018.1564331
Ladwig, K.-H., Marten-Mittag, B., Deisenhofer, I.,
Hofmann, B., Schapperer, J., Weyerbrock, S., Erazo, N.,
& Schmitt, C. (2002). Psychophysiological correlates of
peritraumatic dissociative responses in survivors of life-
threatening cardiac events. Psychopathology,35(4), 241
248. https://doi.org/10.1159/000063825
Lanius, R. A., Bluhm, R., Lanius, U., & Pain, C. (2006). A
review of neuroimaging studies in PTSD: Heterogeneity
of response to symptom provocation. Journal of
Psychiatric Research,40(8), 709729. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.jpsychires.2005.07.007
Lanius,R.A.,Boyd,J.E.,McKinnon,M.C.,Nicholson,A.
A., Frewen, P., Vermetten, E., Jetly, R., & Spiegel, D.
(2018).Areviewoftheneurobiologicalbasisof
trauma-related dissociation and Its relation to cannabi-
noid- and opioid-mediated stress response: A transdiag-
nostic, translational approach. Current Psychiatry
Reports,20(12), 114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-
018-0983-y
Lanius, R. A., Brand, B., Vermetten, E., Frewen, P. A., &
Spiegel, D. (2012). The dissociative subtype of posttrau-
matic stress disorder: Rationale, clinical and neurobiolo-
gical evidence, and implications. Depression and Anxiety,
29(8), 701708. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.21889
Lanius, R. A., Vermetten, E., Loewenstein, R. J., Brand, B.,
Schmahl, C., Bremner, J. D., & Spiegel, D. (2010).
Emotion modulation in PTSD: Clinical and neurobiolo-
gical evidence for a dissociative subtype. American
Journal of Psychiatry,167(6), 640647. https://doi.org/
10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09081168
Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Bluhm, R. L., Densmore,
M., Boksman, K., Neufeld, R. W. J., Gati, J. S., &
Menon, R. S. (2005). Functional connectivity of dissocia-
tive responses in posttraumatic stress disorder: A func-
tional magnetic resonance imaging investigation.
Biological Psychiatry,57(8), 873884. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.011
Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Boksman, K., Densmore,
M., Gupta, M., Neufeld, R. W. J., Gati, J. S., & Menon,
R. S. (2002). Brain activation during script-driven ima-
gery induced dissociative responses in PTSD: A func-
tional magnetic resonance imaging investigation.
Biological Psychiatry,52(4), 305311. https://doi.org/10.
1016/s0006-3223(02)01367-7
Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Densmore, M., Boksman,
K., Gupta, M. A., Neufeld, R. W., Gati, J. S., & Menon,
R. S. (2001). Neural correlates of traumatic memories
in posttraumatic stress disorder: A functional MRI
investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry,158(11),
19201922. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1920
Lenhard, W., & Lenhard, A. (2016). Berechnung von
Eektstärken [Calculation of eect sizes]. In
Psychometrica. https://www.psychometrica.de/
eektstaerke.html
Lima, A. A., Fiszman, A., Marques-Portella, C., Mendlowicz,
M. V., Coutinho, E. S., Maia, D. C., Berger, W., Rocha-
Rego, V., Volchan, E., Mari, J. J., & Figueira, I. (2010,
Mar). The impact of tonic immobility reaction on the
prognosis of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of
Psychiatric Research,44(4), 224228. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.jpsychires.2009.08.005
Limberg, A., Barnow, S., Freyberger, H. J., & Hamm, A. O.
(2011). Emotional vulnerability in borderline personality
disorder is cue specic and modulated by traumatization.
Biological Psychiatry,69(6), 574582. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.024
Lotnia, S., Soorgi, Z., Mertens, Y., & Daniels, J. (2020).
Structural and functional brain alterations in psychiatric
patients with dissociative experiences: A systematic
review of magnetic resonance imaging studies. Journal
of Psychiatric Research,128,515. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.jpsychires.2020.05.006
Ludascher, P., Valerius, G., Stiglmayr, C., Mauchnik, J.,
Lanius, R. A., Bohus, M., & Schmahl, C. (2010). Pain
sensitivity and neural processing during dissociative
states in patients with borderline personality disorder
with and without comorbid posttraumatic stress dis-
order: A pilot study. Journal of Psychiatry and
Neuroscience,35(3), 177184. https://doi.org/10.1503/
jpn.090022
Lyssenko, L., Schmahl, C., Bockhacker, L., Vonderlin, R.,
Bohus, M., & Kleindienst, N. (2018, Jan 1). Dissociation
in psychiatric disorders: A meta-analysis of studies
using the dissociative experiences scale. American
Journal of Psychiatry,175(1), 3746. https://doi.org/10.
1176/appi.ajp.2017.17010025
MacLean, P. D. (1990). The triune brain in evolution: Role in
paleocerebral functions. Springer Science & Business
Media.
Malta, L. S., Giosan, C., Szkodny, L. E., Altemus, M. M.,
Rizzo, A. A., Silbersweig, D. A., & Difede, J. (2021).
Development of a virtual reality laboratory stressor.
Virtual Reality,25(2), 293302. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10055-020-00455-5
Marmar,C.R.,Weiss,D.S.,&Metzler,T.J.(1997). The
peritraumatic dissociative experiences questionnaire.
In J. P. Wilson & T. M. Keane (Eds.), Assessing psycho-
logical trauma and PTSD (pp. 412428). The Guilford
Press.
22 S. BEUTLER ET AL.
Marmar, C. R., Weiss, D. S., Schlenger, W. E., Fairbank, J.
A., Jordan, B. K., Kulka, R. A., & Hough, R. L. (1994,
Jun). Peritraumatic dissociation and posttraumatic stress
in male Vietnam theater veterans. American Journal of
Psychiatry,151(6), 902907. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.
151.6.902
McKinnon, M. C., Boyd, J. E., Frewen, P. A., Lanius, U. F.,
Jetly, R., Richardson, J. D., & Lanius, R. A. (2016, Sep). A
review of the relation between dissociation, memory,
executive functioning and social cognition in military
members and civilians with neuropsychiatric conditions.
Neuropsychologia,90, 210234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuropsychologia.2016.07.017
McTeague, L. M., & Lang, P. J. (2012). The anxiety spectrum
and the reex physiology of defense: From circumscribed
fear to broad distress. Depression and Anxiety,29(4), 264
281. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.21891
Mertens, Y. L., Manthey, A., Sierk, A., Walter, H., & Daniels,
J. K. (2022, Jun 10). Neural correlates of acute post-trau-
matic dissociation: A functional neuroimaging script-dri-
ven imagery study. BJPsych Open,8(4), e109. https://doi.
org/10.1192/bjo.2022.65
Metz, S., Duesenberg, M., Hellmann-Regen, J., Wolf, O. T.,
Roepke, S., Otte, C., & Wingenfeld, K. (2020, Nov).
Blunted salivary cortisol response to psychosocial stress
in women with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of
Psychiatric Research,130, 112119. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.jpsychires.2020.07.014
Mickleborough, M. J., Daniels, J. K., Coupland, N. J., Kao,
R., Williamson, P. C., Lanius, U. F., Hegadoren, K.,
Schore, A., Densmore, M., Stevens, T., & Lanius, R. A.
(2011). Eects of trauma-related cues on pain processing
in posttraumatic stress disorder: An fMRI investigation.
Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience,36(1), 614.
https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.080188
Mobbs, D., Marchant, J. L., Hassabis, D., Seymour, B., Tan,
G., Gray, M., Petrovic, P., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D.
(2009). From threat to fear: The neural organization of
defensive fear systems in humans. Journal of
Neuroscience,29(39), 1223612243. https://doi.org/10.
1523/JNEUROSCI.2378-09.2009
Murray, J., Ehlers, A., & Mayou, R. A. (2002, Apr).
Dissociation and post-traumatic stress disorder: Two
prospective studies of road trac accident survivors.
British Journal of Psychiatry,180(4), 363368. https://
doi.org/10.1192/bjp.180.4.363
Nagpal, M. L., Gleichauf, K., & Ginsberg, J. P. (2013). Meta-
analysis of heart rate variability as a psychophysiological
indicator of posttraumatic stress disorder. Trauma &
Treatment,03(182), 18. https://doi.org/10.4172/2167-
1222.1000182
Nicholson, A. A., Densmore, M., McKinnon, M. C.,
Neufeld, R. W. J., Frewen, P. A., Theberge, J., Jetly, R.,
Richardson, J. D., & Lanius, R. A. (2019, Sep). Machine
learning multivariate pattern analysis predicts classi-
cation of posttraumatic stress disorder and its dissociative
subtype: A multimodal neuroimaging approach.
Psychological Medicine,49(12), 20492059. https://doi.
org/10.1017/S0033291718002866
Nicholson, A. A., Friston, K. J., Zeidman, P., Harricharan, S.,
McKinnon, M. C., Densmore, M., Neufeld, R. W. J.,
Theberge, J., Corrigan, F., Jetly, R., Spiegel, D., &
Lanius, R. A. (2017, Nov). Dynamic causal modeling in
PTSD and its dissociative subtype: Bottom-up versus
top-down processing within fear and emotion regulation
circuitry. Human Brain Mapping,38(11), 55515561.
https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23748
Nijenhuis, E. R., Spinhoven, P., Van Dyck, R., Van der Hart,
O., & Vanderlinden, J. ((1996, Nov). The development
and psychometric characteristics of the Somatoform
Dissociation Questionnaire (SDQ-20). The Journal of
Nervous and Mental Disease,184(11), 688694. https://
doi.org/10.1097/00005053-199611000-00006
Ogden, P., Minton, K., & Pain, C. (2006). Trauma and the
body: A sensorimotor approach to psychotherapy.
W. W. Norton & Company.
Orr, S. P. (1997). Psychophysiologic reactivity to trauma-
related imagery in PTSD. Diagnostic and theoretical
implications of recent ndings. In R. Yehuda & A. C.
McFarlane (Eds.), Psychobiology of posttraumatic stress
disorder (pp. 114124). New York Academy of Sciences.
Orr, S. P., Metzger, L. J., & Pitman, R. K. (2002).
Psychophysiology of post-traumatic stress disorder.
Psychiatric Clinics of North America,25(2), 271293.
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0193-953x(01)00007-7
Orr, S. P., & Roth, W. T. (2000). Psychophysiological assess-
ment: Clinical applications for PTSD. Journal of Aective
Disorders,61(3), 225240. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-
0327(00)00340-2
Pineles, S. L., & Orr, S. P. (2018). The psychophysiology
ofPTSD.InC.B.Nemero&C.R.Marmar(Eds.),
Post-traumatic stress disorder (pp. 393408). Oxford
University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/med/
9780190259440.003.0022
Pole, N. (2007, Sep). The psychophysiology of posttraumatic
stress disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,
133(5), 725746. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.
5.725
Pole, N., Cumberbatch, E., Taylor, W. M., Metzler, T. J.,
Marmar, C. R., & Neylan, T. C. (2006). Comparisons
between high and low peritraumatic dissociators in cardi-
ovascular and emotional activity while remembering
trauma. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation,6(4), 5167.
https://doi.org/10.1300/J229v06n04_04
Porges, S. W. (2003, Dec). Social engagement and attach-
ment: A phylogenetic perspective. Annals of the
New York Academy of Sciences,1008(1), 3147. https://
doi.org/10.1196/annals.1301.004
Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological
Psychology,74(2), 116143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
biopsycho.2006.06.009
Porges, S. W. (2011). The polyvagal theory:
Neurophysiological foundations of emotions, attachment,
communication, and self-regulation. W W Norton & Co.
Powers, A., Mekawi, Y., Fickenwirth, M., Nugent, N. R.,
Dixon, H. D., Minton, S., Kim, Y. J., Gluck, R., Carter,
S., Fani, N., Schwartz, A. C., Bradley, B., Umpierrez, G.
E., Pace, T. W. W., Jovanovic, T., Michopoulos, V., &
Gillespie, C. F. (2021, Oct). Emotion dysregulation and
dissociation contribute to decreased heart rate variability
to an acute psychosocial stressor in trauma-exposed Black
women. Journal of Psychiatric Research,142, 125131.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.07.032
Rabellino, D., Densmore, M., Theberge, J., McKinnon, M.
C., & Lanius, R. A. (2018). The cerebellum after trauma:
Resting-state functional connectivity of the cerebellum in
posttraumatic stress disorder and its dissociative subtype.
Human Brain Mapping,39(8), 33543374. https://doi.
org/10.1002/hbm.24081
Raq, S., Campodonico, C., & Varese, F. (2018). The
relationship between childhood adversities and dis-
sociation in severe mental illness: A meta-analytic review.
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,138(6), 509525. https://
doi.org/10.1111/acps.12969
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTRAUMATOLOGY 23
Rhudy, J. L., Davis, J. L., Williams, A. E., McCabe, K. M., &
Byrd, P. M. (2008). Physiologicalemotional reactivity to
nightmare-related imagery in trauma-exposed persons
with chronic nightmares. Behavioral Sleep Medicine,6(3),
158177. https://doi.org/10.1080/15402000802162539
Roelofs, K. (2017). Freeze for action: Neurobiological mech-
anisms in animal and human freezing. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
372(1718), 110. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0206
Roydeva, M. I., & Reinders, A. A. T. S. (2021). Biomarkers of
pathological dissociation: A systematic review.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,123, 120202.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.11.019
Sack, M., Cillien, M., & Hopper, J. W. (2012). Acute dis-
sociation and cardiac reactivity to script-driven imagery
in trauma-related disorders. European Journal of
Psychotraumatology,3(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.3402/
ejpt.v3i0.17419
Schalinski, I., Elbert, T. R., & Schauer, M. (2013). Cardiac
defense in response to imminent threat in women with
multiple trauma and severe PTSD. Psychophysiology,50
(7), 691700. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12051
Schalinski, I., Schauer, M., & Elbert, T. (2015). The shut-
down dissociation scale (shut-d). European Journal of
Psychotraumatology,6(1), 113. https://doi.org/10.3402/
ejpt.v6.25652
Schauer, M., & Elbert, T. (2010). Dissociation following
traumatic stress. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of
Psychology,218(2), 109127. https://doi.org/10.1027/
0044-3409/a000018
Schäein, E., Sattel, H., Schmidt, U., & Sack, M. (2018). The
enemy in the mirror: Self-perception-induced stress
results in dissociation of psychological and physiological
responses in patients with dissociative disorder. European
Journal of Psychotraumatology,9(sup3), 116. https://doi.
org/10.1080/20008198.2018.1472991
Schmahl, C. G., Elzinga, B. M., Ebner, U. W., Simms, T.,
Sanislow, C., Vermetten, E., McGlashan, T. H., &
Douglas Bremner, J. (2004). Psychophysiological reactiv-
ity to traumatic and abandonment scripts in borderline
personality and posttraumatic stress disorders: A prelimi-
nary report. Psychiatry Research,126(1), 3342. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2004.01.005
Schneider, M., & Schwerdtfeger, A. (2020, Sep). Autonomic
dysfunction in posttraumatic stress disorder indexed by
heart rate variability: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Medicine,50(12), 19371948. https://doi.org/10.1017/
S003329172000207X
Seligowski, A. V., Lebois, L. A. M., Hill, S. B., Kahhale, I.,
Wol, J. D., Jovanovic, T., Winternitz, S. R., Kaufman,
M. L., & Ressler, K. J. (2019). Autonomic responses to
fear conditioning among women with PTSD and dis-
sociation. Depression and Anxiety,36(7), 625634.
https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22903
Siegel, D. J. (1999). The developing mind: Toward a neuro-
biology of interpersonal experience. Guilford Press.
Simeon, D., Yehuda, R., Knutelska, M., & Schmeidler, J.
(2008). Dissociation versus posttraumatic stress:
Cortisol and physiological correlates in adults highly
exposed to the World Trade Center attack on 9/11.
Psychiatry Research,161(3), 325329. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.psychres.2008.04.021
Sledjeski, E. M., & Delahanty, D. L. (2012). Prior peritrau-
matic dissociative experiences aect autonomic reactivity
during trauma recall. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation,
13(1), 3250. https://doi.org/10.1080/15299732.2011.
608628
Spiegel, D., Loewenstein, R. J., Lewis-Fernandez, R., Sar, V.,
Simeon, D., Vermetten, E., Cardena, E., Brown, R. J., &
Dell, P. F. (2011, Dec 21). Dissociative disorders in
DSM-5. Depression and Anxiety,28(12), E17E45.
https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20923
Stein, D. J., Koenen, K. C., Friedman, M. J., Hill, E.,
McLaughlin, K. A., Petukhova, M., Ruscio, A. M.,
Shahly, V., Spiegel, D., Borges, G., Bunting, B., Caldas-
de-Almeida, J. M., de Girolamo, G., Demyttenaere, K.,
Florescu, S., Haro, J. M., Karam, E. G., Kovess-Masfety,
V., Lee, S., Kessler, R. C. (2013). Dissociation in post-
traumatic stress disorder: Evidence from the world men-
tal health surveys. Biological Psychiatry,73(4), 302312.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.08.022
Stewart, L. A., Clarke, M., Rovers, M., Riley, R. D.,
Simmonds, M., Stewart, G., Tierney, J. F., & Group, P.-
I. D. (2015, Apr 28). Preferred reporting items for sys-
tematic review and meta-analyses of individual partici-
pant data: The PRISMA-IPD statement. JAMA,313(16),
16571665. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2015.3656
Stiglmayr, C., Braakmann, D., Haaf, B., Stieglitz, R. D., &
Bohus, M. (2003, Jul). Development and characteristics
of dissociation-tension-scale acute (DSS-Akute). PPmP -
Psychotherapie · Psychosomatik · Medizinische
Psychologie,53(7), 287294. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-
2003-40495 (Entwicklung und psychometrische
Charakteristika der Dissoziations-Spannungs-Skala akut
(DSS-akut).)
Stiglmayr, C., Schimke, P., Wagner, T., Braakmann, D.,
Schweiger, U., Sipos, V., Fydrich, T., Schmahl, C., Ebner-
Priemer, U., Kleindienst, N., Bischkopf, J., Auckenthaler,
A., & Kienast, T. (2010, May). Development and psycho-
metric characteristics of the dissociation tension scale.
Journal of Personality Assessment,92(3), 269277. https://
doi.org/10.1080/00223891003670232
Stiglmayr, C., Schmahl, C., Bremner, J. D., Bohus, M., &
Ebner-Priemer, U. (2009). Development and psycho-
metric characteristics of the DSS-4 as a short instrument
to assess dissociative experience during neuropsychologi-
cal experiments. Psychopathology,42(6), 370374. https://
doi.org/10.1159/000236908
Strigo,I.A.,Simmons,A.N.,Matthews,S.C.,Grimes,E.
M.,Allard,C.B.,Reinhardt,L.E.,Paulus,M.P.,&
Stein, M. B. (2010). Neural correlates of altered pain
response in women with posttraumatic stress disorder
from intimate partner violence. Biological Psychiatry,
68(5), 442450. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.
2010.03.034
Terpou, B. A., Harricharan, S., McKinnon, M. C., Frewen,
P., Jetly, R., & Lanius, R. A. (2019). The eects of trauma
on brain and body: A unifying role for the midbrain peri-
aqueductal gray. Journal of Neuroscience Research,97(9),
11101140. https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.24447
Thornton, A., & Lee, P. (2000). Publication bias in meta-
analysis its causes and consequences. Journal of Clinical
Epidemiology,53(2), 207216. https://doi.org/10.1016/
s0895-4356(99)00161-4
van der Kolk, B. A. (1988). The trauma spectrum: The inter-
action of biological and social events in the genesis of the
trauma response. Journal of Traumatic Stress,1(3), 273
290. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.2490010302
Vermes,J.S.,Ayres,R.,Goes,A.S.,Real,N.D.,Araujo,A.C.,
Schiller,D.,Neto,F.L.,&Corchs,F.(2020, Nov 1).
Targeting the reconsolidation of traumatic memories with
a brief 2-session imaginal exposure intervention in post-
traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Aective Disorders,
276, 487494. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.06.052
24 S. BEUTLER ET AL.
Volchan, E., Rocha-Rego, V., Bastos, A. F., Oliveira, J. M.,
Franklin, C., Gleiser, S., Berger, W., Souza, G. G. L.,
Oliveira, L., David, I. A., Erthal, F. S., Pereira, M. G., &
Figueira, I. (2017). Immobility reactions under threat: A
contribution to human defensive cascade and PTSD.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,76(Pt A), 2938.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.025
Volchan, E., Souza, G. G., Franklin, C. M., Norte, C. E.,
Rocha-Rego, V., Oliveira, J. M., David, I. A.,
Mendlowicz, M. V., Coutinho, E. S., Fiszman, A.,
Berger, W., Marques-Portella, C., & Figueira, I. (2011).
Is there tonic immobility in humans? Biological evidence
from victims of traumatic stress. Biological Psychology,88
(1), 1319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.06.
002
Vonderlin, R., Kleindienst, N., Alpers, G. W., Bohus, M.,
Lyssenko, L., & Schmahl, C. (2018). Dissociation in vic-
tims of childhood abuse or neglect: A meta-analytic
review. Psychological Medicine,48(15), 24672476.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718000740
EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOTRAUMATOLOGY 25
... On the physiological level, studies in healthy individuals support hypothesized positive associations between dissociation and autonomic arousal at low dissociation levels (Danböck et al., 2021;Danböck, Franke, et al., 2023). However, two recent reviews of research in individuals with PTSD have failed to provide robust evidence for the postulated inverse association at high dissociation levels (Beutler et al., 2022;Roydeva & Reinders, 2021). Specifically, only few underpowered (N ≤ 22) studies investigated autonomic arousal during acute dissociation in individuals with PTSD (Beutler et al., 2022). ...
... However, two recent reviews of research in individuals with PTSD have failed to provide robust evidence for the postulated inverse association at high dissociation levels (Beutler et al., 2022;Roydeva & Reinders, 2021). Specifically, only few underpowered (N ≤ 22) studies investigated autonomic arousal during acute dissociation in individuals with PTSD (Beutler et al., 2022). Consequently, most studies yielded null findings on associations between acute dissociation and (a) heart rate (HR; Bichescu-Burian et al., 2017;Chou et al., 2018;Schäflein et al., 2018;Schmahl et al., 2004), a measure of overall psychophysiological arousal driven by sympathetic (increase) and parasympathetic (decrease) branches of the autonomic nervous system, (b) skin conductance (Bichescu-Burian et al., 2017;Schmahl et al., 2004;Vermes et al., 2020), a measure of sympathetic activation, and (c) high-frequency heart rate variability (HF-HRV; Chou et al., 2018; but see Krause-Utz et al., 2019 for a positive association), a measure of parasympathetic activity. ...
Article
Full-text available
Dissociative symptoms, such as depersonalization and derealization, are experienced by about half of individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Theoretical models propose that acute dissociation is accompanied by specific behavioral, physiological, and experiential alterations and contributes to unfavorable PTSD symptom course. Yet, empirical evidence is scarce. Here, we explored associations between dissociative and behavioral, physiological, and experiential threat responses as well as effects of dissociative responding on PTSD symptom course. Individuals with PTSD (N = 71) participated in a preregistered script-driven imagery study including exposure to standardized, detail-enriched trauma, and neutral scripts. Stabilometry, eye-tracking, facial electromyography, autonomic psychophysiology, and self-report data were collected. Moreover, PTSD symptoms were assessed before and 3 months after testing. Analyses did not link acute dissociation to bodily and facial immobility or staring in response to trauma scripts. However, dissociation displayed an inverted U-shaped relationship with heart rate and was linked to higher nonspecific skin conductance fluctuation and higher high-frequency heart rate variability in response to trauma scripts. Moreover, acute dissociation was linked to higher self-reported negative affect responses to trauma scripts and displayed a U-shaped relationship with unfavorable PTSD symptom course. While results did not confirm hypothesized behavioral markers of dissociation, they do support defense-cascade model assumptions of an inverted U-shaped relationship between dissociation and psychophysiological arousal resulting from a progression of parasympathetic versus sympathetic dominance with increasing dissociation. On an experiential level, results did not confirm posttraumatic dissociation-induced emotional numbing, questioning theoretical notions. The observed nonlinear associations may help explain the heterogeneity of prior findings and might inform an updated conceptualization of posttraumatic dissociation.
... Empirical studies partly link D-PTSD to specific risk factors, neurobiological alterations, elevated symptom severity, comorbidity, chronicity and functional impairment as well as differential treatment outcomes (for reviews see Atchley & Bedford, 2021;Beutler et al., 2022;Lanius et al., 2010;Roydeva & Reinders, 2021;Schiavone et al., 2018). However, results are still inconclusive, which might not least be due to studies using different measures for the dissociative symptoms of D-PTSD. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The fifth version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) introduced the dissociative subtype of posttraumatic stress disorder (D-PTSD). To assess this subtype, the Dissociative Subtype of PTSD Scale (DSPS), a 15-item self-report measure to identify lifetime and current dissociative symptoms of D-PTSD, was developed. However, so far, the scale has only been validated in war veterans. Moreover, criterion validity and diagnostic utility have not been examined yet.Objective: We aimed to validate the DSPS in two samples of civilian trauma-exposed German-speaking participants.Methods: In Study 1, a pre-registered online study, participants with and without PTSD symptoms (N = 558) answered questionnaires about traumatic experiences, dissociation, PTSD, depression, generalized anxiety disorder, somatic symptom disorder, alcohol use disorder, absorption, and dissociative responding to trauma-related questionnaires. In Study 2, which used secondary data of a pre-registered clinical study, participants with a PTSD diagnosis (N = 71) answered questionnaires about traumatic experiences, dissociation, PTSD, depression, generalized anxiety disorder, somatic symptom disorder, and dissociative responding to standardized trauma exposure. Moreover, PTSD, D-PTSD, and other diagnoses were assessed with structured clinical interviews.Results: Analyses confirmed a three-factor structure as well as high internal consistency, and high convergent, discriminant, and criterion validity of the DSPS. Moreover, the scale was able to identify a latent D-PTSD group and individuals with D-PTSD diagnosis.Conclusions: The DSPS constitutes a reliable and valid tool to assess D-PTSD symptoms in clinical practice and research and thereby may contribute to a better understanding of these debilitating symptoms.
... Beutler et ai. revisou o conhecimento sobre a relação entre a dissociação relacionada ao trauma e o sistema nervoso autônomo (Beutler et al., 2022). Trauma e dissociação também foram discutidos por van der Hart (van der Hart, 2021). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Ayahuasca é uma bebida vegetal de origem indígena amazônica. Possui efeitos psicodélicos, antiinflamatórios, neu-roprotetores, citotóxicos e antiparasitários, principalmente devidos aos inibidores da monoamina oxidase (IMAO) e N,N-dimetiltriptamina (DMT). Este estudo de caso retrospectivo descreve o caso de uma mulher de trinta e tantos anos com trauma complexo devido a abuso sexual grave durante anos na primeira infância, resultando em uma condição crônica de décadas envolvendo tendências suicidas. Ela foi diagnosticada com transtorno bipolar e transtorno de personalidade limítrofe, mas se recusou a aceitar qualquer um deles. Ela apresentava delírios para-sitários e profunda dissociação. Apesar de ser gravemente psicótica em particular, ela parecia funcionar bem em público, escondendo a maioria de seus sintomas. Com trinta e poucos anos, ela participou de uma cerimônia de ayahuasca em um ambiente legal. Resolveu sua tendência suicida, eliminou seu isolamento social e reduziu sua vergonha relacionada ao trauma inicial. Mais nove cerimônias aliviaram ainda mais sua angústia. Seu agressor também participou de uma cerimônia de ayahuasca e confirmou suas memórias de abuso na infância. A primeira entrevista foi realizada 1,5 anos após sua primeira cerimônia e uma entrevista de acompanhamento 2,5 anos depois. Ela passou por dezesseis cerimônias adicionais, reconheceu a validade de seu diagnóstico de transtorno bipolar e acreditou que seu trauma precoce era aúnica causa. Seu trauma central permaneceu parcialmente sem resolução, mas seus sintomas dissociativos continuaram a diminuir. Ela observou vários outros casos de psicose e transtorno bipolar nos quais a ayahuasca resultou em efeitos positivos. Este estudo de caso contribui para uma melhor compreensão do uso da ayahuasca no transtorno bipolar e no traumatismo grave. Também revisa o estado da arte atual no tratamento do transtorno bipolar usando ayahuasca em baixas doses e um caso em que o transtorno bipolar foi resolvido com LSD.
... In addition, laboratory studies have shown increased dissociation during or shortly after exposure to various stressors such as personalized stressful narratives [15], arousal induced by the hyperventilation provocation test [53], panic induced by carbon dioxide inhalation [62], psychosocial stress [23,48,83], and trauma reminders ( [16,88], see [39,45] for reviews). Changes in physiological parameters that serve as markers for autonomous nervous system activity during dissociation have also been investigated, but current evidence is mixed (see [7,63] for reviews). For example, laboratory studies find increases, decreases, or no changes in cardiovascular measures during dissociation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Dissociation is a ubiquitous clinical phenomenon. Dissociative disorders (DD) are primarily characterized by dissociation, and dissociative states are also a criterion for borderline personality disorder (BPD) and the dissociative subtype of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Dissociative reactions (e.g., depersonalization/derealization or gaps in awareness/memory) across diagnostic categories are believed to be affect contingent and theorized to serve affect regulation functions. What is not clear, however, is how self-reported affect and physiological reactivity unfold within dissociative episodes. To address this issue, the present project aims to investigate the hypothesis (1) whether self-reported distress (as indicated by arousal, e.g., feeling tense/agitated, and/or valence, e.g., feeling discontent/unwell) and physiological reactivity increase before dissociative episodes and (2) whether self-reported distress and physiological reactivity decrease during and after dissociative episodes in a transdiagnostic sample of patients with DD, BPD, and/or PTSD. Methods We will use a smartphone application to assess affect and dissociation 12 times per day over the course of one week in everyday life. During this time, heart and respiratory rates will be remotely monitored. Afterwards, participants will report affect and dissociative states eight times in the laboratory before, during, and after the Trier Social Stress Test. During the laboratory task, we will continuously record heart rate, electrodermal activity, and respiratory rate, as well as measure blood pressure and take salivary samples to determine cortisol levels. Our hypotheses will be tested using multilevel structural equation models. Power analyses determined a sample size of 85. Discussion The project will test key predictions of a transdiagnostic model of dissociation based on the idea that dissociative reactions are affect contingent and serve affect regulation functions. This project will not include non-clinical control participants. In addition, the assessment of dissociation is limited to pathological phenomena.
... Theoretically, TI and PD are two separate constructs, reflected in intact or even enhanced cognition for TI (e.g., heightened awareness) versus impairment for PD (deMello et al., 2022;Gallup et al., 1980;Hagenaars & Hagenaars, 2020). TI and PD may be interrelated peritraumatic responses; however, it is unclear whether current research findings on peritraumatic responses are sufficient to conclusively separate or combine the two constructs (Abrams et al., 2012;Beutler et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
BACKGROUND: Tonic immobility (TI) is a reflexive, involuntary response that causes motor inhibition, vocal suppression, and analgesia. TI is elicited by extreme fear and perception of entrapment in a life-threatening situation. Research suggests that TI is a frequent peritraumatic response and may be related to subsequent posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). However, findings are mixed and, as of yet, no systematic or meta-analytic review examining associations between TI and PTSD has been published. OBJECTIVE: We systematically and meta-analytically reviewed the literature and investigated whether TI is associated with the development, severity, and course of PTSD. Additionally, we evaluated whether different types of traumatic events are differentially associated with TI, and whether TI severity differs according to sex. METHODS: A systematic literature search was conducted using Embase, PubMed, PsycINFO, and Scopus. Meta-analyses were performed on the included articles. RESULTS: We identified 27 eligible articles. We found a significant association between TI and PTSD symptom severity (r=0.39, 95% CI: 0.34 to 0.44; p<.0001). TI was more severe among females (Cohen’s d=0.37, 95% CI: 0.25 to 0.48; p<.0001) and was more often elicited in situations involving interpersonal violence. We found limited longitudinal data to perform a meta-analysis of the association between TI and the development and/or course of PTSD. However, the literature available seems to support the role of TI in both the development and course of PTSD. CONCLUSIONS: Peritraumatic TI is associated with PTSD symptom severity, occurs more often during interpersonal violence, and is more severe among females. More longitudinal research is needed to investigate the role of TI in psychopathology development and course.
... In addition, laboratory studies have shown increased dissociation during or shortly after exposure to various stressors such as personalized stressful narratives (Chung et al., 2020), arousal induced by the hyperventilation provocation test (Nixon & Bryant, 2006), panic induced by carbon dioxide inhalation (Rassovsky & Kushner, 2003), psychosocial stress (Graumann et al., 2023;Metz et al., 2020;Wingenfeld et al., 2018), and trauma reminders (Zoellner et al., 2007;see Krause-Utz et al., 2021;Lynn et al., 2019 for reviews). Changes in physiological parameters that serve as markers for autonomous nervous system activity during dissociation have also been investigated, but current evidence is mixed (see Beutler et al., 2022; Roydeva & Reinders, 2021 for reviews). For example, laboratory studies find increases, decreases, or no changes in cardiovascular measures during dissociation. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Dissociation is a ubiquitous clinical phenomenon. Dissociative disorders (DD) are primarily characterized by dissociation, and dissociative states are also a criterion for borderline personality disorder (BPD) and the dissociative subtype of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Dissociative reactions across diagnostic categories are believed to be affect contingent and theorized to serve affect regulation functions. What is not clear, however, is how self-reported affect and physiological reactivity unfold within dissociative episodes. To address this issue, the present project aims to investigate the hypothesis (1) whether self-reported distress (as indicated by arousal, e.g., feeling tense/agitated, and/or valence, e.g., feeling discontent/unwell) and physiological reactivity increase before dissociative episodes and (2) whether self-reported distress and physiological reactivity decrease during and after dissociative episodes in a transdiagnostic sample of patients with DD, BPD, and/or PTSD. Methods We will use a smartphone application to assess affect and dissociation 12 times per day over the course of one week in everyday life. During this time, heart and respiratory rates will be remotely monitored. Afterwards, participants will report affect and dissociative states eight times in the laboratory before, during, and after the Trier Social Stress Test. During the laboratory task, we will continuously record heart rate, blood pressure, electrodermal activity, and respiratory rate, and take salivary samples to determine cortisol levels. Our hypotheses will be tested using multilevel structural equation models. Power analyses determined a sample size of 85. Discussion The project will test key predictions of a transdiagnostic model of dissociation based on the idea that dissociative reactions are affect contingent and serve affect regulation functions. • This project will not include non-clinical control participants. In addition, the assessment of dissociation is limited to pathological phenomena.
... Further, in the only study of SCR in a sample of trauma-exposed individuals with both PTSD and dissociative symptoms, higher dissociation was related to lower SCR during a fear potentiated startle paradigm (Seligowski et al., 2019). However, a recent systematic review did not find a clear trend in the relation between trauma-related dissociation and ANS activation during a range of tasks (Beutler et al., 2022). Taken together, PTSD symptoms that are present, but remain unaccounted for, in conjunction with study-specific differences in tasks and procedures, may contribute to inconsistent findings regarding the association between dissociative symptoms and SCR. ...
Article
Background: Dissociative and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms are commonly co-occurring responses to psychological trauma. Yet, these two groups of symptoms appear to be related to diverging patterns of physiological response. To date, few studies have examined how specific dissociative symptoms, namely, depersonalization and derealization, relate to skin conductance response (SCR), a marker of autonomic function, within the context of PTSD symptoms. We examined associations among depersonalization, derealization, and SCR during two conditions - resting control and breath-focused mindfulness - in the context of current PTSD symptoms. Methods: Sixty-eight trauma-exposed women (82.4% Black; Mage = 42.5, SDage = 12.1) were recruited from the community for a breath-focused mindfulness study. SCR data were collected during alternating resting control and breath-focused mindfulness conditions. Moderation analyses were conducted to examine relations among dissociative symptoms, SCR, and PTSD for these different conditions. Results: Moderation analyses revealed that depersonalization was linked to lower SCR during resting control, B = 0.0005, SE = 0.0002, p = .006, in participants low-to-moderate PTSD symptoms; however, depersonalization was associated with higher SCR during breath-focused mindfulness, B = -0.0006, SE = 0.0003, p = .029, in individuals with similar levels of PTSD symptoms. No significant interaction between derealization and PTSD symptoms on SCR was observed. Conclusions: Depersonalization symptoms may associate with physiological withdrawal during rest, but greater physiological arousal during effortful emotion regulation in individuals with low-to moderate levels of PTSD, which has significant implications for barriers to treatment engagement as well as treatment selection in this population.
... Empirical studies partly link D-PTSD to specific risk factors, neurobiological alterations, elevated symptom severity, comorbidity, chronicity and functional impairment as well as differential treatment outcomes (for reviews see Atchley & Bedford, 2021;Beutler et al., 2022;Lanius et al., 2010;Roydeva & Reinders, 2021;Schiavone et al., 2018). However, results are still inconclusive, which might not least be due to studies using different measures for the dissociative symptoms of D-PTSD. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background: The fifth version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) introduced the dissociative subtype of posttraumatic stress disorder (D-PTSD). To assess this subtype, Wolf et al. (2017) developed the Dissociative Subtype of PTSD Scale (DSPS), a 15-item self-report measure to identify lifetime and current dissociative symptoms of D-PTSD. However, so far, the scale has only been validated in war veterans. Moreover, criterion validity and diagnostic utility have not been examined yet. Objective: We aimed to validate the DSPS in two samples of civilian trauma-exposed German-speaking participants. Methods: In Study 1, a pre-registered online study, participants with and without PTSD symptoms (N=558) answered questionnaires about traumatic experiences, dissociation, PTSD, depression, generalized anxiety disorder, somatic symptom disorder, alcohol use disorder, absorption, and dissociative responding to trauma-related questionnaires. In Study 2, which used secondary data of a pre-registered clinical study, participants with a PTSD diagnosis (N=71) answered questionnaires about traumatic experiences, dissociation, PTSD, depression, generalized anxiety disorder, somatic symptom disorder, and dissociative responding to standardized trauma exposure. Moreover, PTSD, D-PTSD, and other diagnoses were assessed with structured clinical interviews. Results: Analyses confirmed a three-factor structure as well as high internal consistency, and high convergent, discriminant, and criterion validity of the DSPS. Moreover, the scale was able to identify a latent D-PTSD group and individuals with D-PTSD diagnosis. Conclusions: The DSPS constitutes a reliable and valid tool to assess D-PTSD symptoms in clinical practice and research and thereby may contribute to a better understanding of these debilitating symptoms.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Bilateral stimulation is a core element of Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing Therapy, a psychotherapeutic intervention for the treatment of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). Promising previous findings showed measurable physiological effects of bilateral stimulation in healthy individuals, but studies that replicated these findings in PTSD patients are sparse. Methods 23 patients with PTSD and 30 healthy controls were confronted with affective standard scripts (pleasant, neutral, unpleasant) while bilateral tactile stimulation was applied. Monolateral and no stimulation served as control conditions. Noise-induced startle reflex response (valence measure) and galvanic skin response (arousal measure) were used for physiological responses and the valence and arousal scale of the Self-Assessment-Manikin for subjective responses. Results Both groups showed a subjective distress reduction for unpleasant scripts and a subjective attention increase for positive scripts under bilateral stimulation. In healthy individuals, this was also for physiological measures, and a general startle-reducing effect of bilateral stimulation in the absence of affective stimuli was found. In PTSD patients, however, the effects were restricted on the subjective level, and no concomitant physiological effects were observed. Conclusions and significance The findings indicate, that generalizing the effects of BLS in healthy individuals to PTSD patients may be problematic. The herein-reported group differences can be explained by PTSD-specific peculiarities in emotion processing and cognitive processing style.
Chapter
We often assume that the autonomic nervous system (ANS) sends genuine signals of emotions. Nonetheless, people can misunderstand autonomic cues (e.g., blinking, pallor, blushing, salivation, sweating). These responses are usually the outcome of multiple factors. The ANS tasks go beyond homeostasis: other tasks are predisposition to specific actions, delivery of signals for social purposes, and monitoring digestive processes. These functions are not entirely independent of central control. Indeed, several brain regions (e.g., the hypothalamus, the amygdaloid complex, and the insular cortex) and neurotransmitters (e.g., dopamine) can exert some influence on autonomic activity.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Current neurobiological models of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) assume excessive medial frontal activation and hypoactivation of cortico-limbic regions as neural markers of post-traumatic dissociation. Script-driven imagery is an established experimental paradigm that is used to study acute dissociative reactions during trauma exposure. However, there is a scarcity of experimental research investigating neural markers of dissociation; findings from existing script-driven neuroimaging studies are inconsistent and based on small sample sizes. Aims: The current aim was to identify the neural correlates of acute post-traumatic dissociation by employing the script-driven imagery paradigm in combination with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Method: Functional neuroimaging data was acquired in 51 female patients with PTSD with a history of interpersonal childhood trauma. Blood-oxygen-level-dependent response during the traumatic (versus neutral) autobiographical memory recall was analysed, and the derived activation clusters were correlated with dissociation measures. Results: During trauma recall, enhanced activation in the cerebellum, occipital gyri, supramarginal gyrus and amygdala was identified. None of the derived clusters correlated significantly with dissociative symptoms, although patients reported increased levels of acute dissociation following the paradigm. Conclusions: The present study is one of the largest functional magnetic resonance imaging investigations of dissociative neural biomarkers in patients with PTSD undergoing experimentally induced trauma confrontation to elicit symptom-specific brain reactivity. In light of the current reproducibility crisis prominent in neuroimaging research owing to costly and time-consuming data acquisition, the current (null) findings highlight the difficulty of extracting reliable neurobiological biomarkers for complex subjective experiences such as dissociation.
Article
Full-text available
This research report describes the development of a virtual reality (VR) laboratory stressor to study the effects of exposure to stressful events. The aim of the research was to develop a VR simulation that would evoke stressor responses at a level that was tolerable for participants. Veterans with and without warzone-related posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) were presented with VR simulations of combat stressors. There was one complaint of feeling hot during simulations but no incidents of simulator sickness. Participants denied experiencing the simulations as overly distressing, and there were no reports of any distress or problems related to study participation when they were contacted two weeks after the VR challenge. Simulations elicited moderate levels of anxiety and mild levels of dissociation that were significantly greater in Veterans with PTSD. Simulations were less successful in eliciting differential heart rate reactivity and stress hormone secretion, though history of civilian trauma exposure was associated with elevated heart rates during the second simulation. The study demonstrated that the VR paradigm was feasible and tolerable and that it holds promise as a new method with which to conduct controlled laboratory research on the effects of exposure to stressful events.
Article
Full-text available
Background Changes in autonomic nervous system (ANS) function have been observed in a variety of psychological disorders, including posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Analysis of heart rate variability (HRV) provides insight into the functioning of the ANS. Previous research on PTSD found lower HRV in PTSD patients compared to controls, indicating altered sympathetic and parasympathetic activity, but findings are inconsistent. The purpose of this meta-analysis was to examine differences in HRV indices between individuals with PTSD and healthy controls at baseline and during stress. Methods The included primary studies present an aggregate of studies analyzing different HRV indices. Examined HRV indices were standard deviation of the normalized NN-intervals (SDNN), root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD), low-frequency (LF) and high-frequency (HF) spectral components, LF/HF ratio, and heart rate (HR). Moderating effects of study design, HRV and PTSD assessment, and sample characteristics were examined via subgroup-analyses and meta-regressions. Results Random-effects meta-analyses for HRV parameters at rest revealed significant group differences for RMSSD and HF-HRV, suggesting lower parasympathetic activity in PTSD. The aggregated effect size for SDNN was medium, suggesting diminished total variability in PTSD. A small effect was found for LF-HRV. A higher LF/HF ratio was found in the PTSD sample as compared to controls. Individuals with PTSD showed significantly higher HR. During stress, individuals with PTSD showed higher HR and lower HF-HRV, both indicated by small effect sizes. Conclusions Findings suggest that PTSD is associated with ANS dysfunction.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction There is currently no general agreement on how to best conceptualize dissociative symptoms and whether they share similar neural underpinnings across dissociative disorders. Neuroimaging data could help elucidate these questions. Objectives The objective of this review is to summarize empirical evidence for neural aberrations observed in patients suffering from dissociative symptoms. Methods A systematic literature review was conducted including patient cohorts diagnosed with primary dissociative disorders, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), or borderline personality disorder. Results Results from MRI studies reporting structural (gray matter and white matter) and functional (during resting-state and task-related activation) brain aberrations were extracted and integrated. In total, 33 articles were included of which 10 pertained to voxel-based morphology, 2 to diffusion tensor imaging, 10 to resting-state fMRI, and 11 to task-related fMRI. Overall findings indicated aberrations spread across diverse brain regions, especially in the temporal and frontal cortices. Patients with dissociative identity disorder and with dissociative PTSD showed more overlap in brain activation than each group showed with depersonalization/derealization disorder. Conclusion In conjunction, the results indicate that dissociative processing cannot be localized to a few distinctive brain regions but rather corresponds to differential neural signatures depending on the symptom constellation.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: The functions of both the central and peripheral autonomic nervous system, indexed by heart rate variability (HRV), are affected by psychology and physiology. This review summarizes the results of studies comparing the evaluation of HRV parameters between individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and healthy controls. Methods: Eligible studies (n=499) were identified through literature searches of the EMBASE, Medline, PubMed and Web of Science databases. Nineteen studies met our inclusion criteria. A random effects model was used, and standardized mean differences for highfrequency HRV(HF-HRV), low-frequency HRV(LF-HRV) and the root mean square of successive R-R interval differences (RMSSD) were calculated. Results: Significant effects were found for HF-HRV [p<0.0001, Z=4.18; Hedges'g=-1.58, 95% confidence interval (CI) (-2.32, -0.84); k=14] and RMSSD [p<0.00001, Z=4.80; Hedges'g=-1.96, 95% CI (-2.76, -1.16); k=9] relative to healthy controls. Considerable heterogeneity was revealed, but the main effects for HF-HRV and RMSSD were robust in subsequent meta-regression and subgroup analyses. Conclusion: Given the relationships among low vagal state, inflammation, and alterations in brain structure and function, including executive function and emotion regulation, reduced HRV may be regarded as an endophenotype in PTSD research.
Article
‘IT IS NOT RATIONALITY. NOT EMOTION. NOT IMAGINATION, OR CREATIVITY, OR EMPATHY. YOUR BRAIN’S MOST IMPORTANT JOB IS TO CONTROL YOUR BODY…’ (P.10)
Article
Reduced heart rate variability (HRV) in response to stress is a biomarker of emotion dysregulation (ED) and is related to posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), yet less is known about its role with dissociation in trauma-exposed adults. The goals of the current study were to examine unique patterns of associations between ED, dissociation, and PTSD with HRV at 15, 30, and 45 min (T1, T2, T3) following an acute psychosocial stressor task in a sample of 49 trauma-exposed, urban-dwelling Black women. Associations with baseline psychophysiology measures were also examined. ED and dissociation were assessed using self-report; PTSD was determined using a semi-structured interview. Heart rate (HR) and HRV, indexed with low frequency/high frequency (LF/HF) ratio and respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), were measured with electrocardiogram recordings. ED and dissociation were positively correlated with LF/HF ratio at T3 (p < .05). There were no significant differences between individuals with PTSD versus those without PTSD in HR or HRV following acute stressor; PTSD diagnosis was related to higher HR at baseline. Latent growth modeling revealed that ED was associated with higher LF/HF ratio directly following acute stressor, while dissociation was associated with increase in LF/HF ratio over time. These findings demonstrate that ED is related to higher sympathetic reactivity for a prolonged period of time following stress exposure, while dissociation shows a delayed association with LF/HF ratio, suggesting a distinct impaired parasympathetic activation pattern exists for dissociation.
Article
Pathological dissociation is a severe, debilitating and transdiagnostic psychiatric symptom. This review identifies biomarkers of pathological dissociation in a transdiagnostic manner to recommend the most promising research and treatment pathways in support of the precision medicine framework. A total of 205 unique studies that met inclusion criteria were included. Studies were divided into four biomarker categories, namely neuroimaging, psychobiological, psychophysiological, and genetic biomarkers. The dorsomedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, bilateral superior frontal regions, (anterior) cingulate, posterior association areas, and basal ganglia were identified as neurofunctional biomarkers of pathological dissociation and decreased hippocampal, basal ganglia, and thalamic volumes as neurostructural biomarkers. Increased oxytocin and prolactin and decreased tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-α) were identified as psychobiological markers. Psychophysiological biomarkers, including blood pressure, heart rate and skin conductance, were inconclusive. For the genetic biomarker category studies related to dissociation were limited and no clear directionality of effect was found to warrant identification of a genetic biomarker. Recommendations for future research pathways and possible clinical applicability are provided.
Article
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is characterized by alterations in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and sympathetic nervous system (SNS). There is evidence for a blunted HPA axis reactivity to psychosocial stress. Less is known about how the SNS reacts to psychosocial stress. Here, we compared the HPA axis and SNS responses to psychosocial stress and a non-stressful condition in patients with PTSD and in healthy individuals. Twenty-one women with PTSD and 32 healthy women participated in the Trier social stress test (TSST) and placebo TSST (P-TSST). We measured salivary cortisol, alpha amylase activity and blood pressure before and after the tests. Subjective perceived stress response was also assessed. We found a blunted cortisol response to the TSST in patients with PTSD compared with healthy participants 10 minutes (t(51) = -2.58, p = .01) and 25 minutes (t(51) = -2.16, p = .04) after TSST. We found no evidence for an increased SNS reactivity after psychosocial stress in patients with PTSD (all p > .05). Patients with PTSD, but not healthy participants, reported more dissociative symptoms (t(20) = -2.31, p = .03) and being more tired (t(20) = 2.90, p = .01) directly after TSST compared with the placebo condition. Our results suggest a blunted HPA stress reactivity and an increased subjective perceived stress response in female patients with PTSD. Longitudinal studies could test if these altered stress responses constitute a predisposition to or a cause of PTSD. Future studies should investigate whether these results are transferable to men.
Article
Background Evidence suggests that extinction during memory reconsolidation diminishes the return of defensive responses. In order to translate these effects to the clinical setting, we tested whether retrieving a traumatic memory and delivering a brief two-sessions imaginal exposure intervention during its reconsolidation would produce stronger decreases in reactivity to these memories than standard imaginal exposure method. Methods Participants with Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) had either their traumatic (n = 21) or a neutral (n = 21) memory retrieved 1 hour before an imaginal exposure session for two consecutive days. One day before and one day after, participants were exposed to script-driven imagery of their traumatic event, during which skin conductance responses were measured and, immediately after, subjective responses were assessed by means of Visual Analogue Scales. Results Traumatic retrieval improved the physiological, but not the subjective effects of imaginal exposure intervention on over-reactivity to traumatic memories. Conclusions Our results suggest that delivering extinction-based treatments over the reconsolidation of traumatic memories may enhance its effects. These results suggest that this is a promising path toward the development of new therapeutic techniques.