ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Continuum beliefs of mental illness: a systematic review of measures

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Purpose The continuum of mental health/illness has been subject to scientific debate for decades. While current research indicates that continuum belief interventions can reduce mental health stigma and improve treatment seeking in affected populations, no study has yet systematically examined measures of continuum beliefs. Methods This preregistered systematic review summarizes measures of continuum beliefs. Following the PRISMA statement, three scientific databases (PubMed, PsycInfo and PsycArticles via EBSCOhost, Web of Science) are searched, instruments are described and discussed regarding their scope, and methodological quality. Results Overall, 7351 records were identified, with 35 studies reporting relevant findings on 11 measures. Most studies examined general population samples and used vignette-based measures. Schizophrenia and depression were most commonly examined, few studies focused on dementia, ADHD, OCD, eating disorders, and problematic alcohol use, or compared continuum beliefs across disorders. Validity was very good for most measures, but reliability was rarely tested. Measures mostly assessed beliefs in the normality of mental health symptoms or the normality of persons with such symptoms but rarely nosological aspects (i.e., categorical v continuous conceptualization of mental disorders). Conclusions Current research provides psychometrically sound instruments to examine continuum beliefs for a variety of mental disorders. While studies suggest utility for general population samples and mental health professionals, more research is necessary to corroborate findings, for instance, regarding age (e.g., in adolescents), gender, or type of mental disorder. Future research should also compare self-report ratings, and vignette-based measures, include measures of nosological concepts to fully grasp the continuum concept of mental illness. Preregistration PROSPERO: CRD42019123606.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Vol.:(0123456789)
1 3
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-022-02345-4
REVIEW
Continuum beliefs ofmental illness: asystematic review ofmeasures
S.Tomczyk1 · S.Schlick1· T.Gansler1· T.McLaren1 · H.Muehlan1 · L.‑J.Peter2 · G.Schomerus2,3 ·
S.Schmidt1
Received: 17 February 2022 / Accepted: 19 July 2022 / Published online: 5 August 2022
© The Author(s) 2022
Abstract
Purpose The continuum of mental health/illness has been subject to scientific debate for decades. While current research
indicates that continuum belief interventions can reduce mental health stigma and improve treatment seeking in affected
populations, no study has yet systematically examined measures of continuum beliefs.
Methods This preregistered systematic review summarizes measures of continuum beliefs. Following the PRISMA state-
ment, three scientific databases (PubMed, PsycInfo and PsycArticles via EBSCOhost, Web of Science) are searched, instru-
ments are described and discussed regarding their scope, and methodological quality.
Results Overall, 7351 records were identified, with 35 studies reporting relevant findings on 11 measures. Most studies
examined general population samples and used vignette-based measures. Schizophrenia and depression were most com-
monly examined, few studies focused on dementia, ADHD, OCD, eating disorders, and problematic alcohol use, or compared
continuum beliefs across disorders. Validity was very good for most measures, but reliability was rarely tested. Measures
mostly assessed beliefs in the normality of mental health symptoms or the normality of persons with such symptoms but
rarely nosological aspects (i.e., categorical v continuous conceptualization of mental disorders).
Conclusions Current research provides psychometrically sound instruments to examine continuum beliefs for a variety of
mental disorders. While studies suggest utility for general population samples and mental health professionals, more research
is necessary to corroborate findings, for instance, regarding age (e.g., in adolescents), gender, or type of mental disorder.
Future research should also compare self-report ratings, and vignette-based measures, include measures of nosological
concepts to fully grasp the continuum concept of mental illness.
Preregistration PROSPERO: CRD42019123606.
Keywords Mental health· Public health· Systematic review· Stereotyping· Continuum· Assessment
Introduction
The nosological concept of mental disorders has been subject
to long-standing discussions. To date, there is no undisputable
consensus on their categorical or dimensional nature, although
developments of the DSM 5 [1] as well as comprehensive
literature seem to favor continuous measures of psychopa-
thology which furthers a dimensional understanding [2, 3].
Schizophrenia, for example, is described along the prone-
ness–persistence–impairment continuum describing psychotic
and subsyndromal experiences among the general population
with only a small proportion reporting persistent symptoms
that may lead to an impairment [4, 5]. This concept has impli-
cations for prevention, diagnosis and treatment, as it informs
researchers, policymakers and practitioners alike. For example,
a continuum model of schizophrenia emphasizes the need for
selective prevention in at-risk groups [6], and identifies sub-
groups with persistent symptoms for personalized treatment
purposes. It also points to groups with subsyndromal experi-
ences as target groups for early prevention [7]. A categorical
understanding of schizophrenia, on the other hand, facilitates
stigmatizing attitudes, because it allows a clear distinction of
* S. Tomczyk
samuel.tomczyk@uni-greifswald.de
1 Department Health andPrevention, Institute ofPsychology,
University ofGreifswald, Robert-Blum-Straße 13,
17489Greifswald, Germany
2 Department ofPsychiatry andPsychotherapy, Medical
Faculty, Leipzig University, Leipzig, Germany
3 Department ofPsychiatry andPsychotherapy, University
ofLeipzig Medical Center, Leipzig, Germany
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
2 Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
social groups, that is people with and without schizophrenia
[8]. It should be noted, however, that other researchers criticize
such a continuum model from a methodological perspective
[9, 10]. Linscott and van Os [9], for example, point to meth-
odological flaws and challenges of the conception of continua
that might overshadow categorically derived findings, such as
latent classes. A similar debate between categorical and con-
tinuous conceptualizations can be seen for eating disorders
[1113], obsessive–compulsive disorder [14], generalized anx-
iety disorder [15], depression [16, 17], and at-risk substance
use [18, 19] or gambling [20]. This debate is not limited to
the scientific community but it also affects patients and the
public. Previous research shows that the public perception of
mental illness as a categorical construct is connected to public
stigma [21, 22] and mental health stigma is recognized as a
barrier to treatment seeking [2329]. It is also linked to nega-
tive psychosocial outcomes, for example, lower self-esteem
and self-efficacy and poor quality of life [3034]. Conversely,
a continuum model of mental illness is related to more positive
mental health outcomes [35], and lower stigmatizing attitudes.
Therefore, promoting continuum beliefs to the public might be
a promising approach to reducing public stigma [36].
In this manner, Angermeyer and Schulze [21] describe
two core strategies of public communication in line with
either categorical beliefs (i.e., medicalization) or continuum
beliefs (i.e., normalization). The first strategy encompasses
medical treatments of individuals with distinct disorders,
such as schizophrenia, and is more prominent among medi-
cal professionals and connected to biomedical causal beliefs
of mental disorders [3739]. The second strategy sees psy-
chiatric symptoms as a normal experience but connects men-
tal disorders to an increased level of stress and insufficient
coping resources. It is more prominent among non-medical
health care workers as well as support groups, and it is more
strongly connected to psychosocial causal beliefs [37, 38].
In spite of their potential for public mental health and social
psychiatry, for instance, by reducing stigmatizing attitudes
and thus lowering the barrier to entry into treatment no study
has systematically reviewed and summarized measures for
continuum beliefs regarding mental health and mental ill-
ness, which makes it difficult to assess their validity and
utility. For instance, an experienced-based measure might
be more valid for clinical samples but less applicable to gen-
eral population samples, whereas a vignette-based measure
might be more applicable but also more strongly affected
by bias (e.g., gender bias in case of gendered vignettes).
Therefore, this systematic review aims to review and assess
previously utilized measures for continuum beliefs to har-
monize research efforts and answer the following questions.
(1) What are the characteristics of existing continuum
belief instruments (e.g., country of origin, setting/target
group, examined disorders, mode of administration)?
(2) What are the psychometric properties of continuum
belief measures?
(3) Which areas of the continuum of mental health and
mental illness are covered by continuum belief meas-
ures?
Method
This systematic review was conducted in accordance with
the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and
Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) Statement [40] and is registered
with the PROSPERO registry (https:// www. crd. york. ac. uk/
prosp ero; CRD42019123606). Three scientific databases
(PubMed, PsycInfo and PsycArticles via EBSCOhost, Web
of Science) were searched for peer-reviewed articles on
continuum beliefs that were published before June 2022.
The search was performed in line with a review and meta-
analysis on the association between continuum beliefs and
mental health stigma [36]; therefore, initial database search
and abstract and title screening was similar in this study,
but eligibility criteria differed between studies. Search terms
comprised continuum AND stigma AND mental health OR
mental illness, search strategies are presented in Peter etal.
[36]. In addition, reference lists of included studies were
checked to identify additional eligible studies.
Eligibility criteria
Eligibility criteria were described in accordance with the
PICO process [41]:
Population: Human beings from the general population
without any age restrictions.
Intervention: Studies that investigate continuum beliefs
were included, either as observational or interventional stud-
ies. Continuum beliefs refer to the nosological concept of
mental illness, either as a general, transdiagnostic concept
of continuity of mental illness/mental health problems or as
a specific concept for distinct mental disorders. Other forms
of continua, such as the continuum of care [42] or the dual-
continua model of mental health and mental illness [4345],
were not included, because they represent broader concepts
within psychiatric and psychological research regarding
health care structures as well as psychological functioning,
which transcend the current research question that focuses
on the conceptualization of mental disorders.
Comparison: Experimental as well as observational quan-
titative studies were included; therefore, there was no restric-
tion regarding a potential control group.
Outcome: Studies should measure continuum beliefs,
either as a predictor, an intermediary variable, or as an
outcome.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
3Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
Studies were not limited to a particular design (e.g.,
experimental studies or observational cohort studies) or
method (e.g., quantitative data assessment). Finally, the
search was limited to studies published in English, German,
French, or Polish. Titles and abstracts of identified studies
were screened by the first and second author and full texts
were obtained of potentially relevant studies. Full texts were
then screened against eligibility criteria independently by
the first and second author. Differences were discussed with
the third author and solved by mutual agreement to include
or exclude studies.
Data extraction, synthesis, andanalysis
The first and second author independently extracted data on
authors, date of publication, study design, sample, meas-
ures and psychometric properties (if reported in the original
studies). The first and third author then independently rated
dimensions of methodological quality and psychometric
properties of the measures following the reporting guide-
lines proposed by Bennett etal. [46] to compare measures.
The following dimensions were examined: readability (avail-
ability and length of the measure), cultural translation (avail-
ability in multiple (target) languages), respondent burden
(over/under 60 items), content validity (theoretical founda-
tion and expert consultation), criterion validity (correlation
with external criteria), construct validity (correlation with
related/non-related constructs), internal consistency (Cron-
bach’s alpha below/above 0.70), inter-rater reliability (agree-
ment between different raters), intra-rater reliability (agree-
ment within one rater), test–retest reliability (significant
test–retest correlation across at least two timepoints), floor
or ceiling effects, and responsiveness (successful manipula-
tion check). The definitions are also listed in the table notes
of Table3, but a concise definition of these aspects can be
found elsewhere [47]. Differences in ratings or extracted
information were discussed and solved with the second
author. The narrative synthesis reports identified measures
of continuum beliefs, their assessment method, their content
as well as a rating of their methodological quality. For each
study, design, sample size and composition, and country of
origin are also reported.
Results
The initial database search resulted in 7351 records (Pub-
Med: 3197, Web of Science: 2209, EBSCOhost: 1945), with
73 records being additionally identified from reference lists
of potentially relevant studies. After removing duplicates,
7120 records remained. A screening of titles and abstracts
lead to an exclusion of 6995 records. Finally, 125 full texts
were assessed for eligibility, wherefrom 90 studies were
excluded, leading to a sample of 35 studies for the synthesis
(see Fig.1).
The excluded studies did not assess mental health/illness
but other aspects, such as the continuum of care; they did
not provide measures (e.g., editorials or theoretical work) or
they were based on other concepts of a continuum such as
the dual continua model [43, 45] that refer to psychological
functioning (i.e., the intersection of mental wellbeing and
mental health/illness) rather than nosological concepts of
mental health/illness.
Study description
The included studies [22, 4881] investigated continuum
beliefs regarding multiple mental disorders (more than one
disorder per study in 15 out of 35 studies). Most studies
were conducted in Germany (n = 14), followed by the United
States of America (n = 10), Australia, Canada, France, and
Singapore (n = 2) as well as United Kingdom, Ireland and
the Netherlands (n = 1). An overview of included studies is
given in Table1.
Overall, most studies focused on schizophrenia (n = 23)
or depression (n = 20), followed by alcohol use disorder or
addiction (n = 5), OCD (n = 3), and dementia (n = 2). One
study each measured continuum beliefs regarding ADHD,
social anxiety disorder/generalized anxiety disorder, eating
Fig. 1 PRISMA flow diagram
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
4 Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
Table 1 Overview of included studies measuring continuum beliefs (n = 35)
No Study Design Sample Population Country Measure No. of items Examined disor-
ders
Method Response scale
1 Angermeyer etal.
(2015)
Cross-sectional
(online)
n = 1600;
16–65years;
50% male
General population
(representative)
France Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
1 Depression;
schizophrenia
Vignette Likert
(1–5)
2 Bahlmann etal.
(2015)
Cross-sectional
(same as no. 20)
n = 3642;
> 18years
General population
(representative)
Germany Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
1 Depression; schiz-
ophrenia; alcohol
use disorder
Vignette Likert
(1–5)
3 Buckwitz etal.
(2021)
Online experiment n = 478;
mean
age = 34.1years;
59% male
MTurk sample USA Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
3 Depression Rating Likert
(1–5)
4 Buckwitz etal.
(2022)
Online experiment
(same as no. 3)
n = 304;
mean
age = 34.1years;
59% male
MTurk sample USA Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
3 Depression Rating Likert
(1–5)
5 Cassidy etal.
(2020)
Online experiment n = 398;
18–75years;
mean
age = 36.76years;
50.3% male
MTurk sample USA Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
4 Bipolar disorder Vignette Likert
(1–5)
6 Cole etal. (2019) Online experiment n = 178;
mean
age = 38.01years;
35.4% male
MTurk sample USA Continuum and
categorical
beliefs
1 OCD Vignette Likert
(0–4)
7 Corrigan etal.
(2016)
Online experiment n = 598;
mean
age = 35.6years;
48.3% male
MTurk sample USA CBQ 16 Schizophrenia Vignette Likert
(1–6)
8 Dolphin etal.
(2017)
Online experiment n = 156;
mean
age = 16.25years;
48.7% male
Students Ireland Agreement with
continuum scale
1 Depression Vignette Likert
(1–6)
9 Fernandez etal.
(2022a)
Cross-sectional
(online)
n = 193;
mean
age = 17.5years;
21% male
Adolescents (com-
munity sample)
Australia Continuity beliefs;
fundamental dif-
ferences
4 Depression;
schizophrenia
Vignette Likert
(1–7)
10 Fernandez etal.
(2022b)
Cross-sectional
(online)
n = 271;
mean
age = 31.7years;
52% male
General population Australia Continuum and
categorical
beliefs
4 Schizophrenia Rating Likert
(1–4)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
5Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
Table 1 (continued)
No Study Design Sample Population Country Measure No. of items Examined disor-
ders
Method Response scale
11 Helmus etal.
(2019)
Intervention with
follow-up (paper–
pencil)
t1: n = 202;
mean
age = 45.5years;
34.7% male
t2: n = 131;
mean
age = 45.2years;
35.1% male
Mental health care
professionals
Netherlands CBQ 16 Schizophrenia Rating Likert
(1–6)
12 Makowski etal.
(2016)
Cross-sectional
(online)
n = 2006;
mean
age = 47.5years;
47.9% male
General population Germany Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
1 Depression;
schizophrenia
Vignette Likert
(1–4)
13 Makowski etal.
(2021)
Cross-sectional
(telephone
survey)
n = 1009;
18 to ≥ 65years;
49% male
General population Germany Continuity beliefs;
fundamental dif-
ferences
4 Depression Vignette Likert
(1–4)
14 Morris etal. (2020) Cross-sectional
(online)
n = 597;
mean
age = 37.21years;
52.9% male
General population United Kingdom PDBS 5 Alcohol use dis-
order
Vignette Likert
(1–5)
15 Norman etal.
(2008)
Cross-sectional
(paper–pencil)
n = 200;
mean
age = 21.5years;
45% male
Undergraduate
students
Canada Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
3 Depression;
schizophrenia
Vignette Likert
(1–5)
16 Norman etal.
(2010)
Repeated cross-
sectional (paper–
pencil)
Study 1 n = 200;
mean
age = 21.5years;
45% male
Study 2 n = 103;
mean
age = 55.7years;
50.5% male
Study 1: under-
graduate students
Study 2: commu-
nity service club
members
Canada Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
3 Depression;
schizophrenia
Vignette Likert
(1–5)
17 Paulus etal. (2015) Cross-sectional
(online)
n = 270;
mean
age = 26.8years;
19.6% male
Undergraduate
students
USA Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
1 Depression; social
anxiety disorder;
generalized anxi-
ety disorder
Vignette Severity rating
(0–8)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
6 Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
Table 1 (continued)
No Study Design Sample Population Country Measure No. of items Examined disor-
ders
Method Response scale
18 Schlier etal. (2016) Repeated cross-
sectional (online)
Study 1: n = 95;
mean age = 26.37;
50.5% male;
Study 2: n = 363;
mean
age = 27.4years;
34.7% male
Study 3: n = 229;
mean
age = 37.3years;
38.4% male
Study 1: online
sample
Study 2: online
sample
Study 3: general
population
Germany CBQ; CBQ-R 16; 14 Schizophrenia Rating Likert
(1–7)
19 Schlier etal. (2019) Online experiment n = 137;
mean
age = 27.8years;
28.5% male
Undergraduate
students + online
sample
Germany Perceived similar-
ity
4 Depression;
schizophrenia
Vignette Likert
(1–6)
20 Schomerus etal.
(2013)
Cross-sectional
(face to face,
paper–pencil)
n = 3642;
> 18years;
45.6% male
General population
(representative)
Germany Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
1 Depression; schiz-
ophrenia; alcohol
use disorder
Vignette Likert
(1–5)
21 Schomerus etal.
(2015)
Repeated cross-
sectional (online)
Study 1 n = 598;
Study 2 n = 806;
> 15years
General population
(representative)
Germany Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
1 Depression;
schizophrenia
Vignette Likert
(1–5)
22 Schomerus etal.
(2016)
Online experiment n = 1679;
> 15years;
49% male
General population
(representative)
Germany Continuity beliefs;
fundamental dif-
ferences
4 Depression;
schizophrenia
Vignette Likert
(1–5)
23 Schomerus etal.
(2022)
Repeated cross-
sectional (face-
to-face)
Study 1: n = 2455;
18 to ≥ 61years;
45.6% male;
Study 2: n = 3042;
18 to ≥ 61years;
47.2% male;
General population
(representative)
Germany Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
1 Depression;
schizophrenia
Vignette Likert
(1–5)
24 Seow etal. (2017) Cross-sectional
(online)
n = 500;
16.6% male
Undergraduate
students
Singapore Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
1 Depression; schiz-
ophrenia; alcohol
use disorder;
dementia, OCD
Vignette Likert
(1–5)
25 Speerforck etal.
(2019)
Cross-sectional
(telephone
survey)
n = 1008;
> 18years
General population
(representative)
Germany Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
1 ADHD Vignette Likert
(1–5)
26 Subrahamian etal.
(2017)
Cross-sectional
(online)
n = 3006;
18–65years;
50.9% male
General population
(representative)
Singapore Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
1 Depression; schiz-
ophrenia; alcohol
use disorder;
dementia, OCD
Vignette Likert
(1–5)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
7Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
Table 1 (continued)
No Study Design Sample Population Country Measure No. of items Examined disor-
ders
Method Response scale
27 Thibodeau & Peter-
son (2018)
Laboratory experi-
ment (paper–pen-
cil)
n = 135;
mean
age = 18.7years;
23.0% male
Undergraduate
students
USA Endorsement of
Continuum/Cat-
egorical beliefs
4 Schizophrenia Vignette Likert
(1–4)
28 Thibodeau (2017) Online experiment n = 308;
mean
age = 33.8years;
54.9% male
MTurk sample USA Continuum and
categorical
beliefs
1 Schizophrenia Vignette Likert
(1–5)
29 Thibodeau (2020) Online experiment n = 654;
mean
age = 29.6years;
39.1% male
MTurk sample USA Endorsement of
Continuum/Cat-
egorical beliefs
4 Depression Vignette Likert
(1–4)
30 Thibodeau, Shanks
etal. (2018)
Laboratory experi-
ment (paper–pen-
cil)
n = 69;
mean
age = 18.7years;
17.4% male
Undergraduate
students
USA Endorsement of
Continuum/Cat-
egorical beliefs
4 Schizophrenia Vignette Likert
(1–4)
31 Thoerel etal.
(2022)
Online experiment n = 725;
mean
age = 32.03years;
31.3% male
General population Germany General concept of
mental health
8 Eating disor-
ders (anorexia
nervosa, bulimia
nervosa, binge
eating disorder)
Vignette Likert
(1–5)
32 Violeau etal.
(2020)
Online experiment n = 565;
mean
age = 26.0years;
34.5% male;
General popula-
tion (mainly
undergraduate
students)
France QBCS (adapted
from the CBQ)
4 Schizophrenia Rating Likert
(1–7)
33 von dem Knese-
beck etal. (2015)
Repeated cross-
sectional (tel-
ephone survey)
Study 1: n = 650;
> 18years;
47.9% male
Study 2: n = 601;
> 18years;
48.1% male
General population
(representative)
Germany Belief in a contin-
uum of symptom
experience
1 Depression Vignette Likert
(1–5)
34 Wiesjahn etal.
(2014)
Cross-sectional
(online)
n = 120;
mean
age = 31.5years;
21.7% male
General population Germany CBQ 16 Schizophrenia Rating Likert
(1–6)
35 Wiesjahn etal.
(2016)
Online experiment n = 1189;
mean
age = 30.98years;
32.3% male
General population Germany CBQ 16 Schizophrenia Rating Likert
(1–6)
Notes. All measures were self-report measures and one-dimensional; CBQ Continuum Beliefs Questionnaire, PDBS Problem Drinking Belief Scale, OCD Obsessive Compulsive Disorder,
MTurk Amazon Mechanical Turk, a crowdsourcing platform, QBCS Questionnaire of Belief in a Continuum in Schizophrenia
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
8 Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
disorder, and bipolar disorder. To elicit continuum beliefs,
27 out of 35 studies utilized vignettes, sometimes personal-
ized with names and/or gender. These vignettes consisted of
short descriptions of either a person with a specific disorder
or typical symptoms of said disorder based on its diagnostic
criteria according to DSM-IV or ICD-10. Eight studies used
a rating scale, for instance the Continuum Beliefs Question-
naire (CBQ), that measures continuum beliefs independent
of a vignette [52, 58, 64, 65]. All studies, except one [57],
used four-point to seven-point Likert scales as response
measures (i.e., agreement with statements about a person,
symptoms or a condition). The remaining study [57] asked
participants to rate the severity of presented vignettes on a
scale from 0 to 8 and provided a hint that experts perceived
a rating above four as clinically relevant.
Eighteen studies investigated general population samples,
with nine explicitly mentioning representativeness of their
sample (e.g., stratified sampling and weighted analysis).
However, studies rarely mentioned how representativeness
was achieved, for instance, via quota sampling or probabil-
ity sampling; therefore, this information is not included in
Table1. Seven studies examined (undergraduate) students,
seven used Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) samples, and
three investigated adolescents [51, 70], or mental health pro-
fessionals [52].
Content ofcontinuum belief measures
Eleven different measures were used across studies, and all
were analyzes as one-dimensional measures. They ranged
from single-item measures for general continuum beliefs
[e.g., “Basically we are all sometimes like this person. It’s
just a question how pronounced this state is.“; 60] to illness-
specific scales with sixteen items [schizophrenia; 64], four
items [schizophrenia; 81] and five items [problem drink-
ing/addiction; 54]. Three measures, namely, Continuum
Beliefs Questionnaire (CBQ), Questionnaire of Belief in a
Continuum of Schizophrenia (QBCS), and Problem Drink-
ing Belief Scale (PDBS), received distinct labels, and other
measures did not, despite being used in multiple studies. The
single-item measure by Schomerus etal. [60], for instance,
was used or adapted by ten of the included studies [22, 48,
49, 51, 53, 59, 6163, 74], one of which [49] performed
additional analyses with the same data set as the original
study [60]. Two studies [66, 67] also referred to one data
set. Most measures aim to assess beliefs in a continuum
of symptom experience (see Table1). However, a closer
look at the items used in these measures reveals three dif-
ferent aspects of continuum beliefs, namely, (1) continu-
ity of symptoms [e.g., "The transition between normal and
delusional thinking is fluent"; 58], (2) normality of mental
health problems [e.g., “To some extent, most persons will
experience problems that are similar to those of Anne”; 59],
and (3) normality of persons with mental health problems
[e.g., “Basically, we are all sometimes like this person”; 60].
Conceptually, the first continuum closely resembles the con-
tinuous understanding of mental health and mental illness,
as expressed, for instance, in the dimensional operationali-
zation of mental disorders in the DSM 5 or the psychosis
continuum [1, 4]. The second and third continua rely either
on a personal experience of symptoms or the identification
with a person with mental illness (i.e., a vignette). Both
refer to a norm of inclusivity (e.g., we are all like this per-
son, most people experience these symptoms) rather than a
continuum of symptoms to represent mental illness. They
are not necessarily linked to the nosological concept of an
illness but rather to its phenotype and prevalence (second
continuum) and the perceived similarity or lack of perceived
differentness regarding the vignette (third continuum). Per-
ceived differentness is often used as an indicator of stigma-
tizing attitudes, since it depicts the differentiation between
us and them, which is a core process of stigmatization [8].
The identified continua, exemplary items, and the assigned
studies are presented in Fig.2.
While most measures focus on only one or two aspects of
continuum beliefs, two measures represent all three aspects
of continuum beliefs, namely, the scale developed by Scho-
merus etal. [59] and the CBQ [64]. The former is generic
and vignette-based, the latter was specifically developed as
a rating scale for continuum beliefs regarding schizophrenia.
Despite their inclusion of all three aspects, both measures
were analyzed as one-dimensional scales, and the conceptual
differences between continua were not explored any further.
In addition, no study has empirically compared different
measures or operationalizations of continuum beliefs.
In the next step, we examined methodological quality,
psychometric properties, and utility (i.e., readability, cul-
tural translation, respondent burden) of continuum belief
measures across studies. Categories and ratings were based
on previous research [46, 47], and rated independently by
the first and third author. Differences were discussed and
resolved with the second author (see Table2).
Overall, most studies pointed to good readability, content
validity and low respondent burden. Criterion validity was
also very positive for most measures across studies. Cul-
tural translation of some measures was proven, for instance,
the adapted measure of Schomerus etal. [60]. All measures
were comparably short (1–16 items), which makes them
highly economical and efficient. Content validity and cri-
terion validity were also high for most studies, since meas-
ures were based on theoretical considerations, pretested and
validated, for example, via manipulation tests, and expert
consultations. Construct validity was mostly tested as dis-
criminant validity resulting in either low or negative correla-
tions between continuum beliefs and stigmatizing attitudes
in most studies except for one study on OCD [50]. Fewer
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
9Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
studies reported (satisfactory) internal consistency (e.g.,
Cronbach’s alpha > 0.7), test–retest reliability was reported
in two studies [55, 56]. Floor or ceiling effects were not
explicitly reported in any of the included studies. Since all
measures were self-reports and few studies examined con-
tinuum beliefs at multiple timepoints to calculate test–retest
reliability, intra-rater reliability as well as inter-rater reliabil-
ity were also not reported. Responsiveness was very good, as
many studies used experimental designs and manipulation
checks to measure changes in continuum beliefs following
continuum belief interventions. None of the studies reported
known-groups validity (e.g., based on gender, age or type of
disorder) regarding continuum beliefs measures. As a sum-
mary, an overview of measures is provided in Table3.
Discussion
This systematic review summarizes and evaluates measures
of continuum beliefs of mental illness. The search identified
eleven different measures that ranged from single items to
multi-item scales. Most scales were generic, but some were
developed for specific disorders (i.e., schizophrenia, alcohol
use disorder). The measures seem to have high objectiv-
ity, since the instructions are clear, readability is high, and
they are easy to implement. Most measures also have high
validity due to their theory-based development, pretests, and
psychometric testing (see Table2). Yet, other psychometric
properties such as reliability (e.g., test–retest reliability) as
well as clinical utility have rarely been investigated beyond
initial piloting studies and reports of internal consistency.
Thus, more extensive psychometric studies are needed to test
factorial validity and measurement invariance, test–retest
reliability, and cross-cultural validity. The latter is particu-
larly important given cross-cultural differences in concep-
tualizing mental disorders that might influence continuum
beliefs [e.g., 82, 83].
Although some measures have been adapted to different
European, American, and Asian contexts [60], further com-
parative cross-cultural research is encouraged. Moreover, the
development, harmonization, and monitoring of continuum
belief measures should be connected to novel developments
in describing and diagnosing mental disorders. Paradigms
such as HiTOP [84] aim to provide an overarching hierar-
chy of psychopathology that pays respect to cross-cultural
differences and focuses on phenotypical similarities, thus
continuum belief measures could be developed and extended
in tandem.
The continuum belief measures were mostly implemented
in general population samples which supports their feasibil-
ity and applicability for epidemiological research. Epidemio-
logical mental health cohorts, for instance, could incorporate
Fig. 2 Three measured core aspects of continuum beliefs
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
10 Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
Table 2 Psychometric properties of continuum belief measures in the included studies (n = 35)
Notes. Empty cells mean that no information was available/reported; Readability: + items available but lengthy; + + items available, short, and comprehensive; Cultural translation: + only available in English; + + available in English
and/or language(s) of the target population; Respondent burden: + over 60 items; + + under 60 items; Content validity: + theoretical foundation; + + theoretical foundation, and experts consulted; Criterion validity: + + Correlation coef-
ficient with external criteria calculated (e.g., other measures of continuum or categorical beliefs); Construct validity:—non-significant correlations with related (i.e., concurrent validity) and/or non-related constructs (i.e., discriminant
validity); + low correlations with related and/or non-related constructs; + + moderate to strong correlations with related and/or non-related constructs; Internal consistency:—mean Cronbach’s alpha below .70; + mean Cronbach’s alpha
between .70 and .80; + + mean Cronbach’s alpha at least equal to .80; Test–retest reliability: + + significant test–retest correlation across at least two different timepoints; Responsiveness:—unsuccessful manipulation check; + + suc-
cessful manipulation check (i.e., significant changes in continuum beliefs following a continuum belief intervention)
No. Study Readability Cultural transla-
tion
Respondent
burden
Content validity Criterion
validity
Construct
validity
Internal consist-
ency
Test–retest reli-
ability
Responsiveness
1 Angermeyer etal. (2015) + + + + + + + +
2 Bahlmann etal. (2015) + + + + + +
3 Buckwitz etal. (2021) + + + + + + + + + + +
4 Buckwitz etal. (2022) + + + + + + + + + + +
5 Cassidy etal. (2020) + + + + + + + + +
6 Cole etal. (2019) + + + + + + - + +
7 Corrigan etal. (2016) + + + + + + +
8 Dolphin etal. (2017) + + + + + + + +
9 Fernandez etal. (2022a) + + + + + + + + -
10 Fernandez etal. (2022b) + + + + + - + +
11 Helmus etal. (2019) + + + + + + + + +
12 Makowski etal. (2016) + + + + + + + + + +
13 Makowski etal. (2021) + + + + + -
14 Morris etal. (2020) + + + -
15 Norman etal. (2008) + + + + + + + + + + + + +
16 Norman etal. (2010) + + + + + + + + + +
17 Paulus etal. (2015) + + + + +
18 Schlier etal. (2016) + + + + + + + + + + + +
19 Schlier etal. (2019) + + + + + + + +
20 Schomerus etal. (2013) + + + + + + + + +
21 Schomerus etal. (2015) + + + + +
22 Schomerus etal. (2016) + + + + + + + + + + + + +
23 Schomerus etal. (2022) + + + + + + + + +
24 Seow etal. (2017) + + + + + + + + +
25 Speerforck etal. (2019) + + + + + + + + + + + +
26 Subrahamian etal. (2017) + + + + + + + + + +
27 Thibodeau & Peterson (2018) + + + + + + + + +
28 Thibodeau (2017) + + + + + + + + + +
29 Thibodeau (2020) + + + + + + + + + + +
30 Thibodeau, Shanks etal. (2018) + + + + + + + + +
31 Thoerel etal. (2022) + + + + + + + + + +
32 Violeau etal. (2020) + + + + + + + -
33 von dem Knesebeck etal. (2015) + + + + + + +
34 Wiesjahn etal. (2014) + + + + + + + + + + + +
35 Wiesjahn etal. (2016) + + + + + + + + + + + + +
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
11Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
these measures to assess not only stigmatizing attitudes but
also continuum beliefs. Similarly, anti-stigma campaigns
could include continuum belief measures to measure effi-
cacy concerning public health impact, due to mostly robust
negative associations between continuum beliefs and stig-
matizing attitudes [36]. However, in some studies [e.g., 50,
81], this association was not significant; the continuum
belief intervention even lead to an increase in self-stigma
(i.e., being weird/unpredictable is typical of me) in one study
[81]. The authors [81] argue that this type of non-threaten-
ing self-stigma (e.g., weird as opposed to dangerous) is an
expression of increasing perceived similarities to the target
group thus strengthening shared social identity. However, it
is unclear how this affects persons with more severe symp-
toms and perceived similarity with more threatening attrib-
utes (e.g., dangerous). Potentially, continuum belief inter-
ventions could exacerbate group differences in samples with
more severe symptoms, because vignettes of disorders with
mild to moderate severity (as used in continuum belief meas-
ures) highlight the discrepancy between normal functioning
and their personal experience. For example, in a study by
Thibodeau and Peterson [78], the continuum belief interven-
tion increased fear. This conclusion is merely hypothetical,
though because of a lack of studies with a varying severity
of symptoms and mental disorders.
Overall, more studies with clinical samples and men-
tal health professionals are needed to assess clinical util-
ity and practicability. One study with persons with at risk
alcohol use [54] provided tentative evidence that promot-
ing continuum beliefs might increase problem recognition.
Problem recognition is an important predictor of treatment
motivation following the transtheoretical model of health
behavior change [85, 86], and it can lead to lower drop-out
rates, which is very promising for this field [87]. Therefore,
the function of continuum beliefs in treatment processes
needs to be studied more closely. This is also true for more
diverse populations (e.g., children and adolescents, older
adults). One study with adolescents showed good psycho-
metric properties of continuum belief measures [51], but
more research is necessary to confirm these findings. Since
several studies used random online samples (gathered via
MTurk), their results should also be interpreted with caution
when thinking about adapting scales to applied contexts,
since there is an ongoing debate about data quality and valid-
ity of MTurk data and similar online panels and services
compared to pragmatic, and community samples [8890].
Hence, multi-group comparisons of samples from different
providers and sources are recommended.
Furthermore, the conceptualization of continuum beliefs
needs to be examined. The CBQ, the PDBS, and the QBCS
were developed for specific disorders, which is why they
can refer to disorder-specific symptoms without including
vignettes or descriptions of mental disorders as a frame
of reference. Consequently, other studies did not need to
adapt or pretest additional materials. These scales could also
directly describe a disorder-specific continuum of symptoms
(e.g., the psychosis continuum; [4]) as an indicator of mental
stress leading to mental illness, which is in line with the
approach of normalization proposed by Angermeyer und
Schulze [21]. Vignette-based studies with more generic
scales, on the other hand, were more flexible and allow
direct comparisons of beliefs regarding different disorders—
which lends credibility to the idea of an underlying concept
of continuity or dimension of mental health and illness. This
way of thinking corresponds to current positive psychologi-
cal approaches, such as the dual continuum model of men-
tal health [43, 44], and the HiTOP model with its focus on
phenotypes rather than diagnostic labels or categories [84].
This more generic approach, however, also requires
validated vignettes to assess continuum beliefs. This is
challenging for multiple reasons: First, the included stud-
ies used different vignettes which could have biased the
results. Second, most studies controlled for confounding
influences by either presenting no gender or name or ran-
domizing gendered vignettes. However, these vignettes
still required participants to imagine the person and their
symptoms, which requires sufficient perceived realism of
each vignette and consensus regarding the described experi-
ence (e.g., of a depressive episode) [91]. Therefore, future
research should compare continuum beliefs across different
vignettes. Third, other aspects such as age or ethnicity of
the presented or imagined person were not controlled and
might have additional influence on continuum beliefs [92].
Hence, future studies should examine the differential impact
of different disorder-specific vignettes on multiple measures
of continuum beliefs. These vignettes could also be tested
or constructed based on population assessments, similar to
the measure of Paulus etal. [57] In their study, they asked
participants to rate the severity of different symptoms and
behaviors ranging from healthy to clinically relevant. While
this is closely connected to a diagnostic approach (e.g., in
psychotherapeutic training), it also provides the opportunity
to customize (sub-)clinical vignettes of specific disorders
concerning type and intensity of symptoms and assess sub-
sequent ratings to examine the extent of continuum beliefs.
In this sense, future research could build upon scale-based
measures, such as the CBQ that requires similar assessments
(e.g., regarding hallucinations) via Likert scales.
Finally, different operationalizations of continuum beliefs
are also a promising avenue for future research, similar to
the area of health literacy, where multiple objective tests and
subjective self-reports are state of the art [93, 94]. While the
identified measures captured between one and three aspects
of the continuum (see Fig.2), certain aspects were rarely
examined, for example, the categorical v continuous con-
ceptualization of mental illness [2, 3]. Items measuring this
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
12 Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
Table 3 Overview of eleven measures of continuum beliefs (plus a revised version of the Continuum Beliefs Questionnaire) including their origin, number of items, assessment method, and the
dimensions of continuum reflected with each measure as well as examined disorders
Measure Origin No. of items Method Country Type of continuum Examined disorders
Belief in a continuum of symp-
tom experience
Schomerus etal. (2013) 1 Vignette France; Germany; Singapore;
Ireland
Normality of persons with
mental health problems
Depression; schizophrenia;
alcohol use disorder; dementia,
obsessive–compulsive disorder;
attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder
Continuity beliefs Schomerus etal. (2016) 4 Vignette Germany Continuity of symptoms
Normality of mental health
problems
Normality of persons with
mental health problems
Depression; schizophrenia
Continuum beliefs Thibodeau (2017) 1 Vignette U SA Normality of mental health
problems
Schizophrenia; obsessive–com-
pulsive disorder
Endorsement of continuum
beliefs
Thibodeau, Shanks etal. (2018) 4 Vignette USA Continuity of symptoms Schizophrenia
Continuum Beliefs Question-
naire
Wiesjahn etal. (2014) 16 Rating Germany; USA; Netherlands Continuity of symptoms
Normality of mental health
problems
Normality of persons with
mental health problems
Schizophrenia
Continuum Beliefs Question-
naire-revised
Schlier etal. (2016) 14; 16 Rating Germany Continuity of symptoms
Normality of mental health
problems
Normality of persons with
mental health problems
Schizophrenia
Belief in a continuum of symp-
tom experience
Norman etal. (2008) 3; 4 Vignette Canada; USA Normality of mental health
problems
Normality of persons with
mental health problems
Depression; schizophrenia; bipo-
lar disorder
Perceived similarity Schlier etal. (2019) 4 Vignette Germany Normality of persons with
mental health problems
Depression; schizophrenia
Belief in a continuum of symp-
tom experience
Paulus etal. (2015) 1 Vignette USA Continuity of symptoms Depression; social anxiety
disorder; generalized anxiety
disorder
Problem Drinking Belief Scale Morris etal. (2020) 5 Vignette USA Continuity of symptoms Alcohol use disorder
General concept of mental
health
Thoerel etal. (2022) 8 Vignette Germany Normality of mental health
problems
Normality of persons with
mental health problems
Eating disorders
Questionnaire of Belief in a
Continuum in Schizophrenia
Violeau etal. (2020) 4 Rating France Continuity of symptoms
Normality of mental health
problems
Schizophrenia
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
13Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
nosological concept were included in the development of the
CBQ, but they were eventually excluded from the final meas-
ure [64]. It might be beneficial to compare measures of such
conceptual beliefs with continuum beliefs measures, and
compare multiple measures of continuum beliefs, to assess
similarities and differences and examine their responsive-
ness in future interventional studies. Nevertheless, it should
also be added that a more conceptual measure of continuum
beliefs requires a more abstract assessment of nosological
concepts of illness and health, which might be rather difficult
for laypersons, meaning population samples without previ-
ous education about this issue.
In sum, when choosing a measure of continuum beliefs, a
researcher needs to think about the population (e.g., a sam-
ple with clinical depression vis-à-vis a healthy population
sample), the context (e.g., disorder-specific versus transdi-
agnostic assessments), the method (e.g., rating scales versus
vignettes), and the overall aim of the study (e.g., compar-
ing attitudes across groups or disorders versus examining
predictive utility or validity of continuum beliefs). In an
epidemiological study of depression-related attitudes in
the population, a disorder-specific measure using vignettes
might be most appropriate, whereas a comparative study of
continuum beliefs across different disorders might benefit
from a short, generic measure that has a low respondent
burden and allows for transdiagnostic comparisons. While
our review shows that some types of measures have received
more attention than others so far, the usefulness and merit of
each measure strongly depends on the context of investiga-
tion. This review provides a framework for decision-making
and further research in continuum beliefs of mental illness.
The review is not without limitations. The search was
limited to three data bases, and preregistered search cri-
teria (e.g., regarding search terms, language) as well as
peer-reviewed literature, which might have neglected grey
literature and other studies that could not be identified by
the initial search. The review focused on continuum beliefs
of mental illness, while previous literature defined different
continua (e.g., continuum of care, dual continua model) that
might be associated with continuum beliefs. For instance,
the continuum of care assumes different needs and respon-
sibilities for different stages of an illness, such as prevention,
acute treatment, or recovery [95]. These stages are associ-
ated with different levels of severity of an illness, which
might serve as a reference for assessing continuum beliefs.
Similarly, the dual continua model assumes parallel continua
of mental well-being and mental health/illness. It is unclear
how different constellations of well-being and mental health
(e.g., flourishing) are associated with continuum beliefs. The
study used established ratings of methodological quality and
it reported results in accordance with the PRISMA state-
ment, but it did not examine risk of bias or use different
rating systems of measures. This could be the focus of future
work. Despite its weaknesses, however, this review identi-
fied several measurement instruments of continuum beliefs
with applications in multiple cultural contexts, and initial
evidence of good validity, and applicability in general popu-
lation samples. Hence, the potential of continuum beliefs
regarding public mental health and the economic modes of
assessment are quite promising.
Acknowledgements This review was supported by the German
Research Foundation (DFG; GS, grant number SCHO-1337/4-2; SiS,
grant number SCHM-2683/4-2). The DFG did not have any role in
study development, analysis, or interpretation of the data. The funding
body was not involved in writing this report nor the decision to submit
it for publication.
Author contributions Conceptualization, ST and HoM.; methodology,
ST, SaS, TG, and LJP.; software, ST; validation, ST, SaS, TG, TM, and
HoM; formal analysis, ST, SaS, and TG; investigation, ST, SaS, TG,
and LJP; resources, ST, SaS, TG, and LJP; data curation, ST, SaS, TG,
and LJP; writing—original draft, ST; writing—review and editing, TM,
HoM, LJP, GS, and SiS; visualization, ST; supervision, ST, TG, and
HoM; supervision, ST, HoM, GS, and SiS; project administration, ST,
HoM, GS, and SiS; funding acquisition, GS and SiS. All authors have
read and approved the final version of the manuscript.
Funding Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt
DEAL.
Data availability statement Data sharing is not applicable to this article
as no new data were created or analyzed in this study.
Declarations
Conflict of interests All authors declare that they have no conflicts of
interest.
Ethical standards Not applicable.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attri-
bution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adapta-
tion, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long
as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source,
provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes
were made. The images or other third party material in this article are
included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated
otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in
the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will
need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a
copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.
References
1. American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statisti-
cal manual of mental disorders: DSM 5 5edn. American Psychi-
atric Association, Washington
2. Markon KE, Chmielewski M, Miller CJ (2011) The reliability and
validity of discrete and continuous measures of psychopathology:
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
14 Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
a quantitative review. Psychol Bull 137(5):856–879. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1037/ a0023 678
3. Haslam N, Holland E, Kuppens P (2012) Categories versus dimen-
sions in personality and psychopathology: a quantitative review
of taxometric research. Psychol Med 42(5):903–920. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1017/ s0033 29171 10019 66
4. van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krab-
bendam L (2009) A systematic review and meta-analysis of the
psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness–per-
sistence–impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med
39(2):179–195. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/ s0033 29170 80038 14
5. Asai T, Yamauchi T, Sugimori E, Bando N, Tanno Y (2010) Schi-
zotypy and schizophrenia as points on a continuum. Japanese Psy-
chol Rev 53(2):240–261
6. Seidman LJ, Nordentoft M (2015) New targets for prevention of
schizophrenia: Is it time for interventions in the premorbid phase?
Schizophr Bull 41(4):795–800. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ schbul/
sbv050
7. Appelbaum PS (2015) Ethical challenges in the primary preven-
tion of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 41(4):773–775. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1093/ schbul/ sbv053
8. Link BG, Phelan JC (2001) Conceptualizing stigma. Ann Rev
Sociol 27(1):363–385. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1146/ annur ev . soc. 27.1.
363
9. Linscott RJ, van Os J (2013) An updated and conservative sys-
tematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence
on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway
from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across
mental disorders. Psychol Med 43(6):1133–1149. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1017/ s0033 29171 20016 26
10. Sulis W (2018) Assessing the continuum between temperament
and affective illness: psychiatric and mathematical perspectives.
Philosoph Transact Royal Soc B-Biol Sci 373:1744. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1098/ rstb. 2017. 0168
11. Elbourne KE, Chen J (2007) The continuum model of obliga-
tory exercise: A preliminary investigation. J Psychosom Res
62(1):73–80. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jpsyc hores. 2004. 12. 003
12. Hesse-Biber S (1992) Report on a panel longitudinal study of
college women’s eating patterns and eating disorders: Noncon-
tinuum versus continuum measures. Health Care Women Int
13(4):375–391. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 07399 33920 95160 15
13. Lindeman M, Stark K, Keskivaara P (2001) Continuum and lin-
earity hypotheses on the relationship between psychopathology
and eating disorder symptomatology. Eating Weight Disorders
Stud Anorexia Bulimia Obesity 6(4):181–187. https:// doi. org/
10. 1007/ bf033 39741
14. Fullana MA, Mataix-Cols D, Caspi A, Harrington H, Grisham
JR, Moffitt TE, Poulton R (2009) Obsessions and compulsions
in the community: prevalence, interference, help-seeking, devel-
opmental stability, and co-occurring psychiatric conditions. Am
J Psychiatry 166(3):329–336
15. Kertz SJ, McHugh RK, Lee J, Björgvinsson T (2014) Examin-
ing the latent structure of worry and generalized anxiety in a
clinical sample. J Anxiety Disord 28(1):8–15. https:// doi. org/
10. 1016/j. janxd is. 2013. 11. 003
16. Siddaway AP, Wood AM, Taylor PJ (2017) The center for epide-
miologic studies-depression (CES-D) scale measures a contin-
uum from well-being to depression: Testing two key predictions
of positive clinical psychology. J Affect Disord 213:180–186.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jad. 2017. 02. 015
17. Goldstein B, Rosselli F (2003) Etiological paradigms of depres-
sion: The relationship between perceived causes, empowerment,
treatment preferences, and stigma. J Ment Health 12(6):551–
563. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 09638 23031 00016 27919
18. Mahmoud KF, Finnell D, Savage CL, Puskar KR, Mitchell
AM (2017) A concept analysis of substance misuse to inform
contemporary terminology. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 31(6):532–540.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. apnu. 2017. 06. 004
19. Rehm J, Marmet S, Anderson P, Gual A, Kraus L, Nutt DJ,
Room R, Samokhvalov AV, Scafato E, Trapencieris M, Wiers
RW, Gmel G (2013) Defining substance use disorders: do we
really need more than heavy use? Alcohol Alcohol 48(6):633–
640. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ alcalc/ agt127
20. Strong DR, Kahler CW (2007) Evaluation of the continuum of
gambling problems using the DSM-IV. Addiction 102(5):713–
721. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/j. 1360- 0443. 2007. 01789.x
21. Angermeyer MC, Schulze B (2001) Reinforcing stereotypes: How
the focus on forensic cases in news reporting may influence pub-
lic attitudes towards the mentally ill. Int J Law Psychiatry 24(4–
5):469–486. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ s0160- 2527(01) 00079-6
22. von dem Knesebeck O, Mnich E, Angermeyer MC, Kofahl C,
Makowski A (2015) Changes in depression stigma after the Ger-
manwings crash—Findings from German population surveys. J
Affect Disord 186:261–265. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jad. 2015.
07. 029
23. Schomerus G, Auer C, Rhode D, Luppa M, Freyberger HJ,
Schmidt S (2012) Personal stigma, problem appraisal and
perceived need for professional help in currently untreated
depressed persons. J Affect Disord 139(1):94–97. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1016/j. jad. 2012. 02. 022
24. Schomerus G, Stolzenburg S, Freitag S, Speerforck S, Janowitz
D, Evans-Lacko S, Muehlan H, Schmidt S (2019) Recognizing
personal mental illness and seeking help – a prospective study
among untreated persons with mental illness. Eur Arch Psy-
chiatry Clin Neurosci 269(4):469–479. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/
s00406- 018- 0896-0
25. Schnyder N, Michel C, Panczak R, Ochsenbein S, Schimmel-
mann BG, Schultze-Lutter F (2018) The interplay of etiological
knowledge and mental illness stigma on healthcare utilisation
in the community: A structural equation model. Eur Psychiatr
51:48–56. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. eurpsy. 2017. 12. 027
26. Schnyder N, Panczak R, Groth N, Schultze-Lutter F (2017)
Association between mental health-related stigma and active
help-seeking: systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Psy-
chiatry 210(4):261–268. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1192/ bjp. bp. 116.
189464
27. Clement S, Schauman O, Graham T, Maggioni F, Evans-Lacko
S, Bezborodovs N, Morgan C, Rüsch N, Brown JSL, Thornicroft
G (2015) What is the impact of mental health-related stigma on
help-seeking? A systematic review of quantitative and qualitative
studies. Psychol Med 45(1):11–27. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/ s0033
29171 40001 29
28. Jennings KS, Cheung JH, Britt TW, Goguen KN, Jeffirs SM, Pea-
sley AL, Lee AC (2015) How are perceived stigma, self-stigma,
and self-reliance related to treatment-seeking? A three-path
model. Psych Rehab J 38(2):109–116. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/
prj00 00138
29. Vogel DL, Wade NG, Hackler AH (2007) Perceived public stigma
and the willingness to seek counseling: The mediating roles of
self-stigma and attitudes toward counseling. J Couns Psychol
54(1):40–50. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0022- 0167. 54.1. 40
30. Corrigan PW, Watson AC, Barr L (2006) The self-stigma of men-
tal illness: Implications for self-esteem and self-efficacy. J Soc
Clin Psychol 25(8):875–884. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1521/ jscp. 2006.
25.8. 875
31. Rüsch N, Corrigan PW, Powell K, Rajah A, Olschewski M,
Wilkniss S, Batia K (2009) A stress-coping model of mental ill-
ness stigma: II. Emotional stress responses, coping behavior and
outcome. Schizophrenia Res 110(13):65–71. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1016/j. schres. 2009. 01. 005
32. Corrigan PW, Morris SB, Michaels PJ, Rafacz JD, Rusch N (2012)
Challenging the public stigma of mental illness: a meta-analysis
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
15Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
of outcome studies. Psychiatr Serv 63(10):963–973. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1176/ appi. ps. 20110 0529
33. Corrigan PW, Watson AC (2002) Understanding the impact of
stigma on people with mental illness. World Psychiatry 1(1):16-20
34. Rüsch N, Angermeyer MC, Corrigan PW (2005) Mental illness
stigma: Concepts, consequences, and initiatives to reduce stigma.
Eur Psychiatr 20(8):529–539. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. eurpsy.
2005. 04. 004
35. Persson L, Dobson KS, Frampton NMA (2021) Evaluation of a
mental health continuum model in two samples. Canad J Behav
Sci/ Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement: Adv
Online Publ https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ cbs00 00273
36. Peter L-J, Schindler S, Sander C, Schmidt S, Muehlan H, McLaren
T, Tomczyk S, Speerforck S, Schomerus G (2021) Continuum
beliefs and mental illness stigma: a systematic review and meta-
analysis of correlation and intervention studies. Psychol Med
51(5):716–726. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/ S0033 29172 10008 54
37. Read R, Moberly NJ, Salter D, Broome MR (2017) Concepts of
mental disorders in trainee clinical psychologists. Clin Psychol
Psychother 24(2):441–450. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1002/ cpp. 2013
38. Wyatt RC, Livson N (1994) The not so great divide? Psycholo-
gists and psychiatrists take stands on the medical and psychosocial
models of mental illness. Prof Psychol Res Pract 25(2):120–131.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0735- 7028. 25.2. 120
39. Harland R, Antonova E, Owen GS, Broome M, Landau S, Deeley
Q, Murray R (2009) A study of psychiatrists’ concepts of men-
tal illness. Psychol Med 39(6):967–976. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/
s0033 29170 80048 81
40. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Prisma Group (2009)
Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analy-
ses: the PRISMA statement. Ann Int Med 151(4):264–269. https://
doi. org/ 10. 7326/ 0003- 4819- 151-4- 20090 8180- 00135
41. Schardt C, Adams MB, Owens T, Keitz S, Fontelo P (2007) Utili-
zation of the PICO framework to improve searching PubMed for
clinical questions. BMC Med Inf Decis Making 7(1):16. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1186/ 1472- 6947-7- 16
42. Berendsen AJ, Groenier KH, de Jong GM, Meyboom-de Jong B,
van der Veen WJ, Dekker J, de Waal MWM, Schuling J (2009)
Assessment of patient’s experiences across the interface between
primary and secondary care: Consumer Quality Index Continuum
of Care. Patient Educ Couns 77(1):123–127. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1016/j. pec. 2009. 01. 011
43. Keyes CLM (2002) The mental health continuum: From languish-
ing to flourishing in life. J Health Soc Behav 43(2):207–222.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 2307/ 30901 97
44. Keyes KM, Krueger RF, Grant BF, Hasin DS (2011) Alcohol
craving and the dimensionality of alcohol disorders. Psychol Med
41(3):629–640. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/ s0033 29171 00005 3x
45. Michalec B, Keyes CLM (2013) A multidimensional perspec-
tive of the mental health of preclinical medical students. Psychol
Health Med 18(1):89–97. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 13548 506. 2012.
687825
46. Bennett C, Khangura S, Brehaut JC, Graham ID, Moher D, Pot-
ter BK, Grimshaw JM (2010) Reporting guidelines for survey
research: an analysis of published guidance and reporting prac-
tices. PLoS Med 8(8):e1001069–e1001069. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1371/ journ al. pmed. 10010 69
47. Scarcella A, Page R, Furtado V (2016) Terrorism, radicalisation,
extremism, authoritarianism and fundamentalism: a systematic
review of the quality and psychometric properties of assessments.
PLoS ONE 11(12):e0166947. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al.
pone. 01669 47
48. Angermeyer MC, Millier A, Remuzat C, Refai T, Schomerus G,
Toumi M (2015) Continuum beliefs and attitudes towards people
with mental illness: Results from a national survey in France. Int
J Soc Psychiatry 61(3):297–303. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 00207
64014 543312
49. Bahlmann J, Schomerus G, Angermeyer MC (2015) Nicht ganz
dasselbe: Krankheitsvorstellungen von Burnout und Depression
in der Allgemeinbevölkerung [Not quite the same: illness beliefs
regarding burnout and depression among the general population].
Psychiatr Prax 42(8):443–447. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1055/s- 0034-
13873 65
50. Cole JL, Warman DM (2019) An examination of Continuum
Beliefs Versus Biogenetic beliefs in reducing stigma toward vio-
lent intrusive thoughts in OCD. J Obsess Compuls Relat Disord
23: 100478. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jocrd. 2019. 100478
51. Dolphin L, Hennessy E (2017) Labelling effects and adolescent
responses to peers with depression: An experimental investi-
gation. BMC Psychiatry 17(1):228. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1186/
s12888- 017- 1389-9
52. Helmus K, Schaars IK, Wierenga H, de Glint E, van Os J (2019)
Decreasing stigmatization: Reducing the discrepancy between
“us” and “them”. An intervention for mental health care profes-
sionals. Front Psych 10:243. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyt. 2019.
00243
53. Makowski AC, Mnich EE, Angermeyer MC, Knesebeck OV
(2016) Continuum beliefs in the stigma process regarding persons
with schizophrenia and depression: results of path analyses. PeerJ
4:e2360. https:// doi. org/ 10. 7717/ peerj. 2360
54. Morris J, Albery IP, Heather N, Moss AC (2020) Continuum
beliefs are associated with higher problem recognition than
binary beliefs among harmful drinkers without addiction experi-
ence. Addict Behav 105:106292. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. addbeh.
2020. 106292
55. Norman RMG, Sorrentino RM, Windell D, Manchanda R (2008)
The role of perceived norms in the stigmatization of mental ill-
ness. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 43(11):851. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1007/ s00127- 008- 0375-4
56. Norman RMG, Windell D, Manchanda R (2010) Examining dif-
ferences in the stigma of depression and schizophrenia. Int J Soc
Psychiatry 58(1):69–78. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 00207 64010
387062
57. Paulus DJ, Wadsworth LP, Hayes-Skelton SA (2015) Mental
health literacy for anxiety disorders: How perceptions of symp-
tom severity might relate to recognition of psychological distress.
J Public Ment Health 14(2):94–106. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1108/
JPMH- 09- 2013- 0064
58. Schlier B, Scheunemann J, Lincoln TM (2016) Continuum beliefs
about psychotic symptoms are a valid, unidimensional construct:
Construction and validation of a revised continuum beliefs ques-
tionnaire. Psychiatry Res 241:147–153. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j.
psych res. 2016. 04. 085
59. Schomerus G, Angermeyer MC, Baumeister SE, Stolzenburg S,
Link BG, Phelan JC (2016) An online intervention using infor-
mation on the mental health-mental illness continuum to reduce
stigma. Eur Psychiatr 32:21–27. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. eurpsy.
2015. 11. 006
60. Schomerus G, Matschinger H, Angermeyer MC (2013) Con-
tinuum beliefs and stigmatizing attitudes towards persons with
schizophrenia, depression and alcohol dependence. Psychiatry Res
209(3):665–669. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. psych res. 2013. 02. 006
61. Schomerus G, Stolzenburg S, Angermeyer MC (2015) Impact of
the Germanwings plane crash on mental illness stigma: results
from two population surveys in Germany before and after the inci-
dent. World Psychiatry 14(3):362–363. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1002/
wps. 20257
62. Speerforck S, Stolzenburg S, Hertel J, Grabe HJ, Strauß M, Carta
MG, Angermeyer MC, Schomerus G (2019) ADHD, stigma
and continuum beliefs: A population survey on public attitudes
towards children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
16 Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2023) 58:1–16
1 3
disorder. Psychiatry Res 282:112570. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j.
psych res. 2019. 112570
63. Subramaniam M, Abdin E, Picco L, Shahwan S, Jeyagurunathan
A, Vaingankar JA, Chong SA (2017) Continuum beliefs and stig-
matising beliefs about mental illness: results from an Asian com-
munity survey. BMJ Open 7(4):e014993. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1136/
bmjop en- 2016- 014993
64. Wiesjahn M, Brabban A, Jung E, Gebauer UB, Lincoln TM (2014)
Are continuum beliefs about psychotic symptoms associated with
stereotypes about schizophrenia? Psychosis: Psychological. Soc
Integrat Approach 6(1):50–60. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 17522 439.
2012. 740068
65. Wiesjahn M, Jung E, Kremser JD, Rief W, Lincoln TM (2016) The
potential of continuum versus biogenetic beliefs in reducing stig-
matization against persons with schizophrenia: An experimental
study. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 50:231–237. https:// doi. org/
10. 1016/j. jbtep. 2015. 09. 007
66. Buckwitz V, Porter PA, Bommes JN, Schomerus G, Hinshaw SP
(2021) Continuum beliefs and the stigma of depression: An online
investigation. Stigma and Health 6(1):113–122. https:// doi. or g/ 10.
1037/ sah00 00272
67. Buckwitz V, Bommes JN, Hinshaw SP, Schomerus G (2022) Con-
tinuum beliefs and the perception of similarities and differences to
a person with depression. Compr Psychiatry 116:152314. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. compp sych. 2022. 152314
68. Cassidy C, Erdal K (2020) Assessing and addressing stigma in
bipolar disorder: The impact of cause and treatment information
on stigma. Stigma Health 5:104–113. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/
sah00 00181
69. Corrigan PW, Schmidt A, Bink AB, Nieweglowski K, Al-Khouja
MA, Qin S, Discont S (2016) Changing public stigma with con-
tinuum beliefs. J Ment Health 26(5):411–418. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1080/ 09638 237. 2016. 12072 24
70. Fernandez DK, Deane FP, Vella SA (2022) Adolescents’ contin-
uum and categorical beliefs, help-seeking intentions, and stigma
towards people experiencing depression or schizophrenia. Int J
Ment Health Addict. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s11469- 022- 00766-5
71. Fernandez DK, Deane FP, Vella SA (2022) Effects of online
continuum and categorical belief manipulations on schizophre-
nia stigma, help-seeking, and help-provision. Psychiatry Res
307:114293. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. psych res. 2021. 114293
72. Makowski AC, Schomerus G, von dem Knesebeck O (2021)
Public continuum beliefs for different levels of depression sever-
ity. Front Psych 12:666489. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyt. 2021.
666489
73. Schlier B, Lincoln TM (2019) The stigma of mental illness:
Testing for the implicit bias in diagnostic labels. Psychiatry Res
275:221–227. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. psych res. 2019. 03. 028
74. Schomerus G, Schindler S, Baumann E, Angermeyer MC
(2022) Changes in continuum beliefs for depression and schizo-
phrenia in the general population 2011–2020: a widening gap.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/
s00127- 022- 02272-4
75. Seow LSE, Chua BY, Xie H, Wang J, Ong HL, Abdin E, Chong
SA, Subramaniam M (2017) Correct recognition and continuum
belief of mental disorders in a nursing student population. BMC
Psychiatry 17(1):289. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1186/ s12888- 017- 1447-3
76. Thibodeau R, Shanks LN, Smith BP (2017) Do continuum beliefs
reduce schizophrenia stigma? Effects of a laboratory intervention
on behavioral and self-reported stigma. J Behav Ther Exp Psy-
chiatry 58:29–35. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jbtep. 2017. 08. 002
77. Thibodeau R (2017) Continuum beliefs and schizophrenia
stigma: Correlational and experimental evidence. Stigma Health
2(4):266–270. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ sah00 00061
78. Thibodeau R, Peterson KM (2018) On continuum beliefs and psy-
chiatric stigma: Similarity to a person with schizophrenia can feel
too close for comfort. Psychiatry Res 270:731–737. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1016/j. psych res. 2018. 10. 070
79. Thibodeau R (2020) Continuum belief, categorical belief, and
depression stigma: Correlational evidence and outcomes of an
online intervention. Stigma Health 5(4):404–412. https:// doi. org/
10. 1037/ sah00 00211
80. Thörel N, Thörel E, Tuschen-Caffier B (2022) Effects of con-
tinuum and categorical beliefs on attitudes related to eating dis-
order stigma. Stigma Health. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ sah00 00386.
supp(Suppl ement al)
81. Violeau L, Valery KM, Fournier T, Prouteau A (2020) How con-
tinuum beliefs can reduce stigma of schizophrenia: The role of
perceived similarities. Schizophr Res 220:46–53. https:// doi. org/
10. 1016/j. schres. 2020. 04. 014
82. Schomerus G, Angermeyer MC (2021) Blind spots in stigma
research? Broadening our perspective on mental illness stigma
by exploring ‘what matters most’ in modern Western societies.
Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci 30:e26. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/ S2045
79602 10001 11
83. Sam DL, Moreira V (2012) Revisiting the mutual embeddedness
of culture and mental illness. Online Read Psychol Cult. https://
doi. org/ 10. 9707/ 2307- 0919. 1078
84. Kotov R, Krueger RF, Watson D (2018) A paradigm shift in psy-
chiatric classification: the Hierarchical Taxonomy Of Psychopa-
thology (HiTOP). World Psychiatry 17(1):24–25. https:// doi. org/
10. 1002/ wps. 20478
85. Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC (1992) Stages of change in the
modification of problem behaviors. Progress on behavior modifi-
cation. Sycamore Press, Sycamore
86. Prochaska JO, Velicer WF (1997) The transtheoretical model of
health behavior change. Am J Health Prom 12(1):38–48. https://
doi. org/ 10. 4278/ 0890- 1171- 12.1. 38
87. Ray LA, Hutchison KE, Bryan A (2006) Psychosocial predictors
of treatment outcome, dropout, and change processes in a phar-
macological clinical trial for alcohol dependence. Addict Disord
Treat 5(4):179–190. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1097/ 01. adt. 00002 10701.
63165. 5a
88. Kees J, Berry C, Burton S, Sheehan K (2017) An analysis of
data quality: Professional panels, student subject pools, and Ama-
zon’s Mechanical Turk. J Advert 46(1):141–155. https:// doi. org/
10. 1080/ 00913 367. 2016. 12693 04
89. Chmielewski M, Kucker SC (2020) An MTurk crisis? Shifts in
data quality and the impact on study results. Soc Psychol Personal
Sci 11(4):464–473. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 19485 50619 875149
90. Smith SM, Roster CA, Golden LL, Albaum GS (2016) A multi-
group analysis of online survey respondent data quality: Compar-
ing a regular USA consumer panel to MTurk samples. J Bus Res
69(8):3139–3148. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jbusr es. 2015. 12. 002
91. O’Dell L, Crafter S, de Abreu G, Cline T (2012) The problem of
interpretation in vignette methodology in research with young
people. Qual Res 12(6):702–714. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 14687
94112 439003
92. Swami V (2012) Mental health literacy of depression: gender
differences and attitudinal antecedents in a representative british
sample. PLoS ONE 7(11):e49779. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ
al. pone. 00497 79
93. Mancuso JM (2009) Assessment and measurement of health lit-
eracy: An integrative review of the literature. Nurs Health Sci
11(1):77–89. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/j. 1442- 2018. 2008. 00408.x
94. Haun JN, Valerio MA, McCormack LA, Sørensen K, Paasche-
Orlow MK (2014) Health literacy measurement: An inventory
and descriptive summary of 51 instruments. J Health Commun
19(S2):302–333. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 10810 730. 2014. 936571
95. Evashwick C (1989) Creating the continuum of care. Health
Matrix 7(1):30–39
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... In response to validity problems, alternatives to conventional psychiatric classification have been proposed, such as transdiagnostic and dimensional approaches [26,28,29,30,31]. In these approaches, mental and social phenomena currently categorized as mental disorders would not be regarded as qualitatively different from other kinds of human emotions, thoughts, or behavior, but beyond a certain severity threshold, they would be treated as medical conditions requiring medical treatment [31,32]. However, many of these include unresolved conceptual issues and lack clinical utility [31,33]. ...
... By organizing services according to broader symptom clusters and incorporating severity levels that primarily indicate which types of medical treatment may or may not be effective, the diagnostic practice in psychiatry can realign with its original purpose, while minimizing the problematic reification of diagnoses. This aligns with recent developments in understanding mental health problems [27][28][29]32] and enables better clinical utilization of integrative approaches, including personalization of treatment [3,4,5] based on observable clinical characteristics. ...
Article
Full-text available
The group-level symptom-reduction model of mental health care emphasizes predetermined treatment guidelines for those mental and social difficulties that are diagnosable as mental health disorders on the basis of predetermined diagnostic criteria. The model have produced generalizable information to support medical decisionmaking for symptom reduction. However, it may have also increased the reification of diagnostic labels, and in so doing medicalized and stigmatized complex human-life experiences, with a lack of attention to a range of social determinants and existential factors associated with mental health. Since symptom-reduction model can easily lose sight of essential non-technical and contextual aspects of mental health care, including the quality of the interaction and other common factors needed to understand and treat mental health difficulties, there is doubts that the symptom-reduction model may actually decrease the effectiveness of mental health services, as compared to a holistic approach. Based on recent critiques of the group-level symptom-reduction model to mental health care, and research on common-factor perspectives on mental health treatments, holistic conceptions of humans, and naturalistic outcome studies from several holistic mental health services from different countries, I hypothesized that an ontological turn from the treatment of “mental disorders” to the treatment of “difficult life situations” will lead to a more personalized and comprehensive treatment approach, that mediates an improved effectiveness of mental health services.
... Methodologically, the intervention results regarding the target variables might be limited in terms of comparability due to the lack of validation of the scales and operationalization. For continuum beliefs, for example, the questionnaire was developed specifically for this project due to a lack of existing differentiated and validated questionnaires (Tomczyk et al., 2023). Moreover, we designed a new balanced score operationalizing a biopsychosocial model of causes for mental illness, examining how a participant considers different causal beliefs and how fixed their beliefs are, which has not been reflected by mean values until now (e.g., Carter et al., 2019;Stolzenburg et al., 2018;Tan et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Various interventions have been developed to reduce the stigmatization of people with mental illness, offering diverse forms of psychoeducational information and other components. One promising tool is the use of continuum beliefs, which stress that mental health and illness fall on a continuum of life experiences, reducing notions of “us vs. them.” However, the effects have been inconsistent, and research gaps remain. These include understanding which specific informational components may help to reduce stigma, the effects of different presentational forms, the nature of the samples affected—that is, people with depressive symptoms, and longitudinal effects. These gaps were addressed by examining the effects of different psychoeducational approaches (continuum beliefs, mental health literacy, and a balanced biopsychosocial cause model) on a broad range of internalized and anticipated stigmas for people with as-yet untreated depressive symptoms. In a quasiexperimental online study, interventions were presented to n = 1,375 participants with moderate depression severity (MPHQ = 13.01, Mage = 42.3, 65.2% female, 0.5% diverse). Changes to 10 stigma scores from baseline to postintervention were compared in experimental and control groups, and follow-ups were conducted after 3 and 6 months (difference-of-differences analyses). Rather unspecific time effects on stigma were observed. Those exposed to the continuum beliefs text showed significantly increased agreement with this model. By contrast, an emphasis on causal beliefs proved counterproductive, while the literacy approach had no apparent effects. This study lacks a diversity of perspectives but provides entry points for a more in-depth investigation of combined-effect mechanisms for psychoeducational interventions regarding inclusive, engaging web-based designs for affected samples.
... Higher scores on this scale indicate higher continuum beliefs. The Continuity with Normal Scale is a valid and reliable measure of continuum beliefs [33]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose A pervasive and deeply entrenched stigma of personality disorders exists. For other mental disorders, a large body of research suggests that continuum beliefs (i.e., the endorsement of continuum perspectives on mental health and psychopathology) stimulate more favorable attitudes toward affected persons. Additionally, mental disorder classification systems increasingly incorporate continuous personality disorder models. Yet, it is unclear how continuum beliefs are related to personality disorder stigma. This study evaluated the link of continuum beliefs with personality disorder stigma based on correlational and experimental data. Methods A large general population sample (N = 848) completed self-report measures of continuum beliefs regarding personality disorders, desired social distance, and prejudice toward persons with personality disorders. Additionally, participants were randomly presented with information supporting a continuous or a dichotomous view of personality disorders. Results Continuum beliefs were associated with lower desired social distance (r = − 0.19) and prejudice (r = − 0.22). Additionally, the brief continuum intervention was associated with increased continuum beliefs (d = 0.99) and decreased desired social distance (d = − 0.14) and prejudice (d = − 0.17). Finally, the intervention effects on desired social distance and prejudice were mediated by continuum beliefs. Conclusion This study suggests that highlighting continuum views on personality disorders in public communication and interventions might reduce personality disorder stigma.
... Similarly, the assumption that mental health and illness are either separated by a clear cutoff (categorical conceptualization) or both located within a continuum (continual conceptualization) can have an impact on mental health staff 's attitudes toward service users and their mental health issues (4,5). For example, a categorial view of mental health is associated with more stigmatizing attitudes (6,7), which are known to result in discriminatory behavior (8). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Mental health professionals are often affected by mental health problems and disorders. Yet, the effects of these lived experiences on their causal beliefs and health concepts have not been investigated. The current study investigates how professionals’ lived depressive experiences and their perceived vulnerability to mental illness affect their causal beliefs about mental disorders, their general concept of mental health and their specific concepts of depression and burnout. Methods An online survey was conducted with 218 mental health professionals from 18 psychiatric clinic departments in the German federal states of Berlin and Brandenburg, investigating their experiences with depression, self-assessed vulnerability, their causal beliefs of mental illness, their general health concept and specific illness concepts of depression and burnout. A path model was calculated to examine the relationships between these variables. Participants with and without lived experience of depression were grouped. Results Lived experience of depression was indicated by 126 participants. For participants with no experience of depression, perceived vulnerability negatively predicted beliefs in biological causation, which positively predicted higher differentiation between depression and burnout. For participants with previous depression experiences, perceived vulnerability positively predicted beliefs in psychological and social causation. Continuum belief was predicted only in this group by the three variables of causal beliefs. Psychological and social causation was positively associated, while biological causes were negatively associated with continuum beliefs. Conclusion Mental health professionals are not external to the clinical situation. Their lived experiences do matter, shaping their beliefs and concepts and, thus, possibly also their actions toward patients.
Article
Full-text available
Background Anorexia nervosa has one of the highest mortality rates of all mental illnesses. For those who survive, less than 70% fully recover, with many going on to develop a more severe and enduring phenotype. Research now suggests that genetics plays a role in the development and persistence of anorexia nervosa. Inclusion of participants with more severe and enduring illness in genetics studies of anorexia nervosa is critical. Objective The primary goal of this review was to assess the inclusion of participants meeting the criteria for the severe enduring anorexia nervosa phenotype in genetics research by (1) identifying the most widely used defining criteria for severe enduring anorexia nervosa and (2) performing a review of the genetics literature to assess the inclusion of participants meeting the identified criteria. Methods Searches of the genetics literature from 2012 to 2023 were performed in the PubMed, PsycINFO, and Web of Science databases. Publications were selected per the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR). The criteria used to define the severe and enduring anorexia nervosa phenotype were derived by how often they were used in the literature since 2017. The publications identified through the literature search were then assessed for inclusion of participants meeting these criteria. Results most prevalent criteria used to define severe enduring anorexia nervosa in the literature were an illness duration of ≥ 7 years, lack of positive response to at least two previous evidence-based treatments, a body mass index meeting the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5 for extreme anorexia nervosa, and an assessment of psychological and/or behavioral severity indicating a significant impact on quality of life. There was a lack of consistent identification and inclusion of those meeting the criteria for severe enduring anorexia nervosa in the genetics literature. Discussion This lack of consistent identification and inclusion of patients with severe enduring anorexia nervosa in genetics research has the potential to hamper the isolation of risk loci and the development of new, more effective treatment options for patients with anorexia nervosa.
Article
Full-text available
Zusammenfassung Eine psychische Erkrankung bedeutet für viele Betroffene auch eine Auseinandersetzung mit den Reaktionen des Umfelds. Diese werden geprägt durch kulturell vorherrschende Vorstellungen von Ursache, Behandlung, Verlauf und biografischer Bedeutung der Krankheit. Der vorliegende Artikel gibt einen Überblick über die Entwicklung der Einstellungen der deutschen Bevölkerung zu psychischen Erkrankungen zwischen 1990 und 2020 mit Schwerpunkt auf Depression und Schizophrenie. Mit Blick auf die letzten 30 Jahre zeigt sich, dass die Einstellungen zu psychischen Erkrankungen nicht statisch sind, sondern vielmehr einer Dynamik unterliegen, die sich in Abhängigkeit vom Krankheitsbild erheblich unterscheiden kann. Zusammenfassend ruft eine Depression weitaus weniger negative Gefühle hervor, als es bei der Schizophrenie der Fall ist. Dieser Unterschied hat sich in den letzten 30 Jahren vergrößert: Menschen mit Depressionen treffen heute auf mehr Verständnis als vor 30 Jahren, während die Stigmatisierung von Menschen mit Schizophrenie zugenommen hat. Neben einer zunehmenden Offenheit im Umgang mit psychischen Belastungen haben sich auch Normalitätsvorstellungen und Konzepte von psychischer Krankheit verändert. Depressive Zustandsbilder werden heute stärker mit dem eigenen Erleben in Verbindung gebracht als noch vor 10 Jahren. Eine Schizophrenie erscheint den Menschen dagegen heute eher fremdartiger. Während die Empfehlung sowohl von Psychotherapie als auch von Medikamenten für die Behandlung psychischer Krankheiten zunimmt und sowohl Psychotherapeuten als auch Psychiater häufiger als Anlaufstelle empfohlen werden, nimmt die Empfehlung spiritueller Helfer (Pfarrer, Priester) seit den 1990er-Jahren ab. Wir diskutieren mögliche Ursachen und Konsequenzen dieser divergenten Entwicklungen.
Article
Full-text available
Objective Both continuum beliefs (i.e., that mental disorder exists on a spectrum of normative behavior patterns) and the perception of similarities to a person with schizophrenia have shown mixed effects on reducing mental illness stigma. To our knowledge, this is the first study to address continuum beliefs and the perception of similarities to a person with depression in the context of depression-related stigma. Methods This work is based on an online intervention study in an ethnically diverse sample recruited on Amazon MTurk including previously unanalyzed qualitive responses. Within this cross-sectional, mixed-methods online investigation (N = 304), we examined the relation of perceived similarities to continuum beliefs, social distance, and negative stereotypes in relation to a vignette about depression. A randomly assigned continuum beliefs intervention attempted to induce continuum beliefs about depression. An open-writing task asked participants to describe similarities and/or differences between themselves and the person depicted in the vignette. Results The continuum beliefs intervention was associated to a greater number of perceived similarities to and fewer perceived differences from the target vignette. Moreover, perceived similarities were associated with increased continuum beliefs, less social distance, and less-negative stereotypes. Perceived differences from a person with depression were associated with increased social distance. Limitations Even though the continuum beliefs intervention did not significantly alter stigma measures directly, expressed continuum beliefs were associated to decreased mental illness stigma. Conclusions The findings emphasize that perceived similarities to an outgroup member (i.e., a person with depression) might augment the stigma-reducing mechanism of continuum beliefs.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose The public discourse about mental health and mental illness seems to have become more open over the last decade, giving rise to the hope that symptoms of mental illness have become more relatable. We examine whether continuum beliefs regarding schizophrenia and depression have increased on a population level over a period of 9 years, and whether notions of unfamiliarity and incomprehensibility have decreased. Methods In 2011 ( n = 2455) and 2020 ( n = 3042), two methodologically identical cross-sectional population surveys were conducted in Germany. After the presentation of an unlabeled case vignette depicting someone with schizophrenia or depression, we asked about continuum beliefs, perceived unfamiliarity and perceived incomprehensibility of the person described. Results Multinomial logit models holding sociodemographic variables fixed at their means for both surveys showed that agreement with continuum beliefs increased in depression from 43 to 46% [change 4%; 95% confidence interval (CI) 0; 8], but decreased in schizophrenia from 26 to 20% (change − 6%, 95% CI − 9; − 3). Unfamiliarity (change − 4%; 95% CI − 7; 0) and incomprehensibility (change − 7%, 95% CI − 10; − 4) decreased for depression, while remaining largely unchanged for schizophrenia. Conclusion An already pronounced gap in the perception of both disorders with regard to continuity of symptom experiences and perceived otherness further widened over the last decade. While the public’s increasing familiarity with symptoms of depression might be further aided by using continuum beliefs as part of anti-stigma messages and awareness campaigns, promulgating continuity models for schizophrenia seems more challenging.
Article
Full-text available
The present study investigated the relationships between continuum and categorical beliefs, stigma, and help-seeking in response to vignettes depicting depression and schizophrenia. Participants were 193 adolescents aged 13-18 years. Results showed no relationships between continuum beliefs and stigma or help-seeking intentions for the depression vignette when controlling for other predictors. Stronger categorical beliefs were associated with greater social distance, and endorsement of dangerousness, avoidance, and fear. For the schizophrenia vignette, higher continuum beliefs predicted lower prognostic pessimism, social distance, and stigmatising attitudes. Surprisingly, continuum beliefs predicted greater intentions to not seek help. Categorical beliefs were associated with greater prognostic pessimism, social distance, and stigmatising attitudes. Future research is needed to explore the effect of continuum belief interventions on adolescent mental health stigma.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: The notion that depression is a disorder that moves along a continuum is well-established. Similarly, the belief in the continuity of mental illness is considered an important element in the stigma process. Against this background, it is the aim of this study to examine whether public continuum beliefs vary with the severity of depressive symptoms. Methods: Analyses were based on computer-assisted telephone interviews (CATIs) conducted in winter 2019/2020 in Germany (N = 1,009, response rate 46.8%). Using three vignettes representing mild, moderate, and severe depressive symptoms, beliefs regarding the continuity of symptoms, specifically a fundamental difference, were assessed with seven items. Sociodemographic characteristics and own experiences with depression (affliction and contact) were introduced as covariates. Results: Significant differences between the three groups of severity were found for the majority of the items measuring continuum beliefs or perceived fundamental difference. However, only few items showed a linear trend indicating a parallel between symptom severity and beliefs. Multivariate regression models showed that a moderate degree of depression was positively associated with stronger continuum beliefs but also with greater perceived difference compared to the mild degree, while no significant associations emerged for the severe vignette. Limitations: Although a comparison of our sample with official statistics supports the external validity, we cannot rule out a selection bias. It is arguable in how far short case vignettes convey a holistic picture of a person affected by depressive symptoms. Conclusion: Our results do not indicate a parallel between symptom severity and public continuum beliefs.
Article
Full-text available
Objectif : La Commission de la santé mentale du Canada (CSMC) utilise un modèle de continuum de soins en santé mentale (MCSSM) en tant qu’outil d’autoévaluation dans nombre de ses programmes éducatifs contre la stigmatisation, plutôt qu’une conceptualisation dichotomique de la santé et de la maladie mentale. Cette étude a évalué la consistance interne et la validité des critères du modèle. Méthode : Des étudiants à l’université (N = 454) et des adultes de la communauté (N = 976) ont rempli eux-mêmes un questionnaire du modèle de continuum, ainsi que le GAD-7 (General Anxiety Disorder-7), le PHQ-9 (Patient Health Questionnaire-9) et l’AUDIT (Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test). La consistance interne a été évaluée au moyen d’analyses factorielles, de la mesure de la fiabilité interne et d’une série d’analyses de régression. La validité selon les critères a été évaluée au moyen de la correspondance entre les scores à l’AUDIT, au GAD7 et au PHQ9, ainsi qu’aux évaluations selon le MCSSM. Résultats : La plupart des analyses ont indiqué une consistance interne des items du MCSSM, à l’exception de la consommation d’alcool qui n’était pas reliée de façon constante à l’état perçu de la santé mentale. Les données suggèrent de fortes associations entre le MCSSM et l’anxiété et la dépression, mais un lien moins fort pour une mesure validée de la consommation d’alcool. Conclusions : Cette étude a révélé d’importants aspects du modèle de continuum de soins en santé mentale qu’utilise la CSMC et a fourni des constatations préliminaires au sujet de sa consistance interne et de la validité selon les critères. Il est recommandé d’effectuer la réplique de cette étude avec d’autres échantillons et de mener d’autres recherches de validation pour ce modèle.
Article
Full-text available
Background Promulgating a continuum model of mental health and mental illness has been proposed as a way to reduce stigma by decreasing notions of differentness. This systematic review and meta-analysis examines whether continuum beliefs are associated with lower stigma, and whether continuum interventions reduce stigma. Methods Following a pre-defined protocol (PROSPERO: CRD42019123606), we searched three electronic databases (PubMed, Web of Science, and PsycINFO) yielding 6726 studies. After screening, we included 33 studies covering continuum beliefs, mental illness, and stigma. Of these, 13 studies were included in meta-analysis. Results Continuum beliefs are consistently associated with lower stigma. Interventions were effective at manipulating continuum beliefs but differ in their effects on stigmatising attitudes. Conclusions We discuss whether and to what extent attitudes towards people with mental illness can be improved by providing information on a mental health-mental illness continuum. It appeared to be relevant whether interventions promoted a feeling of ‘us’ and a process of identification with the person with mental illness. We discuss implications for the design of future interventions.
Article
Full-text available
Aims The theory of ‘what matters most’ (WMM) has been developed to understand differences in mental illness stigma between cultures, postulating that stigma becomes most pervasive in situations that matter most in a specific cultural context. The rise of populism in Western societies demonstrates that also within one cultural context, different values ‘matter most’ to different groups. We expand the WMM framework to explore the spectrum of stigma manifestations within Western societies, relating it to both conservative/authoritarian and liberal/modern values. From our findings, we will develop hypotheses on how further research into value orientations and stigma might address potential blind spots in stigma research. Methods Based on a narrative review of the literature on mental illness stigma and value orientations, we apply the WMM framework to cultural mechanisms of stigma within modern Western societies. Results There are several studies showing an association between traditional, authoritarian, conservative values with stronger mental illness stigma, while studies examining the stigma within liberal, modern value orientations are scarce. We hypothesise on situations where encountering a person with mental illness could threaten liberal values and thus might provoke stigma among persons with such value orientations. For example, living with a person with mental illness could be seen as consuming energy and time, thereby jeopardising ‘self-actualisation’, the modern value of realising one's own full potential. As a result, a person highly valuing self-actualisation might try to avoid contact with persons with mental illness. Instances of potential ‘liberal stigma’ also include structural stigma or self-stigma, when, e.g. changing assumptions of what is considered ‘normal’ increase perceptions of being fundamentally different when experiencing mental illness. Conclusions ‘WMM’ appears to be a useful framework to direct research to potential blind spots within the field of stigma research. Looking at instances where liberal values conflict with dealing with a person with mental illness could provide a more comprehensive understanding of stigma experiences among persons with mental illness. However, for measuring stigma, tapping into liberal variations of mental illness stigma is methodologically challenging. Qualitative work could be the first step to elicit potential stigma experiences based on conflicts with liberal values.
Article
Full-text available
Continuum belief interventions that erode boundaries between “normal” individuals and individuals with psychiatric problems may help to reduce psychiatric stigma, but a number of questions persist. The magnitude of belief change attributable to the intervention is unclear. Moreover, most studies have executed continuum interventions to reduce stigma of schizophrenia, and all studies have examined intervention effects on only public stigma. This study utilized a large sample (n = 654) to examine effects of a continuum intervention on depression stigma—public stigma in the full sample and self-stigma among participants with a self-reported history of depression. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three intervention groups: (a) the control group, which read material that merely described depression, (b) the continuum group, which read material that attested to a continuum view of depression, or (c) the categorical group, which read material that attested to a categorical view of depression. Correlational analyses demonstrated that preintervention categorical belief positively predicted, and preintervention continuum belief negatively predicted, depression stigma. Moreover, preintervention categorical belief positively predicted, and preintervention continuum belief negatively predicted, self-stigma among participants with a self-reported history of depression. There was scant evidence that the intervention affected public stigma among participants without a history of depression and no evidence that it affected self-stigma among participants with a history of depression. These findings illuminate a number of key priorities for future research on continuum belief intervention and its prospects for stigma reduction.
Article
Full-text available
The stigma accompanying major depressive disorder (MDD) increases distress and misery worldwide and may incur worse consequences than the condition itself. Over the last decades public knowledge about mental illnesses has increased, but public mental illness stigma has not decreased. Recent research suggests that endorsement of the continuum perspective on mental illness, particularly depression, has the potential to reduce stigma. We examined the impact of the continuum perspective on mental illness stigma (social distance, negative stereotypes) regarding MDD, hypothesizing that an increased endorsement of the continuum perspective would be associated with lower levels of stigma. Participants, recruited through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk, included 487 adults, randomly assigned to one of two conditions containing different information about the nature of depression (continuum vs. categorical perspective) or to a control condition with neutral information. After reading a vignette about someone suffering from depression, participants completed a continuum-beliefs measure and stigma-related questionnaires. Sociodemographic variables, along with the variable of positive former contact with someone experiencing mental illness, were measured. The intervention did not reduce stigma toward individuals with depression. Higher mental health continuum beliefs were related to lower stigma against people with MDD across groups. Moreover, people assigned to the continuum intervention had significantly higher continuum beliefs; positive former contact was positively related to both continuum beliefs and lowered stigma. While findings suggest continuum beliefs and stigma are correlated, we discuss reasons why interventions seem to not (or only partially) affect stigma directly. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Most research investigating the effect of continuum beliefs on stigma has used weak manipulations which may contribute to mixed findings within the experimental literature. There is also a lack of research into how continuum belief manipulations may impact help-seeking and help-provision. This study used an online manipulation of continuum and categorical beliefs about schizophrenia to examine the subsequent impacts on stigma, help-seeking, and help-provision. A total of 271 participants were randomised into either a continuum, categorical, or control condition. Participants received an informational video, a magazine article, and research highlights relevant to their condition. Prosocial support behaviour was assessed through a novel volunteering measure. The magnitude of change between pre-intervention and post-intervention measures of continuum and categorical beliefs was large. Continuum presentations reduced prognostic pessimism and negative stereotyping. Meanwhile, categorical presentations increased prognostic pessimism. Participants across conditions showed increased help-providing intentions after removing the highest scores to avoid ceiling effects. Fear and blame also decreased significantly across all conditions. There was no difference between conditions on our novel volunteer help-provision measure. Our findings have implications for anti-stigma programs and may help inform the design of future continuum belief manipulations.