ArticlePDF Available

Spatial Learning Promotes Adult Neurogenesis in Specific Regions of the Zebrafish Pallium

Frontiers
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology
Authors:
  • Laboratorio de Radiobiología y Biodosimetría-CNEA/INTECNUS

Abstract and Figures

Adult neurogenesis could be considered as a homeostatic mechanism that accompanies the continuous growth of teleost fish. As an alternative but not excluding hypothesis, adult neurogenesis would provide a form of plasticity necessary to adapt the brain to environmental challenges. The zebrafish pallium is a brain structure involved in the processing of various cognitive functions and exhibits extended neurogenic niches throughout the periventricular zone. The involvement of neuronal addition as a learning-related plastic mechanism has not been explored in this model, yet. In this work, we trained adult zebrafish in a spatial behavioral paradigm and evaluated the neurogenic dynamics in different pallial niches. We found that adult zebrafish improved their performance in a cue-guided rhomboid maze throughout five daily sessions, being the fish able to relearn the task after a rule change. This cognitive activity increased cell proliferation exclusively in two pallial regions: the caudal lateral pallium (cLP) and the rostral medial pallium (rMP). To assessed whether learning impinges on pallial adult neurogenesis, mitotic cells were labeled by BrdU administration, and then fish were trained at different periods of adult-born neuron maturation. Our results indicate that adult-born neurons are being produced on demand in rMP and cLP during the learning process, but with distinct critical periods among these regions. Next, we evaluated the time course of adult neurogenesis by pulse and chase experiments. We found that labeled cells decreased between 4 and 32 dpl in both learning-sensitive regions, whereas a fraction of them continues proliferating over time. By modeling the population dynamics of neural stem cells (NSC), we propose that learning increases adult neurogenesis by two mechanisms: driving a chained proliferation of labeled NSC and rescuing newborn neurons from death. Our findings highlight adult neurogenesis as a conserved source of brain plasticity and shed light on a rostro-caudal specialization of pallial neurogenic niches in adult zebrafish.
| Sustained training of zebrafish from 12-30 dpl increases adult neurogenesis exclusively in the cLP and the rMP. (A). Experimental schedule. Fish were immersed in BrdU, and training started at 12 dpl. Training consisted of 15 sessions, distributed in three slots. Every five sessions both cues and exit position were changed. A 2-day interval was fixed between slots. At 30 dpl fish were euthanised for histology. (B). Learning curves for Trained and Control individuals (Two-way RM ANOVA, Treatment effect: F (1, 14) = 67.71 with p < 0.0001, Session effect: F (14, 196) = 6.805 with p < 0.0001. Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test, * depicts p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ****p < 0.0001. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (C). Simple linear regression for each training window for Trained fish (ANCOVA, first vs. 2nd week: F (1,76) = 4.647; 1st vs. 3rd week: F (1,76) = 7.068, * indicates p < 0.05, **, p < 0.01. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (D). BrdU + NeuN + cell quantification in LP (Two-way ANOVA, Treatment effect: F (1, 41) = 2.766 with p = 0.103, Pallium region effect: F (3, 41) = 4.493 with p = 0.0098. Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test, **p < 0.01. Trained, N = 7; Control N = 6). (E). Top: Neuronal fate (%BrdU + NeuN + /BrdU + cells) in caudal LP (Unpaired t test, t (10) = 0.5091, n. s. Trained, N = 7; Control N = 5). Bottom: Cell migration in caudal LP (Mann Whitney test, U = 2419, n. s. Trained, N = 7; Control N = 6). (F). Adult-born neurons (BrdU/NeuN) in cLP for Trained and Control individuals. Scale bar, (Continued )
… 
| Training zebrafish during an early period (3-14 dpl) increases adult neurogenesis in the rMP. (A). Experimental design. (B). Learning curves for Trained and Control individuals (Two-way RM ANOVA, Treatment effect: F (1, 14) = 126.1 with p < 0.0001, Session effect: F (9, 126) = 5.254 with p < 0.0001. Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test, *** depicts p < 0.001, ****, p < 0.0001. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (C). Simple linear regression for each training window for Trained fish (ANCOVA, F (1, 76) = 1.516, n. s. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (D). BrdU + NeuN + cell quantification in LP. (Two-way ANOVA, Treatment effect: F (1, 54) = 10.63 with p = 0.0019, Pallium region effect: F (3, 54) = 8.642 with p < 0.0001. Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test, not significant differences. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (E). Top: Neuronal fate (%BrdU + NeuN + /BrdU + cells) in cLP. (Unpaired t test, t (14) = 4.749, *** depicts p < 0.001. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). Bottom: Cell migration in cLP (Mann Whitney test, U = 6492, n. s. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (F). Adult-born neurons (BrdU + NeuN + ) in cLP for Trained and Control individuals. Scale bar, 20 μm. (G). BrdU + NeuN + cell quantification in MP. (Two-way ANOVA, Treatment effect: F (1, 56) = 15.34 with p = 0.0002, Pallium region effect: F (3, 56) = 2.685 with p = 0.0553. Bonferroni's multiple comparisons test, * depicts p < 0.05. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (H). Top: Neuronal fate (%BrdU + NeuN + /BrdU + cells) in rMP (Unpaired t test, t (14) = 1.396, n. s. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). Bottom: Cell migration in rMP (Mann Whitney test, U = 2088, n. s. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (I). Adult-born neurons (BrdU + NeuN + ) in rMP for Trained and Control individuals. Scale bar, 20 μm. (I'). Higher magnification of the boxed square in I (merge panel). Single focal plane and orthogonal views after three-dimension reconstruction.
… 
Content may be subject to copyright.
Spatial Learning Promotes Adult
Neurogenesis in Specic Regions of
the Zebrash Pallium
Laura S. Mazzitelli-Fuentes
1
,
2
,
3
, Fernanda R. Román
1
,
2
,
4
, Julio R. Castillo Elías
1
,
5
,
Emilia B. Deleglise
1
,
2
,
3
and Lucas A. Mongiat
1
,
2
*
1
Departamento de Física Médica, Centro Atómico Bariloche, Comisión Nacional de Energía Atómica, San Carlos de Bariloche,
Argentina,
2
Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Cientícas y, Técnicas, Argentina,
3
Instituto Balseiro, Centro Atómico Bariloche,
San Carlos de Bariloche, Argentina,
4
Centro Regional Universitario Bariloche, Universidad Nacional del Comahue, San Carlos de
Bariloche, Argentina,
5
Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos
Aires, Argentina
Adult neurogenesis could be considered as a homeostatic mechanism that accompanies
the continuous growth of teleost sh. As an alternative but not excluding hypothesis, adult
neurogenesis would provide a form of plasticity necessary to adapt the brain to
environmental challenges. The zebrash pallium is a brain structure involved in the
processing of various cognitive functions and exhibits extended neurogenic niches
throughout the periventricular zone. The involvement of neuronal addition as a
learning-related plastic mechanism has not been explored in this model, yet. In this
work, we trained adult zebrash in a spatial behavioral paradigm and evaluated the
neurogenic dynamics in different pallial niches. We found that adult zebrash improved
their performance in a cue-guided rhomboid maze throughout ve daily sessions, being the
sh able to relearn the task after a rule change. This cognitive activity increased cell
proliferation exclusively in two pallial regions: the caudal lateral pallium (cLP) and the rostral
medial pallium (rMP). To assessed whether learning impinges on pallial adult neurogenesis,
mitotic cells were labeled by BrdU administration, and then sh were trained at different
periods of adult-born neuron maturation. Our results indicate that adult-born neurons are
being produced on demand in rMP and cLP during the learning process, but with distinct
critical periods among these regions. Next, we evaluated the time course of adult
neurogenesis by pulse and chase experiments. We found that labeled cells decreased
between 4 and 32 dpl in both learning-sensitive regions, whereas a fraction of them
continues proliferating over time. By modeling the population dynamics of neural stem cells
(NSC), we propose that learning increases adult neurogenesis by two mechanisms: driving
a chained proliferation of labeled NSC and rescuing newborn neurons from death. Our
ndings highlight adult neurogenesis as a conserved source of brain plasticity and shed
light on a rostro-caudal specialization of pallial neurogenic niches in adult zebrash.
Keywords: neural stem/progenitor cells, plasticity, telencephalon, danio rerio (zebrash), spatial learning and
memory
Edited by:
Ma Salomé Sirerol Piquer,
Center for Biomedical Research on
Neurodegenerative Diseases
(CIBERNED), Spain
Reviewed by:
Kenji Shimamura,
Kumamoto University, Japan
María Esmeralda Castelló,
Instituto de Investigaciones Biológicas
Clemente Estable (IIBCE), Uruguay
*Correspondence:
Lucas A. Mongiat
lucas.mongiat@cab.cnea.gov.ar
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Stem Cell Research,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental
Biology
Received: 21 December 2021
Accepted: 29 March 2022
Published: 11 May 2022
Citation:
Mazzitelli-Fuentes LS, Román FR,
Castillo Elías JR, Deleglise EB and
Mongiat LA (2022) Spatial Learning
Promotes Adult Neurogenesis in
Specic Regions of the
Zebrash Pallium.
Front. Cell Dev. Biol. 10:840964.
doi: 10.3389/fcell.2022.840964
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 8409641
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 11 May 2022
doi: 10.3389/fcell.2022.840964
INTRODUCTION
Brain plasticity allows neuronal circuits to adapt to
environmental demands, promoting the ability to cope with
the surrounding world. Adult neurogenesis, the generation and
integration of new neurons in the brain of adult organisms,
constitute a major source of brain plasticity (Mongiat and
Schinder, 2011;Sailor et al., 2017;Toda and Gage, 2018). The
occurrence of neurogenesis in adult brains extends throughout
the vertebrate subphylum, although its magnitude varies greatly
along phylogeny (Kaslin et al., 2008;Grandel and Brand, 2013;
Alunni and Bally-Cuif, 2016;Zupanc, 2021). In mammals, adult
neurogenesis is restricted mostly to the hippocampal dentate
gyrus and the olfactory bulb (Lledo et al., 2006), whereas in teleost
sh adult-born neurons are generated throughout their brain
(Kaslin et al., 2008).
The zebrash (Danio rerio) possesses a great neurogenic
potential evidenced by constitutive neural stem cell (NSC)
proliferation in almost all subdivisions of their brain,
generating a variety of adult-born neurons (Zupanc et al.,
2005;Adolf et al., 2006;Grandel et al., 2006). One of the main
brain regions in which adult neurogenesis has been studied in
zebrash is the dorsal telencephalon, or pallium. In contrast to
other vertebrates, the telencephalon of ray-nned sh develops by
an eversion of the neural tube (Folgueira et al., 2012). In
consequence, both pallial hemispheres are separated and
enclosed by a T-shaped ventricle. In this structure, the
neuronal progenitors are localized in the periventricular zone
(Adolf et al., 2006;Grandel et al., 2006). In the zebrash pallium,
there are different kinds of neural progenitor subtypes. Some of
them, notably radial glia (RG) of the pallium, are considered
NSCs, resembling the pallial RG in the mouse adult neurogenic
niches (Than-Trong and Bally-cuif, 2015). These NSCs can be
classied according to their proliferation activity, such as
quiescent and active NSCs. At any time, the active NSCs
correspond to ~5% of total RG in the periventricular zone
(März et al., 2010). In turn, the active NSCs can suffer
symmetric or asymmetric divisions to maintain the NSC
reservoir and give rise to progeny with neurogenic potential
(Than-Trong et al., 2020). Moreover, there is a different subset
of neuronal progenitors in the pallial periventricular zone. These
cells are negative for astroglial markers and exhibit intense
mitotic activity with neurogenic commitment (Rothenaigner
et al., 2011;Than-Trong et al., 2020). Previous studies
proposed that this neuronal progenitor population resembles
the Transit Amplifying Progenitors described in rodents (März
et al., 2010).
After adopting neuronal phenotype, the new neurons migrate
radially a few micrometers into the pallial parenchyma and
become integrated into the adult pallial networks. This
neurogenic process leads to an outside-in architecture, where
neurons generated in early development are positioned at the
center of the pallium, while a layered gradient of newer neurons is
distributed towards the periphery (Furlan et al., 2017). The new
neurons mature and integrate into the pallial neuronal networks
(Grandel et al., 2006;Rothenaigner et al., 2011;Lange et al., 2020;
Than-Trong et al., 2020). Recently, Lange and coworkers
performed single-cell sequencing of NSCs progeny to
characterize the intrinsic heterogeneity of adult-born neurons
in the zebrash telencephalon, revealing a striking homology with
the neuronal types found in the mammalian forebrain (Lange
et al., 2020). Therefore, adult neurogenesis contributes to a
continuous rearrangement of zebrashs pallial networks.
Several studies have been conducted to decipher the
organization of the teleost pallium based on anatomical
characterization and the expression of molecular markers
(Wullimann and Mueller, 2004;Vargas et al., 2009;Mueller,
2011;Harvey-Girard et al., 2012;Ganz et al., 2015). Current
knowledge on the functional role of the different pallial regions, is
complemented with a series of functional studies involving
distinct learning paradigms in combination with the detection
of neuronal activity (by different proxies) and/or lesions on
specic regions (Portavella et al., 2004;Durán et al., 2010;
Trotha et al., 2014;Elliott et al., 2017;Lal et al., 2018;Ausas
et al., 2019). All these works indicate that the teleost dorsomedial
telencephalic region (medial pallium, MP) is homologus to the
basolateral amygdala of mammals, whereas the dorsolateral
telencephalic region (lateral pallium, LP) is homologus to the
mammalian hippocampus. From these studies emerge a strong
relation between the teleost pallium and cognitive activity,
suggesting plastic changes on pallial networks. In particular,
adult neurogenesis could play a relevant role by shaping the
neuronal circuits during learning. In rodents, it is known that
behavioral paradigms such as odor-related and hippocampus-
dependent learning increase the survival of immature adult-born
neurons (Lledo et al., 2006;Anderson et al., 2011;Aasebø et al.,
2018). However, to our knowledge, the role of cognitive activity
on pallial network remodeling has not been explored in
teleosts yet.
In this study, we trained adult zebrash in a cognitive task to
evaluate the impact of sustained neuronal activity on pallial adult
neurogenesis. We found that learning may lead to plastic network
remodeling driven by adult neurogenesis, specically in the
rostral MP and caudal LP regions.
RESULTS
Adult Zebrash Learn in the Cue-Guided
Rhomboid Maze
We hypothesized that sustained pallial neuronal activity,
triggered as a consequence of a learning routine, would
enhance the rate of adult neurogenesis in the circuits
underlying the cognitive function. For this purpose, we looked
for a learning paradigm involving the function of specic pallial
structures, together with a sustained cognitive challenge. We
adapted a cue-guided rhomboid maze, developed for goldsh,
in order to train adult zebrash in learning a spatial task by
relating external cues with their internal-directional information.
Otherwise noticed, 10 ± 1 months-old adult AB-wild type
zebrash (Danio rerio) were used throughout this work.
During training, sh were located randomly at one of the two
possible start sites in each trial. The sh had to learn the
relationship between the cuesposition and the exit to solve
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 8409642
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
FIGURE 1 | Training adult zebrash in a rhomboid maze boosts cell proliferation in the rMP and the cLP. (A). Experimental device. The maze contains two starting
boxes, and two possible exits, one of which is blocked with a glass barrier. Fish were trained to nd the correct exit, orientating themselves with cues placed on two walls.
On the edges of the tank, two glass enclosures contained shs conspecics as social reward. On each trial, sh were placed in a start box, and, once they reached the
central arena, a correct choice was scored if they swam through the exit, and a failure if they bumped against the glass barrier. Each daily session consisted of 24
trials. (A)Behavioral schedule. Cue patterns and exit position (arrow) are specied above. Fish were habituated to the experimental tank for 2 days, and subsequently
trained for ve consecutive days. (B). Learning curves for Trained and Control individual s. Trained sh reach the learning criterion after ve consecutive sessions. Controls
do not exhibit a learning curve (Two-way RM ANOVA, Treatment effect: F
(1, 14)
= 25.78 with p= 0.0002, Session effect: F
(4, 56)
= 3.422 with p= 0.0143. Bonferronis
multiple comparisons test, **** depicts p<0.0001. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). Dashed line at 70% correct choices depicts learning criterion; dashed line at 50%
correct choices indicates random choice. (C). Simple linear regression for Trained and Control individual s (ANCOVA, F
(1, 77)
= 9.010. **** depicts p<0.0001. Trained, N =
8; Control N = 8). Dashed line indicates 95% condence intervals. (D). Left: Sagitta l schematic view of zebrash forebrain, indicating the position of the cross sections on
the right. Right: Cross sections of zebrash telencephalon along rostro-caudal axis (R: rostral, RM: rostro-medial, MC: medio-caudal, C: caudal). The colored regions
depict MP and LP. (E). PCNA
+
cells in LP. (Two-way ANOVA, Treatment effect: F
(1, 37)
= 3.873 with p= 0.0566, Pallium region effect: F
(3, 37)
= 20.27 with p<0.0001.
Bonferronis multiple comparisons test, ** denotes p<0.001. Trained, N = 6; Control, N = 6). (F). Cross sections of telencephalic pallium immunostained for PCNA in cLP
of Trained and Control individuals. Scale bar, 50 μm. Scale bar in higher magnications, 20 μm. Black arrows indicate representative PCNA
+
cells. (G). PCNA
+
cells in
MP. (Two-way ANOVA, Treatment effect: F
(1, 38)
= 7.796 with p= 0.0082, Pallium region effect: F
(3, 38)
= 2.332 with p= 0.0895. Bonferronis multiple comparisons test, *
denotes p<0.05. Trained, N = 6; Control, N = 6). (H). Cross sections of telencephalic pallium immun ostained for PCNA in rMP of Trained and Control individuals. Scale
bar, 50 μm. Scale bar in higher magnications, 20 μm. Black arrows indicate representative PCNA
+
cells.
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 8409643
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
the maze; for example, turn right if cues are located at the same
side of the start compartment and turn left if they are at the
opposite side (Figure 1A). Thus, on each trial the sh must take a
left-right decision based on cuesposition. To encourage the
completion of the task, we placed several conspecics in outer
compartments as social reward. To assess the ability of zebrash
to learn the task, sh were trained for ve consecutive sessions (1
session/day, 24 trials/session, Figure 1A). The Control group
consisted on sh which were subjected to the same training
routine, with the exception that the glass barrier was randomly
placed in any of the exits to avoid learning. The Control group
exhibited a ~50% correct choices throughout the sessions,
remaining close to the chance level (Figure 1B,C,
Supplementary Video S1). In contrast, the Trained group
increased their performance in a daily manner until they
exceeded a 75% of correct choices (Percentage of correct
choices for Session 5, Mean ± SE: Control 50.00 ± 2.362;
Trained 78.65 ± 2.992; N = 8, Supplementary Video S2). It
should be noticed that all trained subjects accomplished the
learning criterion and were included in further analysis. Here,
we introduce the cue-guided rhomboid maze as a novel
behavioral paradigm to assess spatial learning ability in adult
zebrash.
Learning-Induced Cell Proliferation in Adult
Zebrash Pallium
Previous studies in goldsh reported that learning in the cue-
guided rhomboid maze induces a selective increase of metabolic
activity in the LP of trained subjects (Durán et al., 2015;Ocaña
et al., 2017). Thus, we aimed to test whether learning in the
rhomboid maze task would have any effect on cell proliferation in
this neurogenic niche. It must be noticed that the Control group is
subjected to manipulations, environment conditions and social
reward in the same way as Trained subjects. Hence, changes in
proliferation and/or neurogenesis should be related to the
learning process.
To evaluate mitotic activity, we determined the expression of
the Proliferating Cell Nuclear Antigen (PCNA) throughout the
rostro-caudal axis of the pallium (Figures 1DH,Supplementary
Figures S1AE). Expression of PCNA in the pallium was
detected from the rostral to the caudal regions, in the
periventricular zone. Supporting our hypothesis we found a
learning-related increase in the PCNA detection in the LP,
only restricted to its caudal region (cLP, ~180%; Figures
1E,F). Unexpectedly, we also observed a relevant learning-
related gain in PCNA levels in the rostral region of MP (rMP,
~220%, Figures 1G,H).
It is known that senescence leads to cognitive deterioration, a
fact that has also been proven for zebrash (Ruhl et al., 2015;
Adams and Kafaligonul, 2018;Yang et al., 2018). Thus, we
explored whether aged zebrash (21 months-old) could learn
in the cue-guided rhomboid maze and, consequently, increase
proliferation rates in the pallium. As observed for adult-young
zebrash, senescent individuals improve their task performance
in a daily manner (Supplementary Figure S2). In agreement with
our previous observations, aged sh exhibit an enhancement of
PCNA expression exclusively in the rMP (~180%) and the cLP
(~250%) of Trained sh (Supplementary Figure S2).
Interestingly, we observed a slight improvement in the
learning curve and in the basal levels of proliferation, in the
aged zebrash (Figure 1 and Supplementary Figure S2).
Our results indicate that cognitive activity, carried out on the
cue-guided rhomboid maze, induces an increase in cell
proliferation in two delimited neurogenic regions of the
pallium of adult zebrash.
Enhanced Adult Neurogenesis by Learning
in the Rhomboid Maze
Adult neurogenesis is a plastic phenomenon that can be
modulated by network activity at different levels, such as
NSCs proliferation, migration, differentiation and survival of
new neurons. However, the role of cognitive activity on adult
neurogenesis modulation has not been explored in zebrash, yet.
We hypothesized that sustained neuronal activity triggered as a
consequence of learning impinges on immature adult-born
neurons to promote pallial network remodeling. To approach
this question we treated adult sh (~10 months-old) with BrdU, a
thymidine analogue, to label NSCs during the S-phase of mitotic
cell division. We allowed BrdU-labeled neurons to develop over a
12-day period. Then, sh were randomly divided into Trained
and Control (mocked) groups, and subjected to sustained
training in the rhomboid maze from 12 to 30 dpl, a period in
which we estimate that adult-born neurons are maturing
(Figure 2A). To maintain the cognitive challenge during the
training period, we changed the cues and the learning rule at the
beginning of each week. Trained subjects exhibited a good
learning curve, reaching the established criterion at the fth
session of the rst week, and were able to relearn the task
after the rule shift during the following weeks (Figure 2B,
Percentage of correct choices for Session 5, Mean ± SE:
Control 57.81 ± 2.54; Trained 76.56 ± 3.14, N = 8). Notably,
after the rst week of training, the subjects showed a better
performance when relearning the task, evidenced by an increase
in the learning curve slope of Trained sh (Figure 2C, Simple
linear regression, 1
st
week vs. 2 weeks: p= 0.0343; 1
st
week vs.
3
rd
week: p= 0.0096).
After completing the training schedule, we assessed the fate of
BrdU-labeled cells by immunodetection of the neuronal marker
NeuN. The effect of learning on adult neurogenesis was evaluated
by quantifying the number of adult-born neurons (BrdU
+
NeuN
+
)
throughout the pallial rostro-caudal axis. We found that the cLP
of Trained subjects exhibited an increase (~ 250%) in the number
of 30 dpl BrdU-labeled neurons as compared to Control group,
whereas the other rostro-caudal LP regions shown similar levels
of 30 dpl adult-born neurons between both experimental groups
(Figures 2D,F, BrdU
+
NeuN
+
in cLP, Mean ± SE: Control 44.39 ±
12.71, N = 5; Trained 112.28 ± 16.41, N = 7). Similarly, in the MP
we observed an increase of BrdU-labeled neurons (>300%)
exclusively in the rostral region of the MP (Figures 2G,I,I,
BrdU
+
NeuN
+
in rMP, Mean ± SE: Control 57.73 ± 26.00, N =
5; Trained 204.50 ± 56.36, N = 7). Since network activity may
regulate adult-born neurons development, we decided to evaluate
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 8409644
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
FIGURE 2 | Sustained training of zebrash from 1230 dpl increases adult neurogenesis exclusively in the cLP and the rMP. (A). Experimental schedule. Fish were
immersed in BrdU, and training started at 12 dpl. Training consisted of 15 sessions, distributed in three slots. Every ve sessions both cues and exit position were
changed. A 2-day interval was xed between slots. At 30 dpl sh were euthanised for histology. (B). Learning curves for Trained and Control individuals (Two-way RM
ANOVA, Treatment effect: F
(1, 14)
= 67.71 with p<0.0001, Session effect: F
(14, 196)
= 6.805 with p<0.0001. Bonferronis multiple comparisons test, * depicts p<
0.05, **p<0.01, ****p<0.0001.Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (C). Simple linear regression for each training window for Trained sh (ANCOVA, rst vs. 2nd week: F
(1,76)
=
4.647; 1st vs. 3rd week: F
(1,76)
= 7.068, * indicates p<0.05, **, p<0.01. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (D). BrdU
+
NeuN
+
cell quantication in LP (Two-way ANOVA,
Treatment effect: F
(1, 41)
=2.766withp= 0.103, Pallium region effect: F
(3, 41)
=4.493withp= 0.0098. Bonferronis multiple comparisons test, **p<0.01. Trained, N = 7;
Control N = 6). (E). Top: Neuronalfate (%BrdU
+
NeuN
+
/BrdU
+
cells) in caudalLP (Unpaired ttest, t
(10)
= 0.5091, n. s.Trained, N = 7; Control N = 5).Bottom: Cell migration in
caudal LP (Mann Whitney test, U = 2419, n. s. Trained, N = 7; Control N = 6). (F). Adult-born neurons (BrdU/NeuN) in cLP for Trained and Control individuals. Scale bar,
(Continued )
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 8409645
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
if learning affects neuronal differentiation, assessed as the
percentage of labeled cells which adopt neuronal phenotype
(BrdU-labeled cells that express the neuronal marker NeuN,
BrdU
+
NeuN
+
/BrdU
+
), as well as the migration distance of
BrdU-labeled cells, measured from the ventricular boundary
into the nal position in the pallial parenchyma
(Supplementary Figure S3). We analyzed only the regions in
which learning promotes adult neurogenesis. Neither of these
parameters were affected by learning, except for the migration
distance in rMP, which was lower in Trained subjects (Figures
2E,H). The difference in migration distance may account for
late recruited BrdU-labeled progenitors, which were product of
conservative division and now are being activated by learning, a
concept that we are exploring below. Thus, it is expected that
younger BrdU-labeled neurons remains close to the
periventricular zone. These results indicate that training in
the rhomboidal maze task during a 1230 dpl time frame
promotes adult neurogenesis in the cLP and in the rMP,
shedding light on the potential relevance of these pallial
regions during learning.
Critical Period for Learning-Induced Adult
Neurogenesis
Our previous results indicate that learning has an immediate
effect on cell proliferation in the cLP and rMP neurogenic regions
(Figure 1). Also we found a learning-related increase of adult-
born neurons in the in the same pallial regions after a 1230 dpl
training (Figure 2). The neurogenic process involves several
checkpoints where network activity and systemic signaling
regulate distinct developmental processes in a time-dependent
manner (Mu et al., 2010). On these basis, we evaluated if learning
has an impact on adult-born neurons at different maturation
periods. We did not observe signicant changes between the
Control and Trained groups at any rostro-caudal region of LP
when training occurred during an earlier maturation stage of
adult-born neurons (314 dpl, Figures 3AD,F). On the other
hand, we found that learning stimulates an increase (~200%) in
the levels of rMP 30 dpl adult-born neurons (Figures 3G,I,I,
BrdU
+
NeuN
+
in rMP, Mean ± SE: Control 94.20 ± 17.61; Trained
188.30 ± 36.01, N = 8). Furthermore, we evaluated neuronal fate
and observed a signicant increase in the percentage of newborn
neurons only in the cLP. The nal position of the labeled neurons
(migration distance) showed no differences at any region
(Figures 3E,H).
Next, we assessed adult neurogenesis after training sh during
a late period, at 3142 dpl (Figures 4AC). Both experimental
groups exhibited similar levels of neurogenesis (Figure 4D). Our
results indicate that adult neurogenesis in the rMP and the cLP is
sensitive to training in the rhomboid maze, during restricted
periods (Figure 4E). The critical period for learning-induced
adult neurogenesis is slightly shorter in the cLP as compared to
the rMP. This modulation of adult neurogenesis by network
activity could be attributed to two possible non-excluding
mechanisms: 1) activity-related increase in the survival of
adult-born neurons, avoiding cell death programs; 2) activity-
related chained recruitment of BrdU-labeled cells, which were
product of conservative divisions.
Chained Proliferation of Labeled NSCs and
Adult-Born Cell Death Modulate Adult
Neurogenesis in the Pallium
In rodents, it is well established that network activity promotes
synaptic integration of adult-born neurons and, in consequence,
favor the neuronal survival (Ryu et al., 2016). Hitherto, this
hypothesis has not been explored in teleost sh, yet. On the
other hand, Than-Trong and coworkers (2020) proposed that
adult neurogenesis relies on different kinds of NSCs, a reservoir
pool involved in the self-renewal of NSCs (rNSC), and an
operational group with neurogenic function (oNSC) (Than-
Trong et al., 2020). The oNSCs can divide either in a
symmetric or asymmetric way to produce new neurons and to
preserve the NSC pool. In this context, our adult neurogenesis
results would rely on learning-induced sequential recruitment of
BrdU-labeled cells or, alternatively, on an activity-dependent
rescue from death. Therefore, we assessed a time course for
neuronal survival and proliferation of EdU-labeled cells at four
temporal points: 4, 16, 32 and 64 dpl (Figure 5A).
We found a ~37% neuronal survival in the cLP when
comparing 4 vs. 32 dpl (Figures 5B,D, Number of EdU
+
cells
in cLP: 4 dpl 144.7 ± 21.53, N = 8; 32 dpl 54.16 ± 11.47, N = 3),
whereas for rMP the survival of EdU-labeled cells during the
same period was ~55% (Figures 5E,G, Number of EdU
+
cells in
rMP: 4 dpl 245.6 ± 31.45, N = 8; 32 dpl 131.8 ± 23.84, N = 4). The
decrease in the number of labeled cells in both pallial regions
could indicate the death of a portion of these cells. Furthermore,
we assessed the sequential recruitment of EdU-labeled cells by
PCNA expression. We found a relevant fraction (44.03 ± 10,02%)
of mitotic EdU-labeled cells at 4 dpl in the cLP (Figure 5C),
which diminished considerably at later times. In contrast, in the
rMP we found a portion of EdU
+
proliferating cells from 4 to 32
dpl (26.54 ± 6.21%), while we observed scarce proliferation in
labeled cells at 64 dpl. (Figure 5F). Hence, a considerable portion
of proliferating cells retain EdU labeling and continues dividing
in the cLP and the rMP (Figure 5H). Our results point to a
complex regulation of adult neurogenesis in the pallium of
zebrash, where chained proliferation of NSCs and death of
immature neurons are balanced to contribute to the neuronal
addition on these networks.
FIGURE 2 | 20 μm. (G).BrdU
+
NeuN
+
cell quantication in MP. (Two-way ANOVA, Treatment effect: F
(1, 43)
=9.564withp= 0.0035, Palliumregion effect: F
(3, 43)
=0.889with
p= 0.455. Bonferronis multiple comparisons test,*p<0.05. Trained,N = 7; Control N = 6). (H).Top:Neuronalfate(%BrdU
+
NeuN
+
/BrdU
+
cells)in rMP (Mann-Whitney test,U
= 11.5, n. s. Trained, N = 7; Control N = 4). Bottom: Cell migration in rMP (Mann Whitney test, U = 602, * depicts p<0.05. Trained, N = 7; Control, N = 4). (I). Adult-born
neurons (BrdU/NeuN) in rMP for Trained and Control individuals. Scale bar, 20 μm. (I). Higher magnication of the boxed square in I (merge panel). Single focal plane and
orthogonal views after three-dimension reconstruction.
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 8409646
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
FIGURE 3 | Training zebrash during an early period (314 dpl) increases adult neur ogenesis in the rMP. (A). Experimental design. (B). Learning curves for Trained
and Control individuals (Two-way RM ANOVA, Treatment effect: F
(1, 14)
= 126.1 with p<0.0001, Session effect: F
(9, 126)
= 5.254 with p<0.0001. Bonferronis multiple
comparisons test, *** depicts p<0.001, ****, p<0.0001. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (C). Simple linear regression for each training window for Trai ned sh (ANCOVA,
F
(1, 76)
= 1.516, n. s. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (D). BrdU
+
NeuN
+
cell quantication in LP. (Two-way ANOVA, Treatment effect: F
(1, 54)
= 10.63 with p= 0.0019,
Pallium region effect: F
(3, 54)
= 8.642 with p<0.0001. Bonferronis multiple comparisons test, not signicant differences. Trained, N = 8; Cont rol N = 8). (E). Top: Neuronal
fate (%BrdU
+
NeuN
+
/BrdU
+
cells) in cLP. (Unpaired ttest, t
(14)
= 4.749, *** depicts p<0.001. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). Bottom: Cell migration in cLP (Mann Whitney
test, U = 6492, n. s. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (F). Adult-born neurons (BrdU
+
NeuN
+
) in cLP for Trained and Control individuals. Scale bar, 20 μm. (G). BrdU
+
NeuN
+
cell quantication in MP. (Two-way ANOVA, Treatment effect: F
(1, 56)
= 15.34 with p= 0.0002, Pallium region effect: F
(3, 56)
= 2.685 with p= 0.0553. Bonferronis multiple
comparisons test, * depicts p<0.05. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (H). Top: Neuronal fate (%BrdU
+
NeuN
+
/BrdU
+
cells) in rMP (Unpaired ttest, t
(14)
= 1.396, n. s.
Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). Bottom: Cell migration in rMP (Mann Whitney test, U = 2088, n. s. Trained, N = 8; Control N = 8). (I). Adult-born neurons (BrdU
+
NeuN
+
)in
rMP for Trained and Control individuals. Scale bar, 20 μm. (I). Higher magnication of the boxed square in I (merge panel). Single focal plane and orthogonal views after
three-dimension reconstruction.
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 8409647
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
A Population Dynamics Model Mimics the
Learning-Induced Adult Neurogenesis in
the MP
Recently, Than-Trong and coworkers (2020) performed an intra-
vital imaging analysis to track the fate of NSCs in the MP. Based
on the experimental results and a computational model, the
authors estimated the proportions of symmetric and
asymmetric divisions, and possible fates adopted by activated
NSCs. Based on the division rates observed by Than-Trong and
coworkers, we adapted their model to assess the dynamics of
neuronal addition under different conditions: Control and
FIGURE 4 | Training adult zebrash during a late period (3142 dpl) has no effect on pallial neurogenesis. (A). Experimental design. (B). Learning curves for Trained
and Control individuals. (Two-way RM ANOVA, Treatment effect: F
(1, 12)
= 108.0 with p<0.0001, Session effect: F
(9, 108)
= 6.800 with p<0.0001. Bonferronis multiple
comparisons test, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, ****p<0.0001. Trained, N = 7; Control N = 7). (C). Simple linear regression for each training window for Trained sh (ANCOVA,
F
(1, 66)
= 5.914, * indicates p<0.05. Trained, N = 7; Control N = 7). (D). BrdU
+
NeuN
+
cell quantication in LP (left) and MP (right) (KruskalWallis test with: Left panel,
p= 0.1984; Right panel, p= 0.316. Dunns multiple comparisons test, not signicant differences were found. Trained, N = 7; Control, N = 7). (E). Neuronal increase ratio
(Trained/Control) in rMP and cLP for each learning time frame.
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 8409648
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
FIGURE 5 | Balance of adult neurogenesis driven by chained proliferation and loss of adult-born cells. (A). Experimental design. Fish were i. p. injected with EdU
and euthanised for histology at three times points: 4, 16, 32 and 64 dpl. (B). EdU
+
cell quantication in cLP in 4, 16, 32 and 64 dpl. (KruskalWallis test, followed by post-
hoc Dunns test, K-W st = 9.048, ** depicts p<0.01. 4 dpl, N = 8; 16 dpl, N = 4; 32 dpl, N = 3, 64 dpl, N = 6). (C). Number of proliferating EdU cells (top) and % of
proliferating EdU cells (bottom) (KruskalWallis test, followed by post-hoc Dunns test, K-W st = 13.80 and K-W st = 12.88, respectively. * depicts p<0.05, **, p<
0.01. 4 dpl, N = 8; 16 dpl, N = 4; 32 dpl, N = 3, 64 dpl, N = 6). (D). EdU/PC NA in cLP for 4 dpl and 32 dpl. Scale bar, 20 μm. (E). EdU
+
cell quantication in rMP in 4, 16, 32
and 64 dpl. (KruskalWallis test, followed by post-hoc Dunns test, K-W st = 7.88, * depicts p<0.05. 4 dpl, N = 8; 16 dpl, N = 4; 32 dpl, N = 4, 64 dpl, N = 6). (F). Number
of proliferating EdU cells (top) and % of proliferating EdU cells (bottom) (KruskalWallis test, followed by post-hoc Dunns test, K-W st = 9.571 and K-W st = 8.827,
respectively. ** depicts p<0.01. 4 dpl, N = 8; 16 dpl, N = 4; 32 dpl, N = 3, 64 dpl, N = 6). (G). EdU/PCNA in rMP for 4 dpl and 32 dpl. Scale bar, 20 μm. (H). Scheme
summarizing EdU/PCNA results in cLP and rMP.
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 8409649
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
FIGURE 6 | A population NSC dynamics model mimics the learning-induced adult neurogenesis in the rMP. (A). Scheme outlining NSC population dynamics
model. ki indicates rates of division/differentiation. The learning effect on NSC proliferation was simulated by adding a learning factor (λ) during training windows. rNSC:
reservoir neural stem cell; oNSC: operative neural stem cell; n: neuron. (B). Number of cellular divisions at steady state, calculated to estimate the initial populations of
BrdU-labeled cells. (C). Population dynamics under Control condition. (D). Left: Stochastic simulations to estimate the population of adult-born neurons in Control
and Trained subjects during two training periods (314 dpl) and three training periods (1230 dpl). Middle: BrdU-labeled cells division under Control and Training
conditions. Right: Stochastic simulations to estimate the population of adult-born neurons considering a checkpoint (at 15 dpl) for neuronal death, together with a
learning-induced rescue. (E). Adult-born neurons for Control subjects at 30 dpl, with and without neuronal death (Unpaired t-test, t
(38)
= 73.10, **** depicts p<0.0001, N = 20).
(F). Neuronal increase by learning under different conditions (Two-way ANOVA, Period effect: F
(6, 239)
=430.7withp<0.0001, Condition effect: F
(1, 239)
= 1,342 with p<0.0001.
Post-hoc Sidaks test **** depicts p<0.0001- N = 20 for simulation, N = 5 for data).
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 84096410
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
Trained at different learning periods (Figures 6A,B). After
30 days, the model evolves to a proportion ~60% of adult-born
neurons from the initial cohort of labeledNSCs in the rMP
(Figure 6C), a value slightly lower to what is shown in our
experiments (Panels E, H from Figures 2,3). Next, we calculated
the number of chained divisions that each cell of the original NSC
pool goes through. The model indicates an average of ~1.7
divisions during the 30-day period (Figure 6D), a value that
supports the chained proliferation of labeled-NSCs by discarding
a relevant BrdU dilution in the progeny. Then, based on our
PCNA results (see Figure 1), we hypothesized that learning
would burst the activation and proliferation of BrdU-labeled
NSCs, which were a product of NSC-conservative divisions
from the original labeled pool. We aimed to emulate the
neuronal population dynamics in rMP when learning occurs
from 314 dpl (2 weeks of training) and from 1230 dpl
(3 weeks of training), the experimental conditions in which
learning promotes adult neurogenesis. Thus, during these
training windows, the proliferation rate of NSCs is affected by
a learning factor (λ). We observed that learning increased the
number of adult-born neurons at the expense of the operative
NSC pool (Figure 6C). However, in contrast to the changes
observed in our experiments (See Figure 4E), in the simulation
both training conditions (314 and 1230 dpl) exhibited a similar
outcome in the number of new neurons (Figure 6D). Since we
observed neuronal loss in the rMP from 4 to 32 dpl (indicated by
~45% reduction in the number of EdU-labeled cells), we
incorporated to our model a checkpoint starting at 15 dpl to
allow the survival or death of new neurons, together with a
learning-related rescue (See methods). In Control conditions the
death of adult-born neurons maintains a 67.1 ± 5.4% of survival
(Figure 6D,E). As expected, the incorporation of neuronal death
to the model increases the difference in the number of adult-born
neurons when training occurs during 1230 dpl in comparison
with 314 dpl (Figure 6D). This learning-induced adult
neurogenesis prole mimics our experimental results, but with
a lower difference than our experimental data. This result, led us
to interrogate the model under different conditions, as
duplication of the learning factor (2X λ), duplication of all the
division/differentiation rates (2X k
i
), 2X λ+ neuronal death, and
2X k
i
+ neuronal death (Figure 6F,Supplementary Figure S5A).
The only conditions where the model mimicked the experimental
data proles were the ones in which neuronal death was taken
into account, being the 2X λ+ neuronal death the most accurate
condition. A long-term simulation (500 days) showed that adult
neurogenesis is additive to pallial networks, even when
considering neuronal death (Supplementary Figure S5B).
Thus, the model supports that learning-induced adult
neurogenesis in rMP relies on both mechanisms: a boost in
chained NSC proliferation and rescue from neuronal loss.
DISCUSSION
Teleost sh grow throughout their lives, therefore their organs
must adapt to their increasing body size (Jerison, 1973).
Consequently, adult neurogenesis could be considered as a
mechanism that underlies the constant growth of the sh
brain, in agreement with the numerical matching hypothesis
(Zupanc, 2021). As an alternative but not excluding
hypothesis, adult neurogenesis would provide neural networks
with an extra degree of plasticity to adapt the brain to changes in
the environment. Previous studies in teleosts suggest functional
specialization of neurogenic niches (Zupanc and Horschke, 1995;
Kaslin et al., 2008;Iribarne and Castelló, 2014;Lindsey et al.,
2014;Olivera-Pasilio et al., 2014;Sato et al., 2017;Labusch et al.,
2020). These studies demonstrate that different brain regions
involved in the processing of sensory activity are neurogenic, and
sustained sensory stimulation leads to an increase in newborn
neurons only in the related niches. In the same way, in rodents
and birds it has been shown that behavioral challenges involving
information processing in neurogenic brain nuclei enhance adult
neurogenesis in a stimulus-dependent fashion (Goldman and
Nottebohm, 1983;Barnea et al., 1994;Leuner et al., 2004;Alonso
et al., 2006;Tashiro et al., 2007). These works highlight the
relevance of adult neurogenesis on learning-related changes in a
structure-to-function manner. Here, we challenged zebrash with
a cognitive paradigm to explore the addition of adult-born
neurons to pallial circuits. The subjects were trained in a
spatial learning paradigm to integrate their positional
information with visual cues to solve the task. To avoid
egocentric responses, the start compartments were randomly
chosen and multiple maze rotations were performed during
sessions. Therefore, on each trial, the sh must make a
decision based on spatial information. Adult zebrash
exhibited good performance on this behavioral test, even after
sequential rule-change sessions. We observed that training sh in
this paradigm increases cell proliferation in circumscribed pallial
subregions. The Control group, mocked with a random exit, was
subjected to all the experimental manipulations (isolation,
handling, environment, exploration, and social reward) as the
Trained group. Hence, we conclude that the observed differences
in adult neurogenesis are a consequence of the learning process.
The Trained subjects evidenced a region-specic increase in the
rate of proliferation when compared to Control sh. This nding
suggests that the new neurons could underlie plastic changes in
the pallium as a consequence of the cognitive challenge.
Our results indicate a rostro-caudal specialization in the pallial
circuits, shedding light on the relevance of the cLP (encompassing
the caudal Dlv and Dld) and the rMP (the rostral portion of Dm)
during the execution of the cognitive task implemented here.
Behavioral studies involving distinct teleost sh related the LP to
navigation and spatial learning (Durán et al., 2015;Elliott et al.,
2017;Ocaña et al., 2017;Fotowat et al., 2019). Both of these
cognitive functions are processed by the mammalian
hippocampus. Furthermore, the zebrash LP region expresses
several molecular markers resembling the ones expressed by the
mammalian hippocampus (Mueller and Wullimann, 2009;Ganz
et al., 2015). Our results are in agreement with the involvement of
the teleost LP in processing spatial information and reveal a
rostro-caudal specialization of this structure, being the cLP the
only region in which this spatial task heightens the addition of
new neurons. In this regard, Ocaña and coworkers (2017)
reported progressive changes in metabolic activity throughout
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 84096411
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
the rostro-caudal LP as a response to training goldsh in the same
paradigm. In their work, oxidative metabolism activity was
analyzed during learning showing that rostral and medial
sections of Dlv exhibit transient activation at early stages,
whereas the caudal Dlv shows sustained activity throughout
the training period. In our experiments we found no
differences between the dorsal and ventral LP (Dld and Dlv,
data not shown) and only observed learning-related effects on
adult neurogenesis in the cLP. Very likely, the signaling involved
in oxidative metabolism during this paradigm could be different
than the factors involved during neurogenesis in the LP,
explaining the discrepancies. Unexpectedly, we also found a
learning-related induction of adult neurogenesis in the rMP, a
brain region that has been poorly studied in teleosts. Interestingly,
Lau and coworkers (2011) have proposed that Dm acts as a brain
center whose activity discriminates a choice behavior in zebrash
(Lau et al., 2011). In their work the authors analyzed neuronal
activity by c-fos expression in the pallium, but only at a rostral
level (rMP, slice 71 of the zebrash atlas (Wullimann et al.,
1996)). On this basis, we hypothesize that the rMP learning-
related increase in adult neurogenesis could be attributed to the
role of this neural center on decision-making. However, future
experiments should be conducted to test this idea.
The pallium of sh is considered a simple structure with
specialized regions at the transversal level. However, here we
observed a rostro-caudal specialization in the MP and the LP of
Control sh, evidenced by gradients of PCNA (Figure 1), with
higher proliferation activity in the rMP and the cLP, being both
regions prone to be modulated by learning. Interestingly, our
results highlight the specialization of neural circuits along the
pallial rostro-caudal axis, a concept that should be taken into
account in future studies.
To evaluate the learning effects on adult neurogenesis, we
labeled proliferating progenitor cells with the thymidine analog
BrdU and sh were trained at different periods after progenitor
labeling (314, 1230, 3142 dpl). After a single thymidine analog
pulse (BrdU or EdU), most of the label will be incorporated by the
active NSC (5% of total NSCs, (März et al., 2010;Than-Trong
et al., 2020),) and by a fraction of neuronal progenitors (~626%
(März et al., 2010;Rothenaigner et al., 2011)). After short chase
times, most adult-born neurons will be the product of neurogenic
divisions by the committed neuronal progenitors. While, the late
recruitment of adult-born neurons (here described as chained
proliferation) would result from conservative NSC divisions at
the time of BrdU administration, which will go through
consecutive mitosis and differentiation beyond neurogenic fate.
We found an increase of BrdU-labeled neurons in the rMP and
cLP of Trained subjects. Our results support adult neurogenesis as
an evolutionary conserved source of learning-related brain
plasticity. Interestingly, the neurogenesis observed in these
pallial regions differs in their learning-sensitive critical periods,
where the cLP has a shorter temporal window as compared to the
rMP. The distinct critical periods between these pallial regions
could be attributed to different composition of NSC and
progenitor cells between both niches. Or alternative, could be
explained by neuronal populations with a distinct maturation
pace. In rodents, several activity-dependent critical periods have
been reported during development of adult-born neurons (Ge
et al., 2007;Tashiro et al., 2007;Alvarez et al., 2016); however to
our knowledge this is the rst evidence reported in a
teleost model.
The observed increase in adult neurogenesis after the cognitive
challenge could underlie two distinct, yet not exclusive,
mechanisms: 1) rescue of immature neurons from death
programs; or 2) an expansion of the labeled NSC reservoir by
chained recruitment of BrdU-labeled cells. Here, we found that
both processes could contribute to adult neurogenesis
homeostasis in the zebrash pallium (Figure 6). In rodents,
adult neurogenesis generates neurons in abundance, and their
survival depends on an activity-dependent synaptic integration of
new neurons to rescue them from death programs (Ryu et al.,
2016). In line with this idea, we found a decrease in the number of
adult-born neurons in cLP and rMP over a 64-days lapse (Figures
5B,E). Accordingly, Ampatzis and coworkers (2012) showed
signicant apoptotic activity assessed by TUNEL method in
different regions of adult zebrash pallium (~100200 TUNEL
proles/mm
2
, a value close to the daily adult neurogenesis
contribution) (Ampatzis et al., 2012). On the other hand,
supporting the chained proliferation, we found signicant
amounts of EdU
+
PCNA
+
cells in both pallial subregions
(Figures 5C,F). Whereas the rMP maintains a steady level of
cell proliferation over the 432 days period, the cLP shows an
early boost of proliferation activity, which declined by day 16. The
differences in the proliferation dynamics between both sub-
regions could be attributed to heterogeneous cellular
compositions (März et al., 2010;Lindsey et al., 2012;Dirian
et al., 2014;Anand and Mondal, 2017). Thus, at 4 dpl double-
labeled cells in rMP and cLP may correspond to fast-cycling cells
(potentially intermediate amplier progenitors or transitory
amplifying cells), which proliferate over a short period. In cLP,
there is scarce proliferation activity detected after 4 dpl. However,
in the rMP, a relevant portion of EdU
+
cells continues
proliferating at 16 and 32 dpl. These may correspond to slow-
cycling cells, which re-entered the cell cycle after a quiescent state
(Alunni et al., 2010;Olivera-Pasilio et al., 2014). The fast decrease,
after 4 dpl (Figure 5C), in chained proliferation observed in cLP
could explain the absence of learning effects when subjects were
trained at 314 dpl. While the late boost (1230 dpl) of adult
neurogenesis in this region (Figure 2D), could be attributed to an
activity-dependent rescue from cell death programs. On the other
hand, in rMP both chained proliferation and death rescue seem to
be synergistic to the learning-related increase of 1230 dpl adult-
born neurons.
In agreement with our observations and supporting a chained
proliferation of pallial NSCs, a persisting proliferation of BrdU-
labeled cells (~40 dpl) in the zebrash telencephalon was also
reported by other authors (Grandel et al., 2006;März et al., 2010).
The chained proliferation of labeled NSCs has also been proposed
by Prickaerts and collaborators (Prickaerts et al., 2004). In their
words, the effect of proliferation alone, on every day after
injection, is added to the number of cells counted as well as
the survival of those cells which were labeled earlier in the week
and have not continued to proliferate. Therefore, our results
argue in favor of complex regulation of adult neurogenesis in
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 84096412
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
which learning promotes the chained division of NSCs, together
with activity-dependent survival. However, it is not clear whether
new-born neuron integration and survival is related to NSC
proliferation in a causative manner, a question that should be
addressed in future research. The concept of chained-
proliferation is supported by our NSC population dynamics
model, in which proliferation and death act in a synergistic
way to induce pallial circuit modications by adult
neurogenesis. Although the model reproduces the adult
neurogenesis proles observed in our experimental conditions,
the proportion of adult-born neurons as well as its learning-
induced increase exhibit lower values when compared to our
experimental data. We speculate that these discrepancy could be
related to different factors, such as different division/
differentiation (k
i
) rates in our sh as compared to the ones
calculated in the Than-Trong work, underestimation of the
learning factor (λ), or the impact of neuronal death
implemented in our model.
Taken together our results indicate that learning in a cognitive
paradigm, involving spatial and positional information together with
decision making, induces the addition of new neurons into specic
pallial circuits. However, from our experiments, it is not clear whether
neurons generated during training would participate in encoding
information related to the learning process itself, since these new
neurons could still be in an immature stage. If this is true, then the
learning-induced neurogenesis would prepare the related neuronal
circuits by adding new neurons for future challenges. In line with this
idea,itwasrecentlyshownthatsilencing hippocampal adult-born
neurons, which were immature during learning, impairs remote
memory reconsolidation in rats, indicating a learning-related
priming of immature neurons (Lods et al., 2021). This idea, as well
as elucidating the timing for maturation and synaptic integration of
adult-born neurons should be explored in future works.
CONCLUSION
The addition of adult-born neurons by adult neurogenesis
represents a major source of brain plasticity. Although
zebrash possess high levels of adult neurogenesis broadly
distributed throughout their brain, the involvement of
neuronal addition as a cognitive-related plastic mechanism has
not been explored in this model, yet. The zebrash pallium has
numerous well described neurogenic niches, and has been proved
to be critical for the execution of spatial and emotional learning
tasks. In this work, we trained adult zebrash in a cue-guided
maze and found an improvement in their performance in a daily
manner throughout ve sessions. This cognitive challenge
induces an increase in proliferation activity only in two
restricted pallial areas, the cLP and the rMP. In addition,
adult-born neurons in rMP and cLP are being produced on
demand during the learning process but with distinct critical
periods. Finally, based on a NSC population dynamics model we
propose that adult neurogenesis is regulated in a complex manner
by promoting NSC proliferation together with neuronal death
programs. We propose that both processes are prone to be
regulated by learning-induced network activity.
METHODS
Subjects and housing. All experiments were carried out using
10 ± 1 months-old AB-wild type zebrash (Danio rerio) line in
AB background, except for the experiment shown in
Supplementary Figure S2, in which 21 months-old individuals
were used. Adult zebrash were housed in a zebrash standalone
rack system (ZS560, Aquaneering Inc.), were sh were kept in
small groups (5 sh/l) with aerated and ltered water at a
constant temperature of 27 ± 0.5°C, pH = 7.2 ± 0.2. The
aquarium room was subjected to a 14:10 h light/dark cycle.
Dry food and Artemia salina were provided three-times a day.
During the experimental period sh were housed individually in
1.4 L tanks. Both sexes were used indistinguishably. Experimental
procedures were conducted in accordance with the National
regulations and following the Universities Federation for
Animal Welfare Handbook on the Care and Management of
Laboratory and other Research Animals. This work has the
consent of the Comisión Nacional de Energía Atómicas
IACUC, protocol #05-2018-02.
Behavioral paradigm. Subjects were trained in a square tank (30
× 30 cm) containing the experimental rhomboid maze in the
center, as was previously described (Ingle and Sahagian, 1973).
ThemazewasmadeoutofgreenPVC(10×10cm),toforma
diamond-shaped box with two circular starting compartments
in opposite corners. The remaining corners of the box served as
exits. One of them was kept open, while the other was blocked
with a transparent glass barrier.Spatialcues,consistingof
removable striped panels, were placed on two walls of the
box. On the edges of the tank,andbehindthestarting
compartments, two glass enclosures containing conspecics
(two per enclosure) were used as social reward. The
experimental tank was illuminated with two LED lamps,
located above the tank.
Two days prior to training, subjects were habituated to the
experimental apparatus. During habituation sh were allowed to
swim through both exits of the apparatus. Neither the cues nor
the glass barrier were used during habituation. For individual
habituation sessions conspecics were also placed in the tank
enclosures. After habituation, the training sessions were carried
out. Each training session consisted of an acclimation period
(5 min) followed by a maze solving period (5 min), which
included chasing and capturing the sh with a small plastic
vessel. Each session consisted of 24-trial trials, and was
divided into two slots, one in the morning and the other in
the afternoon to avoid exhaustion. For each trial, sh were
randomly placed in either of the starting compartments (50%
each one), and allowed to enter the central arena by removing a
sliding PVC barrier. The initial decision of the sh was registered.
A correct choice was scored when it swam through the exit, and a
failure if it bumped against the glass barrier. In case of a failure,
the experimenter waited until the sh found the correct exit. Once
out of the maze, the sh were allowed to explore the tank and
conspecics for 10 s. Learning criterion was established at 70% of
correct choices. The maze, cues and glass barrier were
consistently rotated every ve trials to avoid the use of
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 84096413
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
external cues. In the Control group, the glass barrier was
randomly placed in any of the exits. Other from that,
conditions remained the same as the Trained group.
BrdU labeling. To label cycling cells, zebrash were immersed in
5 mM 5-bromo-2-deoxyuridine (Sigma) solution overnight. The
BrdU was dissolved in aquarium water. Fish were immersed in
groups up to 16 individuals, at a 50 ml/sh ratio. The following
days after immersion, they were successively rinsed with system
water and reincorporated to the sh facility.
EdU labeling. To label cycling cells, sh were anaesthetised in
0.01% tricaine methanosulfonate (MS222, Sigma) and a single 5-
ethynyl-2-deoxyuridine (EdU) injection (40 μL, 10 mM) was
delivered intraperitoneally (i.p.). Individuals were subsequently
allowed to recover in a holding tank, and returned to the sh facility.
Brain preparation and sectioning. After the last session, sh were
deeply anaesthetised in 0.02% tricaine methanosulfonate (MS222,
Sigma), brains were dissected and xed in 4% PFA in 10 mM PBS
overnight. Tissue was overprotected by immersion in 30% sucrose
for 2 days, immediately frozen in liquid nitrogen and stored at
20°C. Telencephalic frontal sections of 20 μm were cut on a cryostat
(Microm, HM 550), and mounted on positively charged slides. Slides
were air-dried for 24 h prior to inmunouorescence.
To analyze the pallium throughout the rostro-caudal axis, we
selected four sections for each individual, designated as rostral,
rostro-medial, medio-caudal and caudal. Sections were chosen
according to the topological atlas Neuroanatomy of the Zebrash
Brain (Wullimann et al., 1996), see Supplementary Figure S1.
Immunouorescence. Slides were rinsed three times with Tris-
buffered saline (TBS) (pH = 7.4) for 5 min, incubated with
100 mM ammonium chloride for 20 min and rinsed three
more times with 0.3% TritonX-100/TBS for 5 min. Then,
slides were blocked with 6% bovine serum albumin (BSA), 6%
normal goat serum (NGS) in TBS for 1 h and incubated overnight
at 4°C with primary antibodies diluted in 6% BSA, 6% NGS in
TBS. Then, they were washed with 0.3% TritonX-100/TBS four
times for 5 min, and incubated with secondary antibodies coupled
to Alexa Fluor 488 or 594 (1:500 dilution) for 2 h at room
temperature (RT). Sections were then washed three times
(5 min each) in 0.3% TritonX-100/TBS, and mounted using
Fluorescence Mounting Medium (Abcam). Sections were
stored at 4 °C. When double immunostaining was performed
both primary, or secondary, antibodies were incubated at the
same time. For BrdU and PCNA immunodetection antigen
retrieval was performed before blocking: 30 min in 2 N HCl at
37°C, followed by 10 min neutralization in 0.1 M borate buffer
(pH = 8,5). Primary antibodies used were: rat monoclonal anti-
BrdU 1:500 (ab6326, Abcam), mouse monoclonal anti-PCNA 1:
300 (PC10, sc-56, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Inc.), mouse
monoclonal anti-NeuN 1:500 (ab104224, Abcam), mouse
monoclonal anti-NeuroD1 1:500 (ab60704, Abcam).
Secondary antibodies used were: goat polyclonal anti-Rat IgG
Alexa Fluor 488 (ab150165, Abcam), goat polyclonal anti-Mouse
IgG Alexa Fluor 488 (ab150117, Abcam), goat polyclonal anti-Rabbit
IgG Alexa Fluor 594 (ab150084, Abcam), goat polyclonal anti-
Mouse IgG Alexa Fluor 594 (ab150120, Abcam). The images
were acquired by using an epiuorescence microscope Nikon
Eclipse e800 and an ad-hoc built two-photon microscope.
Colocalization of uorescent markers were performed on
single-plane images acquired in the two-photon microscope.
Images were processed in FIJI (ImageJ v1.53). Immunostained
sections in which the tissue was broken or folded were excluded
from the analysis.
Population dynamics model. In this work we performed a
population dynamics simulation based on the work of Than-
Trong and coworkers (2020) with modications. Briey, the model
contemplates two NSC populations, the reservoir pool (rNSCs) and
the operative pool (oNSCs). Both kind of NSCs divided and
differentiated following the rates reported by Than-Trong: 1) rNSC
rNSC + oNSC: (k
1
= 0.007/day); 2) oNSC death: (k
2
=0.017/day);
3) oNSC oNSC + oNSC: (k
3
= 0.006/day); 4) oNSC oNSC + n:
(k
4
= 0.018/day); 5) oNSC n+n:(k
5
=0.004/day);6)oNSCn:
(k
6
= 0.013/day). To adapt the model to our experiments, we rstly
determined the system dynamics at equilibrium to establish the basal
proportions of rNSC (r) and oNSC (o). Based on the reaction rates
described above we developed differential equations to evaluate the
temporal progression of r and o populations:
As rNSCrNSC + oNSC: dr
dt 0, at equilibrium r
eq
= r(t
0
)
Whereas do
dt k1r−(k3+k5+k6k2)of(r, o),
at equilibrium do
dt f(req,o
eq)0,
with oeq 1
4req
Once established the contribution of r
eq
and o
eq
, we developed
a stochastic simulation to determine the proportions of reactions
associated to cellular divisions (d
i
), which will be used as target for
BrdU labelingsimulation (Figure 6C). Hence, from this
simulation we established the following parameters: d
1
= 0.5,
d
3
= 0.11, d
4
= 0.32, d
5
= 0.07.
According to this, given an initial population (P
0
)ofBrdU-
labeledNSCs we established the progeny fate after a single division:
r(t0)P0d1
o(t0)P0(d1+2d3+d4)
n(t0)P0(d4+2d5)
Next, we compute the population dynamics through a 30 days
period (see Figure 6C).
To address the way in which learning activity impinges on
population dynamics, we applyed a learning factor (λ) to the rates
of division and differentiation (k
i
) of the reactions that took place
only during training periods (See Figure 6D). Based on our
experiments of learning and proliferation (Figure 1 and
Supplementary Figure S2), we estimated a λ=3.
Neuronal death was incorporated under the hypothesis that
adult-born neurons mature over time to reach a checkpoint (at
day 15th), in which a portion of these neurons die with a probability
p
death
= 0.550, in accordance with the results shown in Figure 5.
Next, we implemented an activity-dependent rescue to decrease
neuronal death up to ten times during learning.
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 84096414
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
All the stochastic simulations were implemented in Python
3.8.5 using NumPy 1.19.2, and the codes are available in https://
gitlab.com/n806/stochasticsimulation_learning_and_neurog.
Statistical analysis. Every data set was tested by the Grubbs
outlier test, with alpha = 0.05. Normality was assessed using the
Shapiro-Wilk test, with a p-value of 0.05. Homoscedasticity was
analyzed by the Levene test, with a p-value of 0.05. When data
met criteria, unpaired t-test or Two-way ANOVA with
Bonferroni post-hoc test were used as indicated. In cases
where data did not meet normality criteria, nonparametric
tests used were Mann-Whitney or KruskalWallis. Behavioral
results were analyzed by Two-way ANOVA for repeated
measures as well as by linear regressions. In all cases,
statistical signicance was assumed when p<0.05. Unless
otherwise specied, data are presented as mean ± SE. Box
plots indicate median (line), 2575% percentile (box limits),
maximum and minimum values (whiskers). Simple linear
regression slopes were compared with an ANCOVA test. All
the statistical analyses performed in this work are detailed in the
supplementary information le.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The original contributions presented in the study are included in
the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be
directed to the corresponding author.
ETHICS STATEMENT
This study was conducted in accordance with the National
regulations and following the Universities Federation for
Animal Welfare Handbook on the Care and Management of
Laboratory and Other Research Animals. The experimental
procedures were reviewed and approved by the Comisión
Nacional de Energía Atómicas IACUC, protocol #05-2018-02.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
The working hypothesis and experimental design were conducted
by LM-F and LM. Training sh in the rhomboidal maze was
performed by LM-F and ED. FR was responsible for the EdU
pulse and chase experiments and the evaluation of adult-born
neuronsmigration distance. Immunouorescence and two-
photon microscopy were conducted by LM-F and FR. The
population dynamics model was performed by JC. All the
authors participated in the results discussion and
interpretation. The manuscript was written by LM and LM-F.
FUNDING
This work was funded by: 1) Wellcome Trust Foundation, Seed
Award in Science 210219/Z/18/Z; 2) ANPCyT-MinCyT PICT
2018-1031; 3) ANPCyT-MinCyT PICT 2019-0225; all to LM.
LM-F, FR, and ED are supported by Ph.D. fellowships from
CONICET. JC is supported by a masters fellowship from
Instituto Balseiro. LM is a research member of CONICET.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Alex Fainstein, Axel Bruchhausen, Maximiliano
Guyón, and Maia Brunstein for sharing the two-photon
microscope and for their time spent aiding in trouble xing.
Finally, we want to thank Maria Soledad Espósito and Luciano
Marpegan for their careful reading of the manuscript; their
suggestions and comments have improved our work.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at:
https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fcell.2022.840964/
full#supplementary-material
REFERENCES
Aasebø, I. E. J., Kasture, A. S., Passeggeri, M., and Tashiro, A. (2018). A Behavioral
Task with More Opportunities for Memory Acquisition Promotes the Survival
of New Neurons in the Adult Dentate Gyrus. Sci. Rep. 8, 111. doi:10.1038/
s41598-018-25331-w
Adams, M. M., and Kafaligonul, H. (2018). Zebrash-A Model Organism for
Studying the Neurobiological Mechanisms Underlying Cognitive Brain Aging
and Use of Potential Interventions. Front. Cel. Dev. Biol. 6, 15. doi:10.3389/
fcell.2018.00135
Adolf, B., Chapouton, P., Lam, C. S., Topp, S., Tannhäuser, B., Strähle, U., et al.
(2006). Conserved and Acquired Features of Adult Neurogenesis in the
Zebrash Telencephalon. Dev. Biol. 295, 278293. doi:10.1016/j.ydbio.2006.
03.023
Alonso, M., Viollet, C., Gabellec, M.-M., Meas-Yedid, V., Olivo-Marin, J.-C., and
Lledo, P.-M. (2006). Olfactory Discrimination Learning Increases the Survival
of Adult-Born Neurons in the Olfactory Bulb. J. Neurosci. 26, 1050810513.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2633-06.2006
Alunni, A., and Bally-Cuif, L. (2016). A Comparative View of Regenerative
Neurogenesis in Vertebrates. Development 143, 741753. doi:10.1242/dev.
122796
Alunni, A., Hermel, J.-M., Heuzé, A., Bourrat, F., Jamen, F., and Joly, J.-S. (2010).
Evidence for Neural Stem Cells in the Medaka Optic Tectum Proliferation
Zones. Devel. Neurobio. 70, 693713. doi:10.1002/dneu.20799
Alvarez, D. D., Giacomini, D., Yang, S. M., Trinchero, M. F., Temprana, S. G.,
Büttner, K. A., et al. (2016). A Disynaptic Feedback Network Activated by
Experience Promotes the Integration of New Granule Cells. Science 354,
459465. doi:10.1594/PANGAEA.85856810.1126/science.aaf2156
Ampatzis, K., Makantasi, P., and Dermon, C. R. (2012). Cell Proliferation Pattern
in Adult Zebrash Forebrain Is Sexually Dimorphic. Neuroscience 226,
367381. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.09.022
Anand, S. K., and Mondal, A. C. (2017). Cellular and Molecular Attributes of
Neural Stem Cell Niches in Adult Zebrash Brain. Devel. Neurobio. 77,
11881205. doi:10.1002/dneu10.1002/dneu.22508
Anderson, M. L., Sisti, H. M., Curlik, D. M., Shors, T. J., Curlik, D. M., and Shors , T.
J. (2011). Associative Learning Increases Adult Neurogenesis During a Critical
Period. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 175181. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07486.x
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 84096415
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
Ausas, M. S., Mazzitelli-Fuentes, L., Roman, F. R., Crichigno, S. A., De Vincenti, A.
P., and Mongiat, L. A. (2019). Social Isolation Impairs Active Avoidance
Performance and Decreases Neurogenesis in the Dorsomedial
Telencephalon of Rainbow Trout. Physiol. Behav. 198, 110. doi:10.1016/j.
physbeh.2018.10.006
Barnea, A., Nottebohm, F., Barnea, A., and Nottenbhom, F. (1994). Seasonal
Recruitment of Hippocampal Neurons in Adult Free-Ranging Black-Capped
Chickadees. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91, 1121711221. doi:10.1073/pnas.91.
23.11217
Dirian, L., Galant, S., Coolen, M., Chen, W., Bedu, S., Houart, C., et al. (2014).
Spatial Regionalization and Heterochrony in the Formation of Adult Pallial
Neural Stem Cells. Dev. Cel. 30, 123136. doi:10.1016/j.devcel.2014.05.012
Durán, E., Ocaña, F. M., Broglio, C., Rodríguez, F., Salas, C., Durán, E., et al. (2010).
Lateral but Not Medial Telencephalic Pallium Ablation Impairs the Use of
Goldsh Spatial Allocentric Strategies in a Hole-BoardTask. Behav. Brain
Res. 214, 480487. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.06.010
Elliott, S. B., Harvey-Girard, E., Giassi, A. C. C., and Maler, L. (2017).
Hippocampal-Like Circuitry in the Pallium of an Electric Fish: Possible
Substrates for Recursive Pattern Separation and Completion. J. Comp.
Neurol. 525, 846. doi:10.1002/cne.24060
Folgueira, M., Bayley, P., Navratilova, P., Becker, T. S., Wilson, S. W., and Clarke,
J. D. (2012). Morphogenesis Underlying the Development of the Everted
Teleost Telencephalon. Neural Dev. 7, 32. doi:10.1186/1749-8104-7-32
Fotowat, H., Lee, C., Jun, J. J., and Maler, L. (2019). Neural Activity in a
Hippocampus-Like Region of the Teleost Pallium Is Associated with Active
Sensing and Navigation. Elife 8, 125. doi:10.7554/elife.44119
Furlan, G., Cuccioli, V., Vuillemin, N., Dirian, L., Muntasell, A. J., Coolen, M., et al.
(2017). Life-Long Neurogenic Activity of Individual Neural Stem Cells and
Continuous Growth Establish an Outside-In Architecture in the Teleost
Pallium. Curr. Biol. 27, 32883301. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.052
Ganz, J., Kroehne, V., Freudenreich, D., Machate, A., Geffarth, M., Braasch, I., et al.
(2015). Subdivisions of the Adult Zebrash Pallium Based on Molecular Marker
Analysis. F1000Res 3, 308318. doi:10.12688/f1000research.5595.1
Ge, S., Yang, C.-h., Hsu, K.-S., Ming, G.-l., and Song, H. (2007). A Critical Period
for Enhanced Synaptic Plasticity in Newly Generated Neurons of the Adult
Brain. Neuron 54, 559566. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.002
Goldman, S. A., and Nottebohm, F. (1983). Neuronal Production, Migration, and
Differentiation in a Vocal Control Nucleus of the Adult Female Canary Brain.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 80, 23902394. doi:10.1073/pnas.80.8.2390
Grandel, H., and Brand, M. (2013). Comparative Aspects of Adult Neural Stem Cell
Activity in Vertebrates. Dev. Genes Evol. 223, 131147. doi:10.1007/s00427-
012-0425-5
Grandel, H., Kaslin, J., Ganz, J., Wenzel, I., and Brand, M. (2006). Neural Stem Cells
and Neurogenesis in the Adult Zebrash Brain: Origin, Proliferation Dynamics,
Migration and Cell Fate. Dev. Biol. 295, 263277. doi:10.1016/j.ydbio.2006.
03.040
Harvey-Girard, E., Giassi, A. C. C., Ellis, W., and Maler, L. (2012). Organization of
the Gymnotiform Fish Pallium in Relation to Learning and Memory: IV.
Expression of Conserved Transcription Factors and Implications for the
Evolution of Dorsal Telencephalon. J. Comp. Neurol. 520, 33953413.
doi:10.1002/cne.23107
Ingle, D., and Sahagian, D. (1973). Solution of a Spatial Constancy Problem by
Goldsh. Psychobiology 1, 8384. doi:10.3758/BF03326873
Iribarne, L., and Castelló, M. E. (2014). Postnatal Brain Development of the Pulse
Type, Weakly Electric Gymnotid Fish Gymnotus Omarorum. J. Physiology-
Paris 108, 4760. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.05.001
Jerison, H. J. (1973). Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence. 1973rd ed. New York:
Elsevier. Academic Press. Available at: https://www.sciencedirect.com/book/
9780123852502/evolution-of-the-brain-and-intelligence#book-description.
Kaslin, J., Ganz, J., and Brand, M. (2008). Proliferation, Neurogenesis and
Regeneration in the Non-Mammalian Vertebrate Brain. Phil. Trans. R. Soc.
B363, 101122. doi:10.1098/rstb.2006.2015
Labusch, M., Mancini, L., Morizet, D., and Bally-Cuif, L. (2020). Conserved and
Divergent Features of Adult Neurogenesis in Zebrash. Front. Cel Dev. Biol. 8,
128. doi:10.3389/fcell.2020.00525
Lal, P., Tanabe, H., Suster, M. L., Ailani, D., Kotani, Y., Muto, A., et al. (2018).
Identication of a Neuronal Population in the Telencephalon Essential for Fear
Conditioning in Zebrash. BMC Biol. 16, 118. doi:10.1186/s12915-018-0502-y
Lange, C., Rost, F., Machate, A., Reinhardt, S., Lesche, M., Weber, A., et al. (2020).
Single Cell Sequencing of Radial Glia Progeny Reveals Diversity of Newborn
Neurons in the Adult Zebrash Brain. Development 147, 1855951. doi:10.1242/
dev.185595
Lau,B.Y.B.,Mathur,P.,Gould,G.G.,andGuo,S.(2011).Identication of a
Brain center Whose Activity Discriminates a Choice Behavior in
Zebrash. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A.108, 25812586. doi:10.1073/
pnas.1018275108
Leuner, B., Mendolia-Loffredo, S., Kozorovitskiy, Y., Samburg, D., Gould, E., and
Shors, T. J. (2004). Learning Enhances the Survival of New Neurons beyond the
Time when the hippocampus Is Required for Memory. J. Neurosci. 24,
74777481. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0204-04.2004
Lindsey, B. W., Darabie, A., and Tropepe, V. (2012). The Cellular Composition of
Neurogenic Periventricular Zones in the Adult Zebrash Forebrain. J. Comp.
Neurol. 520, 22752316. doi:10.1002/cne.23065
Lindsey, B. W., Di Donato, S., Kaslin, J., and Tropepe, V. (2014). Sensory-specic
Modulation of Adult Neurogenesis in Sensory Structures Is Associated with the
Type of Stem Cell Present in the Neurogenic Niche of the Zebrash Brain. Eur.
J. Neurosci. 40, 35913607. doi:10.1111/ejn.12729
Lledo, P.-M., Alonso, M., and Grubb, M. S. (2006). Adult Neurogenesis and
Functional Plasticity in Neuronal Circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 179193.
doi:10.1038/nrn1867
Lods, M., Pacary, E., Mazier, W., Farrugia, F., Mortessagne, P., Masachs, N., et al.
(2021). Adult-born Neurons Immature during Learning Are Necessary for
Remote Memory Reconsolidation in Rats. Nat. Commun. 12. doi:10.1038/
s41467-021-22069-4
März, M., Chapouton, P., Diotel, N., Vaillant, C., Hesl, B., Takamiya, M., et al.
(2010). Heterogeneity in Progenitor Cell Subtypes in the Ventricular Zone of
the Zebrash Adult Telencephalon. Glia 58, NA. doi:10.1002/glia.20971
Mongiat, L. A., and Schinder, A. F. (2011). Adult Neurogenesis and the Plasticity of
the Dentate Gyrus Network. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 10551061. doi:10.1111/j.
1460-9568.2011.07603.x
Mu, Y., Lee, S. W., and Gage, F. H. (2010). Signaling in Adult Neurogenesis. Curr.
Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 416423. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2010.04.010
Mueller, T. (2011). The Conserved Bauplan of the Teleostean Telencephalon. Brain
Behav. Evol. 78, 259260. doi:10.1159/000331869
Mueller, T., and Wullimann, M. F. (2009). An Evolutionary Interpretation of
Teleostean Forebrain Anatomy. Brain Behav. Evol. 74, 3042. doi:10.1159/
000229011
Ocaña, F. M., Uceda, S., Arias, J. L., Salas, C., Rodríguez, F., and Rodríguez, F.
(2017). Dynamics of Goldsh Subregional Hippocampal Pallium Activity
Throughout Spatial Memory Formation. Brain Behav. Evol. 90, 154170.
doi:10.1159/000478843
Olivera-Pasilio, V., Peterson, D. A., and Castelló, M. a. E. (2014). Spatial
Distribution and Cellular Composition of Adult Brain Proliferative Zones in
the Teleost, Gymnotus Omarorum. Front. Neuroanat. 8, 119. doi:10.3389/
fnana.2014.00088
Portavella, M., Torres, B., and Salas, C. (2004). Avoidance Response in
Goldsh: Emotional and Temporal Involvement of Medial and Lateral
Telencephalic Pallium. J. Neurosci. 24, 23352342. doi:10.1523/
JNEUROSCI.4930-03.2004
Prickaerts, J., Koopmans, G., Blokland, A., and Scheepens, A. (2004). Learning and
Adult Neurogenesis: Survival with or without Proliferation? Neurobiol. Learn.
Mem. 81, 111. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2003.09.001
Rothenaigner, I., Krecsmarik, M., Hayes, J. A., Bahn, B., Lepier, A., Fortin, G., et al.
(2011). Clonal Analysis by Distinct Viral Vectors Identies Bona Fide Neural
Stem Cells in the Adult Zebrash Telencephalon and Characterizes Their
Division Properties and Fate. Development 138, 14591469. doi:10.1242/dev.
058156
Ruhl, T., Jonas, A., Seidel, N. I., Prinz, N., Albayram, O., Bilkei-Gorzo, A., et al.
(2015). Oxidation and Cognitive Impairment in the Aging Zebrash.
Gerontology 62, 4757. doi:10.1159/000433534
Ryu, J. R., Hong, C. J., Kim, J. Y., Kim, E.-K., Sun, W., and Yu, S.-W. (2016). Control
of Adult Neurogenesis by Programmed Cell Death in the Mammalian Brain.
Mol. Brain 9, 43. doi:10.1186/s13041-016-0224-4
Sailor, K. A., Schinder, A. F., and Lledo, P.-M. (2017). Adult Neurogenesis Beyond
the Niche: Its Potential for Driving Brain Plasticity. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 42,
111117. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2016.12.001
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 84096416
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
Sato, Y., Yano, H., Shimizu, Y., Tanaka, H., and Ohshima, T. (2017). Optic Nerve
Input-Dependent Regulation of Neural Stem Cell Proliferation in the Optic
Tectum of Adult Zebrash. Devel. Neurobio. 77, 474482. doi:10.1002/dneu.
22423
Tashiro, A., Makino, H., and Gage, F. H. (2007). Experience-Specic Functional
Modication of the Dentate Gyrus Through Adult Neurogenesis: A Critical
Period During an Immature Stage. J. Neurosci. 27, 32523259. doi:10.1523/
JNEUROSCI.4941-06.2007
Than-Trong,E.,Kiani,B.,Dray,N.,Ortica,S.,Simons,B.,Rulands,S.,etal.
(2020). Lineage Hierarchies and Stochasticity Ensure the Long-Term
Maintenance of Adult Neural Stem Cells. Sci. Adv. 6, eaaz542415.
doi:10.1126/sciadv.aaz5424
Than-Trong, E., and Bally-cuif, L. (2015). Radial Glia and Neural Progenitors in the
Adult Zebrash Central Nervous System. Glia 63, 14061428. doi:10.1002/glia.
22856
Toda, T., and Gage, F. H. (2018). Review: Adult Neurogenesis Contributes to
Hippocampal Plasticity. Cell Tissue Res. 373, 693709. doi:10.1007/s00441-017-
2735-4
Trotha, J. W., Vernier, P., Bally-Cuif, L., von Trotha, J. W., Vernier, P., Bally-cuif,
L., et al. (2014). Emotions and Motivated Behavior Converge on an Amygdala-
Like Structure in the Zebrash. Eur. J. Neurosci. 40, 33023315. doi:10.1111/ejn.
12692
Uceda,S.,Ocaña,F.M.,Martín-Monzón,I.,Rodríguez-Expósito,B.,Durán,
E., Rodríguez, F., et al. (2015). Spatial Learning-Related Changes in
Metabolic Brain Activity Contribute to the Delimitation of the
Hippocampal Pallium in Goldsh. Behav. Brain Res. 292, 403408.
doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2015.06.018
Vargas, J. P., López, J. C., and Portavella, M. (2009). What Are the Functions of Fish
Brain Pallium? Brain Res. Bull. 79, 436440. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2009.
05.008
Wullimann, M. F., and Mueller, T. (2004). Teleostean and Mammalian Forebrains
Contrasted: Evidence from Genes to Behavior. J. Comp. Neurol. 475, 143162.
doi:10.1002/cne.20183
Wullimann, M. F., Rupp, B., and Reichert, H. (1996). Neuroanatomy of the
Zebrash Brain: A Topological Atlas. 1st ed. Basel: Birkhäuser Basel. doi:10.
1007/978-3-0348-8979-7Neuroanatomy of the Zebrash Brain
Yang, P., Kajiwara, R., Tonoki, A., and Itoh, M. (2018). Successive and Discrete
Spaced Conditioning in Active Avoidance Learning in Young and Aged
Zebrash. Neurosci. Res. 130, 17. doi:10.1016/j.neures.2017.10.005
Zupanc, G. K. H. (2021). Adult Neurogenesis in the Central Nervous System of
Teleost Fish: From Stem Cells to Function and Evolution. J. Exp. Biol. 224.
doi:10.1242/JEB.226357
Zupanc, G. K. H., Hinsch, K., and Gage, F. H. (2005). Proliferation, Migration,
Neuronal Differentiation, and Long-Term Survival of New Cells in the Adult
Zebrash Brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 488, 290319. doi:10.1002/cne.20571
Zupanc, G. n. K. H., and Horschke, I. (1995). Proliferation Zones in the Brain of
Adult Gymnotiform Fish: A Quantitative Mapping Study. J. Comp. Neurol. 353,
213233. doi:10.1002/cne.903530205
Conict of Interest: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the
absence of any commercial or nancial relationships that could be construed as a
potential conict of interest.
Publishers Note: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors
and do not necessarily represent those of their afliated organizations, or those of
the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in
this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or
endorsed by the publisher.
Copyright © 2022 Mazzitelli-Fuentes, Román, Castillo Elías, Deleglise and Mongiat.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons
Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is
permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited
and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted
academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not
comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Cell and Developmental Biology | www.frontiersin.org May 2022 | Volume 10 | Article 84096417
Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al. Adult Neurogenesis Modulation by Learning
... The high regenerative and proliferative capacity of the juvenile salmon brain suggests that most aNSPCs are probably multipotent, as they are capable to replace almost all cell lines lost through damage, including NE cells, radial glia, oligodendrocytes, and neurons. To date, this hypothesis has been supported to a large extent by studies on quiescent Müller retinal glia in adult humans [9] and Danio rerio [11,18,19]. However, the unique regenerative profile of certain cell phenotypes in heterogeneous salmon brainstem cell niches is still unclear. ...
... The patterns of the IHC activity of molecular markers of neurogenesis of Pacific salmon juveniles are quite complex, and for the verification of cellular localization, given the complexity of the organization of the caudal parts of the brain and weak anatomical compliance with the results of immunolabeling on other model objects, in particular Danio rerio [11,12,15,18,19], immunolabeling is a necessary stage of the study of the aNSPCs' morphology. ...
Article
Full-text available
The central nervous system of Pacific salmon retains signs of embryonic structure throughout life and a large number of neuroepithelial neural stem cells (NSCs) in the proliferative areas of the brain, in particular. However, the adult nervous system and neurogenesis studies on rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss, are limited. Here, we studied the localization of glutamine synthetase (GS), vimentin (Vim), and nestin (Nes), as well as the neurons formed in the postembryonic period, labeled with doublecortin (DC), under conditions of homeostatic growth in adult cerebellum and brainstem of Oncorhynchus mykiss using immunohistochemical methods and Western Immunoblotting. We observed that the distribution of vimentin (Vim), nestin (Nes), and glutamine synthetase (GS), which are found in the aNSPCs of both embryonic types (neuroepithelial cells) and in the adult type (radial glia) in the cerebellum and the brainstem of trout, has certain features. Populations of the adult neural stem/progenitor cells (aNSPCs) expressing GS, Vim, and Nes have different morphologies, localizations, and patterns of cluster formation in the trout cerebellum and brainstem, which indicates the morphological and, obviously, functional heterogeneity of these cells. Immunolabeling of PCNA revealed areas in the cerebellum and brainstem of rainbow trout containing proliferating cells which coincide with areas expressing Vim, Nes, and GS. Double immunolabeling revealed the PCNA/GS PCNA/Vim coexpression patterns in the neuroepithelial-type cells in the PVZ of the brainstem. PCNA/GS coexpression in the RG was detected in the submarginal zone of the brainstem. The results of immunohistochemical study of the DC distribution in the cerebellum and brainstem of trout have showed a high level of expression of this marker in various cell populations. This may indicate: (i) high production of the adult-born neurons in the cerebellum and brainstem of adult trout, (ii) high plasticity of neurons in the cerebellum and brainstem of trout. We assume that the source of new cells in the trout brain, along with PVZ and SMZ, containing proliferating cells, may be local neurogenic niches containing the PCNA-positive and silent (PCNA-negative), but expressing NSC markers, cells. The identification of cells expressing DC, Vim, and Nes in the IX-X cranial nerve nuclei of trout was carried out.
... Further analyses of specific markers involved in emotional behaviors proposed this region to be homologous to the mammalian amygdala (von Trotha et al., 2014;Porter and Mueller, 2020;Lal et al., 2018). Likewise, the lateral pallium is involved in spatial learning and fulfills hippocampus-like functions (Broglio et al., 2010;Mueller, 2012;Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Research on learning and memory formation at the level of neural networks, as well as at the molecular level, is challenging due to the immense complexity of the brain. The zebrafish as a genetically tractable model organism can overcome many of the current challenges of studying molecular mechanisms of learning and memory formation. Zebrafish have a translucent, smaller and more accessible brain than that of mammals, allowing imaging of the entire brain during behavioral manipulations. Recent years have seen an extensive increase in published brain research describing the use of zebrafish for the study of learning and memory. Nevertheless, due to the complexity of the brain comprising many neural cell types that are difficult to isolate, it has been difficult to elucidate neural networks and molecular mechanisms involved in memory formation in an unbiased manner, even in zebrafish larvae. Therefore, data regarding the identity, location, and intensity of nascent proteins during memory formation is still sparse and our understanding of the molecular networks remains limited, indicating a need for new techniques. Here, we review recent progress in establishing learning paradigms for zebrafish and the development of methods to elucidate neural and molecular networks of learning. We describe various types of learning and highlight directions for future studies, focusing on molecular mechanisms of long-term memory formation and promising state-of-the-art techniques such as cell-type-specific metabolic labeling.
... Similarly, various forms of associative learning and memory, which are not robust in the first week of development, are linked to telencephalic function [103,104]. Avoidance conditioning, for instance, requires neurons in the adult dorsomedial telencephalon that are postulated to be analogous to the mammalian amygdala [105], whereas adult maze learning correlates with neurogenesis in caudal and medial regions of the pallium [106]. ...
Article
The last decade has seen a dramatic rise in the number of genes linked to neurological disorders, necessitating new models to explore underlying mechanisms and to test potential therapies. Over a similar period, many laboratories adopted zebrafish as tractable model for studying brain development, defining neural circuits, and performing chemical screens. Here we discuss strengths and limitations of using the zebrafish system to model neurological disorders. The underlying premise for many disease models is the high degree of homology between human and zebrafish genes, coupled with the conserved vertebrate Bauplan and repertoire of neurochemical signaling molecules. Yet we caution that important evolutionary divergences often limit the extent to which human symptoms can be meaningfully modeled in zebrafish. We outline advances in genetic technologies that allow human mutations to be faithfully reproduced in zebrafish. Together with methods that visualize the development and function of neuronal pathways at the single cell level, there is now an unprecedented opportunity to understand how disease-associated genetic changes disrupt neural circuits, a level of analysis that is ideally suited to uncovering pathogenic changes in human brain disorders.
... The gymnotiform thalamus and pallium includes circuitry (Giassi et al., 2012a,b;Trinh et al., 2016;Elliot et al., 2016) and physiology (Trinh et al., 2019;Elliot and Maler, 2015), consistent with the Recanatesi et al. (2021) model. These pallial regions are already believed to be responsible for spatial learning and navigation (Fotowat et al., 2019;Wallach et al., 2018;Mazzitelli-Fuentes et al., 2022). Pulse gymnotiform fish may therefore be a simple model for investigating the neural bases of apparently random behavior over a wide range of time scales. ...
Article
We analysed the trajectories of freely foraging Gymnotus sp., a pulse type gymnotiform weakly electric fish, swimming in a dark arena. We compared the fish's initial behaviour as it learned the relative location of landmarks and food, to its behaviour after learning was complete, i.e., after time/distance to locate food had reached a minimal asymptotic level. During initial exploration when the fish did not know the arena layout, trajectories included many sharp angle head turns that occurred at nearly completely random intervals. After spatial learning was complete, head turns became far smoother. Interestingly, the fish still did not take a stereotyped direct route to the food but instead took smooth but variable curved trajectories. We measured the fish's heading angle error (heading angle – heading angle towards food). After spatial learning, the fish's initial heading angle errors were strongly biased to zero, i.e., the fish mostly turned towards the food. As the fish approached closer to the food, they switched to a random search strategy with a more uniform distribution of heading angle errors.
Article
The study introduced and evaluated learning paradigms for Maylandia callainos cichlids using a modified version of the rodent T-maze, filled with tank water (the "sunken" modification). Both male and female fish underwent training in two distinct conditioning paradigms. Firstly, simple operant conditioning involved placing a food reward in either the right or left compartment. Cichlids demonstrated the ability to purposefully find the bait within 6 days of training, with a persistent place preference lasting up to 6 days. Additionally, the learning dynamics varied with sex: female cichlids exhibited reduction in latency to visit the target compartment and consume the bait, along with a decrease in the number of errors 3 and 4 days earlier than males, respectively. Secondly, visually-cued operant conditioning was conducted, with a food reward exclusively placed in the yellow compartment, randomly positioned on the left or right side of the maze during each training session. Visual learning persisted for 10 days until reaction time improvement plateaued. Color preference disappeared after 4 consecutive check-ups, with no sex-related interference. For further validation of visually-cued operant conditioning paradigm, drugs MK-801 (dizocilpine) and caffeine, known to affect performance in learning tasks, were administered intraperitoneally. Chronic MK-801 (0.17 mg/kg) impaired maze learning, resulting in no color preference development. Conversely, caffeine administration enhanced test performance, increasing precision in fish. This developed paradigm offers a viable approach for studying learning and memory and presents an effective alternative to rodent-based drug screening tools, exhibiting good face and predictive validity.
Article
Full-text available
Memory reconsolidation, the process by which memories are again stabilized after being reactivated, has strengthened the idea that memory stabilization is a highly plastic process. To date, the molecular and cellular bases of reconsolidation have been extensively investigated particularly within the hippocampus. However, the role of adult neurogenesis in memory reconsolidation is unclear. Here, we combined functional imaging, retroviral and chemogenetic approaches in rats to tag and manipulate different populations of rat adult-born neurons. We find that both mature and immature adult-born neurons are activated by remote memory retrieval. However, only specific silencing of the adult-born neurons immature during learning impairs remote memory retrieval-induced reconsolidation. Hence, our findings show that adult-born neurons immature during learning are required for the maintenance and update of remote memory reconsolidation.
Article
Full-text available
Adult neurogenesis, i.e., the generation of neurons from neural stem cells (NSCs) in the adult brain, contributes to brain plasticity in all vertebrates. It varies, however, greatly in extent, location and physiological characteristics between species. During the last decade, the teleost zebrafish (D. rerio) was increasingly used to study the molecular and cellular properties of adult NSCs, in particular as a prominent NSC population was discovered at the ventricular surface of the dorsal telencephalon (pallium), in territories homologous to the adult neurogenic niches of rodents. This model, for its specific features (large NSC population, amenability to intravital imaging, high regenerative capacity) allowed rapid progress in the characterization of basic adult NSC features. We review here these findings, with specific comparisons with the situation in rodents. We specifically discuss the cellular nature of NSCs (astroglial or neuroepithelial cells), their heterogeneities and their neurogenic lineages, and the mechanisms controlling NSC quiescence and fate choices, which all impact the neurogenic output. We further discuss the regulation of NSC activity in response to physiological triggers and non-physiological conditions such as regenerative contexts.
Article
Full-text available
The cellular basis and extent of neural stem cell (NSC) self-renewal in adult vertebrates, and their heterogeneity, remain controversial. To explore the functional behavior and dynamics of individual NSCs, we combined genetic lineage tracing, quantitative clonal analysis, intravital imaging, and global population assessments in the adult zebrafish telencephalon. Our results are compatible with a model where adult neurogenesis is organized in a hierarchy in which a subpopulation of deeply quiescent reservoir NSCs with long-term self-renewal potential generate, through asymmetric divisions, a pool of operational NSCs activating more frequently and taking stochastic fates biased toward neuronal differentiation. Our data further suggest the existence of an additional, upstream, progenitor population that supports the continuous generation of new reservoir NSCs, thus contributing to their overall expansion. Hence, we propose that the dynamics of vertebrate neurogenesis relies on a hierarchical organization where growth, self-renewal, and neurogenic functions are segregated between different NSC types.
Article
Full-text available
Zebrafish display widespread and pronounced adult neurogenesis, which is fundamental for their regeneration capability after central nervous system injury. However, the cellular identity and the biological properties of adult newborn neurons are elusive for most brain areas. Here, we used short-term lineage tracing of radial glia progeny to prospectively isolate newborn neurons from the her4.1+ radial glia lineage in the homeostatic adult forebrain. Transcriptome analysis of radial glia, newborn neurons and mature neurons using single cell sequencing identified distinct transcriptional profiles including novel markers for each population. Specifically, we detected 2 separate newborn neuron types, which showed diversity of cell fate commitment and location. Further analyses showed homology of these cell types to neurogenic cells in the mammalian brain, identified neurogenic commitment in proliferating radial glia and indicated that glutamatergic projection neurons are generated in the adult zebrafish telencephalon. Thus, we prospectively isolated adult newborn neurons from the adult zebrafish forebrain, identified markers for newborn and mature neurons in the adult brain, revealed intrinsic heterogeneity among adult newborn neurons and their homology to mammalian adult neurogenic cell types.
Article
Full-text available
Most vertebrates use active sensing strategies for perception, cognition and control of motor activity. These strategies include directed body/sensor movements or increases in discrete sensory sampling events. The weakly electric fish, Gymnotus sp., uses its active electric sense during navigation in the dark. Electric organ discharge rate undergoes transient increases during navigation to increase electrosensory sampling. Gymnotus also use stereotyped backward swimming as an important form of active sensing that brings objects towards the electroreceptor dense fovea-like head region. We wirelessly recorded neural activity from the pallium of freely swimming Gymnotus. Spiking activity was sparse and occurred only during swimming. Notably, most units tended to fire during backward swims and their activity was on average coupled to increases in sensory sampling. Our results provide the first characterization of neural activity in a hippocampal (CA3)-like region of a teleost fish brain and connects it to active sensing of spatial environmental features.
Article
Full-text available
Classically, the zebrafish model organism has been used to elucidate the genetic and cellular mechanisms related to development since the embryo forms and grows externally following fertilization. This provides insight into the genetic control of developmental processes in humans because their genomes are similar. Also, unlike other animal models, the genes of zebrafish can be manipulated quite easily by using reverse genetic screens tools such as morpholinos, which transiently silence target genes of interest or systems such as the transposon-mediated insertional mutagenesis or CRISPR-Cas9. Moreover, one pair of fish will provide up to 300 offspring, which means that if there is a gene of interest that is manipulated, then it can be transmitted to a large population of fish. What is beginning to emerge is that similar to other mammals, adult zebrafish have an integrated nervous system, which is proposed to contain homologous brain structures to those found in humans, as well as equivalent cellular and synaptic structure and function. Moreover, like humans, zebrafish exhibit age-related declines in cognitive functions, and a convergence of evidence has indicated that subtle changes in cellular and synaptic integrity underlie these changes. Therefore, the zebrafish is a powerful model organism for studying the neurobiological consequences of aging-related behavioral and biological changes, which offers the potential to identify possible interventions that would promote healthy aging. In what follows, we present and discuss recent findings and advances along these directions.
Article
Full-text available
Alterations in the social environment, such as isolating an individual that would normally live in a social group, can generate physiological responses that compromise an individual's capacity to learn. To investigate this, we tested whether social isolation impairs learning skills in the rainbow trout. We show that rainbow trout can achieve an active avoidance (AA) learning program with inter-individual variability. Moreover, c-Fos expression in dorsomedial telencephalon (Dm) correlates with the AA performance, indicating that this structure is involved in this cognitive task. Given that Dm participates in AA learning and this region is under plastic remodelling by addition of new-born neurons, we tested whether social isolation impinges on adult neurogenesis and, consequently, on the Dm cognitive outcome. Trout were reared for four weeks in control or isolated conditions. We found that social isolation diminished the percentage of adult-born neurons that are being incorporated into Dm network. Interestingly, the same isolation treatment also induced a severe deficit in the AA performance. Our results demonstrate a structure-to-function relationship between the Dm and the learning ability in an AA task, indicate that social isolation reduces the incorporation of adult-born neurons into Dm, and show that social isolation impairs the Dm-related cognitive function.
Article
Full-text available
It has been suggested that the dentate gyrus, particularly its new neurons generated via adult neurogenesis, is involved in memory acquisition and recall. Here, we trained rats in two types of Morris water maze tasks that are differentially associated with these two memory processes, and examined whether new neurons are differently affected by the two tasks performed during the second week of neuronal birth. Our results indicate that the task involving more opportunities to acquire new information better supports the survival of new neurons. Further, we assessed whether the two tasks differentially induce the expression of an immediate early gene, Zif268, which is known to be induced by neuronal activation. While the two tasks differentially induce Zif268 expression in the dentate gyrus, the proportions of new neurons activated were similar between the two tasks. Thus, we conclude that while the two tasks differentially activate the dentate gyrus, the task involving more opportunities for memory acquisition during the second week of the birth of new neurons better promotes the survival of the new neurons.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Fear conditioning is a form of learning essential for animal survival and used as a behavioral paradigm to study the mechanisms of learning and memory. In mammals, the amygdala plays a crucial role in fear conditioning. In teleost, the medial zone of the dorsal telencephalon (Dm) has been postulated to be a homolog of the mammalian amygdala by anatomical and ablation studies, showing a role in conditioned avoidance response. However, the neuronal populations required for a conditioned avoidance response via the Dm have not been functionally or genetically defined. Results: We aimed to identify the neuronal population essential for fear conditioning through a genetic approach in zebrafish. First, we performed large-scale gene trap and enhancer trap screens, and created transgenic fish lines that expressed Gal4FF, an engineered version of the Gal4 transcription activator, in specific regions in the brain. We then crossed these Gal4FF-expressing fish with the effector line carrying the botulinum neurotoxin gene downstream of the Gal4 binding sequence UAS, and analyzed the double transgenic fish for active avoidance fear conditioning. We identified 16 transgenic lines with Gal4FF expression in various brain areas showing reduced performance in avoidance responses. Two of them had Gal4 expression in populations of neurons located in subregions of the Dm, which we named 120A-Dm neurons. Inhibition of the 120A-Dm neurons also caused reduced performance in Pavlovian fear conditioning. The 120A-Dm neurons were mostly glutamatergic and had projections to other brain regions, including the hypothalamus and ventral telencephalon. Conclusions: Herein, we identified a subpopulation of neurons in the zebrafish Dm essential for fear conditioning. We propose that these are functional equivalents of neurons in the mammalian pallial amygdala, mediating the conditioned stimulus–unconditioned stimulus association. Thus, the study establishes a basis for understanding the evolutionary conservation and diversification of functional neural circuits mediating fear conditioning in vertebrates.
Article
Adult neurogenesis, the generation of functional neurons from adult neural stem cells in the central nervous system (CNS), is widespread, and perhaps universal, among vertebrates. This phenomenon is more pronounced in teleost fish than in any other vertebrate taxon. There are up to 100 neurogenic sites in the adult teleost brain. New cells, including neurons and glia, arise from neural stem cells harbored both in neurogenic niches and outside these niches (such as the ependymal layer and parenchyma in the spinal cord, respectively). At least some, but not all, of the stem cells are of astrocytic identity. Aging appears to lead to stem cell attrition in fish that exhibit determinate body growth but not in those with indeterminate growth. At least in some areas of the CNS, the activity of the neural stem cells results in additive neurogenesis or gliogenesis - tissue growth by net addition of cells. Mathematical and computational modeling has identified three factors to be crucial for sustained tissue growth and correct formation of CNS structures: symmetric stem cell division, cell death and cell drift due to population pressure. It is hypothesized that neurogenesis in the CNS is driven by continued growth of corresponding muscle fibers and sensory receptor cells in the periphery to ensure a constant ratio of peripheral versus central elements. This 'numerical matching hypothesis' can explain why neurogenesis has ceased in most parts of the adult CNS during the evolution of mammals, which show determinate growth.