ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Canine olfactory detection and its relevance to medical detection

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The extraordinary olfactory sense of canines combined with the possibility to learn by operant conditioning enables dogs for their use in medical detection in a wide range of applications. Research on the ability of medical detection dogs for the identification of individuals with infectious or non-infectious diseases has been promising, but compared to the well-established and–accepted use of sniffer dogs by the police, army and customs for substances such as money, explosives or drugs, the deployment of medical detection dogs is still in its infancy. There are several factors to be considered for standardisation prior to deployment of canine scent detection dogs. Individual odours in disease consist of different volatile organic molecules that differ in magnitude, volatility and concentration. Olfaction can be influenced by various parameters like genetics, environmental conditions, age, hydration, nutrition, microbiome, conditioning, training, management factors, diseases and pharmaceuticals. This review discusses current knowledge on the function and importance of canines’ olfaction and evaluates its limitations and the potential role of the dog as a biomedical detector for infectious and non-infectious diseases.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12879-021-06523-8
REVIEW
Canine olfactory detection andits relevance
tomedical detection
Paula Jendrny1, Friederike Twele1, Sebastian Meller1, Albertus Dominicus Marcellinus Erasmus Osterhaus2,
Esther Schalke3 and Holger Andreas Volk1*
Abstract
The extraordinary olfactory sense of canines combined with the possibility to learn by operant conditioning enables
dogs for their use in medical detection in a wide range of applications. Research on the ability of medical detection
dogs for the identification of individuals with infectious or non-infectious diseases has been promising, but compared
to the well-established and–accepted use of sniffer dogs by the police, army and customs for substances such as
money, explosives or drugs, the deployment of medical detection dogs is still in its infancy. There are several factors
to be considered for standardisation prior to deployment of canine scent detection dogs. Individual odours in disease
consist of different volatile organic molecules that differ in magnitude, volatility and concentration. Olfaction can
be influenced by various parameters like genetics, environmental conditions, age, hydration, nutrition, microbiome,
conditioning, training, management factors, diseases and pharmaceuticals. This review discusses current knowledge
on the function and importance of canines’ olfaction and evaluates its limitations and the potential role of the dog as
a biomedical detector for infectious and non-infectious diseases.
Keywords: Biomedical detection dogs, Olfaction, Olfactory sense, Screening method, Sniffer dogs
© The Author(s) 2021. Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which
permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the
original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or
other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line
to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory
regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this
licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http:// creat iveco
mmons. org/ publi cdoma in/ zero/1. 0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
Background
Canines are macrosmatics with an extraordinary olfac-
tory sense and memory [1, 2]. Olfaction is mandatory
for the dog to perceive environmental information,
which has been used successfully by humans for track-
ing and detection of pest and prey animals and other
food sources [3]. Routinely, dogs nowadays are pre-
dominantly deployed for the identification of explosives,
drugs, currencies, people, endangered animal species and
parasites [4]. In recent years, medical scenting dogs have
been trained to detect different medical conditions, but
this area of work is still relatively in its infancy [5]. e
use of odour detection as a diagnostic tool is of increas-
ing interest in recent [5, 6]. is review will summarise
information on odour origin and composition, neuro-
anatomy and physiology of the canine olfaction, differ-
ent impacts on the olfactory sense and majorly current
research outcomes critically evaluating the possible role
of the dog as a biomedical detector.
Methods
Search strategies for this review included electronic
search engines for publication databases, searching ref-
erence lists of published papers and information from
relevant scientific conferences and discussion groups.
For the section about biomedical detection dogs three
databases (Google Scholar, Science Direct and PubMed)
were searched for studies reporting the training, testing
and deployment of biomedical detection dogs between
2004 and 2021. e searches were performed by the
authors using the following keywords: “biomedical detec-
tion dogs” or “detection dogs” or “canines” in combina-
tion with “infectious diseases”, “non-infectious diseases”,
Open Access
*Correspondence: holger.volk@tiho-hannover.de
1 Department of Small Animal Medicine and Surgery, University
of Veterinary Medicine Hannover, Bünteweg 9, 30559 Hannover, Germany
Full list of author information is available at the end of the article
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 2 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
“malaria”, “SARS-CoV-2”, “COVID-19”, “hypoglycaemia”,
“epileptic seizure”, “cancer”, “bacteria” or “viral”. Peer-
reviewed studies and pre-prints published in English and
results presented at the WHO R&D Blueprint COVID-19
consultation [6] were evaluated. Literature that addressed
detection rate and/or diagnostic accuracy (sensitivity and
specificity) of biomedical detection dogs without restric-
tions to year of publication was included in this review
article. Only double-blinded and randomised studies for
SARS-CoV-2 detection were reported in this study.
Main text
Research onbiomedical detection dogs
e use of biomedical detection dogs for various infec-
tious and non-infectious diseases like Helicobacter pylori
[7], different cancer types [817], hypoglycaemia in dia-
betes mellitus patients [1820], epileptic seizures [21],
bacteriuria [22], bovine virus diarrhoea [23], COVID-19
[2433], Malaria [34] and Clostridium difficile-infections
[35] is still in its infancy (Table1). Most of these studies
indicate a disease-specific body odour or a specific vola-
tile organic compound (VOC)-pattern associated with
metabolic changes secondary to an infection [36]. In case
of an infection with a virus, VOCs are generated purely
by the host cell, but for bacteria, VOCs are generated
by the host and the bacteria respectively [36]. For many
diseases the exact odour molecules that are recognised
and indicated by dogs remain unknown. Disease-specific
VOC-patterns have been identified in diseases such as
asthma, several types of cancer, cystic fibrosis, diabe-
tes mellitus, dental diseases, gastrointestinal diseases,
heart allograft rejection, heart diseases, liver diseases,
pre-eclampsia, renal disease, cholera and tuberculosis
[3638].
Canine medical scent detection appears more promis-
ing for infectious diseases than non-infectious diseases
such as cancer, diabetes mellitus and epileptic seizures.
Despite some initially promising medical dog scent
detection studies, published data can vary significantly
for the identification of cancer. Studies with trained
sniffer dogs achieved very different results in the identifi-
cation of different cancer types, such as bladder, prostate
or ovarian cancer, lung and breast cancer as well as colo-
rectal neoplasms. Diagnostic accuracies varied with sen-
sitivities ranging from 19 to 99% and specificities from
73 to 99% when compared to histopathology [817]. Dif-
ferent sample materials were used for presentation, e.g.
urine, blood, breath or faeces, which could explain the
variability in findings. Another influencing factor that
plays a role regarding the variability of the results is the
lack of standardisation of training and the trainer bias,
which may have a major influence on the training results
of detection dogs [39]. More published data about cancer
detection by dogs is reviewed elsewhere [40, 41].
Medical scent detection dogs have also been deployed
for patients with diabetic mellitus. Identifying hypogly-
caemic conditions is crucial for people with diabetes mel-
litus because of the potential severity of such a condition.
A drop of blood is required to measure blood glucose,
which is an invasive method that must be performed
consciously and regularly. Not every patient is able to
take blood themselves. e deployment of a hypoglycae-
mia sniffer dog is a non-invasive method, but satisfactory
results have not been achieved. e researchers found
sensitivities between 36% [20] and 88% [42] and specifi-
cities of 49% [19]–98% [42] compared to the standard
method blood glucose measurement via blood glucose
meter.
e prediction of an epileptic seizure could help the
affected person to find a safe environment before the
seizure begins or to take emergency medication. It is
assumed that canines have the ability to detect an altera-
tion of the body odour. Due to a high variability of the
types and causes of epilepsy, it is still unknown which
specific odour the dogs detect but chemical analyses
could identify seizure-specific odour molecules [21].
In another study using sweat of persons with epilepsy,
canines distinguished between interictal and ictal sweat
with a probability of 93% and warned the individual
before a clinical seizure occurred with a probability of
82% [43]. Some studies also report seizure alerting dogs
that did not undergo any systematic training [4446].
ese dogs may detect specific odour-alterations as well
as visual cues or behavioural changes of the person with
epilepsy [4446].
Studies including the detection of infectious diseases
by dogs appear to be more promising. e training of
detection dogs in the following studies was reward-
based (based on positive reinforcement). Guest et al.,
2019, performed a study for the detection of protozoal
Malaria to develop a non-invasive screening method for
infected individuals [34]. Even in asymptomatic chil-
dren the dogs had a sensitivity and specificity of 72% and
91%, respectively. Previously worn nylon socks were pre-
sented to the two trained dogs. e results were higher
than the threshold for WHO malaria diagnostics [34].
e training of dogs to identify bacterial infections like
bacteriuria in urine [22] or Clostridium difficile in stool
samples [35] also generated promising results. Mau-
rer et al., 2016, trained dogs to improve strategies for
detecting early stages of bacteriuria before the infection
becomes serious. e dogs detected different pathogens
(Escherichia coli, Enterococcus, Klebsiella, Staphylococ-
cus aureus) with an overall sensitivity of close to 100%
and specificity of above 90% [22]. For the detection of
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 3 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
Table 1 Overview medical detection dog studies
Publication Authors Detection of Study design Sample material Sample size Results
Real-Time Detection of a
Virus Using Detection Dogs
[23]
Angle et al. (2016) Bovine viral diarrhea virus Randomised, blinded Cell culture n = 15 Sensitivity 91%
Specificity 99%
Trained dogs identify people
with malaria parasites by
their odour [34]
Guest et al. (2019) Malaria infection Randomised, blinded Body odour (socks) n = 175 Sensitivity 72%
Specificity 91%
Detection of Bacteriuria by
Canine Olfaction [22]Maurer et al. (2016) Bacteriuria Randomised, blinded Urine n = 687 Sensitivity near 100% Specific-
ity above 90%
Using Dog Scent Detec-
tion as a Point-of-Care
Tool to Identify Toxigenic
Clostridium difficile in Stool
[35]
Taylor et al. (2018) Toxigenic Clostridium difficile Randomised, blinded Faeces n = 300 Sensitivity 85%
Specificity 85%
Olfactory detection of
human bladder cancer by
dogs: Proof of principle
study [8]
Willis et al. (2004) Bladder cancer Randomised, blinded Urine n = 144 mean success rate 41%
Olfactory Detection of Pros-
tate Cancer by Dogs Sniff-
ing Urine: A Step Forward
in Early Diagnosis [9]
Cornu et al. (2011) Prostate cancer Randomised, blinded Urine n = 66 Sensitivity 91%
Specificity 91%
Key considerations for the
experimental training and
evaluation of cancer odour
detection dogs: lessons
learnt from a double-blind,
controlled trial of prostate
cancer detection [10]
Elliker et al. (2014) Prostate cancer Randomised, blinded Urine n = 181 Sensitivity 19%
Specificity 73%
A Proof of concept: Are
Detection Dogs a Useful
Tool to Verify Potential
Biomarkers Biomarkers for
lung cancer? [11]
Fischer-Tenhagen et al.
(2018) Lung cancer Randomised, blinded Absorbed breath samples n = 60 correct identification average
95%, correct negative indica-
tions average 60%
Accuracy of Canine Scent
Detection of Non–Small
Cell Lung Cancer in Blood
Serum [12]
Junqueira et al. (2019) Non–small cell lung cancer Randomised, blinded Blood serum n = 10 Sensitivity 97%, Specificity 98%
Diagnostic accuracy of
canine scent detection in
early- and late-stage lung
and breast cancers [14]
McCulloch et al. (2006) Lung and breast cancer Randomised, blinded Breath n = 169 Lung cancer: Sensitivity 99%
Specificity 99%
Breast cancer: Sensitivity 88%
Specificity 98%
How dogs learn to detect
colon cancer-Optimizing
the use of training aids [15]
Schoon et al. (2020) Colon cancer Randomised, blinded Faeces n = 70 Average hit rate 84%
Average false positive rate 12%
(for new unknown samples)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 4 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
Table 1 (continued)
Publication Authors Detection of Study design Sample material Sample size Results
Colorectal cancer screening
with odour material by
canine scent detection [17]
Sonoda et al. (2011) Colorectal cancer Randomised, blinded Breath and faeces n = 350 Breath: Sensitivity 91%
Specificity 99%
Faeces: Sensitivity 97%
Specificity 99%
Cancer odor in the blood of
ovarian cancer patients:
a retrospective study of
detection by dogs during
treatment, 3 and 6 months
afterward [16]
Horvath et al. (2013) Ovarian cancer Randomised, blinded Blood plasma n = 262 Sensitivity 97%
Specificity 99%
Can Trained Dogs Detect a
Hypoglycemic Scent in
Patients With Type 1 Diabe-
tes? [123]
Dehlinger et al. (2013) Hypoglycaemia Blinded Skin odour n = 24 Sensitivity 56%
Specificity 53%
Dogs Can Be Success-
fully Trained to Alert to
Hypoglycemia Samples
from Patients with Type 1
Diabetes [42]
Hardin et al. (2015) Hypoglycaemia Randomised, blinded Sweat n = 56 Sensitivity 50%-88%
Specificity 90%-98%
How effective are trained
dogs at alerting their own-
ers to changes in blood gly-
caemic levels?: Variations in
performance of glycaemia
alert dogs [18]
Rooney et al. (2019) Hypoglycaemia Not applicable Breath and sweat Not applicable Median sensitivity 83%
Variability of Diabetes Alert
Dog Accuracy in a Real-
World Setting [19]
Gonder-Frederick et al. (2017) Hypoglycaemia Not applicable Body odour Not applicable Sensitivity 57%
Specificity 49%
Reliability of Trained Dogs
to Alert to Hypoglycemia
in Patients With Type 1
Diabetes [20]
Los et al. (2017) Hypoglycaemia Not applicable Body odour Not applicable Sensitivity 36%
Dogs demonstrate the exist-
ence of an epileptic seizure
odour in humans [21]
Catala et al. (2019) Epileptic seizure Pseudo-randomised, blinded Breath and sweat n = 5 Sensitivity 87%
Specificity 98%
Canine detection of volatile
organic compounds
unique to human epileptic
seizure [43]
Maa et al. (2021) Epileptic seizure Randomised, blinded Sweat n = 60 Probability of distinguishing
ictal versus interictal sweat
93%
Probability of canine detection
of seizure scent preceded
clinical seizure 82%
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 5 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
toxigenic Clostridium difficile in stool samples, two dogs
were trained and achieved sensitivities of 78% and 93% as
well as specificities of 85%, respectively. e aim of this
study was to evaluate the dog method as a “point-of-care”
diagnostic tool [35]. Lastly, it was also possible to train
dogs to detect viral infections with bovine viruses [23] or
with the coronavirus SARS-CoV-2 in various body fluids
[6, 2433] with high rates of diagnostic accuracy. Real-
time methods for the identification of viral infections are
often limited or not existing, especially for resource-lim-
ited environments. Angle etal., [23] examined the abil-
ity of two dogs to detect and discriminate bovine viral
diarrhoea virus cell cultures from cell cultures infected
with bovine herpes virus 1, bovine parainfluenza virus
and controls with high rates of sensitivity and specificity
(Table1).
Recently, there is a rapid, growing body of evidence for
detection dogs being used for identifying SARS-CoV-2
infected individuals [2433]. In the SARS-CoV-2 detec-
tion dog studies, different sample material, study designs
and dog breeds were used in different countries. Most of
these studies achieved promising results (Table2). e
most common dog breeds used were Malinois, other
shepherd breeds and Labrador Retrievers. ese dogs
are specifically bred for scent detection, selected for
their scenting ability with an appropriate cognition and
motivation behaviour making them popular breeds for
biomedical detection [47]. e samples were collected
initially mainly from hospitalised COVID-19 patients,
but now as well as from asymptomatic and mildly symp-
tomatic infected individuals with a variety of symptoms.
Some researchers used distractors (samples from indi-
viduals suffering from other respiratory diseases than
COVID-19) in the training and testing phases, which
were slightly different from the target scent to better
represent conditions in the field where other respira-
tory diseases different from COVID-19 will be to be also
presented. A wide variety of human body fluids (saliva,
tracheobronchial secretions, urine, sweat) as well as
nasopharyngeal swabs, breath samples and masks or
clothing were used as sample materials for presentation
to dogs during training and testing [2433]. Interestingly,
dogs trained with saliva samples were also able to detect
samples of infected individuals in sweat and urine with-
out further training which is indicative for a successful
generalisation process [25]. In detection dog training, it
is important to pay equal attention to generalisation and
discrimination. Generalisation means that after success-
ful training, the dog also reacts to new, unknown stimuli
that have similar odour properties to the training odour,
whereas discrimination means the ability to distinguish
between similar stimuli [48]. Without successful general-
isation by the dogs, they would memorise the individual
odours of the training samples individually and would
have great difficulty recognising new samples as positive
or negative. A lack of discrimination process would mean
that the dogs would not only indicate the specific disease
they were trained for, but would also indicate similar
odours, e.g. respiratory diseases other than COVID-19,
which would preclude the dog’s use as a screening
method.
e various sample materials differ in the ease of collec-
tion, VOCs contained and infectivity, e.g. sweat or urine
samples seem to be less infectious than saliva [49, 50].
Our own canine experience has shown that sensitivity
and specificity of each dog appeared slightly different for
each presented sample, but fairly similar overall for each
bodyfluid [25]. e reason for this could be that not every
dog learned the same VOC-pattern as being positive but
slightly different ones. When working with detection
dogs, it is not possible to know exactly to which specific
VOCs the dogs were conditioned to. It is also unknown
whether each dog had learned the same disease-specific
VOC-patterns as being positive. Nevertheless, the study
results of the different research groups indicate that all
dogs could be successfully conditioned to a specific virus-
induced odour, otherwise the results listed below could
not have been achieved [24, 25]. Most studies have not
pre-selected dogs, but this would be needed when using
them as a diagnostic test, with only the best performing
dogs being used.
Sensitivities in the different studies ranged from 65 to
100%, specificities from 76 to 99% (Table2).
e training and test design in the various studies dif-
fered, but the dog training in all of them was based on
positive reinforcement. As training and testing setup,
most of the studies included the dogs working on a line-
up with different numbers of samples presented. Sample
material from SARS-CoV-2 positive individuals was used
as target samples, negative controls were obtained from
healthy individuals and only some groups also used dis-
tractors (sample material from individuals suffering from
other respiratory diseases other than COVID-19) to train
and test the detection dogs.
Origin andcomposition ofodours
What are dogs scenting when they identify an infected
individual? e complex process of odour recognition
starts with the development and composition of odours
[51]. e majority of odours detected by dogs through
inhaling are VOCs in different compositions residing
in the air [51]. VOCs can differ in magnitude, volatility,
and concentration. e odour concentration in the air
correlates with the concentration of its source, volatil-
ity, the sources odour releasing surface area, the volume
flow rate, ambient air movements and diffusion velocity
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 6 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
within its source [52]. On top of that and depending on
the materials in contact with specific odours, adsorption
or absorption of VOCs occurs which is important for
sampling and sample presentation for biomedical detec-
tion dogs. In general, liquids and plastic polymers absorb
odours whereas surfaces like metal, glass, wood and cot-
ton adsorb and release them [53].
e term ‘VOC’ describes atmospheric trace gases
except for carbon dioxide and monoxide. Biogenic VOCs
e.g. are isoprene and monoterpenes (most prominent
compounds), as well as alkanes, alkenes, carbonyls, alco-
hols, esters, ethers and acids [52], have a strong odour
and are produced as well as emitted by animals, plants
and micro-organisms. e VOC-pattern of an organ-
ism is governed by VOC-producing cells or tissues and
largely determined by its physiological or patho-physio-
logical metabolism, the latter being subject to exogenous
influences like infections, skin emanations or smokers’
breath [52]. Different diseases cause the emergence and
emission of more or less specific VOC-patterns [36],
which can be used as diagnostic olfactory biomarkers.
Abd El Quader et al. [54] identified pathogen-related
VOCs emanated from viral and bacterial cultures and
Steppert etal., [55] found a difference in emanated VOCs
between SARS-CoV-2 and Influenza-A infections in
human breath. Various possibilities of measuring spe-
cific VOCs are existent, such as gas chromatography
mass-spectrometric techniques (GC–MS) for identifica-
tion and characterisation [36]. e diagnostic potential
of scent detection dogs for VOC-based disease detection
has been discussed recently [51].
VOCs are liberated from various tissues and body flu-
ids. e most common body fluids or tissues for diagnos-
tic testing are skin emanations, urine, blood, saliva and
faeces differing in their VOC-composition [56]. Human
bodies emit an extensive repertoire of VOCs that vary
with age, diet, gender, genetics and physiological or
pathological status and can be considered as individual
attributes [36]. Pathological processes influence the
body odour either by producing new VOCs or by chang-
ing the VOC-pattern which dogs may be able to detect
[36]. During the training of biomedical detection dogs
it is therefore important that dogs are not conditioned
to the individual odours of the subjects or the environ-
ment were samples were produced (e.g. hospital smell)
but learn the disease-specific odour (VOC-pattern) and
successfully complete the generalisation process. It is also
important to emphasize that the biochemical origins for
some of the VOCs have not been completely elucidated
until now.
Neuroanatomy ofolfaction
e anatomical construction of the olfactory system is
highly structured in order to ensure efficient nasal odor-
ant transport as well as respiratory airflow [57]. e sen-
sory impression emerges through the olfactory system
[58]. e substantial elements of the canine olfactory sys-
tem are the outer nose with nares and nasal wings, nasal
cavity, the olfactory epithelium with receptors, the vome-
ronasal organ, the olfactory bulb and the olfactory cortex
of the cerebrum [58]. e bilateral nasal cavity is divided
in the median plane by the nasal septum. Each side
includes a nasal vestibule lined with cutaneous mucosa,
a respiratory and an olfactory region, but also comprises
a naso-, maxillo- (lined with respiratory epithelium and
a small number of olfactory neurons) and ethmotur-
binate (olfactory epithelium) to increase the olfactory
mucosal surface, especially in macrosmatics [58, 59]. e
three turbinates divide the nasal cavity’s chamber into
three meatuses of the nose, whereby the ventral meatus
is responsible for the respiration (inspiration and expi-
ration). e dorsal meatus leads to the olfactory organ,
whereas the middle nasal meatus terminates in the para-
nasal sinuses [58].
Figure1 shows the general structure of the olfactory
mucosa. All components between the lumen of the nasal
cavity and the cribriform plate are shown in simplified
form. e nasal cavity lining has the function to sepa-
rate odour molecules by their partition coefficients into
the mucosa [60] and to create different flow dynamics in
order to distribute odour molecules to the receptors, thus
patterning the odorants [61]. Olfactory molecules in the
nasal cavity lumen bind to olfactory receptors on the cilia
of olfactory receptor cells embedded between supporting
cells. e respiratory epithelium consists of a multi-row
ciliated epithelium with goblet cells [58]. e olfactory
epithelium implies a pseudostratified columnar neuro-
epithelium [62] located next to the cribriform plate and
lining the turbulate bones symmetrically in the nasal cav-
ity [63] with millions of olfactory receptor cells (ORC)
and olfactory receptors (OR), but also supporting susten-
tacular cells regulating the nasal mucous composition,
isolating the ORCs and protecting the epithelium from
inhaled potentially dangerous substances [64]. Moreover,
basal cells are located adjacent to the lamina propria in
the olfactory epithelium and comprise Bowman’s glands
in the lamina propria whose secretion builds a mucous
layer in combination with the sustentacular cells’ sub-
stances which maintain nasal humidity and capture odor-
ants [58, 62]. e lamina propria itself is adjacent to the
bony lamina cribrosa, which is traversed by olfactory
nerve fibres. e regular olfactory perception depends on
this area [64].
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 7 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
An additional olfactory system can be found in the
vomeronasal organ of dogs (Fig.2).
Via unique airflow patterns, environmental odorants
selectively bind to the ORs to initiate odour perception
[59]. e ORC is a bipolar neuron: the dendrite extends
in the direction of the olfactory epithelium (nasal cavity)
and terminates with the ORs located in the membrane
of multiple cilia in the mucous layer, whereas the axons
of all ORCs build the olfactory ‘nerve’ (fila olfactoria)
passing through the cribriform plate and to the olfac-
tory bulb. e complex structure gives dogs the ability
to detect an enormous quantity of different odour mole-
cules with subtle shape, size or stereoisomeric differences
[65, 66]. e exact sequence of the olfactory process at
the molecular level has been reviewed elsewhere [58].
e glomeruli approach dendrites of mitral cells and
tufted cells whose axons constitute the lateral olfactory
tract that conducts the signal to the piriform cortex and
project it to the olfactory cortex in the medial temporal
lobes [64].
e olfactory cortex receives sensory signals from the
olfactory bulb. e processing of olfactory signals in the
brain is also beyond the scope of this review and can be
found elsewhere [58]. An overview of important olfac-
tory characteristics of dogs is presented in Table3.
Figure2 shows a comparison of the olfactory system
between dog and human. Components of the olfactory
system are shown in colour. Particularly noticeable are
the differences in extent, shape and position of the olfac-
tory bulbs and the vomeronasal organ, which is present
only in dogs but not in humans. It is located bilaterally
symmetric on the ventro-rostral bottom of the nasal cav-
ity behind the canine teeth and associated to the nasal
and oral cavity. Its sensory epithelium detects mainly
pheromones and non-volatile molecules for intra-spe-
cies-specific communication and reproduction. e
transmission follows a separate pathway directly to the
hypothalamus [59, 67]. Figure3 presents important inner
structure of the olfactory system.
Physiology ofolfaction anduid dynamics
Animals use olfaction to find and select nourishment or
prey [68, 69], for recognition of social partners, preda-
tors or environmental toxins as well as for orientation
and communication [36, 70]. Body odours function as
indicators of the metabolic status of individuals [36].
Table 2 Overview of SARS-CoV-2 detection dog studies
All included studies were double-blinded and randomised
Country Sample material Number of sample
presentations (test) Results
Sensitivity Specicity
France [26] Sweat n = 321 90% and 88% 90% and 85%
Germany [24, 25] Inactivated saliva/tracheobronchial
secretion n = 1012 83% 96%
Non-inactivated saliva n = 2513 82% 96%
Sweat n = 531 91% 94%
Urine n = 594 95% 98%
Iran [29] Nasopharyngeal n = 80 65% 89%
Masks and clothes n = 120 86% 93%
Colombia [28] Saliva/respiratory secretions n = 9200 89% 97%
Brazil [6] Not applicable Not applicable Not applicable Not applicable
United Arab Emirates [30] Axillary sweat n = 1368 92% 96%
United Arab Emirates [32] Sweat n = 3290 83% 99%
Argentina [6] Not applicable Not applicable 93% 89%
Australia [6] Not applicable Not applicable 100% 95%
Lebanon [6] Sweat Not applicable 100% 92%
Sweat (airport) Not applicable 96% 90%
Chile [6] Sweat Not applicable 90% 97%
Finland [6] Sweat, urine, saliva Not applicable 100% 91%
Belgium [6] Sweat Not applicable 81% 98%
United Kingdom [31] Breath and sweat n = 2261 82%-94% 76%-92%
USA [27] Saliva and urine n = 59 11–22% 94–100%
Urine 71% 99%
USA [33] Breath n = 160 Not applicable Not applicable
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 8 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
e canine’s scent detection ability limit for VOCs has a
reported range of parts per million and parts per trillion
[71]. Different types of airways in the canine nasal cavity
are shown in Fig.4.
In the cognition process of various stimuli, hemispheric
specialization takes place [72]. For the olfactory path-
way, it means, that olfactory stimuli ascend ipsilaterally
from the detection place in the nasal cavity to the place
of perception in the olfactory cortex [72]. Dogs use pref-
erentially the right nostril to detect conspecific arousal
or novel odours, transmitting sensory input to the right
cerebral hemisphere to process alarming stimuli. e left
nostril is preferentially used to sniff non-aversive, famil-
iar and heterospecific arousal odours as well as target
odours by detection dogs [73].
Current literature is not clear about breed-specific
olfactory capabilities and discusses the influence of
genetic polymorphism in comparison to behaviour and
trainability [47, 74, 75].
Factors impacting olfaction
ere are several circumstances which can affect the
olfactory sense of dogs [58]. Some are of physiological
origin, others are pathological. Especially when working
with biomedical detection dogs, it is important to know
these factors and adjust the working conditions for the
dogs as best as possible.
Physiological variation in the olfactory capability is
most frequently caused by differences in genetics. In gen-
eral, macrosmatic animals have an olfactory gene array of
greater extent, much larger than microsmatics. A com-
prehensive overview of the genetic influence on the sense
of smell in dogs can be found in the according literature
[7681].
Differences in the olfactory capabilities of different dog
breeds and wolves are also described. Polgàr etal., [47],
compared detection abilities of dog breeds selected for
scenting abilities, dog breeds for other purposes, brachy-
cephalic dog breeds and hand-raised wolves. As a result,
the breeds selected for odour work (e.g. Shepherd Dogs
or Labradors) and in some tests even the wolves per-
formed better than the other dogs and the short-nosed
breeds.
Secondly, the environmental conditions influence the
odour sensing abilities. Relative humidity and baromet-
ric pressure may directly affect olfaction, besides their
effects on odour emergence and movement itself, while
heat has only indirect effects [58, 8284]. Acclimatiza-
tion to the environment, physical fitness and an adequate
hydration state can prevent heat stress of the dogs [84].
Age can influence the sensory process [85]. Olfaction
and its cognition are impacted by age in humans [86] as
well as in dogs [58]. Age affects various functional parts
of the olfactory system in dogs at an age of older than
14years [86, 87].
Fig. 1 Schematic structure of the olfactory mucosa
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 9 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
Conditioning, training, and management play a major
role for the use of dogs as detection dogs [58]. Exer-
cise and condition deficiencies are described as physi-
cal stressors which may affect the olfaction in canines
directly or indirectly [58]. Physical exercise affects the
olfaction of detection dogs by decreasing finding rates,
especially in dogs with poor physical conditions [88,
89]. As a result, a working dog should be well trained
to have an optimal physical condition [90]. Scent detec-
tion training techniques can improve odour sensitivity
and discrimination [8992]. Housing and general man-
agement may influence the dogs’ detection work as well
by affecting the learning capability. Lower stress levels
due to social contact and an enriched, secure environ-
ment were shown to enhance cognitive performance
[87]. Positive rewards with particularly tasty treats as
well as a specific toy in some dogs increase working
motivation of dogs whereas aversive training methods
decrease motivation and have also negative effects on
their physical and mental health [93].
Hydration [84], nutrition [88, 89], and the micro-
biome [58] of dogs manipulate the olfactory sense as
well. As mentioned above, heat stress influences olfac-
tion due to provocation of panting but dogs are able
to develop heat tolerance by establishing an adequate
hydration status [84, 89].
e nutritional factor influencing the olfactory sense
of dogs includes the feeding time, amount of food per
meal, ingredients like the fat/protein ratio and the fat
source [88, 89, 9498].
Various diseases and medication can affect the olfac-
tion of dogs and lead to hyposmia or anosmia. More
information on hyposmia can be found elsewhere [58,
99].
Diseases or disorders potentially leading to hyposmia
or anosmia in humans and potentially in dogs [100] are
congenital and neurodegenerative diseases [86, 101],
metabolic, endocrine (hyperadrenocorticism, diabetes
mellitus, and hypothyreoidism [101]) and neurologi-
cal diseases like nasal/brain tumours, granulomatous
Fig. 2 Schematic structure of the olfactory system in dogs and humans
Table 3 Olfactory characteristics of dogs
Characteristics Dog
Airflow A sniff creates unique unidirectional laminar airflow patterns to transport environmental odor-
ants to the olfactory epithelium [122]
Size of olfactory mucosa 95–126 cm2 (German Shepherd) [124]
Olfactory genes in genome > 1000; 80% functional receptor genes and 20% pseudogenes [125]
Amount of olfactory receptor cells (ORCs) 200–300 million in nasal cavity [58, 59]
Cilia per ORC 20 to 100 cilia per cell [58]
Extent, shape and position of olfactory bulb Proportionally larger than in humans and prominently at the ventro-rostral area of the brain [126]
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 10 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
meningoencephalitis or head trauma [102], general
inflammation and systemic diseases, exposure to dust
and toxic chemicals/materials, uraemia and blood flow
changes as well as the hydration state [103]. Different
infections of the upper respiratory tract, e.g. SARS-
CoV-2-infections, can also cause anosmia in humans
[104]. Whether dogs play a role in the infection inci-
dence of SARS-CoV-2 is controversially discussed in
the literature and there is no evidence that dogs might
be affected by anosmia through a SARS-CoV-2-infec-
tion so far, but it also cannot be completely excluded by
now [105107]. Dog-specific viral diseases like canine
distemper virus and canine parainfluenza virus [108]
cause conductive hyposmia by generating nasal inflam-
mation and increasing mucous secretion and result
in vascular congestion that alters the air flow. Fur-
thermore, allergic rhinitis and turbinate engorgement
caused by hypocapnia, cold air, irritating chemicals or
an increased parasympathetic tone result in olfactory
decrease or loss [109].
Some pharmaceuticals used in human medicine are
also applicable for dogs and may potentially have sim-
ilar effects in dogs [110]. Only specific effects of ster-
oids, antibiotics and anaesthetics on the dog’s olfaction
are documented in the scientific literature at this time
[58]. Other medications potentially endangering dog’s
olfaction are described elsewhere [58, 111116].
Discussion
e special structure and physiology of the canine olfac-
tory system contain a huge potential of olfactory power
[58]. e dog’s sense of smell is mainly used to attract
prey and to perceive the environment but could also be
promoted and meaningfully used by humans for biomed-
ical purposes. Since the vomeronasal organ (VNO) has
an important function in intra-species communication or
the detection of pheromones and is capable of processing
a wide variety of molecules, it may be possible that direct
detection of viruses or viral proteins (not VOCs) by the
VNO occurs, thus representing a different mechanism of
odour perception. However, this is only a hypothesis and
has not yet been proven.
Various diagnostic studies have addressed the detec-
tion of different diseases by dogs. Despite the promising
results of the scent detection dogs, this method is only
marginally or not used in the field of human medicine.
e majority of medical professionals continues to rely
on diagnostic standard methods although the canine
medical detection method achieved equal or even higher
rates of diagnostic accuracy. For example, electronic
noses have a limit of detection of 100 to 400 parts per
Fig. 3 Sagittal magnetic resonance imaging highlighting the inner structures of the olfactory system. The blue area represents the vomeronasal
organ, the respiratory epithelium in the maxilloturbinates is highlighted in yellow, the olfactory epithelium in the ethmoturbinates near the lamina
cribrosa is shown in green, and the red area contains the bulbus olfactorius
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 11 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
billion (ppb) (1 × 10–7) [117] whereas the olfactory detec-
tion threshold of dogs is lower than 0.001ppb (1 × 10–12)
[71], so they surpass this technology by far. But incon-
sistent findings and the complexity of this research area
prevents the practitioners from including this method
in their daily routine. Moreover, a medical device or
health technology requires an approval by national health
organizations before permission for usage is granted. For
such approval, ethical, social, organisational, and legal
aspects are assessed alongside technological, economic
and safety aspects, as well as clinical effectiveness [5].
Other limitations of the medical scenting dog method
is the current lack of standardisation of the training and
deployment of biomedical detection dogs (although this
has been tried in several detection dog studies [5, 118]) as
each dog has an individual character, an individual train-
ing level or training requirements, and there are several
different breeds with a variation of characters and olfac-
tory thresholds. But there are legislated guidelines and
commission regulations for the use of explosive detec-
tion dogs, which could and should be used in a modified
form for biomedical detection dogs as well [119, 120]. In
addition, dogs are living creatures with varying detection
performances at different times. Moreover, the training
condition has to be maintained with regular training,
unlike in machines. Variation of detection accuracy may
be caused by failure in odour conditioning, lack of moti-
vation, inappropriate training methods (e.g. alternative
forced choice without blank trials [5]) or other confound-
ing factors [40]. A test run at the beginning of the detec-
tion work could reduce the error frequency of the dogs.
e disease detection dog studies differ in terms of exper-
imental setup, sample material (urine, breath, blood,
saliva, faeces, sweat/body odour) and sampling method,
individual dog characteristics, dog training methods and
evaluation strategies of the results. For some diseases like
different cancer types, the canine method seems to be
not very useful due to the need of reliable identification
of early, preclinical stages that require surgical interven-
tion. To reliably diagnose a certain disease, laboratory
testing or equivalent methods still have to be performed
because of the fact that the canine method is not gener-
ally accepted and approved.
e advantages of the canine method are especially
the non-invasiveness, speed, nearly immediate results,
effectiveness of testing, cost effectiveness, mobility, high
sensitivity and specificity of the dogs’ noses, safety for
persons to be tested and persons performing the test (dog
handlers), the simplicity and security of the sampling,
testing procedure, specimen storage and evaluation of
the results. Acquisition and training of a medical detec-
tion dog is maybe less cost-intensive than the purchase
of expensive high-tech equipment. e sample collec-
tion requires no special abilities of the performing person
and is not associated with any health risk for the patient
due to the non-invasiveness in contrast to some standard
methods. e samples can be preserved for some time
and presented to several dogs which may increase the
diagnostic accuracy. e sample material can be adapted
to the disease to be tested (disease-specific VOCs) and
even varied if necessary to reduce or eliminate the infec-
tion risk of the operating persons and dogs. e training
period for emerging diseases is much less time-consum-
ing than inventing a new technological test method. If
the training period is once completed, the testing pro-
cedure is easy and time saving. While testing, there are
four possibilities for the dogs to respond to the presented
samples: True positive means, the dog correctly indicates
Fig. 4 Three-dimensional computed tomographic reconstruction of
a canine skull. The arrows represent the airways, with the pink arrow
showing the common airflow and the red and blue arrows showing
the olfactory and respiratory airflow, respectively. The nostrils, the
olfactory and respiratory epithelium as well as the olfactory bulb, and
the tracheal tube are labeled. During inhalation the air flows from
the nares and the nasal vestibule to the maxilloturbinates, then into
the ethmoturbinates and the paranasal sinuses towards the pharynx
[122]. There is a major difference between breathing and sniffing
in dogs. While breathing, most of the inspired air flows through the
nasopharynx into the lungs but only a small percentage (12–13%)
reaches the olfactory areas [59]. The sniffing process generates
external (outside the nostrils) and internal (within the nasal cavity)
fluid dynamics. The ambient air is inhaled from the front and exhaled
to the side for efficient odorant sampling, whereas each nostril
samples separately. A sniff is the first critical step of the olfactory
process with the function of generating unique unidirectional
laminar airflow patterns to transport environmental odorants into the
nasal cavity to the olfactory epithelium [122]. Furthermore, sniffing
increases odour sensitivity and affects the intensity of odorants [60,
90]
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 12 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
a disease-positive sample; false positive means, the dog
incorrectly indicates a negative control or distractor sam-
ple; true negative: the dog correctly does not indicate a
negative control or distractor sample; and false negative:
the dog incorrectly does not indicate a positive sample.
For evaluation of the results and assessment of the diag-
nostic accuracy, the use of contingency tables can be
useful. For testing of unknown samples, the possible indi-
cations are of binary character (disease-positive or -nega-
tive). e effectiveness of the dog method is also a great
advantage. Dogs can screen large amounts of people in a
short time with high rates of diagnostic accuracy. After
a successful training phase, the dogs can be deployed in
any setting or terrain, whereas most technological meth-
ods require standardised environmental conditions to
function reliably, highlighting mobility as another mean-
ingful advantage of the dogs. In summary, this method
has promising potential for the effective detection of var-
ious infectious and non-infectious diseases after major
limitations have been eliminated. Especially in countries
with a lack of access to high technology screening meth-
ods or as a preliminary mass screening for infectious dis-
eases at major events or airports the canine method has a
huge potential.
Conclusion
e use of biomedical detection dogs has many advan-
tages and potential, but also some limitations. e lit-
erature shows that detection dogs can be considered as
a screening method, especially for infectious diseases
but may not be considered as a substitute for standard
diagnostic methods until standardised and validated. In
order to use biomedical detection dogs as an approved
screening method for disease detection, the following
issues need to be addressed: Standardisation of training
and deployment techniques (ensuring generalisation to
specific disease stages, symptomatic and asymptomatic
patients), reproducibility within and between detec-
tion dogs, and (re-)certification by an official body. At
this time, it should be recognised as an additional non-
invasive, rapid diagnostic tool to effectively detect early
stages of specific diseases in great confluences of people.
Additional research is necessary to create a standardised,
operationally viable system for canine olfactory detec-
tion of various human diseases. In addition, the ability
of dogs to be able to discriminate between healthy and
diseased patients can support identification of diseases in
which VOCs could be characterised, e.g. via GC–MS like
in Sethi etal. [121] for the development of different VOC
based test systems.
Abbreviations
VOC: Volatile organic compound; COVID-19: Coronavirus disease 2019; OR:
Olfactory receptor; ORC: Olfactory receptor cell; ppb: Parts per billion.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Antja Watanangura for the artwork.
Authors’ contributions
PJ drafted the manuscript and did most of the literature review. FT, SM, and
HAV also drafted the manuscript, helped with the literature review, and the
design of the article. ES provided helpful input regarding the training and
deployment of biomedical detection dogs. All authors read and approved the
final manuscript.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Availability of data and materials
Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or
analysed during the current study.
Declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Not applicable.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Author details
1 Department of Small Animal Medicine and Surgery, University of Veterinary
Medicine Hannover, Bünteweg 9, 30559 Hannover, Germany. 2 Research Center
for Emerging Infections and Zoonoses, University of Veterinary Medicine Han-
nover, Bünteweg 17, 30559 Hannover, Germany. 3 Bundeswehr School of Dog
Handling, Gräfin-Maltzan-Kaserne, Hochstraße, 56766 Ulmen, Germany.
Received: 17 May 2021 Accepted: 3 August 2021
References
1. Barrios AW, Sánchez-Quinteiro P, Salazar I. Dog and mouse: Toward a
balanced view of the mammalian olfactory system. Front Neuroanat.
2014;8:1–7.
2. Pirrone F, Albertini M. Olfactory detection of cancer by trained sniffer
dogs: a systematic review of the literature. J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res.
2017;19:105–17.
3. Marchal S, Bregeras O, Puaux D, Gervais R, Ferry B. Rigorous training
of dogs leads to high accuracy in human scent matching-to-sample
performance. PLoS ONE. 2016;11(2): e0146963. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/
journ al. pone. 01469 63.
4. Brown C, Stafford K, Fordham R. The use of scent-detection dogs. Ir Vet
J. 2006;59(2):97–104.
5. Koivusalo M, Reeve C. Biomedical scent detection dogs: would they
pass as a healthtechnology. Pet Behav Sci. 2018;6:1–6. https:// doi. org/
10. 21071/ pbs. v0i6. 10785.
6. WHO R&D Blueprint COVID-19 Consultation on the use of trained dogs
forscreening COVID-19 cases. WHO. 8 March 2021. https:// cdn. who.
int/ media/ docs/ defau lt- source/ blue- print/ who- consu ltati on- scree
ning- dogs-- 8th- march- 2021. pdf? sfvrsn= a0d8c bda_ 1& downl oad= true.
Accessed 16 May 2021.
7. Pavlou AK, et al. An intelligent rapid odour recognition model in dis-
crimination of Helicobacter pylori and other gastroesophageal isolates
in vitro. Biosens Bioelectron. 2000;15:333–42.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 13 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
8. Willis CM, et al. Olfactory detection of human bladder cancer by dogs:
proof of principle study. Br Med J. 2004. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1136/ bmj.
329. 7468. 712.
9. Cornu JN, Cancel-Tassin G, Ondet V, Girardet C, Cussenot O. Olfactory
detection of prostate cancer by dogs sniffing urine: a step forward in
early diagnosis. Eur Urol. 2011;59:197–201.
10. Elliker KR, et al. Key considerations for the experimental training and
evaluation of cancer odour detection dogs: lessons learnt from a
double-blind, controlled trial of prostate cancer detection. BMC Urol.
2014;14:1.
11. Fischer-Tenhagen C, Johnen D, Nehls I, Becker R. A proof of concept:
are detection dogs a useful tool to verify potential biomarkers for lung
cancer? Front Vet Sci. 2018;5:1–6.
12. Junqueira H, et al. Accuracy of canine scent detection of non–small cell
lung cancer in blood serum. J Am Osteopath Assoc. 2019;119:413–8.
13. Amundsen T, Sundstrom S, Buvik T, Gederaas OA, Haaverstad R. Can
dogs smell lung cancer? First study using exhaled breath and urine
screening in unselected patients with suspected lung cancer. Acta
Oncol (Madr). 2014;53:307–15.
14. McCulloch M, et al. Diagnostic accuracy of canine scent detection
in early- and late-stage lung and breast cancers. Integr Cancer Ther.
2006;5:30–9.
15. Schoon GAA, De Jonge D, Hilverink P. How dogs learn to detect colon
cancer—optimizing the use of training aids. J Vet Behav. 2020;35:38–44.
16. Horvath G, Andersson H, Nemes S. Cancer odor in the blood of ovarian
cancer patients: a retrospective study of detection by dogs during
treatment, 3 and 6 months afterward. BMC Cancer. 2013;13:396. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1186/ 1471- 2407- 13- 396.
17. Sonoda H, et al. Colorectal cancer screening with odour material by
canine scent detection. Gut. 2011;60:814–9.
18. Rooney NJ, Guest CM, Swanson LCM, Morant SV. How effective are
trained dogs at alerting their owners to changes in blood glycaemic
levels? variations in performance of glycaemia alert dogs. PLoS ONE.
2019;14:1–16.
19. Gonder-Frederick LA, et al. Variability of diabetes alert dog accuracy in a
real-world setting. J Diabetes Sci Technol. 2017;11:714–9.
20. Los EA, Ramsey KL, Guttmann-Bauman I, Ahmann AJ. Reliability of
trained dogs to alert to hypoglycemia in patients with Type 1 diabetes.
J Diabetes Sci Technol. 2017;11:506–12.
21. Catala A, et al. Dogs demonstrate the existence of an epileptic seizure
odour in humans. Sci Rep. 2019;9:1–7.
22. Maurer M, McCulloch M, Willey AM, Hirsch W, Dewey D. Detection of
bacteriuria by canine olfaction. Open Forum Infect Dis. 2016;3:1–6.
23. Angle TC, et al. Real-time detection of a virus using detection dogs.
Front Vet Sci. 2016;2:1–6.
24. Jendrny P, Schulz C, Twele F, Meller S, von Köckritz-Blickwede M,
Osterhaus ADME, et al. Scent dog identification of SARS-CoV-2
infection. BMC Infect Dis. 2020;20:536. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1186/
s12879- 020- 05281-3.
25. Jendrny P, Twele F, Meller S, et al. Scent dog identification of SARS-
CoV-2 infections in different body fluids. BMC Infect Dis. 2021;21:707.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1186/ s12879- 021- 06411-1.
26. Grandjean D, Sarkis R, Lecoq-Julien C, Benard A, Roger V, Levesque
E, et al. Detection dogs as a help in the detection of COVID-19 Can
the dog alert on COVID-19 positive persons by sniffing axillary sweat
samples ? Proof-of-concept study. PLOS ONE. 2020;15(12):e0243122.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 02431 22.
27. Essler J, Kane SA, Nolan P, Akaho EH, Berna AZ, Deangelo A, et al. Dis-
crimination of SARS-CoV-2 infected patient samples by detection dogs:
a proof of concept study. PLOS ONE. 2021;16(4):e0250158. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 02501 58.
28. Vesga O, Valencia AF, Mira A, et al. Dog Savior: Immediate Scent-
Detection of SARS-COV-2 by Trained Dogs. bioRxiv 2020.06.17.158105;
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1101/ 2020. 06. 17. 158105.
29. Eskandari E, Marzaleh MA, Roudgari H, Farahani RH, Nezami-Asl A, Lar-
ipour R, et al. Sniffer dogs as a screening/diagnostic tool for COVID-19:
a proof of concept study. BMC Inf Dis. 2021;21:243. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1186/ s12879- 021- 05939-6.
30. Grandjean D, Al Marzooqi DH, Lecoq-Julien C, et al. Use Of Canine
Olfactory Detection For COVID-19 Testing Study On U.A.E. Trained
Detection Dog Sensitivity. bioRxiv 2021.01.20.427105; https:// doi. org/
10. 1101/ 2021. 01. 20. 427105.
31. Guest C, Dewhirst SY, Allen DJ. Using trained dogs and organic semi-
conducting sensors to identify asymptomatic and mild SARS-CoV-2
infections. https:// www. lshtm. ac. uk/ media/ 49791. Accessed 22 June
2021.
32. Hag-Ali M, AlShamsi AS, Boeijen L, et al. The detection dogs test is more
sensitive than real-time PCR in screening for SARS-CoV-2. Commun Biol.
2021;4:686. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ s42003- 021- 02232-9.
33. Mendel J, Frank K, Edlin L, Hall K, Webb D, Mills J, et al. Preliminary
accuracy of COVID-19 odor detection by canines and HS-SPME-GC-MS
using exhaled breath samples. Foren Sci Int Synergy. 2021. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1016/j. fsisyn. 2021. 100155.
34. Guest C, Pinder M, Doggett M, Squires C, Affara M, Kandeh B, et al.
Trained dogs identify people with malaria parasites by their odour.
Lancet Infect Dis. 2019;19:578–80.
35. Taylor MT, McCready J, Broukhanski G, Kirpalaney S, Lutz H, Powis J.
Using dog scent detection as a point-of-care tool to identify toxigenic
clostridium difficile in stool. Open Forum Infect Dis. 2018;5:1–4.
36. Shirasu M, Touhara K. The scent of disease: Volatile organic com-
pounds of the human body related to disease and disorder. J Biochem.
2011;150:257–66.
37. Corradi M, Gergelova P, Mutti A. Exhaled volatile organic compounds
in nonrespiratory diseases. Exhaled Biomark. 2010. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1183/ 10254 48x. 00018 809.
38. Dent AG, Sutedja TG, Zimmerman PV. Exhaled breath analysis for lung
cancer. J Thorac Dis. 2013;5:S540.
39. Johnen D, Heuwieser W, Fischer-Tenhagen C. An approach to identify
bias in scent detection dog testing. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2017;189:1–
12. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. appla nim. 2017. 01. 001.
40. Moser E, McCulloch M. Canine scent detection of human can-
cers: a review of methods and accuracy. J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res.
2010;5:145–52.
41. Jezierski T, Walczak M, Ligor T, Rudnicka J, Buszewski B. Study of the art:
canine olfaction used for cancer detection on the basis of breath odour
Perspect Limitations. J Breath Res. 2015;9:027001.
42. Hardin DS, Anderson W, Cattet J. Dogs can be successfully trained to
alert to hypoglycemia samples from patients with type 1 diabetes.
Diabetes Ther. 2015;6:509–17.
43. Maa E, Arnold J, Ninedorf K, Olsen H. Canine detection of volatile
organic compounds unique to human epileptic seizure. Epilepsy Behav.
2021;115:107690. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. yebeh. 2020. 107690.
44. Report C. Seizure-alert dogs: a review and preliminary study. Seizure.
2003. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ S1059- 1311(02) 00225-X.
45. Martinez-Caja MA, De Herdt V, Boon P, Brandl U, Cock H, Parra J, et al.
Seizure-alerting behavior in dogs owned by people experiencing
seizures. Epilepsy Behav. 2019;94:104–11.
46. Karl S, Huber L. Empathy in dogs: with a little help from a friend—a
mixed blessing. Animal Sentience. 2017;2:13. https:// doi. org/ 10. 51291/
2377- 7478. 1271.
47. Polgár Z, Kinnunen M, Újváry D, Miklosi A, Gácsi M. A test of canine
olfactory capacity: comparing various dog breeds and wolves in a natu-
ral detection task. PloS one. 2016;11:0154087. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/
journ al. pone. 01540 87.
48. Lazarowski L, Foster ML, Gruen ME, Sherman BL, Fish RE, Milgram NW,
Dorman DC. Olfactory discrimination and generalization of ammonium
nitrate and structurally related odorants in Labrador retrievers. Anim
Cogn. 2015;18(6):1255–65. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10071- 015- 0894-9.
49. Fathizadeh H, Taghizadeh S, Safari R, Khiabani SS, Babak B, Hamzavi F,
Ganbarov K, Esposito S, Zeinalzadeh E, Dao S, Köse Ş, Kafil HS. Study
presence of COVID-19 (SARS-CoV-2) in the sweat of patients infected
with Covid-19. Microb Pathog. 2020;149:104556. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1016/j. micpa th. 2020. 104556.
50. Wang W, Xu Y, Gao R, Lu R, Han K, Wu G, Tan W. Detection of SARS-CoV-2
in different types of clinical specimens. JAMA. 2020;323(18):1843–4.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1001/ jama. 2020. 3786.
51. Angle C, Waggoner LP, Ferrando A, Haney P, Passler T. Canine detection
of the volatilome: a review of implications for pathogen and disease
detection. Front Vet Sci. 2016;24(3):47. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fvets.
2016. 00047.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 14 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
52. Kesselmeier J, Staudt M. Biogenic volatile organic compounds (VOC):
an overview on emission, physiology and ecology. Environ Pollut.
2000;109:175.
53. Goss KU. The physical chemistry of odors—consequences for the work
with detection dogs. Forensic Sci Int. 2019;296:110–4.
54. Qader AEA, et al. Volatile organic compounds generated by cultures
of bacteria and viruses associated with respiratory infections. Biomed
Chromatogr. 2015;29:1783–90.
55. Steppert C, Steppert I, Sterlacci W, Bollinger T. Rapid detection of
SARS-CoV-2 infection by multicapillary column coupled ion mobility
spectrometry (MCC-IMS) of breath. A proof of concept study. J Breath
Res. 2021;15:027105.
56. Amann A, et al. The human volatilome: volatile organic compounds
(VOCs) in exhaled breath, skin emanations, urine, feces and saliva. J
Breath Res. 2014;8:034001.
57. Craven BA, et al. Reconstruction and morphometric analysis of the
nasal airway of the dog (Canis familiaris) and implications regarding
olfactory airflow. Anat Rec. 2007;290:1325–40.
58. Jenkins EK, DeChant MT, Perry EB. When the nose doesn’t know: canine
olfactory function associated with health, management, and potential
links to microbiota. Front Vet Sci. 2018;5:56. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/
fvets. 2018. 00056.
59. Bamford K. Canine Olfaction : An Overview of the Anatomy , Physiology
and Genetics (2015). https:// www. seman ticsc holar. org/ paper/ Canine-
Olfac tion-% 3A- An- Overv iew- of- the- Anato my-% 2C- and- Bamfo rd/ 5a43b
175f9 25c56 37258 48400 8ef4c 5c825 25d46. Accessed 12 June 2021.
60. Mozell MM. Evidence for sorption as a mechanism of the olfactory
analysis of vapours. Nature. 1964;203:1181–2.
61. Moulton DG. Spatial patterning of response to odors in the peripheral
olfactory system. Physiol Rev. 1976;56:578–93.
62. Morrison EE, Costanzo RM. Morphology of olfactory epithelium in
humans and other vertebrates. Microsc Res Tech. 1992;23:49–61.
63. Mori K, Yoshihara Y. Molecular recognition and olfactory processing in
the mammalian olfactory system. Prog Neurobiol. 1995;45:585–619.
64. Hawkes CH, Doty RL. The neurology of olfaction. Neurol Olfaction. 2009.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/ CBO97 80511 575754.
65. Buck LB. The molecular architecture of odor and pheromone sensing in
mammals. Cell. 2000;100:611–8.
66. Riezzo I, Neri M, Rendine M, Bellifemina A, Cantatore S, Fiore C, et al.
Cadaver dogs: Unscientific myth or reliable biological devices? Forensic
Sci Int. 2014;244:213–21.
67. Dennis JC, Allgier JG, Desouza LS, Eward WC, Morrison EE. Immunohis-
tochemistry of the canine vomeronasal organ. J Anat. 2003;203:329–38.
68. Houpt KA, Hintz HF, Shepherd P. The role of olfaction in canine food
preferences. Chem Senses. 1978;3:281–90.
69. Bradshaw JWS. Sensory and experiential factors in the design of foods
for domestic dogs and cats. Proc Nutr Soc. 1991;50:99–106.
70. Firestein S. How the olfactory system makes sense of scents. Nature.
2001;413:211–8.
71. Walker DB, Walker JC, Cavnar PJ, Taylor JL, Pickel DH, Biddle Hall S, et al.
Naturalistic quantification of canine olfactory sensitivity. Appl Anim
Behav Sci. 2006;97:241–54.
72. Siniscalchi M, Sasso R, Pepe AM, Dimatteo S, Vallortigara G, Quaranta A.
Sniffing with the right nostril: lateralization of response to odour stimuli
by dogs. Anim Behav. 2011;82:399–404.
73. Siniscalchi M. Olfaction and the Canine Brain. In: Canine olfaction
science and law: advances in forensic science, medicine, conservation,
and environmental remediation. Taylor and Francis; 2016; pp. 31–38.
doi: https:// doi. org/ 10. 1201/ b20027.
74. Jezierski T, Adamkiewicz E, Walczak M, Sobczyńska M, Górecka-Bruzda
A, Ensminger J, et al. Efficacy of drug detection by fully-trained police
dogs varies by breed, training level, type of drug and search environ-
ment. Forensic Sci Int. 2014;237:112–8.
75. Hall NJ, Glenn K, Smith DW, Wynne CDL. Performance of Pugs, German
Shepherds, and Greyhounds (Canis lupus familiaris ) on an Odor-
Discrimination Task Performance of Pugs, German Shepherds, and
Greyhounds. J Comp Psychol. 2015;129:237.
76. Tacher S, Quignon P, Rimbault M, Dreano S, Andre C, Galibert F. Olfac-
tory receptor sequence polymorphism within and between breeds of
dogs. J Hered. 2005;96:812–6.
77. Quignon P, Giraud M, Rimbault M, Lavigne P, Tacher S, Morin E, et al. The
dog and rat olfactory receptor repertoires. Genome Biol. 2005;6:R83.
78. Quignon P, Kirkness E, Cadiue E, Touleimat N, Guyon R, Renier C, et al.
Comparison of the canine and human olfactory receptor gene reper-
toires. Genome Biol. 2003;4:67–77.
79. Issel-Tarver L, Rine J. Organization and expression of canine olfactory
receptor genes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93:10897–902.
80. Lesniak A, Walczak M, Jezierski T, Sacgarczuk M, Gawkowski M, Jaszczak
K. Canine olfactory receptor gene polymorphism and its relation to
odor detection performance by sniffer dogs. J Hered. 2008;99:518–27.
81. Robin S, Tacher S, Rimbault M, Vaysse A, Dréano S, André C, et al.
Genetic diversity of canine olfactory receptors. BMC Genomics.
2009;16:1–16.
82. Schauber EM. Predator–prey dynamics: the role of olfaction, by Michael
R. Conover. J Wildlife Manag. 2008;72(1):337–8.
83. Majumder S, Bhadra A. When love is in the air: understanding why dogs
tend to mate when it rains. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:1–15.
84. Otto CM, Hare E, Nord JL, Palermo SM, Kelsey KM, Darling TA, et al.
Evaluation of three hydration strategies in detection dogs working in a
hot environment. Front Vet Sci. 2017;4:1–10.
85. Hirai T, Kojima S, Shimada A, Umemura T, Sakai M, Itakura C. Age-related
changes in the olfactory system of dogs. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol.
1996;22:531–9.
86. Hüttenbrink KB, Hummel T, Berg D, Gasser T, Hähner A. Riechstörun-
gen: Häufig im alter und wichtiges frühsymptom neurodegenerativer
erkrankungen. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2013;110:1–8.
87. Troisi CA, Mills DS, Wilkinson A, Zulch HE. Behavioral and cognitive fac-
tors that affect the success of scent detection dogs. Comp Cogn Behav
Rev. 2019;14:51–76.
88. Angle CT, Wakshlag JJ, Gillette RL, Steury T, Haney P, Barrett J, et al. The
effects of exercise and diet on olfactory capability in detection dogs. J
Nutr Sci. 2014. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/ jns. 2014. 35.
89. Altom EK, Davenport GM, Myers LJ, Cummins KA. Effect of dietary fat
source and exercise on odorant-detecting ability of canine athletes. Res
Vet Sci. 2003;75:149–55.
90. Gazit I, Terkel J. Explosives detection by sniffer dogs following strenuous
physical activity. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2003;81:149–61.
91. Fischer-Tenhagen C, Johnen D, Heuwieser W, Becker R, Schallschmidt K,
Nehls I. Odor perception by dogs: evaluating two training approaches
for odor learning of sniffer dogs. Chem Senses. 2017;42:435–41.
92. Byosiere SE, Feng LC, Rutter NJ. Factors that may affect the success of
scent detection dogs: exploring nonconventional models of prepara-
tion and deployment. Comp Cogn Behav Rev. 2019;14:81–6.
93. Ziv G. The effects of using aversive training methods in dogs—a review.
Anim Behav. 2017;19:50–60.
94. Mullis RA, Witzel AL, Price J. Maintenance energy requirements of odor
detection, explosive detection and human detection working dogs.
PeerJ. 2015;2015:1–8.
95. Aimé P, Duchamp-Viret P, Chaput MA, Savigner A, Mahfouz M, Julliard
AK. Fasting increases and satiation decreases olfactory detection for a
neutral odor in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2007. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. bbr.
2007. 02. 012.
96. Critchley HD, Rolls ET. Hunger and satiety modify the responses of
olfactory and visual neurons in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. J
Neurophysiol. 1996;75:1673–86.
97. Tong J, Mannea E, Aime P, Pfluger PT, Yi C-X, Castaneda TR, et al. Ghrelin
enhances olfactory sensitivity and exploratory sniffing in rodents and
humans. J Neurosci. 2011;31:5841–6.
98. Koskinen K, Reichert JL, Hoier S, Schachenreiter J, Duller S, Moissl-
Eichinger C, et al. The nasal microbiome mirrors and potentially shapes
olfactory function. Sci Rep. 2018;8:1–11.
99. Henkin RI. Drug-induced taste and smell disorders. Drug Saf. 1994.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 2165/ 00002 018- 19941 1050- 00004.
100. Cho SH. Clinical diagnosis and treatment of olfactory dysfunction.
Hanyang Med Rev. 2014;34:107.
101. Jia H, Pustovyy OM, Waggoner P, Beyers RJ, Schumacher J, Wildey C,
et al. Functional MRI of the olfactory system in conscious dogs. PLoS
One. 2014;9:e86362.
102. Myers LJ. Dysosmia of the dog in clinical veterinary medicine. Prog Vet
Neurol. 1990;1(2):171–9.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Page 15 of 15
Jendrnyetal. BMC Infect Dis (2021) 21:838
fast, convenient online submission
thorough peer review by experienced researchers in your field
rapid publication on acceptance
support for research data, including large and complex data types
gold Open Access which fosters wider collaboration and increased citations
maximum visibility for your research: over 100M website views per year
At BMC, research is always in progress.
Learn more biomedcentral.com/submissions
Ready to submit your research
Ready to submit your research
? Choose BMC and benefit from:
? Choose BMC and benefit from:
103. Szetei V, Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V. When dogs seem to lose their
nose: an investigation on the use of visual and olfactory cues in com-
municative context between dog and owner. Appl Anim Behav Sci.
2003;83:141–52.
104. Brann DH, Tsukahara T, Weinreb C, Lipovsek M, van den Berge K, Gong
B, et al. Non-neuronal expression of SARS-CoV-2 entry genes in the
olfactory system suggests mechanisms underlying COVID-19-associ-
ated anosmia. Sci Adv. 2020;6:1–20.
105. Temmam S, Barbarino A, Maso D, Behillil S, Enouf V, Huon C, et al.
Absence of SARS-CoV-2 infection in cats and dogs in close contact
with a cluster of COVID-19 patients in a veterinary campus. One Heal.
2020;10:100164.
106. To EMW, et al. Infection of dogs with SARS-CoV-2. Nature. 2020. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1038/ s41586- 020- 2334-5.
107. Fritz M, Rosolen B, Krafft E, Becquart P, Elguero E, Vratskikh O, et al. High
prevalence of SARS-CoV-2 antibodies in pets from COVID-19+ house-
holds. One Heal. 2020;11:100192.
108. Myers LJ, Nusbaum KE, Swango LJ, Hanrahan LN, Sartin E. Dysfunction
of sense of smell caused by canine parainfluenza virus infection in
dogs. Am J Vet Res. 1988;49:188.
109. Hawkes CH, Doty RL. Smell and taste disorders 2–3. Cambridge: Cam-
bridge University Press; 2018.
110. Lötsch J, Knothe C, Lippmann C, Ultsch A, Hummel T, Walter C. Olfac-
tory drug effects approached from human-derived data. Drug Discov
Today. 2015;20(11):1398–406. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. drudis. 2015. 06.
012.
111. Lötsch J, Geisslinger G, Hummel T. Sniffing out pharmacology: Interac-
tions of drugs with human olfaction. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2012.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tips. 2012. 01. 004.
112. Jenkins EK, Lee-Fowler TM, Craigangle T, Behrend EN, Moore GE. Effects
of oral administration of metronidazole and doxycycline on olfactory
capabilities of explosives detection dogs. Am J Vet Res. 2016. https://
doi. org/ 10. 2460/ ajvr. 77.8. 906.
113. Schiffman SS. Influence of medications on taste and smell. World J
Otorhinolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2018;4:84–91.
114. Ezeh PI, Myers LJ, Hanrahan LA, Kemppainen RJ, Cummins KA. Effects
of steroids on the olfactory function of the dog. Physiol Behav. 1992.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ 0031- 9384(92) 90306-M.
115. Lien J (2018). The Acute Effects of Isoflurane and Propofol on the
Olfactory-Cognitive Ability of Brown Root Rot Disease Fungus
Detection Dogs. (Publication No. 10791220) [Master of Science thesis,
Purdue University]. ProQuest dissertations. https:// docs. lib. purdue. edu/
disse rtati ons/ AAI10 791220/.
116. Bromley SM. Smell and taste disorders: a primary care approach. Am
Fam Physician. 2000;61(2):427–36, 438.
117. Szulejko JE, McCulloch M, Jackson J, McKee DL, Walker JC, Solouki T,
et al. Evidence for cancer biomarkers in exhaled breath. IEEE Sens J.
2010;10:185–210.
118. Edwards T, Browne C, Schoon A, Cox C, Poling A. Animal olfactory
detection of human diseases: guidelines and systematic review. J Vet
Behav Clin Appl Res. 2017. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jveb. 2017. 05. 002.
119. EU Guidance on Operating Procedures for Explosive Detection Dogs
in Public Spaces. https:// ec. europa. eu/ newsr oom/ pps/ items/ 696384.
Accessed 5 July 2021
120. COMMISSION REGULATION (EU) No 573/2010of 30 June 2010 amend-
ing Regulation (EU) No 185/2010 laying down detailed measures
for the implementation of the common basic standards on aviation
security. https:// eur- lex. europa. eu/ LexUr iServ/ LexUr iServ. do? uri= OJ:L:
2010: 166: 0001: 0005: EN: PDF. Accessed 5 July 2021.
121. Sethi S, Nanda R, Chakraborty T. Clinical application of volatile organic
compound analysis for detecting infectious diseases. Clin Microbiol
Rev. 2020;26(3):462–75. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1128/ CMR. 00020- 13.
122. Craven BA, Paterson EG, Settles GS. The fluid dynamics of canine olfac-
tion: unique nasal airflow patterns as an explanation of macrosmia. J R
Society Interface. 2010;7:933–43.
123. Dehlinger K, Tarnowski K, House JL, Los E, Hanavan K, Bustamante B.
Can trained dogs detect a hypoglycemic scent in patients with type
1 diabetes? Diabetes Care. 2013;36(7):98–9. https:// doi. org/ 10. 2337/
dc12- 2342.
124. Issel-Tarver L, Rine J. The evolution of mammalian olfactory receptor
genes. Genetics. 1997;145:185–95.
125. Quignon P, Rimbault M, Robin S, Galibert F. Genetics of canine olfaction
and receptor diversity. Mamm Genome. 2012;23:132–43.
126. McGann JP. Poor human olfaction is a 19th-century myth. Science.
2017;356:eaam7263.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in pub-
lished maps and institutional affiliations.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... The high impact of illicit drugs on Societies worldwide requires hard work in the fight against drug trafficking, including scanners, video monitoring, intelligence services, and Narcotic Detection Dogs (NDDs) with high olfactory power and accessible training capacities (1) for Customs and border inspection (2,3). The danger and price linked to narcotrafficking for the security of the population and countries justify investments in NDD units (4). ...
... Dogs detect odors in the most diverse environments and are used to search for several substances, products, animals, and even man worldwide without direct contact (3). These dogs, with their exceptional sense of smell, combined with learning and conditioning training, can find hidden targets such as explosives, illicit drugs, and missing persons, as well as detect diseases and the need for certain medications by persons (1,2,10). The effectiveness of dogs as detectors has been proposed as a result of their olfactory anatomy (neurons, olfactory receptors, and olfactory bulb) (11) and their behavior in searching for the desired information (12). ...
Article
Full-text available
Narcotic Detection Dogs (NDDs) are essential tools in the fight against drug trafficking, acting with high precision and improving efficiency at border posts. When trained efficiently, these dogs can detect a great variety of compounds, such as cocaine, marijuana and its derivatives, and synthetic drugs, among others. Most of the knowledge on canine detection processes and efficiency has been determined in experimentally controlled conditions, but narcotic seizures detected by dogs in realistic anti-drug operations have not yet been critically determined in a Country with continental dimensions such as Brazil. This study aimed to evaluate the data set concerning the performance, operations, efficiency, and success rate of NDDs used by the Brazilian Customs Authority (Aduana) in the fight against drug trafficking. Narcotic seizure rates increased in luggage and packages detected by NDDs working at border crossings from 2010 to 2020, with an estimated value of over US$ 2 billion in losses to the cocaine drug trafficking business. NDD units also increased most narcotic groups seized in the same period. The number of NDDs and anti-drug operations, and Customs Border Post (CBP) influenced the rates of drugs seized. NDDs provided an increase of 3,157 kg/animal of drugs seized for every new dog introduced into the inspection systems.
... The identification of volatile organic compounds (VOCs), which are released during the decomposition of human bodies, is regarded as an important key element for detecting human remains open in the field or hidden under vegetation and debris, buried in the ground, or submerged in water [1][2][3][4][5][6][7]. Especially when it comes to train human remains detection (HRD) dogs, the knowledge of a unique VOC signature released by human bodies would be of major importance and help [8,9]. But the analysis of the VOC composition could also be helpful to narrow down the post-mortem interval (PMI), especially for those cases in which the early post-mortem changes such as body temperature, rigor mortis and livor mortis can no longer be used [10,11]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study examines for the first time the emission patterns and olfactory signatures of 9 complete human corpses of different stages of decomposition. Air sampling was performed inside the body bags with solid sorbents and analysed by coupled gas chromatography-mass spectrometry after thermal desorption (TD-GC-MS). Furthermore, odour-related substances were detected by gas chromatography-olfactometry (GC-O). Sulfurous compounds (mainly dimethyl di- and trisulfide) were identified as most important to the odour perception. Around 350 individual organic substances were detected by TD-GC-MS, notably sulfurous and nitrogenous substances as well as branched alkanes, aldehydes, ketones, alcohols, carboxylic acids, carboxylic acid esters and ethers. A range of terpenes was detected for the first time in a characteristic emission pattern over all decomposition stages. Concentrations of the substances varied greatly, and no correlation between the emission patterns, the stage of decomposition and the cause of death could be found. While previous studies often analysed pig cadavers or only parts of human tissue, the present study shows the importance of analysing complete human corpses over a range of decomposition stages. Moreover, it is shown that using body bags as a kind of “emission test chamber” is a very promising approach, also because it is a realistic application considering the usual transport and store of a body before autopsy. Graphical abstract
... If proven effective, it would be a point-of-care, low-cost, handheld, noninvasive tool that overcomes many of the shortcomings associated with currently available methods. For now, dogs seem to outperform e-noses (53,54,57). Research needs to continue to pursue the elite abilities of odor detection dogs to investigate disease detection whilst simultaneously harnessing their skills to refine and enhance the e-nose technology for scalability to bring cultureindependent point-of-care-assays to the clinic floor. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction The study investigated the utilization of odor detection dogs to identify the odor profile of Staphylococcus aureus (S. aureus) biofilms in pure in vitro samples and in in vivo biosamples from animals and humans with S. aureus periprosthetic joint infection (PJI). Biofilms form when bacterial communities aggregate on orthopedic implants leading to recalcitrant infections that are difficult to treat. Identifying PJI biofilm infections is challenging, and traditional microbiological cultures may yield negative results even in the presence of clinical signs. Methods Dogs were trained on pure in vitro S. aureus biofilms and tested on lacrimal fluid samples from an in vivo animal model (rabbits) and human patients with confirmed S. aureus PJI. Results The results demonstrated that dogs achieved a high degree of sensitivity and specificity in detecting the odor profile associated with S. aureus biofilms in rabbit samples. Preliminary results suggest that dogs can recognize S. aureus volatile organic compounds (VOCs) in human lacrimal fluid samples. Discussion Training odor detection dogs on in vitro S. aureus , may provide an alternative to obtaining clinical samples for training and mitigates biosecurity hazards. The findings hold promise for culture-independent diagnostics, enabling early disease detection, and improved antimicrobial stewardship. In conclusion, this research demonstrates that dogs trained on in vitro S. aureus samples can identify the consistent VOC profile of PJI S. aureus biofilm infections. The study opens avenues for further investigations into a retained VOC profile of S. aureus biofilm infection. These advancements could revolutionize infectious disease diagnosis and treatment, leading to better patient outcomes and addressing the global challenge of antimicrobial resistance.
... The preceding examples point to the remarkable capability of the dog's nose in scent-detection and it is often commented upon (Hepper, 1988;Jendrny et al., 2021). How scent detection is processed within dog cognition is remarked upon less often. ...
Article
Full-text available
Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) have excellent olfactory processing capabilities that are utilized widely in human society e.g., working with customs, police, and army; their scent detection is also used in guarding, hunting, mold-sniffing, searching for missing people or animals, and facilitating the life of the disabled. Sniffing and searching for odors is a natural, species-typical behavior and essential for the dog's welfare. While taking advantage of this canine ability widely, we understand its foundations and implications quite poorly. We can improve animal welfare by better understanding their olfactory world. In this review, we outline the olfactory processing of dogs in the nervous system, summarize the current knowledge of scent detection and differentiation; the effect of odors on the dogs’ cognitive and emotional processes and the dog-human bond; and consider the methodological advancements that could be developed further to aid in our understanding of the canine world of odors.
Article
Full-text available
The research on the use of forensic olfaction is relevant due to the need to highlight its content and rules of practical application in the analysis of odour traces of a criminal, which ensures improvement of crime solving and investigation. The study aims to analyse the olfaction information properties which individually identify a person about the odour traces left at the crime scene; to study the current possibilities of the methodology for conducting olfaction examinations; and to formulate proposals for improving the process of expert analysis of olfaction information in criminal proceedings. The study uses comparative legal, terminological, systemic, and structural, formal and logical methods, as well as the method of expert experiment. The author confirms the data on the individuality of each personʼs smell, in particular, based on cases from investigative practice, the author shows the possibility of establishing the individuality of odour traces and their belonging to a particular person, even in the case of a crime committed by two monozygotic twins. The author substantiates the possibility of collecting odour traces from various objects with which several persons had contact, and of isolating those odour particles, allowing detector dogs to identify their specific carrier. The study defines the general conditions and procedure for conducting an olfaction analysis of odour traces of a person being tested in connection with a criminal offence investigation. It is generalised that the work of detector dogs for the most effective odour analysis should be carried out in a special room without extraneous odours at a temperature of +20°C and relative humidity of 60-80%. The study systematises the general prohibitions that should be observed during an olfaction examination, which relate to the non-use of control and auxiliary odour samples of persons familiar to detector dogs; the work of an olfaction expert and a dog handler in a special room is separated to prevent the specialist from obtaining information about the specific location of the storage jar with the odour information that is being installed. The practical significance of the study is determined by the expansion of the ability of law enforcement agencies to identify persons involved in a crime by their odour traces left at the scene and to conduct forensic examinations using the method of forensic olfaction
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is an impairing mental health condition with high prevalence among military and general populations alike. PTSD service dogs are a complementary and alternative intervention needing scientific validation. We investigated whether dogs can detect putative stress-related volatile organic compounds (VOCs) in the breath of people with trauma histories (54% with PTSD) exposed to personalized trauma cues. Methods Breath samples were collected from 26 humans over 40 experimental sessions during a calm (control breath sample) and stressed state induced by trauma cue exposure (target breath sample). Two scent detection canines were presented with the samples in a two alternative forced choice (2AFC) discrimination and yes/no detection task. The 2AFC task assessed the dogs' ability to discriminate between the two states within the breath samples of one individual. The detection task determined their ability to generalize the target odour across different individuals and different stressful events of one individual. Signal Detection Theory was applied to assess dogs' sensitivity, specificity, precision, and response bias. Results The dogs performed at ∼90% accuracy across all sample sets in the discrimination experiment, and at 74% and 81% accuracy, respectively, in the detection experiment. Further analysis of dog olfactory performance in relation to human donor self-reported emotional responses to trauma cue exposure suggested the dogs may have been detecting distinct endocrine stress markers. One dog's performance correlated with the human donors' self-reported fear responses and the other dog's performance correlated with the human donors' self-reported shame responses. Based on these correlations between dog performance and donor self-report measures, we speculate that the VOCs each dog was detecting likely originated from the sympathetico-adreno-medullary axis (SAM; adrenaline, noradrenaline) in the case of the first dog and the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA; glucocorticoids) in the case of the second dog. Conclusion Our proof-of-concept study is the first to demonstrate that some dogs can detect putative VOCs emitted by people with trauma histories when experiencing distress theoretically associated with the intrusion and arousal/reactivity symptoms of PTSD. Results have potential to improve the effectiveness and training protocol of PTSD service dogs with a focus on enhancing their alert function.
Article
Full-text available
Background A rapid, accurate, non-invasive diagnostic screen is needed to identify people with SARS-CoV-2 infection. We investigated whether organic semi-conducting (OSC) sensors and trained dogs could distinguish between people infected with asymptomatic or mild symptoms, and uninfected individuals, and the impact of screening at ports-of-entry. Methods Odour samples were collected from adults, and SARS-CoV-2 infection status confirmed using RT-PCR. OSC sensors captured the volatile organic compound (VOC) profile of odour samples. Trained dogs were tested in a double-blind trial to determine their ability to detect differences in VOCs between infected and uninfected individuals, with sensitivity and specificity as the primary outcome. Mathematical modelling was used to investigate the impact of bio-detection dogs for screening. Results 3921 adults were enrolled in the study and odour samples collected from 1097 SARS-CoV-2 infected and 2031 uninfected individuals. OSC sensors were able to distinguish between SARS-CoV-2 infected individuals and uninfected, with sensitivity from 98% (95% CI 95–100) to 100% and specificity from 99% (95% CI 97–100) to 100%. Six dogs were able to distinguish between samples with sensitivity ranging from 82% (95% CI 76–87) to 94% (95% CI 89–98) and specificity ranging from 76% (95% CI 70–82) to 92% (95% CI 88–96). Mathematical modelling suggests that dog screening plus a confirmatory PCR test could detect up to 89% of SARS-CoV-2 infections, averting up to 2·2 times as much transmission compared to isolation of symptomatic individuals only. Conclusions People infected with SARS-CoV-2, with asymptomatic or mild symptoms, have a distinct odour that can be identified by sensors and trained dogs with a high degree of accuracy. Odour-based diagnostics using sensors and/or dogs may prove a rapid and effective tool for screening large numbers of people. Trial Registration NCT04509713 (clinicaltrials.gov).
Article
Full-text available
Background The main strategy to contain the current SARS-CoV-2 pandemic remains to implement a comprehensive testing, tracing and quarantining strategy until vaccination of the population is adequate. Scent dogs could support current testing strategies. Methods Ten dogs were trained for 8 days to detect SARS-CoV-2 infections in beta-propiolactone inactivated saliva samples. The subsequent cognitive transfer performance for the recognition of non-inactivated samples were tested on three different body fluids (saliva, urine, and sweat) in a randomised, double-blind controlled study. Results Dogs were tested on a total of 5242 randomised sample presentations. Dogs detected non-inactivated saliva samples with a diagnostic sensitivity of 84% (95% CI: 62.5–94.44%) and specificity of 95% (95% CI: 93.4–96%). In a subsequent experiment to compare the scent recognition between the three non-inactivated body fluids, diagnostic sensitivity and specificity were 95% (95% CI: 66.67–100%) and 98% (95% CI: 94.87–100%) for urine, 91% (95% CI: 71.43–100%) and 94% (95% CI: 90.91–97.78%) for sweat, 82% (95% CI: 64.29–95.24%), and 96% (95% CI: 94.95–98.9%) for saliva respectively. Conclusions The scent cognitive transfer performance between inactivated and non-inactivated samples as well as between different sample materials indicates that global, specific SARS-CoV-2-associated volatile compounds are released across different body secretions, independently from the patient’s symptoms. All tested body fluids appear to be similarly suited for reliable detection of SARS-CoV-2 infected individuals.
Article
Full-text available
The novel coronavirus SARS-CoV-2, since its initial outbreak in Wuhan, China has led to a worldwide pandemic and has shut down nations. As with any outbreak, there is a general strategy of detection, containment, treatment and/or cure. The authors would argue that rapid and efficient detection is critical and required to successful management of a disease. The current study explores and successfully demonstrates the use of canines to detect COVID-19 disease in exhaled breath. The intended use was to detect the odor of COVID-19 on contaminated surfaces inferring recent deposition of infectious material from a COVID-19 positive individual. Using masks obtained from hospitalized patients that tested positive for COVID-19 disease, four canines were trained and evaluated for their ability to detect the disease. All four canines obtained an accuracy >90% and positive predictive values ranging from ∼73 to 93% after just one month of training.
Article
Full-text available
In January 2020, the coronavirus disease was declared, by the World Health Organization as a global public health emergency. Recommendations from the WHO COVID Emergency Committee continue to support strengthening COVID surveillance systems, including timely access to effective diagnostics. Questions were raised about the validity of considering the RT-PCR as the gold standard in COVID-19 diagnosis. It has been suggested that a variety of methods should be used to evaluate advocated tests. Dogs had been successfully trained and employed to detect diseases in humans. Here we show that upon training explosives detection dogs on sniffing COVID-19 odor in patients’ sweat, those dogs were able to successfully screen out 3249 individuals who tested negative for the SARS-CoV-2, from a cohort of 3290 individuals. Additionally, using Bayesian analysis, the sensitivity of the K9 test was found to be superior to the RT-PCR test performed on nasal swabs from a cohort of 3134 persons. Given its high sensitivity, short turn-around-time, low cost, less invasiveness, and ease of application, the detection dogs test lends itself as a better alternative to the RT-PCR in screening for SARS-CoV-2 in asymptomatic individuals.
Article
Full-text available
While the world awaits a widely available COVID-19 vaccine, availability of testing is limited in many regions and can be further compounded by shortages of reagents, prolonged processing time and delayed results. One approach to rapid testing is to leverage the volatile organic compound (VOC) signature of SARS-CoV-2 infection. Detection dogs, a biological sensor of VOCs, were utilized to investigate whether SARS-CoV-2 positive urine and saliva patient samples had a unique odor signature. The virus was inactivated in all training samples with either detergent or heat treatment. Using detergent-inactivated urine samples, dogs were initially trained to find samples collected from hospitalized patients confirmed with SARS-CoV-2 infection, while ignoring samples collected from controls. Dogs were then tested on their ability to spontaneously recognize heat-treated urine samples as well as heat-treated saliva from hospitalized SARS-CoV-2 positive patients. Dogs successfully discriminated between infected and uninfected urine samples, regardless of the inactivation protocol, as well as heat-treated saliva samples. Generalization to novel samples was limited, particularly after intensive training with a restricted sample set. A unique odor associated with SARS-CoV-2 infection present in human urine as well as saliva, provides impetus for the development of odor-based screening, either by electronic, chemical, or biological sensing methods. The use of dogs for screening in an operational setting will require training with a large number of novel SARS-CoV-2 positive and confirmed negative samples.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Sniffer dogs are able to detect certain chemical particles and are suggest to be capable of helping diagnose some medical conditions and complications, such as colorectal cancer, melanoma, bladder cancer, and even critical states such as hypoglycemia in diabetic patients. With the global spread of COVID-19 throughout the world and the need to have a real-time screening of the population, especially in crowded places, this study aimed to investigate the applicability of sniffer dogs to carry out such a task. Methods: Firstly, three male and female dogs from German shepherd (Saray), German black (Kuzhi) and Labrador (Marco) breeds had been intensively trained throughout the classical conditioning method for 7 weeks. They were introduced to human specimens obtained from the throat and pharyngeal secretions of participants who were already reported positive or negative for SARS-COV-2 infection be RT-PCR. Each dog underwent the conditioning process for almost 1000 times. In the meantime another similar condition process was conducted on clothes and masks of COVID-19 patient using another three male and female dogs from Labrador (Lexi), Border gypsy (Sami), and Golden retriever (Zhico) breeds. In verification test for the first three dogs, 80 pharyngeal secretion samples consisting of 26 positive and 54 negative samples from different medical centers who underwent RT-PCR test were in a single-blind method. In the second verification test for the other three dogs, masks and clothes of 50 RT-PCR positive and 70 RT-PCR negative cases from different medical center were used. Results: In verification test using pharyngeal secretion, the sniffer dogs’ detection capability was associated with a 65% of sensitivity and 89% of specificity and they amanged to identify 17 out of the 26 positive and 48 out of the 54 true negative samples. In the next verification test using patients’ face masks and clothes, 43 out of the 50 positive samples were correctly identified by the dogs. Moreover, out of the 70 negative samples, 65 samples were correctly found to be negative. The sensitivity of this test was as high as 86% and its specificity was 92.9%. In addition, the positive and negative predictive values were 89.6 and 90.3%, respectively. Conclusion: Dogs are capable of being trained to identify COVID-19 cases by sniffing their odour, so they can be used as a reliable tool in limited screening.
Preprint
Full-text available
This study aimed to evaluate the sensitivity of 21 dogs belonging to different United Arab Emirates (UAE) Ministry of Interior (MOI), trained for COVID-19 olfactory detection. The study involved 17 explosives detection dogs, two cadaver detection dogs and two dogs with no previous detection training. Training lasted two weeks before starting the validation protocol. Sequential five and seven-cone line-ups were used with axillary sweat samples from symptomatic COVID-19 individuals (SARS-CoV-2 PCR positive) and from asymptomatic COVID-19 negative individuals (SARS-CoV-2 PCR negative). A total of 1368 trials were performed during validation, including 151 positive and 110 negative samples. Each line-up had one positive sample and at least one negative sample. The dog had to mark the positive sample, randomly positioned behind one of the cones. The dog, handler and data recorder were blinded to the positive sample location. The calculated overall sensitivities were between 71% and 79% for three dogs, between83% and 87% for three other dogs, and equal to or higher than 90% for the remaining 15 dogs (more than two thirds of the 21 dogs). After calculating the overall sensitivity for each dog using all line-ups, “matched” sensitivities were calculated only including line-ups containing COVID-19 positive and negative samples strictly comparable on confounding factors such as diabetes, anosmia, asthma, fever, body pain, diarrhoea, sex, hospital, method of sweat collection and sampling duration. Most of the time, the sensitivities increased after matching. Pandemic conditions in the U.A.E., associated with the desire to use dogs as an efficient mass-pretesting tool has already led to the operational deployment of the study dogs. Future studies will focus on comparatives fields-test results including the impact of the main COVID-19 comorbidities and other respiratory tract infections.
Article
Full-text available
Objective Literature accounts of service dogs alerting patients prior to their seizures are a mix of historically poor quality data and confounding diagnoses. In a group of epilepsy patients, Canine Assistants and Florida International University characterized a unique scent combination of volatile organic compounds present during the immediate postictal period, but never at other times. The current study was designed to confirm prospectively if this unique scent, and potential biomarker, can: (1) be detected in an epilepsy monitoring unit (EMU), (2) whether this scent is present with nonepileptic seizures, and (3) whether this scent also precedes the clinical-electrographic seizure. Methods Following consent and approval, sweat samples taken from EMU admissions at Denver Health Medical Center were sent to Canine Assistants in Georgia. Their team of service dogs, who had been imprinted to recognize the unique scent, were then asked to process these sweat samples in a simple yes/no identification paradigm. Results Sixty unique subjects were enrolled over a two-year period. In the first part of this study, a total of 298 ictal sweat samples of 680 total observations were collected. The dogs had a 93.7% (OR: 14.89, 95% CI: 9.27, 23.90) probability of correctly distinguishing between ictal and interictal sweat samples. In the nonepileptic seizure population, 18 of the 19 NES events that were accompanied by sweat sample collections were not associated with identification of the unique seizure scent. In the second part of the study, in which subjects had samples collected every hour, dogs identified the unique seizure scent presence before 78.7% of all seizures captured, at a probability of 82.2% (OR: 4.60, 95% CI: 0.98, 21.69) of a positive detection predicting a seizure. The average duration of the warning phase of the scent was 68.2 min. The average duration of the tail phase of the scent faded after 81 min. Significance This study confirms the unique seizure scent identified by Canine Assistants and FIU may be collected and recognized by dogs trained to do so, in a prospective manner. A significant number of seizures appear to be associated with the unique scent presence prior to clinical-electrical onset of the seizure itself, and therefore further study of this biomarker is warranted.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this proof-of-concept study was to evaluate if trained dogs could discriminate between sweat samples from symptomatic COVID-19 positive individuals (SARS-CoV-2 PCR positive) and those from asymptomatic COVID-19 negative individuals. The study was conducted at 2 sites (Paris, France, and Beirut, Lebanon), followed the same training and testing protocols, and involved six detection dogs (three explosive detection dogs, one search and rescue dog, and two colon cancer detection dogs). A total of 177 individuals were recruited for the study (95 symptomatic COVID-19 positive and 82 asymptomatic COVID-19 negative individuals) from five hospitals, and one underarm sweat sample per individual was collected. The dog training sessions lasted between one and three weeks. Once trained, the dog had to mark the COVID-19 positive sample randomly placed behind one of three or four olfactory cones (the other cones contained at least one COVID-19 negative sample and between zero and two mocks). During the testing session, a COVID-19 positive sample could be used up to a maximum of three times for one dog. The dog and its handler were both blinded to the COVID-positive sample location. The success rate per dog (i.e., the number of correct indications divided by the number of trials) ranged from 76% to 100%. The lower bound of the 95% confidence interval of the estimated success rate was most of the time higher than the success rate obtained by chance after removing the number PLOS ONE PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.
Article
There is an urgent need for screening of patients with a communicable viral disease to cut infection chains. Recently, we demonstrated that ion mobility spectrometry coupled with a multicapillary column (MCC-IMS) is able to identify influenza-A infections in patients’ breath. With a decreasing influenza epidemic and upcoming SARS-CoV-2 infections we proceeded further and analyzed patients with suspected SARS-CoV-2 infections. In this study, the nasal breath of 75 patients (34 male, 41 female, aged 64.4 ± 15.4 years) was investigated by MCC-IMS for viral infections. Fourteen were positively diagnosed with influenza-A infection and sixteen with SARS-CoV-2 by reverse transcription polymerase chain reaction (RT-PCR) of nasopharyngeal swabs. In one patient RT-PCR was highly suspicious of SARS-CoV-2 but initially inconclusive. The remaining 44 patients served as controls. Breath fingerprints for specific infections were assessed by a combination of cluster analysis and multivariate statistics. There were no significant differences in gender or age according to the groups. In the cross validation of the discriminant analysis 72 of the 74 clearly defined patients could be correctly classified to the respective group. Even the inconclusive patient could be mapped to the SARS-CoV-2 group by applying the discrimination functions. Conclusion: SARS-CoV-2 infection and influenza-A infection can be detected with the help of MCC-IMS in breath in this pilot study. As this method provides a fast non-invasive diagnosis it should be further developed in a larger cohort for screening of communicable viral diseases. A validation study is ongoing during the second wave of COVID-19. Trial registration: ClinicalTrial.gov, NCT04282135 Registered 20 February 2020—Retrospectively registered, https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT04282135?term=IMS&draw=2&rank=1