ArticlePDF Available

Image of Death and Attitude Towards Death

Authors:

Abstract

Mental images play a vital role in cognitive processing and behaviour. The aim of this study was to assess the influence of image of death (positive or negative), and sex on the attitude towards death. A sample of 250 university undergraduates were applied the subscales of the Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R). Undergraduates with a positive image of death had less Fear of Death (FD) and Death Avoidance (DA), more Escape Acceptance (EA), and higher scores in Neutral Acceptance (NA) than individuals with a negative image of death. The results revealed no significant sex differences in any of the subscales of the Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R). Further lines of research are proposed.
______________________________
Author info: Correspondence should be sent to:
Alfredo Campos, University
of Santiago de Compostela. Department of Basic Psychology, 15782 Santiago de
Compostela, Spain, or alfredo.campos@usc.es
North American Journal of Psychology, 2020, Vol. 22, No. 4, 701 -710.
NAJP
Image of Death and Attitude towards Death
María Esther López-Pérez & Margarita Pino-Juste
University of Vigo
Alfredo Campos
University of Santiago de Compostela
Mental images play a vital role in cognitive processing and behaviour.
The aim of this study was to assess the influence of image of death
(positive or negative), and sex on the attitude towards death. A sample of
250 university undergraduates was administered the subscales of the
Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R). Undergraduates with a positive
image of death had less Fear of Death (FD) and Death Avoidance (DA),
more Escape Acceptance (EA), and higher scores in Neutral Acceptance
(NA) than individuals with a negative image of death. The results
revealed no significant sex differences in any of the subscales of the
Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R). Further lines of research are
proposed.
Keywords: Mental imagery, positive mental images, negative mental
images, death attitude.
Mental images play a vital role in cognitive processing and behaviour
(Campos & Campos-Juanatey, 2019, 2020; Pérez-Fabello & Campos,
2017; Pérez-Fabello et al., 2018), and directly affect attitudes,
particularly if images are conceived as beyond the visual, and involve
auditory, gustative, olfactory, and tactile images (Campos & Fuentes,
2016; Campos & Pérez-Fabello, 2005).
Attitudes towards death vary according to the historical period, the
country and culture, ethnicity, sex, education, as well as an array of other
variables that make up a person’s idea of death (Lee, 2009; McGovern &
Barry, 2000). Attitudes towards death are also related to ideology and
religiosity (Castano et al., 2011; Dadfar & Lester, 2017). One of the
variables influencing attitudes towards death is mental imagery, in
particular whether the mental image of death is positive or negative
(Borkovec et al., 2004). López-Pérez et al. (in press) found that, in a
group of teenagers, the individuals who had greater image control had
significantly less Fear of Death (FD), lower scores on the Avoidance of
702 NORTH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY
Death (DA) scale, and higher scores on the scale of Neutral Acceptance
(NA) than students who had lower image control.
Thus, in a study where half of the participants listened to audio
recordings of descriptions of imagined unpleasant scenarios, and were
instructed to imagine the scenarios, whereas the other half listened to the
same audio recordings of descriptions of imagined unpleasant scenarios,
and were instructed to verbally elaborate on the same information,
emotion was found to be higher in the former than the latter (Holmes &
Matheus, 2005). Recently, a study applying the Death Imagery Scale
measuring five types of imagery: re-enactment, rescue, revenge, reunion,
and remorse to 130 violent death survivors, who sought (and received)
therapeutic treatment at a community health care clinic, found that the
most frequent mental images in violent death survivors were re-
enactment, rescue, and remorse imagery, and all forms of imagery were
associated with post-traumatic stress disorder (Baddeley et al., 2015).
In numerous cases, individuals with extremely negative attitudes
towards death leading to behavioural disorders require treatment to
transform these images and negative attitudes into positive ones (Clark et
al., 2006; Ng et al., 2014; Nilsson et al., 2012; Stopa et al., 2012; Wild et
al., 2008). Negative images are quite common in clinical and non-clinical
populations, but negative images are also characteristic of several mental
disorders (Brewin, 2010). For instance, an absence of positive mental
images and a presence of negative images have been found in depression
severity (WeBlau, 2015). Moreover, mental images influence affective
responses (Deeprose & Holmes, 2010; Holmes et al., 2008), and states of
mood (Deeprose & Holmes, 2010; Holmes et al., 2008).
López-Pérez et al. (2020) applied the Image of Death Scale (IDS;
López-Pérez et al., 2020) to determine if image of death was positive or
negative, and the Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R) to a sample of
280 adolescents, and found participants with a positive image of death
scored higher on Approach Acceptance, Escape Acceptance, and Neutral
Acceptance, and lower in Fear of Death, and Death Avoidance, than
adolescents with a negative image of death. The aim of this study was to
replicate this study in a sample of adults and contrast the results.
Sex is a variable influencing attitudes towards death (Lee, 2009;
McGovern & Barry, 2000); notwithstanding, the results are inconsistent.
No significant differences between women and men were found in the
description of death, either in the description of positive images or
negative images between the groups of undergraduates (McDonald &
Hilgendorf, 1986). However, in a study on a group of 144 Atlantic
Canadian students who were administered the Multidimensional Fear of
Death Scale (MFODS), women exhibited more Fear for Significant
Others and Fear of the Death (Power & Smith, 2008).
Lopez-Perez, Pino-Juste, & Campos IMAGE OF DEATH 703
Moreover, Wong et al.’s (1994) comparison of 100 young adults
(mean age = 23.3 years), 100 middle-aged adults (mean age = 41.8
years), and 100 elderly adults (mean age = 72.9 years) found sex
significantly related to the scores on certain subscales of the Death
Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R; Wong et al., 1994): Approach
Acceptance (AA), Death Avoidance (DA), and Escape Acceptance (EA).
Wong et al. (1994) found women were significantly more accepting of
life after death and significantly more accepting of death as an escape
from life than men. In contrast, men tended to avoid all thoughts of death
more than women. No differences were found between women and men
on the Neutral Acceptance (NA), or the Fear of Death (FD) subscales.
López-Pérez et al. (2020) found in a group of adolescents that women
scored higher than men on the Fear of Death (FD) Subscale, and the
Escape Acceptance (EA) Subscale. However, no significant differences
were found between women and men on the other subscales of the Death
Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R): Approach Acceptance (AA), Death
Avoidance (DA), or Neutral Acceptance (NA).
Thus, the aim of this study was to determine if a person’s image of
death (positive or negative) and sex influenced the attitude towards death.
The hypothesis was that an adult’s image of death (positive or negative)
and sex would significantly influence attitudes towards death. The
independent variables were image of death and sex, and the dependent
variables were the subscales of the Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-
R): Approach Acceptance (AA), Fear of Death (FD), Death Avoidance
(DA), Escape Acceptance (EA), and Neutral Acceptance (NA). The
authors contend that in the present climate of COVID-19, it is a topic of
current interest. If the hypothesis is confirmed, it will entail a major
advance for clinical practice.
METHOD
Participants
A group of 250 third and fourth-year science and humanities
undergraduates from the University of Vigo (201 women and 49 men,
mean age 22.03 years, standard deviation 2.38, range 19 to 28 years)
participated in this study.
Instruments
The Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R), a version of the Death
Attitude Profile (DAP, Gesser et al., 1988), which is a multidimensional
measure of attitudes towards death developed by Wong et al. (1994), was
administered. The scale consists of 32 items scored on a Likert type scale
ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree. The subscales
are: Fear of Death (FD), Death Avoidance (DA), Neutral Acceptance
704 NORTH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY
(NA), Approach Acceptance (AA), and Escape Acceptance (EA). Wong
et al (1994) obtained a Cronbach’s alpha for Approach Acceptance (AA)
of .97, an alpha for Fear of Death (FD) of .86, an alpha for Death
Avoidance (DA) of .88, the alpha for Escape Acceptance (EA) was .84,
and the alpha for Neutral Acceptance (NA) was .65. The test had no
completion time limit.
The Image of Death Scale (IDS; López-Pérez et al., 2020) is a Likert
type scale with only one item: “If you had to describe your image of
death, Would you say it is positive (rest, absence of pain, etc.), or
negative (loss of life, family, etc.)?”. Responses were on 6-point scale
where 1 = Very negative, 2 = Fairly negative, 3 = Negative, 4 = Positive,
5 = Fairly positive, 6 = Very positive.
Procedure
Participants were administered in their usual classrooms the Death
Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R; Wong et al., 1994), and the Image of
Death Scale (IDS; López-Pérez et al., 2020). Test instructions were given
and the order of test presentation was counterbalanced. All
undergraduates freely volunteered to participate in the study and were not
rewarded. Informed consent was obtained, and participants were
guaranteed their data would remain anonymous and confidential. The
tests were administered in the students’ usual classrooms in groups of
approximately 20 undergraduates during school hours. The instructions
given to participants were the instructions for the tests. The study was
approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Santiago de
Compostela and complied with the Declaration of Helsinki 2013.
Having gathered the data, participants were classified as either
Positive image of death for scores 4 or higher on the Image of Death
Scale (IDS; López-Pérez et al., 2020), or Negative image of death for
scores below 4 on the Image of Death Scale (IDS; López-Pérez et al.,
2020).
Data Analysis
Cronbach’s Alpha was used to evaluate the reliability of the subscales
of the Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R; Wong et al., 1994). In
order to analyse the influence of Image of Death and sex on each of the
subscales of the Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R), a 2 (Image of
Death) x 2 (Sex) Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) was
performed. The dependent variables were the subscales of the Death
Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R): Approach Acceptance (AA), Fear of
Death (FD), Death Avoidance (DA), Escape Acceptance (EA), and
Neutral Acceptance (NA). The statistical data analyses were performed
using the SPSS 22.0 software program.
Lopez-Perez, Pino-Juste, & Campos IMAGE OF DEATH 705
RESULTS
The first step was to assess the reliability of the Death Attitude
Profile-Revised subscales (DAP-R; Wong et al., 1994). The Cronbach’s
alpha for Approach Acceptance (AA) was .89, the alpha for Fear of
Death (FD) was .85, the alpha for Death Avoidance (DA) was .91, the
alpha for Escape Acceptance (EA) was .83, the alpha for Neutral
Acceptance (NA) was .72, and the alpha for the Test total of the (DAP-R)
was .85.
MANOVA was performed to determine if Image of Death (positive
or negative) and Sex influenced the scores on the subscales of the Death
Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R): Approach Acceptance (AA), Fear of
Death (FD), Death Avoidance (DA), Escape Acceptance (EA), and
Neutral Acceptance (NA). The means and standard deviations of the
groups are shown in Table 1. Results of the MANOVA revealed
significant differences in the scores of different subscales of the Death
Table 1 Means & Standard Deviations (in Brackets) of the Scales of
AttitudeTowards Death (AA, FD, DA, EA, NA) According to the Image
of Death (IDS) & Sex
Subscales IDS Sex Total
DAP-R Positive Negative Men Women M
AA 2.49
2.49 2.61 2.46 2.49
(1.29)
(1.20) (1.25) (1.21) (1.22)
FD 3.34
4.37 3.48 4.26 4.11
(1.30)
(1.39) (1.28) (1.43) (1.44)
DA 3.33
4.32 3.49 4.22 4.07
(1.43)
(1.64) (1.57) (1.63) (1.64)
EA 3.49
2.93 3.26 3.02 3.07
(1.50)
(1.45) (1.36) (1.50) (1.48)
NA 5.99
5.22 5.47 5.40 5.42
(0.62)
(0.90) (0.93) (0.89) (0.90)
Note: IDS = Image of Death Scale; AA =Approach Acceptance; FD =Fear of Death; DA
= Death Avoidance; EA=Escape Acceptance; NA=Neutral Acceptance
Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R): Approach Acceptance (AA), Fear of
Death (FD), Death Avoidance (DA), Escape Acceptance (EA), and
Neutral Acceptance (NA), between participants with a positive image
and with a negative image of death, Wilks’ Lambda =.88, F(5, 240) =
6.37, p < .001, η
p
2
= .12, power = 1.
The univariate analysis showed significant differences between the
scores of participants with a positive image of death and those with a
negative image of death on the subscales of the Death Attitude Profile-
Revised (DAP-R): Fear of Death (FD), Death Avoidance (DA), Escape
Acceptance (EA), and Neutral Acceptance (NA), with the exception of
Approach Acceptance (AA). Undergraduates with a positive image of
706 NORTH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY
death (M = 3.34, SD = 1.30) had less Fear of Death (FD) than
undergraduates with a negative image of death (M = 4.37, SD = 1.39),
and this difference was significant, F(1, 244) = 8.07, p = .005, η
p
2
= .03,
power = .81.
Individuals with a positive image of death (M = 3.33, SD = 1.43) had
less Death Avoidance (DA) than individuals with a negative image of
death (M = 4.32, SD = 1.64), and this difference was significant, F(1,
244) = 7.79, p = .006, η
p
2
= .03, power = 0.79. Undergraduates with a
positive image of death scored higher on Escape Acceptance (EA) (M =
3.49, SD = 1.50) than undergraduates with a negative image of death (M
= 2.93, SD = 1.45), and this difference was significant, F(1, 244) = 5.98,
p = .01, , η
p
2
= .02, power = .68. Finally, individuals with a positive
image of death scored higher on Neutral Acceptance (NA) (M = 5.99, SD
= 0.62) than individuals with a negative image of death (M = 5.22, SD =
0.90), and the difference was significant, F(1, 244) = 24.42, p < .001, η
p
2
= .09, power = 1. No significant difference was observed in Approach
Acceptance (AA), between university undergraduates with a positive
image of death and those with a negative image of death, F(1, 244) =
0.04, p = .84, η
p
2
= .01, power = .06.
The next step was to analyse significant differences between women
and men in the scores of the subscales of the Death Attitude Profile-
Revised (DAP-R): Approach Acceptance (AA), Fear of Death (FD),
Death Avoidance (DA), Escape Acceptance (EA), and Neutral
Acceptance (NA). The results of the MANOVA revealed no significant
differences between women and men in any of the subscales, Wilks’
Lambda = .96, F(5, 240) = 1.78, p = .12, η
p
2
= .04, power = .61. The
interaction between both variables, Image of Death and Sex was not
significant, Wilks’ Lambda = .97, F(5, 244) = 1.59, p = .16, η
p
2
= .03,
power = .55.
DISCUSSION
The Cronbach’s alphas in the study ranged from .72, for the Neutral
Acceptance (NA) subscale, to .91 for Death Avoidance (DA). These
alphas are considered by George and Mallery (2003) to be “acceptable”
and “excellent.” Moreover, significant differences were found between
participants with a positive image and those with a negative image of
death, on certain subscales of the Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-
R): Fear of Death (FD), Death Avoidance (DA), Escape Acceptance
(EA), and Neutral Acceptance (NA). Undergraduates with a positive
image of death had less Fear of Death (FD), and less Death Avoidance
(DA), but more Escape Acceptance (EA), and scored higher in Neutral
Acceptance (NA) than individuals with a negative image of death. These
results corroborated the work of López-Pérez et al. (2020), except for the
Approach Acceptance (AA) subscale, which in López-Pérez’ et al.
Lopez-Perez, Pino-Juste, & Campos IMAGE OF DEATH 707
(2020) study participants with a positive image of death scored higher on
Approach Acceptance (AA), than individuals with a negative image of
death. In this study, no significant differences were found between both
groups in the Approach Acceptance subscale (AA). It is worth noting that
adolescents participated in the study of López-Pérez et al., whereas the
participants in the present study were university undergraduates.
In this study, no significant differences were found between women
and men in the scores of the subscales of the Death Attitude Profile-
Revised (DAP-R): Approach Acceptance (AA), Fear of Death (FD),
Death Avoidance (DA), Escape Acceptance (EA), and Neutral
Acceptance (NA). This result corroborated other studies in observing no
significant differences between women and men: McDonald and
Hilgendorf (1986) found no significant differences between women and
men in descriptions of death in a group of undergraduate students, in
neither positive, nor negative images. Wong et al. (1994) found no
differences between women and men on the Neutral Acceptance (NA),
and the Fear of Death (FD), subscales, and López-Pérez et al. (2020)
found no significant differences between women and men in certain
subscales of the Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R): Approach
Acceptance (AA), Death Avoidance (DA), and Neutral Acceptance
(NA). However, the results of the present study disagree with the
findings of studies reporting significant differences in certain attitudes
towards death between women and men: Power and Smith (2008) found
women had more Fear for Significant Others and Fear of the dead than
men, and López-Pérez et al. (2020) found women scored higher than men
on the Fear of Death (FD) scale, and the Escape Acceptance (EA) scale
in a group of adolescents.
The influence of a positive or negative image of death on attitudes to
death has rarely been examined. The few studies mentioned in the
literature suggest image has an influence, but the results are inconsistent,
which underscores the need for further studies to measure the influence
of age, ethnicity, culture, and sex using the same test of attitudes to death.
The results of comparisons between women and men in attitude towards
death are inconsistent, which highlights the need for research to elucidate
this relationship and determine the influence of culture and age. In this
study no significant sex differences were found between women and
men, which may be due to the lack of sex parity in the sample. It would
be interesting to replicate this study with sex parity.
The data of the present study were gathered prior to the COVID-19
pandemic, and it would be most interesting to collect data from the same
individuals, or individuals of a similar age group and socioeconomic
characteristic to evaluate the effects of COVID-19 on attitudes towards
death. As throughout the pandemic everybody has experienced the death
708 NORTH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY
of a loved one at some time, as well as being personally exposed to the
fear of death itself, it would seem reasonable to believe the COVID-19
pandemic has had an impact on attitudes to death.
The authors foresee these results will have a significant impact on the
practical application of clinical psychology since negative images of
death can be modified and converted into positive ones. The resulting
change in attitudes towards death will reduce anxiety towards death and
enhance a person’s well-being.
REFERENCES
Baddeley, J. L., Williams, J. L., Rynearson, T., Correa, F., Saindon, C., &
Rheingold, A. (2015). Death thoughts and images in treatment-seekers after
violent loss. Death Studies, 39(2), 84-91. https://doi.org/10.
1080/07481187.2014.893274
Borkovec, R. D., Alacine, O, & Behar, E. (2004). Avoidance theory of worry and
generalized anxiety disorder. In R. G. Heimberg, C. L. Turk, & D. S Mennin
(Eds.). Generalized anxiety disorder: Advances in research and practice (pp.
77-108). The Guilford Press.
Brewin, C. R., Gregory, J. D., Lipton, M., & Burgess, N. (2010). Intrusive images
in psychological disorders: Characteristics, neural mechanisms, and treatment
implications. Psychological Review, 117, 210-232. https://doi.org/10.
1037/a0018113
Campos, A., & Campos-Juanatey, D. (2019). Gender differences in the rotation of
city maps. American Journal of Psychology, 132(3), 303-3013.
https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.132.3.0303
Campos, A., & Campos-Juanatey, D. (2020). Measure of the ability to mentally
rotate maps. North American Journal of Psychology, 22(2), 289-298.
Campos, A., & Fuentes, L. (2016). Musical studies and the vividness and clarity
of auditory imagery. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 36(1), 75-84.
Campos, A., & Pérez-Fabello, M. J. (2005). The Spanish version of Betts`
Questionnaire Upon Mental Imagery. Psychological Reports, 96(1), 51-56.
https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.96.1.51-56
Castano, E., Leider, B., Bonacossa, A., Nikkah, J., Perrulli, R., Spencer, F., &
Humphrey, N. (2011). Ideology, fear of death, and death anxiety. Political
Psychology, 32(4), 601-621. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2011.
00822.x
Clark, D. M., Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A., McManus, F., Fennell, M., Grey, N.,
Waddington, L., & Wild, J. (2006). Cognitive therapy versus exposure and
applied relaxation in social phobia: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(3), 568-578.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006x.74.3.568
Dadfar, M., & Lester, D. (2017). Religiously, spirituality and death anxiety.
Austin Journal of Psychiatry and Behaviovioral Sciences, 4(1), 1061.
Deeprose, C., & Holmes, E. (2010). An exploration of prospective imagery: The
Impact of Future Events Scale. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy,
38(2), 201-209. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465809990671
Lopez-Perez, Pino-Juste, & Campos IMAGE OF DEATH 709
George, D., & Mallery, P. (2003). SPSS for Windows step by step: A simple guide
and reference.11.0 update. Allyn & Bacon.
Gesser, G., Wong, P., & Reker, G. (1988). Death attitudes across the life-span:
The development and validation of the Death Attitude profile. Omega
Jounal of Death and Dying, 18, 113-128. https://doi.org/10.2190/0DQB-
7Q1E-2BER-H6YC
Holmes, E. A., Geddes, J. R., Colom, F., & Goodwin, G. M. (2008). Mental
imagery as an emotional amplifier: Application to bipolar disorder. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 46(12), 1251–1258. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.brat.2008.09.005
Holmes, E. A., & Matheus, A. (2005). Mental imagery and emotion: A special
relationship? Emotion, 5(4), 489-497. https://doi.org/ 10.1037/1528-
3542.5.4.489
Lee, S. K. (2009). East Asian attitudes toward death a search for the ways to
help east Asian elderly dying in contemporary America. Permanente Journal,
13 (3), 55-60. https://doi.org/10.7812/TPP/08-068
López-Pérez, M. E., Pino-Juste, M., & Campos, A. (2020). Mental imagery and
anxiety towards death in adolescence (Manuscript submitted for publication).
Department of Psychology, University of Santiago de Compostela.
López-Pérez, M. E., Pino-Juste, M., & Campos, A. (in press). Viveza, control de
imagen y actitud ante la muerte [Image vividness, image control and attitude
towards death]. Revista de Estudios e Investigación en Psicología y
Educación.
McGovern, M., & Barry, M. M. (2000). Death education: Knowledge, attitudes,
and perspectives of Irish parents and teachers. Death Studies, 24, 325-333.
https://doi.org/10.1080/074811800200487
Ng., A. S., Abbott, M. J., & Hunt, C. (2014). The effect of self-imagery on
symptoms and process in social anxiety: A systematic review. Clinical
Psychology Review, 34, 620-633.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.09.003
Nilsson, J., Lundh, L., & Viborg, G. (2012). Imagery rescripting of early
memories in social anxiety disorder: An experimental study. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 50, 387-392. https://doi.org/10.1016/j-brat.
2012.03.004
Pérez-Fabello, M. J., & Campos, A. (2017). Dissociative experiences and
vividness of auditory imagery. Creativity Research Journal, 29 (2), 200-205.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2017.1303310
Pérez-Fabello, M. J., Campos, A., & Felisberti, F. (2018). Object-spatial imagery
in Fine Arts, Psychology and Engineering. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 27,
131-138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2017.12.005
Power, T L., & Smith, S. M. (2008). Predictors of fear of death and self-
mortality: An Atlantic Canadian perspective. Death Studies, 32, 253-272.
https://doi.org/10.1080/07481180701880935
Stopa, L., Brown, M. A., & Hirsch, C. R. (2012). The effects of repeated imagery
practice on self-concept, anxiety, and performance in socially anxious
participants. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3(2), 223-242.
https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.021511
710 NORTH AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY
WeBlau, C., Closs, M., Höfling, V., & Steil, R. (2015). Visual mental imagery
and symptoms of depression results from a large-scale web-based study.
BMC Psychiatry, 15, 308. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-015-0689-1
Wild, J., Hackmann, A., & Clark, D. M. (2008). Rescripting early memories
linked to negative images in social anxiety disorder: A pilot study. Behavior
Therapy, 39(1), 47-56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth. 2007.04.003
Wong, P., Reker, G., & Gesser, G. (1994). Death Attitude Profile-Revised: A
multidimensional measure of attitudes toward death. In R. A. Neimeyer (Ed.).
Death anxiety handbook: Research, instrumentation, and application (pp.
121-148). Taylor and Francis.
... These attitudes are conventionally differentiated as positive (accepting) and negative (avoidance) [1,16,17]. Attitudes towards death change over time with the great influence of culture, sex, education, country, historical period and religion, as well as due to personal mental imagery and the idea of death [1,18]. The questions and conversations about death and dying are most commonly raised in terms of the time of closeness to large numbers of deaths, e.g., wars and pandemics such as COVID-19 [19][20][21]. ...
... It was shown that attitudes toward death are influenced by country, education, gender, culture and religion [1]. Regarding the negative death attitudes domain results, the studies worldwide show that mean values of FD were higher than values of DA in Germany, Portugal, Jordan, Spain, Poland, USA, Turkey and Brazil [3,[16][17][18]25,26,[41][42][43]. Moving to the east, the results from China show that the greater value in this domain was found in DA with the exception of the research made after the outbreak of COVID-19, where the values of FA and DA were similar [1,12,44]. ...
... Regarding positive death attitudes, the majority of research showed the highest values in NA dimensions, with the exception of those from Portugal, Jordan and Poland [1,3,7,12,[16][17][18]25,26,[41][42][43][44]. These results are in coordination with the observed Serbian population in our study with some exceptions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and Objectives: Death is an unavoidable experience in any person’s life and affects not only the dying person but also their caregivers. The dying process has been displaced from homes to health care facilities in the majority of cases. Facing death and dying has become an everyday life of health care professionals (HCP), especially in palliative care (PC) settings. This study aimed to investigate the death attitudes among HCPs in Serbia. Materials and Methods: The Serbian version of the Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-RSp) was used as a measurement instrument. Results: The average age of the 180 included participants was 42.2 ± 9.9 years; the majority were females (70.0%), with more than 10 years of working experience (73.0%), physicians (70.0%) and those working in a non-oncological (non-ONC) field (57.78%). The mean total score of DAP-RSp was 124.80 ± 22.44. The highest mean score was observed in the neutral acceptance dimension (NA) (5.82 ± 0.90) and lowest in the Escape acceptance (EA) (2.57 ± 1.21). Higher negative death attitudes were reported among nurses compared to physicians (p = 0.002). Statistically significant differences were observed in the fear of death (FD) and death avoidance (DA) domains, favoring PC specialists and oncologists (p = 0.004; p = 0.015). Physicians working in Oncology (ONC) showed lower FD values (p = 0.001) compared to non-ONC departments. Conclusions: Attitudes toward death among HCPs are of great importance for the well-being of both HCPs and patients. Negative attitudes can lead to deficient care. The fear of death is highly represented among Serbian HCPs working in non-ONC fields, including both nurses and physicians. This study emphasizes the need for further research to comprehensively explore and understand HCPs’ attitudes toward death. This research highlights the need for the development of an educational curriculum across all levels of medical education, aimed at overcoming the fear of death and enhancing coping strategies, which will improve the care for patients diagnosed with terminal illnesses.
... Similarly, nurses who score high on neutral acceptance have been shown to have more positive attitudes towards caring for the dying [47]. Neutral acceptance is also positively correlated with indifference towards death [46], and a more positive image of death in young people [51,52]. Individuals who score high on neutral acceptance also have more negative views on life extension (i.e., attempts to extend life and fight dying through whatever means available, a concept which overlaps with the aforementioned maladaptive attempts to extend life found in many anxiety-related disorders) [53]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Growing empirical evidence has revealed the central role of death anxiety in numerous mental health conditions. Given this, there is a significant need for treatments which specifically address fears of death. Whilst such treatments have only recently been developed within clinical psychology, the discipline of philosophy has a long history of offering valuable perspectives on death which may ameliorate this fear. In particular, we propose that the philosophy of Stoicism, which originated in ancient Greece and influenced the early development of cognitive and behaviour therapy (CBT), is of particular benefit to clinicians seeking to address death anxiety. We present a summary of Stoic philosophy and its arguments concerning death. Through integrating the ideas of Stoicism, we argue that current treatments for death anxiety would benefit from directly integrating Stoic perspectives on death. Lastly, we review evidence which suggests that cultivating attitudes to death which are consistent with Stoic philosophy may be associated with reduced death anxiety. We conclude that an awareness of Stoic philosophy has the potential to guide and improve CBT treatments for fears of death. Further research is needed in order to confirm whether treatments centering on Stoic perspectives on death lead to significant reductions in death anxiety.
... The negative attitude towards death related significantly to education level, consistent with the findings of Lee et al. (2006a), who reported that the fear of death in uneducated people is higher than that in people with high school or higher education. The research finding was also corroborated by López-Pérez et al. (2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Aim: To explore the mediating role of depression in older people receiving haemodialysis on social support and the attitude of participants towards death. Design: A cross-sectional questionnaire survey. Methods: Data were collected from older people undergoing dialysis (N = 209) at two regional hospitals in the north of Taiwan. Confirmatory factor analysis with structural equation model was used to clarify the strength of relationships and intermediary effects of three scales in which with 5,000 bootstrap samples using LISREL 9.31. Results: The final model provided a good fit for the data. Social support and depression have statistically significant effects on dialysis older person' negative death attitudes. The direct effect of social support on depression was the strongest (p<.001). Overall, depression completely mediates social support and positive death attitudes. Depression partially mediates social support and negative death attitude.
Article
Full-text available
Bu çalışmanın amacı farklı disiplinlerde eğitime devam eden üniversite öğrencilerinin ötenaziye ilişkin görüşlerini; ölüme karşı tutum, tanımlayıcı özellik ve kişisel tecrübe değişkenleri çerçevesinde inceleyerek aralarındaki ilişkiyi tespit etmektir. Çalışma, İstanbul Üniversitesi ve İstanbul Üniversitesi-Cerrahpaşa’da eğitim gören 354 öğrenci ile gerçekleştirilmiştir. Araştırmada; “Bilgi Formu”,“ Ölüme Karşı Tutum Ölçeği” ve “Ötenaziye Yönelik Tutumlar ve Görüşler Anket Formu” araçlarından yararlanılmıştır. Verilerin analiz işleminde; bağımsız örneklemler t testi, tek yönlü varyans analizi, spearman’s rho ve ki kare testi uygulanmıştır. Araştırma sonucunda; öğrencilerin eğitime devam ettikleri bölüm, ağrılı hastalık yaşayan bir yakına şahit olma, kayıp yaşama ve dindarlık düzeyi değişkenleri ile öğrencilerin ölüme karşı tutumları ve ötenaziye ilişkin görüşleri arasında anlamlı bir farklılaşma tespit edilmiştir. Aynı zamanda elde edilen bulgular neticesinde öğrencilerin ötenazi yaklaşımları ile ölüme karşı tutumları arasında anlamlı bir ilişki tespit edilmiştir.
Article
Full-text available
This study analyzed the influence of the image of death (positive or negative) and gender on death anxiety and assessed several subscales of the Death Attitude Profile-Revised in a sample of 280 compulsory secondary education students. Individuals with a positive image of death were found to exhibit less death anxiety than individuals with a negative image of death, and death anxiety was higher in boys than girls. Students with a positive image of death had greater Approach Acceptance, less Fear of Death, less Death Avoidance, more Escape Acceptance, and more Neutral Acceptance than students with a negative image of death.
Article
Full-text available
Las personas pueden tener imágenes mentales vivas sobre la muerte, y pueden tener control sobre ellas. El objetivo de esta investigación era saber si la viveza de imagen y el control de imagen influían en la actitud ante la muerte en un grupo de adolescentes (Estudio 1) y en un grupo de adultos (Estudio 2). Las variables independientes fueron la viveza de imagen y el control de imagen de los participantes, y como variables dependientes se utilizaron las subescalas del Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R): Aceptación de Acercamiento (AA), Miedo a la Muerte (FD), Evitación de la Muerte (DA), Aceptación de Escape (EA), y Aceptación Neutral (NA). Se encontró que, en el grupo de adolescentes, los individuos que tenían un mayor control de imagen tuvieron significativamente menos Miedo a la Muerte (FD), menores puntuaciones en la escala de Evitación de la Muerte (DA), y mayores puntuaciones en la escala de Aceptación Neutral (NA) que los estudiantes que tuvieron un menor control de imagen. En el Estudio 2 no se encontró influencia de la viveza de imagen ni del control de imagen sobre las subescalas del Death Attitude Profile-Revised (DAP-R). Se analizan las repercusiones de estos resultados en el ámbito escolar.
Article
Full-text available
Background Mental imagery may influence the onset and maintenance of depression, but specific mechanisms have not yet been determined. Methods Nine hundred twelve participants completed questionnaires on positive and negative mental images, as well as images of injury and death that lead to positive emotions (“ID-images”), and depressive symptomatology. The assessment was carried out online to reduce effects of social desirability. Results Positive images were reported by 87 % of the sample, negative images by 77 %. ID-images were present in one-third of the sample. A connection with depression severity was found for the absence of positive mental images and the presence of negative images as well as ID-images. Higher depression scores were associated with more frequent and vivid negative images, greater imagery distress, and a higher proportion of negative relative to positive images. Conclusions Mental images are clearly related to depression. Future research should focus on ID-images and their connection to suicide-risk in depressed patients.
Article
Full-text available
Four orthogonal factors were identified by principal component factor analysis: Fear of Death/Dying, Approach-Oriented Death Acceptance, Escape-Oriented Death Acceptance, and Neutral Death Acceptance. Theta estimates of the internal consistency of the factor scales ranged from fair (.60) to good (.89). An elderly sample (n = 50) showed less fear of death and more acceptance (all three kinds of acceptance) than the middle age (n = 50) and the young (n = 50) samples. As predicted, Fear of Death/Dying was negatively related to happiness, but positively related to hopelessness, whereas Escape-Oriented Death Acceptance was positively related to hopelessness, thus providing some evidence of concurrent validity of the DAP.
Article
This study assessed gender differences in city map rotation, and the differences between architecture, business studies, fine arts, and psychology undergraduates. Participants were given 90 pairs of maps positioned at different rotation angles (0°, 90°, and 180°). Participants were required to ascertain as quickly as possible whether rotating the map on its axis to the right (without lifting it up) aligned it with the model. In Test 1, no rotation was required because the angle was 0°, whereas in Tests 2 and 3 the angle of rotation was 90° or 180°, respectively. The number of correct rotations (correct choices minus errors) were calculated for women and men and for field of study. Men made more correct choices minus errors than women. Architecture undergraduates made more correct choices minus errors than other students. This study indicates that maps placed on physical elements located in streets or buildings should be positioned at 0° in relation to the surrounding environment. Additional studies are needed to analyze the causes underlying differences between women and men and differences in map rotation between undergraduates of other university degrees.
Article
How do students of Fine Arts, Psychology and Engineering visualize objects mentally? To address this basic but important educational question, the students’ ability to process figure-ground and spatial imagery was compared using the Object-Spatial Imagery and Verbal Questionnaire (OSIVQ), the Mental Rotation Test (MRT), and the Measure of the Ability to Form Spatial Mental Imagery (MASMI). There were 99 Fine Arts, 92 Psychology, and 90 Engineering second-year university undergraduates. The results revealed that students preferred image to verbal processing, but differences were observed in their preferred style of image processing. Fine Arts and Psychology students were the most inclined to use object image processing while Engineering students preferred spatial processing. Verbal processing was only prominent in Psychology students. In all three academic disciplines, the students’ performance in the spatial ability tests is consistent with the features specific of the cognitive style. Overall, men were better at mentally rotating objects than women. We believe that there is a need to design artistic spatial imagery measurement tests to access the students’ skills in mental image processing, including image rotation, to identify weaknesses and to develop efficient training strategies in order to correct or attenuate them.
Article
The relationship between dissociation and auditory imagery were assessed, 2 variables that sometime influence on artistic creativity. A total of 170 fine arts undergraduates (94 women and 76 men) received 2 dissociation questionnaires—the Dissociative Ability Scale (DAS), and the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES)—and 2 auditory imagery questionnaires—the Auditory Imagery Questionnaire (AIQ) and the Clarity of Auditory Imagery Scale (CAIS). Significant differences on the auditory imagery (CAIS) have been found between individuals scoring high or low on the DES and DAS. In this study, however, significant differences were only observed on the Auditory Imagery Questionnaire (AIQ) between individuals scoring high or low on the DAS. Moreover, conflicting results have been obtained on the influence of dissociative experiences on dissociative skills in 2 auditory imagery tests. The discrepancies in the results only lead to more questions being asked than answered. Several lines of research are proposed.
Article
Different studies have analyzed mental auditory imagery and mental imagery derived from the other senses. This study analyzed the relationship of musical training with auditory imagery vividness and mental imagery derived from the other senses. A group of 200 participants (100 music students and 100 Compulsory Secondary Education students) were administered the Betts’ Questionnaire upon Mental Imagery and the Clarity of Auditory Imagery Scale. Students of music obtained higher in cutaneous, kinesthetic, gustatory, visual, and auditory imagery vividness scores than students not studying music. Moreover, music students obtained higher clarity of auditory imagery scores than nonmusicians. The gender of participants did not influence reported auditory imagery vividness or the clarity of auditory imagery. No gender differences were observed in the other sensorial modalities, except for visual imagery vividness, in which women scored higher than men. The interaction between gender and music training was not significant. The results are discussed and new lines of research are proposed.
Article
Current cognitive models of social phobia, all stress the importance of negative self-perceptions in maintaining social anxiety (Moscovitch, 2009, Hofmann, 2007; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997; Clark & Wells, 1995). In some models, (Clark & Wells, 1995) negative self-perceptions are embodied in the form of a visual image. In this study, 58 socially anxious participants were assigned to either a positive (n =19) or a negative (n =19) self-imagery condition and we tested the impact of repeated imagery practice on self-concept (self-esteem and self-concept clarity), and on anxiety and performance in a conversation with a stooge. Participants in each condition practiced holding either a positive or a negative self-image in mind over eight days while at the same time imagining themselves in a series of increasingly challenging social situations. After the final imagery practice, participants took part in a conversation with a stooge. We hypothesised that repeated practice with a positive image would produce higher levels of self-esteem, more self-concept clarity, and would lead to less anxiety and better ratings of performance than holding a negative image. The hypotheses were partially supported. Positive self- imagery practice led to higher self-esteem ratings and higher self-concept clarity on a computerised measure of self-concept clarity, but not on a self-report measure. Positive self-imagery practice also produced better performance ratings in the social test (in both subjective and objective assessments of performance) and a trend towards reduced anxiety. The results are discussed in relation to Conway and Pleydell-Pearce’s (2000) self-memory model and the clinical implications are considered.
Article
Background Cognitive models of Social Anxiety Disorder suggest that negative self-images maintain social fears despite repeated exposure to benign social situations. An accumulating body of evidence supports this notion, and preliminary data indicate that modifying self-imagery can potentially reduce fears of negative evaluation in socially anxious individuals. This systematic review aims to evaluate the existing research on the effect of self-imagery on symptoms and processes in social anxiety. Methods The following databases were searched: PsycInfo, Medline, PubMed, and Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials. Articles were limited to adult samples and English language papers that examined the effect of self-imagery on at least one measure related to social anxiety. Risk of bias and study results were reviewed for each study. Results In the 17 studies that were identified, negative self-imagery had a consistently adverse effect on anxiety, self and observer-rater performance appraisals, and negative thoughts for both socially anxious and non-clinical participants. Effects of negative self-imagery were generally not significantly greater for socially anxious participants. Conclusions Further research is warranted to determine the precise role of different types of self-imagery for both clinical and non-clinical individuals.