ArticlePDF Available

The Effect of a Secondary Task on Kinematics during Turning in Parkinson's Disease with Mild to Moderate Impairment

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) show typical gait asymmetries. These peculiar motor impairments are exacerbated by added cognitive and/or mechanical loading. However, there is scarce literature that chains these two stimuli. The aim of this study was to investigate the combined effects of a dual task (cognitive task) and turning (mechanical task) on the spatiotemporal parameters in mild to moderate PD. Participants (nine patients with PD and nine controls (CRs)) were evaluated while walking at their self-selected pace without a secondary task (single task), and while repeating the days of the week backwards (dual task) along a straight direction and a 60 • and 120 • turn. As speculated, in single tasking, PD patients preferred to walk with a shorter stride length (p < 0.05) but similar timing parameters, compared to the CR group; in dual tasking, both groups walked slower with shorter strides. As the turn angle increased, the speed will be reduced (p < 0.001), whereas the ground-foot contact will become greater (p < 0.001) in all the participants. We showed that the combination of a simple cognitive task and a mechanical task (especially at larger angles) could represent an important training stimulus in PD at the early stages of the pathology.
Content may be subject to copyright.
symmetry
S
S
Article
The Eect of a Secondary Task on Kinematics during
Turning in Parkinson’s Disease with Mild to
Moderate Impairment
Francesca Nardello , Emanuele Bertoli, Federica Bombieri, Matteo Bertucco * and
Andrea Monte
Department of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences University of Verona, 43, 37131 Verona,
Italy; francesca.nardello@univr.it (F.N.); emanuele.bertoli@studenti.univr.it (E.B.);
federica.bombieri@univr.it (F.B.); andrea.monte@univr.it (A.M.)
*Correspondence: matteo.bertucco@univr.it; Tel.: +39-045-8425131
Received: 10 July 2020; Accepted: 29 July 2020; Published: 3 August 2020


Abstract:
Patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) show typical gait asymmetries. These peculiar motor
impairments are exacerbated by added cognitive and/or mechanical loading. However, there is scarce
literature that chains these two stimuli. The aim of this study was to investigate the combined eects
of a dual task (cognitive task) and turning (mechanical task) on the spatiotemporal parameters in mild
to moderate PD. Participants (nine patients with PD and nine controls (CRs)) were evaluated while
walking at their self-selected pace without a secondary task (single task), and while repeating the days
of the week backwards (dual task) along a straight direction and a 60
and 120
turn. As speculated,
in single tasking, PD patients preferred to walk with a shorter stride length (p<0.05) but similar
timing parameters, compared to the CR group; in dual tasking, both groups walked slower with
shorter strides. As the turn angle increased, the speed will be reduced (p<0.001), whereas the
ground–foot contact will become greater (p<0.001) in all the participants. We showed that the
combination of a simple cognitive task and a mechanical task (especially at larger angles) could
represent an important training stimulus in PD at the early stages of the pathology.
Keywords: Parkinson’s disease; gait; dual task; turning; kinematics
1. Introduction
Gait functionality is accomplished with good health, whereas locomotion disturbances are
associated with an advancing age or with the presence of pathologies. Within 3 years of diagnosis, over
85% of people with Parkinson’s disease (PD) develop gait deteriorations [
1
,
2
], as well as a reduced
walking symmetry [
2
4
]. Such motor dysfunctions could be exacerbated under various conditions,
such as the dual task performance and the changing of the walking direction (i.e., turning).
Specifically, the execution of the two tasks simultaneously (“concurrent performance”) presents a
challenge for PD patients because of their disabled lower-level spinal centers and basal nuclei [
5
7
].
Studies have shown that, when compared to controls, subjects with PD show greater reductions in their
stride length [
8
,
9
] and, therefore, walking speed, as well as an increased variability of the kinematic
parameters [
10
16
]. These patterns are aggravated with the severity of the pathology [
17
,
18
] and with
the complexity of the concurrent task [
5
,
13
,
19
,
20
]. Since these findings strengthen the idea that the
cognitive function could contribute to gait regulation, it could be helpful to understand the eect of
dierent cognitive tasks, even very simple ones, in this kind of pathology [
21
23
]. This knowledge
could provide an insight on what is the best training stimulus to be administered in those patients.
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284; doi:10.3390/sym12081284 www.mdpi.com/journal/symmetry
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284 2 of 14
Concerning the walking direction, it has been shown that subjects with PD report turning
diculties, which are associated with an augmented risk of falling [
24
27
]. These impairments are due
to the central nervous system’s involvement in body re-orientation when travelling in the new direction.
From a kinematical point of view, patients with PD highlighted a smaller step width [
9
,
28
],
a reduction in the step length and a decrease in their walking velocity during a change of direction of
45
in the stepping tasks [
29
]. If the turning angle increased (i.e., to 90
), patients approached turns
with a slower step length and by performing the turn with a larger number of steps [
30
]. Walking
with an auditory cue reduced the gait-timing variability, as well as the step length, and increased the
radius of gyration during a turn of 180
[
31
]. Moreover, recent literature has shown that subjects with
PD spent more time during the turn (between 30
and 180
) and exhibit a reduction in the walking
stability compared to controls [
32
]. Turning 360
in place seemed to be a more compromised condition
when compared to turning while walking, with similar impairments with and without episodes of
freezing [
33
,
34
]. As in the case of the cognitive additional demand, the turning conditions could
also represent an important stimulus in such a population. Hence, investigating dierent turning
conditions could provide important information about the optimal training strategy in people with PD.
To date, a lingering question in the literature regards the combination of mechanical and cognitive
perturbations. Indeed, few studies have combined the dual task condition with the mechanical
perturbations (e.g., change of direction) [
35
37
]. Furthermore, previous studies usually used turn
angles larger than 90, which represents per se an important perturbation for PD patients [30].
Therefore, we used the most common turning angles during daily life (60
and 120
) [
38
],
in combination with a simple cognitive task (repeating the days of the week backwards), to better
understand the eects of these perturbations on gait kinematics. This combined approach has involved
participants with mild to moderate PD in order to provide additional information on how the typical
Parkinson’s disease walking pattern is modified under simultaneous cognitive and mechanical loads.
Hence, the primary aim was to investigate if the presence of a “dual task” condition can further
alter the kinematics (i.e., spatiotemporal parameters) of walking, or, on the contrary, if it can represent
a good training stimulus in mild and moderate Parkinson’s disease. This cognitive task was studied
during forward/linear walking and during turning.
Based on the previously mentioned literature, we hypothesized that the simple cognitive and the
mechanical tasks will exhibit similar eects in both populations (patients and controls), whereas the
combination of the two stimuli will show a higher impact on PD patients.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
This study has a cross-sectional, analytical, observational design. Nine patients with mild to
moderate PD (3 women and 6 men; mean
±
SD, 68.2
±
5.95 years old, 74.2
±
11.8 kg body mass,
1.70 ±0.10 m
height, and 25.6
±
3.07 kg
·
m
2
body mass index (BMI)) and nine healthy age-matched
controls (4 women and 5 men; 67.2
±
3.45 years old, 72.6
±
13.1 kg, 1.68
±
0.08 m, and
25.7 ±3.43 kg·m2
)
were recruited in this study.
All participants received written and oral instructions before the study and gave their written
informed consent for the experimental procedure. The study was conducted in accordance with the
Declaration of Helsinki, and the experimental protocol was approved by the Institutional Review
Board of the Department of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, University of Verona
(protocol number 2018-UNVRCLE-0451799).
Participants were recruited from a sample of late adulthood people attending the adapted physical
activity program at the School of Sport and Exercise Science at our University (see their characteristics
in Table 1).
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284 3 of 14
Table 1. Characteristics of subjects with Parkinson’s disease.
Subject
No.
Age
(yrs) Sex
Body
Mass
(kg)
Body
Height
(m)
BMI
(kg·m2)
Disease
Duration
(yrs)
Y&H
Score
UPDRS
Score MMSE Medication
Side of Appearance of
the First Motor
Symptom
PD1 77 F 80 1.64 29.74 4 1 26 29 Levodopa/carbidopa Left
PD2 66 F 63 1.58 25.24 4 3 37 27 Levodopa/carbidopa Left
PD3 71 M 70 1.70 24.22 3 1 24 28 Levodopa/benserazide Right
PD4 59 M 65 1.70 22.49 9 1 26 29 Levodopa/carbidopa Left
PD5 67 M 80 1.66 29.03 3 1 30 26 Levodopa/carbidopa Right
PD6 67 M 80 1.77 25.54 6 1 30 28 Levodopa/benserazide Left
PD7 63 F 55 1.59 21.76 1 1 23 29 Levodopa/benserazide Right
PD8 77 M 82 1.88 23.20 8 1 25 24 Levodopa/carbidopa Right
PD9 67 M 93 1.78 29.35 7 2 32 25 Levodopa/carbidopa Left
BMI: body mass index; Y&H: Hoehn and Yahr; UPDRS: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (motor section); MMSE: Mini Mental State Education.
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284 4 of 14
Patients were excluded if their medical condition proved unstable due to other neurological,
orthopedic, metabolic or cardiovascular co-morbidity factors aecting gait. There was no type of
rehabilitation in the month prior to recruitment or disease-modifying therapy that was not well defined.
A diagnosis of idiopathic PD was carried out by a neurologist, in accordance with the guidelines
established by the London Brain Bank [
39
]. The disease severity was classified according to the
modified Hoehn and Yahr scale (H&Y) [
40
], whereas the assessment of the degree of motor and
functional impairment was obtained using part III of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale
(UPDRS) [
41
]. Finally, cognitive function was assessed by using the Mini Mental State Examination
(MMSE), with scores of up to 30 (higher scores correspond to greater cognitive function).
We conducted an a priori screening to evaluate whether participants were able to carry out the
task instructions required for the experimental protocol (see the “Experimental procedures” later).
Based on this a priori screening, only individuals with PD with an MMSE score of 24 or higher were
able to perform the double stimuli, and therefore were recruited in the study.
2.2. Experimental Procedures
Tests were conducted in the Biomechanics Laboratory at the Department of Neurosciences,
Biomedicine and Movement Sciences. The spatial (distance) and temporal (time) characteristics of
the step pattern were measured using an eight-camera motion capture system (MX Ultranet, VICON,
Oxfordshire, UK), sampling at 100 Hz. This apparatus recorded the position of markers positioned
bilaterally on the feet: the calcaneus, lateral malleolus and 5th metatarsal-phalangeal joint.
Participants started from a standing position and walked barefoot at their self-selected speed over
a 10 m walkway (Figure 1, panel a). Each participant performed three dierent conditions: (i) forward
walking (WF (Figure 1, panel b)); (ii) walking, turning at 60
, walking (T60); (iii) walking, turning at
120
, walking (T120). Furthermore, T60 and T120 were conducted turning in both directions (left and
right (Figure 1, panels c and d)). During all turning tasks, a cone was positioned in the center of the
walkway (see the black hexagon in Figure 1) to identify the turning point. Furthermore, an operator
positioned himself at the end of the corridor lane that the subjects had to move. All directions were
tested in single and dual tasks, and the cognitive load consists of walking while repeating the days of
the week backwards [
17
]. Six trials of each condition were performed and the order of gait conditions
was randomly allocated (Figure 2). Subjects sat and rested in a chair for three minutes between trials.
Patients were tested at the peak dose in the medication cycle.
Symmetry2020,12,xFORPEERREVIEW4of15
Patientswereexcludediftheirmedicalconditionprovedunstableduetootherneurological,
orthopedic,metabolicorcardiovascularcomorbidityfactorsaffectinggait.Therewasnotypeof
rehabilitationinthemonthpriortorecruitmentordiseasemodifyingtherapythatwasnotwelldefined.A
diagnosisofidiopathicPDwascarriedoutbyaneurologist,inaccordancewiththeguidelinesestablished
bytheLondonBrainBank[39].ThediseaseseveritywasclassifiedaccordingtothemodifiedHoehnand
Yahrscale(H&Y)[40],whereastheassessmentofthedegreeofmotorandfunctionalimpairmentwas
obtainedusingpartIIIoftheUnifiedParkinson’sDiseaseRatingScale(UPDRS)[41].Finally,cognitive
functionwasassessedbyusingtheMiniMentalStateExamination(MMSE),withscoresofupto30(higher
scorescorrespondtogreatercognitivefunction).
Weconductedanaprioriscreeningtoevaluatewhetherparticipantswereabletocarryoutthetask
instructionsrequiredfortheexperimentalprotocol(seethe“Experimentalprocedures”later).Basedonthis
aprioriscreening,onlyindividualswithPDwithanMMSEscoreof24orhigherwereabletoperformthe
doublestimuli,andthereforewererecruitedinthestudy.
2.2.ExperimentalProcedures
TestswereconductedintheBiomechanicsLaboratoryattheDepartmentofNeurosciences,
BiomedicineandMovementSciences.Thespatial(distance)andtemporal(time)characteristicsofthestep
patternweremeasuredusinganeightcameramotioncapturesystem(MXUltranet,VICON,Oxfordshire,
UK),samplingat100Hz.Thisapparatusrecordedthepositionofmarkerspositionedbilaterallyonthefeet:
thecalcaneus,lateralmalleolusand5thmetatarsalphalangealjoint.
Participantsstartedfromastandingpositionandwalkedbarefootattheirselfselectedspeedovera10
mwalkway(Figure1,panela).Eachparticipantperformedthreedifferentconditions:i)forwardwalking
(WF(Figure1,panelb));ii)walking,turningat60°,walking(T60);iii)walking,turningat120°,walking
(T120).Furthermore,T60andT120wereconductedturninginbothdirections(leftandright(Figure1,
panelscandd)).Duringallturningtasks,aconewaspositionedinthecenterofthewalkway(seetheblack
hexagoninFigure1)toidentifytheturningpoint.Furthermore,anoperatorpositionedhimselfattheend
ofthecorridorlanethatthesubjectshadtomove.Alldirectionsweretestedinsingleanddualtasks,and
thecognitiveloadconsistsofwalkingwhilerepeatingthedaysoftheweekbackwards[17].Sixtrialsofeach
conditionwereperformedandtheorderofgaitconditionswasrandomlyallocated(Figure2).Subjectssat
andrestedinachairforthreeminutesbetweentrials.Patientsweretestedatthepeakdoseinthemedication
cycle.
Figure 1.
Schematic representation of walking conditions (
a
), and steps during walking (forward
walking (WF), (b) and turning (T60 in (c) and T120 in (d)).
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284 5 of 14
Symmetry2020,12,xFORPEERREVIEW5of15
Figure1.Schematicrepresentationofwalkingconditions(a),andstepsduringwalking(forwardwalking
(WF),(b)andturning(T60in(c)andT120in(d)).
Figure2.Flowchartoftheresearchdesign.
2.3.DataReduction
Kinematicdatawererecordedfrom3mbeforeandaftertheturningstep.Thequantitativegait
assessmentincludedbothtemporalandspatialparameters(Figure3),thatare:i)stancetime(s),theperiod
oftimewhenthefootisincontactwiththeground;ii)swingtime(s),theperiodoftimewhenthefootis
notincontactwiththeground;iii)stridetime(s),theintervaloftimetocompleteagaitstride;iv)cadence
(stridemin
1
),thenumberofstridestakeninaunitoftime;v)stridelength(m),thedistancefromtheinitial
contactofonefoottothefollowinginitialcontactofthesamefoot.Alltheseparameterswereobtainedusing
standarddefinitionsaccordingtoanalgorithmprogrammedinLabView(version10,NationalInstruments,
Austin,TX,USA).Walkingspeed(ms
1
)wasappreciatedasthedistancetravelledduringacompletestride
cycle.
Figure 2. Flow chart of the research design.
2.3. Data Reduction
Kinematic data were recorded from 3 m before and after the turning step. The quantitative gait
assessment included both temporal and spatial parameters (Figure 3), that are: (i) stance time (s),
the period of time when the foot is in contact with the ground; (ii) swing time (s), the period of time
when the foot is not in contact with the ground; (iii) stride time (s), the interval of time to complete a
gait stride; (iv) cadence (stride
·
min
1
), the number of strides taken in a unit of time; (v) stride length
(m), the distance from the initial contact of one foot to the following initial contact of the same foot. All
these parameters were obtained using standard definitions according to an algorithm programmed in
LabView (version 10, National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA). Walking speed (m
·
s
1
) was appreciated
as the distance travelled during a complete stride cycle.
Symmetry2020,12,xFORPEERREVIEW6of15
Figure3.Gaitparameterassessmentforwalking/turningconditions.
Theaveragevalueofalltheperformedstepsineachconditionhasbeenutilizedinfurtheranalyses.
Thenumberoferrors(i.e.,whentellingadaybackwardswentwrong)wascountedandcollectedbyan
operatorduringthewalkingtrialsaswell.Forcontrols,wepooledtogetherthedatafromtherightandleft
lowerlimbs(n=9),whereasforPDpatients,thedatareferringtothelessaffectedside(PDNA)andtothe
moreaffectedside(PDA)wereconsideredseparately(n=9).
Finally,wecalculatedthelocomotorrehabilitationindex“LRI”as:LRI=100*(selfselectedspeed
(SSWS)/optimalspeed(OWS)),wheretheselfselectedspeed(SSWS)hasbeendirectlymeasuredandthe
optimalspeed(OWS)hasbeenestimatedaccordingtoapreviousstudy[42].
2.4.DataAnalysis
ThedataanalysiswasconductedusingSPSS19.0(SPSSInc,Chicago,IL,USA).Descriptivestatistics
wereusedtocomputethemeansandstandarddeviationsfortheoutcomevariables.Allthespatiotemporal
gaitdatawerenormallydistributed(Kolmogorov–SmirnovandShapiro–Wilktests)anddidnotviolatethe
assumptionsofhomogeneity.Inordertotestthemainhypotheses,aseriesof2(tasks:singletaskvs.dual
task)×3(gaitdirections:WFvs.T60vs.T120)repeatedmeasuresANOVAs,withgroups(CR,PDNA,PDA)
asthebetweenfactors,wereusedtoanalyzethegaitdata.Whenasignificantmaineffectwasfound(critical
pvalue<0.05),aposthocttestwasperformed.ABonferronicorrectionwasappliedwhenneeded.
3.Results
3.1.CognitiveTask
FortheMMSE,controls(28.1±1.66)showedsimilarscorestopatients(27.2±1.85(p=ns)).
Themajorityofthedualtasktrailsweresuccessful.WhereasindividualswithPDperformedthe
cognitivetaskwith97.2±6%,94.9±8%and95.2±10%accuracyduringWF,T60andT120,respectively,
andcontrolsperformedthesametrailswithanaccuracyof96.7±5%,95.3±6%and94.9±9%.Regarding
Figure 3. Gait parameter assessment for walking/turning conditions.
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284 6 of 14
The average value of all the performed steps in each condition has been utilized in further analyses.
The number of errors (i.e., when telling a day backwards went wrong) was counted and collected by
an operator during the walking trials as well. For controls, we pooled together the data from the right
and left lower limbs (n=9), whereas for PD patients, the data referring to the less aected side (PDNA)
and to the more aected side (PDA) were considered separately (n=9).
Finally, we calculated the locomotor rehabilitation index “LRI” as: LRI =100 * (self-selected speed
(SSWS)/optimal speed (OWS)), where the self-selected speed (SSWS) has been directly measured and
the optimal speed (OWS) has been estimated according to a previous study [42].
2.4. Data Analysis
The data analysis was conducted using SPSS 19.0 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL, USA). Descriptive
statistics were used to compute the means and standard deviations for the outcome variables. All the
spatiotemporal gait data were normally distributed (Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Shapiro–Wilk tests)
and did not violate the assumptions of homogeneity. In order to test the main hypotheses, a series
of 2 (tasks: single task vs. dual task)
×
3 (gait directions: WF vs. T60 vs. T120) repeated measures
ANOVAs, with groups (CR, PDNA, PDA) as the between factors, were used to analyze the gait data.
When a significant main eect was found (critical pvalue <0.05), a post-hoc t-test was performed.
A Bonferroni correction was applied when needed.
3. Results
3.1. Cognitive Task
For the MMSE, controls (28.1 ±1.66) showed similar scores to patients (27.2 ±1.85 (p =ns)).
The majority of the dual task trails were successful. Whereas individuals with PD performed
the cognitive task with 97.2
±
6%, 94.9
±
8% and 95.2
±
10% accuracy during WF, T60 and T120,
respectively, and controls performed the same trails with an accuracy of 96.7
±
5%, 95.3
±
6% and
94.9
±
9%. Regarding the gait direction, more correct answers were given while walking forward as
compared to turning (p<0.01 for T60 and T120) similarly in both groups.
3.2. Spatiotemporal Parameters
There were no dierences between groups for age, body mass and height, or body mass index.
The average
±
SD values of the measured temporal and spatial variables have been reported in
Tables 2and 3, as well as the post-hoc results.
The stance time diered among tasks (F (1, 24) =34.456, p<0.001) and walking direction
(
F (2, 48) =27.234
,p<0.001), whereas there were no dierences between groups (F (2, 24) =0.262,
p=ns
). In particular, the dual task showed an increased stance time compared to the single one.
The highest stance phase was observed during turning at 120
, while the lowest was during walking in
the straight direction.
The swing phase showed a significant eect for direction (F (2, 48) =4.648, p<0.05), but not for
task (F (1, 24) =3.745, p =ns) and group (F (2, 24) =0.883, p =ns). Particularly, the 120
turn took a
longer swing time in comparison to the other directions.
The cycle time showed significant dierences among tasks (F (1, 24) =31.840, p<0.001) and
directions (F (2, 48) =34.227, p<0.001), whereas there were no group changes (F (2, 24) =0.618, p =ns).
Both groups increased the cycle time during the dual task condition, and the 120
turn took the longest
time compared with the other conditions.
Therefore, the step frequency and cadence changed significantly with regard to task
(
F (1, 24) =30.257
and 30.317, p<0.001) and direction (F (2, 48) =31.329 and 31.441, p<0.001),
but not with group (F (2, 24) =0.458 and 0.457, p =ns).
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284 7 of 14
Table 2. Gait temporal parameters at single and dual tasking in all directions. Data are means ±SD.
Stance Time (s) Swing Time (s)
Walking Direction CR PDNA PDA CR PDNA PDA
WF_ST 0.72 ±0.05 0.74 ±0.12 a0.74 ±0.18 c,l 0.33 ±0.03 0.33 ±0.04 0.33 ±0.02 a
T60_ST 0.73 ±0.07 a0.76 ±0.13 a,h,m 0.75 ±0.12 b,g 0.33 ±0.04 0.33 ±0.04 0.35 ±0.03
T120_ST 0.75 ±0.07 b0.78 ±0.14 b,h,m 0.78 ±0.14 c,l,g 0.33 ±0.04 0.35 ±0.03 0.35 ±0.03
WF_DT 0.75 ±0.07 m0.80 ±0.12 a,m 0.78 ±0.11 c,l 0.33 ±0.04 0.34 ±0.04 0.35 ±0.05 a
T60_DT 0.78 ±0.11 a0.84 ±0.14 a0.82 ±0.14 b0.33 ±0.03 0.34 ±0.04 0.35 ±0.04
T120_DT 0.81 ±0.11 b,m 0.85 ±0.14 b,m 0.83 ±0.13 c,l 0.34 ±0.04 0.35 ±0.05 0.37 ±0.06
Stride Time (s) Cadence (Stride·min1)
Walking Direction CR PDNA PDA CR PDNA PDA
WF_ST 1.05 ±0.08 a1.07 ±0.12 a,n 1.08 ±0.12 b,m 114.79 ±8.68 a113.55 ±12.27 c,n 112.74 ±11.91 b,d,m
T60_ST 1.06 ±0.11 a1.09 ±0.12 a,h 1.10 ±0.12 b113.77 ±10.01 a111.35 ±12.10 c,i 109.92 ±11.32 b,d
T120_ST 1.08 ±0.11 a1.14 ±0.14 c,n,h 1.13 ±0.13 a,m 112.16 ±9.96 b106.90 ±12.98 a,i,n 107.04 ±12.10 a,m
WF_DT 1.09 ±0.10 a,l 1.13 ±0.11 a,n 1.13 ±0.12 b,n 110.94 ±10.08 a,m 106.76 ±10.79 c,n 106.79 ±10.92 b,m
T60_DT 1.11 ±0.13 a1.18 ±0.13 a1.18 ±0.13 b108.88 ±12.57 a,g 102.94 ±11.55 c103.04 ±11.02 b
T120_DT 1.14 ±0.12 a,l 1.20 ±0.15 c,n 1.20 ±0.14 a,n 105.84 ±10.87 b,g,m 101.00 ±12.46 a,n 101.44 ±11.97 a,m
CR: control group; PDNA: Parkinson’s disease not aected side; PDA: Parkinson’s disease more aected side. WF: walking forward; T60: turning at 60
; T120: turning at 120
. ST: single
task; DT: dual task.
a
=p<0.05,
b
=p<0.01,
c
=p<0.001: significant dierence by comparing ST to DT.
d
=p<0.05: significant dierence by comparing the WF to T60.
g
=p<0.05;
h=p<0.01, i=p<0.001: significant dierence by comparing the T60 to T120. l=p<0.05, m=p<0.01, n=p<0.001: significant dierence by comparing the WF to T120.
Table 3. Step frequency, stride length and walking speed at single and dual tasking in all directions. Data are means ±SD.
Step Frequency (Hz) Stride Length (m) Walking Speed (m·s1)
Walking
Direction CR PDNA PDA CR PDNA PDA CR PDNA PDA
WF_ST 0.95 ±0.08 a0.95 ±0.10 c,n 0.94 ±0.10 b,m 1.34 ±0.10 a,n,#,1.23 ±0.14 b,n,#
1.24
±
0.14
c,n,1.28 ±0.12 c,m,#,1.17 ±0.21 b,f,n,#
1.17
±
0.21
b,e,n,
T60_ST 0.95 ±0.08 a0.93 ±0.10 c,h 0.92 ±0.10 b1.33 ±0.08 b,h,#,1.20 ±0.14 b,i,# 1.23 ±0.16 c,i,1.26 ±0.12 c,i,#,1.06 ±0.17 f,i,# 1.06 ±0.18 e,i,
T120_ST 0.93 ±0.08 b0.89 ±0.11 a,h,n 0.89 ±0.10 a,m 1.24 ±0.07 a,h,n,#,1.05 ±0.13 i,n,#
1.05
±
0.11
i,n,1.15 ±0.12 b,i,m,#,0.95 ±0.15 i,n,# 0.95 ±0.14 i,n,
WF_DT 0.92 ±0.08 a,m 0.89 ±0.09 c,n 0.89 ±0.09 b,m 1.27 ±0.06 a,n,#,1.14 ±0.12 b,n,#
1.15
±
0.13
c,n,1.18 ±0.10 c,n,#,
1.09
±
0.17
b,d,n,# 1.11 ±0.18 b,n,
T60_DT 0.91 ±0.10 a,g 0.86 ±0.10 c0.86 ±0.09 b1.26 ±0.07 b,i,#,1.14 ±0.15 b,i,# 1.14 ±0.15 c,i,1.14 ±0.15 c,i,#,1.03 ±0.18 d,i,# 1.06 ±0.17 i,
T120_DT 0.88 ±0.09 b,g,m 0.84 ±0.10 a,n 0.84 ±0.10 a,m 1.18 ±0.08 a,i,n,#,1.02 ±0.16 i,n,#
1.01
±
0.16
i,n,1.04 ±0.14 b,i,n,#,0.89 ±0.16 i,n,# 0.89 ±0.14 i,n,
CR: control group; PDNA: Parkinson’s disease not aected side; PDA: Parkinson’s disease more aected side. WF: walking forward; T60: turning at 60
; T120: turning at 120
. ST: single
task; DT: dual task.
a
=p<0.05,
b
=p<0.01,
c
=p<0.001: significant dierence by comparing ST to DT.
d
=p<0.05,
e
=p<0.01,
f
=p<0.001: significant dierence by comparing the WF
to T60.
g
=p<0.05;
h
=p<0.01,
i
=p<0.001: significant dierence by comparing the T60 to T120.
m
=p<0.01,
n
=p<0.001: significant dierence by comparing the WF to T120.
#=p<0.05: significant dierence by comparing the CR group to the PDNA group. =p<0.05: significant dierence by comparing the CR group to the PDA group.
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284 8 of 14
The stride length was aected by group (F (2, 24) =4.415, p<0.05), task (F (1, 24) =41.213,
p<0.001
) and direction (F (2, 48) =132.916, p<0.001). In particular, healthy controls walked with
longer strides when compared to patients, whereas no dierences were observed among the sides in
the pathological group. All groups reduced their stride lengths during the dual task condition, and the
120turn showed the lowest value compared with the other conditions.
Additionally, the walking velocity changed with group (F (2, 24) =2.764, p<0.05), task
(
F (1, 24) =21.818
,p<0.001) and direction (F (2, 48) =104.061, p<0.001). In general, controls
walked faster than patients, and the latter group showed similar values in both sides. Furthermore, the
larger the turning the lower the velocity: the 120
turn showed the lowest velocity value compared to
other directions.
The calculated LRI index significantly changed with group (F (2, 24) =4.672, p<0.05), task
(
F (1, 24) =14.712
,p<0.001) and direction (F (2, 48) =70.937, p<0.001). Because of the lack of a
significant dierence, the data of the PDNA and PDA were pooled together. Our data confirmed that
patients walked with a lower LRI compared to controls in all the investigated conditions because of
their reduced walking speed, and such a pattern is more pronounced at turning 120(Figure 4).
Symmetry2020,12,xFORPEERREVIEW9of15
Thestancetimedifferedamongtasks(F(1,24)=34.456,p<0.001)andwalkingdirection(F(2,
48)=27.234,p<0.001),whereastherewerenodifferencesbetweengroups(F(2,24)=0.262,p=ns).
Inparticular,thedualtaskshowedanincreasedstancetimecomparedtothesingleone.Thehighest
stancephasewasobservedduringturningat120°,whilethelowestwasduringwalkinginthe
straightdirection.
Theswingphaseshowedasignificanteffectfordirection(F(2,48)=4.648,p<0.05),butnotfor
task(F(1,24)=3.745,p=ns)andgroup(F(2,24)=0.883,p=ns).Particularly,the12turntooka
longerswingtimeincomparisontotheotherdirections.
Thecycletimeshowedsignificantdifferencesamongtasks(F(1,24)=31.840,p<0.001)and
directions(F(2,48)=34.227,p<0.001),whereastherewerenogroupchanges(F(2,24)=0.618,p=
ns).Bothgroupsincreasedthecycletimeduringthedualtaskcondition,andthe12turntookthe
longesttimecomparedwiththeotherconditions.
Therefore,thestepfrequencyandcadencechangedsignificantlywithregardtotask(F(1,24)=
30.257and30.317,p<0.001)anddirection(F(2,48)=31.329and31.441,p<0.001),butnotwithgroup
(F(2,24)=0.458and0.457,p=ns).
Thestridelengthwasaffectedbygroup(F(2,24)=4.415,p<0.05),task(F(1,24)=41.213,p<
0.001)anddirection(F(2,48)=132.916,p<0.001).Inparticular,healthycontrolswalkedwithlonger
strideswhencomparedtopatients,whereasnodifferenceswereobservedamongthesidesinthe
pathologicalgroup.Allgroupsreducedtheirstridelengthsduringthedualtaskcondition,andthe
12turnshowedthelowestvaluecomparedwiththeotherconditions.
Additionally,thewalkingvelocitychangedwithgroup(F(2,24)=2.764,p<0.05),task(F(1,24)
=21.818,p<0.001)anddirection(F(2,48)=104.061,p<0.001).Ingeneral,controlswalkedfasterthan
patients,andthelattergroupshowedsimilarvaluesinbothsides.Furthermore,thelargertheturning
thelowerthevelocity:the12turnshowedthelowestvelocityvaluecomparedtootherdirections.
ThecalculatedLRIindexsignificantlychangedwithgroup(F(2,24)=4.672,p<0.05),task(F(1,
24)=14.712,p<0.001)anddirection(F(2,48)=70.937,p<0.001).Becauseofthelackofasignificant
difference,thedataofthePDNAandPDAwerepooledtogether.Ourdataconfirmedthatpatients
walkedwithalowerLRIcomparedtocontrolsinalltheinvestigatedconditionsbecauseoftheir
reducedwalkingspeed,andsuchapatternismorepronouncedatturning12(Figure4).
Figure4.Comparisonoflocomotorrehabilitationindexvaluesbetweenthecontrolgroup(whitebars)
andthepathologicalgroup(blackbars),fortheforwarddirection(WF),turning60°(T60)andturning
120°(T120),inbothsingle(ST)anddual(DT)tasks.Averagevalues±SDhavebeenreported;dataof
thePDNAandPDAwerepooledtogether.Asignificantposthoctesthasbeenreported:*=p<0.05
bycomparingthecontrolgrouptothepathologicalone;c=p<0.001:significantdifferenceby
Figure 4.
Comparison of locomotor rehabilitation index values between the control group (white bars)
and the pathological group (black bars), for the forward direction (WF), turning 60
(T60) and turning
120
(T120), in both single (ST) and dual (DT) tasks. Average values
±
SD have been reported; data of
the PDNA and PDA were pooled together. A significant post-hoc test has been reported:
*=p<0.05
by
comparing the control group to the pathological one; c =p<0.001: significant dierence by comparing
the ST to DT; f =p<0.001: significant dierence by comparing WF to T60; i =p<0.001: significant
dierence by comparing T60 to T120; n=p<0.001: significant dierence by comparing WF to T120
(same legend as in Tables 2and 3).
Finally, iso-velocity curves were calculated for each group and condition to understand the
complex interaction between stride length and frequency in determining the walk velocity (Figure 5).
During the straight gait (Figure 5, panel a), in single tasking, the patients with PD used the same
step frequency compared to controls, but they walked with a shorter stride length. As a consequence,
their walking velocities were lower. During the dual task, all curves moved down and to the left:
this implied a decrease in the walking speed for controls and patients, and the ratio between the stride
length and step frequency was the same.
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284 9 of 14
Figure 5.
Stride length is plotted as a function of the step frequency (i.e., the iso-velocity curves):
(
a
) walking forward (WF); (
b
) turning at 60
(T60); (
c
) turning at 120
(T120). The symbols refer to
dierent groups as follows: CRs in the single task
; CRs in the dual task
#
; the PDNA in the single
task
; PDNA in the dual task
; PDA in the single task
N
; PDA in the dual task
. For PDNA sometimes
the symbols are not seen because they exactly coincide with the data of the other two groups.
This trend occurs similarly in turning (Figure 5, panels b and c), independently of group and task.
Importantly, the most significant reduction in the walking velocity occurred while turning 120
and
confirms that a larger angle of turning leads to a lower walking velocity.
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284 10 of 14
4. Discussion
The present study focused on the eects of dual task and turning on the kinematic parameters in
mild to moderate Parkinson’s disease. We hypothesized that the simple cognitive and mechanical
tasks will display similar eects in both populations, while the combination of these two stimuli will
show a higher impact on patients. We found that: (i) the temporal walking parameters were aected by
the mental task, as well as by the mechanical demand (turning), but no significant dierences among
populations were observed. On the contrary, the stride length and walking speed were lower in PD
patients compared to controls; (ii) the turning task had the capability to alter the walking parameters,
especially in people with PD, and the major changes in the walking strategy have been observed while
turning at a larger angle (120
); (iii) the combination of the cognitive and the mechanical task was
challenging for patients. Indeed, their stride length and walking velocity showed significant alterations
compared to controls in all walking conditions; finally, (iv) no significant dierences were observed
between the not aected/more aected side in all the investigated parameters, suggesting an equal
symmetry between the right and left body side.
Taking them together, our results highlighted that a simple mental task alone is not sizeable
enough to alter the walking strategy in patients with PD with mild to moderate impairments, whereas
the combination of this cognitive task with a change of direction has the capability to modify the
walking strategy, especially with a higher turning angle (120).
4.1. Eects on Gait Variables: A Task Comparison
In single tasking, our data confirmed that patients with mild to moderate PD walked with a
reduced gait velocity and a shorter step length compared to controls. Therefore, patients showed a
lower locomotor index rehabilitation if compared to controls with no evident dierences between
the not aected and more aected side. This finding suggests that the mild to moderate pathology
compromises, in a similar way, the gait kinematics of the inferior limbs (e.g., it did not compromise the
symmetry among body sides). Since, postural control and gait are linked to cognitive function both in
healthy and pathological subjects [
18
,
22
,
42
], it is possible to assume that PD patients reduced their
stride length in order to increase the time spent on the ground, increasing the walking stability.
As we hypothesized, a simple dual task condition (repeating the days of the week backwards)
aected both groups similarly: all participants increased the ground contact (and the cycle time) and
decreased their cadence and frequency [
12
,
19
]. This finding endorses that subjects tried to maintain their
postural stability by spending more time on the ground, as this would prevent the risk of falling [
18
].
The lack of a dierence among groups confirms that, by adding a cognitive load, a low disease
severity could not play a major role in determining motor impairments [
23
]. In addition, the high
focus on the additional task means a larger proportion of the attentional capacity is at the expense of
walking performance: people walked even more slowly with much shorter steps [
7
,
12
,
15
,
22
,
43
46
].
This “compensatory strategy” could be useful in achieving a greater control of gait and balance
disruption [20], and in counterbalancing the fluctuations of the center of mass.
Finally, the similar ratio between the stride length and step frequency means that subjects did not
change their stride pattern. This latter outcome suggests that the simple cognitive task was probably
not too demanding for all participants. More complex cognitive tasks (i.e., concurrent loads, mental
tracking) would probably point out the gap among groups [19,20].
4.2. Eects on Gait Variables: A Direction Comparison
In the present study, we investigated two turning angles that are very common during daily
activity [38]: the first (60) represents a simple turn and the other (120) a more dicult turn.
Our spatiotemporal data are in line with the previous literature [
8
,
10
,
18
,
21
]. With respect to
directions, whereas healthy participants were impacted by the 120
turn, PD patients were also aected
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284 11 of 14
by the 60
turn, but only for the walking velocity. Additionally, in this case, no appreciable dierences
between the not aected and more aected side were observed.
Generally, the 120
turning is characterized by an increased support time and a reduced number
of strides (i.e., cadence) and frequency, as well as a decreased speed and length. This angle requires
additional attentional resources: in fact, it relies more heavily on proprioception (i.e., directed by the
basal ganglia function) than both forward walking and the 60
turn [
47
]. By considering the mechanical
approach, the impact of a larger turning could be explained by the kinetics of the movement. Indeed,
to move into a larger angle of turn, the subjects decreased their speed more (just for a greater angle of
turn) and, probably, used their turn foot as “pivot foot”. When the turn angle increased, the ankle
plantar flexion moment (and its peak) increased and the external rotators of the lower extremity played
a much greater role than in straight walking [
48
50
]. These alterations of the plantar flexors have
been accomplished with a reduced ankle power generated in the pre-swing phase (push-opower).
Therefore, the larger the angle of turn the higher the mechanical demand. As a result, PD patients
tended to reduce the step length and to increase the contact time primarily to improve the stability of
the body. They showed an initial alteration of the walking pattern at a 60
turn, but only the walking
velocity was aected. On the other hand, a 120
turn showed evident dierences when compared to
the straight direction in terms of both the temporal and spatial parameters; this finding supports the
idea of a more challenging stimulus.
Therefore, whereas the 60
turn could represent a not suitable training stimulus, the 120
may be
a good challenge for people with PD. As expected, all gait alterations are exacerbated during walking
with a combination of the two stimuli, especially in PD patients.
4.3. Eects on Gait Variables: Cognitive Task and Mechanical Task
The combined eects of dual (cognitive) and mechanical (turn) tasks represent an important
training stimulus in people with PD. Indeed, our data shows that the concomitant presence of a simple
mental and turning (60
) task produced only a marginal eect on the main kinematics. In particular,
the timing parameters showed no significant dierences compared with the straight line, and the
dierences between the single and dual task are comparable to the ones obtained for healthy subjects.
Furthermore, even the step length and the step frequency showed a similar trend.
On the contrary, the matching between the same mental task and a more complex mechanical
demand (120
turn) played a greater “destructive” impact especially in patients with PD. Indeed, their
temporal and spatial variables showed a much more marked gap than the controls’ ones.
Our results were based on a relatively small heterogeneous sample of disabled patients, who
lived independently in the community. Therefore, further researches are necessary to extend these
findings to a larger sample (i.e., patients with more severe gait deficits or episodes of freezing), or to
other conditions (i.e., OFF phase performance).
5. Conclusions
Our data showed that a mechanical task (i.e., turning) has the potential to modify gait strategy in
people with Parkinson’s disease, without changes in symmetry of the lower limbs. Of greatest interest,
the concomitant presence of a mechanical task and a simple cognitive task did not produce a further
impairment of this gait strategy. Therefore, using the investigated combined condition (turning and
repeating the days of the week backwards) could represent a significant training stimulus in such
patients. Indeed, the improvement of the mental and physical characteristics is very important in
improving the functionality of patients at the early stages of their pathology.
Author Contributions:
Conceptualization, F.N., F.B., A.M., M.B.; methodology, F.N., A.M.; software, F.N., E.B.;
data curation, F.N., E.B., A.M.; writing—original draft preparation, F.N., A.M., M.B.; writing—review and editing,
F.N., A.M., M.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research received no external funding.
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284 12 of 14
Acknowledgments:
We would like to thank Alessandro Corsi, Gianluca Fedel and Davide Nisi for their help in
data collection, and the subjects for participating in the study.
Conflicts of Interest:
The authors acknowledge that there are no conflicts of interest pertaining to this manuscript.
References
1.
De Lau, L.M.; Breteler, M.M. Epidemiology of Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol.
2016
,5, 525–535. [CrossRef]
2.
Kelly, V.E.; Eusterbrock, A.J.; Shumway-Cook, A. A review of dual-task walking deficits in people with
Parkinson’s disease: Motor and cognitive contributions, mechanisms, and clinical implications. Parkinsonism
Disord. 2012,2012, 918719. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
3.
Cummings, J.L. Intellectual impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Clinical, pathological, and biochemical
correlates. J. Ger. Psychiatry Neurol. 1988,1, 24–36. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
4.
Creaby, M.W.; Cole, M.H. Gait characteristics and falls in Parkinson’s disease: A systematic review and
meta-analysis. Parkinsonism Related Disord. 2018,57, 1–8. [CrossRef]
5.
Kelly, V.E.; Eusterbrock, A.J.; Shumway-Cook, A. The eects of instructions on dual-task walking and
cognitive task performance in people with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Dis. 2012. [CrossRef]
6.
Nocera, J.R.; Roemmich, R.; Elrod, J.; Atlmann, L.J.P.; Hass, C.J. Eects of cognitive task on gait initiation in
Parkinson disease: Evidence of motor prioritization? J. Rehabil. Res. Develop. 2013,50, 699–708. [CrossRef]
7.
Rochester, L.; Galna, B.; Lord, S.; Burn, D. The nature of dual-task interference during gait in incident
Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience 2014,265, 83–94. [CrossRef]
8.
Morris, M.E.; Iansek, R.; Matyas, T.A.; Summers, J.J. Stride length regulation in Parkinson’s disease:
Normalization strategies and underlying mechanisms. Brain 1996,119, 551–568. [CrossRef]
9.
Morris, M.E.; Huxham, F.; McGinley, J.; Dodd, K.; Iansek, R. The biomechanics and motor control of gait in
Parkinson disease. Clin. Biomech. 2001,16, 459–470. [CrossRef]
10.
Canning, C.G. The eect of directing attention during walking under dual task conditions in Parkinson’s
disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 2005,11, 95–99. [CrossRef]
11.
Plotnik, M.; Dagan, Y.; Gurevich, T.; Giladi, N.; Hausdor, J.M. Eects of cognitive function on gait and dual
tasking abilities in patients with Parkinson’s disease suering from motor response fluctuations. Exp. Brain
Res. 2011,208, 169–179. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
12.
O’Shea, S.; Morris, M.E.; Iansek, R. Dual task interference during gait in people with Parkinson disease:
Eects of motor versus cognitive secondary tasks. Phys. Ther. 2002,82, 888–897. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
13.
Rochester, L.; Hetherington, V.; Jones, D.; Nieuwboer, A.; Willems, A.; Kwakkel, G.; van Wegen, E. Attending
to the task: Interference eects of functional tasks on walking in Parkinson’s disease and the roles of cognition,
depression, fatigue and balance. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehab. 2004,85, 1578–1585. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
14.
Brauer, S.G.; Morris, M.E. Can people with Parkinson’s disease improve dual tasking when walking? Gait
Posture 2010,31, 229–233. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
15.
Brown, L.A.; de Bruin, N.; Doan, J.B.; Suchowersky, O.; Hu, B. Novel challenges to gait in Parkinson’s disease:
The eect of concurrent music in single and dual task contexts. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehab.
2009
,90, 1578–1583.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
16.
Salazar, R.D.; Ren, X.; Ellis, T.; Toraif, N.; Barthelemy, O.J.; Neargarder, S.; Cronin-Golomb, A. Dual tasking in
Parkinson’s disease: Cognitive consequences while walking. Neuropsychology
2016
,31, 613–623. [CrossRef]
17.
Campbell, C.M.; Rowse, J.L.; Ciol, M.A.; Shumway-Cook, A. The eect of cognitive demand on timed up and
Go performance in older adults with and without Parkinson disease. Neurol. Rep.
2003
,27, 2–7. [CrossRef]
18.
Camicioli, R.; Oken, B.S.; Sexton, G.; Kaye, J.A.; Nutt, J.G. Verbal fluency task aects gait in Parkinson’s
disease with motor freezing. J. Geriatr. Psych. Neurol. 1998,11, 181–185. [CrossRef]
19.
Galletly, R.; Brauer, S.G. Does the type of concurrent task aect preferred and cued gait in people with
Parkinson’s disease. Austr. J. Physiother. 2005,51, 175–180. [CrossRef]
20.
Lapointe, L.L.; Stierwalt, J.A.G.; Maitland, C.G. Talking with walking: Cognitive loading and injurious falls
in Parkinson’s disease. Intern. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2010,12, 445–459. [CrossRef]
21.
Stegemoller, E.L.; Wilson, J.P.; Hazamy, A.; Shelly, M.C.; Okun, M.S.; Altmann, L.P.; Hass, C.J. Associations
between cognitive and gait performance during single- and dual-task walking in people with Parkinson
disease. Phys. Ther. 2014,94, 757–766. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284 13 of 14
22.
Yogev, G.; Giladi, N.; Peretz, C.; Springer, S.; Simon, E.S.; Hausdor, J.M. Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and
Parkinson’s disease: Which aspects of gait are attention demanding? Eur. J. Neurosci.
2005
,22, 1248–1256.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
23.
Wild, L.B.; de Lima, D.B.; Balardin, J.B.; Rizzi, L.; Giacobbo, B.L.; Oliveira, H.B.; de Lima, A.;
Peyre-Tartaruga, L.A.; Rieder, C.R.; Bromberg, E. Characterization of cognitive and motor performance
during dual-tasking in healthy older adults and patients with Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol.
2013
,260,
580–589. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
24.
Hong, M.; Perlmutter, J.S.; Earhart, G.M. A kinematic and electromyographic analysis of turning in people
with Parkinson disease. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 2009,23, 166–176. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
25.
Stack, E.; Jupp, K.; Ashburn, A. Developing methods to evaluate how people with Parkinson’s disease turn
180: An activity frequently associated with falls. Disab. Rehabil. 2004,26, 478–484. [CrossRef]
26.
Stack, E.L.; Ashburn, A.M.; Jupp, K.E. Strategies used by people with Parkinson’s disease who report
diculty turning. Parkinson. Relat. Disord. 2006,12, 87–92. [CrossRef]
27.
Adamson, M.B.; Gilmore, G.; Stratton, T.W.; Baktash, N.; Jog, M.S. Medication status and dual tasking on
turning strategies in Parkinson disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 2019,396, 206–212. [CrossRef]
28.
Chou, P.; Lee, S. Turning deficits in people with Parkinson’s disease. Tzu Chi Med. J.
2013
,25, 200–202.
[CrossRef]
29.
Vaugoyeau, M.; Viallet, F.; Mesure, S.; Massion, J. Coordination of axial rotation and step execution: Deficits
in Parkinson’s disease. Gait Posture 2003,18, 150–157. [CrossRef]
30.
Crenna, P.; Carpinella, I.; Rabuetti, M.; Calabrese, E.; Mazzoleni, P.; Nemni, R.; Ferrarin, M. The association
between impaired turning and normal straight walking in Parkinson’s disease. Gait Posture
2007
,26, 172–178.
[CrossRef]
31.
Willems, A.M.; Nieuwboer, A.; Chavret, F.; Desloovere, K.; Dom, R.; Rochester, L.; Kwakkel, G.; van Wegen, E.;
Jones, D. Turning in Parkinson’s disease patients and controls: The eect of auditory cues. Mov. Disord.
2007
,
22, 1871–1878. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
32.
Mellone, S.; Mancini, M.; King, L.A.; Horak, F.B.; Chiari, L. The quality of turning in Parkinson’s disease: A
compensatory strategy to prevent postural instability? J. Neuroeng Rehabil.
2016
,13, 39. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
33.
Bertoli, M.; Della Croce, U.; Cereatti, A.; Mancini, M. Objective measures to investigate turning impairments
and freezing of gait in people with Parkinson’s disease. Gait Posture
2019
,74, 187–193. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
34.
Encarna Mic
ó
-Amigo, M.; Kingma, I.; Heinzel, S.; Nussbaum, S.; Heger, T.; Lummel, R.C.V.; Dieën, J.H.V.
Dual vs. Single Tasking during Circular Walking: What Better Reflects Progression in Parkinson’s Disease?
Front. Neurol. 2019. [CrossRef]
35.
Spildooren, J.; Vercruysse, S.; Desloovere, K. Freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: The impact of
dual-tasking and turning. Mov. Disord. 2010,25, 2563–2570. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
36.
Bayot, M.; Dujardin, K.; Tard, C.; Defebvre, L.; Bonnet, C.T.; Allart, E.; Delval, A. The interaction between
cognition and motor control: A theoretical framework for dual-task interference eects on posture, gait
initiation, gait and turning. Clin. Neurophys. 2018,48, 361–375. [CrossRef]
37.
Hackney, M.E.; Earhart, G.M. The eects of a secondary task on forward and backward walking in Parkinson
disease. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 2010,24, 97–106. [CrossRef]
38.
Patla, A.E.; Prentice, S.D.; Robinson, C.; Neufeld, J. Visual control of locomotion: Strategies for changing
direction and for going over obstacles. J. Exp. Psychol. 1991,17, 603–634. [CrossRef]
39.
Hughes, A.J.; Daniel, S.E.; Kilford, L.; Lees, A.J. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s
disease: A clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry
1992
,55, 181–184.
[CrossRef]
40.
Schenkman, M.L.; Clark, K.; Xie, T.; Kuchibhatla, M.; Shinberg, M.; Ray, L. Spinal movement and performance
of a standing reach task in participants with and without Parkinson disease. Phys. Ther.
2001
,81, 1400–1411.
[CrossRef]
41.
Movement Disorder Society Task Force on Rating Scales for Parkinson’s Disease. The Unified Parkinson’s
Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS): Status and recommendations. Mov. Disord.
2003
,18, 738–750. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
42.
Peyre-Tartaruga, L.A.; Monteiro, E.P. A new integrative approach to evaluate pathological gait: Locomotor
rehabilitation index. Clin. Transl. Degener. Dis. 2016,1, 86–90. [CrossRef]
Symmetry 2020,12, 1284 14 of 14
43.
Woollacott, M.; Shumway-Cook, A. Attention and the control of posture and gait: A review of an emerging
area of research. Gait Posture 2002,16, 1–14. [CrossRef]
44.
Plotnik, M.; Giladi, N.; Hausdor, J.M. Bilateral coordination of gait and Parkinson’s disease: The eects of
dual tasking. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psych. 2009,80, 347–350. [CrossRef]
45.
Hausdor, J.M.; Balash, J.; Giladi, N. Eects of cognitive challenge on gait variability in patients with
Parkinson’s disease. J. Geriatr. Psych. Neurol. 2003,16, 53–58. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
46.
De Melo Roiz, R.; Avezedo Cacho, E.W.; Macedo Pazinatto, M.; Guimaraes Reis, J.; Cliquet, A.;
Barasnevicius-Quagliato, E.M.A. Gait analysis comparing Parkinson’s disease with healthy elderly subjects.
Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 2010,68, 81–86. [CrossRef]
47.
Sofuwa, O.; Nieuwboer, A.; Desloovere, K.; Willems, A.M.; Chavret, F.; Jonkers, I. Quantitative gait analysis
in Parkinson’s disease: Comparison with a healthy control group. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil.
2005
,86,
1007–1013. [CrossRef]
48.
Taylor, M.J.D.; Dadbnichki, P.; Strike, S.C. A three-dimensional biomechanical comparison between turning
strategies during the stance phase of walking. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2005,24, 558–573. [CrossRef]
49.
Segal, A.; Orendur, M.; Flick, K.; Berge, J.; Klute, G. Ankle biomechanics during a spin turn. In Proceedings
of the 28th Annual Meeting of the American Society of Biomechanics, Boulder, CO, USA, 15 July 2004; p. 168.
50.
Xu, D.; Chow, J.W.; Wang, Y.T. Eects of turn angle and pivot foot on lower extremity kinetics during walk
and turn actions. J Appl. Biomech. 2006,22, 74–79. [CrossRef]
©
2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
... As the disease progresses, other autonomic, motor and non-motor symptoms may develop, resulting in the reduction of the functional capacity of these individuals (Mirelman et al., 2019). Within three years of a PD diagnosis, about 85% of people develop gait deterioration (Nardello et al., 2020). These motor dysfunctions can be exacerbated under various conditions, such as starting or stopping walking, changing direction and/or crossing obstacles (Spildooren et al., 2019). ...
... Gait disturbance is a key component of motor impairment in parkinsonian patients. Therefore, it is not surprising that a large body of experimental work was aimed at describing and monitoring locomotion abnormalities in PD, most often through automatic motion analysis techniques [10,14,24,[26][27][28][44][45][46][47][48]. ...
Article
Full-text available
It is important to find objective biomarkers for evaluating gait in Parkinson’s Disease (PD), especially related to the foot and lower leg segments. Foot-switch signals, analyzed through Statistical Gait Analysis (SGA), allow the foot-floor contact sequence to be characterized during a walking session lasting five-minutes, which includes turnings. Gait parameters were compared between 20 PD patients and 20 age-matched controls. PDs showed similar straight-line speed, cadence, and double-support compared to controls, as well as typical gait-phase durations, except for a small decrease in the flat-foot contact duration (−4% of the gait cycle, p = 0.04). However, they showed a significant increase in atypical gait cycles (+42%, p = 0.006), during both walking straight and turning. A forefoot strike, instead of a “normal” heel strike, characterized the large majority of PD’s atypical cycles, whose total percentage was 25.4% on the most-affected and 15.5% on the least-affected side. Moreover, we found a strong correlation between the atypical cycles and the motor clinical score UPDRS-III (r = 0.91, p = 0.002), in the subset of PD patients showing an abnormal number of atypical cycles, while we found a moderate correlation (r = 0.60, p = 0.005), considering the whole PD population. Atypical cycles have proved to be a valid biomarker to quantify subtle gait dysfunctions in PD patients.
Article
Full-text available
Pisa syndrome is one of the possible postural deformities associated with Parkinson’s disease and it is clinically defined as a sustained lateral bending of the trunk. Some previous studies proposed clinical and biomechanical investigation to understand the pathophysiological mechanisms that occur, mainly focusing on EMG patterns and clinics. The current research deals with the assessment of a standardized biomechanical analysis to investigate the Pisa syndrome postural effects. Eight patients participated in the experimental test. Both static posture and gait trials were performed. An optoelectronic system and two force plates were used for data acquisition, while a custom multi-segments kinematic model of the human spine was used to evaluate the 3D angles. All subjects showed an important flexion of the trunk superior segment with respect to the inferior one, with a strong variability among patients (range values between 4.3° and 41.0°). Kinematics, ground reaction forces and spatio-temporal parameters are influenced by the asymmetrical trunk posture. Moreover, different proprioception, compensation and abilities of correction were depicted among subjects. Considering the forces exchanged by the feet with the floor during standing, results highlighted a significant asymmetry (p-value = 0.02) between the omo and contralateral side in a normal static posture, with greater load distribution on the same side of lateral deviation. When asked to self-correct the posture, all patients demonstrated a reduction of asymmetry, but without stressing any statistical significance. All these aspects might be crucial for the definition of a PS patients’ classification and for the assessment of the efficacy of treatments and rehabilitation.
Article
Full-text available
Background and Aim: Reliable, valid and sensitive measures of dual-task-associated impairments in patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) may reveal progressive deficits unnoticed under single-task walking. The aim of this study was to quantitatively identify markers of progressive gait deficits in idiopathic PD while walking over a circular trajectory condition in single-task walking and in different dual-task conditions: (1) circular walking while checking boxes on a paper sheet as fast as possible and (2) circular walking while performing subtraction of 7 as fast as possible. In addition, we aimed to study the added value of dual-tasking assessment over single (circular) walking task assessment in the study of PD progression. Methods: The assessments were performed every 6 months over a (up to) 5 years period for 22 patients in early-stage PD, 27 patients in middle-stage PD and 25 healthy controls (HC). Longitudinal changes of 27 gait features extracted from accelerometry were compared between PD groups and HCs using generalized estimating equations analysis, accounting for gait speed, age, and levodopa medication state confounders when required. In addition, dual-task-interference with gait and cognitive performance was assessed, as well as their combination. Results: The results support the validity and robustness of some of the gait features already identified in our previous work as progression markers of the disease in single-task circular walking. However, fewer gait features from dual-task than from single-task assessments were identified as markers of progression in PD. Moreover, we did not clearly identify progressive worsening of dual-task-interference in patients with PD, although some group differences between early and middle stages of PD vs. the control group were observed for dual-task interference with the gait task and with the concurrent tasks. Conclusions: Overall, the results showed that dual-tasking did not have added value in the study of PD progression from circular gait assessments. Our analyses suggest that, while single-task walking might be sensitive enough, dual-tasking may introduce additional (error) variance to the data and may represent complex composite measures of cognitive and motor performance.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Given the high rate of falls during walking in people with idiopathic Parkinson's disease (PD), identifying at risk individuals and developing targeted interventions to reduce falls incidence is paramount. Numerous studies have investigated gait-related risk factors for falls in PD, however findings are inconsistent across studies, and thus a synthesis of the current evidence is needed to guide clinical practice and the development of interventions to reduce falls risk. The objective of this study was to systematically review the literature regarding the association between walking biomechanics and falls in people with PD, and where possible, perform meta-analyses. Methods: The study was performed in accordance with the PRISMA guidelines. Databases were searched until January 2018 to identify articles that reported on the association between walking biomechanics and prospective or retrospective falls in people with PD. Results: Twenty-six articles were included (15 prospective studies, 11 retrospective studies). Articles reported on spatiotemporal and kinematic characteristics, and muscle activation patterns. Meta-analyses revealed slower walking speed, lower cadence, shorter strides and more mediolateral head and pelvis motion in those at higher risk of future falls. Findings from prospective and retrospective articles were largely consistent. Conclusion: Our findings identify spatiotemporal and kinematic characteristics of gait that are risk factors for falls in PD. Modification of these characteristics may have the potential to mediate falls risk, and future research to investigate this possibility is merited. The influence of body and ground reaction forces, and muscle activation patterns on falls risk in PD is currently under-researched.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Cognitive deficits are common in Parkinson’s disease (PD) and exacerbate the functional limitations imposed by PD’s hallmark motor symptoms, including impairments in walking. Though much research has addressed the effect of dual cognitive-locomotor tasks on walking, less is known about their effect on cognition. The purpose of this study was to investigate the relation between gait and executive function, with the hypothesis that dual tasking would exacerbate cognitive vulnerabilities in PD as well as being associated with gait disturbances. Method: Nineteen individuals with mild–moderate PD without dementia and 13 age- and education-matched normal control adults (NC) participated. Executive function (set-shifting) and walking were assessed singly and during dual tasking. Results: Dual tasking had a significant effect on cognition (reduced set-shifting) and on walking (speed, stride length) for both PD and NC, and also on stride frequency for PD only. The impact of dual tasking on walking speed and stride frequency was significantly greater for PD than NC. Though the group by condition interaction was not significant, PD had fewer set-shifts than NC on dual task. Further, relative to NC, PD showed significantly greater variability in cognitive performance under dual tasking, whereas variability in motor performance remained unaffected by dual tasking. Conclusions: Dual tasking had a significantly greater effect in PD than in NC on cognition as well as on walking. The results suggest that assessment and treatment of PD should consider the cognitive as well as the gait components of PD-related deficits under dual-task conditions.
Article
Full-text available
This article reviews the concept of locomotor rehabilitation index (LRI) from the principle of dynamical similarities and the theory of mechanism minimizing the energy expenditure in pathological walking. This index is defined as the percentage ratio between self-selected speed and optimum speed (algebraically LRI = 100 × self-selected speed/optimum walking speed). First, we analyze the mechanical foundations of human walking especially focusing on general size effects. Then, we discuss the descriptive physiology of pendular mechanism, evidencing the path that leads to the view of reductionist and extremely descriptive view of pathological gait. Integrative models, generated by the first evidence presented in our previous papers around the LRI, represent a crucial change of perspective. This model is discussed in details and criticized concerning the ensuing experimental findings. Finally, we discuss the case of Parkinson′s disease using the Nordic walking as a neat example of application of LRI on pathological locomotion. To conclude , the concept of LRI is reinforced by the substantial evidence, showing that this new proposal for assessing the gait functionality is extremely promising and should be stimulated in studies that examine the effects of therapies on gait functionality in degenerative diseases.
Article
Full-text available
Background The ability to turn while walking is essential for daily living activities. Turning is slower and more steps are required to complete a turn in people with Parkinson’s disease (PD) compared to control subjects but it is unclear whether this altered strategy is pathological or compensatory. The aim of our study is to characterize the dynamics of postural stability during continuous series of turns while walking at various speeds in subjects with PD compared to control subjects. We hypothesize that people with PD slow their turns to compensate for impaired postural stability. Method Motion analysis was used to compare gait kinematics between 12 subjects with PD in their ON state and 19 control subjects while walking continuously on a route composed of short, straight paths interspersed with eleven right and left turns between 30 and 180°. We asked subjects to perform the route at three different speeds: preferred, faster, and slower. Features describing gait spatio-temporal parameters and turning characteristics were extracted from marker trajectories. In addition, to quantify dynamic stability during turns we calculated the distance between the lateral edge of the base of support and the body center of mass, as well as the extrapolated body center of mass. Results Subjects with PD had slower turns and did not widen the distance between their feet for turning, compared to control subjects. Subjects with PD tended to cut short their turns compared to control subjects, resulting in a shorter walking path. Dynamic stability was smaller in the PD, compared to the healthy group, particularly for fast turning angles of 90°. Conclusions The slower turning speeds and larger turning angles in people with PD might reflect a compensatory strategy to prevent dynamic postural instability given their narrow base of support.
Article
Full-text available
Background Cognitive impairments in Parkinson's disease (PD) manifest as deficits in speed of processing, working memory and executive function/attention abilities. The gait impairment in PD is well documented to include reduced velocity, shortened step lengths and increased step to step variability. However, there is a paucity of research examining the relationship between over-ground walking and cognitive performance in persons with PD.Objective We sought to examine the relationship between both the mean and variability of gait spatiotemporal parameters, and cognitive performance across a broad range of cognitive domains.DesignCross-sectional.Methods Thirty-five non-demented participants diagnosed with idiopathic PD completed a battery of 12 cognitive tests that yielded three orthogonal factors: processing speed, working memory, and executive function/attention. Participants completed ten trials of over-ground walking (single task walking), and five trials of over ground walking while counting backwards by threes (dual task walking).ResultsAll gait measures were impaired by the dual task. Cognitive processing speed correlated with stride length and walking speed. Executive function correlated with step width variability. There were no significant associations with working memory. Regression models relating speed of processing to gait spatiotemporal variables revealed that including dual task costs in the model significantly improved the fit of the model.LimitationsParticipants with PD were tested only in the on medication state.Conclusions Different characteristics of gait are related to distinct types of cognitive processing, which may be differentially affected by dual task walking due to the pathology of PD.
Article
Background Parkinson disease (PD) patients have turning impairments that may increase fall risk. Clinics lack specialized kinematic equipment used in gait and turn analysis and require a simple method to evaluate fall risk and advise patients in turning strategy selection. Objectives To enhance understanding of PD turning strategies and determine if turning can be assessed using a video-recording and categorization method, we compared 180-degree and 90-degree turns as a function of medication status and dual-tasking (DT). Methods 21 PD participants (H&Y stage 1–3) in PD-ON and PD-OFF medication states and 16 controls completed 180-degree and 90-degree turn-tasks with and without DT. Video-recordings of tasks permitted classification of 180-degree turns into Few-Step turns (FST) vs. Multi-Step turns (MST) and 90-degree turns into Step vs. Spin-turns. FST were further sub-classified into Twisting vs. Sideways turns and MST into Backward, Festination, Forward or Wheeling turns. Percentages of subtypes were analyzed across groups by task. Results IN 180-degree tasks, there was an effect of group: FST vs. MST F(2,55) = 9.578, p < .001. PD participants in the off-medication state (PD-OFF) produced significantly more MST with a larger number of different turning subtypes vs. controls or PD on medication (PD-ON). In 90-degree tasks, controls significantly increased their proportion of Step-turns while DT (p < .001), an adaptation not observed in PD-ON or PD-OFF. Conclusions PD turning impairments may stem from an inability to select a unified turning strategy and to adapt to the turning environment, which may be exacerbated in PD-OFF. Video-analysis may prove beneficial in predicting a clinical course for PD patients by revealing features of turning dysfunction.
Article
Studies of dual-tasks (i.e. situations during which an individual performs two tasks simultaneously) and the subsequent inter-task interference have shown that locomotion and posture involves motor and cognitive components. Dual-tasks therefore constitute a promising avenue for improving the diagnosis, prevention and management of falls or cognitive impairment in populations at risk. However, tackling these major public health concerns with dual-task interventions requires a better understanding of the mechanisms underlying dual-task interference. In this context, we review (i) the main dual-task theories proposed to date and (ii) the factors that can influence dual-task interference effects in healthy young individuals and might therefore explain the current lack of consensus on the mechanisms of dual-tasks. We also consider cognitive-motor dual-tasks in which the motor task is a less frequently studied transition movement (such as gait initiation or turning), rather than only the often-studied gait and posture tasks. In general, the review focuses on the behavioral effects of dual-tasking.
Article
The Movement Disorder Society Task Force for Rating Scales for Parkinson's Disease prepared a critique of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS). Strengths of the UPDRS include its wide utilization, its application across the clinical spectrum of PD, its nearly comprehensive coverage of motor symptoms, and its clinimetric properties, including reliability and validity. Weaknesses include several ambiguities in the written text, inadequate instructions for raters, some metric flaws, and the absence of screening questions on several important non-motor aspects of PD. The Task Force recommends that the MDS sponsor the development of a new version of the UPDRS and encourage efforts to establish its clinimetric properties, especially addressing the need to define a Minimal Clinically Relevant Difference and a Minimal Clinically Relevant Incremental Difference, as well as testing its correlation with the current UPDRS. If developed, the new scale should be culturally unbiased and be tested in different racial, gender, and age-groups. Future goals should include the definition of UPDRS scores with confidence intervals that correlate with clinically pertinent designations, "minimal," "mild," "moderate," and "severe" PD. Whereas the presence of non-motor components of PD can be identified with screening questions, a new version of the UPDRS should include an official appendix that includes other, more detailed, and optionally used scales to determine severity of these impairments.