ArticlePDF Available

Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying working memory encoding and retrieval in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Working memory (WM) impairments in ADHD have been consistently reported along with deficits in attentional control. Yet, it is not clear which specific WM processes are affected in this condition. A deficient coupling between attention and WM has been reported. Nevertheless, most studies focus on the capacity to retain information rather than on the attention-dependent stages of encoding and retrieval. The current study uses a visual short-term memory binding task, measuring both behavioral and electrophysiological responses to characterize WM encoding, binding and retrieval comparing ADHD and non-ADHD matched adolescents. ADHD exhibited poorer accuracy and larger reaction times than non-ADHD on all conditions but especially when a change across encoding and test displays occurred. Binding manipulation affected equally both groups. Encoding P3 was larger in the non-ADHD group. Retrieval P3 discriminated change only in the non-ADHD group. Binding-dependent ERP modulations did not reveal group differences. Encoding and retrieval P3 were significantly correlated only in non-ADHD. These results suggest that while binding processes seem to be intact in ADHD, attention-related encoding and retrieval processes are compromised, resulting in a failure in the prioritization of relevant information. This new evidence can also inform recent theories of binding in visual WM.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
1
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2020) 10:7771 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64678-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Neurocognitive mechanisms
underlying working memory
encoding and retrieval in Attention-
Decit/Hyperactivity Disorder
Rodrigo Ortega1,2, Vladimir pez3,4, Ximena Carrasco5, María Josena Escobar2,
Adolfo M. García
6,7,8,9, Mario A. Parra10,11 & Francisco Aboitiz4 ✉
Working memory (WM) impairments in ADHD have been consistently reported along with decits in
attentional control. Yet, it is not clear which specic WM processes are aected in this condition. A
decient coupling between attention and WM has been reported. Nevertheless, most studies focus
on the capacity to retain information rather than on the attention-dependent stages of encoding and
retrieval. The current study uses a visual short-term memory binding task, measuring both behavioral
and electrophysiological responses to characterize WM encoding, binding and retrieval comparing
ADHD and non-ADHD matched adolescents. ADHD exhibited poorer accuracy and larger reaction
times than non-ADHD on all conditions but especially when a change across encoding and test displays
occurred. Binding manipulation aected equally both groups. Encoding P3 was larger in the non-
ADHD group. Retrieval P3 discriminated change only in the non-ADHD group. Binding-dependent ERP
modulations did not reveal group dierences. Encoding and retrieval P3 were signicantly correlated
only in non-ADHD. These results suggest that while binding processes seem to be intact in ADHD,
attention-related encoding and retrieval processes are compromised, resulting in a failure in the
prioritization of relevant information. This new evidence can also inform recent theories of binding in
visual WM.
Attention-Decit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a highly prevalent neurodevelopmental disorder charac-
terized by attentional diculties, hyperactivity and impulsivity13. Nevertheless, attention is neither the only
cognitive process aected in this condition nor the most aected one. For instance, the search for specic impair-
ments in selective attention and orienting attention in ADHD has not yielded consistent results46. Among other
cognitive processes, frontal executive functions impairments are consistently reported in ADHD7,8. Moreover,
Working Memory (WM) impairment is considered a signicant cognitive feature dierentiating between ADHD
and non-ADHD children9.
e predominant theoretical model of WM is Baddeley’s multi-component model10,11. WM is dened as a
limited-capacity system responsible for encoding, retaining or maintaining, and manipulating cognitive rep-
resentations of stimuli. Such memory system encompasses independent phonological (PH) and visuospatial (VS)
subsystems, and a central executive (CE) component, responsible for the attentional control. A fourth compo-
nent, the episodic buer, was later added12. Other authors, like Cowan13 or Engle14, have proposed WM models
1Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile. 2Center for Social
and Cognitive Neuroscience (CSCN), Escuela de Psicología, Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez, Santiago, Chile. 3Escuela de
Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Ponticia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile. 4Laboratorio de
Neurociencias Cognitivas, Departamento de Psiquiatría, Centro Interdisciplinario de Neurociencias, Facultad de
Medicina, Ponticia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile. 5Servicio de Neurología y Psiquiatría, Hospital
de Niños Dr. Luis Calvo Mackenna, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile. 6Universidad de
San Andrés, Buenos Aires, Argentina. 7National Scientic and Technical Research Council (CONICET), Buenos Aires,
Argentina. 8Faculty of Education, National University of Cuyo, Mendoza, Argentina. 9Departamento de Lingüística y
Literatura, Facultad de Humanidades, Universidad de Santiago de Chile, Santiago, Chile. 10School of Psychological
Sciences and Health, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, UK. 11Facultad de psicología, Universidad Autónoma del
Caribe, Barranquilla, Colombia. e-mail: faboitiz@puc.cl
OPEN
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
2
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2020) 10:7771 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64678-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
that emphasize the predominant role of attention throughout WM stages. Attentional involvement in each mem-
ory stage has also been a matter of debate. It has been suggested that attention is necessary for encoding, updating
and retrieval but has a limited role during retention15.
In the experimental psychology literature, several studies have been reported which aimed to investigate the
extent to which binding surface features in visual WM is an automatic or an attentional demanding function1619.
e evidence gathered to date suggests that binding of features in visual WM requires no more attentional
resources than processing individual features. e consistency of these ndings across a thorough experimental
series, led Allan Baddeley to revise the WM model20. Such a revision tried to address the concept and function of
the episodic buer; a WM component wherein binding functions were thought to be carried out via the support
from attention. Despite the attractiveness of these studies, they focused on healthy samples of university students
who, although subjected to experimental manipulations of attention, may have enough available resources to cope
with attentional interference and still perform the task at a high level of accuracy. It would be highly desirable to
further investigate visual WM binding in individuals with attentional disorders, such as those diagnosed with
ADHD.
Traditionally, WM is considered part of the executive functions. Both WM and executive functions, have been
criticized due to their limited specication3,9. Nevertheless, processes like conict detection, detecting mismatch
from expectations, shiing or interrupting a response, and the eortful allocation and maintenance of attention
and working memory resources towards the attainment of a future goal appear to be compromised in ADHD9.
Metanalytic studies suggest that processes such as executive attention, working memory, along with decision
making factors like motivation and reward are central to understand the ADHD cognitive prole21. Reaction
times variability is also considered part of this cognitive prole3.
When children, adolescents and adults with ADHD are assessed, WM and other executive dysfunctions stand
out as the ones with the most reliable discriminative power21. Regarding the specic WM decit in ADHD both,
phonological and visuospatial components seem to be aected, being the task demands on the central executive
(CE) one of the key moderators to explain the results22,23. at is, the most sensitive WM tasks in ADHD are those
with high demands of CE component. For example, those that require the participants to remember stimuli and
later recall them in a dierent pattern than the originally presented, or those that require to compare a newly pre-
sented stimulus with a representation in WM and to update that representation. ADHD subtype (predominantly
Inattentive or Combined) seems to have no signicant eect on WM dysfunction, perhaps due to their shared
inattention symptomatology24,25.
e close relationship between selective attention and WM has long been considered a natural candidate to
explain WM impairments in ADHD26. ese impairments have been related to academic underachievement due
to poor acquisition of cognitive skills in children, which may also have a long-term impact in social development
and quality of life27,28. Unfortunately, despite the consensus about the relevance of WM decits in ADHD, the
precise mechanisms that aect ADHD performance in WM tasks are poorly understood29,30. Moreover, WM
training seems to have a limited benecial impact on ADHD, even when a signicant improvement in WM per-
formance is achieved31. is emphasizes the need to understand and empirically document the nature of the WM
impairment in ADHD in relation with other process such as attention, to improve the development of diagnostic
or intervention tools.
Electrophysiological measures such as event related potentials (ERP) are especially useful to study WM, as
they allow to dierentiate stages of encoding, retention and retrieval which cannot be directly inferred from
behavioral responses. Encoding and retrieval are systematically associated with P3-like ERP components32,33.
Interestingly, reduced P3 amplitude in ADHD has been described both in children34 and adults35. e retention
stage is commonly studied by means of contralateral delayed activity (CDA) which is sensitive to WM load and
capacity36. is ERP component is a negative slow deection usually detected at contralateral parietal sites that
exhibit larger amplitude (compared to the ipsilateral sites) as the number of items in WM increases37.
Change detection tasks (CDT) have proved to be a successful paradigm to specically explore attention and
WM1619,38. is task usually consists of the presentation of an array of stimuli for a short period of time (S1),
which must be kept in memory (retention period) until the presentation of a test stimuli array (S2), where the
subject must respond whether the test stimuli is the same or dierent (Trial Type). is design allows the eval-
uation of the three stages described for WM: encoding, retention and retrieval. In a recent study, Spronk et al.29
used a CDT to evaluate the impact of distractors on the retention capacity of WM, by comparing adolescents
and adults with ADHD and healthy controls. ey found that adolescents were more aected than adults by the
presence of distractors but found no dierences regarding encoding and retention between groups. However,
this study evaluated only up to the retention period and not later stages where the subject must contrast the tar-
get with the memory representation and generate a response. Post-retention is highly overlooked in most WM
and ADHD studies34,39. Nevertheless, ADHD diculties in working memory updating and retrieval have been
previously reported using a dierent WM task39. Additionally, previous results of our groups suggest that the use
of cognitive resources and particularly attentional resources in ADHD reects a dierential style more than a
decit pattern or a decient capacity1,2. Such types of functional impairments could well aect the use of WM in
the post-retention period more than the process of forming representations in WM. us, an accurate character-
ization of encoding and retrieval working memory stages in ADHD could be relevant to better understand WM
role in this condition.
In summary, consensus exists regarding a poor performance of subjects with ADHD in tasks that explore
WM functioning. Nonetheless, there is no clear evidence concerning which process or mechanism is actually
compromised. Moreover, whether this decit depends on encoding, retention, or retrieval processes (or some
combination of them) is still unknown. Likewise, the retrieval of the information from WM is also an important
and mostly overlooked stage that should be explored in ADHD.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
3
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2020) 10:7771 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64678-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Here we develop a novel approach aimed to explore functional indicators (behavioral and electrophysiolog-
ical) of the dierent WM stages to further our knowledge on their role in the ADHD related WM impairment.
Parra et al.40 designed a visual short-term memory binding task that allows studying binding and change detec-
tion controlling the potential inuence of the spatial location and spatial relations within the stimuli arrays. It
has allowed to describe specic patterns of WM impairment in other conditions (e.g. Alzheimer’s Disease)20,40,41.
In the present study we use a modied version of this experimental design that diers from previous studies
controlling two possible confounding aspects. First, it minimizes the possibility of linguistic rehearsal by using
no nameable polygons and non-primary colors as stimuli. Second, it allows controlling the use of spatial cues by
changing the spatial location of stimuli between S1 and S2 displays. WM binding is studied by contrasting blocks
in which all shapes are presented in black, so only the shape should be retained (Shape-Only) and others in which
the binding of shape and color is necessary to solve the task (Color-Shape binding).
Taking into account the ADHD performance on other related tasks, as well as evidence gleaned from the
experimental psychology literature, we expect in this study that impaired attention would impact on both encod-
ing and retrieval, resulting in a poorer overall performance in ADHD participants42,43. is would be especially
manifest when S2 stimuli are dierent from S1. e need for WM binding (color and shape) could impact overall
behavioral performance in all participants, but we expect no specic dierences regarding ADHD diagnosis. At
the electrophysiological level, we predict that encoding and retrieval impairment in ADHD will be associated
with a reduced amplitude of the corresponding P3 ERP components. Additionally, we expect that these electro-
physiological markers should be correlated (i.e., P3 at encoding with P3 at retrieval stages). In the retrieval stage,
group dierences should be more evident when S1 and S2 are dierent. Notwithstanding, these electrophysiolog-
ical patterns should hold regardless of whether individual or bound features are the memoranda.
Methods
Participants. A group of 18 adolescents diagnosed by a certified pediatric neurologist with ADHD
Combined Subtype according to the DSM-V criteria, that were being treated at the neurology service of the Luis
Calvo-Mackenna Children’s Hospital in Santiago and an equal number of non-ADHD adolescents, from public
schools of the same metropolitan area, voluntarily participated in the study. eir ages were from 12 to 14 years
(12.61 ± 0.80). ey were matched by age (ADHD: 12.66 ± 0.76, non-ADHD: 12.55 ± 0.85, F(1, 34) = 0.16832,
p = 0.68419), IQ (ADHD: 99.66 ± 7.12, non-ADHD: 103.66 ± 7.17, F(1, 34) = 2.8138, p = 0.10263) and educa-
tional level (school grade). A complete clinical neurological and psychological evaluation was conducted in all
the participants to rule out any potential confound. at included Conner ‘s Rating scale for parents and teachers,
MINI-KID, STAI anxiety inventory and WISC III test. Subjects with antecedents of any other Neurological or
Psychiatric disease were excluded from the study. Comorbid symptoms of anxiety and conduct disorder were
observed, but no ADHD participant met the criteria for any mayor comorbid disorder. ey were being treated
with methylphenidate for at least four months, but suspended medication 24 hours prior to the study.
e required sample size was calculated a priori using G*Power 344 according to the sample sizes, statistical
power and eect sizes described in previous studies using the same task and/or comparing the same dependent
variables45,46. Expecting a small to moderate eect sizes, the required sample size was of 16 participants per group.
Recruitment of participants were conducted according to the standards set forth in the Declaration of
Helsinki. Aer a clear explanation of the purpose and nature of the research they were asked to formally express
their williness to participate. Informed consent was obtained from a parent and/or legal guardian and participants
also signed an informed assent form. ey were explicitly informed that they were free to nish their partici-
pation at any moment without any question. e whole protocol was examined, approved and followed by the
ethical committee of the Pontical Catholic University of Chile.
Experimental design. This experimental design is an adaptation from previous studies of our
group20,40,41,45,47,48, in particular one adapted for EEG recordings and presenting additional control of potential
confounding such as linguistic rehearsal and spatial information49.
Stimuli. No nameable geometric shapes and non-primary colors were used to minimize verbal rehearsal40. Two
arrays, of three items each, were presented to the le and to the right of a xation cross. Each array was presented
using a virtual 3×3 grid (4° horizontally x 8° vertically), 3° to the le and right of a central xation cross on a grey
background. Each item size was 1° and was, at least, 2° apart from any other item. Items for the study display (S1)
were randomly selected from a set of eight polygons and eight colors and randomly allocated to the 9 positions
within the virtual grid. During the test phase (S2), the same three locations used during the study phase were used
but items locations were interchanged. Hence, items were never presented in the same locations across the study
and test display. By this way, spatial location was render uninformative (see Fig.1).
Design. e task consisted of four blocks counterbalanced across participants. Two of them were of the shape-only
condition where the stimuli consisted of three shapes in black color. e other two were Shape-Color Binding con-
dition and stimuli were three colored shapes. For each block there was a short practice session (8 trials per block)
followed by the test. Each block presented 80 trials (40 right and 40 le of which 20 are same trials – “Same”, S1 = S2 -
and 20 are dierent – “Dierent”, S1 S2 – trials). In the last case, two shapes or two colors were replaced by dierent
ones. ere was a total of 320 trials. e total duration of this task was approximately 35 minutes.
During each trial a xation cross was presented, and participants were asked to keep their eyes on it and to
press a key to initiate the trial. Fixation remained on screen throughout the trial. Aer 400 ms, two arrows were
presented for other 400 ms above and below xation. Arrows direction indicated which side should be attended.
Aer a delay of 600 ms the S1 array was presented for 1000 ms, followed by a 1000 ms retention interval. en, the
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
4
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2020) 10:7771 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64678-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
S2 array is presented and remained on until the participant responded “Same” or “Dierent”, by pressing a specic
key with their dominant hand.
Experimental procedure. EEG recording sessions began by asking participants to sit comfortably in a dimly lit,
electrically and acoustically shielded room. A shin rest device was used to reduce unwanted head movements.
Participants receive standard verbal instructions about the experimental procedure and the experimental task. All
participants were evaluated individually.
Data acquisition. Electrophysiological signals were recorded using a NeuroScan 40-channel Digital
Electroencephalograph with a high-resolution NuAmp amplier. A 40-channel cap (Quick-Cap) from the same com-
pany was used for electrode placement following the international 10/20 electrode sites and linked mastoids as the
reference. Impedances were kept below 5 k throughout the recordings. A/D sampling frequency was set at 1000 Hz.
A band-pass digital lter between 0.1 and 30 Hz was later applied to remove unwanted frequency components. Two
additional bipolar derivations were used to monitor vertical and horizontal ocular movements (VEOG, HEOG).
Data analyses. For behavioral data, the percentage of correct responses (accuracy) and reaction times (RTs)
were measured in all subjects and conditions. Regarding ERPs, oine EEG signals were analyzed using EEGLAB/
ERPLAB Matlab toolbox50,51. Eye movements or blink artefacts were corrected using ICA (Independent compo-
nent analysis). Remaining trials that contained voltage uctuations exceeding ±100 μV (microvolts), transients
exceeding ±100 μV, or electro-oculogram activity exceeding ±50 μV were rejected. Artifact free waveforms were
segmented into 1200 ms epochs starting 200 ms before the onset of S1 and S2 arrays. Separate average waveforms
for each condition were generated.
We used a mixed model ANOVA with repeated measures for behavioral (RT, accuracy) and ERP variables.
e Encoding period of ERP components analysis has two levels: (1) Group (ADHD vs no-ADHD); and (2)
Condition (Single Shape vs. Shape-Color Binding). For the retrieval period (ERP components and behavioral
results), a three levels analysis was performed: 1) Group (ADHD vs no-ADHD); 2) Condition (Single Shape vs.
Shape-Color Binding); and 3) Trail type (Same vs. Dierent). All statistical calculations on ERPs were performed
using individual waveforms. Mean amplitude in the windows 100–130 ms for P1, 180–210 ms for N1, 320–430 ms
for early-P3, 430–600 ms for encoding late-P3, and 320–430 for Retrieval P3 were selected. P1 and N1 amplitudes
were measured on the occipital region (electrodes O1, Oz, O2) yielding similar results. Encoding early and late
P3 and retrieval P3 were measured in Parieto-Occipital midline region (electrodes CPz, Pz, Oz). Selection of
electrodes sites and ERP measures was conducted following the recommendations previously described for this
type of procedures52. For simplicity, only the results from posterior midline (Pz and Oz) were shown. Post hoc
comparisons were assessed with Tukey HSD test. Greenhouse-Geisser and Bonferroni corrections were applied
to compensate for violations of sphericity and multiple comparisons. Only statistically signicant results of ERPs
Figure 1. Schematic view of the experimental task. Le: Shape-Only condition, Dierent trial type. Right:
Color-Shape condition, Same trial type. Time in milliseconds (ms).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
5
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2020) 10:7771 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64678-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
(p < 0.05) were used to test the association among sequential ERP components. Correlations were explored using
Pearson’s correlation coecient (r). Fisher’s R to Z procedure was later used to compare correlations coecients53.
Follow up Bayesian analysis were performed to explore non-signicant interactions in early and late encoding P3
components using JASP soware 0.11.154.
Results
Behavioral Results. Regarding accuracy, the ADHD group showed a poorer performance on all condi-
tions (F(1, 34) = 10.047, p = 0.00322, η2 = 0.23) compared to the non-ADHD group. ere was a signicant and
expected main eect for condition type, whereby Only-Shape resulted in better performance (F(1, 34) = 39.803,
p = 0.00000, η2 = 0.54) than Color-Shape condition, but no Group x Condition interaction (F(1, 34) = 0.020,
p = 0.88969, η2 = 0.00) was found. Trial Type (same or dierent) was also signicant. When S2 was dierent from
the S1, a signicant reduction in the hit rate (F(1,34) = 13.817, p = 0.00072, η2 = 0.29) was observed. A signicant
interaction was found between Group and Trail Type (F(1,34) = 5.2559, p = 0.02818, η2 = 0.13). Post-hoc analyses
showed a signicant drop of performance in the ADHD group during dierent trials (i.e., S2 dierent from S1)
(MSE = 0.01165, df = 66.789, p = 0.0017) (see Fig.2).
Figure 2. Behavioral results, Accuracy and Reactions Times. ADHD: black bars, non-ADHD: grey bars. Time
in seconds (s).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
6
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2020) 10:7771 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64678-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Regarding reaction times (RTs), the ADHD group showed slower responses than the non-ADHD group in
all conditions (F(1,34) = 13.035, p = 0.00097, η2 = 0.28). e Color-shape condition showed slower RTs than the
Only-Shape condition (F(1,34) = 6.2918, p = 0.01706, η2 = 0.16). No other eects were observed.
In summary, ADHD had longer RTs and a poorer performance on all conditions but especially in the dierent
trials. Binding manipulation aected equally ADHD and non-ADHD participants.
Electrophysiological Results. The P1 component at encoding showed larger amplitudes for the
Shape-Only condition (F(1,34)=12.304, p = 0.00129, η2 = 0.27) compared to the Color-Shape condition. No
dierences in amplitude between groups were observed (F(1,34)=00440, p = 0.94751, η2 = 0.00). e following
N1 component showed no amplitudes dierences between groups (F(1,34)=0.08056, p = 0.77826, η2 = 0.00), or
conditions (F(1,34)=1.3163, p = 0.25927, η2 = 0.03). Aer this negativity, a wide P3-like positivity was identi-
ed in the parieto-occipital region of the scalp, with a peak around 340 ms and extended in time up to 600 ms.
e earlier segment of this component, between 320 to 430 ms, exhibited larger amplitude in the non-ADHD
group than in the ADHD (F(1,34)=5.3294, p = 0.02718, η2 = 0.14), no signicant amplitude dierence by con-
dition (F(1,34)=2.9067, p = 0.0973, η2 = 0.07) or Group x Condition interaction (F(1,34)=0.1811, p = 0.67310,
η2 = 0.00) were found. e later part of this positivity (430–600) also showed larger amplitude in the non-ADHD
(F(1,34)=6.4352, p = 0.01595, η2 = 0.16), and larger amplitudes for the Color-shape condition (F(1,34)=5.8871,
p = 0.02071, η2 = 0.15). Again, no significant Group x Condition interaction (F(1,34)=2.4440, p = 0.12723,
η2 = 0.06) was found (see Fig.3). A follow up analysis using a Bayesian approach to explore the odds in favor of
null hypothesis regarding the Group x Condition Interactions showed moderate support for the null in the P3
early window (BF10 = 0.3111) and strong support in the P3 late windows (BF10 = 0.014).
A similar P3 like positive wave was evoked by S2. is retrieval P3 showed no signicant main eect for
groups (F(1,34)=0.26277, p = 0.61154, η2 = 0.00). ere was a signicant main eect of trial type, due to larger
P3 amplitude in the Same trials compare to the Dierent ones (F(1,34)=4.9394, p = 0.03301, η2 = 0.13). A statisti-
cally signicant interaction between group and Trial type was observed (F(1,34)=4.3989, p = 0.04347, η2 = 0.11).
Follow up post-hoc contrasts showed that while P3 amplitude dierentiated between Same and Dierent trials
in the non-ADHD group (MSE = 12.724, df = 37.905, p = 0.0216), it was not the case for the ADHD group
(MSE = 12.724, df = 37.905, p = 0.9996) (see Fig.4).
In summary, P3 at encoding was larger in the non-ADHD group. P3 at retrieval discriminated the presence
or absence of a change only in the non-ADHD group. Binding-dependent ERP modulations were not sensitive
to group membership.
Associations among sequential ERPs at dierent stages. e amplitude of the encoding early-P3
signicantly correlated with that of the retrieval period, both in the Same (r = 0.48077, p = 0.00299) and Dierent
(r = 0.38179, p = 0.02157) trial types. Follow up analysis showed that these signicant correlations were driven by
the results from the non-ADHD group: Same (r = 0.50955, p = 0.03077) and Dierent (r = 0.49540, p = 0.03656).
e equivalent analysis in the ADHD group showed no signicant correlation: Same (r = 0.35457, p = 0.14881)
and Dierent (r = 0.25418, p = 0.30877) (see Fig.5). Comparing the correlation coecients between the groups
using Fischer’s R to Z approach resulted in non-discriminative observed Z: Same Z(obs)= 0.5230, Dierent Z(obs)=
0.7750.
Discussion
In the present study, we found a poorer overall performance and larger RTs in ADHD versus non-ADHD partic-
ipants. Particularly, ADHD participants produced signicantly fewer hits (i.e., correctly detect if S1 and S2 were
dierent). e electrophysiological results evidenced signicant dierences between the groups in ERP compo-
nents elicited during encoding and signicant interaction Group x Trial Type during retrieval. e need to bind
color and shape resulted in no signicant Group x Condition interaction, suggesting that ADHD has no dieren-
tial impact on binding functions carried out in WM. ere was a signicant correlation between the amplitude
of the P3 component elicited during encoding and that elicited during retrieval that was signicant only in the
non-ADHD group. ese results have important implications for our understanding of the involvement of WM
in ADHD and the functional organization of this cognitive function. We discuss these implications below.
Implications for WM functions in ADHD. e behavioral results of the current study supported our orig-
inal hypothesis. All participants showed better accuracy in the “Shape-Only” than in the “Color-Shape” condi-
tion. is result has been previously observed in other studies using similar experimental designs20,45. ey are
interpreted as the cost of integrating features into objects to be kept in WM and are in line with the predictions
from the feature integration theory55. Additionally, all participants performed better when the study (S1) and the
test arrays (S2) were composed of the same items relative to trials where they had to detect and report changes
happening in the test array. at is, when they had to update the WM representation to account for a change.
ese results are in line with previous studies using similar WM tasks40,56. Our hypothesis of ADHD’s poorer
performance in all conditions was also conrmed, supporting previous reports in the literature9,21,42. Interestingly,
this was signicantly increased when a WM updating was needed.
Traditionally, poor behavioral performance of ADHD individuals on WM tasks has been explained in terms
of a dysfunctional attentional process that impairs proper use of WM resources57. For instance, a decient l-
tering of the incoming information could overload WM, rendering it also decient58,59. is idea implies that
attention and WM resources operate in tandem to process the available stimuli with the former supporting the
latter. Nevertheless, the characterization of attention impairments in ADHD does not support this notion. e
idea of a decient ltering in ADHD causing an overload of working memory and resources depletion has been
disputed58,59. Previous studies from our group1,2 point in a dierent direction. First, although ADHD do have
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
7
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2020) 10:7771 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64678-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
problems when dealing with distractors it is not necessarily due to a decient attentional ltering. Instead, they
seem to follow task relative relevance to select and pay attention to objects2. Furthermore, several studies have
proven that specic attention decits in ADHD could be elusive5. e most consistent nding points to a dys-
function in executive attention, as part of a more general executive functions impairment that also include WM60
(but see also3). In this way, administering attention and WM resources seems to be the most typical problem.
erefore, a clear description of how the dierent WM sub-processes (encoding, binding-retention and retrieval)
operate in this population and how they relate to each other (and to attention) seems critical to understand WM
decits in ADHD.
Figure 3. Encoding stage ERPs and topographic maps. Color-Shape: blue lines, Shape-Only: red lines. ADHD:
solid lines, non-ADHD: dashed lines. Amplitudes in Microvolts (µV). Time in milliseconds (ms).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
8
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2020) 10:7771 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64678-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
As previously stated, behavioral responses do not allow to discriminate between the dierent WM stages and
their potential contribution to the impairment. ERPs have a high temporal resolution and dierent components
have been described as functional indicators of distinct attention and WM processes. Attention allocation impacts
the amplitude of early components of the visual ERP (P1, N1), increasing their amplitude61. In the present study,
we found signicant amplitude dierences between conditions but no dierences between groups. ese ndings
also point against a decient early visual ltering as a mechanism that could explain attention-WM impairment
in ADHD1,2. On the contrary, the P3 component has been linked to working memory and attention since its ear-
liest descriptions62. P3 amplitude has been suggested to indicate working memory updating32 but also resource
allocation63. e amplitude of P3 is known to be aected by attention allocation and, interestingly, a reduced P3
amplitude has been reported in ADHD patients through a wide variety of cognitive tests34.
In the present study, the encoding and the retrieval periods were characterized by the presence of the P3 like
component elicited by the study array and the test array respectively. In both cases these components had larger
amplitude in non-ADHD than in ADHD. ese WM-related P3 components have been previously reported in
several WM tasks33,64. Its amplitude has been related with the ecacy of encoding and retrieval65,66. For example,
Friedman and Johnson67 found that items subsequently recognized or remembered elicited larger encoding P3 than
those that were later missed. In this line, the decreased P3 amplitude in ADHD would point to a decient WM
encoding process. is way of interpreting P3 amplitude falls within the frame of the “context updating theory”
proposed by Donchin and Coles32 which suggested that P3 amplitude reects the eort to continuously update new
relevant information to the representation held in WM. Another view (non-necessarily opposite) suggests that P3
amplitude reects the allocation of attentional resources necessary to categorize stimuli for encoding and to discrim-
inate its relevance in the retrieval stage64. Although the exact meaning of WM-related P3 amplitude modulations
Figure 4. Retrieval stage ERPs and topographic maps. Same: blue lines, Dierent: red lines. ADHD: solid lines,
non-ADHD: dashed lines. Amplitudes in Microvolts (µV). Time in milliseconds (ms).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
9
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2020) 10:7771 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64678-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
is still a matter of discussion, the correlation between its amplitude and WM eciency seems reliable. WM rep-
resentations are exible and can be modulated dynamically according to changing goals and expectations68, and
such process requires dynamic allocation of attention and representation updating which modulates P3 amplitude.
Regarding the retrieval stage, we found larger P3 amplitude for the “Same” condition compared to the
“Dierent” one. ese eects are in line with previous results described as the new-old eect in studies of recog-
nition memory69, where larger P3 amplitudes are reported for the old items compared to new ones. It has been
suggested that such amplitude modulation reects activity from a recollection-sensitive regions in the lateral
parietal cortex, functionally indexing the representation of recollected information66. Alternatively, in the context
of a change detection task, this amplitude modulation could also be interpreted as reecting a more exhaustive
memory search in the “Same” condition until the presence of a change has been ruled out. e latter view is con-
sistent with the notion that correctly detecting change implies recollection while detecting “sameness” or absence
Figure 5. Encoding and Retrieval P3 component Amplitudes correlations. Pearson’s correlation coecient (r).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
10
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2020) 10:7771 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64678-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
of change, involve identifying familiarity70. Taken together the results from the P3 component presented here
suggest that both the encoding and retrieval WM processes could be compromised in ADHD.
e amplitude of Encoding and Retrieval P3 components were signicantly correlated both in the same
and in the different trial types. This correlation was apparently driven by the results from the non-ADHD
group. Nevertheless, a follow up analysis comparing correlations coefficients between groups resulted
non-discriminative, probably due to limited sample size. Given the sequential nature of the task and the probable
participation of attention both during encoding and retrieval the correlations of this neurophysiological indica-
tors was an expected result but needs further conrmation using a larger sample of participants.
Another WM process that could potentially account for the impairments seen in ADHD is the binding process
itself. at is the construction of an integrative process of objects in WM. In the current work we did not nd signi-
cant dierence between groups regarding the binding condition (Shape-Only Vs. Color-Shape), neither behaviorally
nor electrophysiologically. Even though, the Color-Shape condition resulted in larger reaction times in all partici-
pants. To our knowledge this is rst study to assess WM binding processes in ADHD. e lack of group dierences
could be explained by that fact that the neural system subserving binding processes has been reported to involve
a posterior network of parietal, temporal and occipital areas20, and not the Prefrontal Cortex anterior executive
network usually reported to be aected in ADHD71. It should be noted that parietal regions have also been reported
to be aected in ADHD72. Nevertheless, metanalytic studies suggest that ADHD impairments in dierent neural
networks are closely related to the tasks or domains being evaluated71,73. While parietal dysfunctions in ADHD have
been mostly related to attentional orienting tasks (right inferior parietal cortex)73, visuospatial working memory
tasks have been mostly correlated with frontal regions dysfunctions74. ese ndings have important implications
for current understanding of the functional organization of WM which we address in the next section. According to
our results, binding functions carried out in WM seem to be intact in ADHD. Nevertheless, more specic studies are
undoubtfully needed to explore in depth WM binding in this condition71.
In this context, our results could be interpreted as a failure in ADHD adolescents to update WM representa-
tions to accomplish task demands or a failure in the prioritization of representations as suggested by Myers et
al.75. is could potentially impact dierent subprocesses. First, during the encoding stage reduced P3 amplitude
could reect decient attention allocation to the relevant aspects when creating a representation. en, during
the retrieval stage the smaller P3 amplitude in ADHD, which also fails to discriminate between “Same” and
“Dierent” (as it occurs in the non-ADHD group) could be interpreted as evidence of a widespread failure to
assign post-selection priority to the representations held in memory and their posterior update to solve the task
at hand. is seems to be especially clear when the test stimuli were dierent from the study ones.
Implications for the functional organization WM. This study provides valuable evidence to fur-
ther assess recent positions regarding the functional organization of WM. A question that has received sub-
stantial attention in recent years is whether binding functions operating in WM require additional attentional
resources1719. oroughly conducted experiments have manipulated attention during visual WM binding tasks
using dierent approaches and all have failed to demonstrate that binding requires resources above and beyond
those needed to process individual features. Baddeley12 envisaged that the episodic buer was the WM compo-
nent where such binding operations would occur supported by attention. Should this proposal be valid, any WM
operation requiring binding would be dramatically aected if attentional resources are not available during such
operation. Clearly, that was not the case in the series of experiments above described. ese consistent ndings
led Baddeley and collaborators16 to revise the WM models and reconsider the function of the episodic buer. e
new revision suggests that low-level binding functions, such as those needed to integrate surface features and
form objects identity, can be carried out outside the episodic buer, being areas in the posterior part of the brain
likely neural correlates20. However, a potential limitation of these experiments is that attention was experimen-
tally manipulated making it possible that individual dierences in attentional resources would have impacted on
such outcomes. A more reliable approach would involve individuals with attentional impairments such as those
diagnosed with ADHD. In the current study we addressed this issue in such a population. We have conrmed that
individuals with attention impairment, as demonstrated by their clinical proles and general WM functions, are
still able to hold bound information in WM. is is the rst study reporting such ndings which, to the authors’
views, support the notion that such binding operations could be automatic.
A potential account for such relation between attention and binding function of WM has been linked to
the type of attention needed to support this function. While executive attention seemingly driven by functions
of the prefrontal cortex might be crucial for binding operations happening within the episodic buer16, other
bottom-up low-level attentional functions might support the binding of surface features within integrated objects.
Such functions, which are seemingly supported by a posterior network involving parietal, temporal and occipital
areas20,45,49,76, might be less vulnerable to conditions impacting on attention such as ADHD or even depression40.
Recent studies have pointed to a functional integration decit of connectivity-based pathophysiologic process
in ADHD7780. Control networks recruited during WM tasks are sensitive to neurodevelopmental factors which
aect the patterns of connectivity integration/and segregation81. A signicant body of literature suggests that
frontal networks, as those sub-serving WM, seem to be aected in ADHD8285. is would explain the overall
WM impairment seen in these patients in the current study.
Limitations and future directions. e current study has limitations that should be addressed in future
studies. First, current and previous studies use no nameable geometric shapes and non-primary colors trying to
avoid phonological coding of non-verbal material20,45. is approach reduces but can’t ensure the complete avoid-
ance of implicit verbal rehearsal86. e presence of such strategy was not measure and its potential impact can’t
be ruled out. Future studies should consider this in their design to directly address this issue. Second, ADHD is
a complex, multisystem and highly heterogenic condition. Although inattention is probably its most consistent
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
11
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2020) 10:7771 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64678-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
characteristic, the presence of the diagnosis cannot be equated to a constant or stable decit. ere is large intra
and inter subject’s variability. e inferences regarding the relations between WM, binding and Attention should
be further investigated using dierent experimental designs and larger sample sizes. ird, future studies should
separate more systematically attention and working memory and also should address the potential impact of
stimulant medication on WM decits in ADHD, as well as, dierent ADHD subtypes.
is work opens a new agenda investigating the role of inter-coupling among attention and WM process and
networks in ADHD and other neuropsychiatric conditions that impact on these cognitive abilities. In sum, stud-
ying the interaction between attentional guided dynamic prioritization and WM in ADHD could be a promising
approach to understand the pathophysiology of the condition and to rene understanding of models of memory.
Data availability
e datasets generated during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable
request.
Received: 20 August 2019; Accepted: 14 April 2020;
Published: xx xx xxxx
References
1. Ortega, ., López, V., Carrasco, X., Anllo-Vento, L. & Aboitiz, F. Exogenous orienting of visual-spatial attention in ADHD children.
Brain Res. 1493, 68–79, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.11.036 (2013).
2. López, V. et al. Attention-decit hyperactivity disorder involves dierential cortic al processing in a visual spatial attention paradigm.
Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 2540–2548, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2006.07.313 (2006).
3. Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Bare, E. J., Milham, M. P. & Tannoc, . Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond executive
dysfunction. Trends Cogn Sci 10, 117–123, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011 (2006).
4. Durston, S. A review of the biological bases of ADHD: What have we learned from imaging studies? Mental Retardation and
Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 9, 184–195, https://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.10079 (2003).
5. Huang-Polloc, C. L. & Nigg, J. T. Searching for the attention deficit in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: the case of
visuospatial orienting. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 23, 801–830, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(03)00073-4 (2003).
6. Huang-Polloc, C. L., aralunas, S. L., Tam, H. & Moore, A. N. Evaluating Vigilance Decits in ADHD: A Meta-Analysis of CPT
Performance. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 121, 360–371, https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027205 (2012).
7. Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V. & Pennington, B. F. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-decit/
hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1336–1346, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006 (2005).
8. Nigg, J. T., Blasey, L. G., Huang-Polloc, C. L. & appley, M. D. Neuropsychological executive functions and DSM-IV ADHD
subtypes. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 41, 59–66, https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200201000-00012 (2002).
9. Nigg, J. T. Neuropsychologic theory and ndings in attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder: the state of the eld and salient
challenges for the coming decade. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1424–1435, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.011 (2005).
10. Baddeley, A. D. Woring memory: looing bac and looing forward. Nat Rev Neurosci 4, 829–839, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201
(2003).
11. Baddeley, A. D. Woring memory. Curr. Biol. 20, 136–140, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.014 (2010).
12. Baddeley, A. D. e episodic buer: a new component of woring memory? Trends Cogn Sci 4, 417–423, https://doi.org/10.1016/
s1364-6613(00)01538-2 (2000).
13. Cowan, N. Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human
information-processing system. Psychol. Bull. 104, 163–191, https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.104.2.163 (1988).
14. Engle, . W. Woring Memory Capacity as Executive Attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science 11, 19–23, https://doi.
org/10.1111/1467-8721.00160 (2002).
15. Fougnie, D. In New research on short-term memory (ed N.B. Johansen) Ch. 1, 1–45 (Nova Science Publishers, 2008).
16. Baddeley, A. D., Allen, . J. & Hitch, G. J. Binding in visual woring memory: the role of the episodic buer. Neuropsychologia 49,
1393–1400, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.042 (2011).
17. arlsen, P. J., Allen, . J., Baddeley, A. D. & Hitch, G. J. Binding across space and time in visual woring memory. Mem. Cognit 38,
292–303, https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.3.292 (2010).
18. Allen, . J., Baddeley, A. D. & Hitch, G. J. Is the binding of visual features in woring memory resource-demanding? J. Exp. Psychol.
Gen 135, 298–313, https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.2.298 (2006).
19. Allen, . J., Hitch, G. & Baddeley, A. Cross-modal binding and woring memory. Vis. cogn 17, 83–102, https://doi.
org/10.1080/13506280802281386 (2009).
20. Parra, M. A., Della Sala, S., Logie, . H. & Morcom, A. M. Neural correlates of shape-color binding in visual woring memory.
Neuropsychologia 52, 27–36, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.036 (2014).
21. Pievsy, M. A. & McGrath, . E. e Neurocognitive Prole of Attention-Decit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A eview of Meta-
Analyses. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 33, 143–157, https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acx055 (2018).
22. Alderson, . M., asper, L. J., Hudec, . L. & Patros, C. H. Attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and woring memory
in adults: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 27, 287–302, https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032371 (2013).
23. asper, L. J., Alderson, . M. & Hudec, . L. Moderators of woring memory decits in children with attention-decit/hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD): a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 32, 605–617, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2012.07.001 (2012).
24. Martel, M., Niolas, M. & Nigg, J. T. Executive function in adolescents with ADHD. J.Am Acad.Child Adolesc.Psychiatry 46,
1437–1444, https://doi.org/10.1097/chi.0b013e31814cf953 (2007).
25. Schweitzer, J. B., Hanford, . B. & Medo, D. . Woring memory decits in adults with ADHD: is there evidence for subtype
dierences? Behav Brain Funct 2, 43, https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-2-43 (2006).
26. Barley, . A. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol.
Bull. 121, 65–94, https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65 (1997).
27. Biederman, J. et al. Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic
outcomes in children. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 72, 757–766, https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.757 (2004).
28. Huang-Polloc, C. L. & aralunas, S. L. Woring memory demands impair sill acquisition in children with ADHD. J. Abnorm.
Psychol 119, 174–185, https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017862 (2010).
29. Spron, M., Vogel, E. . & Jonman, L. M. No behavioral or EP evidence for a developmental lag in visual woring memory capacity
or ltering in adolescents and adults with ADHD. PLoS One 8, e62673, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0062673 (2013).
30. Stroux, D. et al. Decient interference control during woring memory updating in adults with ADHD: An event-related potential
study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 452–463, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2015.05.021 (2016).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
12
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2020) 10:7771 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64678-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
31. C or te s e, S. et al. Cognitive training for attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of clinical and neuropsychological
outcomes from randomized controlled trials. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 54, 164–174, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jaac.2014.12.010 (2015).
32. Donchin, E. & Coles, M. G. H. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? Behav. Brain Sci. 11, 357–374, https://
doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00058027 (1988).
33. o, A. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology 38, 557–577, https://doi.org/10.1017/
S0048577201990559 (2001).
34. Barry, . J., Johnstone, S. J. & Clare, A. . A review of electrophysiology in attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder: II. Event-related
potentials. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 184–198, https://doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00363-2 (2003).
35. Szuromi, B., Czobor, P., omlosi, S. & Bitter, I. P300 decits in adults with attention decit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis.
Psychol. Med. 41, 1529–1538, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291710001996 (2011).
36. Vogel, E. . & Machizawa, M. G. Neural activity predicts individual dierences in visual woring memory capacity. Nature 428,
748–751, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02447 (2004).
37. Luria, ., Balaban, H., Awh, E. & Vogel, E. . e contralateral delay activity as a neural measure of visual woring memory.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 62, 100–108, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.01.003 (2016).
38. Luc, S. J. & Vogel, E. . e capacity of visual woring memory for features and conjunctions. Nature 390, 279–281, https://doi.
org/10.1038/36846 (1997).
39. eage, H. A. et al. EP indices of woring memory updating in AD/HD: differential aspects of development, subtype, and
medication. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 25, 32–41, https://doi.org/10.1097/WNP.0b013e318163ccc0 (2008).
40. Parra, M. A., Abrahams, S., Logie, . H. & Della Sala, S. Visual short-term memory binding in Alzheimer's disease and depression.
J. Neurol. 257, 1160–1169, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-010-5484-9 (2010).
41. Parra, M. A., Abrahams, S., Logie, . H. & Sala, S. D. Age and binding within-dimension features in visual short-term memory.
Neurosci. Lett. 449, 1–5, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.10.069 (2009).
42. Martinussen, ., Hayden, J., Hogg-Johnson, S. & Tannoc, . A meta-analysis of woring memory impairments in children with
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 44, 377–384, https://doi.org/10.1097/01.
chi.0000153228.72591.73 (2005).
43. rieger, V. & Amador-Campos, J. A. Assessment of executive function in ADHD adolescents: contribution of performance tests and
rating scales. Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence, 1–25, https://
doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2017.1386781 (2017).
44. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G. & Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A exible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral,
and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods 39, 175–191, https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146 (2007).
45. Pietto, M. et al. Behavioral and Electrophysiological Correlates of Memory Binding Decits in Patients at Dierent is Levels for
Alzheimer's Disease. J Alzheimers Dis 53, 1325–1340, https://doi.org/10.3233/jad-160056 (2016).
46. Wiegand, I. et al. EEG correlates of visual short-term memory as neuro-cognitive endophenotypes of ADHD. Neuropsychologia 85,
91–99, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.011 (2016).
47. Parra, M. A. et al. Short-term memory binding decits in Alzheimer's disease. Brain 132, 1057–1066, https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/
awp036 (2009).
48. Parra, M. A. et al. Brain Information Sharing During Visual Short-Term Memory Binding Yields a Memory Biomarer for Familial
Alzheimer's Disease. Current Alzheimer research 14, 1335–1347, https://doi.org/10.2174/1567205014666170614163316 (2017).
49. Smith, . et al. Locating Temporal Functional Dynamics of Visual Short-Term Memory Binding using Graph Modular Dirichlet
Energy. Scientic reports 7, 42013, https://doi.org/10.1038/srep42013 (2017).
50. Delorme, A. & Maeig, S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent
component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 (2004).
51. Lopez-Calderon, J. & Luc, S. J. EPLAB: an open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials. Front Hum Neurosci 8,
213, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00213 (2014).
52. eil, A. et al. Committee report: publication guidelines and recommendations for studies using electroencephalography and
magnetoencephalography. Psychophysiology 51, 1–21, https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12147 (2014).
53. Fisher, . A. On the “Probable Error” of a coecient of correlation deduced from a small sample. Metron 1, 3–32 (1921).
54. JASP (Version 0.11.1) (Amsterdam, e Netherlands, 2019).
55. Treisman, A. M. & Gelade, G. A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognit. Psychol. 12, 97–136, https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-
0285(80)90005-5 (1980).
56. Brocmole, J. ., Parra, M. A., Sala, S. D. & Logie, . H. Do binding decits account for age-related decline in visual woring
memory? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 15, 543–547, https://doi.org/10.3758/pbr.15.3.543 (2008).
57. Burgess, G. C. et al. Attentional control activation relates to woring memory in attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol.
Psychiatry 67, 632–640, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.10.036 (2010).
58. Jonman, L. M., enemans, J. L., emner, C., Verbaten, M. N. & van Engeland, H. Dipole source localization of event-related brain
activity indicative of an early visual selective attention decit in ADHD children. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 1537–1549, https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.clinph.2004.01.022 (2004).
59. Jonman, L. M. et al. Attentional capacity, a probe EP study: Dierences between children with attention-decit hyperactivity
disorder and normal control children and eects of methylphenidate. Psychophysiology 37, 334–346, https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-
8986.3730334 (2000).
60. Castellanos, F. X. & Tannoc, . Neuroscience of attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder: the search for endophenotypes. Nat Rev
Neurosci 3, 617–628, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn896 (2002).
61. Hillyard, S. A. & Anllo-Vento, L. Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
95, 781–787, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.3.781 (1998).
62. Polich, J. & o, A. Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review. Biol. Psychol. 41, 103–146, https://doi.
org/10.1016/0301-0511(95)05130-9 (1995).
63. o, A. Event-related-potential (EP) reections of mental resources: a review and synthesis. Biol. Psychol. 45, 19–56, https://doi.
org/10.1016/S0301-0511(96)05221-0 (1997).
64. Polich, J. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2128–2148, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
clinph.2007.04.019 (2007).
65. im, S., Liu, Z., Glizer, D., Tannoc, . & Woltering, S. Adult ADHD and woring memory: neural evidence of impaired encoding.
Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 1596–1603, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2013.12.094 (2014).
66. ugg, M. D. & Curran, T. Event-related potentials and recognition memory. Trends Cogn Sci 11, 251–257, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
tics.2007.04.004 (2007).
67. Friedman, D. & Johnson, ., Jr. Event-related potential (EP) studies of memory encoding and retrieval: a selective review. Microsc.
Res. Tech. 51, 6–28, 10.1002/1097-0029(20001001)51:1<6::AID-JEMT2>3.0.CO;2- (2000).
68. uo, B. C., Stoes, M. G. & Nobre, A. C. Attention modulates maintenance of representations in visual short-term memory. J. Cogn.
Neurosci. 24, 51–60, https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00087 (2012).
69. ugg, M. D., Allan, . & Birch, C. S. Electrophysiological evidence for the modulation of retrieval orientation by depth of study
processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 664–678, https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900562291 (2000).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
13
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2020) 10:7771 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-64678-x
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
70. Cowan, N., Naveh-Benjamin, M., ilb, A. & Saults, J. S. Life-span development of visual woring memory: when is feature binding
dicult? Dev. Psychol. 42, 1089–1102, https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1089 (2006).
71. ubia, . Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention Decit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Its Clinical Translation. Front Hum
Neurosci 12, 100, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00100 (2018).
72. Cortese, S. et al. Toward systems neuroscience of ADHD: a meta-analysis of 55 fMI studies. Am J Psychiatry 169, 1038–1055,
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11101521 (2012).
73. Hart, H., adua, J., Naao, T., Mataix-Cols, D. & ubia, . Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of
inhibition and attention in attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder: exploring tas-specic, stimulant medication, and age eects.
JAMA Psychiatry 70, 185–198, https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.277 (2013).
74. van Ewij, H. et al. Neural correlates of visuospatial woring memory in attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder and healthy
controls. Psychiatr y Res. 233, 233–242, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.07.003 (2015).
75. Myers, N. E., Stoes, M. G. & Nobre, A. C. Prioritizing Information during Woring Memory: Beyond Sustained Internal Attention.
Trends Cogn Sci 21, 449–461, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.010 (2017).
76. Parra, M. A. et al. Brain information sharing during visual short-term memory binding yields a memory biomarer for familial
Alzheimer's disease. Current Alzheimer research 14, 1335–1347, https://doi.org/10.2174/1567205014666170614163316 (2017).
77. Li, F. e t al. Intrinsic Brain Abnormalities in Attention Decit Hyperact ivity Disorder: A esting-State Functional M Imaging Study.
Radiology, 131622, https://doi.org/10.1148/radiol.14131622 (2014).
78. B arttfeld, P. et al. Functional connectivity and temporal variability of brain connections in adults with attention decit/hyperactivity
disorder and bipolar disorder. Neuropsychobiology 69, 65–75, https://doi.org/10.1159/000356964 (2014).
79. Liu, T., Chen, Y., Lin, P. & Wang, J. Small-World Brain Functional Networs in Children With Attention-Decit/Hyperactivity
Disorder evealed by EEG Synchrony. Clin EEG Neurosci, https://doi.org/10.1177/1550059414523959 (2014).
80. Cao, M., Shu, N., Cao, Q., Wang, Y. & He, Y. Imaging Functional and Structural Brain Connectomics in Attention-Deficit/
Hyperactivity Disorder. Mol. Neurobiol., https://doi.org/10.1007/s12035-014-8685-x (2014).
81. Fair, D. A. et al. Development of distinct control networs through segregation and integration. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104,
13507–13512, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0705843104 (2007).
82. Leech, . & Sharp, D. J. e role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain 137, 12–32, https://doi.org/10.1093/
brain/awt162 (2014).
83. ubia, ., Alegria, A. & Brinson, H. Imaging the ADHD brain: disorder-specicity, medication eects and clinical translation.
Expert review of neurotherapeutics 14, 519–538, https://doi.org/10.1586/14737175.2014.907526 (2014).
84. Sonuga-Bare, E. J. & Fairchild, G. Neuroeconomics of attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder: dierential inuences of medial,
dorsal, and ventral prefrontal brain networs on suboptimal decision maing? Biol. Psychiatry 72, 126–133, https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.04.004 (2012).
85. Castellanos, F. X. & Proal, E. Large-scale brain systems in ADHD: beyond the prefrontal-striatal model. Trends Cogn Sci 16, 17–26,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.007 (2012).
86. Logie, . H. e Functional Organization and Capacity Limits of Woring Memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science 20,
240–245, https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411415340 (2011).
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by postdoctoral grant CONICYT/FONDECYT 3150195, and CONICYT PAI/
Concurso Nacional Inserción de Capital Humano Avanzado en la Academia Convocatoria año 2017 PAI79170011
grant to RO, and by CONICYT/FONDECYT 1160258 to FA and VL. VL work was also supported by CONICYT/
FONDECYT 1150241. MAP work was supported by Alzheimer’s Society, Grant # AS-R42303.A.M.G.work was
supported byCONICET andPrograma Interdisciplinario de Investigación Experimental en Comunicación y
Cognición (PIIECC), Facultad de Humanidades, USACH. Patricia Opazo provided valuable technical assistance.
Author contributions
R.O., V.L. and M.A.P. formulated the hypotheses and adapted the experimental design. X.C., V.L. and M.J.E.
conducted the neurological and psychological evaluation of participants. R.O. collected the data. R.O., V.L.
performed the analysis of the data and wrote the manuscript. F.A., M.A.P. and A.M.G. contributed to the
theoretical and methodological framework and critically contributed to the nal version of the manuscript. All
authors reviewed and approved the manuscript.
Competing interests
e authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to F.A.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre-
ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per-
mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the
copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
© e Author(s) 2020
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... Despite several ongoing studies, the exact pathogenesis of ADHD remains unclear. However, the most prominent attention deficits are generally associated with frontal-subcortical dysfunction, which is linked to executive function [13,14]. In addition to these research trends, research on topics related to the analysis of neuropsychological outcomes in ADHD has gained interest [3,14,15] because they can characterize the manifestations of functional impairment in adult ADHD. ...
... Although these impairments have been suggested to be associated with attention problems, the exact processes involved are not yet fully understood. Impairments in working memory are considered the most important cognitive feature of the differences between the ADHD and non-AD-HD groups [13,27]. Since previous studies have suggested that working memory impairment plays a significant role in ADHD, the finding that the reduction in WMI is greater in patients with ADHD and comorbidities warrants further investigation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives This study aimed to identify the psychiatric comorbidity status of adult patients diagnosed with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and determine the impact of comorbidities on neuropsychological outcomes in ADHD. Methods The study participants were 124 adult patients with ADHD. Clinical psychiatric assessments were performed by two boardcertified psychiatrists in accordance with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition. All participants were assessed using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview Plus version 5.0.0 to evaluate comorbidities. After screening, neuropsychological outcomes were assessed using the Comprehensive Attention Test (CAT) and the Korean version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Fourth Edition (K-WAIS-IV). Results Mood disorders (38.7%) were the most common comorbidity of ADHD, followed by anxiety (18.5%) and substance use disorders (13.7%). The ADHD with comorbidities group showed worse results on the Perceptual Organization Index and Working Memory Index sections of the K-WAIS than the ADHD-alone group (p=0.015 and p=0.024, respectively). In addition, the presence of comorbidities was associated with worse performance on simple visual commission errors in the CAT tests (p=0.024). Conclusion These findings suggest that psychiatric comorbidities are associated with poor neuropsychological outcomes in adult patients with ADHD, highlighting the need to identify comorbidities in these patients.
... It allows information to be kept "in mind" over short timescales (Goldman-Rakic, 1995) and is central to the organization of goal-directed behavior (Chatham & Badre, 2015;Miller & Cohen, 2001). In addition, impairments of WM feature prominently in a wide variety of neuropsychiatric disorders (e.g., attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Ortega et al., 2020), schizophrenia (Silver et al., 2003)). Understanding WM is thus of key interest to both cognitive and clinical neuroscientists. ...
Chapter
Working memory (WM) refers to a set of processes that makes task-relevant information accessible to higher-level cognitive processes including abstract reasoning, decision-making, learning, and reading comprehension. In this chapter, we introduce the concept of WM and outline key behavioral and neural evidence for a number of critical subprocesses that support WM and which have become recent targets of cognitive neuroscience. We discuss common approaches to linking brain and behavior in WM research seeking to identify the neural basis of WM subprocesses. We draw attention to limitations of common approaches and suggest that much progress could be made by applying several of the recent methodological advances in model-based cognitive neuroscience discussed throughout this book (see Chapters “An Introduction to EEG/MEG for Model-Based Cognitive Neuroscience”, “Ultra-High Field Magnetic Resonance Imaging for Model-Based Neuroscience”, “Advancements in Joint Modeling of Neural and Behavioral Data”, “Cognitive Models as a Tool to Link Decision Behavior With EEG Signals”, and “Linking Models with Brain Measures”). Overall, the purpose of this chapter is to give a broad overview of WM as seen through the lens of model-based cognitive neuroscience and to summarize our current state of knowledge of WM subprocesses and their neural basis. We hope to outline a path forward to a more complete neurocomputational understanding of WM.
... However, existing research has not empirically established such relationships. Instead, prior studies highlight the conceptual overlap and functional interdependence between working memory and attention [73,74], suggesting the difficulty in isolating the differential susceptibility of each function to individual differences. Similarly, the distinct impact of window views on verbal working memory and classroom sizes on visuo-spatial working memory poses questions that are yet unanswered by empirical research. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study probed how baseline cortisol (BC), negative emotionality (NE), and environmental facets—classroom size and window view—interact to affect executive function (EF) in preschoolers using virtual reality (VR). In a cohort of 144 children aged 61–85 months, BC levels were measured by saliva assays and NE by parental surveys. Participants completed computerized EF assessments both pre- and post-exposure to one of four VR conditions, which varied by classroom size (large vs. small) and window view (natural vs. built). Due to missing data and outlier responses, three children were removed from the analyses. Regression analyses, accounting for initial EFs, revealed that higher BC was significantly associated with better Digit-span task scores in the nature view, while lower BC correlated with improved performance in the built view. With regard to classroom size, children with varying levels of NE benefitted from the large classroom environment, as evidenced by marginally significant improvements on the Corsi block task. However, higher NE children outperformed their lower NE peers in the large classroom, while a trend inverted in the small classroom context. The findings illuminate how the physical components of preschool environments may interact with children’s physiological reactivity, potentially influencing the development of working memory.
... These different cognitive domains are interrelated and decline with normal ageing [24]. For instance, deficits in attention may impair memory encoding and retrieval processes, limiting an individual's ability to effectively acquire and recall information [25], while executive function difficulties can affect problem-solving abilities and task switching, making it challenging for individuals to adapt to new situations and effectively handle complex tasks [26]. Meanwhile, cognitive training on one specific cognitive domain showed significant transfer effects on untrained domains [27,28]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Quality of life (QoL) of older adults has become a pivotal concern of the public and health system. Previous studies found that both cognitive decline and neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) can affect QoL in older adults. However, it remains unclear how these symptoms are related to each other and impact on QoL. Our aim is to investigate the complex network relationship between cognitive and NPS symptoms in older adults, and to further explore their association with QoL. Methods A cross-sectional study was conducted in a sample of 389 older individuals with complaints of memory decline. The instruments included the Neuropsychiatric Inventory, the Mini Mental State Examination, and the 36-item Short Form Health Survey. Data was analyzed using network analysis and mediation analysis. Results We found that attention and agitation were the variables with the highest centrality in cognitive and NPS symptoms, respectively. In an exploratory mediation analysis, agitation was significantly associated with poor attention (β = -0.214, P < 0.001) and reduced QoL (β = -0.137, P = 0.005). The indirect effect of agitation on the QoL through attention was significant (95% confidence interval (CI) [-0.119, -0.035]). Furthermore, attention served as a mediator between agitation and QoL, accounting for 35.09% of the total effect. Conclusions By elucidating the NPS-cognition-QoL relationship, the current study provides insights for developing rehabilitation programs among older adults to ensure their QoL.
... These altered hippocampus-precuneus-middle frontal gyrus connections may clarify the compromised attention-related encoding and retrieval processes leading to cognitive deficits in ADHD (Ortega et al., 2020). A thesis by Roya (2016) hypothesized that ADHD patients possess decreased resting-state activity in pathways through the hippocampus because of reduced volume and executive functioning frequency. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most widespread and highly heritable neurodevelopmental disorders affecting children worldwide. Although synaptosomal-associated protein 25 (SNAP-25) is a possible gene hypothesized to be associated with working memory deficits in ADHD, little is known about its specific impact on the hippocampus. The goal of the current study was to determine how variations in ADHD’s SNAP-25 Mnll polymorphism (rs3746544) affect hippocampal functional connectivity (FC). Methods A total of 88 boys between the ages of 7 and 10 years were recruited for the study, including 60 patients with ADHD and 28 healthy controls (HCs). Data from resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) and clinical information were acquired and assessed. Two single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNP) in the SNAP-25 gene were genotyped, according to which the study’s findings separated ADHD patients into two groups: TT homozygotes (TT = 35) and G-allele carriers (TG = 25). Results Based on the rs-fMRI data, the FC of the right hippocampus and left frontal gyrus was evaluated using group-based comparisons. The corresponding sensitivities and specificities were assessed. Following comparisons between the patient groups, different hippocampal FCs were identified. When compared to TT patients, children with TG had a lower FC between the right precuneus and the right hippocampus, and a higher FC between the right hippocampus and the left middle frontal gyrus. Conclusion The fundamental neurological pathways connecting the SNAP-25 Mnll polymorphism with ADHD via the FC of the hippocampus were newly revealed in this study. As a result, the hippocampal FC may further serve as an imaging biomarker for ADHD.
... Recently, VSTM has been linked to deficits in various disorders such as dyslexia (Stein, 2019), schizophrenia (Yang et al., 2020), ADHD (Ortega et al., 2020), and many others (for a review see Melby-Lervåg & Hulme, 2013). Therefore, some of my findings can be used to inform clinical research. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
The sensory visual cortex (SVC) is involved in encoding information in visual short-term memory (VSTM). Yet, it remains unclear if the SVC is a necessary component of the brain network necessary for maintaining information in VSTM. The aim of this thesis was to shed light on the debated role of the SVC in VSTM. Thus, I focused on transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). TMS uses a coil to transfer electromagnetic stimulation at localized brain areas making the exploration of causal evidence plausible. Through a systematic review and meta-analysis of previous SVC TMS studies I indicated that the SVC is similarly involved in both the encoding and maintenance VSTM phase, and that the controversy was likely due to methodological issues in TMS studies. Building on these findings I conducted two TMS experiments that covered the previous methodological oversights by ensuring the monocular presentation of orientation stimuli. TMS was delivered at different times during the maintenance phase of a delayed change-detection VSTM task, on one side of the occipital hemisphere. Decreased VSTM performance in the ipsilateral occipital hemisphere to visual hemifield, and in the real TMS (compared to sham TMS) condition indicated inhibitory TMS effects, and thus, a causal involvement of the SVC during VSTM maintenance. After establishing the role of the SVC in VSTM maintenance through TMS, I turned to memory load manipulations to further investigate the relationship between short-term memory and perception. I combined short-term memory tasks with perceptual detection tasks, where I manipulated the sensory load of the memory items and measured the effect of this load manipulation on perceptual detection. A combined VSTM and visual perception task provided additional evidence in favor of the sensory recruitment framework, since visual detection was reduced due to the increased VSTM load. Evidence against any cross-modal effects between VSTM and auditory perception was found, and evidence from a combined auditory short-term memory and auditory perception task indicated that sensory recruitment was not supported for the auditory modality. Overall, my findings support the sensory recruitment framework of VSTM, which proposes that sensory visual areas have a dual function: they are involved in the precise sensory encoding of elemental visual features and the short-term maintenance of this information.
Article
Full-text available
This systematic review with narrative synthesis aimed to examine the available evidence on the relationship between neurodiversity and cognitive load in online learning. Despite the known relationship between working memory impairments and neurodiversity, there has been a lack of systematic investigation into how cognitive load impacts neurodivergent students in online learning environments. This review, which includes 90 studies conducted in 21 countries, reveals that the majority (92%) did not consider neurodiversity as a potential factor influencing cognitive load in online learning. The few that did suggest distinct patterns of cognitive load in online learning for neurodivergent students, suggesting a need for more research in this area. The results also point to the potential for methodological advancements in measuring cognitive load in online learning, highlighting that 80% of studies with only neurotypical participants and 60% of studies including neurodivergent participants relied solely on subjective measures of cognitive load. This review contributes to the field by offering a comprehensive overview of our current knowledge of the relationship between neurodiversity and cognitive load in online learning, identifying significant gaps and suggesting directions for future studies.
Article
Full-text available
Working memory is integral to a range of critical cognitive functions such as reasoning and decision‐making. Although alterations in working memory have been observed in neurodivergent populations, there has been no review mapping how cognitive load is measured in common neurodevelopmental conditions such as attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and dyslexia. This scoping review explores the neurophysiological measures used to study cognitive load in these specific populations. Our findings highlight that electroencephalography (EEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) are the most frequently used methods, with a limited number of studies employing functional near‐infrared spectroscopy (fNIRs), magnetoencephalography (MEG) or eye‐tracking. Notably, eye‐related measures are less commonly used, despite their prominence in cognitive load research among neurotypical individuals. The review also highlights potential correlates of cognitive load, such as neural oscillations in the theta and alpha ranges for EEG studies, blood oxygenation level‐dependent (BOLD) responses in lateral and medial frontal brain regions for fMRI and fNIRS studies and eye‐related measures such as pupil dilation and blink rate. Finally, critical issues for future studies are discussed, including the technical challenges associated with multimodal approaches, the possible impact of atypical features on cognitive load measures and balancing data richness with participant well‐being. These insights contribute to a more nuanced understanding of cognitive load measurement in neurodivergent populations and point to important methodological considerations for future neuroscientific research in this area.
Article
Full-text available
Background Research has shown that a fundamental frequency of 40 Hz in continuous neural oscillation is indicative of normal brain activity; in Alzheimer disease (AD) patients, these oscillations either disappear or are significantly interrupted. Research has also indicated that the degenerative impacts of AD in mice were mitigated by the synchronization of 40-Hz acousto-optic stimulation (AOS). Objective To examine the impact of employing a 40-Hz AOS intervention on the induction of a substantial 40-Hz frequency entrainment and improvement in working memory performance among a sample of young individuals in good health. We conduct an analysis of event-related potentials (ERPs) derived from electroencephalogram (EEG) data following the presentation of AOS. Methods We recruited 20 healthy volunteers (median age: 25 years; 8 female subjects). Following the administration of various stimuli, including no stimuli, 40-Hz AOS, pink noise, and 40Hz acoustic stimuli (AS), the participants were required to complete a working memory task. A total of 62 electrodes were used to record EEG data, which was subsequently analyzed to investigate the impact of AOS on the activity of working memory. We also aimed to determine if AOS lead to a more pronounced 40-Hz frequency entrainment. Results Following the administration of AOS, a notable enhancement in the 40-Hz power of pertinent cerebral areas was observed, accompanied by a substantial improvement in the performance of the subjects on working memory tests subsequent to the stimulation. Conclusion The findings unequivocally establish the efficacy of using AOS to enhance the 40-Hz power and working memory.
Article
Full-text available
This review focuses on the cognitive neuroscience of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) based on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies and on recent clinically relevant applications such as fMRI-based diagnostic classification or neuromodulation therapies targeting fMRI deficits with neurofeedback (NF) or brain stimulation. Meta-analyses of fMRI studies of executive functions (EFs) show that ADHD patients have cognitive-domain dissociated complex multisystem impairments in several right and left hemispheric dorsal, ventral and medial fronto-cingulo-striato-thalamic and fronto-parieto-cerebellar networks that mediate cognitive control, attention, timing and working memory (WM). There is furthermore emerging evidence for abnormalities in orbital and ventromedial prefrontal and limbic areas that mediate motivation and emotion control. In addition, poor deactivation of the default mode network (DMN) suggests an abnormal interrelationship between hypo-engaged task-positive and poorly “switched off” hyper-engaged task-negative networks, both of which are related to impaired cognition. Translational cognitive neuroscience in ADHD is still in its infancy. Pattern recognition analyses have attempted to provide diagnostic classification of ADHD using fMRI data with respectable classification accuracies of over 80%. Necessary replication studies, however, are still outstanding. Brain stimulation has been tested in heterogeneously designed, small numbered proof of concept studies targeting key frontal functional impairments in ADHD. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) appears to be promising to improve ADHD symptoms and cognitive functions based on some studies, but larger clinical trials of repeated stimulation with and without cognitive training are needed to test clinical efficacy and potential costs on non-targeted functions. Only three studies have piloted NF of fMRI-based frontal dysfunctions in ADHD using fMRI or near-infrared spectroscopy, with the two larger ones finding some improvements in cognition and symptoms, which, however, were not superior to the active control conditions, suggesting potential placebo effects. Neurotherapeutics seems attractive for ADHD due to their safety and potential longer-term neuroplastic effects, which drugs cannot offer. However, they need to be thoroughly tested for short- and longer-term clinical and cognitive efficacy and their potential for individualized treatment.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Numerous meta-analyses have summarized studies comparing the neurocognitive performance of individuals with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) to that of healthy controls. Method: The present study is a systematic review and quantitative summary of those meta-analyses that aimed to determine the extent to which individuals with ADHD differ cognitively from typically developing controls. Results: Of 253 standardized mean differences (SMDs) drawn from 34 meta-analyses, 244 (96%) were positive, indicating better neurocognitive performance in the control group than the ADHD group. The mean effect size was .45 (SD = .27). Unweighted means of SMDs for neurocognitive domains ranged from .35 (set shifting) to .54 (working memory). When weighted by the number of studies aggregated, they ranged from .35 (set shifting) to .66 (reaction time variability). Neurocognitive domains with mean effects over .50 included working memory (.54), reaction time variability (.53), response inhibition (.52), intelligence/achievement (.51), and planning/organization (.51). When weighted by number of aggregated studies, the domains with mean effects over .50 were reaction time variability (.66), intelligence/achievement (.60), vigilance (.56), working memory (.54), and response inhibition (.52). Age moderated the relationship between ADHD diagnosis and neurocognitive functioning, with greater between-groups differences among children and adults than among adolescents. Funding also moderated this relationship: meta-analyses that received drug funding found larger effect sizes than those without drug funding. Conclusions: The evidence suggests that ADHD is associated with substantial deficits across a variety of neurocognitive domains. This is the most in-depth review of the neurocognitive functioning of people with ADHD to date.
Article
Full-text available
Visual short-term memory binding tasks are a promising early marker for Alzheimer's disease (AD). To uncover functional deficits of AD in these tasks it is meaningful to first study unimpaired brain function. Electroencephalogram recordings were obtained from encoding and maintenance periods of tasks performed by healthy young volunteers. We probe the task's transient physiological underpinnings by contrasting shape only (Shape) and shape-colour binding (Bind) conditions, displayed in the left and right sides of the screen, separately. Particularly, we introduce and implement a novel technique named Modular Dirichlet Energy (MDE) which allows robust and flexible analysis of the functional network with unprecedented temporal precision. We find that connectivity in the Bind condition is less integrated with the global network than in the Shape condition in occipital and frontal modules during the encoding period of the right screen condition. Using MDE we are able to discern driving effects in the occipital module between 100-140ms, coinciding with the P100 visually evoked potential, followed by a driving effect in the frontal module between 140-180ms, suggesting that the differences found constitute an information processing difference between these modules. This provides temporally precise information over a heterogeneous population in promising tasks for the detection of AD.
Article
This study aimed to analyze performance on measures of neuropsychological and behavioral executive functions (EF) in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and to evaluate the utility of performance-based tests for predicting scores on behavioral EF ratings. One hundred eighteen adolescents (75 ADHD and 43 controls) aged 12–16 years performed neuropsychological tests and completed a behavior rating scale of EF. The ADHD group presented significantly lower scores than controls on Full Scale IQ (FSIQ) and all indexes of the WISC-IV, except the verbal comprehension index (VCI). The ADHD group had significantly lower scores on performance-based tests of working memory, planning and inhibition, and on EF rating scales. Scores on the cognitive EF working memory, planning and flexibility modestly predicted performance on behavioral EF. The results suggest that the combined use of performance-based tests and rating scales provides valuable complementary information that can improve the assessment of executive domains in ADHD.
Article
Background: Alzheimer's disease (AD) as a disconnection syndrome disrupts both brain information sharing and memory binding functions. The extent to which these two phenotypic expressions are shared pathophysiological mechanisms remains unknown. Objective: To unveil the electrophysiological correlates of integrative memory impairments in AD towards new memory biomarkers for its prodromal stages. Methods: Patients with 100% risk of familial AD (FAD) and healthy controls underwent assessment with the VSTM binding test (VSTMBT) while we recorded their EEG. We applied a novel brain connectivity method (Weighted Symbolic Mutual Information) to EEG data. Results: Patients showed significant deficits during the VSTMBT. A reduction of brain connectivity was observed during resting as well as during correct VSTM binding, particularly over frontal and posterior regions. An increase of connectivity was found during VSTM binding performance over central regions. While decreased connectivity was found in cases in more advanced stages of FAD, increased brain connectivity appeared in cases in earlier stages. Such altered patterns of task-related connectivity were found in 89% of the assessed patients. Conclusions: VSTM binding in the prodromal stages of FAD are associated to altered patterns of brain connectivity thus confirming the link between integrative memory deficits and impaired brain information sharing in prodromal FAD. While significant loss of brain connectivity seems to be a feature of the advanced stages of FAD increased brain connectivity characterizes its earlier stages. These findings are discussed in the light of recent proposals about the earliest pathophysiological mechanisms of AD and their clinical expression.
Article
Working memory (WM) has limited capacity. This leaves attention with the important role of allowing into storage only the most relevant information. It is increasingly evident that attention is equally crucial for prioritizing representations within WM as the importance of individual items changes. Retrospective prioritization has been proposed to result from a focus of internal attention highlighting one of several representations. Here, we suggest an updated model, in which prioritization acts in multiple steps: first orienting towards and selecting a memory, and then reconfiguring its representational state in the service of upcoming task demands. Reconfiguration sets up an optimized perception-action mapping, obviating the need for sustained attention. This view is consistent with recent literature, makes testable predictions, and links WM with task switching and action preparation.
Article
Deficits in visual short-term memory (VSTM) binding have been proposed as an early and specific marker for Alzheimer's disease (AD). However, no studies have explored the neural correlates of this domain in clinical categories involving prodromal stages with different risk levels of conversion to AD. We assessed underlying electrophysiological modulations in patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), patients in the MCI stages of familial AD carrying the mutation E280A of the presenilin-1 gene (MCI-FAD), and healthy controls. Moreover, we compared the behavioral performance and neural correlates of both patient groups. Participants completed a change-detection VSTM task assessing recognition of changes between shapes or shape-color bindings, presented in two consecutive arrays (i.e., study and test) while event related potentials (ERPs) were recorded. Changes always occurred in the test array and consisted of new features replacing studied features (shape only) or features swapping across items (shape-color binding). Both MCI and MCI-FAD patients performed worse than controls in the shape-color binding condition. Early electrophysiological activity (100-250 ms) was significantly reduced in both clinical groups, particularly over fronto-central and parieto-occipital regions. However, shape-color binding performance and their reduced neural correlates were similar between MCI and MCI-FAD. Our results support the validity of the VSTM binding test and their neural correlates in the early detection of AD and highlight the importance of studies comparing samples at different risk for AD conversion. The combined analysis of behavioral and ERP data gleaned with the VSTM binding task can offer a valuable memory biomarker for AD.
Article
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) frequently persists into adulthood. A reduction in visual short-term memory (vSTM) storage capacity was recently suggested as a potential neuro-cognitive endophenotype, i.e., a testable marker of an individual’s liability for developing ADHD. This study aimed at identifying markers of the brain abnormalities underlying vSTM reductions in adult ADHD. We combined behavioral parameter-based assessment with electrophysiology in groups of adult ADHD patients and healthy age matched controls. Amplitudes of ERP markers of vSTM storage capacity, the contralateral delay activity (CDA) and the P3b, were analyzed according to (i) differences between individuals with higher vs. lower storage capacity K and (ii) differences between ADHD patients and control participants. We replicated the finding of reduced storage capacity in adult ADHD. Across groups, individuals with higher relative to lower storage capacity showed a larger CDA and P3b. We further found differences between the patient and control groups in the ERPs: The CDA amplitude was attenuated in an early time window for ADHD patients compared to control participants, and was negatively correlated with ADHD patients’ symptom severity ratings. Furthermore, the P3b was larger in ADHD patients relative to control participants. These electrophysiological findings indicate altered brain mechanisms underlying visual storage capacity in ADHD, which are characterized by deficient encoding and maintenance, and increased recruitment of control processes. Accordingly, (quantifiable) ERP markers of vSTM in adult ADHD bear candidacy as neuro-cognitive endophenotypes of the disease.