ArticlePDF Available

A Meta-Analysis of Self-Determination Theory-Informed Intervention Studies in the Health Domain: Effects on Motivation, Health Behavior, Physical, and Psychological Health

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

There are no literature reviews that have examined the impact of health-domain interventions, informed by self-determination theory (SDT; Ryan & Deci, 2017), on SDT constructs and health indices. Our aim was to meta-analyze such interventions in the health promotion and disease management literatures. Studies were eligible if they used an experimental design, tested an intervention that was based on SDT, measured at least one SDT-based motivational construct, and at least one indicator of health behavior, physical health, or psychological health. Seventy-three studies met these criteria and provided sufficient data for the purposes of the review. A random-effects meta-analytic model showed that SDT-based interventions produced small-to-medium changes in most SDT constructs at the end of the intervention period, and in health behaviors at the end of the intervention period and at the follow-up. Small positive changes in physical and psychological health outcomes were also observed at the end of the interventions. Increases in need support and autonomous motivation (but not controlled motivation or amotivation) were associated with positive changes in health behavior. In conclusion, SDT-informed interventions positively affect indices of health; these effects are modest, heterogeneous, and partly due to increases in self-determined motivation and support from social agents.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rhpr20
Health Psychology Review
ISSN: 1743-7199 (Print) 1743-7202 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rhpr20
A meta-analysis of self-determination theory-
informed intervention studies in the health
domain: effects on motivation, health behavior,
physical, and psychological health
Nikos Ntoumanis, Johan Y.Y. Ng, Andrew Prestwich, Eleanor Quested, Jennie
E. Hancox, Cecilie Thøgersen-Ntoumani, Edward L. Deci, Richard M. Ryan,
Chris Lonsdale & Geoffrey C. Williams
To cite this article: Nikos Ntoumanis, Johan Y.Y. Ng, Andrew Prestwich, Eleanor Quested, Jennie
E. Hancox, Cecilie Thøgersen-Ntoumani, Edward L. Deci, Richard M. Ryan, Chris Lonsdale &
Geoffrey C. Williams (2020): A meta-analysis of self-determination theory-informed intervention
studies in the health domain: effects on motivation, health behavior, physical, and psychological
health, Health Psychology Review, DOI: 10.1080/17437199.2020.1718529
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2020.1718529
© 2020 The Author(s). Published by Informa
UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis
Group
Accepted author version posted online: 27
Jan 2020.
Published online: 03 Feb 2020.
Submit your article to this journal Article views: 72
View related articles View Crossmark data
A meta-analysis of self-determination theory-informed
intervention studies in the health domain: eects on motivation,
health behavior, physical, and psychological health
Nikos Ntoumanis
a,b
, Johan Y.Y. Ng
c
, Andrew Prestwich
d
, Eleanor Quested
a,b
, Jennie
E. Hancox
e
, Cecilie Thøgersen-Ntoumani
a,b
, Edward L. Deci
f,g
, Richard M. Ryan
h
,
Chris Lonsdale
h
and Georey C. Williams
i
a
School of Psychology, Curtin University, Perth, Australia;
b
Physical Activity and Well-Being Group, Curtin University,
Perth, Australia;
c
Department of Sports Science and Physical Education, Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong
Kong;
d
School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK;
e
Division of Primary Care, School of Medicine, University
of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK;
f
Department of Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, USA;
g
School of
Management, University of South-east Norway, Oslo, Norway;
h
Institute for Positive Psychology and Education,
Australian Catholic University, Sydney, Australia;
i
Department of Medicine, Psychology, and Psychiatry, Center for
Community Health and Prevention, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, USA
ABSTRACT
There are no literature reviews that have examined the impact of health-
domain interventions, informed by self-determination theory (SDT), on
SDT constructs and health indices. Our aim was to meta-analyse such
interventions in the health promotion and disease management
literatures. Studies were eligible if they used an experimental design,
tested an intervention that was based on SDT, measured at least one
SDT-based motivational construct, and at least one indicator of health
behaviour, physical health, or psychological health. Seventy-three
studies met these criteria and provided sucient data for the purposes
of the review. A random-eects meta-analytic model showed that SDT-
based interventions produced small-to-medium changes in most SDT
constructs at the end of the intervention period, and in health
behaviours at the end of the intervention period and at the follow-up.
Small positive changes in physical and psychological health outcomes
were also observed at the end of the interventions. Increases in need
support and autonomous motivation (but not controlled motivation or
amotivation) were associated with positive changes in health behaviour.
In conclusion, SDT-informed interventions positively aect indices of
health; these eects are modest, heterogeneous, and partly due to
increases in self-determined motivation and support from social agents.
ARTICLE HISTORY
Received 5 November 2019
Accepted 16 January 2020
KEYWORDS
Need support; psychological
needs; autonomous
motivation; wellness
Applications of Self-Determination Theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2017) in the health
domain have increased substantially in the last 15 years. Although the majority of early SDT-based
studies employed observational designs, in recent years there has been a considerable increase in
the volume of intervention studies that aim to foster health-conducive behaviours (e.g., increased
physical activity, healthy eating, abstaining from use of tobacco) or support health treatments
(e.g., medication adherence, diabetes self-management). Such intervention studies are needed,
© 2020 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the
original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way.
CONTACT Nikos Ntoumanis nikos.ntoumanis@curtin.edu.au
Supplemental data for this article can be accessed https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2020.1718529
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW
https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2020.1718529
given the diculty people have in initiating and maintaining healthy behaviours over time, and the
alarming global statistics on the causes of ill-health. For example, in 2018, the World Health Organ-
ization estimated that non-communicable diseases accounted for 71% of worldwide deaths in 2016.
The vast majority of deaths attributed to non-communicable diseases were caused by cardiovascular
disease (44%), cancer (22%), chronic respiratory disease (9%), and diabetes (4%). Changes in lifestyle
can prevent or delay the onset of these diseases, improve their management, and increase psycho-
logical wellbeing. Hence, health researchers have used a number of dierent approaches, including
SDT-informed interventions, to support positive changes in health behaviours and, indirectly,
improve physical and psychological health.
A brief overview of SDT
Both biomedical ethics (Beauchamp & Childress, 2009) and medical professionalism (Project of the
ABIM Foundation, ACP-ASIM Foundation, and European Federation of Internal Medicine, 2002)
have elevated personal autonomy to the highest-level outcome of health care, equivalent to enhan-
cing patient well-being and social justice. Such developments make SDT-based interventions that
intend to enhance personal self-determination highly relevant to health care.
According to Ryan and Deci (2017), human behaviours are inuenced to a great extent by personal
and contextual motivational factors. With regard to personal factors, experimental applications of
SDT in the health domain have focused on two: types of motivation and psychological needs. Motiv-
ation has been conceptualised and measured within SDT as a multifaceted construct with several
regulatory styles lying on a continuum of relative autonomy or self-determination (e.g., see Fig. 8.1
in Ryan & Deci, 2017). On the self-determined end of this continuum is intrinsic motivation, reecting
behavioural engagement as a result of enjoyment and personal interest in the behaviour. In contrast,
extrinsic motivation is comprised of several regulatory styles that are varied in their relative auton-
omy. Specically, integrated and identied regulations, although extrinsic motivations, are highly
self-determined regulatory styles. Integrated regulation represents reasons for behavioural enact-
ment that align with ones identity and core values; identied regulation refers to motivation stem-
ming from personal values and endorsement of a behaviour or its outcomes. For example, a person
might comply with a dicult regimen of diet and physical activity because he or she understands and
endorses its value for long-term health. The behaviours entailed would not be intrinsically motivated,
but would be autonomous and experienced as volitional. However, extrinsic motivation can have
controlled forms of regulation. The rst one, introjected regulation concerns being motivated by con-
tingent self-esteem and desire for self-or other-approval. The second controlled type of extrinsic
motivation is external regulation, which is the least self-determined as it represents behaviours motiv-
ated by external pressures or contingent rewards.
These diverse regulatory styles are applicable in the health domain as individuals can be motiv-
ated to engage in a health behaviour (e.g., be more physically active) for a diverse array of
reasons, including enjoyment of exercise, experiencing its health benets, avoiding letting oneself
or others down by not exercising, or being pressured by a spouse or a health professional to be
active. Lastly, in addition to intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, Ryan and Deci (2017) identied amo-
tivation, a state in which individuals lack any type of intention or motivation to engage in a given
behaviour. Typically, researchers in the SDT literature in the health domain have either measured
each of the aforementioned regulations separately (e.g., Wilson, Rodgers, Blanchard, & Gessell,
2006), or have combined them into composites for autonomous motivation (intrinsic, integrated,
and identied regulations) and controlled motivation (introjected and external regulations; e.g.,
Rouse, Duda, Ntoumanis, Jolly, & Williams, 2016), or used indices representing relative autonomous
motivation (autonomous minus controlled motivation) (e.g., Duda et al., 2014).
The second personal motivational dimension studied in the SDT-applications literature for health
is that of basic psychological needs. Three key needs have been identied by Ryan and Deci (2017):
autonomy (feel a sense of choice about ones behaviour); competence (being able to bring about
2N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
positive changes in desired outcomes); and relatedness (feeling accepted by ones social milieu). By
and large, the majority of SDT-based work in the health domain has studied how the satisfaction of
these three psychological needs predicts autonomous motivation, adaptive behaviours, and health
(e.g., Kinnack, Thogersen-Ntoumani, & Duda, 2016), although there is growing research on how
the frustration of these needs can result in controlled motivation, amotivation, and ill-being (e.g.,
Ng, Ntoumanis, Thogersen-Ntoumani, Stott, & Hindle, 2013). Some of the work in the health
domain (e.g., Williams, Freedman, & Deci, 1998) has used the term perceived competenceinstead
of competence need satisfaction; however, from a measurement perspective, the two constructs
have been operationalised in very similar ways.
Ryan and Deci (2017) have also highlighted the role of social environments in supporting or
thwarting one or more of the three psychological needs, and in turn inuencing the degree to
which motivation is autonomous, and concomitant health behaviours and health-related outcomes
are positive. A broad distinction has been made between behaviours of signicant others (e.g., health
practitioners, romantic partners, parents) that are supportive of the three aforementioned needs, and
behaviours that thwart such needs. For example, a health practitioner can support weight loss
attempts by oering meaningful choices, providing positive and informative feedback, and
empathising with and acknowledging the patients perspective. In contrast, a parent can try to encou-
rage his/her overweight child to lose weight by using pressure, conditionally accepting the child, or
oering tangible rewards if the child agrees to sign up for a weight loss programme. Interventions
stemming from SDT have focused on enhancing perceptions of need support, often by training sig-
nicant others to utilise behaviours that facilitate experiences of psychological need satisfaction and
foster self-determined motivation for behavioural engagement (e.g., Ntoumanis, Thogersen-Ntou-
mani, Quested, & Hancox, 2017).
Reviews of SDT applications in the health domain
Ng et al. (2012) published the rst meta-analysis of applications of SDT in the health domain. They
identied 184 independent datasets, primarily non-experimental studies. The included studies exam-
ined relations between SDT constructs and health behaviours (e.g., physical activity, smoking absti-
nence), and indices of health (e.g., dental hygiene, depression, quality of life). The identied eect
sizes ranged from small to medium. Ng et al. also tested a path model utilising meta-analysed cor-
relations, based on a conceptual model by Ryan, Patrick, Deci, and Williams (2008). Results showed
that perceptions of autonomy support predicted reports of autonomy (β= .41), competence (β
= .33), and relatedness (β= .47) need satisfaction. In turn, the three psychological needs predicted
autonomous motivation, although the standardised beta coecient was substantially larger for com-
petence (β= .35) than those for autonomy (β= .13), and relatedness (β= .15). Competence also had
direct eects on psychological health (β= .39) and physical health (β= .20). The eects of auton-
omous motivation on psychological health (β= .06) and physical health (β= .11) were small.
Overall, competence emerged as the major predictor of motivation and health outcomes in the
path analysis. A potential limitation of the Ng et al. (2012) meta-analysis is that it combined
indices of physical health with health behaviours. Another limitation was that it included many
non-experimental studies. Experimental design was a moderator with respect to the eect sizes
between autonomy and physical activity, autonomy and intrinsic motivation, and autonomy and
external regulation; experimental studies had larger eect sizes than non-experimental studies.
A more recent review of the SDT literature by Gillison, Rouse, Standage, Sebire, and Ryan (2019)
meta-analysed 74 intervention studies to promote motivation and need satisfaction for health behav-
iour change. The results of eect size calculations showed that such interventions resulted in changes
in autonomy support (g= 0.84), autonomy satisfaction (g= 0.81), competence satisfaction (g= 0.63),
relatedness satisfaction (g= 0.28), and autonomous motivation (g= 0.41). Gillison et al. also coded the
included studies in terms of use of 18 SDT-based techniques (e.g., choice, provision of meaningful
rationales) to promote need satisfaction. Meta-regressions did not identify particular strategies
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 3
that induced meaningful changes in need satisfaction; hence, the authors concluded that a combi-
nation of such strategies might be necessary to promote need satisfaction. The meta-analysis by Gil-
lison et al. did not calculate eect sizes pertaining to some important SDT-based constructs
(controlled motivation and amotivation) or associated health-behaviour, physical-health, or psycho-
logical-health outcomes. In fact, several of the included studies had only motivation-related variables
as outcomes. Further, the meta-analysis by Gillison et al. did not identify the extent to which changes
in SDT-based constructs were associated with changes in behavioural, physical, or psychological
health outcomes. Lastly, the meta-analysis by Gillison et al. included studies from sport in which
the emphasis was on performance and not on health (e.g., Fransen, Boen, Vansteenkiste, Mertens,
& Vande Broek, 2017) and did not establish the robustness of possible moderators by taking into
account potential confounding between moderators.
Aims of the present study
Advancing the SDT literature in the health domain, we present a meta-analysis of experimental
studies in that context. We extended both the Ng et al. (2012) and Gillison et al. (2019) meta-analyses
by addressing the limitations identied above. We included experimental studies that tested changes
in at least one SDT variable and at least one health-behaviour, physical-health outcome, or psycho-
logical-health outcome. Our primary aim was to calculate eect sizes pertaining to changes in these
variables at the end of the intervention and at the latest follow-up reported in the studies. Based on
the two aforementioned reviews, we hypothesised that SDT interventions would produce medium to
large eect sizes in changes in perceptions of need support and competence; small to medium eect
sizes in changes in autonomy satisfaction, relatedness satisfaction, and autonomous motivation; and
small eect sizes in changes in controlled motivation, health behaviours, and health outcomes. We
also tested, via meta-regression, whether changes in SDT constructs would be associated with
changes in health behaviours, physical- and psychological-health outcomes. We hypothesised
small eect sizes for such associations. In an exploratory fashion, we also aimed to test, via meta-
regressions, predictors of heterogeneity in such eect sizes, such as specic features of the SDT-
based interventions (e.g., the use of specic autonomy, competence, and relatedness supportive
strategies). We also coded 43 behaviour change techniques (BCTs), using the taxonomy proposed
by Michie et al. (2013), to examine whether the moderating role of SDT-based strategies was con-
founded with the co-delivery of specic BCTs. Further sensitivity analyses accounted for the potential
impact of outliers.
Method
Eligibility criteria
We aimed to include all experimental studies testing an intervention based on tenets of SDT to
improve behaviours or outcomes related to the physical and psychological health of participants.
Specically, studies were eligible if they (1) used an experimental design, such as randomised con-
trolled trials or quasi-experimental studies; (2) tested an intervention that, according to the
authors, was (partially) designed according to SDT principles of motivation and behaviour change;
(3) measured at least one SDT-based motivational construct, and at least one of the following: a
health behaviour (e.g., physical activity), an indicator of physical health (e.g., glycosylated hemo-
globin, HbA1c) or psychological health (e.g., perceived quality of life), at a time point which occurred
after the completion of the intervention. We excluded studies that used SDT-based measures but
employed Motivational Interviewing (Miller & Rollnick, 1991) as their guiding framework, with no
reference to SDT. For a study to be included, the authors had to explicitly mention SDT as the
guiding conceptual framework. SDT and Motivational Interviewing share many similarities, but
there are still issues of contention and debate (e.g., see Deci & Ryan, 2012). No exclusion criteria
4N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
on publication date or language were employed. However, studies that only employed a qualitative
approach, and therefore did not include any quantitative data, were excluded. Systematic reviews
and other meta-analyses were also excluded. Published journal articles, conference proceedings,
theses/dissertations, and unpublished studies were eligible. For papers that did not include the infor-
mation needed for our analyses (e.g., protocol papers), we contacted the authors directly to request
further details.
Information sources
Database searches were conducted on Medline, PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, and PubMed. The nal
search was completed in November 2018. We also posted a message on the SDT email listserv to
request unpublished studies and scanned reference lists of included studies.
Search
We applied two sets of lters in the database search. Both lters were applied to search for terms in
the titles and abstracts of papers within the databases. The rst lter was used to identify studies with
an experimental design (experiment* OR trial* OR manipulat* OR intervention). The second lter was
applied to identify studies that included SDT-based interventions (self-determination theory OR
intrinsic motivation OR basic needs OR basic psychological needs OR autonomy support OR auton-
omy supportive OR need support OR need supportive OR need of autonomy OR need for autonomy
OR autonomy need OR self-determined motivation OR autonomous motivation OR autonomous self-
regulation OR autonomous regulation OR need of competence OR need for competence OR compe-
tence need OR need of relatedness OR need for relatedness OR relatedness need).
Study selection
Information and the full text (if available) of all studies identied in the database search were
imported into a bibliography management software. After removing duplicated studies, a trained
research assistant screened the studies manually and removed studies that did not meet our
inclusion criteria. Our data set is available at https://osf.io/u8csb
Data collection process and data items
The included studies were coded using a data extraction sheet, initially piloted by three authors of the
paper using ten randomly selected studies identied via the database search. The extraction sheet
was modied after the pilot to clarify ambiguity in the coding protocol. The revised version was
then used to code all included studies. Drawing from a pool of three reviewers, all studies were
coded independently by at least two of those reviewers. Discrepancies were resolved following dis-
cussion among the coders. The data extracted included year of publication, study design, number of
treatment conditions and how the intervention across conditions diered, intervention duration,
venue (e.g., school, clinic) and mode (e.g., face-to-face, phone conversations) of intervention delivery,
contact frequencies and durations, background and training of intervention providers, constructs
measured in the study, and participant demographics (i.e., mean age, percentages of males and
females). We also initially coded for frequency and duration of intervention contact, but encountered
some diculties in doing so in a systematic manner (e.g., in some studies participants could access
online information according to their own schedule). Hence, we decided not to include intervention
duration and frequency in our analyses.
The theoretical underpinning and the BCTs used in the intervention and comparison conditions
were also coded. Based on our knowledge of the SDT literature and earlier stages of a consensus
eort to build a classication of techniques used in SDT-informed interventions in the health
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 5
domain (Teixeira et al., 2019), we designed a brief grouping for 17 common need supportive beha-
viours or techniques that were applied in the meta-analysed studies (Gillison et al., 2019, also devel-
oped a grouping of 18 SDT techniques, which only partially overlaps with ours, as both lines of work
developed independently). We categorised the 17 techniques as competence-, autonomy-, or relat-
edness-supportive (with seven, six, and four strategies, respectively; see Supplementary File Table S1).
Behaviour change techniques used in these interventions were also coded using Michie et al.s(2013)
taxonomy. The need supportive and BCT components of included studies were independently coded
by three researchers (two of them also piloted the coding form); each study was coded by at least two
of those researchers. Coding by individual researchers demonstrated substantial agreementacross
coders for need-supportive behaviours (Kappa = .723 p< .001; Landis & Koch, 1977) and moderate
agreementfor BCTs (Kappa = .508, p< .001); the kappa for BCTs is typical for the BCT literature
(e.g., Michie et al., 2015). Discrepancies were discussed and reconciled.
Risk of bias assessment
The risk of bias of primary studies was assessed using an adapted version of the Cochrane Risk of Bias
Tool (Higgins, Altman, & Sterne, 2011). Specically, the degree of risk of bias was assessed based on
(1) generation of randomisation sequence; (2) concealment of group allocation; (3) blinding of (i) par-
ticipants, (ii) individuals responsible for data collection, (iii) researcher(s) who analysed the data, and
(iv) intervention providers; (4) handling of incomplete or missing data; (5) selective reporting of
results; and (6) any other potential threats to the accuracy of the results.
Summary measures
Analyses were conducted with Stata (version15; StataCorp., 2017) using a random-eects model.
Hedgesgwas used to reect eect sizes of comparisons between the experimental and comparison
conditions. Absolute values of gbetween 0.20.5 are considered small, 0.50.8 are medium, and over
0.8 are large (Cohen, 1988).
Synthesis of results
When a study included multiple intervention conditions, Hedgesgwas calculated by comparing the
group receiving the most versus the group receiving the least SDT-based need supportive com-
ponents (based on the coded information). We conducted two separate sets of analyses for outcomes
measured (1) immediately after the completion of the intervention, and (2) at follow-up time points
after the completion of the intervention. When a study measured outcomes at multiple post-inter-
vention follow-up time points, only data from the nal time point was used. When pre-intervention
data were available, eect sizes were adjusted for baseline values. If the primary studies contained
multiple eect sizes under any category, they were combined using methods recommended by Bor-
enstein, Hedges, Higgins, and Rothstein (2011). This step requires the use of correlation coecients
between the constructs; if these coecients were not available from the original studies, an estimate
of r= .50 was used. Further, sample size adjustments, using intraclass correlation coecients, were
applied when clustered designs were used (Borenstein et al., 2011). If an intraclass correlation in a
study was unavailable, a value of 0.05 was used for the adjustment (Michie, Abraham, Whittington,
McAteer, & Gupta, 2009).
To test whether SDT-constructs, health behaviours, physical health, and psychological health can
be changed, separate analyses were conducted for (1) perceived need support (overall or combined
across specic need-support dimensions, depending on what was reported in the primary studies),
(2) psychological need satisfaction (i.e., competence, autonomy, relatedness; overall or combined
across the three needs), (3) autonomous motivation (average of intrinsic motivation, integrated regu-
lation and identied regulation, or composite autonomous motivation scores), (4) controlled
6N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
motivation (average of introjected regulation and external regulation, or composite controlled motiv-
ation), (5) amotivation, (6) health behaviour outcomes (e.g., physical activity, tobacco abstinence), (7)
physical health outcomes (e.g., HbA1c, blood pressure), (8) psychological health outcomes (e.g.,
quality of life, depression). In all analyses, positive gvalues represent more positive changes in the
experimental group over the comparison group.
To test whether changes in SDT-related constructs engender changes in other SDT-related con-
structs, health behaviour, physical health and psychological health, a set of meta-regressions were
conducted. To this end, eect sizes of the interventions on the SDT-related constructs were used
as predictors of eect sizes of the interventions for behavioural or health outcomes.
Identifying and exploring heterogeneity
Heterogeneity of synthesised eect sizes was explored using the Qand I
2
statistics. Specically, a sig-
nicant Qand an I
2
value close to 100% would suggest heterogeneity. In such cases, the eects of
potential moderators were tested using meta-regressions.
We conducted meta-regressions with each need supportive technique and the BCTs utilised in the
included studies (Michie et al., 2013) as predictor variables. A set of meta-regressions examined
whether the relative presence of a specic need-supportive technique or BCT was associated with
larger or smaller eect sizes. Three variables, one each for competence, autonomy, or relatedness,
were created and coded as follows: if competence, autonomy, or relatedness-need support tech-
niques were applied only in the intervention condition (+1), in both or neither groups (0) or only
in the comparison condition (1). These three variables were summed to create a further variable
reecting the total range of need supports applied in the intervention vs. comparison conditions
(coded as +3 to 3). Another set of three variables were created to indicate relative autonomy-, com-
petence-, or relatedness-need support between the two comparison groups, by summing the
number of competence-, autonomy-, or relatedness-supportive techniques (from the list of 17),
respectively, present in the intervention condition and subtracting the equivalent number in the
comparison conditions. Finally, the dierence in overall need support in the comparison condition
was subtracted from the overall need support in the intervention condition. The meta-regressions
for competence, autonomy, and relatedness support as predictors were conducted separately
because there was insucient statistical power to include multiple predictors (i.e., less than 30
eect sizes included, therefore, the ratio of eect size to number of covariates would be smaller
than 101; see Borenstein et al., 2011).
The impact of a range of other moderator variables were also considered using meta-regressions,
including the study design (randomised controlled designs versus quasi-experimental designs), pub-
lication type (journal article versus theses/unpublished dataset/conference abstract), intervention
provider (investigators: yes vs. no/unclear; trained trainers: yes vs. no/unclear), mode of delivery
(e.g., face-to-face component: yes vs. no/unclear), treatment duration (in days), participant character-
istics (mean age; percentage of male participants), risk of bias (e.g., allocation sequence concealed:
yes vs. no/unclear).
Small-study bias
Small-study bias is suggested when observed eect sizes increase with smaller sample sizes (and thus
larger standard errors). A potential cause underlying this bias is publication bias (where the likelihood
of publication is aected by the results of studies). Small-study bias was examined using Eggers test.
Sensitivity analyses
Sensitivity analyses were applied to examine the robustness of the synthesised results. To test the
potential impact of outliers, analyses were repeated by removing outliers. After calculating
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 7
Sample-Adjusted Meta-Analytic Deviancy (Hucutt & Arthur, 1995) scores for each study, potentially
outlying studies were detected on resulting scree-plots (see Supplementary File Figures S1S22). This
approach identies the inuence of each study on the overall eect size by calculating the eect size
without the study present and takes into account the sample size of the study. We also examined
whether any of the BCTs were associated with the eect sizes from individual studies. If this was
found, chi-square analyses (Fishers Exact Test, when appropriate) tested whether the signicant
BCTs were associated with the signicant moderators. Where associations were detected, multi-
variate meta-regressions in which the previously identied moderators were entered alongside
each related BCT were conducted to examine whether the moderators remained signicant. The
results from the main analyses were considered to be robust if the sensitivity analyses did not
yield results that led to dierent conclusions.
Results
Study selection
Using our database search protocol, 2,622 citations were identied. An additional journal article was
included from our request sent through the SDT email listserv. Ten other studies were included via
personal contacts with authors in the eld. After the removal of 994 duplicated items, our initial pool
consisted of 1,639 publications. A trained research assistant ltered the list to 77 entries by reading
the full text of publications and discarding irrelevant ones. Two studies were excluded from the nal
publication pool, as the statistical information required for our analyses was not available in the pub-
lished document, and we were unable to collect the required data from the authors. Another two
studies were excluded because the results were based on duplicated datasets in other included
papers. Therefore, the nal publication pool included 73 studies (there were no studies with multiple
datasets); see Figure 1 for the PRISMA owchart.
Figure 1. PRISMA owchart of study selection.
8N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
Study characteristics
Of the 73 included studies, 68 were published journal articles, three were PhD theses, one was a con-
ference abstract, and one was an unpublished study. In terms of study design, 58 studies used a ran-
domised controlled design, with 20 of these using clusters as the unit of randomisation. The
remaining 15 studies used a quasi-experimental design. A total of 30,088 participants were included
in these studies (average sample size = 412), with approximately 36.6% of participants being male.
Mean age of participants was 35.4 years (ranging from 10.1 to 82.5 years). The experimental
groups included on average 7.4 (SD = 4.6) additional SDT-based strategies relative to the control
groups. There was a large range in the duration over which the intervention was delivered (mean
= 133.4 days; SD = 180.3 days). The nal follow-up period ranged from one week to 30 months
post-intervention. The characteristics of each study are summarised in Supplementary File Table
S2. The majority of studies reported adequate randomisation procedures (74.0%), allocation conceal-
ment (60.3%), adequate handling of incomplete data (76.7%), and were free from selective outcome
reporting (79.5%). However, only a few studies blinded key personnel to study condition (Participants:
26.0% of the included studies; Data Collector: 11.0%; Data Analyzer: 8.2%; Intervention Provider:
4.1%). An overview of the risk of bias for each study is presented in Supplementary File Table S3.
The breakdown of specic health behaviours, physical health, and psychological health outcomes
coded in the meta-analysis is reported in Supplementary File Table S4.
Can interventions enhance SDT constructs?
The results suggest that the following constructs were positively changed, based on assessments
taken at the end of intervention (see Table 1): need support g= 0.64; competence g= 0.31; autonomy
g= 0.37, combined need satisfaction g= 0.37; and autonomous motivation g= 0.30. Overall, there
was no eect of the interventions on relatedness (g= 0.20), controlled motivation (g= 0.07), or amo-
tivation (g=0.07). At follow-up, the eect sizes for need support (g= 1.13), competence (g= 0.55),
and combined need satisfaction (g= 0.49) were larger than the corresponding eect sizes at the end
of the intervention, but had a very wide condence interval and consequently were not signicant.
However, following the removal of outliers on the competence (g= 0.33) and combined need
Table 1. Summary of eect sizes and heterogeneity tests for changes in SDT variables, health behaviours, and health outcomes.
kg 95% CI pQpI²
01a. Need support End of intervention 21 0.643 0.354, 0.932 <.01 193.84 <.01 89.7
01b. Need support Follow-up 6 1.129 0.351, 2.609 .13 467.68 <.01 98.9
02a. Competence End of intervention 22 0.306 0.120, 0.493 <.01 134.60 <.01 84.4
02b. Competence Follow-up 11 0.547 0.045, 1.139 .07 417.85 <.01 97.6
03a. Autonomy End of intervention 17 0.370 0.146, 0.595 <.01 90.66 <.01 82.4
03b. Autonomy Follow-up 6 0.250 0.013, 0.512 .06 18.38 <.01 72.8
04a. Relatedness End of intervention 14 0.202 0.041, 0.445 .10 71.51 <.01 81.8
04b. Relatedness Follow-up 6 0.027 0.199, 0.254 .81 13.81 .02 63.8
05a. Combined need satisfaction End of intervention 23 0.369 0.187, 0.550 <.01 199.25 <.01 89.0
05b. Combined need satisfaction Follow-up 11 0.486 0.048, 1.019 .07 473.93 <.01 97.9
06a. Autonomous motivation End of intervention 37 0.296 0.169, 0.424 <.01 146.39 <.01 75.4
06b. Autonomous motivation Follow-up 14 0.181 0.001, 0.362 .05 41.84 <.01 68.9
07a. Controlled motivation End of intervention 18 0.071 0.042, 0.184 .22 30.01 .03 43.4
07b. Controlled motivation Follow-up 6 0.017 0.239, 0.273 .90 16.14 <.01 69.0
08a. Amotivation End of intervention 14 0.070 0.281, 0.140 .51 34.56 <.01 62.4
08b. Amotivation Follow-up 5 0.255 0.535, 0.025 .07 8.56 .07 53.3
09a. Health Behaviour End of intervention 49 0.450 0.329, 0.571 <.01 334.39 <.01 85.6
09b. Health Behaviour Follow-up 28 0.278 0.172, 0.384 <.01 78.08 <.01 65.4
10a. Physical health End of intervention 16 0.042 0.151, 0.234 .67 52.30 <.01 71.3
10b. Physical health Follow-up 14 0.280 0.033, 0.528 .03 174.12 <.01 92.5
11a. Psychological health End of intervention 22 0.294 0.135, 0.452 <.01 78.00 <.01 73.1
11b. Psychological health Follow-up 10 0.137 0.087, 0.361 .23 36.71 <.01 75.5
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 9
satisfaction (g= 0.28) outcomes at follow-up, these eects emerged as signicant (due to reduced
variation), and this was also the case for autonomous motivation (g= 0.22; see Table 2). All other
eect sizes pertaining to changes in SDT constructs at follow-up were non-signicant.
Few intervention characteristics were signicant moderators (see Table 3). Of the need suppor-
tive techniques, studies that utilised the competence supportive technique to be positive that
the individual can succeedgenerated larger increases in controlled motivation and larger
reductions in amotivation, compared to studies that did not. Moreover, these studies achieved
marginally larger increases in need support, autonomy satisfaction, and autonomous motivation,
all of which became signicant following the removal of outliers (need support: B=1.09,SE =0.38,
t=2.88, p= .01; autonomy satisfaction: B=0.73, SE =0.27, t= 2.73, p= .02; autonomous motiv-
ation: B= 0.49, SE = 0.15, t= 2.78, p= .009). Identifying barriers to changewas associated with
increases in autonomous motivation and conveying a person is valuedwas associated with
increases in autonomy satisfaction, reductions in amotivation, and marginal increases in related-
ness satisfaction. Interventions delivered in community settings were more likely to enhance
relatedness and reduce amotivation than interventions delivered elsewhere. There were no
other intervention characteristics that signicantly increased or decreased the magnitude of
the eect sizes for autonomy support, competence satisfaction, autonomy satisfaction, combined
need satisfaction, autonomous motivation, or controlled motivation at conventional levels of sig-
nicance. The above moderator eects were largely robust to the inuence of outliers, with the
exception of two additional eects emerging once outliers were removed: the technique to
provide a meaningful rationalewas positively associated with larger eect sizes for autonomy,
B=.
60, SE = .25, t=2.40, p= .03, and combined need satisfaction, B=.49, SE = .19, t=2.54, p
= .02. Finally, two study quality characteristics signicantly moderated eects: adequate allo-
cation concealment reduced eect sizes representing the eect of the intervention on auton-
omous motivation, while blinding the intervention provider increased the eect of the
intervention on relatedness. Various BCTs were associated with increased eect sizes for
various SDT constructs (see Table 4). The potential confounding roles of these BCTs are con-
sidered in the Sensitivity Analyses section below.
Table 2. Summary of eect sizes and heterogeneity tests for changes in SDT variables, health behaviours, and health outcomes
following outlier removal.
kg 95% CI pQpI²
01a. Need support End of intervention 19 0.739 0.445, 1.033 <.01 149.42 <.01 88.0
01b. Need support Follow-up$ 6 1.129 0.351, 2.609 .13 467.68 <.01 98.9
02a. Competence End of intervention 20 0.267 0.100, 0.435 <.01 90.30 <.01 79.0
02b. Competence Follow-up 10 0.329 0.046, 0.611 .02* 58.08 <.01 84.5
03a. Autonomy End of intervention 16 0.404 0.174, 0.633 <.01 87.30 <.01 82.8
03b. Autonomy Follow-up$ 6 0.250 0.013, 0.512 .06 18.38 <.01 72.8
04a. Relatedness End of intervention 13 0.242 0.008, 0.493 .06 68.69 <.01 82.5
04b. Relatedness Follow-up$ 6 0.027 0.199, 0.254 .81 13.81 .02 63.8
05a. Combined need satisfaction End of intervention 21 0.343 0.172, 0.514 <.01 152.49 <.01 86.9
05b. Combined need satisfaction Follow-up 10 0.276 0.037, 0.514 .02* 60.19 <.01 85.0
06a. Autonomous motivation End of intervention 35 0.334 0.211, 0.457 <.01 116.10 <.01 70.7
06b. Autonomous motivation Follow-up 13 0.223 0.071, 0.375 <.01* 22.2 .04 45.9
07a. Controlled motivation End of intervention$ 18 0.071 0.042, 0.184 .22 30.01 .03 43.4
07b. Controlled motivation Follow-up$ 6 0.017 0.239, 0.273 .90 16.14 <.01 69.0
08a. Amotivation End of intervention 13 0.074 0.257, 0.174 .71 32.27 <.01 62.8
08b. Amotivation Follow-up$ 5 0.255 0.535, 0.025 .07 8.56 .07 53.3
09a. Health Behaviour End of intervention 46 0.402 0.288, 0.515 <.01 221.72 <.01 79.7
09b. Health Behaviour Follow-up 27 0.267 0.163, 0.371 <.01 72.90 <.01 64.3
10a. Physical health End of intervention 15 0.130 0.003, 0.257 .04* 21.22 .10 34.0
10b. Physical health Follow-up 13 0.245 0.012, 0.502 .06 114.64 <.01 89.5
11a. Psychological health End of intervention$ 22 0.294 0.135, 0.452 <.01 78.00 <.01 73.1
11b. Psychological health Follow-up$ 10 0.137 0.087, 0.361 .23 36.71 <.01 75.5
Note: $ denotes the absence of outliers, hence the values reported in this row as the same as those in Table 1.
10 N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
Table 3. Intervention characteristics meta-regressed on SDT-based outcomes at the end of the intervention.
Study Characteristic
Need Support Competence Autonomy Relatedness Combined Need Satisfaction Autonomous Motivation Controlled Motivation Amotivation
(k= 21) (k= 22) (k=17) (k= 14) (k= 23) (k= 37) (k= 18) (k= 14)
Treatment duration 0.002 0.001 0.000 0.002 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000
Need support techniques
Intervention vs. comparison
Competence support techniques
Optimal challenge 0.42 0.33 0.25 0.48 0.38 0.09 0.16 0.04
Be positive 0.74
0.09 0.52
0.35 0.07 0.33
0.30* 0.68*
Info/positive feedback 0.13 0.01 0.30 0.37 0.06 0.15 0.19 0.26
Identify barriers .19 0.11 0.43 0.61
0.16 0.37** 0.03 0.15
Skills/problem solving 0.03 0.07 0.20 0.37 0.02 0.14 0.08 0.13
Develop plan 0.28 0.37 0.19 0.35 0.37 0.09 0.14 0.09
Reframe failures 0.08 –– 0.15 0.12 ––
Other 0.01 0.15 0.02 0.38 0.19 0.10 0.07 0.18
Autonomy support techniques
Provide rationale 0.01 0.11 0.53
0.35 0.26 0.17 0.13 0.30
Acknowledge feelings 0.38 0.09 0.07 0.02 0.12 0.00 0.00 0.12
Oer choices 0.10 0.04 0.23 0.45 0.08 0.03 0.12 0.26
Explore values 0.19 0.03 0.44 0.52 0.12 0.08 0.09 0.09
Support self-change 0.24 0.25 0.00 0.17 0.20 0.07 0.03 0.47
Non-controlling language 0.15 0.12 0.10 0.11 0.22 0.12 0.18 0.17
Other 0.11 0.04 0.09 0.22 0.00 0.03 0.01 0.05
Relatedness support techniques
Develop empathy 0.13 0.14 0.19 0.15 0.05 0.25
0.25 0.09
Warmth/inclusion 0.31 0.05 0.12 0.33 0.03 0.05 0.18 0.17
Convey value 0.07 0.23 0.54* 0.56
0.27 0.21 0.15 0.52*
Convey respect 0.24 0.07 0.17 0.34 0.08 0.08 0.29
0.00
Other 0.43 0.18 0.19 0.04 0.17 0.01 0.16 0.21
Competence (min. 1 strategy) 0.46 0.28 0.32 0.27 0.25 0.07 0.04 0.25
Autonomy (min. 1 strategy) 0.21 0.13 0.23 0.48 0.00 0.06 0.02 0.04
Relatedness (min. 1 strategy) 0.04 0.09 0.02 0.33 0.02 0.20 0.07 0.07
No. of needs targeted 0.19 0.08 0.07 0.23 0.06 0.04 0.01 0.06
Di. in competence strategies 0.01 0.01 0.12 0.18 0.02 0.08 0.04 0.12
Di. in autonomy strategies 0.01 0.03 0.12 0.09 0.01 0.04 0.04 0.02
Di. in relatedness strategies 0.04 0.04 0.20 0.14 0.03 0.06 0.04 0.10
Di. in total SDT strategies 0.00 0.00 0.06 0.07 0.01 0.03 0.02 0.04
Venue (1 = yes; 0 = no)
Clinic 0.38 0.33 0.06 0.31 0.02 0.18 0.39
Community 0.65 0.39 0.68
1.01** 0.40 0.02 0.18 0.56*
Fitness/Sports 0.56 0.28 0.10 0.00 0.14 0.14 0.03 0.29
(Continued)
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 11
Table 3. Continued.
Study Characteristic
Need Support Competence Autonomy Relatedness Combined Need Satisfaction Autonomous Motivation Controlled Motivation Amotivation
(k= 21) (k= 22) (k=17) (k= 14) (k= 23) (k= 37) (k= 18) (k= 14)
School 0.53 0.16 0.30 0.45 0.17 0.19 0.13 0.06
University 0.77 0.26 0.35 0.67 0.18 0.22 0.08 0.47
Mode (1 = yes; 0 = no)
Face-to-face 0.51 0.25 0.04 0.68 0.21 0.18 0.09 0.03
Phone 0.33 0.22 0.12 0.35 0.18 0.20 0.56
One-to-one 0.64 0.04 0.32 0.12 0.16 0.10 0.25 0.47
One-to-many 0.50 0.02 0.02 0.39 0.04 0.12 0.13 0.04
Many-to-many 0.12 –– 0.52 0.33 0.23
Provider (yes = 1; no/unclear = 0)
Investigators 0.11 0.27 0.13 0.52 0.27 0.02 0.20 0.27
Trained trainers 0.20 0.41
0.34 0.49 0.44
0.22
0.12 0.07
Design
(RCT = 1; Quasi = 0) 0.20 0.45
0.38 0.32 0.33 0.25
0.30
0.24
Analysis (yes = 1; no = 0)
Accounted for baseline 0.09 0.15 0.18 0.28 0.14 0.07 0.03 0.06
Low Risk of Bias (yes = 1; no/unclear = 0)
Sequence generation 0.27 0.02 0.22 0.10 0.05 0.03 0.02 0.29
Allocation concealment 0.39 0.11 0.35 0.27 0.10 0.32* 0.11 0.14
Participants blinded 0.04 0.16 0.20 0.04 0.12 0.25
0.01 0.23
Data collector blinded 0.63 0.48 0.03 0.30 0.32 0.32 0.05 0.17
Data analyst blinded 0.72 0.34 0.40 0.25 0.40 0.36 0.05 0.17
Provider blinded 0.65 0.84 0.96 1.42* 0.98
0.23 0.12 0.18
Missing/incomplete data 0.18 0.30 0.20 0.11 0.23 0.08 0.02 0.29
Selective reporting 0.18 0.30 0.19 0.11 0.23 0.11 0.11 0.12
Other
Participant age (years) 0.01 0.004 0.01 0.01 0.002 0.002 0.01 0.01
Participant sex (% male) 0.01 0.001 0.01 0.01 0.000 0.001 0.001 0.001
Journal publication (yes = 1; no = 0) 0.02 0.42 –– 0.49 0.001 ––
Note: p< .10; *p< .05; **p< .01; ***p< .001. Otherfor need supportive techniques refers to technique reported as being autonomy, competence or relatedness supportive but without sucient
information as to what exactly it entailed.
12 N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
Table 4. Behaviour change techniques meta-regressed on SDT-based outcomes at the end of the intervention.
Behaviour Change Technique
Need Support Competence Autonomy Relatedness Combined Need Satisfaction Autonomous Motivation Controlled Motivation Amotivation
(k= 21) (k= 22) (k= 17) (k=14) (k= 23) (k= 37) (k= 18) (k= 14)
1.1 Goal setting (behaviour) 0.20 0.10 0.02 0.06 0.01 0.02 0.06 0.22
1.2 Problem solving 0.07 0.02 0.43 0.61
0.07 0.25
0.07 0.24
1.3 Goal setting (outcome) 0.43 0.11 0.32 1.15* 0.22 0.59** 0.23 0.09
1.4 Action planning 0.32 0.06 0.12 0.20 0.06 0.25 0.06 0.39
1.5 Review behaviour goals 0.34 0.18 0.18 0.02 0.25 0.12 0.20 0.56
1.6 Goal-beh. Discrepancy 0.43 0.08 0.32 0.08 0.31 0.16
1.8 Behavioural contract 0.40 ––– 0.12 0.13 0.17
1.9 Commitment 0.84 1.11* 1.15* 0.93
0.64* 0.66** 0.67
2.2 Feedback on behaviour 0.31 0.19 0.30 0.46 0.25 0.08 0.05 0.00
2.3 Self-monitoring beh. 0.41 0.17 0.10 0.40 0.01 0.07 0.22 0.50
2.4 Self-monitor outcomes 0.24 0.32 0.34 0.37 0.30 0.23
2.6 Biofeedback 0.10 0.03 0.12 0.10 0.27 ––
2.7 Feedback on outcomes 0.16 0.12 0.10 0.25 0.10 0.41 0.25 0.26
3.1 Soc. Supp. (unspecied) 0.56 0.18 0.39 0.61 0.36 0.33
0.03 0.18
3.2 Soc. Supp. (practical) 0.66 0.41 0.70 0.39 0.37 0.24 0.37
0.28
3.3 Soc. Supp. (emotional) 0.12 0.40 0.51 0.75* 0.33 0.07 0.10 0.02
4.1 Beh. instruction 0.55 0.39 0.43 0.42 0.32 0.17 0.04 0.11
4.2 Info. on antecedents 0.51 0.67 0.63 0.67 0.18 0.18 0.45
5.1 Info. health cons. 0.53 0.28 0.43 0.49 0.26 0.06 0.16 0.10
5.3 Info. soc. cons. 0.34 0.54 0.32 0.30 0.25 0.54 ––
5.4 Monitor emo. cons. 0.43 0.32 0.32 0.68* 0.16
5.6 Info. emo. cons. 1.00 0.15 0.64 0.17 0.12 0.21 0.23 0.55
6.1 Demo. of behaviour 0.72
0.45 0.39 0.56 0.25 0.09 0.03 0.03
6.2 Social comparison 0.43 0.32 0.32 0.68* 0.16
7.1 Prompts/cues 0.05 0.46 0.25 0.09 0.06 0.36 0.57
8.1 Beh. practice/rehearsal 0.37 0.25 0.25 0.42 0.09 0.01 0.03 0.14
8.3 Habit formation 0.35 0.03 –– 0.10 0.06 ––
8.7 Graded tasks 0.69 0.12 –– 0.52 0.09 0.08 0.27
9.1 Credible source 0.12 0.04 0.31 0.45 0.05 0.13 0.07 0.07
9.2 Pros and cons 0.11 0.27 0.13 0.17 0.29
0.03 0.10
10.3 Non-specic reward 0.43 0.84 0.74
1.15* 0.65 0.79** 0.36* 0.67
10.4 Social reward 1.14 0.84 1.11* 1.15* 0.93
0.74** 0.66** 0.67
10.6 Non-specic incentive 0.43 0.32 0.32 0.68* 0.16
10.8 Incentive (outcome) 0.43 0.32 0.32 0.68* 0.16
10.9 Self-reward 0.43 0.32 0.32 0.68* 0.16
10.10 Reward (outcome) 0.43 0.32 0.32 0.68* 0.16
13.1 Identify as role model 0.67 0.34 0.39 0.25 0.40 0.09 0.02 0.10
13.2 Framing/reframing 0.12 0.21 0.23 0.67 0.02 0.46 0.16
(Continued)
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 13
Table 4. Continued.
Behaviour Change Technique
Need Support Competence Autonomy Relatedness Combined Need Satisfaction Autonomous Motivation Controlled Motivation Amotivation
(k= 21) (k= 22) (k= 17) (k=14) (k= 23) (k= 37) (k= 18) (k= 14)
13.5 Identify with beh. –– 0.42 ––
15.1 Verbal persuasion capab.0.43 0.32 0.32 0.68* 0.16
15.2 Mental rehearsal –– 0.26 0.27 0.32
15.4 Self-talk 0.84 1.11* 1.15* 0.93
0.72* 0.66** 0.67
16.3 Vicarious consequences 0.69 ––– 0.44 0.26 0.27
Note: p< .10; *p<.05;**p< .01; ***p< .001. Beh. = behaviour; Soc. = Social; Supp. = Support; Info. = Information; Emo. = emotion; Cons. = consequences; Demo. = Demonstration; Phys. = physical;
Environ. = environment; Capab. = capability.
14 N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
Can SDT-based interventions change health behavior?
SDT-based interventions promoted health behaviours relative to comparison groups, with a medium
eect size at the end of intervention period (g= 0.45), and a small eect size at follow-up (g= 0.28).
Both of these eects were heterogeneous. These eects were marginally larger at the end of inter-
vention and signicantly larger at follow-up in the studies which utilised the technique of being posi-
tive that a person can succeed. These eects were robust to the exclusion of outliers. No other
intervention characteristic increased or decreased the eect sizes on health behaviour prior to the
removal of outliers (see Table 5). After removing outliers, interventions delivered in tness/sports
centres yielded larger eects sizes of health behaviour at the end of intervention period, compared
to interventions delivered elsewhere, B= .63, SE = .30, t= 2.08, p= .04. Quasi-experimental trials gen-
erated larger eect sizes on health behaviours than RCTs (see Table 5). Some BCTs were associated
with increased eect sizes for health behaviour at follow-up (see Table 6). The potential confounding
eects of these BCTs are considered in the Sensitivity Analyses section.
Can SDT-based interventions change physical and psychological health outcomes?
Although there were no immediate eects of the SDT-based interventions on physical health out-
comes at the end of intervention period (g= 0.04), there was a small benet at follow-up (g=
0.28). These eects were somewhat robust, once outliers were removed (end of intervention: g=
0.13; follow-up: g= 0.25; see Table 2). At the end of the intervention period, SDT-based interventions
promoted psychological health relative to the comparison groups (g= 0.29), but there was no benet
at follow-up (g= 0.14); see Table 1.
In terms of moderators for eect sizes associated with physical health (see Table 5), acknowled-
ging feelingsyielded larger intervention eect sizes on physical health at follow-up, as did being
positive that a person can succeed(albeit the latter only when outliers were removed, B= .61, SE
= .25, t= 2.46, p= .03). Surprisingly studies with longer treatment duration yielded smaller eect
sizes on physical health, B=.003, SE = .001, t=2.94, p= .02, though only following the removal
of outliers. Eect sizes, which were calculated taking into account baseline scores, yielded larger
eect sizes on physical health at the end of the intervention, although the moderation eect was
marginal, following the removal of outliers: B= .37, SE = .18, t= 2.02, p= .06.
For moderators of eect sizes associated with psychological health, studies that utilised two tech-
niques considered to provide autonomy support (using non-controlling languageand providing a
meaningful rationale) produced larger eect sizes at the end of intervention and at follow-up than
studies that did not. However, studies that used various competence-support type strategies (ident-
ify barriers to change;skills/problem solving;develop plans appropriate to ability) yielded smaller
eect sizes on psychological health than studies that did not. Studies that incorporated a one-to-
many approach for intervention delivery yielded larger benets in psychological health outcomes
at follow-up than studies that used other delivery modes. Also, adequately concealing allocation
sequence yielded smaller eects on psychological health, compared to studies with no or unclear
allocation sequence concealment (see Table 5). A number of BCTs were associated with eect
sizes relating to physical and psychological health (see Table 6), and their potential confounding
role are considered in the Sensitivity Analyses sections.
Are changes in eect sizes of SDT-based constructs associated with changes in eect sizes
of health behavior?
Changes in the eect sizes of any of the SDT-constructs were not associated with changes in eect
sizes of health behaviours at the end of the intervention when taking into account all available
studies. However, when identifying potential outliers, one study (Ha, Lonsdale, Ng, & Lubans, 2017)
was found to be a negative outlier (yielding smaller eect sizes) on changes in SDT constructs at
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 15
Table 5. Study characteristics meta- regressed on health behaviour, physical health and psychological health at the end of the
intervention and follow-up.
Study Characteristic
Health Behaviour Physical Health Psychological health
End Follow-up End Follow-up End Follow-up
(k= 49) (k= 28) (k= 16) (k= 14) (k= 22) (k= 10)
Treatment duration 0.000 0.001 0.002 0.001 0.001 0.002
Need support techniques
Intervention vs. comparison
Competence support techniques
Optimal challenge 0.08 0.12 0.25 0.03 0.23 0.06
Be positive 0.34
0.35** 0.23 0.52
0.01 0.24
Info./positive feedback 0.16 0.12 0.12 0.17 0.09 0.18
Identify barriers 0.02 0.13 0.10 0.33 0.11 0.53*
Skills/problem solving 0.22 0.03 0.05 0.15 0.04 0.81**
Develop plan 0.20 0.01 0.01 0.44 0.37* 0.87**
Reframe failures 0.25 0.07 0.03 0.14 0.44 0.07
Other 0.03 0.08 0.19 0.25 0.06 0.09
Autonomy support techniques
Provide rationale 0.17 0.12 0.04 0.18 0.37* 0.49
Acknowledge feelings 0.08 0.15 0.20 0.60* 0.14 0.24
Oer choices 0.05 0.20 0.10 0.14 0.19 0.39
Explore values 0.32
0.25
0.23 0.28 0.26 0.49
Support self-change 0.11 0.08 0.42
0.09 0.02 0.11
Non-controlling language 0.21 0.09 0.02 0.40 0.44** 0.74**
Other 0.13 0.08 0.15 0.06 0.25 0.32
Relatedness support techniques
Develop empathy 0.06 0.04 0.22 0.12 0.32 0.20
Warmth/inclusion 0.03 0.10 0.09 0.07 0.00 0.12
Convey value 0.09 0.01 0.06 0.09 0.15 0.20
Convey respect 0.19 0.10 0.27 0.21 0.15 0.20
Other 0.12 0.22 0.20 0.54 0.03 0.05
Competence (min. 1 tech.) 0.07 0.03 0.14 0.15 0.29 0.57
Autonomy (min. 1 tech.) 0.18 0.20 0.06 0.11 0.15 0.45
Relatedness (min. 1 tech.) 0.03 0.12 0.02 0.04 0.23 0.12
No. of needs targeted (3+3) 0.02 0.09 0.04 0.01 0.03 0.01
Di.in Competence 0.00 0.00 0.01 0.04 0.01 0.09
Di. in Autonomy 0.00 0.03 0.04 0.06 0.10 0.12
Diin Relatedness 0.03 0.03 0.02 0.01 0.04 0.06
Di. in total needs targeted 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.00 0.02 0.01
Venue (1 =yes; 0 =no)
Clinic 0.13 0.12 0.17 0.22 0.17 0.09
Community 0.11 0.03 0.28 0.09 0.41
Fitness/Sports 0.61
0.30 0.13 0.04 0.04
School 0.11 0.19 0.08 0.30 0.18 0.34
University 0.13 0.21 0.34 0.12
Mode (1 =yes; 0 =no)
Face-to-face 0.33 0.07 0.03 0.24 0.16 0.07
Phone 0.18 0.16 0.51 0.16 0.03 0.34
One-to-one 0.14 0.14 0.29 0.54
0.30 0.13
One-to-many 0.06 0.03 0.00 0.35 0.02 0.53*
Many-to-many 0.45 0.18 0.34 0.10 0.11
Provider (yes =1; no/unclear =0)
Investigators 0.25 0.10 0.13 0.20 0.31 0.19
Trained trainers 0.21 0.01 0.31 0.28 0.08 0.20
Design
(RCT = 1; Quasi = 0) 0.57** 0.26 0.12 0.09 0.13 0.40
Analysis (yes =1; no =0)
Accounted for baseline 0.02 0.16 0.70** 0.29 0.16 0.03
Low Risk of Bias (yes =1; no/unclear =0)
Sequence generation 0.01 0.06 0.23 0.09 0.24 0.54
Allocation concealment 0.21 0.09 0.24 0.27 0.37
0.74**
Participants blinded 0.02 0.15 0.03 0.24 0.11 0.34
Data collector blinded 0.29 0.09 0.17 0.13 0.30 0.19
Data analyst blinded 0.25 0.10 0.00 0.13 0.41 0.19
(Continued)
16 N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
the end of the intervention but a positive outlier (yielding larger eect sizes) on behaviour at the end
of the intervention. After removing this multivariate outlier study, increases in autonomous motiv-
ation and need support at the end of the intervention were each associated with positive changes
in health behaviours and psychological health at the end of the intervention (see Table 7). Moreover,
changes in these two SDT constructs were also found to predict health behaviours at follow-up (see
Table 8).
Are changes in eect sizes of SDT-based constructs associated with changes in eect sizes
of physical and psychological health outcomes?
Increases in eect sizes of autonomous motivation, combined need satisfaction, relatedness, auton-
omy, and competence need satisfactions, and need support eect sizes at the end of the intervention
were positively associated with increases in psychological health eect sizes at the end of the inter-
vention (see Table 7). Changes in SDT-based construct eect sizes were unrelated with changes in
physical health eect sizes at the end of the intervention. Few studies assessed SDT-based constructs
at the end of the intervention in conjunction with physical health and psychological health at follow-
up; in fact, fewer than three studies assessed the same SDT constructs at the end of the intervention
alongside a physical or psychological health outcome at follow-up, meaning meta-regression models
were not calculable. There was a trend for changes in eect sizes of health behaviours at the end of
the intervention to predict changes in eect sizes of physical health (β= .41; p< .10), but it failed to
reach signicance, likely due to the small number of available studies.
Small-study bias
Based on measures taken at the end of the intervention, Eggers test suggested that small-study bias
may be present for health behaviour outcomes (p= .01) and psychological health (p= .006), but not
for any of the SDT constructs or physical health outcomes.
Sensitivity analyses
While the ndings were generally robust to the impact of outliers (as outlined above), additional sen-
sitivity analyses provided some evidence of confounding. Specically, the moderating role of being
positive that a person can succeedon autonomous motivation appeared to be confounded with
BCT10.3 (non-specic rewards); when entered into a multivariate meta-regression, only BCT10.3 pre-
dicted the outcome, B= 0.78, SE = 0.28, t= 2.82, p= .008, but being positive that a person can
succeeddid not, B= 0.02, SE = 0.19, t= 0.10, p= .92. The moderating role of identify barriers to
changeon autonomous motivation, B= 0.20, SE = 0.15, t= 1.38, p= .18, was similarly confounded
Table 5. Continued.
Study Characteristic
Health Behaviour Physical Health Psychological health
End Follow-up End Follow-up End Follow-up
(k= 49) (k= 28) (k= 16) (k= 14) (k= 22) (k= 10)
Provider blinded 0.49 0.43
0.99
0.28 0.75
Missing/incomplete data 0.04 0.02 0.07 0.22 0.15 0.20
Selective reporting 0.03 0.20 0.37 0.29 0.16 0.73
Other
Participant age (years) 0.01 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.00 0.00
Participant sex (% male) 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00
Follow-up duration 0.00 0.00 0.00
Journal publication (yes = 1; no = 0) 0.26 0.43 0.06 ––
Note: p< .10; *p< .05; **p< .01; ***p< .001. Otherfor need supportive techniques refers to technique reported as being auton-
omy, competence or relatedness supportive but without sucient information as to what exactly it entailed.
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 17
Table 6. Behaviour change techniques meta- regressed on health behaviour, physical health and psychological health at the end of
the intervention and follow-up.
Study Characteristic
Health Behaviour Physical Health Psychological health
End Follow-up End Follow-up End Follow-up
(k= 49) (k= 28) (k= 16) (k= 14) (k= 22) (k= 10)
1.1 Goal setting (behaviour) 0.08 0.07 0.44 0.10 0.25 0.42
1.2 Problem solving 0.09 0.05 0.15 0.33 0.03 0.53*
1.3 Goal setting (outcome) 0.31 0.22 0.42
1.4 Action planning 0.16 0.06 0.07 0.37 0.24 0.53*
1.5 Review behaviour goals 0.01 0.09 0.28 0.18 0.24 0.39
1.6 Goal-beh. Discrepancy 0.04 0.03 0.43 0.08 ––
1.7 Review outcome goals 0.52 0.40 0.47 0.12 0.05 0.09
1.8 Behavioural contract 0.23 0.08 0.18 0.54 0.29 0.20
1.9 Commitment 0.28 0.22 0.42
2.1 Beh. monitor by others 0.13 ––
without feedback
2.2 Feedback on behaviour 0.05 0.09 0.04 0.53
0.01 0.28
2.3 Self-monitoring beh. 0.00 0.16 0.24 0.25 0.20 0.36
2.4 Self-monitor outcomes 0.19 0.14 0.12 0.07 0.34
2.5 Monitor outcomes 0.03 –––
without feedback
2.6 Biofeedback 0.24 0.14 0.27 0.78*
2.7 Feedback on outcomes 0.04 0.05 0.12 0.11 0.45 0.19
3.1 Soc. Support (unspecied) 0.15 0.11 0.30 0.22 0.17 0.13
3.2 Soc. Support (practical) 0.09 0.09 0.05 0.03 0.28 0.25
3.3 Soc. Support (emotional) 0.08 0.10 0.09 0.49
4.1 Beh. instruction 0.12 0.08 0.39* 0.44
0.02 0.22
4.2 Info. on antecedents 0.07 0.22 0.36 0.39
4.4 Behavioural experiments 0.73 ––
5.1 Info. health consequences 0.17 0.12 0.49* 0.11 0.19 0.17
5.2 Salience of consequences 0.73 ––
5.3 Info. soc. consequences 0.36 0.15 0.26 0.27 1.18*
5.4 Monitor emo. Consequences 0.01 ––––
5.6 Info. emo. Consequences 0.06 0.21 ––0.46 0.73
6.1 Demonstration of behaviour 0.09 0.30* 0.10 0.51 0.07 0.34
6.2 Social comparison 0.06 0.18 0.29 ––
6.3 Info. othersapproval 0.48 0.18 0.29 ––
7.1 Prompts/cues 0.13 0.04 0.05 0.13 0.08 0.06
7.3 Reduce prompts/cues ––0.40 1.18*
8.1 Beh. practice/rehearsal 0.29
0.16 0.12 0.56 0.01 0.17
8.2 Beh. substitution 0.47 0.37 0.04 0.34
8.3 Habit formation 0.52 0.10 ––
8.6 Generalisation target beh. 0.73 ––––
8.7 Graded tasks 0.30 0.17 0.13 0.13 0.01 0.19
9.1 Credible source 0.14 0.29* 0.09 0.10 0.78** 0.04
9.2 Pros and cons 0.08 0.12 0.50 0.47 0.36 0.49
9.3 Imagining future outcomes 0.73 ––––
10.3 Non-specic reward 0.78* 0.22 0.67
10.4 Social reward 0.03 0.04 0.27 0.01 0.51* 0.20
10.6 Non-specic incentive 0.39 ––––
10.8 Incentive (outcome) 0.39 –––
10.9 Self-reward 0.39 ––––
10.10 Reward (outcome) 0.39 ––––
11.1 Pharmacological support 0.37 ––
11.2 Reduce negative emo. 0.13 –– 0.60
12.1 Restructure phys. environ. 0.73 ––––
12.2 Restructure soc. environ. 0.73 –––
12.5 Add objects to environ. 0.43 0.25 0.13 0.27 ––
13.1 Identify self as role model 0.27 0.14 0.13 0.27 0.08
13.2 Framing/reframing 0.28 0.22 0.22 0.40
13.4 Valued self-identity 0.35 ––––
13.5 Identify with changed beh. 0.68 0.48 ––
15.1 Verbal persuasion capab. 0.19 ––
15.2 Mental rehearsal 0.27 ––––
(Continued)
18 N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
with BCT1.3 (Goal-setting (outcome)), B= 0.45, SE = 0.19, t= 2.31, p= .03, and the moderating role of
providing a meaningful rationaleon psychological health at the end of the intervention, B= 0.17,
SE = 0.18, t= 0.96, p= .35, was confounded with BCT9.1 (Credible Source), B= 0.65, SE = 0.28, t= 2.33,
p= .03.
The mode of one interventionist delivering to many recipients tended to be used alongside BCTs
1.2 (Problem Solving) and 1.4 (Action Planning) when considering psychological health at follow-up
as the outcome (both χ
2
(1) = 10.00, Fishersp= .008), and being positive that a person can succeed
tended to co-occur with BCT10.3 (Non-specic Reward), χ
2
(1) = 7.88, Fishersp= .04, when consider-
ing controlled motivation. Furthermore, while being positive that a person can succeedand
acknowledging othersfeelings/perspectiveswere not related to any signicant BCT predicting
physical health outcomes at follow-up, they were related to one another, χ
2
(1) = 5.83, Fishersp
= .03. Given these outcomes (psychological health at follow-up, physical health at follow-up, con-
trolled motivation) were assessed in only 10, 14 and 18 studies respectively, multivariate meta-
regressions were not conducted in these instances as there would be fewer than 10 studies per
predictor.
The moderating eects of (a) community venues on relatedness satisfaction, (b) being positive
that a person can succeedon autonomy satisfaction or health behaviour, (c) providing a mean-
ingful rationaleor conveying the person is valuedon autonomy satisfaction, and (d) reducing
controlling behaviour/languageon psychological health, were not aected because these mod-
erators were not associated with signicant behaviour change technique moderators. Similarly,
in the absence of signicant behaviour change technique moderators on autonomy support
and amotivation at the end of the intervention, the moderating role (a) of being positive that a
person can succeedon autonomy support or amotivation, (b) of conveying the person is
valuedon amotivation, and (c) of community settings on amotivation, were also not aected
by confounding with BCTs.
Discussion
We present the results of a meta-analysis of 73 SDT-informed interventions in the health domain. This
is the rst meta-analysis that examines changes in indices of motivation, health behaviours, physical
health, and psychological health as a result of such interventions, as well as how such changes covary
over time. We found that SDT-based interventions can be delivered to positively change most of the
examined SDT-based constructs. However, more work is needed to identify how such interventions
can help reduce controlled motivation and amotivation, and support relatedness satisfaction The
interventions also positively impacted health behaviours, physical health, and psychological
health. Nevertheless, most of the eect sizes were small or medium, varied in strength over time,
and/or were heterogeneous. Changes in need support and autonomous motivation in particular
were positively correlated with changes in health behaviours and psychological health. In sum,
there was evidence demonstrating modest ecacy of SDT-based interventions to change health
behaviours (primarily physical activity, and to a lesser extent dietary behaviours and smoking absti-
nence), and to improve indices of health.
Table 6. Continued.
Study Characteristic
Health Behaviour Physical Health Psychological health
End Follow-up End Follow-up End Follow-up
(k= 49) (k= 28) (k= 16) (k= 14) (k= 22) (k= 10)
15.3 Focus on past success 0.35 –––
15.4 Self-talk 0.45 0.48 0.22 0.67
16.2 Imaginary reward 0.35 ––––
16.3 Vicarious consequences 0.09 –––
Note: p< .10; *p< .05; **p< .01; ***p< .001. Beh. = behaviour; Soc. = Social; Info. = Information; Emo. = emotion; Phys. = phys-
ical; Environ. = environment; Capab. = capability.
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 19
Table 7. Predicting eect sizes of outcomes from eect sizes of predictors (At the End of Interventions).
Outcomes Predictors 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
1 Psychological health (k= 22) 0.06 0.20 0.13 0.48 0.58** 0.41* 0.79** 0.57** 0.39* 0.31*
2 Physical health (k= 16) 0.04 -/ 1.71 0.64 0.34 -/ -/ 0.51 -/
3 Health behaviour (k= 49) 0.67 1.04 0.40 0.04 0.08 0.16 0.04 0.19
4 Amotivation (k= 14) 0.60 0.43 0.34 0.16 0.39
0.32 0.11
5 Controlled motivation (k= 18) 0.21
0.08 0.02 0.04 0.08 0.08
6 Autonomous motivation (k= 37) 0.50** 0.51* 0.53** 0.45** 0.27*
7 Combined need satisfaction (k= 23) 0.89*** 0.86*** 0.99*** 0.38**
8 Relatedness (k= 14) 0.75** 1.04*** 0.28
9 Autonomy (k= 17) 0.97*** 0.46**
10 Competence (k= 22) 0.33*
11 Need support (k=21)
12 Health behaviour (k= 48)$ .47*** ––0.35 0.73 0.66* 0.35 0.41 0.44 0.33 0.39*
Note: Beta coecients are reported in the table. knotes the number of studies measuring each outcome. p< .10; *p< .05; **p< .01; ***p< .001.
-/ number of observations were less than 3; underlined/italic font results- number of observations were between 3 and 5; underlined/normal font results- number of observations between 6 and 9; non-
underlined/normal font results- at least 10 observations. $ denotes results following removal of one multivariate outlier (Ha et al., 2017); this study did not assess physical health.
20 N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
Eects of SDT-interventions on SDT indices of motivation
We rst examined whether SDT-informed interventions can aect motivation-related indices pro-
posed by the developers of this theory (Ryan & Deci, 2017). Many of these interventions aim to
increase the degree to which important others (e.g., healthcare professionals, tness instructors,
spouses) are supportive of individualsthree key basic psychological needs (autonomy, competence,
and relatedness). We found that manipulations of the need supportive features of the social environ-
ment were successful, with a medium to large eect size at the end of interventions and a large eect
size at follow-up (although the latter was not signicant due to a wide condence interval generated
from a fairly small number of studies). We also found that SDT interventions were successful in
increasing perceptions of overall need satisfaction, and individually for competence and autonomy.
These eects were small to medium in size, signicant at the end of the intervention, and at follow-up
after the removal of outliers (although p=0.06 for autonomy). Gillison et al. (2019) reported a much
larger eect for autonomy (g= .84) than we did (see Tables 1 and 2), perhaps reecting dierences in
the inclusion criteria between the two meta-analyses. Our ndings are important, as Ng et al. (2012)
showed that autonomy and competence need satisfaction mediated the eects of need support on
self-determined motivation and health outcomes. Such ndings are also relevant for biomedical
ethics and medical professionalism. For instance, the European Society of Cardiology and European
Atherosclerosis Society guidelines (Catapano et al., 2016) for the management of dyslipidaemias, and
the American College of Cardiology and American Heart Association clinical practice guidelines
(Grundy et al., in press) on the management of blood cholesterol emphasise the importance of sup-
porting patient autonomy for making their own decisions about their treatment and health, and fos-
tering patient self-ecacy for change.
We found that intervention eects on relatedness satisfaction were small and non-signicant;
similar small eects were reported by Gillison et al. (2019). This could be because SDT interventions
in the health domain use techniques that support primarily autonomy and competence and to a
lesser extent relatedness, as shown by the number of listed techniques in Tables 3 and 5. The tech-
niques for relatedness support centre on showing empathy and respect, which might be useful to
support initiation of change but perhaps not maintain it long-term, particularly if the target behaviour
is complex or does not need to take place alongside other people (e.g., being regularly physically
active, eat healthy). In other social contexts (e.g., workplace; Slemp, Kern, Patrick, & Ryan, 2018)
meta-analytic evidence (albeit correlational) shows somewhat stronger eect sizes for relatedness,
comparable to those for the other two needs. It would be interesting for future SDT interventions
in the health domain (and beyond) to manipulate both the relative balance and the intensity with
which they target autonomy, competence, and relatedness supportive techniques.
We also found that SDT interventions had small to medium eects on autonomous motivation,
which were signicant at the end of the intervention and at follow-up (in the latter case, after the
Table 8. Prospective analyses predicting eect sizes of outcomes at follow-up from eect sizes of predictors at the end of
intervention.
Predictors Outcomes Health Behaviour Physical Health Psychological health
1. Amotivation -/ -/ -/
2. Controlled motivation 0.22 -/ -/
3. Autonomous motivation 0.67* -/ -/
4. Combined need satisfaction 0.04 -/ -/
5. Relatedness 0.07 -/ -/
6. Autonomy 0.06 -/ -/
7. Competence 0.09 -/ -/
8. Need support 0.35* -/ -/
9. Health behaviour .26 .41
10. Physical health –– .24
Note: Beta coecients are reported in the table. knotes the number of studies measuring each outcome. p< .10; *p< .05.
-/ number of observations were less than 3. Underlined/italic font results- number of observations between 3 and 5; underlined/
normal font results- number of observations between 6 and 9; non-underlined/normal font results- at least 10 observations.
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 21
removal of outliers). Broadly similar ndings for autonomous motivation were reported in the pre-
vious two meta-analyses by Gillison et al. (2019) and Ng et al. (2012). Changes in autonomous motiv-
ation are purported by Ryan and Deci (2017) to translate to positive and long-term changes in
behaviour, cognition, and aect; below, we discuss evidence from our meta-analysis for such
eects. We also found that the eects of SDT interventions on controlled motivation and amotivation
were small and non-signicant. Gillison et al. did not analyse the eects of such interventions on
these two variables, because they were not considered a positive target for intervention(p. 116).
However, autonomous motivation, controlled motivation, and amotivation are fairly independent
constructs (the Ng et al. meta-analysis reported correlations in the range of .26 to +.44). Hence,
future intervention studies in the health domain may need to focus not only on increasing feelings
of enjoyment and personal utility for behaviour change, but also on addressing internal and external
pressures, and on feelings of helplessness for change. Controlled motivation could be an important
predictor of maladaptive health behaviours, not measured in the included studies. For example, a cor-
relational study of eating behaviours by Pelletier, Dion, Slovinec-DAngelo, and Reid (2004) showed
that autonomous motivation for eating was associated with healthy eating whereas controlled motiv-
ation was related to bulimic symptomatology.
Eects of SDT-interventions on health indices
Extending the Gillison et al. (2019) meta-analysis, we examined the eects of SDT interventions on
health behaviours, psychological health, and physical health. We found that the included interven-
tions had a positive impact on health behaviours, with the eect sizes being small to medium at
the end of intervention and small at follow-up. Further, we also found that changes in autonomous
motivation and perceptions of need support were associated with positive changes in health beha-
viours both at the end of the intervention and at follow-up. This is an important nding which sup-
ports the SDT view about the benecial outcomes of need support and autonomous motivation for
sustained behaviour change. For example, with regard to autonomous motivation, it has been argued
that it is the internalisation of the value for health and the resultant self-regulation of behaviour that
helps individuals lower their health-risk behaviours and adopt health protective behaviours without
continued intervention support (Ng et al., 2012; Ryan & Deci, 2017). Surprisingly, changes in need sat-
isfaction were not associated with changes in health behaviours at either time point. There were com-
paratively fewer studies that provided the necessary information to conduct such analysis with need
satisfaction variables, which might explain the lack of associations. It is also possible that the eects of
psychological need satisfaction on health behaviours are mediated by autonomous motivation (Val-
lerand, 1997).
SDT interventions had a small to medium eect on psychological health at the end of the inter-
vention, but not at follow-up. The reverse pattern of ndings was observed for physical health, with
small and non-signicant eects at the end of the intervention, but small to medium signicant
eects at follow-up. These ndings are consistent with and extend the correlational meta-analyses
by Ng et al. (2012). With regard to physical health, the eect at follow-up is an important one, par-
ticularly when viewed in conjunction with the ndings that SDT interventions result in health behav-
iour maintenance. To see benets in many physical health outcomes, health behaviours need to be
maintained over a period of time (and as reported above, behaviour maintenance is facilitated by
need support and autonomous motivation). For instance, if one stops smoking for 1 year, heart
attacks and strokes are reduced by 50% compared to continued smoking, lung cancer rates fall in
half after 10 years, immune function improves to 50% of normal in 1 year and is returned to baseline
after 5 years (Department of Health and Human Services, 1990,2014). Lowering cholesterol with an
intensive statin medication reduces heart attacks strokes and death by 50% after 1.5 years of treat-
ment (Ridker et al., 2008).
With regard to the eects of the SDT interventions on psychological health, we found that the
positive eects at the end of the intervention were correlated with increases in need support,
22 N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
autonomous motivation, combined need satisfaction, satisfaction of each individual need, and health
behaviours. However, at follow-up, the eect of the SDT interventions was small and non-signicant.
Given that predictors of well-being and quality of life are multi-faceted and not always related to
health behaviours and professional encounters (e.g., quality of personal relationships, nancial cir-
cumstances; Chanfreau et al., 2008), this nding is probably not surprising.
Exploratory moderator analyses
Our exploratory moderator analyses were hypothesis generatingrather than hypothesis-testing,
and as such we did not account for multiple testing. We found that some SDT techniques moderated
intervention eects. For instance, the use of being positive a person can succeedwas associated
with larger eect sizes in terms of intervention eects on various SDT constructs, health behaviour
at follow up, and physical health at follow up. Provision of a meaningful rationalewas associated
with larger eect sizes in autonomy satisfaction, combined need satisfaction (after removal of out-
liers), and psychological health (at the end of intervention and at follow-up). A surprising nding
was that studies that used three competence-support techniques (identify barriers to change;
skills/problem solving;develop plans) yielded smaller eect sizes on psychological health than
studies that did not. It is possible that if these techniques are oered before an individual is auton-
omously motivated, they may lead to perceptions of external control. It is also possible that some of
these techniques were used inappropriately (e.g., problem solving or plan development might not
have been communicated in an autonomy-supportive manner; see also Hagger et al., 2016, and Pre-
stwich, Sheeran, Webb, & Gollwitzer, 2015, for other considerations and suggestions regarding plan-
ning). Quality of intervention delivery, and subsequent delity checking is essential in this eld to
really understand if/why these techniques work or not (Prestwich, Kenworthy, & Conner, 2017;
Quested, Ntoumanis, Thøgersen-Ntoumani, Hagger, & Hancox, 2017). Interestingly, the total
number of techniques in the intervention vs. control conditions or the dierence in the techniques
between the two conditions did not moderate any of the observed eects.
We also explored which BCTs were used in the included studies and how these BCTs were related
to SDT constructs, health behaviours, and health outcomes, at the end of the intervention and at
follow up. Of all SDT constructs, autonomous motivation was related to many more BCTs than any
other SDT construct. There was no particular pattern linking specic BCTs with physical or psychologi-
cal health. We also examined whether any moderating eects of need supportive techniques on SDT
constructs, health behaviours, and health outcomes, would remain signicant when accounting for
the BCTs used in the studies. We found three instances where the moderating eects became no
longer signicant when accounting for BCTs. The degree to which some SDT techniques overlap
with some BCTs is to be empirically determined. For example, in our analyses, the moderating
eect of providing a meaningful rationaleon psychological health was no longer signicant
when accounting for the BCT Credible Source. Nevertheless, it is important to clarify that, concep-
tually, the need supportive techniques aim to foster need satisfaction and autonomous motivation,
whereas the BCTs primarily aim at changing behaviour. This is important because, for example, the
support of autonomy includes that an individual may autonomously decide not to change their
behaviour (i.e., volitional non-adherence). Volitional or autonomous non-adherence (choosing to con-
tinue to smoke, or to not exercise or not take a recommended medicine) is in line with medical ethics
and reects the real world of public health (Beauchamp & Childress, 2009).
The analyses pertaining to BCTs should be treated with caution because the BCTs were coded by
us (with moderate inter-rater reliability) based on the description of study methodologies and were
not explicitly identied by study authors. Further, when looking at the eects of specic BCTs on
specic outcomes, some BCTs were always present when other BCTs were present and were
absent when other BCTs were absent. Consequently, it is impossible to determine whether the posi-
tive (or negative) eects of these BCTs are attributable to an individual BCT or combination of BCTs. It
should also be noted that more than half (i.e., 50) of the 93 BCTs were not utilised dierently in the
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 23
experimental versus comparison conditions in any of the studies included in the review. Conse-
quently, we were unable to examine the potential eects of a large proportion of BCTs on any of
the review outcomes.
We also explored possible moderation eects of other variables associated with intervention
characteristics, participant characteristics, and study methodology. Only a handful of such variables
were moderators; of those, two moderated more than one eect size. Specically, interventions deliv-
ered in community settings were more likely to enhance relatedness and reduce amotivation than
interventions delivered elsewhere. This might be due to the possibility that in such settings partici-
pants self-select their groups, compared to other settings such as universities, schools, or clinics.
Further, we found that studies with low bias in terms of allocation concealment had smaller eect
sizes in terms of autonomous motivation and physical health (at follow-up only) than studies in
which allocation concealment was rated as low or unclear. Given that approximately 40% of the
included studies were rated as low or unclear in terms of allocation concealment, ensuring that
such concealment takes place should be an important methodological consideration for future
SDT trials in the health domain.
Limitations and future research directions
Limitations of our review included that the study selection was not performed in duplicate, that not
all studies were coded by exactly the same assessors (i.e., three assessors worked in pairs to code all
included studies), and that we did not test for indirect eects via putative mediators reecting
mechanisms of action (cf., Rhodes, Boudreau, Josefsson, & Ivarsson, in press). There are also a
number of limitations in the primary studies that restricted the scope of this meta-analysis. For
instance, long-term associations between SDT-based constructs at the end of interventions with
physical or psychological health outcomes at follow-up could not be tested due to insucient
numbers of studies. There is a necessity of more studies that assess long-term changes in motiv-
ation and indices of health. Further, most of the included studies focused primarily on physical
activity promotion. Hence, there is clearly a need for application of SDT to a more diverse range
of health behaviours, so that the comparative eectiveness of interventions across dierent beha-
viours is evaluated. Further, many studies assessed health behaviours but not associated health
outcomes; this is clearly an important gap which needs to be addressed in the future, particularly
by assessing clinically meaningful changes in these outcomes, in order to increase the appeal of the
SDT literature to health care professionals and policy makers. In addition, we computed eect sizes
for autonomous and controlled motivation composites, as opposed to each individual motivational
regulation, as many studies did not report data at the level of individual regulations. Nevertheless,
the use of such composites scores is widespread in the SDT literature and in the latest SDT book by
Ryan and Deci (2017).
In terms of the collective body of studies, there was some indication of small study bias for
health behaviours and psychological health outcomes, but not for the SDT constructs or physical
health outcomes. While Eggers test may be more sensitive than other related tests, it may also
have a higher risk of false positives. Having noted this, it should also be stated that tests of
funnel plot asymmetry typically have low power (e.g., Lin et al., 2018). Moreover, as Eggerstest
is based on funnel plot asymmetry, the identied small-study bias may be reective of various
factors (e.g., poorer methodological quality in studies with smaller sample sizes leading to
inated eects, or outlying studies) rather than, or in addition to, publication bias. Given that we
found considerable variability in the eects of SDT interventions, we went to great lengths to
account for outlier eects and possible confounds via sensitivity analyses. We tested an extensive
range of potential moderators associated with psychological factors (need supportive techniques,
BCTs), intervention characteristics, participant characteristics, and study quality. Coding such infor-
mation was not straightforward. Accordingly, we recommend that researchers use greater care in
reporting exactly how intervention techniques were operationalised. Other potentially important
24 N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
moderating variables such as the frequency and duration of intervention contacts were not poss-
ible to code reliably across studies. Our approach was explorative, hence, research is needed to
experimentally test within the same study moderators such as combinations of dierent need sup-
portive techniques.
Setting is another important moderator to consider. Most of the included studies were in the area
of primary prevention of diseases. It is important to have more studies testing the feasibility and
ecacy of SDT interventions in terms of disease managementthat is, achieving best results once
one has a disease (secondary and tertiary prevention). We found that longer treatment duration
yielded smaller eect sizes on physical health, after the removal of outliers. The included studies
varied considerably in training duration and intensity; this of course might reect variability in the
expertise of trainers and trainees, or time availability constraints. However, there is a clear gap in
the extant literature in identifying optimal training durations for SDT interventions in community
and clinical settings. In addition, future studies in the eld could take into account our ratings for
methodological quality, particularly blinding key personnel to study condition.
Future studies should also be tested for cost-eectiveness and comparative eectiveness with
other health care interventions. As an example, a SDT-informed intervention on tobacco depen-
dence has demonstrated cost-eectiveness (Pesis-Katz, Williams, Niemieic, & Fiscella, 2011), but
this study is an exception to the eld and was limited by assessment of cost-eectiveness with
self-report data only. Future intervention research in the health domain should also consider
expanding the number of SDT constructs targeted. For instance, in the context of weight manage-
ment, there is correlational evidence on the potential undermining eects of a need thwarting
social environment and how such an environment can frustrate onespsychologicalneeds(Ng
et al., 2013). Although some aspects of need thwarting would not be ethically possible to manip-
ulate experimentally (e.g., use of punishment or abusive language), other aspects would be more
amenable to experimental manipulation (e.g., conditional acceptance, oering of engagement-
contingent rewards). In addition, future SDT-informed interventions might consider targeting auto-
matic (e.g., habits, automatic evaluations and motivations) and volitional constructs of action (e.g.,
action plans, coping planning), to help individuals translate motivation to specic(i.e.,where,
when, how) plans, deal with contingencies, and habituate desirable behaviours (cf. Rhodes,
McEwan, & Rebar, 2019).
Conclusions
We found that SDT-informed interventions in the health domain were associated with modest but
signicant improvements in need support, competence and autonomy need satisfactions, as well
as autonomous motivation. These eects were stronger at the end of interventions than at follow
up, which one might reasonably expect. Further, the interventions had modest eects in terms of
changing health behaviours both at the end of the intervention and at follow up; these eects
were associated with changes in autonomous motivation and perceptions of need support. The
eects of the interventions on health behaviours were noted to be at risk of potential small-study
bias. We also found that SDT interventions had positive eects on physical health at follow-up, but
not at the end of intervention. The eect of SDT interventions on psychological health was signicant
at the end of the intervention only; this nding was also potentially at risk of small-study bias. A
strength of this meta-analysis is the extensive sensitivity analyses, searching for potential moderators
and confounders.
In conclusion, we found evidence demonstrating some ecacy of SDT-based interventions to
improve indices of health and we oer several suggestions as to how research in this area could
be advanced, in terms of theory testing and methodological applications. We hope our review will
provide answers to public health institutions and policy makers of whether and how SDT-based inter-
ventions can be integrated within the health promotion (e.g., community programmes) and health
care (e.g., health care worker training and organisational change) systems.
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 25
Disclosure statement
No potential conict of interest was reported by the author(s).
ORCID
Nikos Ntoumanis http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7122-3795
Andrew Prestwich http://orcid.org/0000-0002-7489-6415
Eleanor Quested http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8955-8809
Cecilie Thøgersen-Ntoumani http://orcid.org/0000-0003-0255-1263
Edward L. Deci http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8246-8536
Chris Lonsdale http://orcid.org/0000-0002-2523-5565
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.
*Arrogi, A., Schotte, A., Bogaerts, A., Boen, F., & Seghers, J. (2017). Short- and long-term eectiveness of a three-month
individualized need-supportive physical activity counseling intervention at the workplace. BMC Public Health,17,
52. doi:10.1186/s12889-016-3965-1
*Babic, M. J., Smith, J. J., Morgan, P. J., Lonsdale, C., Plotniko, R. C., Eather, N., Lubans, D. R. (2016). Intervention to
reduce recreational screen-time in adolescents: Outcomes and mediators from the switch-O4 healthy Minds
(S4HM) cluster randomized controlled trial. Preventive Medicine,91,5057. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.07.014
*Badr, H., Smith, C. B., Goldstein, N. E., Gomez, J. E., & Redd, W. H. (2015). Dyadic psychosocial intervention for advanced
lung cancer patients and their family caregivers: Results of a randomized pilot trial. Cancer,121, 150158. doi:10.1002/
cncr.29009
Beauchamp, T. L., & Childress, J. F. (2009). Principles of biomedical ethics (6th ed). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2011). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
*Borkoles, E., Carroll, S., Clough, P., & Polman, R. C. (2016). Eect of a non-dieting lifestyle randomised control trial on
psychological health and weight management in morbidly obese pre-menopausal women. Maturitas,83,5158.
doi:10.1016/j.maturitas.2015.09.010
*Carroll, S., Borkoles, E., & Polman, R. (2007). Short-term eects of a non-dieting lifestyle intervention program on weight
management, tness, metabolic risk, and psychological health in obese premenopausal females with the metabolic
syndrome. Applied Physiology, Nutrition, and Metabolism,32, 125142. doi:10.1139/h06-093
Catapano, A. L., Graham, I., De Backer, G., Wiklund, O., Chapman, M. J., Drexel, H., Reiner, Ž.(2016). 2016 ESC/EAS guide-
lines for the management of dyslipidaemias. European Heart Journal,37, 29993058. doi:10.1093/eurheartj/ehw272
*Caudwell, K. M., Mullan, B. A., & Hagger, M. S. (2018). Testing an online, theory-based intervention to reduce pre-drinking
alcohol consumption and alcohol-related harm in undergraduates: A randomized controlled trial. International Journal
of Behavioral Medicine,25, 592604. doi:10.1007/s12529-018-9736-x
Chanfreau, J., Lloyd, C., Byron, C., Roberts, C., Craig, R., De Feo, D., & McManus, S. (2008). Predicting well-being. London:
Department of Health. Retrieved from http://natcen.ac.uk/media/205352/predictors-of-wellbeing.pdf
*Chatzisarantis, N. L., & Hagger, M. S. (2009). Eects of an intervention based on self-determination theory on self-
reported leisure-time physical activity participation. Psychology & Health,24,2948. doi:10.1080/08870440701809533
*Chatzisarantis, N. L., Hagger, M. S., Kamarova, S., & Kawabata, M. (2012). When eects of the universal psychological need
for autonomy on health behaviour extend to a large proportion of individuals: A eld experiment. British Journal of
Health Psychology,17, 785797. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8287.2012.02073.x
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
*Cossette, S., Belaid, H., Heppell, S., Mailhot, T., & Guertin, M. C. (2016). Feasibility and acceptability of a nursing interven-
tion with family caregiver on self-care among heart failure patients: A randomized pilot trial. Pilot and Feasibility
Studies,2, 34. doi:10.1186/s40814-016-0077-8
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum
Publishing Co.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2012). Self-determination theory in health care and its relations to motivational interviewing: A
few comments. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity,9(1), 24. doi:10.1186/1479-5868-9-24
*Duda, J. L., Williams, G. C., Ntoumanis, N., Daley, A., Eves, F. F., Mutrie, N., Jolly, K. (2014). Eects of a standard provision
versus an autonomy supportive exercise referral programme on physical activity, quality of life and well-being indi-
cators: A cluster randomised controlled trial. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity,11, 10.
doi:10.1186/1479-5868-11-10
*Duncan, L. R., Hall, C. R., Wilson, P. M., & Rodgers, W. M. (2012). The use of a mental imagery intervention to enhance
integrated regulation for exercise among women commencing an exercise program. Motivation and Emotion,36,
452464. doi:10.1007/s11031-011-9271-4
26 N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
*Edmunds, J., Ntoumanis, N., & Duda, J. L. (2008). Testing a self-determination theory-based teaching style intervention in
the exercise domain. European Journal of Social Psychology,38, 375388. doi:10.1002/ejsp.463
*Fortier, M. S., Wiseman, E., Sweet, S. N., OSullivan, T. L., Blanchard, C. M., Sigal, R. J., & Hogg, W. (2011). A moderated
mediation of motivation on physical activity in the context of the physical activity counseling randomized control
trial. Psychology of Sport and Exercise,12,7178. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2010.08.001
Fransen, K., Boen, F., Vansteenkiste, M., Mertens, N., & Vande Broek, G. (2017). The power of competence support: The
impact of coaches and athlete leaders on intrinsic motivation and performance. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine
and Science in Sports,28, 725745. doi:10.1111/sms.12950
*Friederichs, S. A., Oenema, A., Bolman, C., & Lechner, L. (2015). Long term eects of self-determination theory and moti-
vational interviewing in a web-based physical activity intervention: Randomized controlled trial. International Journal
of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity,12, 101. doi:10.1186/s12966-015-0262-9
Gillison, F. B., Rouse, P., Standage, M., Sebire, S. J., & Ryan, R. M. (2019). A meta-analysis of techniques to promote motiv-
ation for health behaviour change from a self-determination theory perspective. Health Psychology Review,13, 110
130. doi:10.1080/17437199.2018.1534071
*González-Cutre, D., Ferriz, R., Beltrán-Carrillo, V. J., Andrés-Fabra, J. A., Montero-Carretero, C., Cervelló, E., & Moreno-
Murcia, J. A. (2013). Promotion of autonomy for participation in physical activity: A study based on the trans-contextual
model of motivation. Educational Psychology,34, 367384. doi:10.1080/01443410.2013.817325
Grundy, S. M., Stone, N. J., Bailey, A. L., Beam, C., Birtcher, K. K., Blumenthal, R. S., Goldberg, R. (in press). 2018 AHA/ACC/
AACVPR/AAPA/ABC/ACPM/ADA/AGS/APhA/ASPC/NLA/PCNA guideline on the management of blood cholesterol: A
report of the American College of Cardiology/American heart association task force on clinical practice guidelines.
Journal of the American College of Cardiology,73(24), 31683209. doi:10.1016/j.jacc.2018.11.002
*Ha, A. S., Lonsdale, C., Lubans, D. R., & Ng, J. Y. Y. (2018). Increasing studentsphysical activity during school physical
education: Rationale and protocol for the SELF-FIT cluster randomized controlled trial. BMC Public Health,18, 11.
doi:10.1186/s12889-017-4553-8
*Ha, A. S., Lonsdale, C., Ng, J. Y. Y., & Lubans, D. R. (2017). A school-based rope skipping program for adolescents: Results of
a randomized trial. Preventive Medicine,101, 188194. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.06.001
Hagger, M. S., Luszczynska, A., De Wit, J., Benyamini, Y., Burkert, S., Chamberland, P. E., Gauchet, A. (2016).
Implementation intention and planning interventions in health psychology: Recommendations from the synergy
expert group for research and practice. Psychology & Health,31, 814839. doi:10.1080/08870446.2016.1146719
*Halvari, A. E., & Halvari, H. (2006). Motivational predictors of change in oral health: An experimental test of self-determi-
nation theory. Motivation and Emotion,30, 294305. doi:10.1007/s11031-006-9035-8
*Halvari, A. E., Halvari, H., Bjornebekk, G., & Deci, E. L. (2012). Self-determined motivational predictors of increases in
dental behaviors, decreases in dental plaque, and improvement in oral health: A randomized clinical trial. Health
Psychology,31, 777788. doi:10.1037/a0027062
*Halvari, H., Healey, J., Olafsen, A. H., Byrkjeland, R., Deci, E. L., & Williams, G. C. (2017). Physical activity and motivational
predictors of changes in health behavior and health among DM2 and CAD patients. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine
&amp; Science in Sports,27, 14541469. doi:10.1111/sms.12757
*Hankonen, N., Heino, M. T., Hynynen, S. T., Laine, H., Araujo-Soares, V., Sniehotta, F.F., Haukkala, A. (2017). Randomised
controlled feasibility study of a school-based multi-level intervention to increase physical activity and decrease seden-
tary behaviour among vocational school students. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity,14,
37. doi:10.1186/s12966-017-0484-0
*Hargreaves, E. A., Mutrie, N., & Fleming, J. D. (2016). A web-based intervention to encourage walking (StepWise): Pilot
randomized controlled trial. JMIR Research Protocols,5, e14. doi:10.2196/resprot.4288
*Havenar, J. (2007). Adapted motivational interviewing for increasing physical activity: A 12 month clinical trial. (Doctor of
Philosophy), Arizona State University.
Higgins, J. P. T., Altman, D. G., & Sterne, J. A. C. (2011). Chapter 8: Assessing risk of bias in included studies. In J. P. T.
Higgins, & S. Green (Eds.), Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions (Vol. 5.1.0 [updated March
2011]). The Cochrane Collaboration.
*How, Y. M., & Wang, J. C. K. (2016). The eectiveness of an autonomy-supportive teaching structure in physical edu-
cation. [Ecacia de la estructura de enseñanza con soporte de autonomía en educación física]. RICYDE. Revista
Internacional de Ciencias del Deporte,12,528. doi:10.5232/ricyde
*How, Y. M., Whipp, P., Dimmock, J., & Jackson, B. (2013). The eects of choice on autonomous motivation, perceived
autonomy support, and physical activity levels in high school physical education. Journal of Teaching in Physical
Education,32, 131148. doi:10.1123/jtpe.32.2.131
*Hsu, Y.-T., Buckworth, J., Focht, B. C., & OConnell, A. A. (2013). Feasibility of a self-determination theory-based exercise
intervention promoting healthy at every size with sedentary overweight women: Project CHANGE. Psychology of Sport
and Exercise,14,283292. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2012.11.007
Hucutt, A. I., & Arthur, W., Jr. (1995). Development of a new outlier statistic for meta-analytic data. Journal of Applied
Psychology,80, 327334.
*Husted, G. R., Thorsteinsson, B., Esbensen, B. A., Gluud, C., Winkel, P., Hommel, E., & Zomann, V. (2014). Eect of guided
self-determination youth intervention integrated into outpatient visits versus treatment as usual on glycemic control
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 27
and life skills: A randomized clinical trial in adolescents with type 1 diabetes. Trials,15, 321. doi:10.1186/1745-6215-15-
321
*Jago, R., Edwards, M. J., Sebire, S. J., Tomkinson, K., Bird, E. L., Baneld, K., Blair, P. S. (2015). Eect and cost of an after-
school dance programme on the physical activity of 11-12 year old girls: The Bristol Girls Dance Project, a school-based
cluster randomised controlled trial. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity,12, 128. doi:10.
1186/s12966-015-0289-y
*Jago, R., Sebire, S. J., Cooper, A. R., Haase, A. M., Powell, J., Davis, L., Montgomery, A. A. (2012). Bristol Girls Dance
Project feasibility trial: Outcome and process evaluation results. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and
Physical Activity,9, 83. doi:10.1186/1479-5868-9-83
*Jago, R., Sebire, S. J., Davies, B., Wood, L., Edwards, M. J., Baneld, K., Montgomery, A. A. (2014). Randomised feasibility
trial of a teaching assistant led extracurricular physical activity intervention for 9 to 11 year olds: Action 3:30.
International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity,11, 114. doi:10.1186/s12966-014-0114-z
*Juul, L., Maindal, H. T., Zomann, V., Frydenberg, M., & Sandbaek, A. (2014). Eectiveness of a training course for general
practice nurses in motivation support in type 2 diabetes care: A cluster-randomised trial. PLoS One,9, e96683. doi:10.
1371/journal.pone.0096683
*Kim, H. R., & Kim, H. S. (2017). Autonomy-supportive, web-based lifestyle modication for cardiometabolic risk in post-
menopausal women: Randomized trial. Nursing &amp; Health Sciences,19, 509517. doi:10.1111/nhs.12375
*Kinnack, F. E., Thogersen-Ntoumani, C., & Duda, J. (2016). The eect of need supportive text messages on motivation
and physical activity behaviour. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,39, 574586. doi:10.1007/s10865-016-9722-1
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics,33, 159174.
*Levy, S. S., & Cardinal, B. J. (2004). Eects of a self-determination theory-based mail-mediated intervention on adults
exercise behavior. American Journal of Health Promotion,18, 345349. doi:10.4278/0890-1171-18.5.345
*Leyton, M., Batista, M., Lobato, S., Aspano, M. I., & Jimenez, R. (2017). Application of two intervention programs in order to
optimize motivation and to improve eating habits in adult and elderly women. Journal of Human Kinetics,59, 131142.
doi:10.1515/hukin-2017-0153
*Li, D. S. (2010). Patient centered care approach to adherence with cardiovascular medications: Self-determination theory
integration. (Doctor of Philosophy), Old Dominion University.
Lin, L., Chu, H., Murad, M. H., Hong, C., Qu, Z., Cole, S. R., & Chen, Y. (2018). Empirical comparison of publication bias tests in
meta-analysis. Journal of General Internal Medicine,33, 12601267. doi:10.1007/s11606-018-4425-7
*Lonsdale, C., Lester, A., Owen, K. B., White, R. L., Peralta, L., Kirwan, M., Lubans, D. R. (2019). An internet-supported
school physical activity intervention in low socioeconomic status communities: Results from the activity and motiv-
ation in physical education (AMPED) cluster randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Sports Medicine,53, 341
347. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2017-097904
*Lonsdale, C., Rosenkranz, R. R., Sanders, T., Peralta, L. R., Bennie, A., Jackson, B., Lubans, D. R. (2013). A cluster random-
ized controlled trial of strategies to increase adolescentsphysical activity and motivation in physical education:
Results of the motivating active learning in physical education (MALP) trial. Preventive Medicine,57, 696702.
doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2013.09.003
Michie, S., Abraham, C., Whittington, C., McAteer, J., & Gupta, S. (2009). Eective techniques in healthy eating and physical
activity interventions: A meta-regression. Health Psychology,28, 690701. doi:10.1037/a0016136
Michie, S., Richardson, M., Johnston, M., Abraham, C., Francis, J., Hardeman, W., Wood, C. E. (2013). The behavior change
technique taxonomy (v1) of 93 hierarchically clustered techniques: Building an international consensus for the report-
ing of behavior change interventions. Annals of Behavioral Medicine,46,8195. doi:10.1007/s12160-013-9486-6
Michie, S., Wood, C. E., Johnston, M., Abraham, C., Francis, J. J., & Hardeman, W. (2015). Behaviour change techniques: The
development and evaluation of a taxonomic method for reporting and describing behaviour change interventions (a
suite of ve studies involving consensus methods, randomised controlled trials and analysis of qualitative data).
Health Technology Assessment,19,1188. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK327619/?report=
reader.
*Mildestvedt, T., Meland, E., & Eide, G. E. (2008). How important are individual counselling, expectancy beliefs and auton-
omy for the maintenance of exercise after cardiac rehabilitation? Scandinavian Journal of Public Health,36, 832840.
doi:10.1177/1403494808090633
Miller, R. W., & Rollnick, S. (1991). Motivational Interviewing: Preparing people to change addictive behavior. New York:
Guilford Press.
*Moreno-Murcia, J. A., & Sánchez-Latorre, F. (2016). The eects of autonomy support in physical education classes
[Efectos del soporte de autonomía en clases de educación física]. RICYDE. Revista Internacional de Ciencias del
Deporte,12,7989. doi:10.5232/ricyde
*Mouratidis, A., Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Sideridis, G. (2008). The motivating role of positive feedback in sport and
physical education: Evidence for a motivational model. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,30, 240268. doi:10.
1123/jsep.30.2.240
*Moustaka, F. C., Vlachopoulos, S. P., Kabitsis, C., & Theodorakis, Y. (2012). Eects of an autonomy-supportive exercise
instructing style on exercise motivation, psychological well-being, and exercise attendance in middle-age women.
Journal of Physical Activity and Health,9, 138150.
28 N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
*Mouton, A., Gillet, N., Mouton, F., Van Kann, D., Bruyere, O., Cloes, M., & Buckinx, F. (2017). Eects of a giant exercising
board game intervention on ambulatory physical activity among nursing home residents: A preliminary study. Clinical
Interventions in Aging,12, 847858. doi:10.2147/CIA.S134760
*Nansel, T. R., Lael, L. M., Haynie, D. L., Mehta, S. N., Lipsky, L. M., Volkening, L. K., Liu, A. (2015). Improving dietary
quality in youth with type 1 diabetes: Randomized clinical trial of a family-based behavioral intervention.
International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity,12, 58. doi:10.1186/s12966-015-0214-4
*Nation-Grainger, S. (2017). Its just PEtill It felt like a computer game: Using technology to improve motivation in phys-
ical education. Research Papers in Education,32, 463480. doi:10.1080/02671522.2017.1319590
Ng, J. Y., Ntoumanis, N., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Duda, J. L., & Williams, G. C. (2012). Self-determi-
nation theory applied to health contexts: A meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science,7, 325340. doi:10.
1177/1745691612447309
Ng, J. Y. Y., Ntoumanis, N., Thogersen-Ntoumani, C., Stott, K., & Hindle, L. (2013). Predicting psychological needs and well-
being of individuals engaging in weight management. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being,5, 291310. doi:10.
1111/aphw.12011
*Niemiec, C. P., Manchester, R. A., Schultz, P. P., Rigby, C. S., & Williams, G. C. (2019). Randomized controlled trial of an elec-
tronic intervention for alcohol misuse informed by self-determination theory. Manuscript under preparation.
*Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Williams, G. C. (2009). Aspiring to physical health: The role of aspirations for phys-
ical health in facilitating long-term tobacco abstinence. Patient Education and Counseling,74, 250257. doi:10.1016/j.
pec.2008.08.015
*Ntoumanis, N., Thogersen-Ntoumani, C., Quested, E., & Hancox, J. (2017). The eects of training group exercise class
instructors to adopt a motivationally adaptive communication style. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in
Sports,27, 10261034. doi:10.1111/sms.12713
*Pardo, B. M., Bengoechea, E. G., Julian Clemente, J. A., & Lanaspa, E. G. (2014). Empowering adolescents to be physically
active: Three-year results of the Sigue la Huella intervention. Preventive Medicine,66,611. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.
04.023
*Pedersen, C., Halvari, H., & Williams, G. C. (2018). Worksite intervention eects on motivation, physical activity, and
health: A cluster randomized controlled trial. Psychology of Sport and Exercise,35, 171180. doi:10.1016/j.
psychsport.2017.11.004
*Pellegrini, C., Chang, R. W., Dunlop, D. D., Conroy, D., Lee, J., VanHorn, L., Cameron, K. (2017). Preliminary comparison
of patient-centered loss programs starting before versus after knee replacement [abstract]. Arthritis Rheumatology,69.
Pelletier, L. G., Dion, S. C., Slovinec-DAngelo, M., & Reid, R. (2004). Why do you regulate what you eat? Relationships
between forms of regulation, eating behaviors, sustained dietary behavior change, and psychological adjustment.
Motivation and Emotion,28, 245277. doi:0146-7239/04/0900-0245/0
*Perlman, D. (2013). The inuence of the social context on students in-class physical activity. Journal of Teaching in
Physical Education,32,4660. doi:10.1123/jtpe.32.1.46
Pesis-Katz, I., Williams, G. C., Niemieic, C. P., & Fiscella, K. (2011). Cost-eectiveness of an intensive tobacco dependence
intervention based on self-determination theory. American Journal of Managed Care,17, e393e398.
Prestwich, A., Kenworthy, J., & Conner, M. (2017). Health behavior change: Theories, methods and interventions. Abingdon:
Routledge.
Prestwich, A., Sheeran, P., Webb, T. L., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2015). Implementation intentions. In M. Conner, & P. Norman
(Eds.), Predicting and changing health behaviour: Research and practice with social cognition models (3rd ed.; pp. 321
357). Maidenhead: Open University Press.
Project of the ABIM Foundation, ACPASIM Foundation, and European Federation of Internal Medicine. (2002). Medical
professionalism in the new millennium: A physician charter. Annals of Internal Medicine,136, 243246. doi:10.7326/
0003-4819-136-3-200202050-00012
*Quaresma, A. M., Palmeira, A. L., Martins, S. S., Minderico, C. S., & Sardinha, L. B. (2014). Eect of a school-based interven-
tion on physical activity and quality of life through serial mediation of social support and exercise motivation: The
PESSOA program. Health Education Research,29, 906917. doi:10.1093/her/cyu056
Quested, E., Ntoumanis, N., Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., Hagger, M. S., & Hancox, J. (2017). Evaluating quality of implemen-
tation in physical activity interventions based on theories of motivation. International Review of Sport and Exercise
Psychology,10, 252269. doi:10.1080/1750984X.2016.1217342
*Resnicow, K., Davis, R. E., Zhang, G., Konkel, J., Strecher, V. J., Shaikh, A. R., Wiese, C. (2008). Tailoring a fruit and veg-
etable intervention on novel motivational constructs: Results of a randomized study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine,35,
159169. doi:10.1007/s12160-008-9028-9
Rhodes, R. E., Boudreau, P., Josefsson, K. W., & Ivarsson, A. (in press). Mediators of physical activity behavior change inter-
ventions among adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review,doi:10.1080/17437199.2019.
1706614
Rhodes, R. E., McEwan, D., & Rebar, A. L. (2019). Theories of physical activity behaviour change: A history and synthesis of
approaches. Psychology of Sport and Exercise,42, 100109. doi:0.1016/j.psychsport.2018.11.010
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 29
Ridker, P. M., Danielson, E., Fonseca, F. A., Genest, J., Gotto Jr, A. M., Kastelein, J. J., Nordestgaard, B. G. (2008).
Rosuvastatin to prevent vascular events in men and women with elevated C-reactive protein. New England Journal
of Medicine,359, 21952207. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa0807646
*Riiser, K., Londal, K., Ommundsen, Y., Smastuen, M. C., Misvaer, N., & Helseth, S. (2014). The outcomes of a 12-week
Internet intervention aimed at improving tness and health-related quality of life in overweight adolescents: The
young & active controlled trial. PLoS One,9, e114732. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0114732
*Robbins, L. B., Pfeier, K. A., Vermeesch, A., Resnicow, K., You, Z., An, L., & Wesolek, S. M. (2013). Girls on the Moveinter-
vention protocol for increasing physical activity among low-active underserved urban girls: A group randomized trial.
BMC Public Health,13, 474. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-13-474
Rouse, P. C., Duda, J. L., Ntoumanis, N., Jolly, K., & Williams, G. C. (2016). The development and validation of the
Interpersonal Support in Physical Activity Consultations Observational Tool. European Journal of Sport Science,16,
106114.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and well-
ness. New York: Guilford Publishing.
Ryan, R. M., Patrick, H., Deci, E. L., & Williams, G. C. (2008). Facilitating health behaviour change and its maintenance:
Interventions based on self-determination theory. European Health Psychologist,10,25.
*Shah, K. N., Majeed, Z., Yoruk, Y. B., Yang, H., Hilton, T. N., McMahon, J. M., Ryan, R. M. (2016). Enhancing physical func-
tion in HIV-infected older adults: A randomized controlled clinical trial. Health Psychology,35, 563573. doi:10.1037/
hea0000311
*Silva, M. N., Markland, D., Minderico, C. S., Vieira, P. N., Castro, M. M., Coutinho, S. R., Teixeira, P. J. (2008). A randomized
controlled trial to evaluate self-determination theory for exercise adherence and weight control: Rationale and inter-
vention description. BMC Public Health,8, 234. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-8-234
Slemp, G. R., Kern, M. L., Patrick, K. J., & Ryan, R. M. (2018). Leader autonomy support in the workplace: A meta-analytic
review. Motivation and Emotion,42, 706724. doi:10.1007/s11031-018-9698-y
*Smith, J. J., Morgan, P. J., Plotniko, R. C., Dally, K. A., Salmon, J., Okely, A. D., Lubans, D. R. (2014). Smart-phone obesity
prevention trial for adolescent boys in low-income communities: The ATLAS RCT. Pediatrics,134, e723e731. doi:10.
1542/peds.2014-1012
*Song, H. Y., & Nam, K. A. (2015). Eectiveness of a stroke risk self-management intervention for adults with prehyperten-
sion. Asian Nursing Research,9, 328335. doi:10.1016/j.anr.2015.10.002
*Spruijt-Metz, D., Nguyen-Michel, S. T., Goran, M. I., Chou, C. P., & Huang, T. T. (2008). Reducing sedentary behavior in
minority girls via a theory-based, tailored classroom media intervention. International Journal of Pediatric Obesity,3,
240248. doi:10.1080/17477160802113415
StataCorp. (2017). Stata statistical software: Release 15. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC.
Teixeira, P. J., Marques, M. M., Silva, M. N., Brunet, J., Duda, J., Haerens, L., Hagger, M. S. (2019). Classication of tech-
niques used in self-determination theory-based interventions in health contexts: An expert consensus study. PsyArXiv,
doi:10.31234/osf.io/z9wqu
*Thompson, D., Cantu, D., Ramirez, B., Cullen, K. W., Baranowski, T., Mendoza, J., Liu, Y. (2016). Texting to increase ado-
lescent physical activity: Feasibility assessment. American Journal of Health Behavior,40, 472483. doi:10.5993/AJHB.
40.4.9
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1990). The health benets of smoking cessation. US Department of Health
and Human Services. Public Health Service, Centers for Disease Control, Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and
Health Promotion, Oce on Smoking and Health. Atlanta, GA: U.S.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2014). The health consequences of smoking: 50 years of progress. A report
of the surgeon general. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,
National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Oce on Smoking and Health. Atlanta, GA: U.S.
Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 271360). San Diego: Academic Press.
*Van Hoecke, A.-S., Delecluse, C., Opdenacker, J., & Boen, F. (2013). Year-round eectiveness of physical activity counsel-
ing on subjective well-being: A self-determination approach among emish sedentary adults. Applied Research in
Quality of Life,9, 537558. doi:10.1007/s11482-013-9251-9
*Vanroy, J., Seghers, J., Bogaerts, A., Devloo, K., De Cock, S., & Boen, F. (2017). Short- and long-term eects of a need-sup-
portive physical activity intervention among patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus: A randomized controlled pilot trial.
PLoS One,12, e0174805. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0174805
*Ward, J., Wilkinson, C., Graser, S. V., & Prusak, K. A. (2008). Eects of choice on student motivation and physical activity
behavior in physical education. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education,27, 385398. doi:10.1123/jtpe.27.3.385
*Weman-Josefsson, K., Fröberg, K., Karlsson, S., & Lindwall, M. (2015). Mechanisms in self-determined exercise motivation:
Eects of a theory informed pilot intervention. Current Psychology,36,90100. doi:10.1007/s12144-015-9388-9
Williams, G. C., Freedman, Z. R., & Deci, E. L. (1998). Supporting autonomy to motivate patients with diabetes for glucose
control. Diabetes Care,21, 16441651. doi:10.2337/diacare.21.10.1644
*Williams, G. C., Cox, E. M., Kouides, R., & Deci, E. L. (1999). Presenting the facts about smoking to adolescents: Eects of an
autonomy-supportive style. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine,153, 959964. doi:10.1001/archpedi.153.9.959
30 N. NTOUMANIS ET AL.
*Williams, G. C., & Deci, E. L. (2001). Activating patients for smoking cessation through physician autonomy support.
Medical Care,39, 813823.
*Williams, G. C., Lynch, M., & Glasgow, R. E. (2007). Computer-assisted intervention improves patient-centered diabetes
care by increasing autonomy support. Health Psychology,26, 728734. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.26.6.728
*Williams, G. C., Niemiec, C. P., Patrick, H., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). The importance of supporting autonomy and
perceived competence in facilitating long-term tobacco abstinence. Annals of Behavioral Medicine,37, 315324. doi:10.
1007/s12160-009-9090-y
*Williams, G. C., Patrick, H., Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Lavigne, H. M. (2011). The smokers health project: A self-
determination theory intervention to facilitate maintenance of tobacco abstinence. Contemporary Clinical Trials,32,
535543. doi:10.1016/j.cct.2011.03.002
Wilson, P. M., Rodgers, W. M., Blanchard, C. M., & Gessell, J. (2006). The relationship between psychological needs, self-
determined motivation, exercise attitudes, and physical tness. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,33, 23732392.
doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01890.x
*Wilson, D. K., Evans, A. E., Williams, J., Mixon, G., Sirard, J. R., & Pate, R. (2005). A preliminary test of a student-centered
intervention on increasing physical activity in underserved adolescents. Annals of Behavioral Medicine,30, 119124.
doi:10.1207/s15324796abm3002_4
*Wilson, D. K., Van Horn, M. L., Kitzman-Ulrich, H., Saunders, R., Pate, R., Lawman, H. G., Brown, P. V. (2011). Results of the
active by choice today(ACT) randomized trial for increasing physical activity in low-income and minority adoles-
cents. Health Psychology,30, 463471. doi:10.1037/a0023390
World Health Organization. (2018). World health statistics 2018: Monitoring health for the sustainable development goals.
Geneva: World Health Organization.
HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW 31
... In this regard, self-determination theory (SDT; Ryan & Deci, 2017) is a comprehensive theory of human motivation that has been useful in understanding PA behavior in the health domain (Ng et al., 2012;Ntoumanis et al., 2021;Teixeira et al., 2020). According to this theory, getting involved in behaviors for autonomous reasons positively affects adaptive health outcomes, including well-being and greater behavioral attainment and adherence. ...
... Providing autonomy-supportive techniques is fundamental to promote autonomous motivation and adaptive behavioral consequences throughout health behavior programs. In this regard, PA has become one of the most analyzed behaviors from SDT (Ntoumanis et al., 2021). In clinical research, patients who experienced autonomy support provided by health staff acquired more autonomous motivation for PA adherence (Williams et al., 2006). ...
Article
Objective: Physical activity (PA) has emerged as an important element of supportive care for cancer patients, but few patients engage with exercise. Considering that autonomy support is associated with healthy lifestyles, it would be useful to know the specific autonomy-supportive techniques that can help to encourage PA in colorectal cancer (CRC) patients. This study aims to qualitatively explore autonomy support perceptions through a self-determination-theory-based exercise program (FIT-CANCER) with CRC patients during chemotherapy treatment. Methods and Measures: A total of 27 participants were included, 16 CRC patients, six relatives, and five healthcare professionals. Qualitative data from semi-structured interviews and observational field notes were analyzed with thematic analysis. Results: Three main themes were identified: Healthcare professionals encouraging enrollment in the exercise program, Relatives supporting attendance to the exercise sessions, Exercise instructor favoring adherence to the exercise program. The different subthemes showed autonomy-supportive techniques from these social agents to promote CRC patients’ participation in the exercise program. Conclusion: The present research showed the importance of autonomy support from healthcare professionals, relatives and the exercise instructor to promote the initiation and maintenance of CRC patients’ PA behavior and improve their quality of life, health and well-being.
... Motivation, defined as a psychological phenomenon that regulates and is regulated by autonomy and self-determination, is a multifaceted construct (Ntoumanis et al. 2021). The variety of theories of motivation is really large. ...
... Intrinsic motivation is related to a type of self-determined motivation that generates involvement due to personal satisfaction, by achieving set objectives and goals, even if they are not encouraged by society. Extrinsic motivation is relatively managed by autonomy, in an introjected way and influenced by contingent self-esteem, by the need for approval from third parties, or in an externally regulated way, when there are pressures or rewards for behavior, such as tangible financial benefits (Ntoumanis et al. 2021). Reiss (2000), an author of the theory of motivational sensitivity, even claims that there is no external motivation, all actions are motivated internally. ...
Article
Full-text available
For over one hundred years, humans have been actively exploring polar regions. At present, each year, several thousand people work at polar stations in the Arctic and Antarctica. Polar stations have unique workplace characteristics; the conditions are classified as isolated, confined, and extreme. This leaves the important question of what motivates people to carry out professional tasks in such a context, particularly during wintering over in a polar station. The current study was designed to answer that question. Eighty-six winterers (22 women and 64 men) from ten different countries described their motivation to winter over. The qualitative approach, thematic analysis, was applied to data analysis. Two coding cycles were applied: initial coding and theming of the data. Fifty-six detailed categories were identified and then grouped thematically, resulting in 17 main categories. The three main motivation categories were challenge and adventure (N = 36), unique experience (N = 16), and performance of the job (N = 15). There were also differences in motivation between explorers from different countries, from Arctic and Antarctica polar stations, from civilian and military polar stations, and between female and male polar expeditioners. Obtained results can be applied to improve selection procedures and better support expeditioners during their stay in the polar station.
... SDT has a long history within health research, from research on patient need satisfaction and healthy behaviour change, to elder care, medical education, and mental illness (Ntoumanis et al., 2020;Ryan & Deci, 2017). Domain-specific SDT measures in this space include the Health Care Climate Questionnaire (Williams et al., 1996), the Treatment Self-regulation Questionnaire (TSRQ) (Williams et al., 1996), and, more recently, The Virtual Care Climate Questionnaire (Smit et al., 2017), all of which have been leveraged within various digital health contexts (see selfdeterminationtheory.org/questionnaires for a summary). ...
... Free will beliefs are characterized by agency and choice, both of which are required to successfully manage health. Moreover, health protective behaviors such as physical activity and fruit and vegetable consumption also require perseverance, self-efficacy, self-control, and autonomy (Crescioni et al., 2011;Ntoumanis et al., 2021). Given that research has demonstrated a relationship between these traits and free will beliefs (Alquist et al., 2013;Crescioni et al., 2016;Li et al., 2018;Rigoni et al., 2012), the latter could therefore be associated with health protective behaviors. ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has demonstrated that a stronger belief in free will contributes to a variety of socially desirable behaviors. We assessed the correlation between free will beliefs and health behaviors. Four studies (N = 1172) provide evidence that belief in free will is positively associated with health protective behaviors (e.g., physical activity, fruit and vegetable consumption, low fat diet) and negatively associated with health risk behaviors (e.g., alcohol consumption, smoking, unhealthy snacking). In relation to the respective health protective and health risk behaviors, we found free will beliefs were more strongly correlated with physical activity and alcohol consumption, respectively. We also found free will beliefs were associated with key social cognition determinants (e.g., attitude, subjective norm, perceived behavioral control, and intention). Overall, our results suggest that belief in free will can have important consequences for health behavior. This contributes to current theorizing about the implications of believing in free will.
Article
Background Behavior change techniques (BCTs) have been extensively used in physical activity interventions for children, however, no systematic reviews have synthesized their effects. Purpose The present review aimed to identify the most promising BCTs used in physical activity interventions associated with (i) increased physical activity behavior and (ii) positive psychosocial outcomes in children with chronic conditions. Methods A systematic search of 6 databases identified 61 articles as eligible for inclusion. Data, including BCTs, were extracted from these studies and analyzed descriptively. Due to the heterogeneity of interventions, chronic conditions, and outcome measures, a meta-analysis was not conducted. Results Social support (unspecified), graded tasks, generalization of target behavior, and credible source were the most commonly reported and most promising (i.e., present in 2+ studies evidencing significant effects) BCTs across all studies. These BCTs were found to be especially relevant to improving psychosocial outcomes in the short- and long-term and improving physical activity behaviors in the long-term. Meanwhile, to improve short-term physical activity behaviors, in addition to social support (unspecified), action planning, goal setting (behavior), and problem solving were found to be promising BCTs. Conclusions The BCTs identified in this review may be relevant to incorporate when planning future interventions to support physical activity and psychosocial outcomes for children with chronic conditions.
Article
Urinary incontinence (UI) is relatively common among middle-aged and elderly women and can have significant impacts on patients' physiological, psychological, and quality-of-life aspects. A higher level of autonomy can encourage better health behaviors in patients, so as to promote rehabilitation of the disease and improve their quality of life. The study is aimed at exploring the level of autonomy and influencing factors among middle-aged and elderly women with UI. A cross-sectional study was conducted at a tertiary hospital in Shenzhen, China. Middle-aged and elderly women were asked to complete the Index of Autonomous Functioning scale (IAF), the International Consultation on Incontinence Questionnaire—Urinary Incontinence Short Form (ICIQ-UI SF), and the Urinary Incontinence Quality of Life (I-QOL) scale. Data were collected in January 2024 to March 2024. Descriptive statistics, univariate analysis, bivariate correlation and multivariate linear regression were used to analyze data. A total of 646 women completed the questionnaire. The means and standard deviations (SD) of the IAF section were 45.40 ± 8.92, those of the ICIQ-UI SF section were 11.54 ± 3.67, and those of the I-QOL section were 43.95 ± 9.83. Multivariate linear regression models for the IAF revealed that questionnaire scores were independently associated with family's monthly income, education level, regular exercise, UI severity, and I-QOL score (p < 0.05). The factors influencing the level of autonomy among middle-aged and elderly women with UI include family's monthly income, education level, regular exercise, UI severity, and I-QOL score. Higher levels of autonomy were observed among women with high family income, high education level, regular exercise habits, mild to moderate UI, and high quality of life.
Article
Full-text available
Environmental degradation induced by buyers' unsustainable buying habits strains ecosystems and impedes economically viable development. A possible approach for mitigating this effect and promoting a more sustainable economy is to minimize or transfer consumption from ordinary to sustainable items. To rationalize the significance of green consumption; the core purpose of the study is to identify the relationship between Environmental motivation (EM) and green purchase intentions (GPI) with the role of Price Sensitivity (PS) as moderating variable. AMOS was recruited to scrutinize the structural nexus between EM and GPI; with the role of PS as a moderating variable. The findings of the study revealed a significant and positive relationship between EM and GPI. Moreover, the PS positively moderated the connection between EM and GPI. The current research relied on the footings of the theory of planned behavior; which serves as a guiding mechanism in realizing the significance of consumers' motivations and cognitive antecedents in consumption habits. PS as a moderating variable regressed considerable impact that can shape their motivational concerns in buying green products. This research adds to the knowledge of sustainable green marketing literature by indulging into factors that trigger the consumer's attitudinal responses toward ecologically sound products. The policy consequence of this research lies in the fact that policymakers will become more aware of sustainable consumption, allowing them to create price systems compatible with consumers' sustainability views.
Chapter
Behavior change can be a challenging but rewarding partnership between nurses and individuals in a variety of health care settings to modify cardiovascular risk factors. Modification of cardiovascular risk factors is vitally important for prevention of cardiovascular diseases and in the presence of disease important to promote the best quality of life for all individuals. Contemporary theories and theoretical models provide guidance in developing treatment plans for behavior change. Empathic communication techniques are important for nurses to engage patients in reducing ambivalence and resistance to change and develop a resilience mindset to maintain behavior change. This chapter addresses these concepts with examples of how to engage effectively with individuals to demonstrate these concepts in practice.
Article
Sugar‐sweetened beverage (SSB) consumption is strongly associated with obesity. Autonomous motivation and self‐efficacy, key concepts of self‐determination theory, may influence SSB consumption. Low‐income mothers of young children experience disproportionate rates of obesity. Whether autonomous motivation and self‐efficacy are associated with SSB consumption in low‐income mothers of young children is unknown. This exploratory secondary data analysis explored whether autonomous motivation or self‐efficacy were associated with SBB consumption using data from a lifestyle intervention for low‐income, overweight or obese mothers with young children. Participants ( N = 311) completed surveys assessing autonomous motivation, self‐efficacy, and SSB consumption at baseline, after the 16‐week intervention, and at 3‐month follow‐up. Using baseline data, we performed linear regression models to explore associations of self‐efficacy and autonomous motivation with SSB consumption. We also performed mixed effects models to explore whether autonomous motivation or self‐efficacy were associated with SSB consumption over time. At baseline, a one‐point increase in autonomous motivation and self‐efficacy were associated with 4.36 ( p < 0.001) and 6.43 ( p = 0.025) fewer ounces of SSB consumption per day, respectively. In longitudinal models, SSB consumption decreased over time. Change in SSB consumption was associated with self‐efficacy ( B = −4.88; p = 0.015) and autonomous motivation ( B = −2.29; p = 0.008). Our findings suggest self‐efficacy and autonomous motivation may influence SSB consumption among mothers of young children with overweight and obesity. Further investigation should explore if self‐efficacy and autonomous motivation have long‐term effects on SSB consumption.
Article
Full-text available
Background: An understanding of physical activity through mediators of behaviour change is important to evaluate the efficacy of interventions. The purpose of this review is to update prior reviews with meta-analysis in order to evaluate the state of physical activity interventions that include proposed mediators of behaviour change. Methods: Literature was identified through searching of five key databases. Studies were eligible if they described a published experimental or quasi-experimental trial in English examining the effect of an intervention on physical activity behaviour and mediators in non-clinical adult populations with the necessary statistical information to be included in the meta-analytic structural equation modelling analysis. Results: Fifty-one articles (49 samples) met the eligibility criteria. Small overall effects were identified for mediation paths a (r = .16; 95% CI = .10 to .22), b (r = .21; 95% CI .16 to .27), and c (r = .24; 95% CI .12 to .35), c’ (r = .05 to .19) and ab (r = .02 to .07) that showed similar findings by theory and construct. Conclusion: The effect sizes seen in physical activity interventions are mediated by our current theories, but the effects are very small and no one construct/theory appears to be a critical driver of the mediated effect compared to any other. Innovation and increased fidelity of interventions is needed.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: The present study tested the efficacy of a theory-based online intervention comprising motivational (autonomy support) and volitional (implementation intention) components to reduce pre-drinking alcohol consumption and alcohol-related harm. Method: Undergraduate students (N = 202) completed self-report measures of constructs from psychological theories, pre-drinking alcohol consumption, and alcohol-related harm at baseline and were randomly assigned to one of four intervention conditions in a 2 (autonomy support: present/absent) × 2 (implementation intention: present/absent) design. Participants completed follow-up measures of all variables at 4 weeks post-intervention. All participants received national guidelines on alcohol consumption and an e-mail summary of intervention content at its conclusion. Participants also received weekly SMS messages in the 4-week post-intervention period restating content relevant to their intervention condition. Results: Neither statistically significant main effect for either the autonomy support or implementation intention intervention components nor an interaction effect was found on the outcome measures. However, statistically significant reductions in pre-drinking alcohol consumption and alcohol-related harm were observed across all groups at follow-up, when compared to baseline. Conclusion: Reductions in outcome measures were likely related to elements common to each condition (i.e., provision of national guidelines, assessment of outcome measures, e-mail summary, and SMS messages), rather than motivational and volitional components.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined the feasibility of a 10-week self-determination theory-based training programme delivered to 24 group exercise instructors. Feasibility of implementing the programme was explored in terms of 1) recruitment, 2) rates of adherence and retention, and 3) acceptability. Results revealed modest uptake (51%) and excellent workshop attendance (either in-person or online) and retention rates (96%). Coding of the interview data revealed 9 main themes with regard to the acceptability of the training programme: satisfaction with content, challenging current motivational practices, perceived relevance of the motivational strategies, social support, mix of theory and practice, usefulness of learning resources, need-supportive delivery, multi-stage structure, and value for professional development. The findings can inform the development, design, and implementation of future research with the target population.
Article
Full-text available
Leader autonomy support (LAS) refers to a cluster of supervisory behaviors that are theorized to facilitate self-determined motivation in employees, potentially enabling well-being and performance. We report the results of a meta-analysis of perceived LAS in work settings, drawing from a database of 754 correlations across 72 studies (83 unique samples, N = 32,870). Results showed LAS correlated strongly and positively with autonomous work motivation, and was unrelated to controlled work motivation. Correlations became increasingly positive with the more internalized forms of work motivation described by self-determination theory. LAS was positively associated with basic needs, well-being, and positive work behaviors, and was negatively associated with distress. Correlations were not moderated by the source of LAS, country of the sample, publication status, or the operationalization of autonomy support. In addition, a meta-analytic path analysis supported motivational processes that underlie LAS and its consequences in workplaces. Overall, our findings lend support for autonomy support as a leadership approach that is consistent with self-determination and optimal functioning in work settings. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (10.1007/s11031-018-9698-y) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Preprint
While evidence suggests that interventions based on self-determination theory can be effective in motivating adoption and maintenance of health-related behaviors, and in promoting adaptive psychological outcomes, the motivational techniques that comprise the content of these interventions have not been comprehensively identified or described. The aim of the present study was to develop a classification system of the techniques that comprise self-determination theory interventions, with satisfaction of psychological needs as an organizing principle. Candidate techniques were identified through a comprehensive review of self-determination theory interventions and nomination by experts. The study team developed a preliminary list of candidate techniques accompanied by labels, definitions, and function descriptions of each. Each technique was aligned with the most closely-related psychological need satisfaction construct (autonomy, competence, or relatedness). Using an iterative expert consensus procedure, participating experts (N=18) judged each technique on the preliminary list for redundancy, essentiality, uniqueness, and the proposed link between the technique and basic psychological need. The procedure produced a final classification of 21 motivation and behavior change techniques (MBCTs). Redundancies between final MBCTs against techniques from existing behavior change technique taxonomies were also checked. The classification system is the first formal attempt to systematize self-determination theory intervention techniques. The classification is expected to enhance consistency in descriptions of self-determination theory-based interventions in health contexts, and assist in facilitating synthesis of evidence on interventions based on the theory. The classification is also expected to guide future efforts to identify, describe, and classify the techniques that comprise self-determination theory-based interventions in multiple domains.
Article
Background: Most people in developed countries are not physically active enough to reap optimal health benefits so effective promotion strategies are warranted. Theories of behaviour change are essential to understand physical activity and provide an organizing framework for effective intervention. The purpose of this paper was to provide a narrative historical overview of four key theoretical frameworks (social cognitive, humanistic, dual process, socioecological) that have been applied to understand and change physical activity over the last three decades. Methods: Our synthesis of research included the brief history, basic efficacy, strengths, and potential weaknesses of these approaches when applied to physical activity. Results: The dominant framework for understanding physical activity has been in the social cognitive tradition, and it has provided valuable information on key constructs linked to physical activity. The humanistic framework for understanding physical activity has seen a surge in research in the last decade and has demonstrated initial effectiveness in both explaining and intervening on behaviour. The most recent and understudied framework for understanding physical activity is dual process models, which may have promise to provide a broader perspective of motivation by considering non-conscious and hedonic determinants of physical activity. Finally, the individual-level focus of all three of these approaches is contrasted by the socioecological framework, which has seen considerable research attention in the last 15 years and has been instrumental in understanding the role of the built environment in physical activity behaviour and critical to shaping public health policy in government. Conclusions: Despite the strengths of all four frameworks, we noted several weaknesses of each approach at present and highlight several newer applications of integrated models and dynamic models that may serve to improve our understanding and promotion of physical activity over the next decade.
Article
A systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted of the techniques used to promote psychological need satisfaction and motivation within health interventions based on self-determination theory (SDT; Ryan & Deci, 2017). Eight databases were searched from 1970-2017. Studies including a control group and reporting pre- and post-intervention ratings of SDT-related psychosocial mediators (namely perceived autonomy support, need satisfaction and motivation) with children or adults were included. Risk of bias was assessed using items from the Cochrane risk of bias tool. 2496 articles were identified of which 74 met inclusion criteria; 80% were RCTs or cluster RCTs. Techniques to promote need supportive environments were coded according to two established taxonomies (BCTv1 and MIT), and 21 SDT-specific techniques, and grouped into 18 SDT based strategies. Weighted mean effect sizes were computed using a random effects model; perceived autonomy support g = 0.84, autonomy g = 0.81, competence g = 0.63, relatedness g = 0.28, and motivation g = 0.41. One-to-one interventions resulted in greater competence satisfaction than group-based (g = 0.96 vs. 0.28), and competence satisfaction was greater for adults (g = 0.95) than children (g = 0.11). Meta-regression analysis showed that individual strategies had limited independent impact on outcomes, endorsing the suggestion that a need supportive environment requires the combination of multiple co-acting techniques.
Article
Background: Most patients risk gaining weight in the years after knee replacement, adding further concern to a population that is mostly overweight/obese prior to surgery. Objective: Via a randomised pilot study, we assessed changes in weight during a Patient Centered Weight Loss Program (PACE) initiated either before or after knee replacement, while simultaneously examining the feasibility of recruiting and retaining participants over 26 weeks. Methods: Recruitment outreach was made to 133 patients scheduled for knee replacement. Sixteen participants were randomised to a 14-session weight loss program that started either ≤6 weeks before surgery (PACE) or at 12 weeks post-op (Delayed PACE). Repeated measures ANOVAs were used to examine preliminary changes in weight, function, patient-reported outcomes, and physical activity across time (baseline/pre-op, 12 and 26 weeks after surgery) and group. Results: Retention was 75% and 69% at 12 and 26 weeks after surgery, respectively. Weight significantly decreased across the 26 weeks (P<0.001). A group by time interaction (P=0.03) demonstrated Delayed PACE [-7.6±5.9kg (-7.9±5.9%)] lost significantly more weight than PACE [-2.5±2.7kg (-2.6±2.6%)] participants at 26 weeks. Significant improvements across time were seen for all function and patient reported outcomes, however activity did not change. Conclusion: Conducting a behavioural intervention was challenging but feasible in a knee replacement population, with preliminary evidence suggesting that initiating a program 12 weeks after surgery produces greater weight losses at 26 weeks compared to a program starting before knee replacement.