ChapterPDF Available

Rejected Children

Authors:
R
Rejected Children
Nivetha Prabaharan
1
and Natalie Spadafora
2
1
Department of Psychology, Brock University,
St. Catharines, ON, Canada
2
Department of Child and Youth Studies,
Brock University, St. Catharines, ON, Canada
Synonyms
Disliked;Low social status;Unpopular
Definition
Children or adolescents who are disliked by
many peers and liked by few peers.
Introduction
Sociometric status refers to how well-liked or
disliked an individual is by their peers.
Children who receive many positive nominations
(e.g., nominations for being liked) and few
negative nominations (e.g., nominations for
being disliked) are classied as popular, whereas
children who receive many negative nominations
and few positive nominations are classied
as rejected. A childs sociometric status can
inuence their social interactions, psycho-
social adjustment, and academic involvement.
Rejection from peers has been associated
with negative developmental outcomes, such
as loneliness, victimization, internalizing and
externalizing problems, and low academic
performance (Ladd 2006; Veenstra et al. 2007;
Zettergren 2003).
Characteristics of Rejected Children
In general, rejected children possess fewer
peer-valued characteristics, such as physical
attractiveness, style, athletic competence, and a
sense of humor, when compared to their peers.
They are also rated to be less socially competent
than their other peers and tend to be left out of
peer activities (Hymel et al. 1993). Rejected
children can be classied into two categories:
aggressive-rejected and withdrawn-rejected
children. In addition to low social status,
aggressive-rejected children exhibit aggressive
tendencies and behaviors which may elicit
dislike and exclusion from their peers. They are
rated as uncooperative, less socially competent,
and having difculty getting along with adults.
However, in terms of peer-valued characteristics,
they were not rated as negatively as withdrawn-
rejected children and were not always excluded
from peer activities (Hymel et al. 1993). Children
who experience peer rejection and exhibit
aggressive behavior were more at risk of
developing externalizing problems, such as
disruption in classrooms, hyperactive and
© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2020
T. K. Shackelford, V. A. Weekes-Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_181-1
distractible behavior, and delinquency. This effect
was stronger during early to middle childhood
than late childhood (Ladd 2006).
Withdrawn-rejected children display a
slightly different prole than aggressive-rejected
children. In comparison to their aggressive-
rejected peers, withdrawn-rejected children are
nominated more negatively by their peers
in regard to not possessing many peer-valued
characteristics. However, their social and behav-
ioral conduct does not fare as poorly as
aggressive-rejected children. Despite being able
to cooperate with their peers and get along
well with adults, similar to their peers of average
status, they are still excluded from peer groups
and activities (Hymel et al. 1993). Additionally,
children who are withdrawn and rejected by
their peers are more likely to also experience
internalizing problems, such as anxious and
depressive feelings. These children were more
likely to experience these problems in middle to
late childhood than early childhood (Ladd 2006).
Victimization
In addition to being disliked by their peers,
rejected children are also at a greater risk
of being mistreated by their peers. Their low
acceptance and social status within the peer
group increase their vulnerability to victimization,
especially by bullying aggression (Veenstra et al.
2007). Bullying refers to a specic type of
aggression in which the perpetrator has greater
power than the victim. Considering this power
imbalance, it is understandable that children
with low status are more often targeted as a victim
in bullying (Volk et al. 2014). From an adaptive
perspective, bullying may be a strategy for
children and adolescents to gain certain evolution-
ary advantages, such as social status (Volk et al.
2014). Children and adolescents who use
aggression instrumentally may choose to target
individuals who are easier to victimize. Rejected
children are more likely to be targeted for
victimization because of their low social status
and emotional and social vulnerability (Veenstra
et al. 2007). Further, research has found
differences between the two categories of rejected
children when it comes to victimization. For
example, withdrawn-rejected children are more
likely to be a target of bullying because they are
socially vulnerable and less likely to retaliate
(Veenstra et al. 2007). These characteristics can
make it easier for a powerful bully to aggress
toward the victim. By choosing a victim who is
less likely to retaliate or does not have friends to
protect them, bullies are able to use aggression
to gain certain benets, such as social status, while
minimizing potential costs. On the other hand,
aggressive-rejected children may be a target
of bullying due to their tendency to provoke
mistreatment from their peers due to their
aggressive behaviors and hostile attribution bias
(Schwartz et al. 2001). In this instance, bullying
may be a reactive action instigated by the victim.
Overall, it is important to consider that rejected
children are not only experiencing more victimi-
zation than their average or high status counter-
parts, but that this increase in victimization may
also be associated with other negative outcomes
or poor adjustment.
School Adjustment
Given the low social acceptance and likability
that rejected children experience, there are also
concerns of school adjustment and well-being.
For instance, a study conducted by Buhs and
Ladd (2001) found that rejected children were
more likely to experience poor treatment from
their peers, report loneliness, perform poorly
academically, and participate less in school. In
general, these students have a lowered desire to
attend school and are more disliked by their
teachers in comparison to their average status
peers. Additionally, rejected children are typically
perceived by their peers to be poor students
and perceived by their teachers to have low self-
condence and be involved in more conicts than
children of average status (Wentzel and Asher
1995). Rejected children tend to have poorer
academic performance and intelligence levels,
in comparison to their popular peers, who
tend to score above average (Zettergren 2003).
2 Rejected Children
Further, it seems that rejected girls have negative
attitudes toward school in general, while the
dropout rate for rejected boys was signicantly
higher than other boys, emphasizing the
potential risk factor academically for rejected
children (Zettergren 2003). Moreover, research
by Wentzel and Asher (1995) also found
academic differences between aggressive-rejected
and submissive-rejected children, in that
aggressive-rejected children tend to have more
issues academically. Specically, aggressive-
rejected children reported being less interested
in school, more impulsive learners, and less likely
to be civil and compliant in class. Further, these
students were perceived by teachers as being
less independent and less likely to be nominated
as liked by both teachers and students, while
these same characteristics were not found in
the submissive-rejected subgroup (Wentzel and
Asher 1995).
Psychosocial Adjustment
Aside from victimization and poor school
adjustment, rejected children may also face
psychosocial challenges. A study by Nesdale
and Lambert (2007) found that peer-group
rejection signicantly increased anxiety in
rejected children. Further, as children get older,
peer rejection was found to increase the childs
risk-taking behavior, while peer acceptance was
found to decrease this behavior (Nesdale and
Lambert 2007). Low levels of peer acceptance
during adolescence were found to be associated
with lower levels of self-regulation and were
found to affect their decision-making. These
adolescents were less able to tolerate distress and
were more emotionally sensitive when dealing
with negative loss in their life (King et al. 2018).
Further, since we know that research has demon-
strated that being a victim of bullying can be
associated with higher levels of social isolation,
anxiety, and depression (Veenstra et al. 2007),
these rejected children who are also victimized
may be more at risk for these outcomes.
Conclusion
In summary, children who face peer rejection are
at greater risk of experiencing a myriad of other
negative developmental outcomes. For decades,
researchers and practitioners have worked on how
to alleviate the difculties and maladjustment
associated with peer rejection. An emphasis has
been put into providing these children with
social skills training and opportunities to integrate
with their peers, such as strategic seating plans
and classroom activities (Bierman 2004). By
providing children a safe classroom environment
without the fear of mistreatment and rejection
from peers, rejected children can participate
more condently in class and improve their
scholastic adjustment and academic competence
(Buhs and Ladd 2001). Furthermore, research has
found that rejected children who possessed more
peer-valued characteristics were less likely to be
targeted for victimization, suggesting that this
may provide an avenue for intervention (Knack
et al. 2012). For example, participating in
sports and peer-valued activities may allow these
children to better integrate with their peers,
develop peer-valued characteristics, and experi-
ence less rejection. Overall, peer rejection
has negative and persistent implications for chil-
dren in many areas of their life, which highlights
the importance of early intervention so that these
children may experience more positive develop-
mental outcomes.
Cross-References
Peer Rejection, Gender Differences in
Response to
Peer Rejection, Sex Differences in Initiation of
Peer Relationships in Childhood
Peers
Status Competition and Peer Relationships in
Childhood
Status Hierarchies
Victims
Rejected Children 3
References
Bierman, K. L. (2004). Peer rejection: Developmental
processes and intervention strategies. New York:
Guilford Press.
Buhs, E. S., & Ladd, G. W. (2001). Peer rejection as an
antecedent of young childrens school adjustment: An
examination of mediating processes. Developmental
Psychology, 37(4), 550.
Hymel, S., Bowker, A., & Woody, E. (1993). Aggressive
versus withdrawn unpopular children: Variations in
peer and self-perceptions in multiple domains.
Child Development, 64(3), 879896.
King, K. M., McLaughlin, K. A., Silk, J., &
Monahan, K. C. (2018). Peer effects on self-regulation
in adolescence depend on the nature and quality of the
peer interaction. Development and Psychopathology,
30(4), 13891401.
Knack, J. M., Tsar, V., Valliancourt, T., Hymel, S., &
McDougall, P. (2012). What protects rejected
adolescents from also being bullied by their peers?
The moderating role of peer-valued characteristics.
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 22(3), 467479.
Ladd, G. W. (2006). Peer rejection, aggressive or
withdrawn behavior, and psychological maladjustment
from ages 5 to 12: An examination of four predictive
models. Child Development, 77(4), 822846.
Nesdale, D., & Lambert, A. (2007). Effects of experimen-
tally manipulated peer rejection on childrens negative
affect, self-esteem, and maladaptive social behavior.
International Journal of Behavioral Development,
31(2), 115122.
Schwartz, D., Proctor, L. J., & Chien, D. H. (2001).
The aggressive victim of bullying: Emotional and
behavioral dysregulation as a pathway to victimization
by peers. In J. Juvonen & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer
harassment in school: The plight of the vulnerable
and victimized (pp. 147174). New York: Guilford
Press.
Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Zijlstra, B. J. H.,
De Winter, A. F., Verhulst, F. C., & Ormel, J. (2007).
The dyadic nature of bullying and victimization:
Testing a dual perspective theory. Child Development,
78, 18431954.
Volk, A. A., Dane, A. V., & Marini, Z. A. (2014). What is
bullying? A theoretical redenition. Developmental
Review, 34, 327343.
Wentzel, K. R., & Asher, S. R. (1995). The academic lives
of neglected, rejected, popular, and controversial
children. Child Development, 66(3), 754763.
Zettergren, P. (2003). School adjustment in adolescence
for previously rejected, average and popular children.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 73(2),
207221.
4 Rejected Children
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Adolescence is a critical period for the development of self-regulation, and peer interactions are thought to strongly influence regulation ability. Simple exposure to peers has been found to alter decisions about risky behaviors and increase sensitivity to rewards. The link between peer exposure and self-regulation is likely to vary as a function of the type and quality of peer interaction (e.g., rejection or acceptance). Little is known about how the nature of interactions with peers influences different dimensions of self-regulation. We examined how randomization to acceptance or rejection by online “virtual” peers influenced multiple dimensions of self-regulation in a multisite community sample of 273 adolescents aged 16–17 years. Compared to a neutral condition, exposure to peers produced increases in cold cognitive control, but decreased hot cognitive control. Relative to peer acceptance, peer rejection reduced distress tolerance and increased sensitivity to losses. These findings suggest that different dimensions of adolescent self-regulation are influenced by the nature of the peer context: basic cognitive functions are altered by mere exposure to peers, whereas more complex decision making and emotion regulation processes are influenced primarily by the quality of that exposure.
Article
Full-text available
A short-term longitudinal design (N = 399) was used to examine peer relations processes that may mediate the relation between peer rejection and children's emotional and academic adjustment during kindergarten. These proposed mediating processes extend the current literature by explicating behavioral pathways via which the attitudinal construct of peer rejection may affect adjustment outcomes. Structural equatiton modeling results supported the hypothesis that negative peer treatment (e.g., victimization, refusal of peer group entry bids, and exclusion from peer activities) and classroom participation partially mediate the relationship between rejection and adjustment outcomes. Rejected children were more likely to experience negative peer treatment, more likely to show decreases in classroom participation, and more likely to report loneliness, to express a desire to avoid school, and to perform less well on achievement measures.
Article
Adolescents rejected by peers are often targets of bullying. However, peer rejection is not a sure path to victimization. We examined whether characteristics valued by peers (i.e., attractiveness, wealth, academic, and athletic ability) moderated the relationship between peer rejection and victimization. We predicted rejected adolescents high on peer-valued characteristics (PVCs) would receive fewer nominations for being victimized than adolescents with lower levels. Canadian students in Grades 6–10 (N = 549; Mage = 13.32, SD = 1.50) provided peer nominations for peer victimization, rejection, attractiveness, wealth, and academic and athletic competence. As hypothesized, each PVC moderated the relationship between peer rejection and peer victimization. No sex differences were found. This study demonstrates the buffering effects of nonbehavioral characteristics valued by peers.
Article
The authors focus their analyses on one distinct subgroup of peer harassment victims: children who are aggressive and victimized. The chapter provides a comprehensive review of relevant research assessing the prevalence, behavioral characteristics, and psychosocial adjustment of aggressive victims. This review suggests that this subgroup of victims is characterized by irritable, impulsive, and overly reactive behavioral responses, similar to children identified with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and/or Conduct Disorder (CD). The authors posit that emotional and behavioral dysregulation might account for the problems of this small but apparently high risk group of victims. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
This book takes a developmental perspective and focuses on understanding and treating peer rejection in middle childhood during the critical grade school years when core social skills and attitudes develop, although some of the material is also relevant to younger children and to adolescents. The book is divided into three general parts, focusing on understanding problematic peer relations (Chapters 1-4), assessment strategies (Chapters 5-9), and intervention methods (Chapters 10-14). Part I provides a brief summary of an extensive body of research that has explored the causes, correlates, and consequences of peer rejection. Interview excerpts and case examples illustrate some of the various forms that peer rejection processes can take during the grade school years, and the impact of those experiences on social-emotional development. Part II describes assessment strategies, with an emphasis on assessments for intervention design and evaluation. Part III focuses on intervention strategies, and highlights developmental and clinical research that provides guidance to the design of effective social competence coaching programs. A major goal of this book is to highlight the importance of integrating developmental research on peer rejection processes with the design and evaluation of interventions for rejected children--an interface that will support further advances in both theoretical and applied domains. On the one hand, familiarity with developmental research can inform intervention design, contributing to more sophisticated and effective intervention strategies. On the other hand, significant research is usually produced by persons who have acquired insights as a function of close familiarity with a phenomenon. Intervention efforts provide this familiarity, as they involve sustained personal contact with rejected children and offer opportunities to observe, and test empirically, processes of change. This book is designed to provide a comprehensive and critical appraisal of the empirical literature on peer rejected children and peer rejection processes, and of its implications for intervention design and evaluation. I hope that it will serve both to clarify the current state of knowledge in this area, and also to stimulate further inquiry and progress in this field. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Children (n = 88) aged 8 and 10 years participated in a minimal group study in which their rejection versus acceptance, by one other person versus a group of three people, was experimentally manipulated. Analysis of their self-reported negative affect, self-esteem, and maladaptive social behavior, revealed that, regardless of the source of the rejection (i.e., an individual versus a group), peer rejection caused a significant increase in the children's negative affect, but had no effect on their self-esteem. The findings also indicated that peer rejection instigated an increased tendency towards maladaptive social behavior, and that the latter effect was fully mediated by the children's negative affect. The implications of the findings for peer-rejection research are discussed. © 2007 The International Society for the Study of Behavioural Development.
Article
The purposes of this study were to examine academically relevant characteristics of different sociometric status groups and to learn about the academic orientations of behavioral subgroups of rejected children. Results from a sample of 423 sixth and seventh graders (ages 11-13) suggested that sociometrically neglected children have quite positive academic profiles. When compared with average status children, these students reported higher levels of motivation, were described by teachers as more self-regulated learners, as more prosocial and compliant, and as being better liked by teachers. Analyses of two behavioral subgroups of rejected children indicated that aggressive-rejected but not submissive-rejected children have problematic academic profiles. Relations of neglected and aggressive-rejected status to academic adjustment in young adolescents' lives is discussed.
Article
The present study examines whether subgroups of unpopular children differ in terms of competence in multiple domains. Specifically, subgroups of aggressive unpopular, withdrawn unpopular, and aggressive-withdrawn unpopular and average status children were identified on the basis of peer evaluations. The subgroups were then compared in terms of peer and self-perceptions of competence in various nonsocial and social domains. Results indicated that the 3 subgroups of unpopular children exhibited distinct profiles according to peer perceptions, with aggressive-withdrawn unpopular children being viewed as deficient in virtually every area assessed, and aggressive unpopular and withdrawn unpopular children viewed as exhibiting particular strengths and weaknesses across domains. In terms of self-perceptions, results indicated that withdrawn-unpopular children expressed more accurate, but negative self-evaluations, while children in the aggressive subgroups tended to overestimate their competencies.
Article
Earlier research shows that peer-rejected children are at risk of a wide range of subsequent adjustment difficulties in different social contexts, as, for example, in school. This study investigated the academic performance and school adjustment in adolescence of children with different peer status in middle childhood. Age 15 boys and girls (N=90), who at age 10 and 11 were sociometrically rejected, popular, or of average popularity in their school class. School marks, intelligence scales, and self-reports were used as adjustment measures. School dropout rate for boys was also included. The academic performance and intelligence level of rejected boys and girls were short of the standards of children from the other status groups, while the scores of popular boys and girls were of superior standard. There were some slight indications that rejected girls (but not rejected boys) had negative attitudes towards school and schoolwork, and that popular girls had positive school attitudes. The school dropout rate of rejected boys was much higher than that of other boys. The results show that the rejected children are a risk group for school problems also over a long period of time. Considering the important developmental aspects of the adolescence years, there appear to be good reasons, therefore, to worry about the future adulthood adjustment of peer-rejected children.