ArticlePDF Available

The impact of intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity on ethical decision-making in management in a non-Western and highly religious country

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The primary purpose of this study was to explore the indirect effect of intrinsic religiosity and extrinsic religiosity on ethical intention through ethical judgment. A review of the literature shows the need for more research at the intersection of religiosity and ethics, especially in non-Western, highly religious contexts. This research, therefore, addresses the research question: Do intrinsic religiosity and extrinsic religiosity indirectly impact ethical intention through influencing the ethical judgment of management professionals? Data were gathered from members of the Management Association of Pakistan through a questionnaire. Pearson correlation results show the overall trend between the constructs of interest. Multiple regression results show that both intrinsic religiosity and extrinsic religiosity are significant positive predictors of ethical judgment. Ethical judgment was also found to be a significant, positive predictor of ethical intention. The main contribution of the study is evidence that ethical judgment acts as a mediator between religiosity (whether intrinsic or extrinsic) and ethical intention in a non-Western highly religious context. This research also found that intrinsic religiosity impacts ethical intention directly as well as indirectly through ethical judgment, but extrinsic religiosity influences ethical intention only through its effect on ethical judgment. We discuss our results along with practical and research implications, and limitations of this research are highlighted to guide future research.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Munich Personal RePEc Archive
The impact of intrinsic and extrinsic
religiosity on ethical decision-making in
management in a non-Western and
highly religious country
Tariq, Samia and Ansari, Nighat G. and Alvi, Tariq Hameed
Institute of Administrative Sciences, University of the Punjab,
Lahore, Pakistan., Institute of Administrative Sciences, University of
the Punjab, Lahore, Pakistan., Business School, University of
International Business and Economics (UIBE), Beijing, China
23 October 2019
Online at https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/96971/
MPRA Paper No. 96971, posted 16 Nov 2019 10:59 UTC
1
The Impact of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Religiosity on Ethical
Decision-Making in Management in a Non-Western and Highly
Religious Country
Samia Tariq, ORCID: 0000-0001-6505-7655
Lecturer
Institute of Administrative Sciences, University of the Punjab, Lahore, Pakistan.
samia.ias@pu.edu.pk
Dr. Nighat G. Ansari
Assistant Professor
Institute of Administrative Sciences, University of the Punjab, Lahore, Pakistan.
ngansari@yahoo.com
Tariq Hameed Alvi (corresponding author), ORCID: 0000-0002-0001-3739
Business School, University of International Business and Economics (UIBE), Beijing,
China
alvi.research@gmail.com
Note. The paper is based on the Mphil dissertation of the first author under the supervi-
sion of second author. The paper has been published in Asian Journal of Business Ethics
(AJBE). The final version as published in AJBE can be found at
https://doi.org/10.1007/s13520-019-00094-3.
2
Abstract
The primary purpose of this study was to explore the indirect effect of intrinsic religiosity
and extrinsic religiosity on ethical intention through ethical judgment. A review of the
literature shows the need for more research at the intersection of religiosity and ethics,
especially in non-Western, highly religious contexts. This research, therefore, addresses
the research question: Do intrinsic religiosity and extrinsic religiosity indirectly impact
ethical intention through influencing the ethical judgment of management professionals?
Data were gathered from members of the Management Association of Pakistan through a
questionnaire. Pearson correlation results show the overall trend between the constructs
of interest. Multiple regression results show that both intrinsic religiosity and extrinsic
religiosity are significant positive predictors of ethical judgment. Ethical judgment was
also found to be a significant, positive predictor of ethical intention. The main
contribution of the study is evidence that ethical judgment acts as a mediator between
religiosity (whether intrinsic or extrinsic) and ethical intention in a non-Western highly
religious context. This research also found that intrinsic religiosity impacts ethical
intention directly as well as indirectly through ethical judgment, but extrinsic religiosity
influences ethical intention only through its effect on ethical judgment. We discuss our
results along with practical and research implications, and limitations of this research are
highlighted to guide future research.
Keywords Religiosity · Intrinsic religiosity · Extrinsic religiosity · Ethical decision-
making · Non-Western highly religious contexts · Indirect effect
3
Introduction
To add to the religion-ethics literature in general and non-Western highly religious
context ethics literature in particular, this study reveals evidence that intrinsic religiosity
and extrinsic religiosity significantly influence ethical intention through the ethical
judgment of management professionals faced with ethical decision-making.
The last few decades have witnessed an increased emphasis on ethics, potentially
because of business scandals in the corporate sector, and this focus has led researchers to
investigate the phenomenon of ethical decision-making (EDM) in greater detail. A review
of the literature confirms that EDM is a highly researched specialized area. This stream
of research has investigated individual factors such as demographics, personality, cultural
values, value orientation, personal values, decision styles, cognitive moral development,
religiosity, and spirituality. Organizational factors studied include rewards, ethical
culture, code of ethics, organization culture, subjective norms, organization size,
competitiveness, policies, and procedures; moderators and mediators have also been
investigated (Craft 2013; Lehnert et al. 2015). However, researchers have not sufficiently
investigated the factors mentioned above, especially in non-Western highly religious
contexts. These contexts, therefore, provide an opportunity for research contributions.
Although ethics research in non-Western contexts is gaining momentum (Chan et al.
2016), publications relating to highly religious, non-Western contexts remain limited.
Furthermore, Western and less-religious countries have witnessed more research
at the interplay of religion and ethics than their highly religious, non-Western
counterparts. Relationships of religion and ethics may differ across the spectrum of
highly religious to less-religious countries, and this potential inspires research to be
4
conducted in highly religious countries followed by cross-cultural research (Oumlil and
Balloun 2009; Parboteeah et al. 2008). The steady increase in the religiosity of people
around the world (CIA 2007, 2012, 2014; Moghadam 2004) has prompted researchers to
study the impact of religious beliefs on the EDM of managers, and how such religiosity
could reduce unethical behavior in the corporate world (Singhapakdi et al. 2013).
Religiosity, in contrast to religion, includes different religious activities, the
degree of dedication, and the belief in religious principles (Craft 2013). One factor which
has not received much attention in non-Western, highly religious contexts is the role of
religiosity in EDM despite the importance of religion in these contexts and the general
public belief that religion is a prerequisite for morality, a belief reported by the Pew
Research Institute (Pew Global Attitudes Project 2007). The limited research and the
mixed findings regarding the relationship between religiosity and EDM (Lehnert et al.
2015; Parboteeah et al. 2008) suggest that this relationship is complicated (Weaver and
Agle 2002). This complexity should motivate investigations of the relationship,
especially in different contexts, so that the phenomenon can be understood and explained
more clearly.
In order to achieve this objective, we consider religiosity from a two-dimensional
rather than a one-dimensional perspective; we look at the dimensions of intrinsic
religiosity and extrinsic religiosity, with the divergent focuses of living the religion vs.
using the religion, respectively (Allport and Ross 1967). With this approach, we
investigated the respective influence of each dimension on EDM. Religiosity means a
person’s belief in God and the degree to which the path prescribed by God is being
5
followed (McDaniel and Burnett 1990). Intrinsic religiosity reflects the inner spirit of the
religious tradition reflected in the dominant motivations of the person. It indicates a
commitment to the religion’s inherent principles and involvement in serving the religion
(Singhapakdi et al. 2013). Extrinsic religiosity denotes the utilitarian use of religion for
selfish motivations such as social approval or to gain personal benefits (Singhapakdi et al.
2013). The context of this research is Pakistan, with its profoundly religious population,
and we undertook this work with the hope that it might provide a better explanation of the
relationship between religiosity and ethical intention in this context, different from the
existing evidence from studies conducted mostly in Western and secular or less-religious
countries.
This study is significant from three standpoints: it is practical, it provides a
theoretical rationale, and it is based on an empirical study. The Pakistani public at large
believes that a person cannot be moral without a belief in God (Pew Global Attitudes
Project 2007). Our study aims to empirically test this using survey of managers in
corporate Pakistan, measuring their religiosity, ethical judgment, and ethical intentions
through their responses to four business ethics scenarios. We also utilize a theoretical
perspective by using the premier H-V model of ethics, wherein religion forms an
essential part of cultural environment and personal characteristics, theorizing that religion
can influence EDM (Hunt and Vitell 1986, 1993, 2006).
Additionally, there has been a significant number of calls to research the interplay
of religion and ethics because prior studies, especially in under-investigated contexts,
have not consistently captured the role of religiosity in that interplay. These calls have
6
emphasized the need to carefully investigate the relationship between religious self-
identity and ethical behavior (Weaver and Agle 2002), the importance and role of
particular religious viewpoints (Craft 2013), and a better understanding of the role of
religious values in EDM (Lehnert et al. 2015). We anticipate this study to be a fruitful
effort toward realizing the above objectives because the context of this research, Pakistan,
brings into consideration religious viewpoints different from those already studied in
Western and secular or less-religious contexts.
This research also helps to identify whether the mainstream constructs apply to
non-Western, highly religious contexts or need adaptation. Specifically, we investigated
the relationship between two dimensions of religiosity (intrinsic and extrinsic) and ethical
intention through ethical judgment in the context of a highly religious, non-Western,
developing country. This work strives to provide another perspective (ethical judgment as
a mediator of religiosity and ethical intention) on the mixed findings on the relationship
between religiosity and ethical judgment or intention. Such mixed findings have been
reported by studies that have been conducted mainly in Western contexts. Such Western
contexts are quite different from their non-Western counterparts in terms of religious
orientation and religious adherence.
Literature review
This section provides an overview of the existing literature, covering studies investigating
the relationship between religiosity and EDM, mixed findings on religiosity, the two-
dimensional nature of religiosity, and the relationship between ethical judgment and
ethical intention.
7
Earlier studies on the relationship between religiosity and EDM
Earlier experimental studies (Hegarty and Sims 1978, 1979) intending to find the
relationship between religious orientation and EDM (in particular, acceptance or rejection
of a kickback) did not find a significant relationship between the two. McNichols and
Zimmer (1985) found a significant correlation between strong religious beliefs and
negative attitudes towards the acceptability of unethical behaviors in eight out of ten
scenarios among undergraduate students. Kidwell et al. (1987) found no relationship
between denomination or church attendance and perception of what is ethical. Hegarty
and Sims (1978, 1979) and Kidwell et al. (1987) found no significant relationship
between religious orientation and EDM, and that finding was widely accepted for quite
some time.
Later, however, religiosity was found to be significantly related to the perception
of an ethical problem (Barnett et al. 1996). Afterward, a study investigating the
relationship between religiosity and ethics found a marginally significant positive
relationship in three of four scenarios, with p < 0.10, between the perception of an ethical
problem and religion (Singhapakdi, Salyachivin, et al. 2000). In another study,
individuals with strong religious beliefs were found to be less likely to assess an unethical
act as being fair (Wagner and Sanders 2001).
Literature reviews of research on religiosity in EDM
In a comprehensive literature review spanning 1996 to 2005 on the management,
spirituality, and religion domain,” Dean and Fornaciari (2007) called for further
innovative and interdisciplinary empirical research on religion and ethical behavior in
organizations owing to the paucity of research in this particular domain. In another
8
Academy of Management study, Weaver and Agle (2002) reported that there have been
relatively few empirical studies delineating the interaction of religion and business ethics,
and they even went on to assert that inability to discover clear connection between
religious role expectations and ethical behavior is analogous to a failure to analyze
religious role expectations sufficiently. They further emphasized the importance of
detailed analysis in this regard.
In another comprehensive literature review of EDM, spanning the years 2004 to
2011, two studies (Kurpis et al. 2008; Oumlil and Balloun 2009) were noted to have
investigated religion as a predictor of EDM, and the reviewer highlighted that the role of
religiosity in ethical decisions had not been consistently captured (Craft 2013). The first
study found that commitment to moral self-improvement is a better predictor of ethical
behavioral intentions than religiosity (Kurpis et al. 2008). In contrast, the second study
above found mixed results while examining the relationship between religiosity and
ethical intentions with US and Moroccan managers as respondents (Oumlil and Balloun
2009). In a following review built on and extending the aforementioned review, Lehnert
et al. (2015) highlighted two studies (Oumlil and Balloun 2009; Rawwas et al. 2006)
which found that religion was not an important factor in the context of EDM, while four
studies found a significant and positive relationship between them (Fernando and
Chowdhury 2010; Ho 2010; McCullough and Faught 2005; Vitell et al. 2009), and some
studies reported mixed results (Bloodgood et al. 2008; Ibrahim et al. 2008). The reviews
emphasized investigating the relationship between religion and EDM while taking into
account contingency factors (moderators and mediators) and noted that the decades of
investigations have not been able to clarify the relationship.
9
Mixed findings on the relationship between religiosity and EDM and the
multidimensional nature of the religiosity scale
Research on the relationship between religiosity and ethics has achieved mixed results
and no conclusive evidence, thus showing that the relationship between religion and
ethics is more nuanced than many researchers assumed (Barak-Corren and Bazerman
2017). These mixed findings necessitate testing this relationship in under-researched
contexts and unearthing the underlying and contingent mechanisms of the relationship. In
contrast to these mixed findings, the influence of religiosity on EDM is widely
acknowledged due to the analysis of models considering religion as a factor affecting
EDM (Bartels 1967; Bommer et al. 2013; Hunt and Vitell 2006). Additionally, a stream
of empirical studies has also found a positive relationship between the two (Hunt and
Vitell 1993; Rallapalli 1995; Singhapakdi et al. 2013; Singhapakdi, Marta, et al. 2000;
Walker et al. 2012).
Empirical studies also converge on the assertion that religiosity is best represented
by both its intrinsic and extrinsic dimensions (Cooper and Pullig 2013). Table 1 lists
selected literature works which employ multidimensional religiosity scales in EDM along
with their specific religiosity findings showing the relationships between religiosity
dimensions, ethical beliefs, and ethical intention.
***********************************************************************
Insert Table 1 here.
************************************************************************
10
The relationship between ethical judgment and ethical intention
Since ethical judgment appeared to be the most significant predictor of ethical intention
or behavior in almost all studies (Craft 2013; Ford and Richardson 1994; Lehnert et al.
2015; O’Fallon and Butterfield 2005), we use ethical judgment as a predictor of ethical
intention. This idea is in line with almost all EDM models (Dubinsky and Loken 1989;
Ferrell and Gresham 1985; Fritzsche 1991; Hunt and Vitell 2006; Jones 1991; Trevino
1986).
Ethics research in under-researched contexts
Ethics research has been pervasive in the US but lacking in other countries (Marta et al.
2004). A recent citation analysis of business ethics research from a global perspective
(Chan et al. 2016) revealed a trend that US institutions may be replaced by European
institutions when it comes to the impact of business ethics research. Asian geographical
regions that lead in this research area include Taiwan, Hong Kong, China, and South
Korea. The other regions of Asia should be the focus of more ethics research, especially
because of the high incidence of ethical transgressions in those regions. Notably, non-
Western, highly religious countries are underrepresented in research at the intersection of
religiosity and ethics (for exceptions see Kashif et al. 2017; Marta et al. 2004;
Singhapakdi, Salyachivin, et al. 2000).
The context of this study, Pakistan, has been the focus of very little research on
religion and ethics. As an exception, Kashif et al. (2017) found that religiosity moderates
the relationship between injunctive norms and behavioral intention and between
perceived behavioral control and behavioral intention. Empirical researchers have paid
11
little attention to how religiosity influences ethical intention indirectly through ethical
judgment, though this idea agrees with the H-V model discussed in the next section.
The above review shows that the literature has not consistently captured the
relationship between religiosity and ethical intention, and therefore, there is a research
gap. The next section methodically develops the argument that religiosity influences
ethical intention through the mediating mechanism of ethical judgment.
Theory
The Hunt-Vitell theory, also known as the H-V model (Hunt and Vitell 1986, 1993,
2006), guides this study because of its extensive use in studying EDM, its role both as a
normative and positive theory of EDM, and its applicability both in professional and
research contexts. This theory includes ethical judgment, intention, and behavior as its
core constituents, all of which are closely linked with the constructs of interest in our
study. The subsequent discussion is divided into three subsections: Religiosity as a
predictor of ethical judgment, Ethical judgment as a predictor of ethical intention, and
Religiosity as an indirect predictor of ethical intention through ethical judgment.
Religiosity as a predictor of ethical judgment
In most EDM theories, religion forms an essential part of the cultural environment
(Bommer et al. 2013; Ferrell and Gresham 1985; Hunt and Vitell 1986). In the revised H-
V theory, religion also constitutes an important part of personal characteristics that
ultimately influence various components of the EDM process (Hunt and Vitell 2006).
The literature contends that religion and religious values predominantly determine ethical
judgment whether through a direct route (Razzaque and Hwee 2002; Tse and Au 1997;
12
Wagner and Sanders 2001; Wimalasiri et al. 1996) or indirect route of religiosity to
deontological consideration to ethical judgment (Hunt and Vitell 2006). These theorists
contended that:
Unquestionably, an individual’s personal religion influences ethical decision-making.
A priori, compared with nonreligious people, one might suspect that (1) highly
religious people would have more clearly defined deontological norms and that (2)
such norms would play a stronger role in ethical judgments (Hunt and Vitell 2006, p.
4).
We propose the idea that religiosity is a determinant of ethical judgment. The H-V
model proposes that religiosity can impact ethical intention through ethical judgment as
part of an element of the cultural environment and personal characteristics. A manager
facing an ethical dilemma can form an ethical judgment on the situation based on
deontological evaluation (a strict universal truth standpoint) or teleological evaluations (a
utilitarian perspective). Almost all previous empirical studies conducted have found that
the most significant predictor of ethical intention is ethical judgment. Therefore, we argue
that religiosity is an indirect predictor of ethical intention through ethical judgment. This
approach, though consistent with the theory, is contrary to some studies that have
undertaken a direct approach of testing religiosity with ethical intention (Kurpis et al.
2008; Oumlil and Balloun 2009; Singhapakdi et al. 2013).
The previous section reviewed the literature on the relationship between
religiosity and EDM based on data from respondents mostly belonging to Western
contexts. We suspect that religiosity will exhibit a relatively stronger relationship with
13
ethical intention through ethical judgment in the part of the world having populations
with stronger religious affiliations than less-religious Western contexts. Because this
study’s respondents live in a country where religiosity is on the higher end of the
spectrum, the extent of the relationship between religiosity and EDM is likely to be more
profound, and thus could show a clearer picture of that relationship. Owing to the
complexity of the religiosity variable, H-V theorists (Hunt and Vitell 2006) have called
for using multidimensional religiosity scales to empirically test the relationship between
religiosity and EDM and thereby know more about the impact of religion on EDM. One
multidimensional scale was previously proposed by Allport and Ross (1967), who
considered intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity.
People having strong intrinsic religiosity will likely have more stringent and
clearly defined moral principles because their main motivation is following a religion,
which means they abide by the religion in all walks of their lives at any cost, internalizing
the doctrine, and living by the religion. These clear principles based on religious tradition
will help form their ethical judgments based on deontological considerations impacted by
their religious traditions. Therefore, we hypothesize that:
H1: There is a positive relationship between intrinsic religiosity and ethical
judgment in managers making decisions that have ethical content.
On the other hand, an “extrinsically motivated person uses his religion” (Allport
and Ross 1967, p. 434) for the individual’s own benefit, compared to the person high in
intrinsic religiosity who lives the religion. Similarly, an “extrinsic type turns to God, but
without turning away from self” (Allport and Ross 1967, p. 434). Such people exhibit
14
instrumental utilitarian motivations for religious behaviors, seeking social approval and
furthering of their business interests. Managers high in extrinsic religiosity do not always
make ethical principles based on their moral identity (Vitell et al. 2009). Since a manager
high in extrinsic religiosity uses the religion and does not live it, this instrumental
motivation will more likely lead to unethical judgment, so we contend:
H2: There is a negative relationship between extrinsic religiosity and ethical
judgment in managers making decisions having ethical content.
Ethical judgment as a predictor of ethical intention
According to H-V theory, ethical judgment is formed based on both the deontological and
teleological evaluation of the ethical problem at hand. Deontological evaluation, a
normative ethical position focused on duty, obligation, or rule-based ethics, largely
hinges on deontological norms that are essentially Kantian ethics and require that a
person should act morally in all circumstances. Teleological evaluation, also known as
consequentialist ethics, is essentially a means-ends theory advocating that ends can
justify means, and that the ends are more important than the means. Ethical judgment
stemming from deontological and teleological evaluation gives rise to intention that
finally translates into behavior. We consider the ethical judgment of the respondents to be
a black box (without investigating the underlying deontological and teleological
evaluations) and argue that it is a predictor of ethical intention.
Additionally, this argument is also in agreement with published research in which
ethical judgment has been found to be the most significant predictor of ethical intention,
as is the case for almost all empirical studies in comprehensive EDM literature reviews
15
(Craft 2013; Ford and Richardson 1994; Lehnert et al. 2015; O’Fallon and Butterfield
2005). Based on the above theoretical arguments and the findings of the empirical
studies, we formulate the following hypothesis:
H3: There is a positive relationship between ethical judgment and ethical
intention in managers making decisions having ethical content.
Religiosity as an indirect predictor of ethical intention through ethical
judgment
As already discussed, previous empirical studies have not been able to delineate the
relationship between religiosity and ethical intention despite the calls for discovering that
connection (Weaver and Agle 2002). Craft (2013) implicitly and Lehnert et al. (2015)
explicitly have called for research to examine mediating variables that can influence the
causal sequence of relationships in the EDM process. The finding of little or no
relationship between religiosity and ethical intention in some studies could be rooted in
the inability of religiosity to influence ethical intention directly. Guided by the H-V
model, we propose that religiosity does affect ethical intention but does so through the
indirect route of religiosity to ethical judgment to ethical intention. As already described,
we consider religiosity from a multidimensional perspective, splitting it into intrinsic
religiosity and extrinsic religiosity. A person whose main motivation is religion
internalizes and follows the religion and its principles at any cost (Allport 1950; Allport
and Ross 1967). Thus, such people are likely to form ethical intention through ethical
judgment either directly due to the overpowering influence of religion in their life
(religiosityethical judgmentethical intention) or indirectly through the formation of
deontological norms (Barak-Corren and Bazerman 2017; Piazza and Landy 2013) as
16
dictated by the religion (religiositydeontological normsethical judgmentethical
intention). Additionally, a symbolic-interactionist perspective of religiosity and
awareness-judgment-intention-behavior process in organizations also contends that the
relationship between religiosity and ethical intention depends on the salience of the
religion for the focal decision maker (Weaver and Agle 2002). Thus, managers who are
religiously intrinsically motivated will likely exhibit positively influenced ethical
intention through the formation of strong ethical judgment due to the salience of their
religious principles.
We, therefore, propose the following hypothesis:
H4: Intrinsic religiosity positively influences managers’ behavioral intentions
through ethical judgment formed in decision-making situations having ethical
content.
On the other hand, their instrumental approach towards religion will likely make
managers high in extrinsic religiosity have teleological dispositions leading to serving
themselves and using religion as a means, instead of having deontological dispositions.
Such managers do not make religious principles part of their identity (Vitell et al. 2011)
and use religion as a means to further their business and social approval. These managers
will likely be low on deontological norms, but high on teleological evaluations focused
on self-interest, subsequently displaying low ethical judgment, and therefore, low ethical
intention. Therefore, we propose the hypothesis:
17
H5: Extrinsic religiosity negatively influences managers’ behavioral intentions to
act ethically through the formation of low ethical judgments in decision situations
having ethical content.
In essence, we propose that both dimensions of religiosityintrinsic and
extrinsicinfluence ethical intention through ethical judgment, the former positively and
the latter negatively. The following is a schematic diagram of the theoretical framework.
************************************************************************
Insert Figure 1 here.
************************************************************************
Methods
The basic purpose of the study was to test hypotheses centered on the relationship
between ethical judgment, intrinsic religiosity, extrinsic religiosity, and ethical intention.
In this investigation, correlations are hypothesized between the predictors and outcome
variables in order to gain an understanding of the general trend of the analysis. The extent
of researcher interference is minimal, as this study has just measured the variables as they
are. We manipulated neither the variables of interest nor the setting, so the study setting
was non-contrived. The unit of analysis was the individual, each of whom was a
management professional. This cross-sectional study aimed to measure individual-related
concepts (such as ethical judgment, intrinsic religiosity, extrinsic religiosity, and ethical
intention).
18
Research setting
Despite an increase in secular orientations, especially among advanced societies of the
world, people with traditional religious views still constitute the vast majority of the
world population (Anheier and Isar 2007). In a 2010 estimate, nonreligious people
accounted for 9.66% of the world population while atheists constituted 2.01%, and
people adhering to some religion made up 88.33% of the world population (CIA 2014).
Estimates for 2009 were similar: 2.04% atheist, 9.42% non-religious, and 88.54%
religious (CIA 2012). Comparing these two estimates with a 2004 estimate, where
atheists were 2.32% of the world population, non-religious 11.77%, and religion-adherent
people 85.91%, an upward trend in religious adherence is evident (CIA 2007). This trend
holds in other years as well, except for a 2010 estimate, where a slight decrease of 0.22%
was witnessed from the 2009 estimate in overall adherence to a religion (CIA 2012).
According to The World Christian Encyclopedia (WCE), Pakistan, until 1970, was 0%
atheist, but that figure has risen to 0.1% in recent years. Despite this slight increase,
almost all of Pakistan’s population remains religiously orientated (Moghadam 2004). A
more recent world report stated that as of 2010, Pakistani people unaffiliated with any
religion make up less than 0.1% of the population (Hackett and Grim 2012). Pakistan is
also among the group of nations where more than 9 out of 10 respondents consider
religion to be very important (The Pew Global Attitudes Project 2008). Specifically,
almost all Pakistanis (98%) consider religion to be important (95% consider it very
important, and 3% consider it somewhat important). In research by the Pew Research
Center USA, Pakistan ranked quite high in religiosity with a score of 2.5 on a continuum
of 0 to 3. The cohort of countries that have scored 2.5 or higher includes Nigeria, Jordan,
Senegal, Indonesia, and Kuwait in addition to Pakistan. The general public in Pakistan
19
(88%) hold that one must believe in God to be moral (Pew Global Attitudes Project
2007). This great emphasis on religion and the belief that religiosity is a prerequisite for
morality should prompt researchers to empirically investigate the interplay of religiosity
and moral decision-making in these highly religious countries. For these reasons, we
chose Pakistan as our research setting.
This study is positioned along the lines of studies conducted in non-Western
(Fernando and Jackson 2006; Marta et al. 2004; Phau and Kea 2007) but highly religious
contexts (Oumlil and Balloun 2009). We were not able to find any study investigating the
relationship between religiosity and EDM in highly religious countries. Thus, this study
fills this gap.
The Sample
The study population consisted entirely of management professionals working in various
organizations in Pakistan. Because of the absence of a population frame of all
management professionals, it was impossible to draw a random sample from the
population. Therefore, we used the Management Association of Pakistan (MAP) as the
sampling frame. MAP, founded in 1964, is the largest and oldest representative and
professional body of management professionals in Pakistan. It has close relationships
with other international management bodies. It is a full member of the Asian Association
of Management Organizations, has signed an MOU with the All India Management
Association, and has a country representative in Pakistan from the Asian Institute of
Management (an organization based in the Philippines). The Lahore chapter of MAP has
about 250 institutional and 495 individual members. A census approach was employed to
solicit responses from the individual members of this MAP chapter.
20
Data collection method
We designed a questionnaire by adopting and adapting measures as listed in the
following “Measures and instrument” section. Questionnaires, along with a cover letter
and postage-paid envelope, were mailed to 307 members who were in good standing1 out
of the 495 individual members of the Lahore MAP. We strove to maintain anonymity due
to the sensitivity of the topic of research, as the respondents were more likely to give
candid and impartial responses in the absence of the researcher. After three months of
rigorous follow-up, we received 109 usable responses, a response rate of 35%, which is
acceptable in mailed questionnaire studies. Many articles in organizational research
mention response rates running from 30% to 94%, as can be seen in an Academy of
Management Review paper which examined published articles in the years 1979-83
(Mitchell 1985). A more recent study that examined survey response rates in articles
published in 17 refereed journals from 2000 to 2005 found an average response rate of
52.7% with a standard deviation of 20.4 where the survey was conducted with individuals
(Baruch and Holtom 2008). In Baruch and Holtom’s study, possible reasons for the low
rate of response, despite rigorous efforts to improve it, were suggested: the busy schedule
of the professionals, the length of the questionnaire, and the sensitivity of the topic of
research. Respondents in our study were clearly quite concerned about the authenticity of
the lead researcher and anonymity of their responses, which was reflected by the multiple
phone calls received by the lead author from various respondents.
Measures and instruments
1 “In good standing” means these members maintain their membership with MAP and regularly attend
MAP’s seminars and events.
21
The questionnaire items for the current study were adopted and adapted from two
different sources. Appendix 1 shows the four scenarios and ethical judgment/intention
items followed by the scales of intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity items. A seven-point
Likert scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree was employed to
measure the constructs concerned. The following paragraphs present a brief account of
the measures and instrumentation.
Religiosity
We follow one of the existing research streams (see Allport and Ross 1967; Cooper and
Pullig 2013; Singhapakdi et al. 2013) for the two-dimensional conceptualization of
religiosity. Our study, however, differs from the study of Singhapakdi et al. (2013) in that
our survey’s respondents work in many business function areas, not just marketing, and
four scenarios are used rather than just one. To be consistent with the theory, we
investigate the impacts of both intrinsic religiosity and extrinsic religiosity on ethical
intention through ethical judgment.
For the religiosity measure, we employ the widely used scale of Allport and Ross
(1967) to measure both intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity. The Allport scale is considered
a valid and reliable measure and has a reported reliability of 0.88 for the intrinsic
dimension and 0.70 for the extrinsic dimension (Singhapakdi et al. 2013). The original
scale, especially its extrinsic dimension, is not directly applicable to Pakistan due to the
wording of some of its items. Thus, we adapted the scale, as shown in the “Factor
analysis” section. Modifying the existing instruments is desirable if it tests different
aspects of EDM (Craft 2013). The advantage of using a multidimensional scale of
religiosity is that it gives a clearer picture of a person’s religiosity and wards off any
22
potential cancellation effect of intrinsic vs. extrinsic religiosity, such as happens in scales
that consider religiosity to be single-dimensional; such cancellation might lead to
insignificant or only somewhat significant results.
EDM
For measuring ethical judgment and ethical intention, we used scenarios
developed by Fritzsche and Becker (1983) owing to their broad and generic applicability
in ethics research. We also adopted one scenario from Singhapakdi et al. (2013).
Ethical judgment. Ethical judgment can be defined as the “perceived degree of
ethicalness of a particular action for solving an ethical problem” (Singhapakdi et al. 2013,
p. 184). We measure ethical judgment using business ethics scenarios, as is customary in
EDM research (Dubinsky and Loken 1989; Fritzsche and Becker 1983; Singhapakdi et al.
2013). Measurement proceeded by asking respondents to express their degree of
agreement or disagreement with the action in the scenario. The ethical judgment item is
I consider the action taken to be ethical.
Ethical intention. Ethical intention can be defined as “an individual’s readiness
or willingness to engage in a particular action” (Kish-Gephart et al. 2010, p. 2). In the
current study, ethical intention is measured using business ethics scenarios, as discussed
in the above paragraph. The item for ethical intention is I would be likely to take the
same action in this situation.”
Ethical behavior. Ethical behavior is the actual behavior undertaken by a person
in a decision-making situation. Ethical behavior is defined as a behavior that is both
“legal and morally acceptable to the larger community” (Jones 1991, p. 367). Since it is
23
difficult to gauge actual (un)ethical behavior due to its sensitive nature and the lack of
access to actual decision situations in the referent organizations, ethical intention is used
as a proxy for ethical behavior, as is customary in ethics research. This approach is also
in line with the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen 1985, 1991, 2011). Apart from using
ethical intention as a proxy of ethical behavior, ethical behavior could also be considered
an outcome variable which was not measured.
Analyses
We employed correlation analysis and multiple regression to analyze the data. The
correlation data was used to understand the association between the study’s constructs,
while OLS linear regression was used to test the hypotheses. Mediation was tested using
the bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval mediation analysis.
Results
This section discusses the demographic profile of the respondents, factor analysis to
ascertain the validity of the constructs, reliability analysis, and correlation analysis
followed by the results of the hypothesis testing.
Demographic profiles of the respondents were collected, including gender, age,
and qualifications, along with responses on the variables of interest. The following table
summarizes the demographic data.
************************************************************************
Insert Table 2 here
************************************************************************
24
Factor analysis
We employed valid and reliable scales to measure study concepts. These scales claim to
be neutral and independent of a culture or a country setting but, to make sure that these
scales are equally applicable in Pakistan, factor analysis was conducted to ascertain their
validity in Pakistan. The religiosity scale needed slight adaptation for the Pakistani
setting because Muslims offer their prayers five times a day with Friday prayer being the
largest meeting, and the term “religious service” used in previous studies is not often
used for regular prayers. Therefore, “religious service” was further qualified by putting
the phrase “(for example, Jumma / Sunday service)” after it. Table 3 below shows only
the five adapted items out of the original seven scale items. The item Prayer is for peace
and happiness was split into two items, Prayer is for peace and Prayer is for
happiness,” for clarity. This also avoids a double-barreled item that is potentially
problematic since respondents may wish to respond differently for the terms peace and
happiness.”
**********************************************************
Insert Table 3 here.
**********************************************************
The need to adapt the scale was recognized when the original scale was
administered to 60 MBA Muslim Pakistani students with more than 50 students enquiring
about exactly what we meant by religious service. On closely interviewing those
students, it was revealed that the items in question need to be adapted for the local
setting. Subsequently, the items containing “religious service(s)” were rephrased and
25
improved and a subsequent administration to these students and another group of 40
students revealed no issues with the new items.
Like other statistical techniques, factor analysis can be performed if the data
meets certain prerequisite assumptions. Thus, necessary assumption testing for each scale
was performed, revealing that all factor analysis assumptions were met (see section B of
Appendix 2).
The ethical intention was measured by soliciting behavioral intentions of each
respondent for four different scenarios having ethical content. The ethical content varied
from one scenario to another, and it was foreseen that respondents might have replied
differently to each scenario, which might have resulted in too much variation. Thus, a
factor analysis of all the varying responses was required to ascertain whether an
aggregate score (i.e., ethical intention construct) of each of the four ethical intention
items, corresponding to each of the four scenarios, is warranted. The same rationale
applies to ethical judgment, whose factor analysis results are described next because the
same set of scenarios was used to gauge ethical judgment. For ethical intention, all the
factor loadings were higher than 0.67 for all four scenarios, thus confirming that
aggregate scores of all these scenarios can be used as a composite measure of ethical
intention. In the case of ethical judgment, all four items (corresponding to the four
scenarios) had loadings higher than 0.75 on a single factor, thus validating aggregate
ethical judgment as a composite scale to measure it.
Factor analysis of intrinsic religiosity revealed that all the items had loadings
greater than 0.75, thus validating the scale. Factor analysis of extrinsic religiosity,
26
however, showed that three items—“I go to religious services (for example, Jumma /
Sunday service) mainly because it helps me to make friends.; “I go to religious services
(for example, Jumma / Sunday service) mostly to spend time with my friends.”; and “I go
to religious services (for example, Jumma / Sunday service) mainly because I enjoy
seeing people whom I know.”—had poor loadings of -0.11, -0.10, and -0.09, so these
items were deleted from the analysis. For the other items—“Prayer is for peace.”; “Prayer
is for happiness.”; “What religion offers me the most is comfort in times of
trouble/sorrow.”; and “I pray to gain relief and protection.”— the loadings were at least
0.839, with an average of 0.90.
Reliability Analysis
Reliability was measured through Cronbach α, Omega, and Greatest Lower Bound
(GLB). Omega and GLB2 were used owing to a host of criticisms of Cronbach α and the
fact that Omega and GLB have been demonstrated to better measure internal consistency
and scale reliability (Peters 2014). All the constructs have reliabilities higher than 0.8,
except for ethical intention, which has Cronbach α = 0.6, Omega = 0.6, and GLB = 0.7,
primarily because of varying responses of respondents to the ethical situation in each
scenario.
Three items of extrinsic religiosity (discussed above) were negatively correlated
with its other items and factor analysis showed them to be a separate factor. Reliability
statistics improved as well when those items were deleted from the scale (see end of
Table 4 for improvement in reliability scores).
2 These two statistics, Omega and GLB, were calculated using the R statistical programming language
package called “userfriendlyscience” which was created by Peters (2014).
27
*****************************************************************
Insert Table 4 here
******************************************************************
Correlation
Correlation analysis was used to identify the association between the constructs
concerned. As can be seen from Table 5 below, overall, these correlation results show
significant positive correlations between intrinsic religiosity, extrinsic religiosity, the
mediating variable ethical judgment, and the outcome variable ethical intention. Intrinsic
religiosity, however, correlates more significantly than extrinsic religiosity with ethical
judgment and ethical intention. This implies that intrinsic religiosity is more instrumental
than extrinsic religiosity in forming ethical judgment and intention.
******************************************************************
Insert Table 5 here.
******************************************************************
Hypothesis testing
Multiple regression was employed to test the hypotheses. Specifically, bias-corrected
bootstrap confidence interval mediation analysis (Hayes 2013) was used to test for the
mediation by running two mediation models while controlling for the demographics and
the other religiosity dimension. For instance, to test the mediation model, intrinsic
religiosityethical judgment ethical intention, we controlled for demographics
(gender, age, and qualification) and extrinsic religiosity.
28
As can be seen from Table 6, the regression model is significant since F = 12.79
with p < 0.001, and intrinsic religiosity describes 38% of the variance in the outcome
variable ethical judgment since R2 = 0.38. Specifically, intrinsic religiosity is a significant
positive predictor of ethical judgment, B = 0.55 with p < 0.001, partialing out the effect
of demographics and extrinsic religiosity. This evidence supports hypothesis H1 wherein
a positive relationship between intrinsic religiosity and ethical judgment was
hypothesized. Similarly, from Table 7, extrinsic religiosity was also found to be a
significant positive predictor of ethical judgment, p < 0.05. This result does not support
H2, in which we proposed a negative relationship between the two.
******************************************************************
Insert Table 6 and Table 7 here.
******************************************************************
To test H3, which proposed a positive relationship between ethical judgment and
ethical intention, we look for evidence in both Table 6 and Table 7, which show results
from the mediation models for intrinsic religiosity and extrinsic religiosity respectively.
Tables 6 and 7 show that ethical judgment is a significant positive predictor of ethical
intention as expected, B = 0.73 p < 0.001, so this evidence supports H3.
We tested mediation for the following two models3, partialing out the effects of
demographics (gender, age, and qualification) and the other dimensions of religiosity:
3 A single composite model could have been used to test for the mediation. We used two separate models
because we are interested in estimating the individual (total, direct, and indirect) effect of each dimension
of religiosity on ethical intention through ethical judgment. For potential dangers of including multiple
predictors in a mediation model, please see Hayes (2013).
29
1. Intrinsic religiosity ethical judgment ethical intention
2. Extrinsic religiosity ethical judgment ethical intention
For model 1 above, a simple mediation analysis using ordinary least squares path
analysis shows that intrinsic religiosity indirectly influenced intentions to act ethically
through its effect on ethical judgment. As can be seen from Table 6, the findings suggest
that intrinsic religiosity could influence their ethical judgment (a = 0.55), and managers
having high ethical judgment in an ethical decision situation are more likely to form the
intention to act ethically (b = 0.73). A bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval for the
indirect effect (ab = 0.40) using 10,000 bootstrap samples was completely above zero
(0.27 to 0.53). Intrinsic religiosity was also able to directly influence ethical intention
independent of ethical judgment (c’ = 0.16, p < 0.001). This evidence supports H4, which
proposed an indirect relationship between intrinsic religiosity and ethical intention
through ethical judgment.
For model 2, a simple mediation analysis using ordinary least squares path
analysis shows that extrinsic religiosity indirectly influenced intentions to act ethically
through its effect on the ethical judgment. As can be seen from Table 7, the findings
suggest that extrinsic religiosity can influence ethical judgment (a = 0.18), and managers
having high ethical judgment in an ethical decision situation are more likely to form the
intention to act ethically (b = 0.73). A bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval for the
indirect effect (ab = 0.13) based on 10,000 bootstrap samples was entirely above zero
(0.03 to 0.23). Extrinsic religiosity was not able to directly influence ethical intention
independent of its effect on ethical judgment (c’ = -.00, p = 0.88). These results show that
extrinsic religiosity positively influences ethical intention through ethical judgment. This
30
counter evidence does not support H5 that proposed an indirect negative relationship
between extrinsic religiosity and ethical intention through ethical judgment such that high
extrinsic religiosity results into low ethical judgment which in turn corresponds to low
ethical intention.
In sum, the results of this study show that both intrinsic religiosity and extrinsic
religiosity can positively influence ethical intention via ethical judgment, although to a
lesser degree in the case of extrinsic religiosity than intrinsic religiosity. As already
discussed, another relevant finding is that intrinsic religiosity can directly influence
ethical intention independent of its effect on ethical judgment, but extrinsic religiosity is
unable to directly influence ethical intention independent of its effect on ethical
judgment.
Discussion
In the previous section, intrinsic religiosity was found to be a significant positive
determinant of ethical judgment as theorized in H1. This finding agrees with the H-V
model (Hunt and Vitell 2006) and empirical studies (Fernando and Chowdhury 2010;
Walker et al. 2012). This finding also shows the role of religious commitment in forming
an ethical judgment in the case of managers high in intrinsic religiosity, as they place
religion highest, above all other callings. Such managers’ moral judgment can be further
developed through ethics training focused on religious principles, with the obvious caveat
that secular or atheist managers should not be intimidated.
Contrary to the hypothesized relationship in H2 (extrinsic religiosity as a negative
predictor of ethical judgment), extrinsic religiosity was found to be a positive predictor of
31
ethical judgment although of lesser magnitude than intrinsic religiosity. This finding
contradicts some previous evidence (e.g., Walker et al. 2012) which found individuals
high in extrinsic religious orientation endorse questionable behavior, but agrees with
research showing that personally oriented extrinsic religiosity (“Ep”) undermines
unethical intentions (Chen and Tang 2013). It is also important to note that some research
in a consumer ethics setting (Vitell et al. 2005) has not found any relationship between
extrinsic religiosity and considering questionable behaviors as wrong.
A potential explanation for the positive relationship between extrinsic religiosity
and ethical judgment is that our study used a highly religious and non-Western context
where the general public considers religion to be a prerequisite for morality; such a
context might have peculiarities when it comes to extrinsic religiosity. For instance, the
instrumental motivation in using religion might have been significantly reduced due to
increased salience of religion in such contexts. Another explanation could be that the
three items deleted due to the factor analysis pertain to extrinsic religiosity about “Es”
(socially oriented extrinsic religiosity) while the other “Ep” items are personally oriented
(Gorsuch and McPherson 1989). Specifically, managers high in personal extrinsic
religiosity (high on “Ep” items) practice religion to gain what they believe their religion
offers them: peace, happiness, relief, protection, and comfort in the times of sorrow or
trouble. The “Ep” extrinsic religiosity, though using religion to seek positive outcomes
and to avoid adverse personal outcomes, may not be detrimental for EDM. This partially
explains the positive relationship between extrinsic religiosity and ethical judgment we
found in this study. However, “Es” items are more instrumental and utilitarian than the
“Ep” items due to their focus on using religion for building social relationships, gaining
32
social approval, and thus furthering one’s business. We, therefore, suggest that future
research to be undertaken to have more granular hypotheses and analysis by further
dividing extrinsic religiosity into extrinsic social (Es) and extrinsic personal (Ep)
religiosity to understand the underlying dynamics of extrinsic religiosity and EDM.
Our empirical study did not support H5’s claim that extrinsic religiosity is a
significant negative predictor of ethical intention through ethical judgment. On the
contrary, this study’s evidence showed that extrinsic religiosity, like intrinsic religiosity,
significantly and positively influences ethical intention through ethical judgment. This
positive effect of extrinsic religiosity necessitates future research to ascertain whether this
finding is specific to Pakistan or also applies to other highly religious, non-Western
contexts. As we suggest above, future research should decompose extrinsic religiosity
into extrinsic social and extrinsic personal to shed light on this finding. At least for
this study context, we can safely say that extrinsic religiosityextrinsic personal (Ep) to
be more precisehelps to foster EDM through an indirect positive mechanism operating
from extrinsic religiosity to ethical intention through ethical judgment.
In line with current theory and literature, we found ethical judgment to be a
significant positive predictor of ethical intention as hypothesized in H3. Ethical intention
was used as a proxy of ethical behavior because it is challenging to observe (un)ethical
behavior due to lack of access to the actual decision-making settings, the arbitrary nature
of decisions, and time constraints. It is a common observation that people act according
to their intentions, which are formed based on their attitude towards behavior that, in
turn, is contingent on their cognitive beliefs. We assume this observation is true and this
study showed that respondents formed intentions in line with their judgments: the
33
management professionals high in ethical judgments were more likely to form ethical
intentions commensurate with their judgments. Thus, organizations wanting to inculcate
EDM among their employees should foster ethical judgment through ethics training.
We argue that religious beliefs are an essential constituent of cognitive beliefs and
people high in religiosity see things through a religious lens, which in turn can influence
ethical judgment, consequently influencing intentions to act ethically in organizational
settings. This is reflected in this study because religiosity was found to be a significant
determinant of ethical intention through ethical judgment. Intrinsic religiosity influences
ethical decisions of the managers more than extrinsic religiosity because the former
involves religious values being deeply inculcated, and the religion is practiced in its true
spirit due to a strong commitment to that religion. Based on our study results, it is natural
for a manager high in intrinsic religiosity to have ethical intention via two routes (indirect
and direct): First, religion guides one to form ethical judgment which in turn influences
ethical intention; and second, religion directly influences a person to form intention to act
ethically in a situation having ethical content. Based on the findings of this research, we
conclude that managers high in intrinsic religiosity are more likely to form ethical
judgment and, in turn, ethical intention as hypothesized in H4. They are also able to form
intentions directly to act ethically independent of ethical judgment, thus showing the
great power of intrinsic religiosity.
This study using respondents from corporate Pakistan substantiates the Pakistani
public contention that one cannot be moral without the religion. Therefore, we can
conclude that a manager high in intrinsic religiosity is less likely to form behavioral
intentions to perform unethically. Extrinsic religiosity was also found to be a positive
34
determinant of ethical intention, though only through ethical judgment; not directly. This
finding implies that people high in extrinsic religiosity are also likely to make ethical
decisions in a non-Western and highly religious context, such as that of this study. This
finding contradicts previous empirical evidence that people high in extrinsic religiosity
are likely to form unethical intentions due to their focus on religiosity as an instrument. A
possible explanation of this disagreement is that the utilitarian perspective of extrinsic
religiosity reflected in socially oriented extrinsic religiosity (i.e., extrinsic social) does
not mesh with personally oriented extrinsic religiosity, at least in Pakistan.
Additionally, our results show that although extrinsic religiosity cannot directly
influence ethical intention independent of ethical judgment, we can conclude that
extrinsic religiosity does play a role in forming judgments and subsequent ethical
intentions. We suggest that this is a result of personally oriented extrinsic religiosity. In
Western societies, people high in extrinsic religiosity might go to religious services to rub
shoulders and to network with people. Typically, there is just one major weekly service
(with smaller, less-attended midweek options), whereas in a Muslim society like
Pakistan, people go for their prayers five times a day; any utilitarian motive might be
significantly reduced due to this higher frequency. These findings may also imply that
intrinsic religious motivation is higher and extrinsic religious motivation lower in
Pakistan than in Western less-religious countries. For this implication to be corroborated,
however, there is a need for cross-cultural research covering other highly religious, non-
Western contexts and less-religious Western contexts.
Research implications
35
The only previous study investigating the direct relationship between religiosity and
EDM in Pakistan is a recent study of banking personnel (frontline and managers) wherein
religiosity was found to moderate the relationship between (1) injunctive norms and (2)
perceived behavioral control and behavioral intentions to act ethically as an outcome
variable (Kashif et al. 2017). We believe that our study better captures ethical judgment
and intention constructs by using four different scenarios, two dimensions of religiosity,
and diverse respondents, a set of managers who are members of the Management
Association of Pakistan. We hope that this current study will pave the way for future
studies focusing on non-Western, highly religious countries and highlighting the role of
extrinsic religiosity. This research would contribute to cross-cultural religion-ethics
research comparing Western less religious and non-Western, highly religious contexts.
We recommend that research building on this paper should solicit responses from other
professional bodies, including the Marketing Association of Pakistan, Pakistan Society
for Human Resource Management, Institute of Chartered Accounts of Pakistan, Institute
of Cost and Management Accountants of Pakistan (ICMA Pakistan), and Chartered
Financial Analysts (CFAs). Using these professional bodies as the sampling frame will
enable understanding of EDM in different professions (Rest 1994) and elucidate the
religion-ethics relationship.
As already described, this study found extrinsic religiosity to be significantly and
positively related to ethical intention via ethical judgment. In contrast, previous research
had found no relationship with ethical judgment in consumer ethics settings (Vitell et al.
2005), a negative relationship with ethical intention (Singhapakdi et al. 2013), and a
positive relationship between personally oriented extrinsic religiosity (“Ep”) and ethical
36
intention (Chen and Tang 2013). This research contributes to the literature by identifying
the indirect relationship between extrinsic religiosity and ethical intention through ethical
judgment, thus partially explaining these mixed results. The intuitive reasons for this are
described in the Discussion section; however, informal discussion with a handful of
respondents after the study was concluded and the results tabulated, showed that
utilitarian motives towards religion are not very profound and therefore are not
detrimental to EDM, at least in Pakistan. This finding calls for qualitative research to
ascertain why this is so. We also believe that there might be a need to revise the extrinsic
religiosity scale, at least for highly religious countries (Islamic countries in particular),
provided the suggested qualitative research points in this direction.
Future research is also needed to understand why intrinsic religiosity can influ-
ence ethical intention directly. Our results show that extrinsic religiosity influences ethi-
cal intention only indirectly through ethical judgment, whereas intrinsic religiosity has
both direct and indirect effects.
Managerial recommendations
Because ethical judgment was found to be the most significant predictor of ethical
intention, we join prior research in recommending that organizations should inculcate
such judgments in their employees through ethics training programs to foster EDM. Our
findings suggest that managers high in intrinsic religiosity are more likely to undertake
EDM as intrinsic religiosity can influence ethical intention through ethical judgment due
to the heightened deontological norms and can directly influence ethical intention as well.
We do not suggest that organizations should endeavor to hire only religious people, but,
if an organization has employees who are high in both dimensions of religiosity, this can
37
relieve the organization and other stakeholders from the detrimental effects of unethical
decisions. We also suggest that religion-oriented ethics training can also be conducted to
further strengthen the ethical judgment of managers, at least in contexts such as Pakistan,
where religiosity has been shown to play a substantial role in ethical decision-making.
Limitations
The most significant limitation of this study was the unavailability of a sampling frame of
all management professionals in Pakistan. To mitigate this limitation, we used members
of the Management Association of Pakistan as respondents. This association, though a
representative body of management professionals of the country, does not have all
management professionals in Pakistan as its members. Another limitation of the research
was the low response rate despite the concerted efforts of the lead researcher to remind
the respondents to send back the completed questionnaires. A better response rate would
have resulted in better generalizability of the results. One other limitation is that variables
other than the controls might influence the outcome variable of ethical intention.
However, we were constrained by the parsimony principle of the research. Future
research should also try to understand the actual (un)ethical behavior rather than just the
(un)ethical intention.
Conclusion
Ethics is an essential area of concern for both researchers and practitioners. The role that
religion plays in EDM in non-Western and highly religious countries is an understudied
38
domain. Two dimensions of religiosity, called intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity, were
found to play decisive roles to inculcate ethical judgment that influences ethical intention.
This research contributes to the understanding of the relationship between religiosity and
ethics by showing how both dimensions of religiosity influence ethical intention through
the mediating mechanism of ethical judgment. The research has important implications
for managers. For example, organizations should provide ethics training programs, with
or without religious teaching, so that the ethical judgment of managers can be raised and
subsequently EDM can be promoted. Continuing research on the interaction of religion
and ethics is needed, especially in highly religious and non-Western countries other than
Pakistan, and we also call for cross-cultural research in such contexts. We suggest repli-
cating this study in professions other than management so that a broader understanding of
the role of religion in ethics can be determined.
39
Appendix-1
SCENARIOS
Scenario 1
The pricing committee of a large video game marketer suggests that prices be raised 20
30 % during the holiday season and that a highly popular game be kept in short supply.
Action: The vice president of marketing decides that this is a good idea since consumer
demand indicates that consumers will be likely to pay the higher prices.
Scenario 2
XYZ Company has been barred from entering the market in a large Asian country by
collusive efforts of the local bicycle manufacturers. XYZ could expect to earn 500
million rupees per year from sales if it could penetrate the market. Last week a
businessman from the country contacted the president of XYZ and stated that he could
smooth the way for the company to sell in his country for a price of Rs. 50,000,000 (50
million).
Action: The president of XYZ pays the Rs. 50,000,000 (50 million).
Scenario 3
Mr. A has recently accepted a job with a new, vigorous microcomputer manufacturer.
Microcomputer manufacturers are engaged in intense competition to become the market
leader with a software package which utilizes the English language and thus is easily
used by the average consumer. Mr. A's former employer is generally known to be the
leader in this software development. When Mr. A was hired, he was led to believe that
his selection was based upon his management potential. The morning beginning the third
week on the new job, Mr. A received the following memo from the president:
Please meet with me tomorrow at 8:15 AM for the purpose of discussing the
developments your former employer has made in microcomputer software.
Action: Mr. A reveals the new product developments made by his former employer.
Scenario 4
Ted Jones, senior editor of J & P Publishing Company, has just received a manuscript
from one of his most successful authors. It provides the most authoritative account yet
published of the history of the development of the atomic bomb. However, the final
chapter contains a detailed description of how the bomb is made. Jones has tried to
convince the author to omit the last chapter stating that such information should not be
made readily available to the mass market in paperback form. The author believes the
chapter is critical to the success of the book and thus will not agree to its deletion.
Action: Ted Jones publishes the book.
40
Note: All the responses to the following items were sought on a Likert scale ranging
from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree).
Ethical judgment and ethical intention items
For each of the four scenarios above, managers respondents were asked right after each
scenario:
I consider the action taken to be ethical. (ethical judgment item)
I would be likely to take the same action in this situation. (ethical intention item)
Intrinsic religiosity
I enjoy reading about my religion.
It doesn’t matter what I believe in as long as I am fair in my dealings. (Reverse coded)
It is important for me to spend time in private worship, for example Salaat or prayer.
I always have a strong sense of God’s presence.
I try to live all aspects of my life according to my religious beliefs.
My whole approach to life is based on my religion.
I believe in my religion but many other things are more important in life. (Reverse coded)
Extrinsic religiosity
I go to religious service (for example, Jumma / Sunday service) mainly because it helps
me to make friends.*
Prayer is for peace.
Prayer is for happiness.
I go to religious services (for example, Jumma / Sunday service) mostly to spend time
with my friends.*
I go to religious services (for example, Jumma / Sunday service) mainly because I enjoy
seeing people whom I know.*
What religion offers me the most is comfort in times of trouble/sorrow.
I pray to gain relief and protection.
* deleted items
41
Appendix-2
Section A: Data processing and assumption testing of parametric data
The responses from the usable questionnaires were initially input into an Excel sheet. In
SPSS 21, appropriate variable names along with their titles were defined, specifying their
data types and sets of permissible values. The data was then entered into the data view of
SPSS. Since the scenarios each contain an ethically questionable action by a manager, the
items essentially measure (un)ethical judgment and (un)ethical intention. Each items
score, as provided by the respondent, was reverse coded by subtracting each item score
from the highest permissible value + 1. For instance, if an item response is 2, recoding it
produced 6 on a scale from 1 to 7 achieved through 7+1-2 = 6. Missing value analysis
revealed the following pattern.
*********************************************************
Insert Table 8 here
*********************************************************
This table indicates that missing values constitute only 0.09% of all the values;
thus dictating no need to compensate for them. The missing values analysis proceeded by
calculating the average score for each construct. Correlation, multiple regression, and
mediation analysis were then run on these average scores only.
In order to perform parametric tests on the data collected, a multi-pronged
approach was applied whereby visual tools (P-P plots) along with normality tests (using
1-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests [K-S]) were undertaken to check for the normality
of the data. This procedure revealed that the outcome, ethical intention, D(109) =1.20, p
42
> 0.05, is significantly normal. Ethical judgment, D(109) = 1.25, p > 0.05, and intrinsic
religiosity, D(109) = 1.09, p > 0.05, all appeared significantly normal. Extrinsic
religiosity, however, D(109) = 2.04, p < 0.001, showed nonnormality, probably owing to
the sensitivity of 1-sample K-S, but the normal P-P plot of extrinsic religiosity clearly
showed normality. For brevity, we have not shown the normal P-P plots for the variables
of the study. The individual values, however, converge to the normal line that pictorially
depicts that the data is normal in all cases. This observation is also in agreement with the
1-sample K-S statistic described above except for extrinsic religiosity.
Assumption testing for generalizing multiple regression model
In order to generalize the findings of the sample to the population, the regression models
should meet certain assumptions. The subsequent discussion will highlight how the data
of this study show these assumptions are met. After performing case-wise diagnostics, the
assumptions of nonzero variance, normally distributed data, no perfect multicollinearity,
homoscedasticity, independent errors, normally distributed errors, independence, and
linearity were checked.
Outlier treatment. The case-wise diagnostics were performed by computing the
standardized residual value, Cook’s distance, average leverage value, and Mahalanobis
distance for identifying outliers. Based on these statistics, no case unduly influences the
regression model, and thus, no case was identified as an outlier.
Nonzero variance. All the predictors have nonzero variance, as can be seen
through their mean and standard deviation values. All the predictors and the outcome
variable follow a normal distribution, as discussed at the start of this appendix.
43
Multicollinearity. In the case of multiple regression, all the tolerance statistics
0.63, 0.67, and 0.93 in case of ethical judgment, intrinsic religiosity, and extrinsic
religiosity respectivelywere well above 0.2, indicating that there was no severe
problem of multicollinearity as Menard (1995) suggests that only values below 0.2 are of
concern.
Homoscedasticity. This assumption requires that at each level of the predictor
variable, the variance of the residual terms should be constant. The scatterplot for each
model shows that all the values are evenly distributed around 0, so the homoscedasticity
assumption holds.
Independent errors. This assumption requires that residuals be uncorrelated.
This assumption can be tested using the Durbin-Watson (D-W) test statistic that should
produce a result between 1 and 3. In the case of multiple regression model, the D-W
value was 1.5, showing that the independence of errors can be assumed.
Normally distributed errors. This assumption requires that the residuals in the
model be random and normally distributed with a mean of 0. To check this assumption,
standardized residuals were generated when running the regression model, and the
residuals were found to form a normal distribution, as shown in the P-P plots. Also, the 1-
sample K-S test statistic was nonsignificant, p > 0.05, thus showing residuals to be
normally distributed in all models.
Independence. This assumption requires that the values of the outcome variable
are independent. Each value comes from a separate entity, the respondent, so
independence can be assumed owing to the correlation design of the study.
44
Linearity. The scatter plots depict linear relationships between all the predictors
and ethical intention, the outcome variable.
Section B: Factor analysis assumption testing
******************************************************************
Insert Table 9 here
******************************************************************
45
References
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In Action
control (pp. 1139). Springer.
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 50(2), 179211.
Ajzen, I. (2011). Theory of planned behavior. Handb Theor Soc Psychol Vol One, 1, 438.
Allport, G. W. (1950). Individual and His Religion: ological interpretation. Oxford,
England: Macmillan.
Allport, G. W., & Ross, J. M. (1967). Personal Religious Orientation and Prejudice.
Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 5(4), 432443. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.5.4.432
Anheier, H. K., & Isar, Y. R. (2007). Conflicts and tensions (Vol. 1). Sage Publications
Ltd.
Barak-Corren, N., & Bazerman, M. (2017). Is saving lives your task or God’s?
Religiosity, belief in god, and moral judgment. Judgment and Decision Making,
12(3), 280296.
Barnett, T., Bass, K., & Brown, G. (1996). Religiosity, ethical ideology, and intentions to
report a peer’s wrongdoing. Journal of Business Ethics, 15(11), 11611174.
doi:10.1007/BF00412815
Bartels, R. (1967). A model for ethics in marketing. The Journal of marketing, 2026.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/002224296703100105
Baruch, Y., & Holtom, B. C. (2008). Survey response rate levels and trends in
organizational research. Human Relations, 61(8), 11391160.
doi:10.1177/0018726708094863
Bloodgood, J. M., Turnley, W. H., & Mudrack, P. (2008). The influence of ethics
instruction, religiosity, and intelligence on cheating behavior. Journal of Business
Ethics, 82(3), 557571. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-007-9576-0
Bommer, M., Gratto, C., Gravander, J., & Tuttle, M. (2013). A behavioral model of
ethical and unethical decision making. In Citation classics from the journal of
business ethics (pp. 97117). Springer. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00382936
Chan, K. C., Fung, A., Fung, H.-G., & Yau, J. (2016). A Citation Analysis of Business
Ethics Research: A Global Perspective. Journal of Business Ethics, 136(3), 557
573. doi:10.1007/s10551-014-2533-9
Chen, Y.-J., & Tang, T. L.-P. (2013). The bright and dark sides of religiosity among
university students: do gender, college major, and income matter? Journal of
Business Ethics, 115(3), 531553. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-012-1407-2
46
CIA. (2007). The World Factbook.
http://www.umsl.edu/services/govdocs/wofact2007/geos/xx.html#People
CIA. (2012). The World Factbook. CIA. https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-
world-factbook/geos/xx.html
CIA. (2014). The World Factbook. CIA. https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-
world-factbook/
Cooper, M. J., & Pullig, C. (2013). I’m number one! Does narcissism impair ethical
judgment even for the highly religious? Journal of Business Ethics, 112(1), 167
176. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-012-1239-0
Craft, J. L. (2013). A Review of the Empirical Ethical Decision-Making Literature:
20042011. Journal of Business Ethics, 117(2), 221259. doi:10.1007/s10551-012-
1518-9
Dean, K. L., & Fornaciari, C. J. (2007). Empirical Research In Management, Spirituality
& Religion During Its Founding Years. Journal of Management, Spirituality &
Religion, 4(1), 334. doi:https://10.1080/14766080709518644
Dubinsky, A. J., & Loken, B. (1989). Analyzing ethical decision making in marketing.
Journal of Business Research, 19(2), 83107. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0148-
2963(89)90001-5
Fernando, M., & Chowdhury, R. M. M. I. (2010). The relationship between spiritual
well-being and ethical orientations in decision making: An empirical study with
business executives in Australia. Journal of Business Ethics, 95(2), 211225.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-009-0355-y
Fernando, M., & Jackson, B. (2006). The influence of religion-based workplace
spirituality on business leaders’ decision-making: An inter-faith study. Journal of
Management & Organization, 12(1), 2339. doi:10.1017/S1833367200004144
Ferrell, O. C., & Gresham, L. G. (1985). A contingency framework for understanding
ethical decision making in marketing. The Journal of marketing, 8796.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/002224298504900308
Ford, R. C., & Richardson, W. D. (1994). Ethical decision making: A review of the
empirical literature. Journal of Business Ethics, 13(3), 205221.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02074820
Fritzsche, D. J. (1991). A model of decision-making incorporating ethical values. Journal
of Business Ethics, 10(11), 841852. doi:10.1007/BF00383700
Fritzsche, D. J., & Becker, H. (1983). Ethical behavior of marketing managers. Journal of
Business Ethics, 2(4), 291299. doi:10.1007/BF00383187
Gorsuch, R. L., & McPherson, S. E. (1989). Intrinsic/extrinsic measurement: I/E-revised
and single-item scales. Journal for the Scientific study of Religion, 348354.
47
doi:https://doi.org/10.2307/1386745
Hackett, C., & Grim, B. J. (2012). The Global Religious Landscape: A Report on the Size
and Distribution of the World’s Major Religious Groups as of 2010. Pew Research
Center’s Forum on Religion & Public Life. http://www.pewforum.org/global-
religious-landscape.aspx
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process
Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. (T. D. Little, Ed.)Methodology in the
Social Sciences. The Guilford Press.
Hegarty, W. H., & Sims, H. P. (1978). Some determinants of unethical decision behavior:
An experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63(4), 451457. doi:10.1037/0021-
9010.63.4.451
Hegarty, W. H., & Sims, H. P. (1979). Organizational philosophy, policies, and
objectives related to unethical decision behavior: A laboratory experiment. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 64(3), 331338. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.64.3.331
Ho, J. A. (2010). Ethical perception: are differences between ethnic groups situation
dependent? Business Ethics: A European Review, 19(2), 154182.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8608.2010.01583.x
Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. (1986). A General Theory of Marketing Ethics. Journal of
Macromarketing, 6(1), 516. doi:10.1177/027614678600600103
Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. J. (1993). The General Theory of Marketing Ethics: A
Retrospective and Revision. In Ethics in Marketing (pp. 775784). Irwin,
Homewood, IL.
Hunt, S. D., & Vitell, S. J. (2006). The General Theory of Marketing Ethics: A Revision
and Three Questions. Journal of Macromarketing, 26(2), 143153.
doi:10.1177/0276146706290923
Ibrahim, N. A., Howard, D. P., & Angelidis, J. P. (2008). The relationship between
religiousness and corporate social responsibility orientation: are there differences
between business managers and students? Journal of Business Ethics, 78(12), 165
174. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-006-9321-0
Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical Decision Making by Individuals in Organizations: An Issue-
Contingent Model. Academy of Management Review, 16(2), 366395.
doi:10.5465/amr.1991.4278958
Kaiser, H. F. (1974). An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika, 39(1), 3136.
Kashif, M., Zarkada, A., & Thurasamy, R. (2017). The moderating effect of religiosity on
ethical behavioural intentions: An application of the extended theory of planned
behaviour to Pakistani bank employees. Personnel Review, 46(2), 429448.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1108/PR-10-2015-0256
48
Kidwell, J. M., Stevens, R. E., & Bethke, A. L. (1987). Differences in ethical perceptions
between male and female managers: Myth or reality? Journal of Business Ethics,
6(6), 489493. doi:10.1007/BF00383291
Kish-Gephart, J. J., Harrison, D. A., & Treviño, L. K. (2010). Bad apples, bad cases, and
bad barrels: meta-analytic evidence about sources of unethical decisions at work.
Journal of applied psychology, 95(1), 1.
Kurpis, L. V, Beqiri, M. S., & Helgeson, J. G. (2008). The effects of commitment to
moral self-improvement and religiosity on ethics of business students. Journal of
Business Ethics, 80(3), 447463. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-007-9430-4
Lehnert, K., Park, Y., & Singh, N. (2015). Research Note and Review of the Empirical
Ethical Decision-Making Literature: Boundary Conditions and Extensions. Journal
of Business Ethics, 129(1), 195219. doi:10.1007/s10551-014-2147-2
Marta, J. K. M., Singhapakdi, A., Attia, A., & Vitell, S. J. (2004). Some important factors
underlying ethical decisions of Middle-Eastern marketers. International Marketing
Review, 21(1), 5367. doi:https://doi.org/10.1108/02651330410522916
McCullough, P. M., & Faught, S. (2005). Rational moralists and moral rationalists value-
based management: Model, criterion and validation. Journal of Business Ethics,
60(2), 195205. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-004-8317-x
McDaniel, S. W., & Burnett, J. J. (1990). Consumer religiosity and retail store evaluative
criteria. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 18(2), 101112.
doi:10.1007/BF02726426
McNichols, C. W., & Zimmerer, T. W. (1985). Situational ethics: An empirical study of
differentiators of student attitudes. Journal of Business Ethics, 4(3), 175180.
doi:10.1007/BF00705616
Menard, S. (1995). Applied logistic regression analysis: Sage university series on
quantitative applications in the social sciences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Mitchell, T. R. (1985). An evaluation of the validity of correlational research conducted
in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 192205.
doi:https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1985.4277939
Moghadam, A. (2004). A Global Resurgence of Religion. Weatherhead Center for
International Affairs, Harvard University.
O’Fallon, M. J., & Butterfield, K. D. (2005). A review of the empirical ethical decision-
making literature: 19962003. Journal of Business Ethics, 59(4), 375413.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-005-2929-7
Oumlil, A. Ben, & Balloun, J. L. (2009). Ethical Decision-Making Differences Between
American and Moroccan Managers. Journal of Business Ethics, 84(4), 457478.
doi:10.1007/s10551-008-9719-y
49
Parboteeah, K. P., Hoegl, M., & Cullen, J. B. (2008). Ethics and Religion: An Empirical
Test of a Multidimensional Model. Journal of Business Ethics, 80(2), 387398.
doi:10.1007/s10551-007-9439-8
Peters, G.-J. Y. (2014). The alpha and the omega of scale reliability and validity: why and
how to abandon Cronbach’s alpha and the route towards more comprehensive
assessment of scale quality. European Health Psychologist, 16(2), 5669.
https://www.ehps.net/ehp/index.php/contents/article/view/ehp.v16.i2.p56
Pew Global Attitudes Project. (2007). World Publics Welcome Global Trade But Not
Immigration: 47-nation Pew Global Attitudes Survey. Pew Research Center.
https://www.pewresearch.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/2/2007/10/Pew-Global-
Attitudes-Report-October-4-2007-REVISED-UPDATED-5-27-14.pdf
Phau, I., & Kea, G. (2007). Attitudes of university students toward business ethics: a
cross-national investigation of Australia, Singapore and Hong Kong. Journal of
Business Ethics, 72(1), 6175. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-006-9156-8
Rallapalli, K. C. (1995). Marketers’ norms: An empirical investigation of the influence of
personal and situational factors on marketers’ deontological norms, ethical
judgments and intentions. doi:https://www.jstor.org/stable/25074267
Rawwas, M. Y. A., Swaidan, Z., & Al-Khatib, J. (2006). Does religion matter? A
comparison study of the ethical beliefs of marketing students of religious and secular
universities in Japan. Journal of Business Ethics, 65(1), 6986.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-006-0023-4
Razzaque, M. A., & Hwee, T. P. (2002). Ethics and purchasing dilemma: A Singaporean
view. Journal of Business Ethics, 35(4), 307326.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1013853021571
Rest, J. (1994). Moral development in the professions: Psychology and applied ethics.
Psychology Press.
Singhapakdi, A., Marta, J. K., Rallapalli, K. C., & Rao, C. P. (2000). Toward an
understanding of religiousness and marketing ethics: An empirical study. Journal of
Business Ethics, 27(4), 305319. doi:https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006342224035
Singhapakdi, A., Salyachivin, S., Virakul, B., & Veerayangkur, V. (2000). Some
important factors underlying ethical decision making of managers in Thailand.
Journal of Business Ethics, 27(3), 271284.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006111004721
Singhapakdi, A., Vitell, S. J., Lee, D.-J., Nisius, A. M., & Yu, G. B. (2013). The
Influence of Love of Money and Religiosity on Ethical Decision-Making in
Marketing. Journal of Business Ethics, 114(1), 183191. doi:10.1007/s10551-012-
1334-2
The Pew Global Attitudes Project. (2008). Unfavorable Views of Jews and Muslims on
50
the Increase in Europe (Vol. 17).
Trevino, L. K. (1986). Ethical decision making in organizations: A person-situation
interactionist model. Academy of Management Review, 11(3), 601617.
doi:https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1986.4306235
Tse, A. C. B., & Au, A. K. M. (1997). Are New Zealand business students more unethical
than non-business students? Journal of Business Ethics, 16(4), 445450.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1017957210848
Vitell, S. J., Bing, M. N., Davison, H. K., Ammeter, A. P., Garner, B. L., & Novicevic,
M. M. (2009). Religiosity and moral identity: The mediating role of self-control.
Journal of Business Ethics, 88(4), 601613. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-008-
9980-0
Vitell, S. J., Keith, M., & Mathur, M. (2011). Antecedents to the justification of norm
violating behavior among business practitioners. Journal of Business Ethics, 101(1),
163173. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-010-0717-5
Vitell, S. J., & Muncy, J. (2005). The Muncy-Vitell consumer ethics scale: A
modification and application. Journal of Business Ethics, 62(3), 267275.
doi:10.1007/s10551-005-7058-9
Vitell, S. J., Paolillo, J. G. P., & Singh, J. J. (2005). Religiosity and consumer ethics.
Journal of Business Ethics, 57(2), 175181. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-004-
4603-x
Wagner, S. C., & Sanders, G. L. (2001). Considerations in ethical decision-making and
software piracy. Journal of Business Ethics, 29(1), 161167.
doi:10.1023/A:1006415514200
Walker, A. G., Smither, J. W., & DeBode, J. (2012). The effects of religiosity on ethical
judgments. Journal of Business Ethics, 106(4), 437452.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-011-1009-4
Weaver, G. R., & Agle, B. R. (2002). Religiosity and Ethical Behavior in Organizations:
A Symbolic Interactionist Perspective. Academy of Management Review, 27(1), 77
97. doi:10.5465/amr.2002.5922390
Wilkes, R. E., Burnett, J. J., & Howell, R. D. (1986). On the meaning and measurement
of religiosity in consumer research. Journal of the academy of Marketing Science,
14(1), 4756. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02722112
Wimalasiri, J. S., Pavri, F., & Jalil, A. A. K. (1996). An empirical study of moral
reasoning among managers in Singapore. Journal of Business Ethics, 15(12), 1331
1341. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00411818
51
Ethical intention
(EI)
Ethical judgment
(EJ)
Intrinsic religiosity
(INT)
Indirect effect (INT-EJ-EI): H4 (+)
H3 (+)
H1 (+)
Ethical intention
(EI)
Ethical judgment
(EJ)
Extrinsic religiosity
(EXT)
Indirect effect (EXT-EJ-EI): H5 (-)
H3 (+)
H2 (-)
Fig. 1
The conceptual model
52
Table 1 Selected studies using multidimensional religiosity scales
Context
Religiosity measure
Findings
EDM of
marketing
professionals
(American
Marketing
Association)
Religiosity
dimensions: church
attendance; perceived
importance of religious
values; and confidence
in religious values
(Wilkes et al. 1986)
Religiosity (the composite of 3 dimensions)
was a significant predictor in one scenario
while it was a marginally significant (p <
0.10) predictor of ethical intention in three
scenarios.
EDM of Thai
managers
enrolled in
executive and
special MBA
programs
Same as above
Religiosity was a significant predictor of
ethical intention in two of four scenarios.
Consumer
ethics setting
(undergraduate
student
consumers)
Religious orientation
scale (Allport and Ross
1967) wherein
religiosity has two
distinct dimensions:
intrinsic dimension and
extrinsic dimension
Intrinsic religiosity explained consumer
ethical beliefs to view the questionable
consumer behaviors as wrong for three out of
the four dimensions.
Extrinsic religiosity did not significantly
explain any of the four dimensions of the
consumer ethics scale.
Undergraduate
business
students
Same as above
Intrinsic religiosity played a role in
counterbalancing the negative impact of
extrinsic religiosity on both dimensions of
moral identity (internalization and
symbolization).
EDM in
marketing
(marketing
managers)
Same as above
Intrinsic religiosity was found to be a positive
predictor of ethical intention.
Extrinsic religiosity was found to be a
negative predictor of ethical intention.
53
Table 2 Demographic profile of the sample
Gender
Male 81.6%
Female 18.4%
Academic Qualification
Doctorate / Postdoctorate 0%
Master’s / MS / M. Phil (Minimum 17 years schooling) 55.2%
Bachelor (Hons) / Master’s (16 years schooling) 37.9%
Bachelor’s (14 years schooling) 6.9%
Age
Mean 30.54 years
Median 29 years
25th percentile 26 years
50th percentile 29 years
75th percentile 34 years
54
Table 3 Extrinsic religiosity scale adaptation
Original item
Adapted item
I go to religious service because it helps me to make friends.
I go to religious service (for example, Jumma /
Sunday service) mainly because it helps me to
make friends.
Prayer is for peace and happiness.
Prayer is for peace.
Prayer is for happiness.
I go to religious services mostly to spend time with my
friends.
I go to religious services (for example, Jumma /
Sunday service) mostly to spend time with my
friends.
I go to religious services mainly because I enjoy
seeing people I know there.
I go to religious services (for example, Jumma /
Sunday service) mainly because I enjoy seeing
people whom I know.
Table 4 Reliability analysis
Construct
No. of
Items
Cronbach α
Omega
Greatest
Lower
Bound
(GLB)
Ethical intention (EI)
4
0.63 0.6, CI: [0.48, 0.73]
0.63 ≈ 0.6, CI: [0.49, 0.73]
0.65 ≈ 0.7
Ethical judgment (EJ)
4
0.84 ≈ 0.8, CI: [0.79, 0.89]
0.85 ≈ 0.9, CI: [0.79, 0.89]
0.86 ≈ 0.9
Intrinsic religiosity
(INT)
7
0.91 ≈ 0.9, CI: [0.87, 0.93]
0.91 ≈ 0.9, CI: [0.87, 0.93]
0.93 ≈ 0.9
Extrinsic religiosity
(EXT) [original scale]
7
0.73 ≈ 0.7, CI: [0.64, 0.8]
0.80 ≈ 0.8, CI: [0.72, 0.85]
0.91 0.9
Extrinsic religiosity
(EXT) [final scale]
4 (three
items
deleted due
to negative
loadings)
0.92 ≈ 0.9, CI: [0.88, 0.95]
0.92 ≈ 0.9, CI: [0.86, 0.95]
0.92 0.9
55
Table 5 Pearson correlation
95% CI
Measure
1
2
3
4
M
SD
LL
UL
1
Ethical
intention
4.56
1.09
4.34
4.79
2
Ethical
judgment
0.91***
4.18
1.19
3.95
4.42
3
Intrinsic
religiosity
0.64***
0.57***
4.26
1.22
4.03
4.50
4
Extrinsic
religiosity
0.22*
0.25**
0.08
4.61
1.42
4.31
4.87
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001; CI = Confidence interval, LL = Lower limit, UL = Upper Limit
56
Table 6 Coefficients for the mediation model [the effect of intrinsic religiosity (INT) on ethical
intention (EI) through ethical judgment (EJ)]
Consequent
M (EJ)
Y (EI)
Antecedent
Coeff.
SE
p
Coeff.
SE
p
Gender
-0.38
0.24
0.11
-0.00
0.11
0.97
Age
-0.01
0.01
0.70
-0.00
0.01
0.91
Qualification
0.03
0.15
0.83
-0.00
0.07
0.96
EXT
0.18
0.07
0.01
0.00
0.03
0.88
X (INT)
a
0.55
(0.03, 2.81)
0.08
< 0.001
c’
0.16 (0.08, 0.25)
0.04
< 0.001
M (EJ)
b
0.73 (0.65, 0.82)
0.04
< 0.001
Constant
i1
1.42
0.70
0.04
i2
0.80
0.32
< 0.001
R2 = 0.38
R2 = 0.85
F (1, 109) = 12.79 , p < 0.001
F (2, 109) = 96.93, p < 0.001
Controls: gender, age, qualification, and extrinsic religiosity (EXT).
Number of bootstrap samples for bias corrected bootstrap confidence intervals (CI) = 10,000.
57
Table 7 Coefficients for the mediation model [the effect of Extrinsic religiosity (EXT) on ethical
intention (EI) through ethical judgment (EJ)]
Consequent
M (EJ)
Y (EI)
Antecedent
Coeff.
SE
p
Coeff.
SE
p
Gender
-0.38
0.24
0.12
-0.00
0.11
0.97
Age
-0.01
0.01
0.70
-0.00
0.01
0.91
Qualification
0.03
0.15
0.83
-0.00
0.67
0.96
INT
0.55 (0.39, 0.70)
0.08
< 0.001
0.16 (0.08, 0.25)
0.04
< 0.001
X (EXT)
a
0.18 (0.05, 0.31)
0.07
0.01
c’
0.00 (-0.06, 0.07)
0.03
0.88
M (EJ)
b
0.73 (0.65, 0.82)
0.04
< 0.001
Constant
i1
1.42 (0.03, 2.81)
0.70
0.04
i2
0.80 (0.17, 1.44)
0.32
0.01
R2 = 0.38
R2 = 0.85
F (1, 109) = 12.79 , p < 0.001
F (2, 109) = 96.93, p < 0.001
Controls: gender, age, qualification, and intrinsic religiosity (INT).
Number of bootstrap samples for bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals (CI) = 10,000.
58
Table 8 Missing Values Analysis
Item title
No. of missing values (Percentage)
Extrinsic religiosity (item 5)
2 (0.09%)
59
Table 9 Factor analysis assumption testing
Assumption
Guideline
Construct
Value
Remarks
Nonexistence of
multicollinearity
Determinant of the correlation matrix
to be greater than threshold value of
0.00001
Ethical intention
Ethical judgment
Intrinsic religiosity
Extrinsic religiosity
0.65
0.19
0.02
0.04
Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin
Measure of
Sampling Adequacy
(KMO) statistic
KMO statistic to be greater than
threshold value of 0.5. Considered:
- Good, if KMO between 0.7 & 0.8
- Great, if KMO between 0.8 & 0.9
(Kaiser 1974)
Ethical intention
Ethical judgment
Intrinsic religiosity
Extrinsic religiosity
0.71
0.80
0.91
0.84
Bartletts Test of
Sphericity
Bartletts Test of Sphericity to be
significant
Ethical intention,
Ethical judgment,
Intrinsic religiosity,
Extrinsic religiosity
p < 0.001
Factor
analysis is
appropriate
... These studies suggested that understanding ethical dilemmas influences MRC, which is positively related to MJd. In connection with MJd and MIT, studies found consistent findings and asserted that MJd was positively related to MIT (Latan et al., 2019;Musbah et al., 2016;Oboh & Omolehinwa, 2022;Tariq et al., 2019). Although several empirical studies found significant and positive relationships among the components of EdM, some studies found inconsistent results with the relationships between MRC and MJd (e.g. ...
... Some empirical studies found that other constructs and MRC related to MIT through the mediation of MJd (e.g. Andersch et al., 2019;Johari et al., 2017;Rottig et al., 2011;Tariq et al., 2019;Yang & Wu, 2009). However, the extant literature concerning the relationship between MRC and MIT remains relatively unexplored. ...
... Moral relativism has a significantly negative influence on moral recognition. (Keller et al., 2007;Kportorgbi et al., 2022;Singhapakdi et al., 2013;Tariq et al., 2019;Vitell et al., 2009). Its ethical theory aligns with deontological ethics; hence, ethical decisions based on religious perspectives tend to form rule-based evaluations (Hunt & Vitell, 2006;Shariff, 2015;Tariq et al., 2019;Vitell et al., 2009). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examines the relationships between the components of ethical decision-making and the factors that contribute to the ethical decision-making of professional accountants. Survey data from 309 professional accountants was analyzed using partial least squares structural equation modeling. The study found positive relationships among moral recognition, moral judgment, and moral intent. Moral judgment mediates the relationship between moral recognition and moral intent in ethical decision-making. The study also found that laws and professional codes, moral philosophies, intrinsic religious orientation, and social responsibility influenced ethical decision-making. However, peer group pressure did not predict moral judgment among professional accountants. This study provides implications for considering the mediating role of moral judgment in the ethical decision-making process. To improve ethical decision-making among professional accountants, understanding the influences and relationships of other factors is crucial in an ethical setting.
... There is a plethora of literature on the respective influences of religiosity (Goel & Misra, 2020;Tariq et al. (2019), ethics education (Kakos, 2019;Parks-Leduc et al., 2021) and professional experience (Barrainkua & Espinosa-Pike, 2018) on ethical decision-making (EDM). There are, however, compelling reasons to study the concurrent and relative influence of these variables on EDM. ...
... Goel and Misra (2020) extracted two components of religious commitmentintrinsic and extrinsic-and found that only intrinsic religious commitment influence EDM. However, Tariq et al. (2019) suggest that both intrinsic and extrinsic commitment positively predict EDM. With a unique sample of ethics education "haves", this study contributes to the contemporary debate on which component of religious commitment influences EDM and to what extent. ...
... Nambukara-Gamage and Rahman (2020) and Longenecker et al. (2004) found that religious variable positively predicts ethical behaviour. Goel and Misra (2020) and Tariq et al. (2019) examined the relationship between two perspectives of religious commitment and EDM. While Goel and Misra (2020) found that only intra-personal religious commitment (a person's metaphysical belief) positively and significantly influences attitude To contribute to this emerging phase of literature, this study tests the following hypotheses: H1 a . ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper investigates the relative effect of re-enforced tax ethics education (RTEE), religious commitment and professional experience on ethical decision-making (EDM). Survey data from 356 tax accountants were analysed using the partial least square structural equation modelling technique. The study found that intra-religious commitment predicts EDM, but inter-personal religious commitment does not predict EDM. Further, all three examined variables concurrently influence EDM, but RTEE is the most influential EDM variable. The findings of this study should energise tax practice organisations to re-enforce tax ethics education among their officers and guide the assignment of officers to ethically sensitive tax engagements. The paper contributes to Kant’s theory of morality.
... In addition, to socialization, cultural values also cause differences to occur in religious identity, religious beliefs and religious practices (Güngör, Bornstein & Phalet, 2012). Religiosity is associated with ethics (Oumlil & Balloun, 2017;Chan & Ananthram, 2019;Tariq, Ansari & Alvi, 2019) and socially responsible consumption behaviors (Akremi & Smaoui, 2015;Bhuian et al., 2018;Islam & Chandrasekaran, 2020;Mansori, Safari, & Mohd Ismail, 2020;Zakaria, Wan-Ismail & Abdul-Talib, 2020) and is considered a cultural value (Güngör et al., 2012). Religiosity is also associated with various intrapersonal factors (Bhuian et al., 2018). ...
... Religiosity is the degree of belief and devotion to a religion (Usman et al., 2017;Bhuian et al., 2018;Tariq et al., 2019;Mansori et al., 2020;Zakaria et al., 2020). Studies evaluate religiosity in two dimensions: internal and external religiosity. ...
... Studies evaluate religiosity in two dimensions: internal and external religiosity. Internal religiosity refers to the desire to live religion, adhere to religious principles, and serve religion (Le & Kieu, 2019;Tariq et al., 2019). External religiosity is the utilitarian use of religion to gain social approval and self-interest (Le & Kieu, 2019;Tariq et al., 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to investigate the mediating role of religiosity on the relationship between individual cultural values and negative opinions on interest. In this way, we aim to obtain an empirical basis for theoretical knowledge and to provide a statistically greater explanation about negative opinions on interest and culture relations based on religiosity. In this context, data were collected between May 14 and June 1, 2020, in Turkey, a country where the majority of individuals are Muslim but Western values and synthesis of Eastern and Western cultures have been adopted. A total of 1.934 people participated in the survey. Exploratory factor analysis was performed on a newly developed scale measuring negative opinion on interest. Next, structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to test the research models and analyze the results from the confirmatory factor analysis. The analyses have determined individuals' cultural values to be a determinant of their religiosity levels and these religiosity levels to increase alongside increases in collectivism, power distance, masculinity, and long-term orientation. In addition, positive and statistically significant relationships have been determined for collectivism, power distance, masculinity, and uncertainty avoidance orientations with negative opinions on interest. Findings have been obtained showing a negative and statistically significant relationship to exist between being long-term orientated and negative opinion on interest. As expected, the greater the religiosity level, the higher the negative opinion on interest. In addition, religiosity has also been reported to have a mediating effect on the relationship collectivism, power distance, masculinity, and negative opinion on interest have with interest.
... Religion is a belief system (Donohue, 2020;Mansori et al., 2020) which shows that people should be valued (Güngör et al., 2012) and allowing the distinction between right and wrong (Oumlil & Balloun, 2017). The level of commitment to religion was defined as religiosity (Bhuian et al., 2018;Mansori et al., 2020;Tariq et al., 2019;Usman et al., 2017;Zakaria et al., 2020). Religion has psychological and social power (Islam & Chandrasekaran, 2020). ...
... It has two dimensions: internal and external religiosity. While in internal religiosity, there is adherence to religious principles and the desire to serve the religion, in external religiosity there is a utilitarian use for personal interests and social acceptance (Le & Kieu, 2019;Tariq et al., 2019). ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
In this study, the effect of religiosity and culture on the preference of financial instruments with interest and non-interest was investigated. For this purpose, 1934 people from Turkey was reached through an online survey and data were collected with convenience sampling. Individual preferences were investigated when interest-bearing and interest-free financial instruments had equal returns and interest-free financial instruments had lower returns. Individual cultural values, religiosity and negative opinions on interest were included as determinants of these choices. Individual cultural values were taken in five dimensions, and it was determined that each dimension has positive correlations with both religiosity and negative opinion on interest. The tendency to financial instruments with or without interest was designed as a three-dimensional categorical variable: (1) those who do not want interest return under any circumstances, (2) those who prefer an interest-free investment instrument when the return of an interest-free investment instrument and an interest-bearing investment instrument is equal and (3) those who always prefer an investment instrument with an interest return, and the effect of independent variables were investigated using discriminant analysis. Accordingly, it was determined that three dimensions of individual cultural values-collectivism, masculinity, power distance, and religiosity as well as negative opinion on interest are significant variables in the correct classification. In addition, those between the ages of 31-50 and those with undergraduate and graduate education have a higher tendency toward interest-bearing financial instruments.
... Further than CSR decisions, the knowledge of how religious and humanistic beliefs interact may also be relevant for informing different types of ethical decisions (Teixeira et al., 2022), that is, decisions that are both legal and morally acceptable to the larger community (Jones, 1991). As moral assessments are often grounded on the teachings of a religious tradition and/or the degree of agreement with a given worldview, both types of beliefs may play a relevant role on ethical decisions (Chen et al., 2021;Mohd Yusoff et al., 2022;Tariq et al., 2019;Valk, 2007). ...
Article
Purpose While the influence of individual beliefs on decision-making has been widely acknowledged, the interaction of different types of beliefs remains an under-researched topic. This study analyses how the simultaneous influence of religious beliefs and nonreligious beliefs shapes individual decision-making. This study aims to contribute to inform organizational decisions on topics potentially associated with these two types of beliefs, including corporate social responsibility matters. This study also aims to provide insights to ethical decision-making in situations of absence of social consensus, a subject that is relevant for individuals, organizations and policymakers. Design/methodology/approach This study uses fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis to identify how different configurational groupings of Christian beliefs and humanistic beliefs lead to the acceptance or nonacceptance of euthanasia in a sample of individuals who identify as Catholic. Findings Among individuals sharing a Catholic religious affiliation, the authors identify three different configurations of beliefs – Cultural Catholics (religious beliefs are absent and humanistic beliefs are present), Observant Catholics (religious beliefs are present and humanistic beliefs are irrelevant) and Secular Catholics (both religious beliefs and humanistic beliefs are present). Originality/value Previous research has put forward the role of religion-related variables, such as religious affiliation and level of religiosity, for views on euthanasia. This study provides a more detailed analysis of the role of belief systems, identifying how different configurational groupings of beliefs lead to a decision grounded in moral and ethical considerations but for which there is an absence of social consensus.
... Intrinsic religiosity mencakup komitemen internal terhadap agama karena spiritualitasnya (Patwardhan, Keith, and Vitell 2012). sedangkan extrinsic religiosity lebih kontekstual karena merupakan bentuk motif utilitarian (Tariq, Ansari, and Alvi 2019). Extrinsic religiosity mengindentifikasi bahwa seseorang yang memandang agama sebagai sarana mengejar kepentingan pribadi seperti hal nya status, sosialisasi atau di jadikan sebagai sarana untuk menghilangkan stres dan ketegangan (Sims and Bingham 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
Dalam penelitian ini bertujuan untuk menganalisis pengaruh Resilience, Confidence, Intrinsic Religiosity, Extrinsic Religiosity terhadap Mental Toughness pada karyawan Hotel di Jakarta. Jumlah responden dalam penelitian ini berjumlah 126 karyawan Hotel di Jakarta dari berbagai divisi. Metode yang digunakan yaitu regresi linear berganda, dimana dalam metode ini menggunakan software SPSS 21. Dalam penelitian ini menunjukkan bahwa Resilience, Confidence, Intrinsic Religiosity berpengaruh positif terhadap Mental Toughness, sedangkan Extrinsic Religiosity tidak ada pengaruh signifikan terhadap Mental Toughness. Berdasarkan penelitian perusahaan diharapkan ikut membantu untuk terus dapat meningkatkan Mental Toughness karyawan yang dimana akan berdampak pada kinerja karyawan seperti karyawan dapat menangani tugas yang sulit dan secara efektif dapat menangani kejadian tidak terduga. Kata Kunci: Resilience, Confidence, Intrinsic Religiosity, Extrinsic Religiosity, Mental Toughness
Article
This review summarizes the empirical ethical decision‐making (EDM) research in business published between 2012 and 2022. Utilizing Rest's (Moral development: advances in research and theory, Praeger, New York, 1986) four‐step model for EDM and Jones' (Acad Manag Rev, 16(2): 366‐395, 1991) theory of moral intensity, 85 articles, resulting in 388 findings, were analyzed. Empirical findings in awareness, intent, judgment, and behavior were categorized by their application to individual and organizational factors resulting in the application of 624 and 62 factors, respectively. A maturing of the research environment is seen through the need for grouping individual factors into themes, essentially forming new connections within EDM research. This article prioritizes new factors and themes, such as bad behaviors, customers and selling, fantasy and imagination, and negative experiences. Additionally, new research in reoccurring themes such as demographics, feelings, personality, and power have elements that were seen in prior reviews but are more nuanced in the current. Furthermore, this quinary review discusses the evolution of EDM research highlighting the relationships studied and modifiers and mediators used. These themes help shape the landscape of EDM research by illustrating the intersectionality of variables. This article advances the understanding of how these foundational models are being nuanced to understand more deeply the EDM process. A call for future research incorporating intersectionality, the continued pursuit of complex relationships, longitudinal research, and major societal and organizational movements is included. Research into the effects of underrepresented demographics such as gender identity, veteran status, and ability is suggested. Furthermore, we question whether Rest (1986) and Jones' (1991) models are inherently Western and if comparing outcomes with a decolonialized research method might give insight into EDM. We present this latest 10‐year collection of EDM empirical research based on Rest (1986) and Jones (1991) as a tool for new and future scholars to utilize in their research endeavors.
Article
The issue of AI ethics is a prominent research subject. While there is a compendious literature that explores this area, surprisingly little of it makes explicit reference to the ethic-theoretical foundations upon which it is built. To address this matter, this study makes an examination of the AI-Ethics literature to identify its ethic-theoretical foundations. The study identifies the lack of AI ethics literature that draws upon seminal ethics works and the ensuing disconnectedness among the publications on this subject. It also uncovers numerous non-Western ethic-theoretical positions that can be adopted and may afford new insight into AI ethics research and practice. Employing these alternative lenses may obviate the tendency for Western worldviews to dominate the academic literature. The study provides some guidance for future AI ethics research which should endeavour to clearly articulate its chosen ethic-theoretical position, and for practice which could benefit from understanding and articulating the principles upon which AI systems are founded. It also provides some observations of, and guidance for, the utilization of Litmaps software in the conduct of Literature reviews.
Article
This research aims to examine the impact of intrinsic religiosity on counterproductive work behavior directed toward the organization (CWB‐O) and the mediating role of Islamic work ethic in this impact. The data were collected from employees working in the hotel and restaurant industry (n = 684). The findings revealed that intrinsic religiosity had a positive and significant effect on Islamic work ethic. It was found that Islamic work ethic also had a negative and significant effect on CWB‐O. Similarly, intrinsic religiosity had a negative and significant effect on CWB‐O. In addition, it was identified that the Islamic work ethic had a mediating role in the impact of intrinsic religiosity on CWB‐O. Taking into consideration that the relationship between intrinsic religiosity and CWB‐O and the role of Islamic work ethic in this relationship has not yet been examined in literature, the theoretical contribution of the current research can be better understood. From the perspective of practical contribution, this research may help and guide managers to understand how intrinsic religiosity affects CWB‐O.
Article
Full-text available
Should a Catholic hospital abort a life-threatening pregnancy or let a pregnant woman die? Should a religious employer allow his employees access to contraceptives or break with healthcare legislation? People and organizations of faith often face moral decisions that have significant consequences. Research in psychology found that religion is typically associated with deontological judgment. Yet deontology consists of many principles, which may, at times, conflict. In three studies, we design a conflict between moral principles and find that the relationship between moral judgment and religiosity is more nuanced than currently assumed. Studies 1 and 2 show that, while religious U.S. Christians and Israeli Jews are more likely to form deontological judgments, they divide between the deontological principles of inaction and indirectness. Using textual analysis, we reveal that specific beliefs regarding divine responsibility and human responsibility distinguish inaction from indirectness deontologists. Study 3 exploits natural differences in religious saliency across days of the week to provide causal evidence that religion raises deontological tendencies on Sundays and selectively increases the appeal of inaction deontology for those who believe in an interventionist and responsible God.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose This paper investigates Pakistani bank front-line employees’ intentions to behave ethically by using the Extended Theory of Planned Behaviour (ETPB) into which religiosity (i.e. religious activity, devotion to rituals and belief in doctrine) is integrated as a moderating variable. Design/methodology/approach The authors collected 234 self-administered questionnaires and analysed them using SmartPLS 2.0, a second generation structural equation modelling technique. Findings This paper demonstrates that the ETPB can explain intentions to behave ethically. Moral norms (i.e. the rules of morality that people believe they ought to follow) and perceived behavioural control (i.e. people's perceptions of their ability to perform a given behaviour) are the best predictors of ethical behavioural intentions. The effects of injunctive norms (i.e. perceptions of which behaviours are typically approved or disapproved in an organisation) and of perceived behavioural control on behavioural intent are moderated by religiosity Practical implications Leading by example, providing ethics training, empowering employees and encouraging the expression of religiosity are proposed as ways to foster an ethical culture in the workplace. Originality/value Even though numerous empirical studies have utilised variants of the TPB to explain consumer behaviour, its applicability to ethical behaviour in the workplace has scarcely been explored. Moreover, its tests in non-western contexts are scant. This study demonstrates the applicability of the ETPB in a broader circumstantial and cultural context and enriches it with religiosity, a pertinent characteristic of billions of people around the world. Finally, this is one of the very few ethics studies focusing on banking, an industry fraught with allegations of moral breaches.
Article
Full-text available
The paper reports the findings of thirteen interviews with prominent Sri Lankan business leaders drawn from Buddhist, Christian, Hindu, and Muslim religious traditions. The in-depth interviews with the leaders were supplemented by documentary sources. When the leaders were asked why they engaged in religion-based workplace spirituality, their responses were often associated with decision-making. Although they had an array of management tools with which to deal with day-to-day management situations, they all indicated that, in ‘difficult’ moments, these tools needed to be complemented by processes by which they connected with the ultimate – variously identified as the transcendent reality, god, or truth that is more powerful, better, and good. The outcomes of decisions, both good and bad, were usually attributed to that connecting experience. The findings suggest that religion plays a significant role in influencing the judgment, emotional and motivational qualities of Sri Lankan leaders' decision-making – in that a frame of reference based on a connection with a transcendent and ultimate reality is likely to be a source of solace, guidance, and inspiration to leaders' critical decision-making.
Article
This article addresses a significant gap in the theoretical literature on marketing ethics. This gap results from the lack of an integrated framework which clarifies and synthesizes the multiple variables that explain how marketers make ethical/unethical decisions. A contingency framework is recommended as a starting point for the development of a theory of ethical/unethical actions in organizational environments. This model demonstrates how previous research can be integrated to reveal that ethical/unethical decisions are moderated by individual factors, significant others within the organizational setting, and opportunity for action.
Article
Is a given marketing action always ethical, never ethical, or ethical relative to time, place, and circumstances? Upon what logical bases can a marketer determine what is “right” for him to do, not only in domestic but also in international business? This article presents a schematic plan for analyzing the variables inherent in the ethics of decision-making; and a framework for social and personal ethics is proposed.
Chapter
A model is developed which identifies and describes various factors which affect ethical and unethical behavior in organizations, including a decision-maker’s social, government and legal, work, professional and personal environments. The effect of individual decision maker attributes on the decision process is also discussed. The model links these influences with ethical and unethical behavior via the mediating structure of the individual’s decision-making process.
Book
Analyzing the relationship between globalization and cultures is the core objective of this volume. In it leading experts track cultural trends in all regions of the world, covering issues ranging from the role of cultural difference in politics and governance to heritage conservation, artistic expression, and the cultural industries. The book also includes a data section that consolidates the recently commenced but still inchoate work of cultural indicators.