ChapterPDF Available

Children's creativity

Authors:

Abstract

Creativity is traditionally defined as the ability to produce something that is original and valued. However, this definition applies to Big-C (domain-changing) creativity (e.g., Darwin's theory of evolution, Picasso and Braque'’s invention of cubism). Children are by definition little-c creators, since they have not mastered a domain sufficiently to challenge it. We distinguish here between creativity and intelligence, and argue for creativity as domain-specific. We discuss the decline in children'’ creativity with age, along with the major methods of assessing children's creativity. We conclude by considering factors that impede as well as enhance children's creativity.
Provided for non-commercial research and educational use.
Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use.
This article was originally published in Encyclopedia of Creativity, 3rd edition,
published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author's
benefit and for the benefit of the author's institution, for non-commercial research and
educational use including without limitation use in instruction at your institution,
sending it to specific colleagues who you know, and providing a copy to your
institution's administrator.
All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including
without limitation commercial reprints, selling or
licensing copies or access, or posting on open
internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or
repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission
may be sought for such use through Elsevier’s
permissions site at:
https://www.elsevier.com/about/our-business/policies/copyright/permissions
From Ershadi, M., Winner, E., 2020. Children's Creativity. In: Runco, M., Pritzker, S.
(Eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity, 3rd edition, vol. 1. Elsevier, Academic Press, pp.
144–148.
ISBN: 9780128156148
Copyright © 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Academic Press
Author's personal copy
Children’s Creativity
Mahsa Ershadi and Ellen Winner, Department of Psychology, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, MA, United States
© 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Introduction 144
The Nature of Childrens Creativity 144
The Assessment of Childrens Creativity 145
The Development of Childrens Creativity 145
Factors Affecting Childrens Creativity 146
Externally Caused Factors 146
Internal Factors 147
Conclusion 147
References 147
Further Reading 147
Introduction
Consider childrens creativity and you may be reminded of a cardboard box transmogried into a space ship, a pillowcase crafted
into a super heros cape, or a magical fort made-up of blankets strewn over strategically arranged dining room chairs. Supply chil-
dren with crayons, paint, clay, paper, and scissors, and they will experiment and construct fantastical images and playthings. In fact,
children will engage in pretend play, improvise songs and dances, tell stories, and make-up imaginary friends often without
abandon or the need for toysin such a spontaneous and natural way that it seems it go without question that children are creative.
While it is easy to see that children are creative, it is also clear that their creativity is qualitatively distinct from the type of domain-
changing creativity associated with eminent artists and scientists such as Beethoven, Maya Angelou, Albert Einstein, and Steve Jobs.
In what follows, we address the nature of creativity seen in typical children, the methods for assessing this disposition, and the
factors which impede and those that promote childrens creativity. We write from the assumption that creativity, like intelligence,
is domain-specic(Gardner, 1983), and that creativity is distinct from intelligence because any intelligence can beused in a more or
less creative way.
The Nature of Children’s Creativity
The standard denition of creativity focusses on product: a creative individual is someone who produces something novel and valu-
able within a domain whether an intangible creation such as a theory, joke, or musical composition, or a physical production such
as an invention, literary work or sculpture. This denition clearly refers to the kind of domain-changing creativity we refer to here as
Big-C creativity. Although levels of creativity lie on a continuum, investigations of creativity have traditionally distinguished little-c
creativity (the kind measured by standard creativity tests) and Big C creativity. Big-C creativity describes revolutionary contributions
like Thomas Edisons invention of the light bulb and Picassos invention (with Braque) of cubism; little-c creativity refers to more
ordinary innovations such as repurposing leftovers for a new delicious meal.
Which of these do children demonstrate? Given that one must master a domain said to take about ten years before one can
change it, then by denition no child can be Big-C creative. Typical children are little-c creators. Even child prodigies cannot be
considered creative in the Big-C sense: Mozarts 5-year-old composition, Minuet and Trio in G major, was stunning for a 5-year-
old, but it did not change the domain of classical music. Blaise Pascals discovery at age of 12 that the interior angles of a triangle
always add up to the sum of two right angles was equally stunning, but did not change the domain of mathematics since this
concept had already been discovered. But, Mozarts and Pascals childhood creativity are qualitatively distinct from the creative
play seen in a typical preschool classroom, and this has led to the concept of mini-c creativity. Beghetto and Kaufman (2007)
propose distinguishing the creativity of the average child from that of the prodigy (or amateur artist, in their terms) by calling
that of the prodigy mini-c.
Unlike the Big/little categories, which focuson creative expression and production, the mini-c category emphasizes creative poten-
tial and the creative process; thus, offering a new standard for evaluating childrens creativity. Mini-c creativity refers to the novel and
personally meaningful interpretation of experiences, actions, and events,to the dynamic, interpretive process of constructing
personal knowledge and understanding within a particular sociocultural context(Kaufman and Beghetto, 2009). The mini-c concept
aligns with the Vygotskian conception of creative development, which posits that all individuals possess creative potential that begins
with an internalization or appropriation of cultural tools and social interaction .not just copying but rather a transformation or
reorganization of incoming information and mental structures based on the individuals characteristics and existing knowledge
(Moran and John-Steiner, 2003, p. 63). The mini-c category underscores the value of recognizing creative aspects associated with
144 Encyclopedia of Creativity, 3rd edition, Volume 1 https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809324-5.23693-6
Encyclopedia of Creativity, Third Edition, 2020, 144–148
Author's personal copy
the beginners mindlike openness to new experiences, active observation, and willingness to be surprised, and explore the unknown
(Richards, 2007). The mini-c category offers a level of specicity that can allow childrens creative interpretations and ideations, as well
as the associated behaviors and attitudes, to be recognized and nurtured by parents and educators (Kaufman and Beghetto, 2009).
Although there has been some effort to develop a systematic coding frame for micro-level measurements of creativity (described later
in this chapter), we do not yet have a standardized valid and reliable way of assessing mini-c creativity.
The Assessment of Children’s Creativity
In 1957, the Soviet Union beat the United States into orbit by successfully launching Sputnik, the rst articial satellite. In response
to this defeat, American psychologists developed a wide variety of tests to assess childrens creative potential, specically in science
and math. Ever since, a plethora of tests have become available for assessing childrens creativity at the level of the person, the crea-
tive product, and the process. A review of the literature on childrens creativity shows that 80% most of studies employ measures on
the level of the person or the product (Kupers et al., 2018). Clearly, more research is needed on the creative process.
Divergent thinking tasks are commonly used as indicators of creative potential, and most measures of childrens creativity have
focused on ideational uency in which participants must generate as many responses as they can to a particular stimulus. An
example of such a task is the Alternative Uses Task where participants are asked to list as many uses as possible for a common
item. Responses are scored in term of originality (as compared to responses from other children), uency (number of responses),
exibility (number of different categories), and elaboration (amount of detail).
The most widely used assessment of divergent thinking is the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking, which includes a verbal section
(Thinking Creatively with Words) consisting of six activities (asking, guessing causes, guessing consequences, product improvement,
unusual uses, unusual questions, and just suppose) and a nonverbal section (Thinking Creatively with Pictures) consisting of three
gural activities (picture construction, picture completion, and lines/circles). As is the case with the Alternative Uses Test, scores on
the Torrance are based on uency, exibility, and originality.
Unlike the Alternative Uses and Torrance Tests, which are domain-general assessments of creativity, the Evaluation of Potential
for Creativity is used to assess domain-specic divergent and convergent thinking via both verbal and graphic tasks appropriate for
elementary through middle school students. The Evaluation of Potential for Creativity is domain-specic, with versions for artistic,
literary, social, math, science, and music creativity (Barbot et al., 2011).
Despite their popularity, some creativity researchers have questioned the usefulness of divergent thinking tasks. Simonton
(2003, p. 216) expressed concern that none of these suggested measures can be said to have passed all the psychometric hurdles
required of established ability tests. For instance, scores on separate creativity tests often correlate too highly with general intelli-
gence (low divergent validity), correlate very weakly among one another (low convergent validity), and correlate very weakly
with objective indicators of overt creative behaviors (low predictive validity).Although new approaches to the assessment of diver-
gent thinking have been proposed (e.g. top-2 scoring method, in which participants are asked to select their two most creative ideas,
which then are evaluated for creativity; Silvia et al., 2008), these evaluations are still just snapshots of creativity that do not capture
in real-time the core of creativity, namely the creative process.
Alternatively, Kaufman and Beghetto (2009) suggest self-assessments and microgenetic methods as more naturalistic and infor-
matively rich approaches to pencil-and-paper measures of creative potential, argued to better illuminate the developmental process
of childrens creativity (mini-c). Self-assessments are useful because they require students to reect on their own creativity, and thus
can reveal how children think about their creativity. The microgenetic method combines close observations (typically video
recorded) of children as they progress through developmental stages, and repeated measurements (such as asking children for
immediate retrospective explanations of their thoughts and actions) over the course of time. This method has been previously
used to study childrens learning (Siegler, 2006), and could provide a detailed illustration of the development of a childs creativity
by allowing researchers to identify and examine dynamic microlevel changes in psychological structures. Five specic dimensions
can be examined through the microgenetic method: the path of change (is the change qualitative or quantitative?); the rate of
change (is the change sudden or slow?); the breadth of change (is the change domain-specic or generalizable across domains?;
the variability of change (how variable is a persons behavior across similar tasks within a domain?); the source of change (what
do the changes in behavior, such as strategy use, suggest about the source of change?; Siegler, 2006). Researchers interested in using
the microgenetic method to study childrens creativity, could employ the coding system proposed by Kupers et al. (2018) which
captures the two core components of creativity novelty and appropriateness on an ordinal four-point scale at each moment
during the creative process. The coding frame follows three steps: rst, researchers must determine the unit of measurement (the
level of detail in which they wish to study the creative process), and they then code the units in terms of novelty and appropriate-
ness. This measure is a preliminary and not yet validated, but could well serve as the state of the art in childrens creative potential
assessment by providing a fuller picture of what is provided by narrower, standard psychometric approaches.
The Development of Children’s Creativity
Elsewhere, we have proposed that artistic development in children follows a U-shaped curve (Gardner and Winner, 1981), with
preschool childrens processes and product similar to those of adult artists, followed by a period between childhood and adulthood
Children’s Creativity 145
Encyclopedia of Creativity, Third Edition, 2020, 144–148
Author's personal copy
characterized by literalism. Thus, childrens drawings are at rst playful, joyful, and unconcerned with the conventions of represen-
tation or realism. By middle childhood their drawings become more stereotyped as they master the conventions of the graphic
domain and focus on the rules of realistic representation. By late adolescence, at least among those children who will become artists,
conventions are now broken. Thus, the preschool child is preconventional; the adult artist is post-conventional. Most adults
however never emerge from the literal stage. This three-stage model has been shown to t both the development of drawing
and of non-literal language use (Gardner and Winner, 1981).
We can draw a connection here to the stages of childrens creativity: it has been argued that young children are more creative
than (typical) adults. Both Pablo Picasso and John Lennon shared the belief that artistic impulse is strongest in the early years of
life and often attenuates as a child matures. This sentiment is echoed in the most watched TED talk of all time titled Do schools
kill creativity?in which educationalist Sir Ken Robinson argues that we dont grow into creativity, we grow out of it.In fact,
a longitudinal study examining creative capacity across childhood showed that when kindergartners were tested, 98 percent scored
at the genius level in divergent thinking on the test employed by the National Aeronautics and Space Administration to select
innovative engineers and scientists. The same children were re-tested when they were ten and 15 years old, at which point
only 32 percent and 12 percent scored as high, respectively. Moreover, only a mere two percent of adults over the age of 25 demon-
strated the same high level of creativity as the ve-year-olds in the study (Land and Jarman, 1992). Kim (2011) found a similar
trend of diminishing creativity with age; her analysis of the Torrance scores of 272, 599 kindergarten through 12th grade students
and adults between 1974 and 2008 showed that since 1990, even as IQ scores grew higher, creative thinking scores have signif-
icantly declined, especially in kindergarten through third grade. These ndings reveal that children have, over the years, become
less able to generate many ideas, generate novel ideas, and elaborate on these ideas, and have become less intellectually curious
and less open to new experiences.
Kims study is based on the assumption that creativity is domain-general. However, a study by Weinstein et al. (2014) reported
a decline in the creativity of adolescent writing since 1990, along with a rise in the creativity of adolescent visual artworks (as
assessed qualitatively rather than by a standardized creativity test). Findings like Kims as well as Weinstein et al.s underscore
the need to discover the factors that impede (as well as promote) childrens creativity.
Factors Affecting Children’s Creativity
This loss of creative potential may result from external factors such as pressure to think and behave according to conventions. This
loss might also reect internal factors such as an increase in reasoning skills and executive functions, which also takes place around
this age and which may interfere with the free play of imagination.
Externally Caused Factors
One factor that may be impeding childrens creativity is the emphasis on rote learning and standardized testing in schools. This
focus stands in direct contrast to play. Beginning at about eighteen months, children engage extensively in pretend play, which
may serve as practice for the types of imaginative thinking observed in the creative activities and innovations of adults. Play
whether physical, constructive, imaginative, dramatic, or games with rules is the primary way that humans learn to understand
and experience the world. Yet, instead of prioritizing play, the focus and investment on narrow high-stakes standardized testing
in schools has left little time or resources for children to employ their creative potential.
A second factor that has been shown to impede creativity in children (as well as adults) is the extrinsic motivation to get a good
grade or win a competition. Research has shown that intrinsic motivation is important for creativity, and that extrinsic motivation is
a strong impediment.
A third factor may be parental focus on obedience. Being creative requires taking risks. Creative people innovate novel ideas that
may be criticized, ridiculed, or ignored by audiences. In fact, creativity is most strongly associated with social risk-taking (i.e. the
willingness to challenge norms), which can be an ostracizing experience. If creativity is to develop, it must be encouraged by caring
others in a supportive environment. However, research shows that child obedience is valued over independence by parents in
nations where poverty and lack of education prevail. Given that creativity necessitates risk-taking and sometimes rule-breaking,
it follows that children living in poverty may face an impediment to their creativity from parenting practices that foster obedience
and conformity. Additionally, children from low-income households often attend Title 1 schools institutions with large concen-
trations of low-income students that adhere to rigid classrooms and high-stakes standardized tests. Furthermore, the No Child Left
Behind Act and Common Core Standards Act caused schools to focus their resources on subjects like reading and math to achieve
high scores on statewide standardized tests. As a result, arts classes (which are more likely to develop creativity than classes requiring
rote learning) received less funding, leading to fewer arts opportunities in schools. The consequence of these budget cuts is less detri-
mental for students who live in afuent communities and receive the benets of arts education through extracurricular programs,
which may be cost-probative for low-income children.
External factors may also be used to promote creativity specically a focus on play. Research has shown that children who partic-
ipated in exible play experiences showed signicantly greater creativity on the Torrance than children participating in highly struc-
tured play experiences. Consistent with these results, Garaigordobil and Berrueco (2011) found verbal and graphic-gural creativity
of preschoolers, and 10- and 11-year-olds improved signicantly from a weekly 75-min or two-hour play session throughout the
146 Children’s Creativity
Encyclopedia of Creativity, Third Edition, 2020, 144–148
Author's personal copy
academic year. Dansky and Silverman (1973) found that children who were given the opportunity to play with objects gave signif-
icantly more uses for those objects than did children in the control condition who did not get a chance to play with the objects
beforehand; ndings from this study highlight the importance of play as a means of encouraging divergent thinking and reducing
functional xedness (a cognitive bias that limits ones mental representation of an object to that objects typical use).
Internal Factors
An internal factor that has been shown to impede creativity in adults is strong cognitive control. Cognitive control, the ability to
limit attention to goal-relevant information, subserves higher-order cognitive functions such as reasoning, attention, planning
and organization. Counterintuitively, decits in these functions have proven advantageous in certain contexts: low cognitive control
means less ltering of attention, and such unltered attention leads to novel solutions in creative problem solving contexts. For
example, young children have weaker cognitive control than do older children, and have also been found to solve creative problems
better than older children (German and Defeyter, 2000). In addition, when cognitive control is compromised because of Attention
Decit Hyperactivity Disorder, age, fatigue, intoxication, and disruption of the pre-frontal cortex due to transcranial direct current
stimulation or lesions, the ability to think out of the boxis enhanced. In short, impaired cognitive control in adults is associated
with heighted creative thinking, and we are now investigating the possibility that children with low cognitive control (often a result
of the stress of poverty) have enhanced creative abilities. Paradoxically, thus, schoolsattempts to enhance cognitive control may
have the unintended consequence of dampening creative potential.
Conclusion
In this chapter, we reviewed research showing that while children demonstrate little-c creativity across a variety of domains, a focus
on mini-c may help to shed light on the process of childrens creativity. A decline of creativity in mid-childhood, the fourth-grade
slump,may be a consequence of education systems and child rearing practices that value obedience over play.
As globalization and technological developments accelerate, we face unprecedented social, economic and environmental chal-
lenges. In order to successfully navigate the uncertainties of the future and solve unpredictable problems, our children will need to
develop their creative capacities. Accordingly, the Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development, United Nations
Educational, Scientic and Cultural Organization, and Partnership for 21st Century Skills have identied creativity and innovation
in problem-solving as fundamental employability skills. Considering that corporate executives and public sector leaders worldwide
cite creativity as the most important leadership quality for success in business, todays children would be best served by rearing prac-
tices and curricula that nurture the development of their creativity.
References
Barbot, B., Besançon, M., Lubart, T.I., 2011. Assessing creativity in the classroom. Open Educ. J. 4, 5866.
Beghetto, R.A., Kaufman, J.C., 2007. Toward a broader conception of creativity: a case for "mini-c" creativity. Psychol. Aesthet. Creativity Arts 1,7379.
Dansky, J., Silverman, F., 1973. Effects of play on associative uency in preschool-aged children. Dev. Psychol. 9, 3843.
Gardner, H., 1983. Frames of Mind. Basic Books, New York, NY.
Gardner, H., Winner, E., 1981. First intimations of artistry. In: Strauss, S. (Ed.), U-shaped Behavioral Growth. Academic Press, New York.
German, T.P., Defeyter, M.A., 2000. Immunity to functional xedness in young children. Psychonomic Bull. Rev. 7, 707712.
Garaigordobil, M., Berrueco, L., 2011. Effects of a play program on creative thinking of preschool children. Span. J. Psychol. 14, 608618.
Kaufman, J.C., Beghetto, R.A., 2009. Beyond big and little: the four c model of creativity. Rev. General Psychol. 13, 112.
Kupers, E., Van Dijk, M., Lehmann-Wermser, A., 2018. Creativity in the here and now: a generic, micro-developmental measure of creativity. Front. Psychol. 9, 114.
Land, G., Jarman, B., 1992. Breakpoint and beyond: Mastering the Future Today. HarperCollins Publishers.
Moran, S., John-Steiner, V., 2003. Creativity in the making: vygotskys contemporary contribution to the dialectic of development and creativity. In: Sawyer, R.K., John-Steiner, V.,
Moran, S., Sternberg, R.J., Feldman, D.H., Nakamura, J., Csikszentmihalyi, M. (Eds.), Creativity and Development. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 6190.
Richards, R., 2007. Everyday creativity: our hidden potential. In: Richards, R. (Ed.), Everyday Creativity and New Views of Human Nature. American Psychological Association,
Washington, DC, pp. 2554.
Siegler, R.S., 2006. Microgenetic analyses of learning. In: Damon, W., Lerner, R., Kuhn, D., Siegler, R.S. (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology, Cognition, Perception, and
Language, sixth ed., vol. 2. Wiley, Hoboken, NJ.
Silvia, P.J., Winterstein, B.P., Willse, J.T., Barona, C.M., Cram, J.T., Hess, K.I., Martinez, J.L., Richard, C.A., 2008. Assessing creativity with divergent thinking tasks: exploring the
reliability and validity of new subjective scoring methods. Psychol. Aesthet. Creativity, Arts 2, 6885.
Simonton, D.K., 2003. Expertise, competence, and creative ability: the perplexing complexities. In: Sternberg, R.J., Grigorenko, E.L. (Eds.), The Psychology of Abilities, Compe-
tencies, and Expertise. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 213239.
Weinstein, E.C., Clark, Z., DiBartolomeo, D.J., Davis, K., 2014. A decline in creativity? It depends on the domain. Creativity research journal 26, 174184.
Further Reading
Amabile, T.M., 1982. Childrens artistic creativity: detrimental effects of competition in a eld setting. Personality Soc. Psychol. Bull. 8 (3).
Amabile, T.M., 1996. Growing up Creative: Nurturing a Lifetime of Creativity, second ed. Creative Education Foundation.
Children’s Creativity 147
Encyclopedia of Creativity, Third Edition, 2020, 144–148
Author's personal copy
Barbot, B., Lubart, T.I., Besançon, M., 2016. Peaks, Slumps, and Bumps: Individual Differences in the Development of Creativity in Children and Adolescents. In: New directions
for Child and Adolescent Development, vol. 2016, pp. 3345.
Beghetto, R.A., Kaufman, J.C., 2007. Toward a broader conception of creativity: a case for "mini-c" creativity. Psychol. Aesthet. Creativity Arts 1,7379.
Kim, K.H., 2011. The creativity crisis: the decrease in creative thinking scores on the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Creativity Res. J. 23, 285295.
148 Children’s Creativity
Encyclopedia of Creativity, Third Edition, 2020, 144–148
Author's personal copy
Article
Drawing on the Four C Model of Creativity and Consensual Assessment Technique (CAT) as frameworks, the multi-site qualitative case study explored the creative practice of 16 Ghanaian secondary visual arts teachers in the Sekondi-Takoradi Metropolis, as the 2019 National Pre-Tertiary Curriculum Framework mandates creativity. The researchers used the interpretivist and constructivist paradigms for the study, which employed interviews, documents review and participant observations to gather data from eight secondary schools. The data were analysed using thematic analysis, with two categories of teachers emerging: teacher-practitioners and non-teacher-practitioners. The practising teachers were categorised under little-c and Pro-c—none fell within the Big-C category. Practising teachers were seen to be more successful in enacting creativity in their teaching practice, meeting the aims of the curriculum framework. We recommend that education providers and stakeholders should encourage the artistic practice of visual arts teachers and make it an integral part of professional practice.
Article
The importance of creativity in addressing a diverse range of issues and the urgency of fostering it through education from a life-long learning perspective have been recognised in strategic and educational policy documents at both European Union and national levels. While recognising the key role of creativity in driving positive change, Lithuania has considerable room for further growth in fostering creativity amongst its citizens. The growing interest of researchers in studying creativity education can make it difficult to remain at the cutting-edge of knowledge. Since creativity is a culturally grounded construct, it is significant to examine the available evidence in a context-specific setting in order to inform future research directions and facilitate comparisons across contexts. The systematic review aims to explore the state of empirical research on creativity education in the Lithuanian context, focusing on young children of early childhood education and care (ECEC) and primary school age. The study involves a systematic search, appraisal, and synthesis of empirical research evidence published in peer-reviewed scientific journals between 2000 and 2022. The review identifies key trends and gaps in the empirical research evidence on the matter and draws recommendations for future research. The studies reviewed (n = 36) tended to focus on young children's creativity education in the arts, the role of the educator in creating a learning environment conducive to creativity development, and the characteristics of such an environment. Limited interest was expressed in creativity education in domains other than the arts, through non-formal educational activities or through the use of state-of-the-art digital tools.
Article
Full-text available
The development of creativity through learning is a significant part of Ghana's pre-tertiary education system framework. Achieving the successful implementation of creativity from policy to practice in schools relies on teachers in the local school system, who are shaped by their past teaching experiences and the training they have received during initial teacher education (ITE). Using interviews and observation data, this case study explored 16 secondary visual arts teachers' experiences and reflections on their training in relation to creativity and its impact on their current practice. Five themes emerged from an inductive analysis: containment, free expression, self-directed learning, replication of similar ideas and accountability-driven artistic productions. The study recommended both in-service training and ITE need to focus on creativity among other 21st-century skills in order for teachers to effectively implement creativity processes in their pedagogies. Additional measures for improving creativity in facets of ITE and teaching practice are discussed.
Article
Developing creativity in learners is contingent on how teachers define and understand creativity. This qualitative multi-site case study investigated secondary visual arts teachers’ perceptions and understanding of creativity in Sekondi-Takoradi in Ghana and the influences that impact their perceptions. Data were gathered from 16 secondary visual arts teachers in Ghana using interviews and documents review. It was evidenced that the secondary visual arts teachers’ perceptions and understanding of creativity are limited and built on historical Ghanaian educational materials. The findings suggest teacher training materials and school-based educational resources in Ghana must be upgraded to extend teachers’ understanding of creativity within education.
Article
Full-text available
Creativity is a relevant yet elusive concept, and consequently there is a large range of methods to assess creativity in many different contexts. Broadly speaking, we can differentiate between creativity measures on the level of the person (such as the Torrance tests), the level of the creative product (consensual assessment), and the level of the creative process. In the recent literature on children's creativity, 80% of the studies employed measures on either the person or the product level (Kupers et al., submitted). However, for parents, teachers, and employers who wish to stimulate creativity, insight in the (often socially embedded) creative process is badly needed. This move from the inter-individual to the intra-individual level of assessment is furthermore in line with research in many other domains in psychology. Although there is some research focusing more on detailed descriptions of creative processes, the studies are usually purely qualitative and therefore highly context-specific, making generalization difficult. In this paper, we present a newly developed coding frame as a systematic, generic, micro-level measure of creativity. What is unique about this coding frame is that it can be applied to observations of creative processes in many different contexts, and for different kinds of creative tasks. The core of the instrument is that it allows us to assess the two core components of creativity - novelty and appropriateness on an ordinal 4-point scale, at each moment during the creative process. The coding frame can be applied in three steps. The first step is to determine the unit of analysis, that is, the level of detail in which the creative process is assessed. The second step and third steps are coding the units on two ordinal scales of novelty and appropriateness, respectively. In order to illustrate the versatility of our instrument, we apply it to two cases of very different creative processes: a musical composition task (open-ended) and a scientific reasoning task (closed- ended). Last, we demonstrate the possibilities for analyzing this type of dense intra-individual measurements of creativity (time series analysis and state space grids) and discuss the future research that is needed to fully validate the instrument.
Article
Full-text available
This article reviews developmental studies of creativity in children and adolescents with a focus on "peaks" and "slumps" that have often been described in the literature. The irregularity of the development of creativity is interpreted in light of conceptual and measurement issues and with regard to the interaction between individual-level resources, task-specific demands, and environmental influences, resulting in apparent individual differences in the development of creativity. The need for longitudinal designs, multidimensional and multi-domain assessment of creative potential limiting the contribution of task-specific factors is outlined and discussed as an important direction for developmental research on creativity.
Article
Full-text available
This article provides a historical presentation of creativity assessment from a psychological perspective (since Guilford), from traditional divergent thinking tasks, to current theoretical models of creativity assessment which allow creativity to be assessed in different domains, capturing the multidimensionality of creative potential (e.g. EPoC). These techniques and alternative assessment tools are contextualized by addressing their interest for educational programs which take into account children's needs (as suggested by their creative profile). Finally, we discuss the importance of assessing creativity reliably in the classroom, to study the efficiency of these educational intervention programs aiming to promote student creativity.
Article
Full-text available
Predicted that children permitted to play freely with 4 types of objects would score higher on an alternate-uses test than children not given this opportunity. 90 nursery school Ss were assigned to play, imitation, and control treatment groups. Play group Ss named significantly more nonstandard uses and used more available environmental cues than Ss using the objects in an imitative context or those having no prior exposure to the objects. No differences were found in the number of standard uses given by the 3 groups or between imitation and control Ss on any analysis. Results were discussed in terms of Piaget's 1962 theory of play and M. A. Wallach and N. Kogan's associative theory of creativity (see PA, Vol. 40:429). (18 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Divergent thinking is central to the study of individual differences in creativity, but the traditional scoring systems (assigning points for infrequent responses and summing the points) face well-known problems. After critically reviewing past scoring methods, this article describes a new approach to assessing divergent thinking and appraises its reliability and validity. In our new Top 2 scoring method, participants complete a divergent thinking task and then circle the 2 responses that they think are their most creative responses. Raters then evaluate the responses on a 5-point scale. Regarding reliability, a generalizability analysis showed that subjective ratings of unusual-uses tasks and instances tasks yield dependable scores with only 2 or 3 raters. Regarding validity, a latent-variable study (n=226) predicted divergent thinking from the Big Five factors and their higher-order traits (Plasticity and Stability). Over half of the variance in divergent thinking could be explained by dimensions of personality. The article presents instructions for measuring divergent thinking with the new method. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Most investigations of creativity tend to take one of two directions: everyday creativity (also called "little-c"), which can be found in nearly all people, and eminent creativity (also called "Big-C"), which is reserved for the great. In this paper, the authors propose a Four C model of creativity that expands this dichotomy. Specifically, the authors add the idea of "mini-c," creativity inherent in the learning process, and Pro-c, the developmental and effortful progression beyond little-c that represents professional-level expertise in any creative area. The authors include different transitions and gradations of these four dimensions of creativity, and then discuss advantages and examples of the Four C Model.
Article
Albert Einstein is often considered one of the greatest creators of the 20th century. Indeed, he is frequently viewed as a prototypical example of creative genius. Yet what was the psychological basis of his creativity? In particular, consider the following three issues: Samuel Johnson (1781, p. 5), the author of the first English dictionary, claimed that “the true Genius is a mind of large general powers, accidentally determined to some particular direction.” In other words, creativity may consist of a generalized information processing capacity that may be channeled to almost any endeavor. Does this statement hold for Einstein? Could he have become a Picasso or a Stravinsky had his childhood experiences only directed him toward art or music rather than toward science? Or did Einstein possess a more specialized ability that would not have served him well had he ventured outside of theoretical physics? Whether Einstein's creative powers were general or specific, where did his capacity originate? Was Einstein's creativity an innate ability, as expressed by John Dryden's (1693/1885, p. 60) famous remark that “genius must be born, and never can be taught”? Or was the capacity slowly and arduously acquired through education, practice, and training? Was Einstein merely the most expert and competent theoretical physicist of his day? […]
Article
The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT) was developed in 1966 and renormed five times: in 1974, 1984, 1990, 1998, and 2008. The total sample for all six normative samples included 272,599 kindergarten through 12th grade students and adults. Analysis of the normative data showed that creative thinking scores remained static or decreased, starting at sixth grade. Results also indicated that since 1990, even as IQ scores have risen, creative thinking scores have significantly decreased. The decrease for kindergartners through third graders was the most significant.
Article
Here is a myth-shattering look at the most important and misunderstood aspect of a child's development: creativity. Based on almost fifteen years of research with hundreds of children, "Growing Up Creative" answers the questions parents and teachers ask most frequently: What is creativity? How is it different from talent or intelligence? And why is it so important? "Growing Up Creative" is filled with dozens of concrete, hands-on exercises and techniques that can help a parent or teacher keep creativity alive at home and at school. Rich with anecdotes and interviews, and drawing upon what we know of the childhoods of some of our most cherished and celebrated musicians, writers, and artists, this is state-of-the-art research sure to be embraced by any adult wanting to help a child be all he or she can be happily, healthily, and creatively. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)