PreprintPDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Does ethnic diversity erode social trust? Continued immigration and corresponding growing ethnic diversity have prompted this essential question for modern societies, but few clear answers have been reached in the sprawling literature. Taking this as point of departure, this article reviews the existing literature on the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust through a narrative review and a meta-analysis of 1,001 estimates from 87 studies. The review clarifies the core concepts, highlights pertinent debates, and tests core claims from the literature on the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust. Several results stand out from the meta-analysis. We find a statistically significant negative relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust across all studies. The relationship is stronger for trust in neighbors, and when studied in more local contexts. Covariate conditioning generally changes the relationship only slightly. The review concludes by discussing avenues for future research.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Ethnic Diversity and Social Trust: A Narrative and Meta-Analytical Review
Peter Thisted Dinesen (University of Copenhagen)
Merlin Schaeffer (University of Copenhagen)
Kim Mannemar Sønderskov (Aarhus University)
Preprint version. Final version is forthcoming in Annual Review of Political Science, Volume 23, 2020.
Abstract
Does ethnic diversity erode social trust? Continued immigration and corresponding growing ethnic diversity
have prompted this essential question for modern societies, but few clear answers have been reached in the
sprawling literature. Taking this as point of departure, this article reviews the existing literature on the
relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust through a narrative review and a meta-analysis of 1,001
estimates from 87 studies. The review clarifies the core concepts, highlights pertinent debates, and tests core
claims from the literature on the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust. Several results stand
out from the meta-analysis. We find a statistically significant negative relationship between ethnic diversity
and social trust across all studies. The relationship is stronger for trust in neighbors, and when studied in
more local contexts. Covariate conditioning generally changes the relationship only slightly. The review
concludes by discussing avenues for future research.
Keywords: Ethnic diversity; Social trust; Immigration; Meta-analysis; Context.
1
Introduction
Does ethnic diversity erode social trust? In essence, the relationship between ethnic diversity and trust is the
question of whether the positive interpersonal ties that make for socially cohesive societies can be preserved
when societies’ inhabitants to a decreasing extent share a common ethnic background. The answer to this
question is crucial for understanding the potential challenges that most developed societies are facing from
increasing ethnic diversity stemming from immigration and refugee settlement. It also provides a potential
explanation for the challenges to governance in countries that have historically been ethnically
heterogeneous (Alesina et al. 1999; Alesina & Glaeser 2004). Further, because social trust stimulates
cooperation between individuals (Gächter et al. 2004), the link between ethnic diversity and trust provides a
plausible answer to why ethnic diversity has been found to inhibit the enactment of redistributive welfare
policies (Alesina et al. 1999; Alesina & Glaeser 2004).
The link between ethnic diversity and social trust has been studied extensively over the past almost 25 years
spawning a plethora of different findings. As highlighted in recent reviews of related outcomes, the evidence
on the relationship between ethnic diversity and trust is far from conclusive (Dinesen & Sønderskov 2018;
Schaeffer 2014: Ch. 2; van der Meer & Tolsma 2014). Therefore, to gauge the major insights that this line of
work has produced, we systematize the literature in a narrative review combined with a quantification of key
overall patterns in the literature through a meta-analysis. Previous reviews of trust and the related wider
phenomena of social cohesion and social capital, have either been purely narrative (Dinesen & Sønderskov
2018; Koopmans et al. 2015; Morales 2013; Portes & Vickstrom 2010), or quantified results using crude
“counting strategies” (i.e. tallying significant relationships) (Schaeffer 2014: Ch. 2; van der Meer & Tolsma
2014), which might overlook more subtle aggregate patterns. Given the mature state of the literature, the
logical next step is to conduct a proper meta-analysis directed at quantifying the overall relationship based
on reported estimated coefficients and associated uncertainty, as well as breaking this down by theoretically
pertinent categories.
In the following, we first clarify the core concepts before pinpointing three essential debates in the literature.
Then, based on the results from the meta-analysis, we highlight key findings in the literature. Lastly, we
conclude the review by discussing avenues for future research on the relationship between ethnic diversity
and social trust.
Conceptual clarification
Given the often relatively loose use of the core concepts of the reviewsocial trust and ethnic diversityit
is important to clarify how we understand them, as this carries implications for the specific mechanisms
stipulated in various theoretical accounts.
Social trust refers to trust in human targets, but the specific targets vary. Here, we focus on four conceptually
and empirically distinct forms of social trust (Freitag & Bauer 2013). First, we look at trust in strangers, also
referred to as generalized social trust. Due to its positive effects on cooperation between strangers (Gächter
et al. 2004), some argue that this is the most important form of social trust in modern societies that are
characterized by a large number of interactions between strangers (Dinesen et al. 2019a; Sønderskov 2011).
We also examine out-group trust, which refers to trust in members of salient ethnic out-groups (as defined
below). This form of trust is akin to measures of out-group sentiments, and can be viewed as an extension of
these (or vice versa). Similarly, we also analyze in-group trust, which is again based on salient social
distinctions (e.g. trust in co-ethnics or trust in fellow natives). As with other group sentiments, out-group and
in-group trust are notat least conceptuallymirror images (Brewer 1999). Trust in neighbors is trust in
people with whom one shares a residential environment, and thus differs from the other forms of trust by
2
being geographically bounded. This form of trust is therefore particularly relevant when ethnic diversity is
studied in local residential contexts.
Ethnic diversity can be conceptualized both broadly and narrowly. The narrow conception focuses strictly on
ethnic diversity per se understood as ethnic fragmentation (fractionalization)that is, the composition of a
given context as a function of the number and size of different ethnic groups (Page 2008; Koopmans &
Schaeffer 2015). However, most work on the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust
conceptualizes ethnic diversity more broadly (Hewstone 2015). This line of work essentially uses ethnic
diversity as an umbrella term connoting different aspects of the ethnic composition of a given setting,
including ethnic diversity (fractionalization) per se, but also concentration or polarization of ethnic groups.
The broader usage probably stems from the fact that in most developed countriesthe primary settings of
the debate about the relationship between ethnic diversity and trustthese different phenomena tend to
overlap to a very considerable extent (Schaeffer 2013), and are therefore used synonymously. To align with
most previous studies, and provide a more comprehensive overlook of the literature, we employ this broader
conception of ethnic diversity. Yet, we acknowledge that this distinction can be consequential for the
conclusion reachede.g. through the choice of analytical strategiesas we discuss further below.
Also pertaining to the conceptualization of ethnic diversity is the fundamental question of what constitutes
ethnicity. We approach this pragmatically by employing a relatively broad definition in line with common
usage in the literature. More specifically, following Weber, we define ‘“ethnic groups” as those human groups
that entertain a subjective belief in their common descent’ (Weber 1987 [1922]: 389), which may also entail
a shared language, religion, nationality, and phenotype. Accordingly, our definition of ethnic diversity refers
to ethnic, linguistic, religious, national, and phenotypic diversity.
People may experience ethnic diversity in different social contexts, including in residential settings,
workplaces, schools, and voluntary associations. Yet, the vast majority of studies have focused on residential
settings, which therefore form the cornerstone of the review (the meta-analysis is restricted to this subset
of studies). We conceptualize “residential context” very broadly including local neighborhoods, but also
residential contexts understood in a more aggregate sense, including municipalities, metropolitan areas,
regions, and countries.
Key debates in the literature
We structure our review of the literature around three key debates, each relating to pertinent theoretical
and methodological questions regarding the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust. From each
debate, we derive testable implications that we subsequently assess empirically in the meta-analysis.
Debate 1: Why does ethnic diversity erode trust?
A range of related theoretical accounts have been put forward to explain the proposed negative relationship
between ethnic diversity and the various types of social trust. Clearly distinguishing between the different
accounts is complicated by the fact that they differ in scope and specificity. To be faithful to the initial
theorizations, we present the various accounts in relation to the specific types of trust they purport to
explain, but at the same time seek to draw common threads.
As a common basis, most accounts implicitly or explicitly assume that people partly infer the trustworthiness
of others based on cues from their local environment, including the ethnic background of other people they
encounter in this context (Glanville & Paxton 2007; Ross, Mirowsky & Pribesh 2001). Further, it is often
argued that greater proximity to interethnic out-groupsi.e. when ethnic diversity is experienced more
locally and members of ethnic out-groups therefore are more directly visibleis more consequential for
social trust (Dinesen & Sønderskov 2015).
3
One account posits that mere exposure to people of different ethnic background erodes social trust (Dinesen
& Sønderskov 2015). This approach does not impose any assumptions regarding the mode or form of
interaction between people in a given context. It is simply “being around” interethnic others that is proposed
to influence trust, although this influence might be accentuated or mitigated by specific forms of interactions
(e.g. competition or positive contact). This account builds on the assumption that people display
heterogeneity or out-group aversion (Alesina & Ferrara 2002; Olson et al. 2005). That is, they trust those who
are different from themselves less, due to similarity being an indicator of shared norms and other behavior-
regulating features relevant for trust. By implication, because ethnicity is oneoften highly visiblecue of
similarity, social trust is predicted to be lower in ethnically diverse settings, where such cues are more
frequent. The out-group aversion account has primarily been applied to explain trust in neighbors and
generalized social trust. It applies straightforwardly to the former—at least when diversity is measured
(relatively) locally. When one’s environment (neighborhood) is composed of more ethnically dissimilar
people, who one trusts less, this lowers the trust in the average neighbor. Yet, this mechanism may also
extend to generalized social trust. Because people evaluate the trustworthiness of the generalized other
partly based on what they experience locally (Glanville & Paxton 2007), exposure to more members of ethnic
out-groups in these surroundingswhom they tend to trust lessimplies a larger dose of negative cues
regarding the trustworthiness of others in general (Dinesen & Sønderskov 2015; 2018). However, since the
environmental link is weaker, so might the association between ethnic diversity and generalized trust be.
Given its general and assumption-free character, the “mere exposure” account may also extend to group-
based forms of trust. However, the link between ethnic diversity and these forms of trust haspresumably
due to conceptual proximity to group-based attitudes more generallybeen explained with reference to
theories of group threat and realistic conflict (sometimes labelled “conflict theory” in work on trust). Beyond
mere exposure to ethnic out-groups, these theories underline group competitiontypically over material
interests, but potentially also over symbolic onesas the driving mechanism (Blalock 1967; Blumer 1958). In
its weaker variant, this account posits that group competition lowers out-group trust as a manifestation of
out-group hostility. Stronger versions of this account additionally predict that in-group trust also increases as
a function of being surrounded by more ethnic out-groups, thus implying an inverse relationship between
trust in ethnic out- and in-groups (Brewer & Miller 1984). While the connection to out-group trustand
potentially also in-group trustis obvious, group threat accounts less straightforwardly apply to generalized
social trust and trust in neighbors, which are evaluations of aggregates of people without a specific ethnic
group component (Dinesen et al. 2019a).
In his much discussed “constrict theory”, Putnam (2007) presented an account explaining why ethnic
diversity may erode social trust, independent of the specific target. This is premised on the idea that ethnic
diversity leads to social isolation. That isusing Putnam’s famous metaphorpeople “hunker down” in more
ethnically diverse areas. Because ethnic diversity is expected to induce such general anomie, this mechanism
predicts that ethnic diversity lowers social trust of all types, including both out- and in-group trust. As such,
it is the most daring and wide-ranging account suggested to link ethnic diversity and social trust.
Yet, the generality of Putnam’s “constrict theory” comes at the cost of specificity regarding the mechanisms
explaining exactly how ethnic diversity brings about anomie. Some authors have therefore triedinspired
by related fields studying public goods production (e.g., Habyarimana et al. 2007; Page 2008) or crime
(Sampson et al. 1997)to flesh out potential (sub-)mechanisms explaining anomie. Schaeffer (2013) and
Koopmans et al. (2015) synthesize three. First, as a consequence of people’s inherent preference to interact
with people like themselves (i.e. to display homophily) (Lazarsfeld & Merton 1954), ethnically diverse settings
might be less socially integrated (e.g. in terms of density of acquaintanceship and friendship networks). This
reduces the potential for sanctioning freeriders and limits the flow of information, which both lay the
4
foundation for trusting others. Second, ethnic diversity might result in preference diversity (i.e., fewer shared
collective goals), thereby lowering people’s expectations that collective endeavors are possible, but also
creating incentives to manipulate process and agenda (Page 2008). Both set people further apart. Third,
ethnic diversity might inhibit communicationand ultimately coordinationdue to linguistic and cultural
differences, which makes trusting others more risky. Importantly, other people who live in such disintegrated
environments are considered less trustworthy, irrespective of whether they are in- or out-group members
themselves, because their behavior is not constrained by the social structure in the local environment. These
inferences mayin an attenuated formextend beyond the local area to trust in specific groups as well as
to trust in other people more generally. Empirically, authors have tried to adjudicate between these sub-
mechanisms by comparing the explanatory power of diversity indices capturing different types of ethnicity
(e.g. linguistic or phenotypic) (Lancee & Dronkers 2011; Leigh 2006) or different types of diversity (e.g.
concentration or polarization measures) (Koopmans & Schaeffer 2015; Schaeffer 2013), both purported to
correspond with specific mechanisms. However, this approach may come with the problem of these ethnic
diversity measures being highly correlated, which makes it difficult to distinguish between them empirically
(Schaeffer 2013).
Testable implications. The various theories often yield parallel predictions and it is therefore challenging to
adjudicate their relative explanatory power. Yet, we can partly address their leverage indirectly in a number
of ways in the meta-analysis. First, we can address the general assumption that greater proximity to ethnic
out-groups is more consequential for social trust by comparing the effects of ethnic diversity measured in
context units of different levels of aggregationfrom immediate neighborhoods to the whole country. A
failure to see stronger effects for more local contextswhere we can more safely infer proximity to out-
groupswould challenge this assumption (Dinesen & Sønderskov 2015). Second, by comparing the effect of
ethnic diversity on different types of trust, we can directly assess Putnam’s constrict theory. If the anomie
mechanism stipulated by constrict theory is correct, we should see an across-the-board negative effect on all
forms of social trust (although perhaps not necessarily of the same magnitude). Third, if we observe sparse
or no effect of ethnic diversity on out-group and, to a lesser extent, (a positive effect on) in-group trust,
compared to other forms of trust, this speaks against mechanisms associated with group threat that
specifically predict effects for such group-based forms of trust. By implication, this would also question their
potential application to other forms of social trust. Fourth, while we cannot adjudicate between the various
anomie sub-mechanisms, we can assess the ability to distinguish between them through different diversity
measures by comparing diversity effects in models including, respectively, one and several diversity measures
simultaneously.
Debate 2: Can contact alleviate the negative effect of ethnic diversity?
The majority of work has focused on explaining the negative relationship between ethnic diversity and social
trust. However, a line of research, drawing on “contact theoryfrom work on intergroup relations within
social psychology (Allport 1954; Pettigrew 1998), has suggested that the effect of ethnic diversity may in fact
depend on the type of interactions that occur in ethnically diverse surroundings (e.g. Schmid et al. 2014;
Stolle et al. 2008; Uslaner 2012). More specifically, this perspective makes the distinction between exposure
to and meaningful contact with interethnic others. Whereas “exposure” makes no assumptions about the
type and quality of interactions, “contact” refers to more intimate interactions with out-group members (e.g.
having regular conversations etc.) (Dinesen & Sønderskov 2015).
According to the original formulation of contact theory, meaningful contact with ethnic out-groups may
under certain facilitating conditions (Allport 1954)reduce erroneous negative stereotypes about these
groups, and thereby build positive intergroup relations (Allport 1954; Brown & Hewstone 2005). The segue
to out-group trust is therefore straightforward; positive interethnic interactions may reduce negative
5
stereotypes about ethnic out-groups (the original contact claim), which then translates into higher trust in
these groups (Rudolph & Popp 2010). As far as out-group and in-group trust are each other’s opposites,
interethnic contact may also reduce in-group trust, although the assumption of inverse effects has been put
into question (Brewer 1999; Putnam 2007). The contact mechanism may also predict a positive relationship
between ethnic diversity and trust in neighbors; more positive interactions with interethnic others in a local
context could increase trust in others (neighbors) in this setting. Whether contact effects can extend beyond
that of specific groups to other people in general (and thus to generalized social trust) is questionable given
the relatively restricted circumstances under which the original contact claim has been found to work (Brown
& Hewstone 2005; Dinesen et al. 2019a), but this could be taken as one implication of theories of secondary
transfer (i.e. contact effects extending to out-groups beyond those with whom one has contact) (Hewstone
2015).
Testable implications. Empirically, the contact perspective has been assessed in a number of ways. One
approach, attempting to reconcile the potential positive and negative diversity effects, includes survey
measures of actual interethnic contact (e.g. having friends of different ethnic background) together with the
ethnic diversity measure to examine how this impinges on the relationship between the latter and social
trust. Because ethnically diverse settings also give rise to more interethnic contact (Schmid et al. 2014), this
may lead to countervailing influences of ethnic diversity on social trust; ethnic diversity may have the
hypothesized direct negative impact on social trust, but at the same time stimulate interethnic contact, which
then has a positive effect on trust (Laurence 2011; Schmid et al. 2014; van der Meer & Tolsma 2014). This
implies that we must control for interethnic contact to isolate the (potential) erosive consequences of ethnic
diversity. We assess results from this approach in the meta-analysis. Further, we narratively review evidence
for the role of contact produced by two alternative approaches: examining how interethnic contact
moderates the effect of ethnic diversity on social trust (i), and scrutinizing the link between diversity and
trust in contexts that are particularly “contact-prone” (e.g. schools or workplaces) (ii).
Debate 3: Is ethnic diversity just a placeholder for social disadvantage?
A frequently raised criticism of the ostensible negative relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust
is that ethnic diversity is in reality just a placeholder for social disadvantage and associated aspects such as
crime (Abascal & Baldassari 2015; Sturgis et al. 2011). That is, the apparent negative ethnic diversity effect
on social trust may be confounded by being deprived or marginalized oneself, or by living in a deprived or
crime-ridden context. This criticism is especially pertinent given that the vast majority of existing studies of
the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust exclusively relies on cross-sectional observational
data (for recent exceptions, see Dinesen et al. 2019a; Finseraas et al. 2019). Consequently, the estimated
relationship is vulnerable to both self-selection (i.e. people sorting into ethnic contexts based on their
anteceding level of trust or other factors related to both) and confounding by other features of the context.
An obvious cause of self-selection is sorting based on ethnic background (ethnic minority status is a form of
disadvantage in most contexts); because ethnic and racial minorities are generally less trusting than
majorities a priori (Dinesen & Hooghe, 2010; Smith 2010), and because more minorities per definition live in
more ethnically diverse areas, this may account for the negative relationship between ethnic diversity and
social trust (a so-called compositional effect as opposed to a contextual effect of ethnic diversity). Economic
inequality or other forms of socioeconomic disadvantage in the local area are alternative explanations of
social trust that plausibly also correlate with ethnic diversity, and may therefore confound the relationship
between the two.
Both concerns can in theory be addressed by controlling statistically for potentially confounding factors
social disadvantage or otherwiseat the individual level and the contextual level, in order to obtain the true
diversity effect on trust. This in turn raises the critical question of which covariates to control for. This is
6
challenging to determine because the causal ordering between ethnic diversity and most potential control
variables is rarely well established (of course, for some relatively fixed individual traits, such as ethnic
minority status, this is less of an issue). For example, is ethnic diversity causally prior or posterior to
contextual socioeconomic disadvantage and crime? Or, put differently, are the latter two variables
confounders or mediators of the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust? This issue is perhaps
especially pertinent for crimeafter all, social disorganization theory holds that ethnic diversity is a cause of
crime exactly for some of the reasons stipulated in the anomie argument discussed above (Shaw & McKay
1942; Sampson et al. 1997). This causal indeterminacy plausibly partly explains why existing studies have
inconsistently included control variables, especially at the contextual level.
Testable implications. Given the challenges involved with specifying the correct statistical model, it is
arguably more prudent at this point to examine whether and how various modeling choices influence the
estimated relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust. That is, under the assumption that a
covariate is causally prior to both ethnic diversity and trust, to what extent does it affect their estimated
relationship? In the meta-analysis, we therefore scrutinize how including indicators of the following four
commonly employed classes of control variables shape the connection between ethnic diversity and trust:
individual ethnic and racial minority status (i), socioeconomic status (ii), contextual socioeconomic
deprivation (iii), and contextual crime (iv).
Data and methods
In the following, we briefly describe how we have generated the data used in the meta-analysis, as well as
give a non-technical explanation of the meta-analytical approach applied. In the Online Supplement, we
describe the data generation, the sample and the meta-analytical approach at length. The Online Supplement
also features a list of all studies (including their study-pooled estimate) included in the meta-analysis and
displays results from alternative meta-analytical approaches, as well as tests for publication bias.
Protocol for generating the universe of relevant studies and estimates
The data used in the meta analysis below is collected as a part of a larger project that seeks to systematize
the literature on the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust (Dinesen et al. 2019b). Using the
Web of Knowledge electronic database and keywords like “ethnic diversity” or “racial diversity” and “social
trust” or “social capital”, we identified approximately 4,000 potentially relevant studies. In the meta analysis
below, we use 1,001 quantitative estimates (and associated uncertainty) of the relationship between ethnic
diversity and social trust, originating in 87 studies. The relevant studies and estimates were identified using
a detailed protocol describing the inclusion criteria (e.g. how trust and ethnic diversity are defined, the unit
of analysis etc.). The full protocol is reported in Online Supplement A. As we explain in Online Supplement A,
in this specific analysis we delimit the sample generated by the protocol to studies that focus on the
residential context (i), study individual-level trust (ii), and employ ethnic diversity measures based on
administrative data (as opposed to subjective assessments) (iii). Table D1 in Supplement D lists the included
studies as well as various descriptive information about them, e.g., the number of estimates they each
contribute to the meta-analysis.
The meta-analytical approach
The rationale behind a meta-analysis is to go beyond specific studies and their idiosyncrasies by pooling their
results to generate an overall meta estimate summarizing the effect of a given intervention on a given
outcome. In our case, where we are generally working with non-experimental data, the ambition is, more
modestly, to provide a meta estimate of the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust. In contrast
to two previous related quantitative reviews (Schaeffer 2014; van der Meer & Tolsma 2014), we go beyond
7
simply counting significant relationships. The meta-analysis produces an overall meta estimate based on the
individual studies and weights the included estimates by their uncertainty, thereby giving importance to
more precise results.
Because studies report different types of effect estimates (e.g., linear or logit coefficients) and uncertainty
estimates, we transform them to partial correlations and associated standard errors to establish a common
metric (see Online Supplement C for details). A partial correlation is the correlation between ethnic diversity
and social trust that is statistically adjusted for all other variables contained in the respective regression
model. The partial correlation is bounded between -1 and 1 (perfect negative and positive association,
respectively). Yet, in our application it is likely to be much smaller given that we are examining between-
context variation in trust (potentially stemming from contextual ethnic diversity), which typically only
constitutes around 5 to 10% of the variation in trust. This restricts the potential range of the partial
correlation (see Online Supplement C for further elaboration). To illustrate, in the highly unlikely event where
the entire 10% contextual variation in trust can be attributed to ethnic diversity alone, the partial correlation
coefficient would be
𝜌"#⋅% =
0.1 0.32
and
𝑅/= 0.1
, despite the fact that the association would be
perfect.
To analyze the meta data, we use meta-analytical multilevel random effects models as implemented in the R
metafor package version 2.1-0 (Viechtbauer 2010). The random effects meta-analysis is especially
advantageous for our purposes of examining the systematic variation in the partial correlation across studies
to gauge how features of study design (outcome, context size, specification etc.) influence the overall meta
estimate.
A cardinal assumption of conventional meta-analyses is that each included estimate (partial correlation)
derives from an independent sample. In our case, many estimates come from the same or partly overlapping
samples, thereby leading to dependencies between them. We can partly address this by including random
effects for the data set used (e.g. the European Social Survey) (Konstantopoulos 2011). Moreover, we follow
Card (2015), and apply a two-step procedure. First, we meta-analyze the coefficients of each study, thereby
obtaining an overall meta-estimate per study that we call the study-pooled estimate (reported in Figure 1
below). Second, we then meta-analyze the study-pooled estimates to get the overall meta estimate of the
relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust. For further details on the meta-analytical procedure
applied, how it allows us to investigate the importance of moderating study characteristics (e.g., type of trust
or context size), and how it compares to alternative meta-analytical procedures, see Online Supplements C,
E, F and G.
Empirical evidence
In the following, we report four sets of results from the meta-analysis of previous studies of the relationship
between ethnic diversity and social trust. First, we report an overall meta estimate of the relationship across
all coded studies. Second, we examine how the relationship varies for different types of trust. Third, we
differentiate the effect of ethnic diversity by the size of the context unit in question. Fourth, we examine how
the relationship between diversity and trust is affected by the inclusion of various control variables and/or
mediators.
An overall meta estimate
Figure 1 shows a forest plot of the average study-pooled estimate and associated confidence intervals for
each of the 87 studies coded as well as the overall meta estimate based on all studies.
8
Figure 1: Forest plot of study-pooled estimates of the relationship between ethnic diversity and trust
Note: 87 study-pooled partial correlation coefficients with associated 95% confidence intervals based on 1,001
estimates. The complete bibliography is found in Online Supplement D.
Ha..kansson, Sjo..holm 2007
Gundelach, Freitag 2014
Stolle, Soroka, et al. 2008
Iyer, Kitson, et al. 2005
Öberg, Oskarsson, et al. 2011
Soroka, Johnston, et al. 2007
Koopmans, Veit 2014
Gundelach, Manatschal 2017
Koopmans, Schaeffer 2016
Guest, Kubrin, et al. 2008
Goldschmidt, Hällsten, et al. 2017
Dyck 2012
Gereke, Schaub, et al. 2018
Tolsma, van der Meer 2017
Gustavsson, Jordahl 2007
Dinesen, Sønderskov, et al. 2019
Laurence 2017
Sturgis, BruntonSmith, et al. 2011
Lundåsen, Wollebæk 2013
Laurence 2013
Costa, Kahn 2003
Dinesen, Sønderskov 2015
Koopmans, Schaeffer 2015
de Vroome, Hooghe, et al. 2013
Lancee, Dronkers 2008
Sibley, Duckitt, et al. 2013
Uslaner 2011
Soroka, Helliwell, et al. 2007
Schaeffer 2013
Ivarsflaten, Stømsnes 2013
Laurence 2011
Wollebæk, Lundåsen, et al. 2012
Tolsma, van der Meer, et al. 2009
Leigh 2006
Overall metaanalysis estmiate
Falk, Zehnder 2013
Wu, Hou, et al. 2018
Mendolia, Tosh, et al. 2016
Demireva, Heath 2014
Vidyattama 2017
Putnam 2007
Tatarko, Mironova, et al. 2017
Alesina, La Ferrara 2002
Tesei 2015
Crepaz 2006
Bakker, Dekker 2012
Kokkonen, Esaiasson, et al. 2014
Stolle, Harell 2013
Dinesen 2013
Posel, Hinks 2012
Gundelach, Traunmueller 2014
Lancee, Dronkers 2011
Abascal, Baldassarri 2015
Anderson, Paskeviciute 2006
Dinesen, Sønderskov 2012
Morales, Echazarra 2013
Ziller 2017
Hooghe, Reeskens, et al. 2009
Gijsberts, van der Meer, et al. 2012
Mavridis 2015
Reeskens, Wright 2013
Ziller 2015
Hou, Wu 2009
McShane 2017
Gesthuizen, van der Meer, et al. 2009
Traunmueller 2011
Ariely 2014
Park, Subramanian 2012
Bécares, Stafford, et al. 2011
You 2012
Olson, Li 2015
Stolle, Petermann, et al. 2013
Koster 2013
Lolle, Torpe 2011
Gerritsen, Lubbers 2010
Levels, Scheepers, et al. 2015
Kesler, Bloemraad 2010
Aizlewood, Pendakur 2007
Kasara 2013
Phan 2008
Buzasi 2015
Gundelach 2014
Rudolph, Popp 2010
Robinson 2017
Dingemans, van Ingen 2015
Rothwell 2012
Pendakur, Mata 2012
Marschall, Stolle 2004
0.2 0.1 0.0 0.1
Partial correlation between ethnic diversity and social trust
Overall meta estmiate
Single (studypooled) estimates
9
The most important insight from the forest plot is that, across studies, the overall meta estimate of the
relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust is negative and significantly different from zero; trust
is on average lower in more ethnically diverse contexts. As the figure shows, the overall meta estimate
roughly parallels the study-pooled estimate from several studies focusing on ethnic diversity in relatively
local contexts in a range of developed countries, including the United States (Alesina & La Ferrara 2002;
Putnam 2007), Britain (Laurence 2011), Germany (Schaeffer 2013) and Denmark (Dinesen & Sønderskov
2015). In substantive terms, the partial correlation of -0.0256 (se = 0.0044) between ethnic diversity and trust
is rather modest. Under the scenario of our back-of-the-envelope calculation (see above), it corresponds to
a 0.66% increase in the aggregate between-context unit
𝑅/
after all other variables in the model have been
accounted for (see Online Supplement C).
The forest plot also reveals variation across studies. Most studies report a negative relationship centering
around the reported overall meta estimate. In most, but far from all, cases, the negative estimates are
significantly different from zero. A smaller number of studies report positive relationships, but only a few of
the positive study-pooled estimates are significantly different from zero. Notably, the study with the highest
positive estimate is based on a rather idiosyncratic sample, namely Marschall & Stolle’s (2004) study of racial
context and generalized social trust in Detroit in the 1970’s. Overall, this indicates that ethnic diversity in
residential settings does not lead to “contact effects” under general circumstances.
Taken together, the meta-analytical evidence thus suggests a negative relationship between ethnic diversity
and trust. Compared to (some) previous reviews, which have been inconclusive (van der Meer & Tolsma
2014), we can thus more firmly conclude that ethnic diversity is negatively associated with social trust,
thereby highlighting the benefit of the meta-analytical approach. We now turn to the analyses differentiating
the estimated relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust by various features of the analysis.
Type of trust
Figure 2 shows the overall meta estimate of the relationship between ethnic diversity subset by the specific
type of social trust analyzed. In addition to the four types of trust defined abovegeneralized social trust,
out-group trust, in-group trust, and trust in neighborswe also add a residual “other” category, which
contain types of social trust (as defined in the protocol) not covered by the other targets, including composite
scales mixing different types of social trust. Generally for Figure 2-4, the black dots refer to the meta estimate
for a given subset (Figure 2: type of social trust) with associated confidence intervals. The grey dots and the
associated confidence intervals indicate whether the study-pooled estimate for a given subset is significantly
different from the reference category (Figure 2: generalized social trust).
10
Figure 2: Meta estimates of the relationship between ethnic diversity and different types of social trust
Note: Meta estimates (partial correlation coefficients) with associated 95% confidence intervals
based on 1,001 estimates reported in 87 studies.
Several interesting findings emerge from Figure 2. First, we observe that the relationship between ethnic
diversity and social trust is negative across all types of trust, although in one case (out-group trust) not
statistically distinguishable from zero at the .05-level. Yet, there is very substantial variation in the
relationship between different trust targets. The strongest correlation is found for trust in neighbors,
followed by the residual category, in-group trust, and generalized social trust. All of these estimates are
statistically significant. There is thus robust evidence for a negative relationship with ethnic diversity for these
types of social trust. The weakest relationship is that between ethnic diversity and out-group trust, which is
also insignificant as noted. The estimate for trust in neighbors is roughly double, and significantly different
from the estimate for generalized social trust, and almost three times stronger than (and significantly
different from) the estimate for out-group trust. The stronger negative relationship observed for trust in
neighbors matches what has been found in previous “counting-based” meta-analyses focusing on social
cohesion more broadly (Schaeffer 2014; van der Meer & Tolsma 2014). In contrast, the negative relationship
found for generalized social trust has not been detected in these analyses, thus highlighting the benefits of
our meta-analytical strategy for uncovering weaker relationships.
Implications for the literature. The most obvious insight from Figure 2 is that differentiating by type of trust
is important for understanding the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust. The different types
of trust are not only conceptually distinct, but also display different empirical relationships with ethnic
diversity. Broad usage of the term “social trust” is therefore unhelpful and scholars should clearly specify the
type of trust in question.
The consistent pattern of negative relationships with ethnic diversity across types of social trust supports
Putnam’s (2007) anomie (social isolation) mechanism predicting a universal decline in trust of all types in
ethnically diverse surroundings. Yet, given the pronounced variation in the strength of this relationship
between types of trust, the anomie argument must be supplemented with auxiliary theoretical arguments
explaining why ethnic diversity matters more for some forms of social trust than for others.
Outgroup
Generalized
(Reference)
Ingroup
Other
Neighbors
0.05 0.04 0.03 0.02 0.01 0.00
Partial correlation between ethnic diversity and social trust
Type of trust
Predictions
Estimates
11
There is little evidence for theories stressing group threat as a consequence of intergroup competition (see
also Koopmans & Schaeffer 2015). As noted earlier, these group-based theories apply most straightforwardly
to out- and in-group trust. Weak versions of such theories predict a negative relationship between ethnic
diversity and out-group trust, whereas stronger versions in addition predict a positive relationship between
ethnic diversity and in-group trust. The results from the meta-analysisan insignificant negative relationship
for out-group trust and a negative relationship for in-group trustclearly runs counter to these predictions.
The limited and even contrary evidence vis-à-vis the primary outcomes predicted by group-based theories
suggest that they are of limited value in explaining non-group-based forms of trust, including trust in
neighbors and trust in people in general.
Lastly, a plausible interpretation of the stronger relationship between ethnic diversity and trust in neighbors
than for generalized social trust is that exposure to ethnically dissimilar others is a stronger and more directly
relevant cue for trust in ones neighbors than for trust in other people in general.
Empirical illustrations. A negative relationship between ethnic diversity and trust in neighbors is one of the
most consistent findings in the literatureunsurprisingly, given the target of trust, primarily when studied in
more local contexts (see the next section). This has been found in a range of countries, including the United
States (Putnam 2007), Britain (Sturgis et al. 2011; Laurence 2011; 2013; 2016), Spain (Morales & Echazarra
2013), Germany (Schaeffer 2013; Gundelach & Freitag 2014), the Netherlands (Tolsma & van der Meer 2017),
and Sweden (Lundåsen & Wollebæk 2013). The negative relationship for generalized social trust has been
confirmed in the United States (Alesina & Ferrara 2002; Putnam 2007), Australia (Leigh 2006), Norway
(Ivarsflaten & Strømsnes 2013), and Denmark (Dinesen & Sønderskov 2012; 2015), with dissenting results
from Britain (Sturgis et al. 2011), the Netherlands (Gijsberts et al. 2012), and Sweden (Wollebæk et al. 2012).
Illustrating the difference in the effect of ethnic diversity for trust in neighbors and generalized social trust,
Sturgis et al. (2011) and Wollebæk et al. (2012) only find significant negative relationships for the former.
Context size
Figure 3 plots the overall meta estimate of the relationship between ethnic diversity and trust broken down
by the specific context unit in which ethnic diversity is measured. This ranges from the immediate local
context (the neighborhood) over more aggregate local contexts (municipalities/regions) to countries as a
whole.
12
Figure 3: Meta estimates of the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust
for different context sizes
Note: Meta estimates (partial correlation coefficients) with associated 95% confidence intervals
based on 1,001 estimates reported in 87 studies.
The figure shows negative relationships between ethnic diversity and social trust across contexts of different
sizes. Yet, it also reveals a striking systematic pattern of a stronger negative connection when ethnic diversity
is observed more locally; the strongest estimate is observed when diversity is measured at the neighborhood
level, followed by the relationship at the municipality/regional level, and, finally, the country level. The
estimates for the two more local levels are both statistically significant. The estimate for the country context
is significantly lower than for the neighborhood context, and not significantly different from zero.
Quantitatively, the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust is almost three times stronger when
diversity is measured at the neighborhood level than at the country level.
Implications for the literature. Finding stronger effects of ethnic diversity on social trust in more proximate
environments corroborates the general theoretical assumption that ethnic diversity is more consequential
for trust when out-group members are closer by and therefore more directly visible (Dinesen & Sønderskov
2015).
Empirical illustrations. Representative of the estimates reported in Figure 3, extant studies have relatively
consistently reported a significant negative relationship between neighborhood-level ethnic diversity and
various forms of social trust (primarily for trust in neighbors and, less consistently, for generalized social
trust), including in the United States (Putnam 2007), Britain (Sturgis et al. 2011; Laurence 2011 2013;
Demireva & Heath 2014), New Zealand (Sibley et al. 2013), Spain (Morales & Echazarra 2013, the Netherlands
(Tolsma & van der Meer 2017), Germany (Gundelach & Freitag 2014), Sweden (Lundåsen & Wollebæk 2013)
and Denmark (Dinesen & Sønderskov 2015). Similarly, several studies also find significant negative
relationships between ethnic diversity and trustagain predominantly trust in neighbors and generalized
social trustat more aggregate contextual levels within countries, for example in the United States (Alesina
& Ferrara 2002), Australia (Leigh 2006), Germany (Schaeffer 2013), Sweden (Öberg, Oskarsson & Svensson
2011; Lundåsen & Wollebæk 2013), Norway (Ivarsflaten & Strømsnes 2013), and Denmark (Dinesen &
Country
Municipality/
Region
Neighborhood
(Reference)
0.04 0.03 0.02 0.01 0.00
Partial correlation between ethnic diversity and social trust
Context size
Predictions
Estimates
13
Sønderskov 2012). Studies from the Netherlands (Tolsma et al. 2009; Gijsberts et al. 2012) are (partial)
exceptions to this rule as they mostly find insignificant negative relationships.
Confounding and mediation
Figure 4 shows how the meta estimates of the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust varies
by adjustment for various covariates. The adjusted estimates are compared to zero (the null hypothesis of
no diversity effect) (the black dots) as well as an estimate (line 7) not adjusted for any of the control variables
or mediators reported in the figure (the grey dots). The latter comparison serves to address potential
confounding or mediation. Because some studies include models both with and without a given covariate,
we canas a more rigorous strategy, holding other between-study factors constantcompare estimates
within studies (i.e. using study fixed effects). This study fixed effects estimates are indicated by hollow dots.
Figure 4: Meta estimates of the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust
conditioned on covariates
Note: Meta estimates (partial correlation coefficients) with associated 95% confidence intervals
based on 1,001 estimates reported in 87 studies.
Contact
The estimated relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust is somewhat weaker in models adjusting
for interethnic contact (line 1), but the difference is far from statistically significant. The adjusted estimates
are insensitive to the inclusion of study fixed effects, thus showing that when estimates are compared within
the same study, taking interethnic contact into account changes the effect of ethnic diversity on social trust
very little.
Implications for the literature. Finding that adjusting for interethnic contact does not systematically change
the estimated relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust carries the important implication that
interethnic exposure in a given context and actual interethnic contact are not only conceptually, but also
Contact
Disadvantage
Diversity
predictors
Reference
0.04 0.02 0.00
Interethnic
contact
Individual
minority status
Contextual
crime
Individual
SES status
Contextual
SES deprivation
Several
diversity
predictors
None of
the above
adjustments
Partial correlation between ethnic diversity and social trust
Adjusted for ...
Predictions (No study FE)
Estimates (No study FE)
Estimates (Including study FE)
14
empirically distinct. Consequently, failure to take interethnic contact into account does not strongly impinge
on the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust.
Empirical illustrations. Relatively few studies interested in contact effects sequentially model the relationship
between ethnic diversity and social trust with and without controlling for interethnic contact, and the results
from these analyses are inconsistent (Demireva & Heath 2014, Koopmans & Veit 2014, Laurence 2011).
Additional tests of the role of contact. Beyond considering interethnic contact as a mediator, two other
approaches examine the role of contact. One approach, premised on the idea that the negative ethnic
diversity effect on trust may be dampened when accompanied by interethnic contact, examines how
interethnic contact moderates the effect of ethnic diversity on trust (Stolle et al. 2008). Several studies find
that more contact with out-group members (or, by proxy, neighbors), tends to dampen the negative
relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust (Stolle et al. 2008; Rudolph & Popp 2010; Sturgis et al.
2011, Gundelach & Freitag 2014). While this is an interesting finding, the mirror image is, of course, that the
negative relationship is even stronger for those without contact. Further, this line of work is challenged by
contact being potentially endogenous to trust as well as by the use of imprecise and biased self-assessments
of contact (Dinesen et al. 2019a).
Another approach, moving away from the use of self-assessed contact measures, looks at contact-prone
contexts to examine whether ethnic diversity may matter differently in settingse.g. schools, workplaces or
voluntary associationswhere contact is more likely to emerge and to be of a more repeated nature than in
the frequently studied residential context. Results are generally inconsistent. In the school setting, the results
are scattered (although mostly negative relationships are reported), varying between countries, types of trust
and specific sub-groups (Dinesen 2011; Janmaat 2015). In the associational realm, Dutch studies have found
little or mixed relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust (Achbari et al. 2018; van der Meer 2016).
Two studies scrutinize the relationship between ethnic diversity in the workplace setting and social trust,
using fine-grained registry data from Sweden (Goldschmidt et al. 2017) and Denmark (Dinesen et al. 2019a),
respectively. The former finds negative, but statistically insignificant relationships between ethnic diversity
and various forms of trust, whereas the latter consistently findseven using panel datastatistically
significant negative relationships between ethnic diversity and generalized social trust. Lastly, a field
experiment examines the effect of sharing a room with ethnic minority members during an eight-week recruit
period in the Norwegian army on trust in minority members in subsequent trust games, and finds contact
effects (Finseraas et al. Forthcoming). This is a convincing demonstration that under certain conditions,
contact-prone contexts can stimulate certain forms of trust. However, given the “strong” and idiosyncratic
nature of the treatment, the generalizability of this finding is questionable. Taken together, the evidence
from contexts more contact-prone than the residential setting is thus inconsistent, and more studies of such
extra-residential contexts are warranted to understand how ethnic diversity in all domains of lifecontact-
prone or notshape social trust.
Control variables
Lines 2-5 in Figure 4 show the estimated relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust when
conditioning on four sets of indicators of social disadvantage and related aspects commonly employed as
control variables: individual socioeconomic status (e.g. education or income), individual minority status (e.g.,
being of immigrant origin or member of a racial minority), contextual socioeconomic deprivation (e.g. mean
income or level of unemployment), and contextual crime.
The adjusted estimates after controlling for the four classes of covariates all remain negative and statistically
significant. Thus, controlling for socioeconomic disadvantage and related aspects does not change the overall
negative relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust. This is underlined by the finding that for three
15
out of the four classes of covariates (individual socioeconomic status, contextual socioeconomic deprivation,
and contextual crime), the adjusted estimate is not significantly different from the unadjusted estimate.
Controlling for individual minority status does significantly reduce the estimated relationship between ethnic
diversity and social trust, but the reduction is relatively minor (by about one third in the within-study
estimates), and the adjusted estimate remains statistically significant by a wide margin. The observed
negatively relationship thus first and foremost reflect a contextual effectrather than being a compositional
artifactof ethnic diversity on social trust.
Implications for the literature. Finding that conditioning on four classes of important covariates does not
fundamentally change the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust implies that even if they are
considered strictly exogenous to ethnic diversity, they do not strongly confound the relationship.
Importantly, this indicates that ethnic diversity is not merely a placeholder for individual or contextual
disadvantage, but rather an independent predictor of social trust. That being said, future observational work
should obviously continue to control for indicators of social disadvantageand other potential
confoundersto obtain a more credible estimate of the relationship. However, given the unclear causal
status of many covariates vis-à-vis ethnic diversity (and social trust), researchers must exercise great care in
their interpretation of the (absence of a) relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust based on
models conditioning on such covariates. As a consequence, we strongly advise reporting models both with
and without various covariates to understand how covariate conditioning influences the estimated
relationship.
Empirical illustrations. Almost all studies control for a range of indicators of disadvantage at both the
individual level and the contextual level (e.g., 83% of the estimates are adjusted for minority status, see
Figure D.3 in the online supplement), as well as standard demographic controls (e.g. gender and age at the
individual level). Only a few studies report several models sequentially adding various control variables to
the bivariate model. One example is Sturgis et al. (2011), which report the association between ethnic
diversity and measures of social trust, both bivariately and conditioned on a very rich set of individual and
contextual covariates. Yet, in this case, adding control variables in a stepwise fashion would have been useful
given the unclear causal relationship between several of the controls (e.g. contextual crime or happiness)
and both ethnic diversity and social trust, thus potentially inducing post-treatment bias and/or endogeneity
bias.
Alternative diversity predictors
Line 6 in Figure 4 plots the estimated relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust for studies
simultaneously including different predictors of ethnic diversity, e.g. several measures of ethnic
fragmentation and/or measures of concentration of given ethnic out-groups. The plot shows that the overall
meta estimate for models with several diversity predictors remains significant, although it is significantly (in
the case of the between-study estimate) reduced by about a fifth compared to the overall unadjusted
estimate. Controlling for multiple diversity predictors thus influences the strength of the relationship
between ethnic diversity and social trust, but does not fundamentally change the relationship.
Implications for the literature. The moderate sensitivity of the estimated relationship between ethnic
diversity and trust to controlling for other diversity predictors suggests that one must exercise caution in
testing different (sub)mechanisms linking diversity and trust by simultaneously including different diversity
measures (Schaeffer 2013; Koopmans & Schaeffer 2015; Dinesen & Sønderskov 2015). The fruitfulness of this
approach ultimately depends on the ability to empirically distinguish between the corresponding indicators
in a given setting. This also has subtle, but important implications for the intersection between concept use
using a narrow versus a broad conception of ethnic diversity (i.e. ethnic fragmentation per se or various
16
aspects of ethnic composition more broadly understood)and statistical modeling. Using the narrow
conception of diversity, one would be interested in separating out the specific effect of ethnic fragmentation
from other aspects of the ethnic composition, including ethnic concentration etc. Yet, this can be challenging
in practice as the various aspects of ethnic composition are often highly correlated empirically, and therefore
bound to explain less of the variance in social trust on their own, which could in turn lead to a faulty
conclusion of absence of a relationship between ethnic diversity and trust (or at least an underestimation of
it).
Empirical illustrations. Several studies have pointed out the high correlation between different indicators of
ethnic diversity (broadly conceived) (Koopmans & Schaeffer 2015; Dinesen & Sønderskov 2015), and
Schaeffer (2013) shows that only under rather specific conditions can they be disentangled statistically.
Results from the US illustrate the potential consequences of including several diversity measures
simultaneously. Alesina & La Ferrara (2002) find that when including measures of racial and ethnic
fragmentation, respectively, separately in bi- and multivariate models of generalized social trust, they both
display the expected negative relationship. However, when included simultaneously, the coefficient on
ethnic fragmentation switches signsa likely indication of collinearity (they are correlated at 0.56 according
to Alesina & Ferrara (2000)). This shows that interpreting the effect of one specific diversity measure in
isolation, when including several measures simultaneously, may lead to biased conclusions of the role of
diversity for trust. This is relevant for the study by Abascal & Baldassari (2015), which reanalyzes the data on
which Putnam’s (2007) controversial results were based. In bivariate models, they replicate the negative
relationship between ethnic fragmentation at the census tract level and various forms of trust, but in
multivariate models, controlling for, inter alia, concentration of whites and concentration of US citizens
(presumably roughly the inverse of the share of immigrants) at the census tract level (Putnam himself
includes only the latter), this relationship is no longer significant. Given that both concentration measures
are presumably highly correlated with ethnic fragmentation, this may explain its loss of significance. Further,
in some (more parsimonious) analyses, the concentrations of whites and of US citizens are significantly
positively correlated with various forms of social trust, and given that they both fall within the broader
conception of ethnic diversity as argued above, it is open to interpretation to what extent these results
debunk Putnam’s findings broadly understood. A sequential building of statistical analyses, examining the
internal correlation between various measures of ethnic composition, introducing them separately and,
subsequently, simultaneously, in models of social trust, would be helpful in this regard.
Summary of empirical evidence and providing a “best” estimate
In summary, the meta-analysis has generated several insights speaking to various aspects of the debates in
the literature presented earlier. First, as a baseline result, across all studies, we observe a statistically
significant negative relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust of moderate size. On average, social
trust is thus lower in more ethnically diverse contexts. That being said, the rather modest size of the
relationship also implies that apocalyptic claims regarding the severe threat of ethnic diversity for social trust
in contemporary societies are exaggerated.
Second, the negative relationship applies for all types of trust, but with substantial variation in strength
between types. The negative relationship is strongest for trust in neighbors, intermediate for in-group trust
and generalized social trust, and weakest for out-group trust (for the latter, the relationship is insignificant).
Ethnic diversity thus matters more for trust in people in ones immediate residential setting, but the effect
also extends beyond this setting to trust in other people in general.
Third, ethnic diversity experienced locallyin neighborhoodsmatters more for social trust than does ethnic
diversity in more aggregate settings. Ceteris paribus, this suggests that proximity to interethnic others is an
17
important facilitating condition accentuating the negative relationship between ethnic diversity and social
trust.
Fourth, the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust is only slightly attenuated and remains
negative and significant, when controlling for potential confounders/mediators, specifically individual
minority background, socioeconomic deprivation (individual and contextual), contextual crime, and
interethnic contact. To properly assess confounding and/or mediation, researchers should sequentially add
control variablestapping social disadvantage or otherwisewith ambiguous causal connections to ethnic
diversity in models of social trust.
Fifth, the diversity-trust connection is reduced (but still significant), when controlling for several predictors
of ethnic diversity (conceptualized broadly). At present, including severaltypically highly correlated
diversity predictors to parse out different theoretical mechanisms will often lead to an underestimation of the
effect of ethnic diversity on social trust.
As a logical conclusion of the meta-analysis, it is relevant to ask what is the overall estimate of the relationship
between ethnic diversity and social trust when considering the lessons from the review. Figure E.1 in the
Online Supplement shows a final overall estimate that focuses only on those 132 estimates reported in 26
studies that study trust in neighbors or in the generalized other (i), focus on small-scale neighborhood
contexts (ii) control for individual socio-economic and minority status as well as contextual socio-economic
deprivation (iii), and contain only one single ethnic diversity predictor in their model (iv). Because the causal
roles of interethnic contact and contextual crime remain ambiguous vis-a-vis ethnic diversity, both estimates
from models including and excluding these controls are included in the overall meta estimate. Interestingly,
the result of this “best practice” analysis vis-a-vis trust in neighbors and in the generalized other closely
mirrors our initially reported finding based on the full sample; we observe a highly significant overall meta
partial correlation of -0.0283 (se = 0.0048). Narrowing in on estimates based on more appropriate research
designs again confirms the overall conclusion drawn from the meta-analysis: ethnic diversity displays a
negative relationship with social trust.
Directions for future research
To conclude, we briefly discuss avenues for future research emanating from the insights generated by our
meta-analytical status of the literature.
Theorization and corresponding empirical tests of why ethnic diversity erodes trust
It is fair to say that the literature on the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust has first and
foremost been concerned with producing empirical evidence, and that the proliferation of empirical analyses
has not been matched with equal theoretical rigor. The potential effect of ethnic diversity on trust has mainly
been explained with generic theoretical mechanisms, which are hard to differentiate empirically. As such,
there is certainly a need for more elaborate theorization of why ethnic diversity ostensibly is connected to
lower trust. As alluded to earlier, the literature on related consequences of ethnic diversity has theorized this
link in more detail and could serve as a source of inspiration (e.g. Habayarimana et al. 2007). Similarly,
inspiration could be found in the related literature on contextual effects on related constructs, which has
seen new theoretical developments regarding when and how ethnic context matters (Hopkins 2010; Enos
2017; Legewie & Schaeffer 2016; Danckert et al. 2017).
The issue of theorization is compounded by the twin issue of corresponding empirical testing. As mentioned
above, a common way to tease apart alternative mechanisms is to use different measures of ethnic diversity.
Yet, such diversity indicators are notoriously correlated, and therefore this strategy is difficult to employ in
practice. One way to bypass this problem is using relational (group-based) diversity measures as proposed
18
by Koopmans & Schaeffer (2015). Yet, it would also be productive for scholars to think about alternative ways
of testing proposed mechanisms. One approachparallel to testing the contact argument by focusing on
contact-prone contextsis to test the relationship between ethnic diversity and trust in contexts or under
circumstances, in which one mechanism is more likely to operate than others.
What mitigates the negative effectis there a role for policy?
Parallel to an interest in understanding why ethnic diversity erodes trust, it is relevant to ask howif at all
this negative effect could be mitigated. From the perspective of policymakers, it is especially relevant to
understand which public policies or institutional means that are at their disposal towards curbing the
negative effect. At present, beyond the contact literature mentioned earlier, relatively limited attention has
been paid to this question. Two strands of research addressing this question are worth highlighting. One line
of work, also originating in the contact argument, looks at the role of local ethnic segregation (as opposed to
integration) as a barrier to interethnic contact, which is then found to accentuate the negative relationship
between ethnic diversity and social trust (e.g., Uslaner 2012; Schaeffer 2014). Another line of work looks at
how at how various (local) integration policies moderate the relationship between ethnic diversity and social
trust and have found mixed results (Kesler & Bloemraad 2010; Gundelach & Manatschal 2017). These studies
provide valuable first steps for exploring the role of policy handles that may be used for alleviating the
negative effect of local contextual ethnic diversity on social trust. Future studies could explore these and
related ideas further using stronger designs (the noted studies primarily rely on observational cross-sectional
designs), e.g. utilizing local housing reforms or gradual implementation of integration policies within
countries as sources of quasi-experimental variation in the moderating variable.
Explore the relationship in new contexts
Research on the relationship between ethnic diversity and trust has overwhelmingly focused on the
residential context. While this is in some ways understandablealmost everyone has a residential context
and spend a significant amount of time thereother spheres of life are clearly also potentially relevant.
Further, as noted above, other contexts could structure interethnic interactions in ways so that the
relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust may play out differently. Residential contexts are
arguably more likely to give way to “mere exposure” than actual contact, which would imply that our
conclusions here are disproportionately negative due to our focus on studies of the residential context
(Dinesen et al. 2019a). Researchers are therefore well advised to continue exploring the relationship between
ethnic diversity and social trust in other spheres of life than the residential context. Further, the interaction
between the role of ethnic diversity in different contextse.g., do interactions with interethnic others in one
context mute or enhance those in anotheris also an interesting question for further research.
Increased focus on causal inference
Following from the fact that the vast majority of existing studies are based on observational cross-sectional
data, the detected negative relationship ethnic diversity and social trust cannot be given a causal
interpretation. As we have discussed, most studies control statistically for potential confounders to rule out
some sources of confounding, but this approach comes with its own problems as it is often ambiguous which
covariates to control for.
At this point, it seems sensible for the literature on the relationship between ethnic diversity and trust to
begin applying more sophisticated strategies to strengthen causal identification. Like the study of related
contextual effects (Laurence & Bentley 2016; Danckert et al. 2017), the use of panel data, which allow for
mapping changes in ethnic diversity to changes in social trust, and thereby bypassing all time-invariant
confounding, would be a logical next step with the emergence of more panel data sets with local geographic
identifiers. To our knowledge, only the above-mentioned study by Dinesen et al. (2019a) has applied panel
19
data, replicating the negative cross-sectional relationship between ethnic diversity and generalized social
trust in workplaces in Denmark.
Similarly, and again parallel to other related fields (Enos 2017; Hangartner et al. 2019), the use of field
experimentswhere exposure to ethnic diversity is randomly assigned by the researcherand natural
experimentsin which quasi-random geographic or temporal disjunctions in exposure to diversity is
utilizedare obvious methodological advances pertinent for future research on the relationship between
ethnic diversity and social trust. There are already promising movements in this direction. Most notably, the
above-mentioned field experiment from Norway, which randomized exposure to ethnic minority members
via room sharing during a recruitment period in the army (Finseraas et al. 2019). Another, less demanding,
approach, along the lines of Koopmans & Veit (2014), is to experimentally prime ethnic diversity among
survey respondents to study its effect on trust.
Connecting the micro-level relationship and macro-level temporal patterns
As stated in the introduction, the premise of the apocalyptic claim regarding the negative effect of ethnic
diversity on social trust is that this leads to an erosion of social trust over time as countries diversify. Inferring
from the robust negative relationship between contextual ethnic diversity and social trust detected in our
meta-analysis, we would expect, ceteris paribus, a limited decline in trust over time as countries become
increasingly ethnically diverse due to immigration. There is some evidence for this proposition vis-à-vis
generalized social trust in a sample of European countries (from 2002 to 2012) (Olivera, 2015).
However, over a longer time span and across a broader set of countries, the relationship between ethnic
diversity and generalized social trust appears very heterogeneous with no immediately obvious trend (Ortiz-
Ospina & Roser 2019). Of course, the famous decline in social trust in the United States from the 1960s
onwardsa period of increasing ethnic diversityfits the pattern, but also lends itself to several other
explanations (Putnam 2000). Yet, other countries have experienced marked increases in trust over the last
decades. Perhaps most strikingly, Denmark, a country that has diversified at a considerable pace since 1980,
saw a dramatic increase in generalized social trustfrom 47% trusting others in 1979 to 79% in 2009in this
period. Further, ethnic diversity in neighborhoods, municipalities and workplaces (but not in schools) have
all been found to be negatively related to generalized social trust in Denmark during this period (Dinesen
2011, Dinesen & Sønderskov 2012, 2015; Dinesen, Sønderskov and Thuesen 2019a), thus highlighting the
sometimes dramatically diverging micro-level and over-time macro-level relationships.
One straightforward explanation for the lacking materialization of the negative individual-level relationship
between contextual ethnic diversity and social trust at the aggregate level is that the negative diversity effect
is overridden by other factors more consequential for trust, e.g. well-functioning government institutions
(Sønderskov & Dinesen 2014; Charron & Rothstein 2016). A more subtle possibility is that this is caused by
increased ethnic segregation at the local level, which in turn implies less exposure to ethnic out-groups and
the associated negative consequences for social trust that we have demonstrated. Yet, this could also have
exacerbated the negative effects as stipulated above. Future theoretical and empirical work seeking to
integrate the micro- and macro-level patterns in the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust
would indeed be valuable.
20
References
Abascal M, Baldassarri D. 2015. Love Thy Neighbor? Ethnoracial Diversity and Trust Reexamined. American
Journal of Sociology. 121(3): 72282.
Achbari W, Gesthuizen M, Holm J. 2018. Ethnic diversity and generalized trust: Testing the contact hypothesis
in Dutch voluntary organizations. Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly. 47(4): 81335.
Alesina A, Baqir R, Easterly W. 1999. Public goods and ethnic divisions. The Quarterly Journal of Economics.
114(4): 124384.
Alesina A, La Ferrara E. 2000. Participation in heterogeneous communities. The Quarterly Journal of
Economics. 115(3): 847-904.
Alesina A, La Ferrara E. 2002. Who trusts others? Journal of Public Economics. 85(2): 20734.
Alesina A, Glaeser E. 2004. Fighting Poverty in the US and Europe: A World of Difference. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Allport GW. 1954. The Nature of Prejudice. New York: Basic Books.
Blalock HM. 1967. Toward a Theory of Minority-Group Relations. New York: Wiley
Blumer H 1958. Race prejudice as a sense of group position. Pacific Sociological Review. 1: 37.
Bobo L, Hutchings VL. 1996 Perceptions of racial group competition: Extending Blumer's theory of group
position to a multiracial social context. American Sociological Review. 61: 95172.
Brewer MB. 1999 The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love and outgroup hate? Journal of Social Issues. 55(3):
42944.
Brewer MB, Miller N. 1984. Beyond the Contact Hypothesis: Theoretical Perspectives on Desegregation. In
Groups in Contact: The Psychology of Desegregation, ed. N Miller, MB Brewer, pp. 281302. New York:
Halstead Press.
Brown R, Hewstone M. 2005. An integrative theory of intergroup contact. Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology. 37: 255343.
Card NA. 2015. Applied Meta-Analysis for Social Science Research. London: Guilford Publications.
Charron N, Rothstein B. 2016. Social Trust, Quality of Government and Ethnic Diversity. An Empirical Analysis
of 206 Regions in Europe. QoG Working Paper Series 2014:20.
Danckert B, Dinesen PT, Sønderskov KM. 2017. Reacting to neighborhood cues? Political sophistication
moderates the effect of exposure to immigrants. Public Opinion Quarterly. 81(1): 3756.
Demireva N, Heath A. 2014. Diversity and the Civic Spirit in British Neighbourhoods: An Investigation with
MCDS and EMBES 2010 Data. Sociology. 48: 64362.
Dinesen PT, Sønderskov KM. 2012. Trust in a Time of Increasing Diversity: On the Relationship between Ethnic
Heterogeneity and Social Trust in Denmark from 1979 until Today. Scandinavian Political Studies. 35(4):
27394.
Dinesen PT. 2011. Me and Jasmina down by the schoolyard: An analysis of the impact of ethnic diversity in
school on the trust of schoolchildren. Social Science Research. 40(2): 572585.
Dinesen PT, Hooghe M. 2010. When in Rome, Do as the Romans Do: The Acculturation of Generalized Trust
among Immigrants in Western Europe. International Migration Review. 44(3): 697727.
21
Dinesen PT, Sønderskov KM. 2015. Ethnic Diversity and Social Trust: Evidence from the Micro-Context.
American Sociological Review. 80(3): 55073.
Dinesen PT, Sønderskov KM. 2018. Ethnic Diversity and Social Trust: A Critical Review of the Literature and
Suggestions for a Research Agenda. In The Oxford Handbook on Social and Political Trust, ed. EM Uslaner,
pp. 175204. New York: Oxford University Press.
Dinesen PT, Sønderskov KM, Thuesen F. 2019a. Working Together? Ethnic Diversity in the Workplace and
Generalized Social Trust. Working Paper.
Dinesen PT, Schaeffer M, Sønderskov KM. 2019b. Ethnic Diversity and Social Trust - A Meta-Analysis.
Retrieved from osf.io/aecs2.
Enos RD. 2017. The Space Between Us: Social Geography and Politics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Finseraas H, Hanson T, Johnsen Å, Kotsadam A, Torsvik G. 2019. Trust, ethnic diversity, and personal contact:
A field experiment. Journal of Public Economics. 173: 7284.
Freitag M, Bauer PC. 2013. Testing for Measurement Equivalence in Surveys: Dimensions of Social Trust Across
Cultural Contexts. Public Opinion Quarterly. 77(S1): 2444.
Gjisberts M, van der Meer T, Dagevos J. 2012. ‘Hunkering Down’ in Multi-Ethnic Neighbourhoods? The Effects
of Ethnic Diversity on Dimensions of Social Cohesion. European Sociological Review. 28(4): 52737.
Glanville JL, Paxton P. 2007. How do we learn to trust? A confirmatory tetrad analysis of the sources of
generalized trust. Social Psychology Quarterly. 70(3): 23042.
Goldschmidt T, Hällsten M, Rydgren J. 2017. Are they hunkering down? Revisiting the relationship between
exposure to ethnic diversity, intergroup contact, and group trust. Working Paper.
Gundelach B, Freitag M. 2014. Neighbourhood diversity and social trust: An empirical analysis of interethnic
contact and group-specific effect. Urban Studies. 51(6): 123656.
Gundelach B, Manatschal A. 2017. Ethnic Diversity, Social Trust and the Moderating Role of Subnational
Integration Policy. Political Studies. 65(2): 41331.
Gächter S, Herrmann B., Thöni C. 2004. Trust, voluntary cooperation, and socio-economic background: survey
and experimental evidence. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization. 55(4): 50531.
Habyarimana J, Humphreys M, Posner DN, Weinstein JM. 2007. Why does ethnic diversity undermine public
goods provision?. American Political Science Review. 101(4): 70925.
Hangartner D, Dinas E, Marbach M, Matakos K, Xefteris D. 2019. Does exposure to the refugee crisis make
natives more hostile?. American Political Science Review. 113(2): 44255.
Hewstone M. 2015. Consequences of diversity for social cohesion and prejudice: The missing dimension of
intergroup contact. Journal of Social Issues. 71(2): 41738.
Hopkins DJ. 2010. Politicized places: Explaining where and when immigrants provoke local opposition.
American Political Science Review. 104(1): 4060.
Ivarsflaten E, Strømsnes K. 2013. Inequality, Diversity and Social Trust in Norwegian Communities. Journal of
Elections, Public Opinion and Parties. 23(3): 32242.
Janmaat JG. 2015. School ethnic diversity and White students’ civic attitudes in England. Social Science
Research. 49: 97109.
22
Kesler C, Bloemraad, I. 2010. Does Immigration Erode Social Capital? The Conditional Effects of Immigration-
Generated Diversity on Trust, Membership, and Participation across 19 Countries, 1981-2000. Canadian
Journal of Political Science. 43(2): 31947.
Konstantopoulos S. 2011. Fixed effects and variance components estimation in three-level meta-analysis.
Research Synthesis Methods. 2(1): 6176.
Koopmans R, Lancee B, Schaeffer M. 2015. Ethnic diversity in diverse societies. An introduction. In Social
Cohesion and Immigration in Europe and North America: Mechanisms, Conditions, and Causality, ed. R
Koopmans, B Lancee, M Schaeffer, pp. 120. New York: Routledge.
Koopmans R, Schaeffer M. 2015. Relational diversity and neighbourhood cohesion. Unpacking variety,
balance and in-group size. Social Science Research. 53: 16276.
Koopmans R, Veit S. 2014. Ethnic Diversity, Trust, and the Mediating Role of Positive and Negative Interethnic
Contact: A Priming Experiment. Social Science Research. 47: 91107.
Lancee B, Dronkers J. 2011. Ethnic, Religious and Economic Diversity in Dutch Neighbourhoods: Explaining
Quality of Contact with Neighbours, Trust in the Neighbourhood and Inter-ethnic Trust. Journal of Ethnic
and Migration Studies. 37(4): 597618.
Laurence J. 2011. The Effect of Ethnic Diversity and Community Disadvantage on Social Cohesion: A Multi-
level Analysis of Social Capital and Interethnic Relations in UK Communities. European Sociological Review.
27(1): 7089.
Laurence J. 2013. Hunkering Down or Hunkering Away.? The Effect of Community Ethnic Diversity on
Residents Social Networks. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties. 23(3): 25578.
Laurence J. 2017. Wider-community Segregation and the Effect of Neighbourhood Ethnic Diversity on Social
Capital: An Investigation into Intra-Neighbourhood Trust in Great Britain and London. Sociology. 51(5):
101133.
Laurence J, Bentley L. 2016. Does ethnic diversity have a negative effect on attitudes towards the community?
A longitudinal analysis of the causal claims within the ethnic diversity and social cohesion debate. European
Sociological Review. 32: 5467.
Lazarsfeld PF, Merton RK 1954. Friendship as a social process: A substantive and methodological analysis.
Freedom and Control in Modern Society. 18(1): 1866.
Legewie J, Schaeffer M. 2016. Contested boundaries: Explaining where ethnoracial diversity provokes
neighborhood conflict. American Journal of Sociology. 122(1): 12561.
Leigh A. 2006. Trust, inequality and ethnic heterogeneity. Economic Record. 82: 26880.
Lundåsen SW, Wollebæk D. 2013. Diversity and Community Trust in Swedish Local Communities. Journal of
Elections, Public Opinion and Parties. 23(3): 299321.
Marschall MJ, Stolle D. 2004. Race and the City: Neighborhood Context and the Development of Generalized
Trust. Political Behavior. 26(2): 12553.
Morales L. 2013. Assessing the Effects of Immigration and Diversity in Europe: Introduction to the Special
Issue. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion & Parties. 23(3): 24154.
Morales L, Echazarra A. 2013. Will We All Hunker Down? The Impact of Immigration and Diversity on Local
Communities in Spain. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties. 23(3): 34366.
23
Olsson A, Ebert JP, Banaji MR, Phelps EA. 2005. The role of social groups in the persistence of learned fear.
Science. 309(5735): 7857.
Ortiz-Ospina, E, Roser M. 2019. Trust. https://ourworldindata.org/trust. Accessed August 21, 2019.
Page SE. 2008. The Difference: How the Power of Diversity Creates Better Groups, Firms, Schools, and
Societies. Princeton: Princeton University Press. New Edition.
Pettigrew TF. 1998. Intergroup Contact Theory. Annual Review of Psychology. 49: 6585.
Portes A, Vickstrom E. 2011. Diversity, Social Capital, and Cohesion. Annual Review of Sociology. 37: 46179.
Putnam RD. 2000. Bowling Alone: The Collapse and Revival of American Community. New York: Simon &
Schuster Paperbacks.
Putnam RD. 2007. E pluribus unum: Diversity and community in the twenty-first century the 2006 Johan Skytte
Prize Lecture. Scandinavian Political Studies. 30: 13774.
Quillian L. 1995. Prejudice as a response to perceived group threat: Population composition and anti-
immigrant and racial prejudice in Europe. American Sociological Review. 60: 586611.
Ross CE, Mirowsky J, Pribesh S. 2001. Powerlessness and the amplification of threat: Neighborhood
disadvantage, disorder, and mistrust. American Sociological Review. 66: 56891.
Rudolph T, Popp E. 2010. Race, Environment, and Interracial Trust. Journal of Politics. 72(1): 7489.
Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW, Earls F. 1997. Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective
efficacy. Science. 277(5328): 91824.
Schaeffer M. 2013. Can competing diversity indices inform us about why ethnic diversity erodes social
cohesion? A test of five diversity indices in Germany. Social Science Research. 42(3): 75574.
Schaeffer M. 2014. Ethnic diversity and social cohesion: Immigration, ethnic fractionalization and potentials
for civic action. Aldershot: Ashgate.
Schmid K, Ramiah AA, Hewstone M. 2014. Neighborhood Ethnic Diversity and Trust The Role of Intergroup
Contact and Perceived Threat. Psychological Science 25(3): 66574.
Shaw CR, McKay HD. 1942. Juvenile delinquency and urban areas. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Sibley CG, Duckitt J, Bergh R, Osborne D, Perry, R et al. 2013. A Dual Process Model of Attitudes towards
Immigration: Person × Residential Area Effects in a National Sample. Political Psychology. 34(4): 55372.
Smith, SS. 2010. Race and Trust. Annual Review of Sociology. 36: 45375.
Stolle D, Soroka S, Johnston R. 2008. When does diversity erode trust? Neighborhood diversity, interpersonal
trust and the mediating effect of social interactions. Political Studies. 56(1): 5775.
Sturgis P, Brunton-Smith I, Read, S, Allum N. 2011. Does ethnic diversity erode trust? Putnam’s ‘hunkering
down’ thesis reconsidered. British Journal of Political Science. 41(1): 5782.
Sønderskov KM. 2011. Explaining large-N cooperation: Generalized social trust and the social exchange
heuristic. Rationality and Society. 23(1): 5174.
Sønderskov KM, Dinesen PT. 2014. Danish Exceptionalism: Explaining the Unique Increase in Social Trust over
the Past 30 Years. European Sociological Review. 30(6): 78295.
24
Tolsma J, van der Meer T. 2017. Losing Wallets, Retaining Trust? The Relationship Between Ethnic
Heterogeneity and Trusting Coethnic and Non-coethnic Neighbours and Non-neighbours to Return a Lost
Wallet. Social Indicators Research. 131(2): 63158.
Tolsma J, van der Meer T, Gesthuizen M. 2009. The impact of neighbourhood and municipality characteristics
on social cohesion in the Netherlands. Acta Politica. 44(3): 286313.
Uslaner EM. 2012. Segregation and mistrust: Diversity, isolation, and social cohesion. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
van der Meer T. 2016. Neither bridging nor bonding: A test of socialization effects by ethnically diverse
voluntary associations on participants' inter-ethnic tolerance, inter-ethnic trust and intra-ethnic
belonging. Social Science Research. 55: 6374.
van der Meer T, Tolsma, J. 2014. Ethnic Diversity and Its Effects on Social Cohesion. Annual Review of
Sociology. 40: 45978.
Viechtbauer W. 2010. Conducting Meta-Analyses in R with the metafor Package. Journal of Statistical
Software. 36(1): 148.
Weber M. 1987. Economy and Society: An Outline of Interpretive Sociology. Berkeley: University of California
Press.
Wollebæk D, Lundåsen W, Trägårdh L. 2012. Three forms of interpersonal trust: evidence from Swedish
municipalities. Scandinavian Political Studies. 35(4): 31946.
Öberg P, Oskarsson S, Svensson T. 2011. Similarity vs. Homogeneity: Contextual Effects in Explaining Trust.
European Political Science Review. 3(3): 34569.
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Although Europe has experienced unprecedented numbers of refugee arrivals in recent years, there exists almost no causal evidence regarding the impact of the refugee crisis on natives’ attitudes, policy preferences, and political engagement. We exploit a natural experiment in the Aegean Sea, where Greek islands close to the Turkish coast experienced a sudden and massive increase in refugee arrivals, while similar islands slightly farther away did not. Leveraging a targeted survey of 2,070 island residents and distance to Turkey as an instrument, we find that direct exposure to refugee arrivals induces sizable and lasting increases in natives’ hostility toward refugees, immigrants, and Muslim minorities; support for restrictive asylum and immigration policies; and political engagement to effect such exclusionary policies. Since refugees only passed through these islands, our findings challenge both standard economic and cultural explanations of anti-immigrant sentiment and show that mere exposure suffices in generating lasting increases in hostility.
Article
Full-text available
This study reflects upon differences in generalized trust levels among active participants of voluntary organizations with varying levels of ethnic diversity. Based on the Family Survey of the Dutch Population 2009, we find that (a) average trust levels are highest among participants of ethnically diverse organizations; (b) On average, length of participation is unrelated to trust, and (c) ethnic diversity does not positively moderate this relationship; (d) Level of trust is considerably higher among recent joiners (0-2 years) of highly-diverse organizations (26%+ minority membership), than among recent joiners of (more) homogeneous organizations, strongly implying that those with higher trust select into diverse organizations. However, (e) we see a substantially lower level of trust among members of highly-diverse organizations (26%+ minority) when comparing mid-term duration of participation (3-10 years) with short-term duration of participation (0-2 years) and again a higher level of trust when participation duration is 11 years or more. Trust scores, thus, converge across different diversity levels at longer duration of participation.
Chapter
Full-text available
Due to its wide-ranging implications for social cohesion in diversifying Western countries, the question of the potential negative consequences of ethnic diversity for social trust is arguably the most contentious question in the literature on social trust. In this chapter we critically review the empirical evidence for a negative relationship between contextual ethnic diversity (measured locally within countries) and social trust. We cautiously conclude that there are indications of a negative relationship, although with important variations across study characteristics including national setting, context unit analyzed, and conditioning on moderating influences. Building on the review, we highlight a number of paths for theoretical and methodological advances, which we argue would push the literature on the relationship between ethnic diversity and social trust forward.
Article
Full-text available
Drawing on insights from political psychology regarding political information processing, this paper argues that politically sophisticated individuals are less sensitive to the social cues manifested in the ethnic composition of their neighborhood when they form political opinions. This prediction is founded on politically sophisticated individuals having a greater comprehension of news and other mass-mediated sources, which makes them less likely to rely on neighborhood cues as sources of information relevant for political attitudes. Based on a unique panel data set with fine-grained information about the ethnic composition of the immediate neighborhood, the paper finds consistent support for the hypothesis: While neighborhood exposure to non-Western immigrants reduces anti-immigration attitudes among individuals with low political sophistication, there is no effect among individuals with high political sophistication. These results thus partially support contact theory and demonstrate that integrating the information processing and ethnic diversity literatures enhances our understanding of outgroup exposure effects.
Article
We study how close personal contact with minorities affects in-group and out-group trust in a field experiment in the armed forces. Soldiers are randomly assigned to rooms with or without ethnic minorities. At the end of the recruit period, we measure trust by using a trust game. Results indicate that close personal contact with minorities increases trust towards a generic immigrant. We replicate the result that individuals coming from more ethnically diverse areas trust minorities less, but random assignment to interact with minority soldiers removes this negative correlation. We conclude that social integration involving personal contact can reduce negative effects of ethnic diversity on trust.
Article
This section presents the third volume of Max Weber's fundamental work Economy and Society which has been translated into Russian for the first time. The third volume includes two works devoted to the sociology of law. The first, 'The Economy and Laws', discusses differences between sociological and juridical approaches to studies of social processes. It describes peculiarities of normative power arenas (orders) at different levels and demonstrates how they influence the economy. The second, 'Economy and Law' ('Sociology of Law'), reviews the evolution of law orders (primarily, the three "greatest systems of law" including Roman Law, Anglo-American Law, and European Continental Law) in the context of changes in the organization of economy and structures of dominancy. Law is considered an influential factor of the rationalization of social life which in turn is affected by a rationalized economy and social management. The Journal of Economic Sociology here publishes an excerpt from the chapter 'Law, Convention and Custom' in this third volume, which shows the role of the habitual in the formation of law; explains the importance of intuition and empathy for the emergence of new orders; and discusses the changeable borders between law, convention and custom. The translation is edited by Leonid Ionin and the chapter is published with the permission of HSE Publishing House. © 2018 National Research University Higher School of Economics. All rights reserved.
Article
Increasing ethnic diversity and whether or not it impacts on social trust are highly debated topics. Numerous studies report a negative relationship between diversity and trust, particularly in the United States. A growing body of follow-up studies has examined the extent to which these findings can be transferred to Europe, but the results remain inconclusive. Moving beyond the discussion of the mere existence or absence of diversity effects on trust, this study is concerned with the moderation of this relationship. It addresses the neglected role of subnational integration policies influencing the impact of diversity on trust. Empirical tests not only indicate that integration policies moderate the relationship but also suggest that the influence of policies varies substantively according to the specific policy aspect under consideration.
Article
Concerns about neighborhood erosion and conflict in ethnically diverse settings occupy scholars, policy makers, and pundits alike; but the empirical evidence is inconclusive. This article proposes the contested boundaries hypothesis as a refined contextual explanation focused on poorly defined boundaries between ethnic and racial groups. The authors argue that neighborhood conflict is more likely to occur at fuzzy boundaries defined as interstitial or transitional areas sandwiched between two homogeneous communities. Edge detection algorithms from computer vision and image processing allow them to identify these boundaries. Data from 4.7 million time- and geo-coded 311 service requests fromNew York City support their argument: complaints about neighbors making noise, drinking in public, or blocking the driveway are more frequent at fuzzy boundaries rather than crisp, polarized borders. By focusing on the broader sociospatial structure, the contestedboundarieshypothesis overcomes the “aspatial” treatment of neighborhoods as isolated areas in research on ethnic diversity.