ArticlePDF Available

Spending at least 120 minutes a week in nature is associated with good health and wellbeing

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Spending time in natural environments can benefit health and well-being, but exposure-response relationships are under-researched. We examined associations between recreational nature contact in the last seven days and self-reported health and well-being. Participants (n = 19,806) were drawn from the Monitor of Engagement with the Natural Environment Survey (2014/15–2015/16); weighted to be nationally representative. Weekly contact was categorised using 60 min blocks. Analyses controlled for residential greenspace and other neighbourhood and individual factors. Compared to no nature contact last week, the likelihood of reporting good health or high well-being became significantly greater with contact ≥120 mins (e.g. 120–179 mins: ORs [95%CIs]: Health = 1.59 [1.31–1.92]; Well-being = 1.23 [1.08–1.40]). Positive associations peaked between 200–300 mins per week with no further gain. The pattern was consistent across key groups including older adults and those with long-term health issues. It did not matter how 120 mins of contact a week was achieved (e.g. one long vs. several shorter visits/week). Prospective longitudinal and intervention studies are a critical next step in developing possible weekly nature exposure guidelines comparable to those for physical activity.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
1
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:7730 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Spending at least 120 minutes a
week in nature is associated with
good health and wellbeing
Mathew P. White1, Ian Alcock1, James Grellier
1, Benedict W. Wheeler1, Terry Hartig2,
Sara L. Warber1,3, Angie Bone1, Michael H. Depledge1 & Lora E. Fleming1
Spending time in natural environments can benet health and well-being, but exposure-response
relationships are under-researched. We examined associations between recreational nature contact in
the last seven days and self-reported health and well-being. Participants (n = 19,806) were drawn from
the Monitor of Engagement with the Natural Environment Survey (2014/15–2015/16); weighted to be
nationally representative. Weekly contact was categorised using 60 min blocks. Analyses controlled for
residential greenspace and other neighbourhood and individual factors. Compared to no nature contact
last week, the likelihood of reporting good health or high well-being became signicantly greater with
contact 120 mins (e.g. 120–179 mins: ORs [95%CIs]: Health = 1.59 [1.31–1.92]; Well-being = 1.23
[1.08–1.40]). Positive associations peaked between 200–300 mins per week with no further gain. The
pattern was consistent across key groups including older adults and those with long-term health issues.
It did not matter how 120 mins of contact a week was achieved (e.g. one long vs. several shorter visits/
week). Prospective longitudinal and intervention studies are a critical next step in developing possible
weekly nature exposure guidelines comparable to those for physical activity.
A growing body of epidemiological evidence indicates that greater exposure to, or ‘contact with’, natural envi-
ronments (such as parks, woodlands and beaches) is associated with better health and well-being, at least among
populations in high income, largely urbanised, societies1. While the quantity and quality of evidence varies across
outcomes, living in greener urban areas is associated with lower probabilities of cardiovascular disease2, obesity3,
diabetes4, asthma hospitalisation5, mental distress6, and ultimately mortality7, among adults; and lower risks of
obesity8 and myopia9 in children. Greater quantities of neighbourhood nature are also associated with better
self-reported health1012, and subjective well-being13 in adults, and improved birth outcomes14, and cognitive
development15, in children.
However, the amount of greenspace in ones neighbourhood (e.g. percent of land cover in a 1 km radius from
the home), or the distance of ones home to the nearest publically accessible green space or park16 is only one way
of assessing an individual’s level of nature exposure. An alternative is to measure the amount of time individuals
actually spend outside in natural environments17,18, sometimes referred to as ‘direct’ exposure19. Both approaches
are potentially informative. Residential proximity to nature may be related to health promoting factors such as
reduced air and noise pollution (although the relationships are complex20); and may also provide ‘indirect’ expo-
sure via views from the property21. Residential proximity is also generally positively related to ‘direct’ exposure;
i.e. people in greener neighbourhoods tend to report visiting greenspace more oen22. Yet many nature visits
take place outside of the local neighbourhood23. Moreover, such visits may compensate for a lack of nature in the
neighbourhood24. In other words, direct exposure, or more specically in the current context, recreational time
spent in natural environments per week, cannot accurately be inferred from neighbourhood greenspace near the
home.
Using data from a representative sample of the adult population of England, we aimed to better understand
the relationships between time spent in nature per week and self-reported health and subjective well-being. Our
research builds directly on a small number of studies that have started to look at similar issues17,18,25,26, and answers
the call made in several recent reviews for more work in this area27,28. Quantication of these ‘exposure-response’
1European Centre for Environment and Human Health, University of Exeter Medical School, Exeter, UK. 2Institute
for Housing and urban Research, Uppsala University, Box 514, SE-75120, Uppsala, Sweden. 3Department of Family
Medicine, University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor, MI, USA. Correspondence and requests for materials
should be addressed to M.P.W. (email: mathew.white@exeter.ac.uk)
Received: 8 May 2018
Accepted: 8 May 2019
Published: xx xx xxxx
OPEN
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
2
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:7730 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
relationships can contribute to the policy process, for example by providing evidence upon which to base rec-
ommendations regarding the amount of time required to be spent in nature per week to promote positive health
and well-being outcomes. A similar process was used to support development of guidelines on the amount of
recommended weekly physical activity needed for health promotion and disease prevention29.
e research advances previous work in three key ways. First, to date, researchers have examined direct nature
exposure-response relationships using either a specic visit duration17, or nature visit frequency over a prolonged
period26, or both independently18. By multiplying the duration of a representative visit within the last week by the
number of visits taken within the last week we were able to develop the rst weekly exposure metric (i.e. minutes
per week) for nature exposure, similar to those used in other health promotion contexts (e.g. physical activity29).
Second, by comparing the coecients of other, well-established, predictors of health and well-being (e.g. area
deprivation) with those for average time spent in nature per week, we were able to assess the relative strength
of any exposure-response relationship. ird, previous studies were constrained in their ability to look at the
generalisability of relationships across dierent socio-demographic groups due to relatively small, geographically
constrained samples. In this study, the current, nationally representative sample enabled us to stratify, a priori, on
socio-demographic characteristics, such as age30, gender31, ethnicity32 and area deprivation33, which appeared to
moderate the nature-health association in previous studies22.
Results
Models using duration categories. Descriptive data on the relationships between time spent in nature in
the last 7 days (in 60 min categories) and self-reported health (Good vs. poor) and subjective well-being (High vs.
low) are presented in Table1. Percentages per category are presented for both the estimation sample (n = 19,806),
and for the sample weighted to be representative of the adult population of England. Similar details for all covar-
iates can be found in Appendix B, and relationships between our key predictor, time in nature, and all other
covariates in Appendix C.
e odds ratios (ORs) and 95% condence intervals (CIs) for the survey weighted binomial logistic regres-
sions predicting health and well-being are presented in Table2 (full models in Appendix D). In the unadjusted
models the odds ratios for reporting ‘good’ health and ‘high’ well-being were signicantly higher for all nature
contact 60 mins per week compared to 0 mins. Contact of 1–59 mins per week was not associated with better
outcomes than 0 mins, and there was also no linear increase above 60 mins; longer durations were not associated
with better outcomes. In the adjusted models, signicance only emerged at the 120 mins per week category;
and again additional duration was not associated with improved outcomes. e relationship appeared somewhat
stronger for health than well-being (Fig.1).
Sensitivity analysis. We conducted three types of sensitivity analysis. First we explored exposure-response
relationships using time spent in nature as a continuous variable, and outcomes modelled as binary variables
using splines (Fig.2). e gures suggested relatively steady increases in the positive relationships for both health
and well-being up to around 120 mins, diminishing marginal returns from then until around 200 mins per week
for health and 300 mins for well-being, and then a attening out or even decrease thereon (though note the very
large CIs > 400 mins). Although Fig.2 should be treated with caution, due to hourly clustering (see Methods, and
Appendix A, Figure C), results broadly support the categorical analyses, with some suggestion that nature expo-
sure beyond 120 mins a week may have some additional benets that did not emerge when health and wellbeing
were treated as binary variables.
Second, we explored exposure-response relationships using time spent in nature as a categorical variable and
health and wellbeing modelled as ordinal variables. Results were again very similar (Appendix E). e only slight
change was signicance at the 60–119 min category for both outcomes, but this nding is not easily comparable
to the binary logistic results for reasons explained in more detail in Appendix E.
Self-reported health Subjective well-being (Life satisfaction)
Raw Ns and %s (Weighted %s) Raw Ns and %s (Weighted %s)
Not good Good Total
N
Not
good Good Low High Total
N
Low High
N % N % % % N % N % % %
Nature visit exposure
Weekly visit duration
300 mins 700 20.1 2784 79.9 3484 (18.1 81.9) 1228 35.2 2256 64.8 3484 (34.5 65.5)
240–299 mins 159 18.0 723 82.0 882 (15.5 84.5) 309 35.0 537 65.0 882 (34.1 65.9)
180–239 mins 207 20.4 807 79.6 1014 (18.1 81.9) 374 36.9 640 63.1 1014 (36.0 64.0)
120–179 mins 232 18.0 1058 82.0 1290 (15.5 84.5) 465 36.0 825 64.0 1290 (35.3 64.7)
60–119 mins 253 22.7 860 77.3 1113 (19.7 80.3) 439 39.4 674 60.6 1113 (38.2 61.8)
1–59 mins 97 27.3 258 72.7 355 (25.2 74.8) 155 43.7 200 56.3 355 (41.7 58.3)
0 mins 3678 31.5 7990 68.5 11668 (27.7 72.3) 5173 44.3 6495 55.7 11668 (42.8 57.2)
Tot a l s 5326 26.914480 73.119806 (23.5 76.5) 8143 41.111663 58.919806 (39.8 60.2)
Table 1. e frequency and percent of respondents in each category of each predictor who reported good/very
good health and high well-being. Notes. Weighted %s (in brackets) take into account sample weights. Similar
details for all covariates available in Supplementary Table1.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
3
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:7730 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Our nal sensitivity analysis modelled both time and well-being as continuous variables (Appendix E, Figure
D). Again the results were very similar to the original model (Fig.2b). Due to the inherently ordinal structure of
the general health variable, we were unable to conduct a comparable sensitivity model for health.
Contextualisation of results. To contextualise the magnitude of the relationship between weekly nature
contact and health and well-being, Fig.3 presents the relevant ORs (CIs) alongside those for selected predictors
including: neighbourhood greenspace and deprivation; physical exercise; individual SES; and relationship status
(see Appendix D for details on all covariates). e gure highlights that 120–179 mins vs. 0 mins of nature contact
per week was associated with: (a) a similar likelihood of reporting good health as, living in an area of low vs. high
deprivation; meeting vs. not meeting physical activity guidelines, and (c) being in a high vs. low SES occupation.
Although the association between nature contact at this level and wellbeing was similar to that between high vs.
low: greenspace, deprivation and physical activity; it was less important than SES and relationship status.
Generalisability of results. Table3 shows results of analyses stratied on key area and individual level
factors (see Appendix F for full details). For these analyses, nature contact was recongured into three duration
levels reecting: (a) ‘no exposure’ (0 minutes, ref); (b) ‘low exposure’, not associated with signicantly greater
likelihood of good health and high wellbeing (1–119 mins); and (c) ‘high exposure, i.e. all durations associated
with signicantly higher likelihood of good health and high well-being combined (120 mins). Estimates from
Self-reported health (Good vs. poor) Subjective well-being (High vs. low)
Unadjusted AdjustedaUnadjusted Adjusteda
OR
95% CIs
OR
95% CIs
OR
95% CIs
OR
95% CIs
Low High Low High Low High Low High
Nature visit exposure
Weekly visit duration
300 mins 1.73*** 1.57 1.91 1.33*** 1.18 1.50 1.42*** 1.31 1.54 1.20*** 1.09 1.31
240–299 mins 2.10*** 1.74 2.53 1.55*** 1.25 1.93 1.45*** 1.24 1.68 1.25** 1.07 1.46
180–239 mins 1.74*** 1.47 2.06 1.44*** 1.18 1.76 1.33*** 1.16 1.53 1.16*1.00 1.34
120–179 mins 2.09*** 1.79 2.44 1.59*** 1.31 1.92 1.37*** 1.21 1.55 1.23** 1.08 1.40
60–119 mins 1.56*** 1.34 1.83 1.13 0.94 1.37 1.21** 1.06 1.39 1.10 0.96 1.27
1–59 mins 1.14 0.88 1.46 1.04 0.76 1.41 1.05 0.83 1.31 0.99 0.78 1.26
0 mins ref ref ref ref ref ref ref ref ref ref ref ref
Covariates
Area NO YES NO YES
Individual NO YES NO YES
Constant 2.61 2.50 2.72 0.28 0.24 0.33 1.34 1.29 1.39 0.36 0.31 0.41
Pseudo R20.01 0.23 0.01 0.05
Valid N 20,264 19,806 20,264 19,806
Table 2. e odds ratios (OR) and 95% condence intervals (CIs) of reporting good health and high well-being
as a function of nature visit duration in the last 7 days. Notes. aFull models including covariates (urbanicity,
neighbourhood greenspace, area deprivation, background PM10, sex, age, SES, restricted functioning, physical
activity, employment status, relationship status, ethnicity, children in household, dog ownership and year)
available in Supplementary Table2. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
0.60
0.80
1.00
1.20
1.40
1.60
1.80
2.00
0 mins 1-59 mins 60-119 mins 120-179 mins 180-239 mins 240-299 mins ≥ 300 mins
gnitroperrofsoitarsddO
htlaehdoogyreV/dooG
0.60
0.80
1.00
1.20
1.40
1.60
1.80
2.00
0 mins 1-59 mins 60-119 mins 120-179 mins 180-239 mins 240-299 mins ≥ 300 mins
hgiHgnitroperrofsoitarsddO
well-being
Time spent visiting natural environments in last 7 days
Figure 1. e odds ratios (OR) and 95% condence intervals of reporting good health and high well-being as a
function of nature visit duration in the last 7 days (0 mins = reference category). Note: Adjusted for urbanicity,
neighbourhood greenspace, area deprivation, background PM10, sex, age, SES, restricted functioning, physical
activity, employment status, relationship status, ethnicity, children in household, dog ownership and year.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
4
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:7730 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
the models of health showed that the positive relationship found for ‘high’ but not ‘low’ exposure, compared to
‘no exposure, in the overall model was consistent across those living in urban and rural, and high and low dep-
rivation, areas. It was also consistent for: both males/females; those above/below 65years old; those of high/low
occupational social grade; those with/without a long-term illness/disability; and for those who did vs. did not
meet physical activity recommendations. Stratication on neighbourhood greenspace suggested those in areas
Figure 2. e probability of reporting (a) good health and (b) high well-being (with 95% condence intervals)
as a function of time spent in nature in the last 7 days using a generalised additive model (GAM) with a
penalized cubic spline for nature contact. Note. e GAM is adjusted for urbanicity, neighbourhood greenspace,
area deprivation, background PM10, sex, age, SES, restricted functioning, physical activity, employment status,
relationship status, ethnicity, children in household, dog ownership and year.
0.60
0.80
1.00
1.20
1.40
1.60
1.80
2.00
120-179 vs. 0 mins
nature
High vs. low greenLow vs. high
deprivation
Meets activity
guidelines vs. not
Top vs. bottom SESCouple vs. single
gnitroperrofsoitarsddO
htlaehdoogyreV/dooG
0.60
0.80
1.00
1.20
1.40
1.60
1.80
2.00
120-179 vs. 0 mins
nature
High vs. low greenLow vs. high
deprivation
Meets activity
guidelines vs. not
Top vs. bottom SESCouple vs. single
hgiHgnitroperrofsoitarsddO
well-being
Selected comparators
Figure 3. e odds ratios (OR) and 95% condence intervals of reporting good health and high well-being as a
function of nature visits and selected covariates (controlling for all other covariates).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
5
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:7730 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
of high (but not low) greenspace also had greater odds of good health if they spent any time in nature per week
compared to 0 mins, possibly reecting the importance of indirect exposure among this cohort. Stratication
on ethnicity showed the threshold was maintained amongst white British, but not ‘other’ respondents. Stratied
models of well-being showed that ‘high’ but not ‘low’ exposure was associated with signicantly greater odds of
high wellbeing in all cases.
Additional analyses found no dierences in health and well-being as a function of how ‘high’ exposure was
achieved (a) one 120+ min visit; (b) two 60+ min visits; or (c) or three/more 40 min visits (see Appendix G for
details).
Discussion
Growing evidence of a positive association between contact with natural environments and health and well-being
has led to calls for improved understanding of any exposure-response relationships27,28. e aim of the current
study was to assess these relationships with a measure based on direct exposure to natural environments, rather
than residential proximity, using data from a large nationally representative sample in England. Exposure was
dened in terms of the self-reported minutes spent in natural environments for recreation in the last seven days;
and outcomes were self-reported health and subjective well-being.
Self-reported health (Good vs. poor) Subjective well-being (High vs. low)
1–119 mins 120 mins 1–119 mins 120 mins
OR
95% CIs
OR
95% CIs
OR
95% CIs
OR
95% CIs
Low High Low High Low High Low High
Area level stratication
Urbanicity
Urban
(n = 18,694) 1.08 0.91 1.27 1.38*** 1.25 1.52 1.07 0.94 1.21 1.19*** 1.11 1.28
Rural (n = 1,112) 1.94 0.96 3.95 2.27*** 1.49 3.47 1.37 0.79 2.39 1.59** 1.17 2.18
Area greenspace
High (n = 8,510) 1.48** 1.16 1.90 1.54*** 1.34 1.78 1.05 0.87 1.27 1.28*** 1.14 1.42
Low (n = 11,966) 0.88 0.71 1.09 1.33*** 1.17 1.52 1.11 0.94 1.30 1.16** 1.06 1.27
Area deprivation
High (n = 11,796) 1.03 0.84 1.27 1.36*** 1.20 1.54 1.10 0.94 1.29 1.29** 1.17 1.41
Low (n = 8,010) 1.23 0.95 1.59 1.53*** 1.32 1.78 1.13 0.93 1.36 1.23** 1.10 1.37
Individual level stratication
Sex
Female
(n = 10,419) 0.99 0.79 1.23 1.38*** 1.21 1.58 1.03 0.88 1.22 1.22*** 1.11 1.35
Male (n = 9,387) 1.25 0.98 1.58 1.46*** 1.27 1.67 1.12 0.94 1.34 1.20** 1.08 1.32
Age
16–64 yrs
(n = 14,667) 1.05 0.87 1.27 1.28*** 1.14 1.44 1.07 0.94 1.29 1.18*** 1.09 1.28
65+yrs
(n = 5,193) 1.27 0.91 1.76 1.87*** 1.57 2.22 1.10 0.84 1.43 1.35*** 1.16 1.57
Restricted f unctioning
Yes (n = 4,545) 1.31 0.97 1.77 1.42*** 1.20 1.68 1.07 0.80 1.43 1.31*** 1.12 1.53
No (n = 15,261) 1.05 0.87 1.26 1.42*** 1.27 1.60 1.08 0.94 1.23 1.19*** 1.10 1.29
SES
AB/C1 (highest)
(n = 8,624) 1.09 0.86 1.39 1.43*** 1.24 1.65 1.01 0.85 1.20 1.16** 1.05 1.28
C2/DE (lowest)
(n = 11,182) 1.16 0.94 1.44 1.43*** 1.26 1.62 1.18 1.00 1.40 1.29** 1.17 1.43
Ethnicity
White British
(n = 15,198) 1.14 0.96 1.36 1.47*** 1.33 1.63 1.12 0.97 1.29 1.21*** 1.11 1.31
Other (n = 4,608) 1.01 0.69 1.48 1.21 0.95 1.53 0.96 0.75 1.21 1.22*1.05 1.42
Meets activity guidelines
No (n = 15,008) 1.13 0.95 1.36 1.48*** 1.32 1.65 1.05 0.92 1.20 1.17*** 1.08 1.27
Yes (n = 4,798) 0.98 0.67 1.43 1.22*1.01 1.47 1.21 0.92 1.60 1.32*** 1.14 1.51
Table 3. e odds ratios (OR) and 95% condence intervals (CIs) of reporting good health and high well-being
as a function of the three main categories of nature visit duration in the last 7 days, stratied on key area and
individual covariates. Notes. In all analyses the reference duration is 0 minutes per week; All analyses control
for area and individual covariates not being stratied. Full models available on request. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01;
***p < 0.001.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
6
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:7730 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Aer a range of covariates had been taken into account, individuals who spent between 1 and 119 mins in
nature in the last week were no more likely to report good health or high well-being than those who reported
0 mins. However, individuals who reported spending 120 mins in nature last week had consistently higher lev-
els of both health and well-being than those who reported no exposure. Sensitivity analyses using splines to
allow duration to be modelled as a continuous variable suggested that beyond 120 mins there were decreasing
marginal returns until around 200–300 mins when the relationship attened or even dropped. We tentatively
suggest, therefore, that 120 mins contact with nature per week may reect a kind of “threshold”, below which
there is insucient contact to produce signicant benets to health and well-being, but above which such benets
become manifest.
In terms of magnitude, the association between health, well-being and 120 mins spent in nature a week, was
similar to associations between health, well-being and: (a) living in an area of low vs. high deprivation; (b) being
employed in a high vs. low social grade occupation; and (c) achieving vs. not achieving recommended levels of
physical activity in the last week. Given the widely stated importance of all these factors for health and well-being,
we interpret the size of the nature relationship to be meaningful in terms of potential public health implications.
at the 120 mins “threshold” was present even for those who lived in low greenspace areas reects the
importance of measuring recreational nature contact directly when possible, rather than simply using residential
proximity as a proxy for all types of nature exposure. People travel beyond their local neighbourhoods to access
recreational nature experiences, and indeed in our own data those who lived in the least green areas had higher
odds of spending 120 mins in nature than those living in greener neighbourhoods (Appendix C). Impoverished
local opportunities need not be a barrier to nature exposure23,24. at the “threshold” was also present for those
with long-term illnesses/disability, suggests that the positive overall association in the data was not simply due to
healthier people visiting nature more oen.
One explanation for our ndings might be that time spent in nature is a proxy for physical activity, and it is
this which is driving the relationship, not nature contact per se. In England, for instance, over 3 million adults
achieve recommended activity levels fully, or in part, in natural settings34. Although: (a) we tried to control for
this by including physical activity over the last 7 days in our models; and (b) the threshold applied to individuals
who did not meet activity guidelines; we were unable to fully untangle these issues. Experimental research, how-
ever, indicates that some benets cannot be due solely to physical activity. Research into shinrin-yoku (Japanese
“forest bathing”)35, for instance, suggested that various psycho-physiological benets can be gained from merely
sitting passively in natural vs. urban settings. Moreover, physical activity conducted in nature may be more psy-
chologically benecial than in other locations36, suggesting a complex interaction between the two which requires
further research to fully understand20.
e current results also suggested that it did not matter how the “threshold” was achieved. is may be
because individuals selected exposures to t their personal preferences and circumstances. For instance, some
may prefer long walks on the weekend in locations further from home; while others may prefer regular shorter
visits to parks in the local area. To recommend the former type of person stops their long weekly visit in favour of
several shorter trips or vice versa may be misguided.
Whilst this study deepens our understanding of the potential value of spending time outdoors in nature to
health and well-being, it is too early to make specic guidance due to several limitations. First, the data are obser-
vational and cross-sectional; and thus, notwithstanding the same pattern holding for those with a long-term
illness/disability, we are unable to rule out the possibility that the association is, at least in part, due to healthier,
happier people spending more time in nature. Prospective longitudinal studies of the kind used to help develop
physical activity guidelines29, and nature-based intervention studies are needed to better understand causality.
Cimprich and Ronis37, for instance, found that women recently diagnosed with breast cancer scored higher on
several attention tasks, compared to standard care controls, following a ve-week period of spending 120 mins
per week in ‘natural restorative environments. e authors argued that the 120 mins per week of nature expo-
sure helped the women restore cognitive resources depleted by the stress of their diagnoses and early treatment.
Although our sample was more heterogeneous, weekly nature exposure may work in a similar fashion by reducing
generally high levels of stress38. Similar studies are needed to see how generalizable any potential “threshold” is
across a range of situations, and to see how long an individual needs to maintain a certain amount of weekly expo-
sure to achieve health and well-being gains. Although eects on attentional processes were observed aer just 5
weeks in Cimprich and Ronis37, health eects may need longer; and it is also important to see whether dierent
types of nature contact might confer dierent benets.
We also note that, although signicant, time in nature explained relatively little variance in either health or
wellbeing in these models based on cross-sectional data (approx. 1% in unadjusted models in both cases). It will
therefore be important to explore eect sizes in prospective/experimental studies to better understand the cost/
benet implications of any potentials interventions.
Another limitation concerned our estimate of weekly exposure. As duration was asked about only a single
randomly selected visit in the last week, we assumed that at the population level this was representative of all vis-
its. Although rigorous collection protocols meant that the eects of a typical visit selection are likely to cancel out
over a sample of nearly 20,000, we recognise that accuracy at the individual level would be improved if duration
were asked about all visits in the last week. We also acknowledge that our data rely on self-reports and thus results
needed to be treated with caution. For instance, self-reported duration is likely to be less accurate than measures
obtained from geo-tracking individuals during specic visits39, or over several days40, and individuals may have
been unsure about, or reluctant to discuss, certain issues which were included as covariates (e.g. long standing
illness/disability). Future studies would ideally collate as much data via non self-report measures as possible. We
note, moreover, that unlike exposure to oen invisible environmental factors such as air pollution, we can poten-
tially ‘re-live’ our experiences of the natural world in memory, for instance during periods of ‘mind wandering,
and derive benets from these recollections independent of those experienced in situ41. us, an exposure in this
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
7
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:7730 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
context may be considered as the time in situ plus all subsequent time spent thinking about the experience42. In
short, we believe further work is needed to think more critically and creatively about what the term ‘exposure
means in the current context.
We also remain cautious about any potential 120 mins “threshold”. In part its emergence may be a conse-
quence of the clustering of duration responses around the hour mark and subsequent stratication, rather than
anything materially dierent occurring at this level of exposure. e spline models, for instance, suggested a more
nuanced pattern. However, this smoothing of the data was still reliant on a highly non-normal distribution, sug-
gesting that we need to be cautious about these analyses as well. Further work is also needed to explore the ‘peak
of returns at around 200–300 mins, to better understand why spending more time in nature is associated with
little marginal gain. us, we see the tentative “threshold” and “peak” discussed here more as a starting points for
discussion and further investigation, than clearly established ndings.
Finally, our results say little about exposure ‘quality’. Research considering the quality of the natural environ-
ment in terms of plant and/or animal species richness suggests that experiences may be better in more biodiverse
settings25,43. Contact with nature is more than just a complex multi-sensory experience, to varying degrees per-
sonal histories and meanings, longstanding cultural practices, and a sense of place play some role in the benets
realised4446, factors which may account for why we did not nd the same pattern for health individuals not iden-
tifying as White British. In the current research, for instance, exposure estimates relied upon visits undertaken
voluntarily, presumably because they had features important to those individuals47 and these eects may not be
found if individuals were to regularly spend 120 mins a week in a natural environment of less personal relevance
(e.g. those who self-identied as ‘White European’). Our estimates also explicitly excluded time spent in one’s own
garden which can be an important form of meaningful nature contact for many people48. All of these issues will
need greater consideration in future research.
To conclude, although this research suggests that spending 120 mins a week in nature may be an important
“threshold” for health and well-being across a broad range of the adult population in England, we believe that
more prospective cohort, longitudinal, and experimental studies are required before any clear conclusions can
be drawn. In addition to improving the duration-exposure estimates used here, more research is also needed to
understand the impact of dierent activities undertaken, as well as the eect of environmental quality and per-
sonal meaning. Nevertheless, we see our ndings as an important starting point for discussions around providing
simple, evidence-based recommendations about the amount of time spent in natural settings that could result in
meaningful promotion of health and well-being.
Methods
Participants & procedure. Participants were drawn from Waves 6 and 7 (2014–2015/2015–2016) of the
Monitor of Engagement with the Natural Environment (MENE) survey (the only Waves where our key out-
comes were consistently measured). e survey, which is part of the UK government’s National Statistics, is repeat
cross-sectional (dierent people take part in each wave), and is conducted across the whole of England and
throughout the year (approx. 4,000 people per week) to reduce potential geographical and seasonal biases49. As
part of the UK’s ocial statistics, sampling protocols are extensive, to ensure as representative a sample of the
adult English population as possible. Full details can be found in the annual MENE Technical Reports49 with
key features including: (a) “a computerised sampling system which integrates the Post Oce Address le with
the 2001 Census small area data at output area level. is enables replicated waves of multi-stage stratied sam-
ples”; (b) “the areas within each Standard Region are stratied into population density bands and within band, in
descending order by percentage of the population in socio-economic Grade I and II”; (c) “[in order to] maximise
the statistical accuracy of the sampling, sequential waves of eldwork are allocated systematically across the sam-
pling frame to ensure maximum geographical dispersion”; (d) “to ensure a balanced sample of adults within the
eective contacted addresses, a quota is set by sex (male, female housewife, female non-housewife); within the
female housewife quota, presence of children and working status and within the male quota, working status”; and
(e) “the survey data is weighted to ensure that the sample is representative of the UK population in terms of the
standard demographic characteristics” (ref.49, p.5). Data is collected using in-home face-to-face interviews with
responses recorded using Computer Assisted Personal Interviewing (CAPI) soware.
Although the total sample for these years was n = 91,190, the health and well-being questions were only asked
in every fourth sampling week (i.e. monthly, rather than weekly) resulting in a reduced sample of n = 20,264.
In order to account for any residual biases in sampling at this monthly level, special ‘month’ survey weights are
included in the data set. ese were applied in the current analysis to ensure that results remained generalisable to
the entire adult population of England. All data were anonymised by Natural England and are publically accessi-
ble at: http://publications.naturalengland.org.uk/publication/2248731?category=47018. Ethical approval was not
required for this secondary analysis of publically available National Statistics.
Outcomes: Self-reported health & subjective well-being. Self-reported health (henceforth: health)
was assessed using the single-item: ‘How is your health in general?’ (sometimes referred to as ‘SF1’). Response
options were: ‘Very bad’, ‘Bad’, ‘Fair’, ‘Good’ and ‘Very good’. Responses are robustly associated with use of med-
ical services50 and mortality51; and crucially, for current purposes, neighbourhood greenspace13. Following ear-
lier work we dichotomised responses into ‘Good’ (‘Good/very good’, weighted = 76.5%) and ‘Not good’ (‘Fair/
bad/very bad’, 23.5%)52. Subjective well-being (henceforth: well-being) was assessed using the ‘Life Satisfaction’
measure, one of the UK’s national well-being measures53: ‘Overall how satised are you with life nowadays?’
with responses ranging from 0 ‘Not at all’ to 10 ‘Completely’. Again, following earlier studies we dichotomised
responses into ‘High’ (8–10, 60.2%) and Low (0–7, 39.8%) well-being54. Histograms of the (non-normal) distribu-
tions for both outcome variables are presented in Appendix A. Of note although the dichotomisation points were
based on prior research, they are consistent with the current data; the 50th percentile for health was in the ‘good’
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
8
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:7730 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
response and for wellbeing in ‘8’. Sensitivity analyses conducted on ordinal (both health and wellbeing) and linear
(wellbeing only) variations of these variables are presented in Appendix E.
Exposure: Recreational nature contact in last 7 days. Recreational nature contact, or time spent in
natural environments in the last week, was derived by multiplying the number of reported recreational visits
per week by the length of a randomly selected visit in the last week. Participants were introduced to the survey
as follows: “I am going to ask you about occasions in the last week when you spent your time out of doors. By out
of doors we mean open spaces in and around towns and cities, including parks, canals and nature areas; the coast
and beaches; and the countryside including farmland, woodland, hills and rivers. is could be anything from a few
minutes to all day. It may include time spent close to your home or workplace, further aeld or while on holiday in
England. However this does not include: routine shopping trips or; time spent in your own garden.” en they were
asked “how many times, if at all, did you make this type of visit yesterday/on <DAY > ” for each of the previous
seven days. Ninety-eight percent of respondents reported 7 visits last week. e remaining 2% were capped at 7
visits to avoid dramatically skewing weekly duration estimates.
Aer basic details of each visit (up to 3 per day) were recorded, a single visit was selected at random by the
CAPI soware, for the interviewer to ask further questions about, including: “How long did this visit last alto-
gether?” (Hours & Minutes). Due to random selection, even if the selected visit was not necessarily representative
for any given individual, the randomisation procedure should reduce potential bias at the population level at
which our analyses were conducted. Weekly duration estimates were thus derived by multiplying the duration
for this randomly selected visit by the number of stated visits in the last seven days (capped at 7). Following the
approach of earlier exposure-response studies in the eld (e.g. Shanahan et al., 2016), duration was categorised
into 7 categories: 0 mins (n = 11,668); 1–59 mins (n = 355); 60–119 mins (n = 1,113); 120–179 mins (n = 1,290);
180–239 mins (n = 1,014); 240–299 mins (n = 882); 300 mins (n = 3,484). An alternative banding at 30 minutes
was problematic because of very low Ns for some bands (e.g. 1–29 mins, n = 85), reecting the fact that weekly
duration estimates clustered around the hour marks, e.g. 78% of the unweighted observations within the 120–
179 mins band were precisely 120 mins (See Appendix A, Figure C for duration histogram). e highest band was
capped at 300 mins due to the large positive skew of the data.
Control variables. Health and well-being are associated with socio-demographic and environmental charac-
teristics at both neighbourhood (e.g. area deprivation) and individual (e.g. relationship status) levels55. As many
of these variables may also be related to nature exposure they were controlled for in the adjusted analyses.
Area level control variables. Area level covariate data was assigned on the spatial level of the Census 2001
Lower-layer Super Output Areas (LSOAs) in which individuals lived. ere were 32,482 LSOAs in England, each
containing approximately 1,500 people within a mean physical area of 4km2.
Neighbourhood greenspace. In order to understand how much greenspace is in an individual’s neigh-
bourhood, we derived an area density metric using the Generalised Land Use Database (GLUD)56. e GLUD
provides, for each LSOA in England, the area covered by greenspace and domestic gardens. ese were summed
and divided by the total LSOA area to provide the greenspace density metric. is metric was allocated to each
individual in the sample, based on LSOA of residence. Following previous literature, individuals were assigned to
one of ve quintiles of greenspace based on this denition (ranging from least green to most green)33. Rather than
derive quintiles of greenspace from the current sample (i.e. divide the current sample into ve equal parts based
on the percentage of greenspace in their LSOA), we assigned individuals instead to one of ve pre-determined
greenspace quintiles based on the distribution of greenspace across all 32,482 LSOAs in England. Although this
meant that we did not get exactly equal 20% shares of our current sample across greenspace quintiles (although
due to the sampling protocol we were still very close to this, see Appendix B) this approach allowed inferences to
be made across the entire country, rather than simply to the current sample. In exploratory sensitivity analyses
we dened greenspace as the GLUD category ‘greenspace’ only, with the GLUD category ‘gardens’ excluded. is
produced very similar results, so we focused on the more inclusive denition including both aspects. In further
exploratory sensitivity analyses, we assigned individuals to ve greenspace categories dened by equal ranges of
greenspace coverage (e.g. 0–20%, 21–40%, 41–60% etc.) rather than quintiles based on percentages of the pop-
ulation. is also produced very similar results, so again we decided to go with the most common approach. In
subsequent analyses the least green quintile acted as the reference category.
Area deprivation. Each LSOA in England is assessed in terms of several parameters of deprivation, includ-
ing unemployment and crime, levels of educational, income, health metrics, barriers to housing and services, and
the living environment. A total Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) score is derived from these subdomains57.
Following previous studies52, we assigned individuals into deprivation quintiles based on the LSOA in which
they lived. As with greenspace, the cut points for area deprivation quintiles were also based on all LSOAs in
England, rather than those in the current sample, to allow inference to the population as a whole (most deprived
quintile = ref).
Air pollution. An indicative measure of air pollution was operationalised as LSOA background PM10 assigned
to tertiles of all LSOAs in England (lowest particulate concentration = ref). PM10 concentrations, based on
Pollution Climate Mapping (PCM) model simulations58, were averaged over the period 2002–2012, and aggre-
gated from 1 km square resolution to LSOAs.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
9
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:7730 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Individual level controls. Individual level controls comparable to earlier studies in this area6,7,12,13,15 included: sex
(male = ref); age (categorised as 16–64 = ref; 65+); occupational social grade (AB (highest, e.g. managerial), C1,
C2 and DE (lowest, e.g. unskilled labour, = ref) as a proxy for individual socio-economic status (SES); employ-
ment status (full-time, part-time, in education, retired, not working/unemployed = ref); relationship status (mar-
ried/cohabiting; single/separated/divorced/widowed = ref); ethnicity (White British; other = ref); number of
children in the household (1 vs. 0 = ref); and dog ownership (Yes; No = ref).
Two further control variables were particularly important. First, the survey asked: ‘Do you have any long stand-
ing illness, health problem or disability that limits your daily activities or the kind of work you can do?’ (‘Restricted
functioning’: Yes; No = ref). Including this variable, at least in part, controls for reverse causality. If similar asso-
ciations between nature exposure and health and well-being are found for both those with and without restricted
functioning, this would support the notion that the associations are not merely due to healthier, more mobile
people visiting nature more oen.
We also controlled for the number of days per week people reported engaging in physical activity >30 mins; in
the current analysis dichotomised as either meeting or not meeting guidelines of 150 mins per week (i.e. 5 days in
the week with physical activity >30 mins). Some people achieve this guideline though physical activity in natural
settings35, thus, any association between time spent in nature and health may simply be due to the physical activity
engaged in these settings. We believe this is not the case in the current context because the (rank order) correla-
tion between weekly nature contact and the number of days a week an individual engaged in >30 mins of physical
activity was just rs = 0.27. Nevertheless, by controlling for weekly activity levels, modelled relationships between
time in nature and health have less bias from this source, and, therefore, improved estimates of association with
nature exposure per se.
Temporal controls. Due to the multi-year pooled nature of the data, year/wave was also controlled for.
Preliminary analysis found no eect of the season in which the data were collected so this was excluded from
nal analyses.
Analysis strategy. Survey weighted binomial logistic regressions were used to predict the relative odds that
an individual would have ‘Good’ health or ‘High’ well-being as a function of weekly nature exposure in terms of
duration categories per week. Model t was provided by pseudo R2; here the more conservative Cox and Snell
estimate. e outcome binary variables were rst regressed against the exposure duration categories to test direct
relationships; adjusted models were then specied to include the individual and area level control variables.
Due to missing area level data for a small minority of participants (n = 456), our estimation samples for these
adjusted models were n = 19,808. Preliminary analysis found that the weighted descriptive proportions among
this reduced estimation sample diered only negligibly from those among all available observations in the wider
MENE sample, suggesting our complete case analysis approach did not distort the population representativeness
of the estimation sample. e full n = 20,264 sample was maintained for the unadjusted model to provide the
most accurate, weighted representation of the data, as reducing unadjusted models to n = 19,808 produced prac-
tically identical results. Although our main analyses used duration categories of weekly nature contact, an explor-
atory analysis used generalized additive models incorporating a penalized cubic regression spline of duration as a
continuous variable (adjusting for the same set of covariates). is enabled us to produce a ‘smoother’ plot of the
data. Analyses and plotting was done using R version 3.4.1, using packages mgcv and visreg59.
To explore the generalisability of any pattern across dierent socio-demographic groups, we also a priori
stratied the analyses on several area and individual covariates (as dened above) which have been found to
be important in previous studies: (a) Urbanicity; (b) Neighbourhood greenspace; (c) Area deprivation; (d) Sex;
(e) Age; (f) Restricted functioning; (g) Individual socio-economic status (SES); (f) Ethnicity; and (g) Physical
activity. In the case of the three multi-category predictors (area greenspace/deprivation, individual SES), binary
classications were derived for the stratied analyses to maintain robust sample sizes in each category. In the case
of LSOA greenspace and deprivation binary splits were made based on the median cut-point for all LSOAs in
England; SES was dichotomised by collapsing the social grade categories in the standard way, A/B/C1 vs. C2/D/E.
References
1. Hartig, T. & ahn, P. H. Living in cities, naturally. Science 352, 938–940 (2016).
2. ardan, O. et al. Neighborhood greenspace and health in a large urban center. Sci ep 5, 11610 (2015).
3. Halonen, J. I. et al. Green and blue areas as predictors of overweight and obesity in an 8year followup study. Obesity 22, 1910–1917
(2014).
4. Astell-Burt, T., Feng, X. & olt, G. S. Is neighborhood green space associated with a lower ris of type 2 diabetes? Evidence from
267,072 Australians. Diabetes Care 37, 197–201 (2014).
5. Alcoc, I. et al. Land cover and air pollution are associated with asthma hospitalisations: A cross-sectional study. Environ Int 109,
29–41 (2017).
6. Mitchell, . J., ichardson, E. A., Shortt, N. . & Pearce, J. . Neighborhood environments and socioeconomic inequalities in mental
well-being. Am J Prev Med 49, 80–84 (2015).
7. Gascon, M. et al. esidential green spaces and mortality: a systematic review. Environ Int 86, 60–67 (2016).
8. Wood, S. L. et al. Exploring the relationship between childhood obesity and proximity to the coast: A rural/urban perspective.
Health Place 40, 129–136 (2016).
9. Dadvand, P. et al. Green spaces and spectacles use in schoolchildren in Barcelona. Environ es 152, 256–262 (2017).
10. Dadvand, P. et al. Surrounding greenness and pregnancy outcomes in four Spanish birth cohorts. Environ Health Persp 120, 1481
(2012).
11. Dadvand, P. et al. Green spaces and cognitive development in primary schoolchildren. PNAS 112, 7937–7942 (2015).
12. Maas, J., Verheij, . A., Groenewegen, P. P., De Vries, S. & Spreeuwenberg, P. Green space, urbanity, and health: how strong is the
relation? J Epidemiol Commun H 60, 587–592 (2006).
13. Mitchell, . & Popham, F. Greenspace, urbanity and health: relationships in England. J Epidemiol Commun H 61, 681–683 (2007).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
10
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:7730 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
14. Seresinhe, C. I., Preis, T. & Moat, H. S. Quantifying the impact of scenic environments on health. Sci ep, 5, https://doi.org/10.1038/
srep16899 (2015).
15. White, M. P., Alcoc, I., Wheeler, B. W. & Depledge, M. H. Would you be happier living in a greener urban area? A xed-eects
analysis of panel data. Psychol Sci 24, 920–928 (2013).
16. Eel, E. D. & de Vries, S. Nearby green space and human health: Evaluating accessibility metrics. Landscape Urban Plan 157,
214–220 (2017).
17. Barton, J. & Pretty, J. What is the best dose of nature and green exercise for improving mental health? A multi-study analysis. Environ
Sci Technol 44, 3947–3955 (2010).
18. Shanahan, D. F. Health benets from nature experiences depend on dose. Sci ep 6, 28551 (2016).
19. eniger, L. E., Gaston, . J., Irvine, . N. & Fuller, . A. What are the benets of interacting with nature? Int J Environ es Pub He 10,
913–935 (2013).
20. Hartig, T., Mitchell, ., de Vries, S. & Frumin, H. Nature and health. Annu ev Publ Health 35, 207–228 (2014).
21. Nutsford, D., Pearson, A. L., ingham, S. & eitsma, F. esidential exposure to visible blue space (but not green space) associated
with lower psychological distress in a capital city. Health Place 39, 70–78 (2016).
22. Grahn, P. & Stigsdotter, U. A. Landscape planning and stress. Urban For Urban Gree 2, 1–18 (2003).
23. Hillsdon, M., Coombes, E., Griew, P. & Jones, A. An assessment of the relevance of the home neighbourhood for understanding
environmental inuences on physical activity: how far from home do people roam? Int J Behav Nutr Phy 12, 100 (2015).
24. Sijtsma, F. J., de Vries, S., van Hinsberg, A. & Diederis, J. Does ‘grey’ urban living lead to more ‘green’ holiday nights? A Netherlands
Case Study. Landscape Urban Plan 105, 250–257 (2012).
25. Cox, D. T. et al. Doses of nearby nature simultaneously associated with multiple health benets. Int J Environ es Pub He 14, 172
(2017).
26. Flowers, E. P., Freeman, P. & Gladwell, V. F. A cross-sectional study examining predictors of visit frequency to local green space and
the impact this has on physical activity levels. BMC Public Health 16, 420 (2016).
27. Shanahan, D. F., Fuller, . A., Bush, ., Lin, B. B. & Gaston, . J. e health benets of urban nature: how much do we need?
Bioscience 65, 476–485 (2015).
28. Frumin, H. et al. Nature contact and human health: A research agenda. Environ Health Persp 125, 075001–1 (2017).
29. Hasell, W. L. et al. Physical activity and public health: updated recommendation for adults from the American College of Sports
Medicine and the American Heart Association. Circulation 116, 1081–93 (2007).
30. Astell-Burt, T., Mitchell, . & Hartig, T. e association between green space and mental health varies across the lifecourse. A
longitudinal study. J Epidemiol Commun H 68, 578–583 (2014).
31. ichardson, E. A. & Mitchell, . Gender dierences in relationships between urban green space and health in the United ingdom.
Soc Sci Med 71, 568–575 (2010).
32. Gentin, S. Outdoor recreation and ethnicity in. Europe—A review. Urban For Urban Gree 10, 153–161 (2011).
33. Mitchell, . & Popham, F. Eect of exposure to natural environment on health inequalities: an observational population study.
Lancet 372, 1655–1660 (2008).
34. White, M. P. et al. ecreational physical activity in natural environments and implications for health: A population based cross-
sectional study in England. Prev Med 91, 383–388 (2016).
35. Par, B. J., Tsunetsugu, Y., asetani, T., agawa, T. & Miyazai, Y. e physiological eects of Shinrin-you (taing in the forest
atmosphere or forest bathing): evidence from eld experiments in 24 forests across Japan. Environ Health Prev 15, 18–26 (2010).
36. ompson Coon, J. et al. Does participating in physical activity in outdoor natural environments have a greater eect on physical
and mental wellbeing than physical activity indoors? A systematic review. Environ Sci Tech 45, 1761–1772 (2011).
37. Cimprich, B. & onis, D. L. An environmental intervention to restore attention in women with newly diagnosed breast cancer.
Cancer Nurs 26, 284–292 (2003).
38. Ward ompson, C. et al. More green space is lined to less stress in deprived communities: Evidence from salivary cortisol patterns.
Landscape Urban Plan 105, 221–229 (2012).
39. Doherty, S. T., Lemieux, C. J. & Canally, C. Tracing human activity and well-being in natural environments using wearable sensors
and experience sampling. Soc Sci Med 106, 83–92 (2014).
40. Cooper, A. . et al. Patterns of GPS measured time outdoors aer school and objective physical activity in English children: the
PEACH project. Int J Behav Nutr Phy 7, 31 (2010).
41. Smallwood, J. & Schooler, J. W. e science of mind wandering: empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annu ev
Psychol 66, 487–518 (2015).
42. Clawson, M. & netsch, J. L. Economics of outdoor recreation: Washington, DC: esources for the Future (1966).
43. de Vries, S., van Dillen, S. M., Groenewegen, P. P. & Spreeuwenberg, P. Streetscape greenery and health: stress, social cohesion and
physical activity as mediators. Soc Sci Med 94, 26–33 (2013).
44. Bell, S. L., Phoenix, C., Lovell, . & Wheeler, B. W. Using GPS and geonarratives: a methodological approach for understanding and
situating everyday green space encounters. Area 47, 88–96 (2015).
45. Hartig, T. et al. Health benets of nature experience: Psychological, social and cultural processes. In Nilsson, . et al. (Eds), Forests,
trees, and human health (pp. 127–168). Dordrecht: Springer (2011).
46. Völer, S. & istemann, T. Developing the urban blue: comparative health responses to blue and green urban open spaces in
Germany. Health Place 35, 196–205 (2015).
47. orpela, . M., Ylén, M., Tyrväinen, L. & Silvennoinen, H. Favorite green, waterside and urban environments, restorative
experiences and perceived health in Finland. Health Promot Int 25, 200–209 (2010).
48. Stigsdotter, U. & Grahn, P. What maes a garden a healing garden. J era Hort 13, 60–69 (2002).
49. Natural England. Monitor of Engagement with the Natural Environment: the national survey on people and the natural
environment. Technical eport to the 2009-16 Surveys. Natural England Joint eport JP023 (2017).
50. Miilunpalo, S., Vuori, I., Oja, P., Pasanen, M. & Urponen, H. Self-rated health status as a health measure: the predictive value of self-
reported health status on the use of physician services and on mortality in the woring-age population. J Clin Epidemiol 50, 517–528
(1997).
51. yn, . G., Goldacre, M. J. & Gill, M. Mortality rates and self-reported health: database analysis by English local authority area.
Brit Med J 329, 887–888 (2004).
52. Wheeler, B. W. et al. Beyond Greenspace: An ecological study of population general health and indicators of natural environment
type and quality. Int J Health Geogr 14, 17, https://doi.org/10.1186/s12942-015-0009-5 (2015).
53. Oce of National Statistics (ONS). Measuring Subjective Well-being. London: Oce of National Statistics (2011).
54. White, M. P., Pahl, S., Wheeler, B. W., Depledge, M. H. & Fleming, L. E. Natural environments and subjective wellbeing: Dierent
types of exposure are associated with dierent aspects of well-being. Health Place 45, 77–84 (2017).
55. Nan, L., Jerey, A. J., James, W. S., David, F. & Joel, S. C. Self-eported Health Status of the General Adult U.S. Population as Assessed
by the EQ-5D and Health Utilities Index. Med Care 43, 1078–1086 (2005).
56. Oce of the Deputy Prime Minister (OPDM). Generalised Land Use Database Statistics for England. London: ODPM Publications
(2005).
57. Department of Communities and Local Government (DCLG). e English Indices of Deprivation 2007. London: Communities and
Local Government (2008).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
11
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:7730 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
58. Brooes, D. M. et al. Technical report on U supplementary assessment under the Air Quality Directive (2008/50/EC), the Air
Quality Framewor Directive (96/62/EC) and Fourth Daughter Directive (2004/107/EC) for 2014 (2016).
59.  Core Team. : A language and environment for statistical computing.  Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria,
https://www.-project.org/ (2017).
Acknowledgements
is work was supported by the National Institute for Health Research Health Protection Research Unit (NIHR
HPRU) in Environmental Change and Health at the London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine in
partnership with Public Health England (PHE), and in collaboration with the University of Exeter, University
College London, and the Met Oce. e funders had no role in the study design, analysis, interpretation of data,
or decision to submit the article for publication. e views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily
those of the NHS, the NIHR, the Department of Health, or Public Health England. We would like thank an earlier
reviewer and the editorial board team for suggestions on how to improve an earlier version of this manuscript.
Author Contributions
M.W. conceived of the study in discussion with T.H., M.D. and L.E.F.; M.W., I.A. and J.G. conducted the analyses;
B.W., S.W. and A.B. made additional analysis suggestions and provided text/references on specic sections. All
authors contributed to the text of the manuscript and reviewed the nal submission.
Additional Information
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44097-3.
Competing Interests: e authors declare no competing interests.
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre-
ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per-
mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the
copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
© e Author(s) 2019
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... The health benefits of being within nature or having exposure to greenspaces have been increasingly described. Those who spend time in nature [1] or have access to greenspace [2] report better health and psychological wellbeing compared to those who do not, with the effects being crosscutting across different occupations, ethnic groups, rich and poor areas, and in people with chronic illnesses and disabilities [1]. Physiological stress reduction [3,4], improved cardiovascular health [5,6], memory improvements [7], increased natural killer cell activity [8], and increased longevity in senior citizens [9] are benefits associated with spending time in or near nature. ...
... The health benefits of being within nature or having exposure to greenspaces have been increasingly described. Those who spend time in nature [1] or have access to greenspace [2] report better health and psychological wellbeing compared to those who do not, with the effects being crosscutting across different occupations, ethnic groups, rich and poor areas, and in people with chronic illnesses and disabilities [1]. Physiological stress reduction [3,4], improved cardiovascular health [5,6], memory improvements [7], increased natural killer cell activity [8], and increased longevity in senior citizens [9] are benefits associated with spending time in or near nature. ...
Article
Full-text available
Nature prescription programs have become more common within healthcare settings. Despite the health benefits of being in nature, nature prescriptions within the context of Indigenous Peoples have received little attention. We therefore sought to answer the following question: What are circumpolar-based physicians’ and Indigenous Elders’ views on nature prescribing in the Northwest Territories, Canada? We carried out thirteen semi-structured interviews with physicians between May 2022 and March 2023, and one sharing circle with Indigenous Elders in February 2023. Separate reflexive thematic analysis was carried out to generate key themes through inductive coding of the data. The main themes identified from the physician interviews included the importance of cultural context; barriers with nature prescriptions in the region; and the potential for nature prescriptions in the North. Reflections shared by the Elders included the need for things to be done in the right way; the sentiment that the Land is not just an experience but a way of life; and the importance of traditional food as a connection with Nature. With expanding nature prescription programs, key considerations are needed when serving Indigenous communities. Further investigation is warranted to ensure that nature prescriptions are appropriate within a given context, are inclusive of supporting Land-based approaches to health and wellbeing, and are considered within the context of Indigenous self-determination.
... Furthermore, too many worries, too much time spent in bright light in the evening, and too little physical activity can result in poor sleep that disrupts our CR (Fernández et al., 2023). Modern man spends more than 87% of his time indoors, with an average of 2.5 h outdoors, half of which is often after sunset (White et al., 2019). Tähkämö et al. (2019) claim that an indoor environment (artificial light) can derange our CR. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective Health can be described as the state of homeostasis and optimal functioning across various bio-psycho-social dimensions and processes, allowing an individual to adapt and respond effectively to extrinsic and intrinsic challenges. Our thoughts, choices, behaviors, experiences, and feelings shape our existence. By transitioning from unconscious reactions to conscious responses, we can establish novel habits and behaviors, actively embracing positive shifts in our lifestyle. Subjects and methods The presented examination focuses on the smartwatch (SW), analyzing the incorporation of potentially progressive attributes that could enrich our lifestyle pursuits. The objective is not the health disorders themselves but the employment of wearable devices to create a strong sense of coherence in the Straussian grounded theory approach. The study had no subjects. Results The potential of the SW has been partially explored in lifestyle intervention, modification, research, and practice. Conclusion Based on our examination, creating an innovative SW capable of aiding individuals in better comprehending their behaviors and motivating them toward comprehensive changes in their lifestyle is a challenging yet attainable endeavor. Our ambition is to bring into existence SW capable of comprehensively measuring and evaluating interoception, circadian rhythm (CR), selected lifestyle pillars, and their associated components, and seamlessly integrating them into current SW features. It focuses on boosting motivation, maintenance, and amelioration regarding one’s lifestyle. The novel approach strives to boost both immediate and underlying factors that actively contribute to improving one’s metacognition.
... In addition to its physical benefits, swimming has positive effects on mental health and psychological well-being, which are particularly relevant within the context of recreational fisheries. The immersive experience of swimming in natural water bodies, such as lakes, rivers, or oceans, provides a sense of tranquility and connection with nature (White et al., 2018). Studies have shown that exposure to natural environments, including aquatic settings, can reduce stress, anxiety, and symptoms of depression (Barton and Pretty, 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
This review examines the pros of swimming for human health and recreational fisheries in the blue economy. It explores the various benefits of both activities and how they contribute to economic growth, environmental sustainability, and social well-being. The article also discusses the challenges and opportunities associated with promoting these activities within the blue economy framework. Swimming as one of the most popular modes of physical activity, it confers significant physical health benefits for both healthy individuals and those with disease; these health benefits extend across the entire life-course-as it has presented evidence that demonstrates favourable effects of aquatic exercise from foetus through to the frail elderly. There is a copious data considering aquatic exercise per se, there is relatively much less exploring the effects of swimming as a unique activity. It is imperative that this is addressed in future research, particularly given the compelling evidence presented that swimmers demonstrate lower all-cause mortality rates, and reduced incidence of falls in later life. The unique nature of the aquatic environment as a medium for exercise and physical activity has been comprehensively described. What is evident is that water-based exercise can confer a number of specific advantages, as compared to land-based exercise. As an environment that offers reduced weight-bearing stress, higher humidity levels, decreased heat load and a greater margin of therapeutic safety in terms of falls-risk, swimming/aquatic exercise is extremely well-placed to safely and effectively meet the needs of a wide-range of individuals, in both the treatment and prevention of physical health issues. Water-based exercise prescription should be a key consideration for health care clinicians and commissioners as we tap into the blue economy wealth.
... Only a few outdoor work activities per week could make a big difference. As little as two hours of nature visits a week have been shown to have an impact upon health and well-being, with the benefits peaking at 200-300 minutes per week (White et al., 2019). OOW does not demand a whole forest or grand park, but green spaces and pedestrian paths, without threats or noise from traffic or machines. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Why don't we usually work outdoors? While some professions require it, most white-collar workers remain indoors, sedentary, and screen-bound. Yet, the potential benefits of outdoor work on health, well-being, learning, and creativity are significant and underexplored, especially given the demands of today's knowledge-intensive work life. The aim of this thesis is to explore the potential of integrating the outdoors into everyday work life by bringing office work outdoors. To accomplish this, the thesis identifies ways by which the potential of outdoor office work may be enabled, in this case through an interactive research approach. This exploration was conducted through the ‘StickUt Malmö’ interactive research project, the Danish ‘Pop Out!’ project, and an interview study within the project ‘Concepts for the Sustainable Office of the Future’, culminating in four papers and a final discussion. In ‘Outdoor Office Work – An Interactive Research Project Showing the Way Out’ (Paper I) it was shown that a range of work activities could be brought outdoors, both individually and collaboratively, and that outdoor office work (OOW) was associated with experiences of well-being, recovery, autonomy, enhanced cognition, better communication, and social relations, but also with feelings of guilt and illegitimacy. Conditions of importance were found in the physical environment, and in the organizational culture, and not the least concerned managers’ attitudes towards OOW, and to autonomy and trust in general. In ‘Greenspace as Workplace – Benefits, Challenges and Essentialities in the Physical Environment’ (Paper II) insights into key aspects of the physical environment relevant for the experienced benefits and challenges of workers exploring OOW were captured through the themes of Simplicity, Safeness, Comfort and Contact with Nature, whereas Sociality, Well-being, and Functioning stood out as the main experienced benefits, and Digital dependency and Illegitimacy as challenges to overcome. Based on an interview study with white-collar workers practicing OOW on a regular basis, a conceptual framework was developed and presented in ‘Unlocking the Transformative Potential of Outdoor Office Work – A Constructivist Grounded Theory Study’ (Paper III). The categories practicing outdoor office work, challenging the taken-for-granted, enjoying freedom and disconnection, feeling connected and interdependent, promoting health and well-being, enhancing performance, and finally, adding a dimension, make up ‘The Lotus of Outdoor Office Work’; a model visualizing the experienced benefits and challenges of OOW, highlighting the dynamic relationship between the practice of working outdoors on one hand, and how this challenges the system in which white-collar work traditionally takes place, on the other. Through a combined theoretical and case discussion in ‘Critical Interaction – Challenging Dualisms by Bringing Office Work Outdoors’ (Paper IV), the possibilities and circumstances under which an interactive research approach may be critical and contribute to learning and knowledge development in the context of exploring OOW was elaborated upon. It was concluded that an interactive research approach can do so by challenging taken-for-granted dualisms, being critical with support from closeness as well as distance, engaging in both action and reflection, and by encompassing both practical and academic interests and outcomes. In conclusion, exploring OOW is a relevant and sustainable way to enhance work life. However, challenges exist, such as the need for a supportive outdoor environment and an organizational culture that values trust and autonomy. The primary challenge lies in societal norms that separate mind and body, perpetuating the belief that white-collar work is incompatible with physical activity. The exploration of OOW turns ideas about work inside out, revealing that only certain activities are traditionally recognized as ‘work’.
... This challenge arises from the transformation in consumer behavior and consumption patterns. It is relevant to acknowledge the existing reserve among customers to deviate from conventional consumption practices toward more sustainable alternatives (White et al., 2019). The circular economy, as promoted by the Ellen MacArthur Foundation, departs from the linear economic model, replacing the extractive "Take-Make-Waste" approach. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to foster the role of circular economy integration in businesses, emphasizing the crucial link between adopting circular practices and achieving economic, environmental, and social performance. It analyzes the role of circular economy integration in businesses, highlighting its potential to foster an awareness mindset regarding the interconnectedness of Earth's systems and the inevitable consequences of negative impacts on various aspects of life. A questionnaire was completed by a sample of 70 participants in Kosovo. The analysis of the questionnaire responses revealed noteworthy findings regarding the impact of circular economy integration on firm performance. The research findings demonstrate a positive relationship between the opportunities for adopting circular economy practices and economic performance. Given these findings, it becomes crucial to establish a comprehensive framework encompassing legal, economic, technological, infrastructural, and educational elements to transform mentalities and cultures surrounding circular economy adoption. From a legal standpoint, policymakers should develop supportive regulations and incentives to encourage businesses to adopt circular practices.
... parks, forests, gardens, fields) or blue spaces (i.e. lakes, rivers, oceans) are well documented (1)(2)(3). However, climate change is disrupting that relationship (4)(5)(6)(7). ...
Article
Full-text available
Messages regarding climate change that are intended to stimulate responsible engagement can impact our mental health in both positive and negative ways, which in turn can increase or limit the potential engagement being sought through those very messages. Increasingly alarmist environmental metaphors are being brought into question due to their possibly detrimental impact on mental health and well-being, and in their place, relational environmental metaphors are proffered to instill hopeful and constructive individual and collective engagement for responsible climate action. This article discusses how both alarmist and relational environmental metaphors interact with eco-emotions. It proposes, in light of concepts arising from Porges’ Polyvagal Theory − on the psychophysiology of autonomic states created in contexts of threatening cues and feelings of safety and connection −, that relational environmental metaphors are preferable for stimulating responsible collective engagement and fostering global well-being in the midst of climate change.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction It is known that exposure to the natural environment may positively modulate mental processes and behaviors; in particular, it can reduce stress, anxiety, and depressive symptoms. This suggests a potential integration of “nature experience” into the treatment for substance use disorder (SUD) since various types of addiction are associated with anxiety and depression. Considering that only one study has been reported to date in patients with alcohol use disorder, the effect of nature experience in SUD patients' needs to be further investigated. This study aimed to test the effects of exposure to a natural lagoon environment on craving and measures of wellbeing in SUD patients in comparison to exposure to an urban environment. Methods Twenty-four SUD patients were divided into three groups of eight participants and exposed to two walking sessions (interspersed with a 1-week wash-out period) in a natural environment typical of the Venetian lagoon, an Urban walk, or staying at the residential center based on a Latin-square design. Before and after each session, drug craving, mood, wellbeing, agency, openness to the future, and restorativeness were assessed. Results The Nature walk significantly decreased craving in participants compared to their pre-walk values, and compared to craving after the Urban walk, with the latter significantly increased vs. pre-walk values. The Nature walk significantly decreased negative mood and increased wellbeing and agency. Openness to the future and restorativeness measures showed significant improvement after the Nature walk compared to the Urban walk. On the other hand, craving scores after the Urban Walk positively correlated with negative mood and a Sense of Negative Agency values and negatively correlated with wellbeing scores. Discussion Our results confirm that “nature experience” may improve mood, wellbeing, attention, stress relief, openness, and sense of being active in SUD patients. Moreover, we also showed a specific effect on drug craving—a key symptom of SUD.
Article
Full-text available
Increasing evidence indicates contact with nature supports mental, physical and social health. However, beyond a widely reported number of barriers to nature contact, the constellation of motivations for human contact with nature is under‐theorised and under‐studied. We begin to develop indicators of autonomous and controlled motivations for nature contact informed by self‐determination theory. These include intrinsic motivation (i.e. enjoyment), integrated regulation (alignment with identify and life goals), identified regulation (a means to an end), introjected regulation (emotional reasons like guilt avoidance) and external regulation (such as peer pressure). We compare these motivation indices in a nationally representative sample of 5082 adults in Australia in 2022 with the Nature Relatedness Scale (NR6), and also test associations between them and five outcomes: time spent in nature, smartphone use in nature, interest in nature prescriptions, physical activity and self‐rated health. Statistical analyses were adjusted for potential confounding. Results demonstrate people have complex mixtures of motivations with varying potency for visiting natural settings and the extent to which those motives are autonomous or controlled matters for what they do, and the benefits accrued. For example, our analyses show that more direct considerations of intrinsic, integrated and identified forms of autonomous motivation have superior explanatory power than the NR6 for time spent in nature, interest in nature prescriptions, adherence to physical activity recommendations and self‐rated health. External regulations emphasising peer approval were associated not only with no additional time in nature but also with more distractive activities when in natural environments, as defined by more smartphone and social media use while there. While introjected regulations emphasising guilt avoidance were associated with increased nature contact, they were similarly associated with time spent on smartphones and social media when in natural environments, which has been shown to undermine restoration. Synthesis and applications: We need to formally measure autonomous and controlled motivations for nature contact to better understand both why some people visit natural environments, and whether they are mindfully maximising the health benefits of those experiences. This will help to inform robust nature‐based interventions that are acceptable, effective and sustainable for everyone. Read the free Plain Language Summary for this article on the Journal blog.
Article
Full-text available
Background: At a time of increasing disconnectedness from nature, scientific interest in the potential health benefits of nature contact has grown. Research in recent decades has yielded substantial evidence, but large gaps remain in our understanding. Objectives: We propose a research agenda on nature contact and health, identifying principal domains of research and key questions that, if answered, would provide the basis for evidence-based public health interventions. Discussion: We identify research questions in seven domains: a) mechanistic biomedical studies; b) exposure science; c) epidemiology of health benefits; d) diversity and equity considerations; e) technological nature; f) economic and policy studies; and g) implementation science. Conclusions: Nature contact may offer a range of human health benefits. Although much evidence is already available, much remains unknown. A robust research effort, guided by a focus on key unanswered questions, has the potential to yield high-impact, consequential public health insights. https://doi.org/10.1289/EHP1663.
Article
Full-text available
Exposure to nature provides a wide range of health benefits. A significant proportion of these are delivered close to home, because this offers an immediate and easily accessible opportunity for people to experience nature. However, there is limited information to guide recommendations on its management and appropriate use. We apply a nature dose-response framework to quantify the simultaneous association between exposure to nearby nature and multiple health benefits. We surveyed ca. 1000 respondents in Southern England, UK, to determine relationships between (a) nature dose type, that is the frequency and duration (time spent in private green space) and intensity (quantity of neighbourhood vegetation cover) of nature exposure and (b) health outcomes, including mental, physical and social health, physical behaviour and nature orientation. We then modelled dose-response relationships between dose type and self-reported depression. We demonstrate positive relationships between nature dose and mental and social health, increased physical activity and nature orientation. Dose-response analysis showed that lower levels of depression were associated with minimum thresholds of weekly nature dose. Nearby nature is associated with quantifiable health benefits, with potential for lowering the human and financial costs of ill health. Dose-response analysis has the potential to guide minimum and optimum recommendations on the management and use of nearby nature for preventative healthcare.
Article
Background: There is increasing policy interest in the potential for vegetation in urban areas to mitigate harmful effects of air pollution on respiratory health. We aimed to quantify relationships between tree and green space density and asthma-related hospitalisations, and explore how these varied with exposure to background air pollution concentrations. Methods: Population standardised asthma hospitalisation rates (1997-2012) for 26,455 urban residential areas of England were merged with area-level data on vegetation and background air pollutant concentrations. We fitted negative binomial regression models using maximum likelihood estimation to obtain estimates of asthma-vegetation relationships at different levels of pollutant exposure. Results: Green space and gardens were associated with reductions in asthma hospitalisation when pollutant exposures were lower but had no significant association when pollutant exposures were higher. In contrast, tree density was associated with reduced asthma hospitalisation when pollutant exposures were higher but had no significant association when pollutant exposures were lower. Conclusions: We found differential effects of natural environments at high and low background pollutant concentrations. These findings can provide evidence for urban planning decisions which aim to leverage health co-benefits from environmental improvements.
Article
Despite growing interest in the relationships between natural environments and subjective wellbeing (SWB), previous studies have various methodological and theoretical limitations. Focusing on urban/peri-urban residents (n=7272) from a nationally representative survey of the English population, we explored the relationships between three types of exposure: i) ‘neighbourhood exposure’, ii) ‘visit frequency’, and iii) ‘specific visit’; and four components of SWB: i) evaluative, ii) eudaimonic, iii) positive experiential and iv) negative experiential. Controlling for area and individual level socio-demographics and other aspects of SWB, visit frequency was associated with eudaimonic wellbeing and a specific visit with positive experiential wellbeing. People who visited nature regularly felt their lives were more worthwhile, and those who visited nature yesterday were happier. The magnitude of the association between weekly nature visits and eudaimonic wellbeing was similar to that between eudaimonic wellbeing and life circumstances such as marital status. Findings are relevant for policies to protect and promote public access to natural environments.
Article
Myopia is one of the major causes of low visual acuity during childhood, and hence of the need for spectacles. It is generally more prevalent in urban areas where children are often less exposed to green spaces than in rural areas. This study evaluated the association between exposure to green space and use of spectacles (as a surrogate measure for myopia) in a cohort of 2727 schoolchildren (7–10 years old) recruited from 39 primary schools in Barcelona (2012–2015). We assessed exposure to green spaces by characterizing outdoor surrounding greenness at home and school and during commuting using satellite data on greenness (Normalized Difference Vegetation Index). We also obtained data on the annual average time children spent playing in green spaces through questionnaires. Cross-sectional analyses were conducted based on prevalent cases of spectacles use at baseline data collection campaign and longitudinal analyses based on incident cases of spectacles use during the three-year period between the baseline and last data collection campaigns. An interquartile range (IQR) increase in exposure to green space at home (500 m buffer) and school and during commuting was associated with respectively 14% (95% CI: 2%, 26%), 27% (95% CI: 6%, 44%), and 20% (95% CI: 5%, 33%) decrease in spectacles use in cross-sectional analyses. In longitudinal analyses, we observed a reduction of 23% (95% CI: 4%, 39%) and 34% (95% CI: 2%, 55%) associated with an IQR increase in greenness at home and school, respectively. Moreover, an IQR increase in time playing in green spaces was associated with a 28% (95% CI: 7%, 45%) reduction in the risk of spectacles use in the longitudinal analysis. Our observed reduced risk of spectacles use associated with higher contact with green space calls for more refined studies of the association between green spaces and refractive errors of visions.
Article
Background: Building on evidence that natural environments (e.g. parks, woodlands, beaches) are key locations for physical activity, we estimated the total annual amount of adult recreational physical activity in England's natural environments, and assessed implications for population health. Methods: A cross-sectional analysis of six waves (2009/10-2014/5) of the nationally representative, Monitor of Engagement with the Natural Environment survey (n=280,790). The survey uses a weekly quota sample, and population weights, to estimate nature visit frequency across England, and provides details on a single, randomly selected visit (n=112,422), including: a) duration; b) activity; and c) environment type. Results: Approximately 8.23 million (95% CIs: 7.93, 8.54) adults (19.5% of the population) made at least one 'active visit' (i.e. ≥30min, ≥3 METs) to natural environments in the previous week, resulting in 1.23 billion (1.14, 1.32) 'active visits' annually. An estimated 3.20 million (3.05, 3.35) of these also reported meeting recommended physical activity guidelines (i.e. ≥5×30min a week) fully, or in part, through such visits. Active visits by this group were associated with an estimated 109,164 (101,736, 116,592) Quality Adjusted Life Years (QALYs) annually. Assuming the social value of a QALY to be £20,000, the annual value of these visits was approximately £2.18 billion (£2.03, £2.33). Results for walking were replicated using WHO's Health Economic Assessment Tool. Conclusions: Natural environments provide the context for a large proportion of England's recreational physical activity and highlight the need to protect and manage such environments for health purposes.
Article
There is growing scientific recognition that contact with nature in general, and contact with urban green more specific, have the potential to positively contribute to human health. For the purpose of developing healthy urban neighbourhoods, this raises the question how to take scientific evidence about these health benefits into account. Accessibility metrics that are well substantiated by empirical evidence are needed. This paper reviews the quantitative and qualitative aspects relevant for accessibility metrics and empirical studies addressing these aspects in relation to health. Studies comparing different types of green space indicators suggest that cumulative opportunities indicators are more consistently positively related to health than residential proximity ones. In contrast to residential proximity indicators, cumulative opportunities indicators take all the green space within a certain distance into account. Comparing results across studies proved to be hard. Green space accessibility was measured in a variety of ways and the green space indicator that was chosen was often not problematized. We feel that it is time for a more function-oriented approach. How precisely does contact with nature impact health and what type and qualities are relevant in this regard? We think this will lead to a new generation of more evidence-based accessibility metrics that will help to advance the field.