ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Lucid dreaming refers to the phenomenon of becoming aware of the fact that one is dreaming during ongoing sleep. Despite having been physiologically validated for decades, the neurobiology of lucid dreaming is still incompletely characterized. Here we review the neuroscientific literature on lucid dreaming, including electroencephalographic, neuroimaging, brain lesion, pharmacological and brain stimulation studies. Electroencephalographic studies of lucid dreaming are mostly underpowered and show mixed results. Neuroimaging data is scant but preliminary results suggest that prefrontal and parietal regions are involved in lucid dreaming. A focus of research is also to develop methods to induce lucid dreams. Combining training in mental set with cholinergic stimulation has shown promising results, while it remains unclear whether electrical brain stimulation could be used to induce lucid dreams. Finally, we discuss strategies to measure lucid dreaming, including best-practice procedures for the sleep laboratory. Lucid dreaming has clinical and scientific applications, and shows emerging potential as a methodology in the cognitive neuroscience of consciousness. Further research with larger sample sizes and refined methodology is needed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
Benjamin Bairda*
Sergio A. Mota-Rolimb
Martin Dreslerc
a Wisconsin Institute for Sleep and Consciousness, University of Wisconsin–Madison,
Madison, Wisconsin, USA
b Brain Institute, Physiology Department and Onofre Lopes University Hospital -
Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, Brazil
c Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical
Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
* Corresponding author
*Address for correspondence:
Dr. Benjamin Baird
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine and Public Health
University of Wisconsin – Madison
6001 Research Park Blvd, Madison, WI, 53719
Email: bbaird@wisc.edu
Published in Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.03.008
2
Abstract
Lucid dreaming refers to the phenomenon of becoming aware of the fact that one is
dreaming during ongoing sleep. Despite having been physiologically validated for
decades, the neurobiology of lucid dreaming is still incompletely characterized. Here we
review the neuroscientific literature on lucid dreaming, including
electroencephalographic, neuroimaging, brain lesion, pharmacological and brain
stimulation studies. Electroencephalographic studies of lucid dreaming are mostly
underpowered and show mixed results. Neuroimaging data is scant but preliminary
results suggest that prefrontal and parietal regions are involved in lucid dreaming. A
focus of research is also to develop methods to induce lucid dreams. Combining training
in mental set with cholinergic stimulation has shown promising results, while it remains
unclear whether electrical brain stimulation could be used to induce lucid dreams.
Finally, we discuss strategies to measure lucid dreaming, including best-practice
procedures for the sleep laboratory. Lucid dreaming has clinical and scientific
applications, and shows emerging potential as a methodology in the cognitive
neuroscience of consciousness. Further research with larger sample sizes and refined
methodology is needed.
Keywords: lucid dreaming; consciousness; meta-awareness; dreaming; REM sleep
3
1. Introduction
Becoming aware that one is dreaming while dreaming, what is today referred to as
lucid dreaming, has been known about since antiquity. In Western literature, it may
have first been mentioned by Aristotle in the fourth century BCE in the treatise On
dreams of his Parva Naturali, in which he states: “often when one is asleep, there is
something in consciousness which declares that what then presents itself is but a dream”
(Aristotle, 1941, p. 624). Likewise, in Eastern cultures, particularly of the south Asian
subcontinent, reports of individuals engaging in practices to cultivate awareness of
dream and sleep states date back millennia (LaBerge, 2003; Norbu and Katz, 1992;
Wallace and Hodel, 2012). These include meditative practices specifically designed to
“apprehend the dream state” (Padmasambhava, 1998, p. 156).
Although numerous references to lucid dreaming can be found throughout world
literature (see LaBerge, 1988a for an overview), the modern nomenclature of lucid
dream was not introduced until 1913 by the Dutch psychiatrist Frederik Van Eeden. In a
detailed and engaging account of his personal experiences with dreams, Van Eeden
(1913) referred to lucid dreams as dreams in which “...the reintegration of the psychic
functions is so complete that the sleeper remembers day-life and his own condition,
reaches a state of perfect awareness, and is able to direct his attention, and to attempt
different acts of free volition” (pp. 149-150). Research over the last four decades has
largely confirmed Van Eeden’s accounts: as we review below, evidence suggests that
during lucid dreams individuals can be physiologically asleep while at the same time
aware that they are dreaming, able to intentionally perform diverse actions, and in some
cases remember their waking life (Dresler et al., 2011; LaBerge, 1985, 1990; LaBerge,
2015; LaBerge, Nagel, Dement and Zarcone, 1981c; Windt, 2015).
Despite the fact that such personal accounts of lucid dreams have been described for
centuries, the topic faced skepticism from some scientists and philosophers (e.g.,
Malcolm, 1959), in part due to the lack of objective evidence for the phenomenon. This
began to change in the late 1970s and early 1980s, however, with the first validation of
lucid dreaming as an objectively verifiable phenomenon occurring during rapid eye
movement (REM) sleep. Building on prior research that showed that shifts in the
direction of gaze within a dream can be accompanied by corresponding movements of
the sleeper’s eyes (Dement and Wolpert, 1958), lucid dreamers were asked to move
4
their eyes in a distinct pre-agreed upon sequence (full-scale up-down or left-right
movements) as soon as they became lucid (Hearne, 1978; LaBerge et al., 1981c).
Through this technique, which has since become the gold standard, reports of lucid
dreams could be objectively verified by the presence of distinct volitional eye
movement patterns as recorded in the electrooculogram (EOG) during
polysomnography-verified sleep (Figure 1). The most common version of the eye
signaling technique asks participants to signal when they realize they are dreaming by
rapidly looking all the way to the left then all the way to the right two times
consecutively then back to center in the dream without pausing (referred to as left-right-
left-right eye signals, abbreviated as LRLR). As can be seen in Figure 1, the LRLR
signal is readily discernable in the horizontal EOG, which exhibits a distinctive shape
containing four consecutive full-scale eye movements that have larger amplitude
compared to typical REMs. As we describe in detail below, lucid dreams can be
validated by this method through the convergence between reports obtained after
awakening of becoming lucid and making the eye movement signals during the dream,
accompanied by the objective eye movement signals recorded in the EOG with
concurrent polysomonographic evidence of REM sleep.
The eye signaling technique also offers a way of objectively contrasting lucid REM
sleep to baseline non-lucid REM sleep, providing a method to investigate the changes in
brain activity associated with lucid dreaming. Furthermore, lucid dreamers can not only
signal to indicate that they are aware that they are dreaming, but they can also make the
eye movement signals to time-stamp the start and end of experimental tasks performed
during lucid dreams (LaBerge, 1990). By providing objective temporal markers, this
technique has opened up a new method for studying the psychophysiology of REM
sleep, allowing, for example, investigations into the neural correlates of dreamed
behaviors (e.g., Dresler et al., 2011; Erlacher, Schredl and LaBerge, 2003; LaBerge,
1990; Oudiette et al., 2018). Lucid dreaming thus provides a way to establish precise
psychophysiological correlations between the contents of consciousness during sleep
and physiological measures, as well as enables experimental control over the content of
dreams, and therefore provides a potentially highly useful experimental methodology.
While neuroscientific studies on lucid dreaming have been performed since the late
1970s, the topic has received increasing attention in recent years due to its relevance to
the emerging neuroscience of consciousness. In this article, we review the existing
5
literature on the neuroscience of lucid dreaming, including electrophysiological,
neuroimaging, brain lesion, pharmacological and brain stimulation studies.
Additionally, we review recent studies that illustrate how lucid dreaming can be used as
a methodology in the cognitive neuroscience of consciousness. Finally, we present
strategies to measure lucid dreaming both physiologically and with questionnaires, and
discuss procedures to investigate lucid dreaming in the sleep laboratory.
6
2. Electrophysiology of lucid dreaming
As noted in the introduction, the first physiological studies of lucid dreaming began
in the late 1970s and early 1980s. These pioneer works established that lucid dreams
occur in REM sleep, characterized by all of the EEG features of REM sleep according
to the Rechtschaffen and Kales (1968) sleep scoring criteria (Hearne, 1978; LaBerge et
al., 1981c). LaBerge (1980b) also observed that lucid dreams are associated with
increased physiological activation, as measured by increased phasic activity (e.g.,
increased REM density). Autonomic nervous system arousal (e.g., heart rate, respiration
rate, and skin potential) were also found to be elevated during lucid REM sleep
compared to non-lucid REM sleep (LaBerge, Levitan and Dement, 1986). Additionally,
lucid dreams were found to occur in REM sleep periods later in the night (LaBerge et
al., 1986). These findings suggest that lucid dreaming is associated with increased
cortical activation (LaBerge, Nagel, Taylor, Dement and Zarcone, 1981a), which
reaches its peak during phasic REM sleep. In addition to physiological markers of
phasic activity, lucid REM sleep was found to be associated with h-reflex suppression
(Brylowski, Levitan and LaBerge, 1989), a spinal reflex that is reliably suppressed
during REM sleep (Hodes and Dement, 1964). Together these results indicate that lucid
dreams occur in activated periods of REM sleep, as opposed to, for example, a state that
is intermediate between waking and REM sleep.
These findings also raise the further question of whether lucid REM sleep is
associated with localized activation of specific brain regions or changes in specific
frequencies of neural oscillations compared to non-lucid REM sleep. In this section, we
will review EEG studies that have attempted to address this question. As will be
discussed below, while these studies represent important first steps toward measuring
the electrophysiological changes associated with lucid dreaming, all of them have
interpretive issues and most suffer from low statistical power. As a result, there is
considerable discrepancy among findings. Below we group and discuss results based on
the regional EEG band power changes reported to be associated with lucid REM sleep
dreaming.
2.1 EEG studies of lucid REM sleep dreaming
7
2.1.1 Central and posterior alpha
Ogilvie and colleagues conducted some of the first studies to examine EEG spectral
changes during lucid REM sleep. In an early case study, Ogilvie, Hunt, Sawicki and
McGowan (1978) compared two lucid REM sleep epochs to six non-lucid REM sleep
epochs and found an increase in the percentage of alpha band (8-12 Hz) power in a
single central EEG channel. A follow-up study examined a larger group of ten
participants for two nights in the sleep laboratory (Ogilvie, Hunt, Tyson, Lucescu and
Jeakins, 1982). Participants were awakened during the two periods of highest alpha
power and two periods of lowest alpha power from a central EEG electrode during
REM sleep, after which they were asked several questions about their lucidity, “pre-
lucidity” (i.e., thoughts pertaining to dreaming without becoming lucid), and degree of
dream control. Unfortunately, statistics for the number of lucid dreams reported by
participants were not documented in this study, nor whether there was a significant
difference between conditions in the number of lucid dreams. Instead, a composite
measure was constructed by collapsing across pre-lucidity, lucidity and control, which
was significantly elevated in the high alpha trials. Therefore, the number (if any) of
lucid dreams that were captured by this procedure is unknown (see LaBerge (1988b) for
further discussion).
Subsequent research has not supported the hypothesis that lucid REM sleep is
associated with increased alpha activity. For example, in a follow-up study Tyson,
Ogilvie and Hunt (1984) found that only pre-lucid but not lucid dreams significantly
differed in alpha activity compared to non-lucid REM sleep. In another study of eight
frequent lucid dreamers using a similar experimental design, Ogilvie, Hunt, Kushniruk
and Newman (1983) observed no difference in the number of lucid dreams following
awakenings from periods of high or low alpha activity. Furthermore, in a replication
attempt of the original case report that observed an increase in alpha during lucid REM
sleep (Ogilvie et al., 1978), LaBerge and colleagues found no significant differences in
alpha power at the same central channel (C3) from a single subject (LaBerge, 1988b).
Finally, a later follow-up case study analyzed EEG spectral power in five lucid REM
sleep epochs compared to non-lucid REM sleep periods, and observed no differences in
alpha power (Ogilvie, Vieira and Small, 1991). Together this evidence does not support
a reliable association between alpha power and lucid dreaming. However, the limited
spatial coverage of EEG montages in these studies (in several cases consisting of only
8
one EEG channel) makes it unclear to what extent these results can be generalized to
other brain areas.
2.1.2 Parietal beta
Holzinger, LaBerge and Levitan (2006) examined EEG spectral changes during
lucid REM sleep in a group of eleven participants who reported prior experience with
lucid dreaming. Six out of the eleven participants succeeded in becoming lucid in the
sleep laboratory, and some during multiple REM periods, for a total of 16 signal-
verified lucid dreams. The authors found increased power in the low beta frequency
range (13-19 Hz) in parietal electrodes for lucid compared to non-lucid REM sleep. This
study has the advantage of analyzing a larger number of lucid REM sleep periods.
However, a limitation is that EEG signals were only evaluated at four electrodes (F3,
F4, P3, P4). Consequently, localized changes in EEG spectra may have been missed by
the low spatial resolution. Furthermore, due to technical limitations, the online low-pass
filter for the EEG recordings in this study was set at 35 Hz, and therefore changes in
higher-frequency activity were unable to be evaluated.
The potential functional significance of increased low beta band power in parietal
areas to lucid dreaming is not understood. Holzinger et al. (2006) speculated that the
increased beta power in the parietal EEG could reflect the understanding of the meaning
of the words “This is a dream.” As discussed below, recent neuroimaging studies have
linked parietal regions to several other cognitive functions associated with lucid
dreaming, including self-reflection (Kjaer, Nowak and Lou, 2002; Lou et al., 2004),
episodic memory (Berryhill, Phuong, Picasso, Cabeza and Olson, 2007; Wagner,
Shannon, Kahn and Buckner, 2005) and agency (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006),
suggesting several other possible interpretations of the results. Neural oscillations in this
frequency range were originally thought to reflect sensorimotor behavior; however,
evidence now suggests that oscillations in this range could play a role in cognitive
processing as well as facilitate large-scale neural integration (e.g., Donner and Siegel,
2011; Engel and Fries, 2010). If these results can be replicated in future studies, one
hypothesis is that the increased oscillatory activity in the beta band could reflect a
mechanism of integration between parietal regions and other areas, which, in some way
still to be understood, helps facilitate lucid dreaming. However, it is also possible that
differences in sensorimotor behavior between lucid and non-lucid REM sleep could
9
boost brain rhythms that overlap with this frequency range (Koshino and Niedermeyer,
1975; Pfurtscheller, Stancak and Neuper, 1996; Vanni, Portin, Virsu and Hari, 1999).
Additional research is needed to clarify these findings.
2.1.3 Frontolateral gamma
Mota-Rolim et al. (2008) and Voss, Holzmann, Tuin and Hobson (2009) reported
increased gamma band (40 Hz) power in frontolateral scalp electrodes during lucid
compared to non-lucid REM sleep in three participants each. However, besides an
unsuccessful replication in patients with narcolepsy (Dodet et al., 2014), interpretation
of these results are restricted by several experimental limitations, including the small
sample size (LaBerge, 2010). Importantly, caution is warranted in interpreting these
findings given that, as briefly discussed by Voss et al. (2009), scalp measurement of
cortical gamma, particularly when selectively localized in the frontal and periorbital
regions, may be confounded with mircrosaccades. Ocular myogenic artifacts, which
occur during both saccades and microsaccades and are distinct from the artifacts
associated with corneo-retinal dipole offsets, may confound scalp measurement of
gamma activity. One type of such artifacts is referred to as the saccadic spike potential
(SP), which occurs due to contraction of the ocular muscles during both saccades and
microsaccades (Yuval-Greenberg and Deouell, 2009; Yuval-Greenberg, Tomer, Keren,
Nelken and Deouell, 2008). The influence of the SP artifact on gamma power was
overlooked for a number of years; however, the need to account for this artifact on scalp
measurement of induced gamma activity has now been thoroughly documented (e.g.,
Hipp and Siegel, 2013; Keren, Yuval-Greenberg and Deouell, 2010).
Voss et al. (2009) corrected for eye movement artifacts using regression of the EOG
signal (Gratton, Coles and Donchin, 1983) and by computing current source densities
(CSD) in addition to scalp potentials. However, regression-based correction procedures
are insufficient to remove the SP artifact, and while the CSD derivation attenuates the
SP artifact at posterior channels, it is not sufficient to remove it at anterior scalp
locations (Keren et al., 2010; Yuval-Greenberg and Deouell, 2009). As Keren et al.
(2010) state: “In conclusion, SCD [CSD] seems to be effective in attenuating the SP
effect at posterior sites. However at sites anterior to Cz and closer to the orbits efficacy
gradually decreases, preserving the temporal and spectral signature of the SP and its
amplitude relative to baseline.” (p. 2258). The influence of the SP artifact on gamma
10
band power in the comparison of lucid to non-lucid REM sleep is particularly relevant
given that, as noted above, lucid REM sleep has been associated with increased phasic
activation and higher eye movement density (e.g., LaBerge, 1990).
It is important to note that the need to account for the SP artifact does not preclude a
potential association between increased frontal gamma power and lucid REM sleep.
Indeed, given the link between gamma band power, local field potentials (LFPs) and the
blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) signal (Lachaux et al., 2007; Nir et al., 2007), if
the transition from non-lucid to lucid REM sleep involves activation or recruitment of
additional frontal brain regions (a plausible hypothesis, also in light of the findings of
Dresler et al. (2012); see Neuroimaging of lucid dreaming below), regional increases in
gamma power might be predicted. However, the spatial topography and frequency
localization of any such effects are likely to be strongly influenced by the correction and
removal of the SP artifact.
With the aforementioned considerations in mind, more research is needed to clarify
whether the increase in frontolateral gamma power during lucid REM sleep observed by
Mota-Rolim et al. (2008) and Voss et al. (2009) reflects ocular myogenic or neural
activity. Furthermore, future studies evaluating the relationship between lucid REM
sleep and gamma activity from sensor-level EEG, particularly at anterior electrodes,
need to control for the SP artifact in order for the results to be interpretable. Several
methods have been shown to be suitable for removal this artifact, including direct
identification and removal of contaminated data by rejecting overlapping windows of
time-frequency transformed data or data correction using independent component
analysis (Hipp and Siegel, 2013). A study that evaluates the effect of direct removal
and/or correction of the SP artifact on gamma power during lucid contrasted with
baseline REM sleep would be an important addition to the literature. In summary,
studies that rigorously control for myogenic artifacts, and in particular the SP artifact,
are needed before conclusions can be drawn regarding the relationship between lucid
REM sleep and frontal gamma activity.
2.1.4 Fronto-central delta
Dodet, Chavez, Leu-Semenescu, Golmard and Arnulf (2014) evaluated changes in
EEG band power during lucid REM sleep in a group of narcoleptic patients. Given that
11
narcoleptic patients often report a high rate of lucid dreams (Rak, Beitinger, Steiger,
Schredl and Dresler, 2015), they are a potentially useful population for cognitive
neuroscience studies of lucid dreaming. In the experiment, while both control and
narcoleptic patients reported achieving lucidity and performing the LRLR signals
during overnight and afternoon nap recordings, only the eye signals of the patients
during the nap recordings could be unambiguously identified. This is likely at least
partially attributable to the specific instructions used for making the eye movement
signal (see Section 9 below for discussion of this issue). Despite this, the study
succeeded in recording 14 signal-verified lucid dreams during naps from seven
narcoleptic patients. The main finding was that EEG power was reduced in the delta
band during lucid REM sleep at frontal and central electrodes. The study also reported
that the coherence between several electrodes was reduced in lucid compared to non-
lucid REM sleep in delta, theta, beta and gamma bands, but these differences are
difficult to interpret since no statistics were reported for this analysis and no corrections
for multiple comparisons were made.
A limitation of the Dodet et al. (2014) study is that EEG signals were only evaluated
at six electrodes, and only in frontal and central scalp regions (Fp1, Fp2, F7, F8, C3,
C4). Parietal electrodes were not included in the EEG montage, and occipital channels
reportedly could not be evaluated due to noise. Thus, it is possible that local changes in
EEG spectra in posterior regions, such as parietal or occipital areas, were missed due to
the limited electrode montage.
The finding of lower delta activity during lucid REM sleep is in line with previous
observations that lucid dreams tend to occur during periods of increased cortical
activation (LaBerge, 1990). Specifically, slow waves, reflected by delta (~0.5-4 Hz)
power in the EEG, are associated with neuronal down states (“off periods) in which
neurons are hyperpolarized (Steriade, Timofeev and Grenier, 2001). Decreased delta
power (EEG activation) therefore reflects recovery of neural activity. While the bi-
stability between “on” and off” periods is a central feature of non-REM sleep, slow
wave activity has also been observed in REM sleep (Baird et al., 2018b; Funk, Honjoh,
Rodriguez, Cirelli and Tononi, 2016). Neuronal down states have also been linked to
the loss of consciousness during both anesthesia and sleep (Purdon et al., 2013; Tononi
and Massimini, 2008), which is hypothesized to be related to the breakdown of causal
interactions between bi-stable neurons (Pigorini et al., 2015; Tononi, Boly, Massimini
12
and Koch, 2016). Therefore, one potential explanation is that this finding reflects
reduced bi-stability and increased causal interactions between cortical neurons in these
areas during lucid REM sleep. Notably, reduced delta power in posterior cortex has
been found to be associated with dreaming as opposed to dreamless sleep in both REM
and NREM sleep (Siclari et al., 2017). An intriguing speculation based on these results
is therefore that this reduction in delta power also extends to frontal regions during lucid
REM sleep dreaming. However, it remains to be seen whether these findings can be
replicated and whether the results generalize to non-clinical populations.
2.2. General discussion of EEG studies of lucid REM sleep
In summary, EEG studies show substantial disagreement regarding the spatial and
spectral changes associated with lucid dreaming. As reviewed above, different studies
have observed an increase in central or posterior alpha, parietal beta, frontolateral
gamma or a reduction in frontocentral delta during lucid compared to baseline REM
sleep. Aside from the general uncertainty in the results of some studies due to low
statistical power, these discrepant results might be partially explained by the use of
limited electrode montages and evaluation of different EEG frequency bands. In the
spatial domain in particular, many studies have used less than six scalp electrodes, in
several cases only covering some scalp regions but not others, precluding analysis of
EEG activity in regions in which significant effects were found in other studies. For
instance, the study by Dodet et al. (2014) did not include electrodes in parietal or
occipital regions, precluding the possibility to replicate the findings of increased parietal
beta by Holzinger et al. (2006). Studies by Ogilvie et al. (1978, 1982) evaluated only a
single central EEG channel (C3). These non-overlapping spatial montages limit the
comparison of results across some of these studies.
Another factor that might contribute to the discrepant findings is the fact that lucid
dreaming can be achieved and executed in different ways. For example, the observed
changes in the EEG during lucid REM sleep might depend in part on the degree of
vividness, working memory, emotional tone, self-consciousness, attention and insight,
which could vary across individuals as well as specific dreams. Relatedly, different
subjective experiences and contents during lucid dreams plausibly have their own
neurobiological substrates (Mota-Rolim, Erlacher, Tort, Araujo and Ribeiro, 2010), just
as in non-lucid dreams (Siclari et al., 2017). Changes in brain activity during lucidity
13
may also partly depend on how experienced the lucid dreamer is. For instance, lucid
dreams of less experienced individuals may often be more ephemeral and involve less
control over dream content, while more experienced lucid dreamers may be more likely
to have longer and more stable lucid dreams, as well as the capacity to exert greater
amounts of control. This might lead to a more distinct signal in the EEG for experienced
lucid dreamers on the one hand, but also presumably less neural activity related to the
effort needed to maintain the state (neural efficiency). In line with this, Dodet et al.
(2014) suggested that the mental effort needed to achieve and sustain lucidity might be
reduced in narcoleptic patients, who may access the lucid REM sleep state with less
effort.
These comments should not be taken to indicate that there is not a consistent
neurobiology of lucid dreaming. However, it does suggest that analysis of the EEG
spectral changes associated with lucid dreaming used in previous studies may need to be
optimized for detecting more subtle and localized effects. All studies reviewed above
have measured the average power over a given spectral band and region over
comparably long time intervals. However, it is possible that lucid dreaming is
associated with spectral changes that can only be detected by a better time resolved
analysis, such as time-frequency analysis, that may be overlooked by averaging over
large windows in time or frequency space.
Furthermore, these considerations emphasize the need for more careful assessment of
the phenomenology of lucid dreams. In this regard, we would like to note that it is
plausible that there are at least two different neural signatures associated with lucid
dreaming. The first captures what might be termed the “moment of lucidity”—that is,
the transient moment of meta-awareness in which one has the metacognitive insight that
one is currently dreaming (Schooler, 2002). The second captures potential sustained
differences in brain activity between lucid and non-lucid REM sleep dreaming. This
second neural signature is unlikely to be a signature of meta-awareness per se, as during
lucid dreams individuals do not continuously engage in metacognitive reflection on
their state of consciousness. Rather, this second signature captures the “state-shift” in
consciousness that occurs from non-lucid to lucid dreaming, with enhanced volition,
episodic memory and accessibility of metacognition (Dresler et al., 2014; Spoormaker,
Czisch and Dresler, 2010). Changes in these aspects of cognition in the shift to lucidity
have been hypothesized to reflect an overall change in the conscious experience of
14
being a cognitive subject (Windt and Metzinger, 2007). Both the physiological
correlates of the moment of lucidity as well as overall differences in brain activity
between lucid and non-lucid dreams are interesting research targets. However, these
research targets are at least conceptually distinct, a point that has, in our view, not
received adequate attention in the research literature. To our knowledge, no studies have
yet evaluated differences in brain activity with EEG or functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI) specifically associated with the moment of lucidity, and this remains an
interesting question for future work.
Larger sample sizes are needed in future studies to achieve adequate statistical
power. One way to approach this would be to undertake more extensive population
screening for high-frequency lucid dreamers. For example, through mass surveys,
thousands of potential participants could be screened and the top few percent reporting
the highest lucid dream frequency could be selected for training before undergoing sleep
laboratory recordings. As we discuss below, new techniques for lucid dream induction
also have the potential to enable efficient collection of larger datasets.
Overall, studies with higher statistical power, better assessment of phenomenological
content, higher spatial resolution EEG montages, and more sophisticated analysis of the
EEG signal will be needed to address these issues and shed light on the conflicting
findings of EEG studies of lucid dreams conducted to date. Studies using high-density
EEG would also be valuable, enabling both higher temporal as well as higher spatial
resolution analysis of neural oscillatory activity. Source modeling of such data could
also potentially be informative for localizing changes in neural oscillations associated
with lucid dreaming to specific cortical areas, though it remains unclear whether
methods for source localization will be able to produce a valid specification of the
generators relevant for lucid dreaming. In particular, the generators might be distributed
widely in the brain and active concurrently.
Another interesting question with respect to the electrophysiology of lucid dreaming
is whether there are possible sleep pattern traits that are associated with lucid dreaming.
It would be informative to investigate whether there are common sleep patterns seen in
frequent lucid dreamers compared to non-frequent lucid dreamers. For example, do
frequent lucid dreamers tend to have more phasic REM sleep, or more fragmented REM
sleep with a higher number of transitions (especially gradual transitions) between REM
15
sleep and waking? As far as we know, no study has investigated lucid dreaming with
this approach; therefore, studies addressing these questions would be valuable.
2.3 EEG studies of lucid dreaming during non-REM sleep
The activated EEG of REM sleep was originally thought to be exclusively associated
with dreaming (Antrobus and Antrobus, 1967; Dement and Wolpert, 1958), while the
low-frequency activity of non-REM (NREM) sleep was thought to be associated with
the absence of dreaming. Subsequent research has shown, however, that participants
report dreams or related forms of sleep mentation in up to 70% of awakenings from
NREM sleep (Siclari et al., 2017; Siclari, LaRocque, Postle and Tononi, 2013;
Stickgold, Malia, Fosse and Hobson, 2001). NREM dreams tend to be less emotional
and visually vivid, as well as more thought-like (Cavallero, Cicogna, Natale,
Occhionero and Zito, 1992; Hobson, Pace-Schott and Stickgold, 2000). Research
suggests that lucid dreams, on the other hand, are predominantly a REM sleep
phenomenon (LaBerge et al., 1986; LaBerge et al., 1981c). However, this does not
imply that lucid dreams cannot occur during NREM sleep. There have been several
reports of lucid dreams during NREM sleep stages N1 (transition from wake state to
sleep) and N2 (consolidated light sleep), although in many of the published cases it is
uncertain whether the lucid episode occurred in an unambiguous stage of NREM sleep
(Dane and Van de Caslte, 1984; LaBerge, 1980b, 1990; LaBerge et al., 1981c;
Stumbrys and Erlacher, 2012).
In one study, LaBerge (1980b) recorded polysomnography from a single subject who
reported frequently experiencing lucid dreams at sleep onset. The participant rested
quietly while drifting to sleep and upon falling asleep was awakened and asked for a
dream report. Forty-two dream reports were collected over three nights, 25 of which the
participant reported as a lucid dream, and all of which reportedly occurred during stage
N1. However, the dream reports were mostly short “dreamlets”, thus it is plausible that
these N1 lucid dreams could differ phenomenologically from REM sleep lucid dreams.
Furthermore, none of these lucid dreams were verified with eye movement signals. In
another study, two participants reported lucid dreams upon spontaneous awakening
from N1 and N2 (LaBerge et al., 1981c). In the N2 instance, the participant reported
16
only a brief moment of lucidity just before waking up. Furthermore, the participant did
not make eye movement signals to time-stamp the moment of lucidity, and it is
therefore difficult to ascertain whether the moment of lucidity occurred in the process of
awakening. The N1 case was also ambiguous: while in this case the participant reported
making eye movements to signal lucidity, the signals could not be verified on the
polysomnogram.
In a study on the effects of posthypnotic suggestion on lucid dreaming, Dane and
Van de Caslte (1984) tested hypnotically susceptible females with no prior experience
in lucid dreaming. Importantly for the present discussion, lucid dreams were reported
following awakening from both REM and NREM sleep. Five lucid dreams were
reported in total from stage N2, but in all cases the LRLR eye signal occurred after
arousal/awakening, and thus none of these could be objectively confirmed. However,
several N1 lucid dreams were confirmed by LRLR signaling. This study was thus the
first to provide objective evidence for lucid dreaming during stage 1 NREM sleep.
However, as noted above, how these N1 dreams compare phenomenologically to REM
sleep lucid dreams remains unclear.
Stumbrys and Erlacher (2012) reported two potential cases of lucid dreams during
NREM with eye signaling. However, due to the study protocol, the experimenters could
only collect dream reports the following morning. In the first case, the participant
reported the next morning making an eye movement signal in a lucid dream early in the
night, but given the long amount of time between the report and the signal there is
uncertainty whether the report corresponds to the observed signal during NREM sleep.
Furthermore, while the two 30-second polysomnography epochs for sleep scoring
preceding the eye signal appear to be unambiguous N2 sleep, the EEG dynamic shifts
during the 30-second epoch containing the eye-signal to lower amplitude activity
without apparent spindles or K-complexes. Without knowing the stage of the epochs
following the eye signal, which was not reported in the study, it is possible that the eye
signal occurred in a transitional sleep stage. In the second case reported by Stumbrys
and Erlacher (2012), the eye signals occurred with some signs of arousal and the
participant had no memory of executing the eye signals the following morning. These
data therefore provide ambiguous evidence for signal-verified lucid dreams during
NREM sleep.
17
In three further case reports of eye movement signaled NREM lucid dreams, one
case was reported to occur in N1 and two cases in N2 visually scored sleep stages
(Mota-Rolim et al., 2015). The first case was scored as N1, since an increase in theta (4-
7 Hz) and a decrease in alpha (7-14 Hz) power was observed in more than half of the 30
second scoring epoch, meeting the AASM criteria for classification of stage N1 sleep.
The other two cases were scored as occurring during N2 episodes since they had
spindles and K-complexesin the 30 second scoring epoch with the eye signals. These
data thus replicate signal-verification of N1 lucid dreams and provide preliminary
evidence for signal-verified N2 lucid dreams.
Together, these results suggest that although most lucid dreams occur during REM
sleep, they can also occur during NREM sleep. However, additional studies providing
objective evidence of NREM lucid dreams confirmed by eye-signaling, particularly in
N2 sleep, are needed. Currently there are no reports of lucid dreams recorded during
NREM stage 3 (N3), also known as deep sleep, or slow wave sleep. While there are
intriguing reports of practitioners of both Transcendental Meditation and Tibetan Dream
Yoga claiming to have developed the ability to maintain a type of lucid awareness
throughout the entire sleep cycle, including also states of “lucid dreamless sleep”
(Gackenbach, Cranson and Alexander, 1986; Mason and Orme-Johnson, 2010; Wallace,
2013; Windt, Nielsen and Thompson, 2016), these claims have not been corroborated
with physiological measures. However, several studies have found a relationship
between meditation practice and lucid dreaming (e.g., Baird, Riedner, Boly, Davidson
and Tononi, 2018c; Gackenbach et al., 1986; Mota-Rolim et al., 2013; Stumbrys,
Erlacher and Malinowski, 2015), which could be due to changes in REM sleep patterns
induced by meditation practice and/or to neurocognitive changes associated with
meditation practice (e.g., increased mental control or meta-awareness).
18
3. Neuroimaging of lucid dreaming
As noted, dream-like mental activity can be observed during all sleep stages.
However, REM sleep dreams tend to be more vivid, emotional, bizarre, and more often
include a narrative structure (Cavallero et al., 1992; Hobson et al., 2000). These
phenomenological characteristics have been suggested to be associated with the neural
activation and deactivation patterns observed during REM sleep (e.g., Nir and Tononi,
2010). For example, higher visual areas show increased regional cerebral blood flow
during REM sleep compared to both wakefulness and slow wave sleep (Braun et al.,
1998), which is in line with the visuospatial experiences that are common during REM
sleep dreaming (e.g., Windt, 2010). Additionally, the amygdala, medial prefrontal
cortex and anterior cingulate cortex show increased regional cerebral blood flow during
REM sleep (Braun et al., 1997; Maquet et al., 1996). All of these brain areas have been
implicated in emotional processing, mirroring the intense emotions that can be
experienced in REM sleep dreams. In contrast, the anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC) and
parietal cortex, including the inferior parietal lobule and precuneus, show low regional
cerebral blood flow during normal REM sleep (Braun et al., 1997; Maquet et al., 1996).
Deactivation of these regions has been postulated to underlie the diminished insight into
the global state of consciousness and restricted volitional control typical of non-lucid
dreaming (e.g., Hobson and Pace-Schott, 2002; Nir and Tononi, 2010).
3.1 Neuroimaging lucid REM sleep dreaming
At the current time, there is only one fMRI study of lucid REM sleep, and it is a case
study (Dresler et al., 2012). Two separate signal-verified lucid dreams were recorded
from a single subject during EEG-verified REM sleep inside the MRI scanner.
Compared to non-lucid REM sleep, lucid REM sleep showed increased fMRI BOLD
signal in a number of cortical regions, including the superior frontal gyrus, aPFC,
medial and lateral parietal cortex, inferior/middle temporal gyri and occipital cortex
(Figure 2a). Interestingly, several of these regions, particularly the parietal regions and
frontal pole, are areas that, as noted above, consistently show reduced regional cerebral
blood flow during non-lucid REM sleep compared to wakefulness (Nir and Tononi,
2010).
19
Evidence linking frontopolar cortex to lucid dreaming is consistent with a role of
this region in metacognition and self-reflection. For example, research has found that
aPFC shows increased activation during self-reflection on internal states, such as the
evaluation of one’s own thoughts and feelings (Christoff, Ream, Geddes and Gabrieli,
2003; McCaig, Dixon, Keramatian, Liu and Christoff, 2011). Individuals can also learn
to voluntarily modulate activity in aPFC through a metacognitive awareness strategy
(McCaig et al., 2011). Furthermore, inter-individual variance in metacognitive ability
has also been linked to aPFC gray matter volume (Fleming, Weil, Nagy, Dolan and
Rees, 2010) and aPFC resting-state functional connectivity (Baird, Smallwood,
Gorgolewski and Margulies, 2013). Finally, patients with damage to this region
frequently display metacognitive deficits such as inability to monitor disease symptoms
or accurately appraise their cognitive functioning (Joseph, 1999; Schmitz, Rowley,
Kawahara and Johnson, 2006), which might be compared to the lack of metacognitive
insight into the state of consciousness characteristic of non-lucid REM sleep dreams
(Dresler et al., 2015).
Dresler et al. (2012) additionally observed increased BOLD signal during lucid
dreaming in the bilateral precuneus and inferior parietal lobules (angular and
supramarginal gyri). As noted above, parietal cortex and the precuneus in particular has
been implicated in self-referential processing, episodic memory, and the experience of
agency (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006), mirroring the increase of these cognitive
capabilities during lucid dreaming. Finally, activation increases during lucid dreaming
were also found in some occipital and inferior temporal regions, which are part of the
ventral stream of visual processing involved in conscious visual perception (Rees et al.,
2002). While these activations may seem puzzling at first sight, as non-lucid dreams are
also characterized by vivid dream imagery, they are in line with reports that lucid
dreams can be associated with increased visual vividness and clarity of the dream scene
(e.g., Green, 1968).
There are several limitations to the study by Dresler et al. (2012) that are important
to note. First, as mentioned above, the findings are based on observations from a single
subject and caution is therefore warranted in generalizing from the results. Currently no
group-level fMRI study of lucid dreaming has been conducted, and such a study, along
with systematic replications, will be needed before firm conclusions can be drawn.
Another limitation is that, as described below (see Section 7.1), the participant was
20
performing a task during the lucid REM sleep segment (repeated eye signaling and hand
clenching). While Dresler and colleagues accounted for task activation via nuisance
regression of the left and right fist clenching task, it is possible that some of the
activations observed still partially reflect task execution and maintenance of
attention/task-set rather than lucidity per se. One way to address this in future studies
would be to contrast periods of lucid REM sleep when the participant is not performing
an explicit task to non-lucid REM sleep, i.e., a “no-task, within-state paradigm” (Siclari
et al., 2013).
3.2 Neuroimaging individual differences in lucid dreaming
Several studies have taken an individual differences approach to neuroimaging of
lucid dreaming. While most people spontaneously experience lucid dreams infrequently,
there is substantial variation in lucid dream frequency, ranging, by current estimates,
from never (approximately 40-50%) to monthly (approximately 20%) to a small
percentage of people that report lucid dreams several times per week or in some cases
every night (Mota-Rolim et al., 2013; Saunders, Roe, Smith and Clegg, 2016; Snyder
and Gackenbach, 1988). This variation invites the question of whether individuals who
experience frequent lucid dreams show differences in anatomical or functional
properties of the brain. Studies addressing this question can provide complementary
evidence on the neurobiology of lucid dreaming.
In the first neuroimaging study to evaluate the relationship between lucid dream
frequency and brain anatomy, Filevich, Dresler, Brick and Kühn (2015) measured
whole-brain gray matter volume using voxel-based morphometry in individuals with
higher (“high lucidity”) vs. lower (“low lucidity”) scores on a scale assessing the
frequency of lucid dreams and/or dream content hypothesized to be related to lucidity.
Consistent with the hypothesized connection between the metacognitive functions of
aPFC and lucid dreaming discussed above, the study found increased gray matter
volume in two regions of the frontal pole (BA9/10), as well as the right anterior
cingulate cortex, left supplementary motor area and bilateral hippocampus in the high
lucidity group. Additionally, the two identified frontopolar regions showed higher
BOLD signal in the monitoring component of a metacognitive thought-monitoring task
performed while awake.
21
A limitation of the study by Filevich et al. (2015) is that participants were not
frequent lucid dreamers per se, but rather subjects from a database who scored higher
relative to other participants on the scale. Lucid dream frequency for the two groups
was not reported in the study, thus it remains unclear to what extent the “high lucidity”
group experienced frequent lucid dreams in absolute terms. Furthermore, the composite
measure of dreaming used to distinguish the two groups measured not only frequency of
lucid dreams but also different dimensions of dream content. While several of these
content dimensions have been found to be higher in lucid dreams (Voss, Schermelleh-
Engel, Windt, Frenzel and Hobson, 2013), it is likely that several of these dimensions
also co-vary more generally with dream recall and/or cognitive content in dreams
unrelated to lucidity. As a consequence, as the authors note, some of the results could
have been partly influenced by differences in dreaming “styles”, content or dream
recall.
More recently, Baird, Castelnovo, Gosseries and Tononi (2018a) evaluated a sample
of high-frequency lucid dreamers who reported lucid dreams in the range of three to
four times per week to multiple times per night compared to a control group who
reported lucid dreams once per year or less. The frequent lucid dream group and control
group were case-control matched on age, gender, and dream recall frequency. Based on
the previous research reviewed above, the primary aim of the study was to investigate
whether individuals who have frequent lucid dreams would show increased gray matter
density and/or resting-state functional connectivity of aPFC. Consistent with this,
compared to the control group, individuals who reported frequent lucid dreams showed
increased resting-state functional connectivity between the left aPFC and the bilateral
angular gyrus, bilateral middle temporal gyrus and right inferior frontal gyrus (Figure
2b). The frequent lucid dream group also showed decreased functional connectivity
between left aPFC and bilateral insula. Whole-brain graph-theoretic analysis revealed
that left aPFC had increased node degree and strength in the frequent lucid dream group
compared to the control group. However, in contrast to the findings of Filevich et al.
(2015), no significant differences in gray matter density were observed between groups
in either a whole-brain analysis or an aPFC region-of-interest analysis.
Given the link to metacognition, it has also been suggested that lucid dreaming may
be linked to large-scale networks that regulate executive control processes, in particular
the frontoparietal control network (Dresler et al., 2015; Spoormaker et al., 2010). To
22
address this question, Baird et al. (2018a) additionally evaluated the association
between frequent lucid dreaming and connectivity within established large-scale brain
networks, including the frontoparietal control network. Consistent with a link between
the frontoparietal control network and lucid dreaming, Baird and colleagues found that
frequent lucid dreamers had increased functional connectivity between aPFC and a
network of regions that showed the greatest overlap with a frontoparietal control sub-
network (Dixon et al., 2018; Yeo et al., 2011). However, neither connectivity within the
frontoparietal control network broadly defined through meta-analysis (or within or
between any other large-scale networks), nor connectivity within frontoparietal control
sub-networks, as defined through parcellation of resting-state networks, showed
significant differences between the lucid dream group and the control group. The
authors speculate that this could be attributed to both the partial overlap of the regions
that showed increased aPFC connectivity in lucid dreamers with the frontoparietal
control network. However, it is important to keep in mind that while this study did not
find a significant difference in resting-state connectivity within the frontoparietal
network in frequent lucid dreamers, it remains an open question whether lucid REM
sleep dreams show increased connectivity within the frontoparietal control network
compared to non-lucid REM sleep dreams.
Overall, the resting-state connectivity results of Baird et al. (2018a) converge with
the fMRI case study of lucid REM sleep dreaming described above (Dresler et al.,
2012), which found that an overlapping network of brain areas increased fMRI BOLD
signal during lucid compared to baseline REM sleep, including bilateral aPFC, bilateral
inferior parietal lobule (including the angular gyrus), and bilateral middle temporal
gyrus (Figure 2). Together, these results suggest that increased intrinsic functional
connectivity between aPFC and the angular gyrus/middle temporal gyrus—regions that,
as reviewed above, show reduced activity in REM sleep (Nir and Tononi, 2010) and
increased activity during lucid REM sleep (Dresler et al., 2012)—is associated with the
tendency to have frequent lucid dreams. However, while the convergence between these
two preliminary studies is encouraging, the paucity of neuroimaging data on this
question limits strong conclusions at the current time.
23
3.3. Future directions for neuroimaging of lucid dreaming
Neuroimaging studies of lucid REM sleep using larger samples sizes are needed. In
particular, a group-level fMRI study of lucid REM sleep dreaming using a no-task,
within-state paradigm is perhaps the most important next step in this line of research. In
addition to evaluating activation and deactivation as revealed by changes in BOLD
signal, it would also be informative to evaluate differences in functional connectivity in
such a study. For example, as noted, one question that arises from the above
neuroimaging findings is whether there could be increased connectivity within the
network of regions identified in Dresler et al. (2012) and Baird et al. (2018a) during
lucid contrasted with non-lucid REM sleep dreaming. Furthermore, building on the
individual differences results, in future work it would also be interesting to evaluate
whether high frequency lucid dreamers show increased functional connectivity and/or
higher BOLD signal in these brain areas during baseline REM sleep. If so, this could
suggest that it may be possible to bias the brain toward increased metacognitive
awareness of dreaming during REM sleep, for example, as discussed in the next
sections on induction of lucid dreams, through techniques to increase activation of these
regions.
In line with these remarks, it has been suggested that not only regional activation of
frontoparietal brain areas but also connectivity between these regions could be
important for lucidity to emerge during REM sleep (Spoormaker et al., 2010). Indeed,
while activation in these frontal and parietal regions has been linked to key functions
associated with lucid dreaming, including metacognition, as discussed above, regional
activations and metabolic increases in these regions have also been observed during
states of global unconsciousness and subliminal information processing. For instance,
subliminally presented no-go stimuli during a response inhibition task activate
frontoparietal cortices in the absence of awareness (van Gaal, Ridderinkhof, Scholte and
Lamme, 2010). Moreover, loss of consciousness during the tonic phase of generalized
tonic-clonic seizures is associated with a transient increase rather than decrease in
metabolism in frontoparietal cortex (Blumenfeld, 2008; Engel, Kuhl and Phelps, 1982).
Recent findings have suggested that a potentially more sensitive marker of
unconscious states may be reduced connectivity between frontoparietal areas,
particularly from frontal to parietal regions. For example, several neuroimaging studies
of patients in a persistent vegetative state or under different categories of general
24
anesthetics have shown a specific impairment of the backward connectivity from frontal
to parietal regions (Boly et al., 2011; Boly et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2013). These findings
converge with theoretical work and computational modeling (Tononi, 2011), which has
suggested a link between consciousness and effective connectivity within a neural
architecture, or the capacity of a set of neural elements to exert causal influence over
other neural groups in a system. At present, it is unclear whether this reduction of top-
down frontoparietal connectivity is linked to changes in global brain activity, alterations
in primary consciousness (i.e., subjective, phenomenal states of seeing, hearing or
feeling), or whether it could relate to self-awareness (i.e., explicit conscious awareness
of oneself and one’s state). A common interpretation of these results is that top-down
frontoparietal connectivity is a marker of global loss of consciousness, including
primary consciousness (e.g., Mashour, 2014). However, given that the reduction in top-
down connectivity has also been observed under ketamine (Lee et al., 2013), during
which patients often report vivid dream-like experiences, it is plausible that the
reduction of frontoparietal connectivity could instead indicate loss of self-awareness.
Cognitive neuroscience studies of lucid dreaming are uniquely placed to contribute
to this question because the comparison of lucid REM sleep to non-lucid REM sleep is
perhaps the only contrast that allows for a direct comparison between the global loss
and recovery of reflective consciousness independently of global shifts in primary
consciousness, arousal or vigilance state. Thus, the question of whether lucid REM
sleep is associated with altered connectivity between frontal and parietal cortices has
implications for several broad questions in the cognitive neuroscience of consciousness.
Future studies evaluating changes in effective connectivity during lucid REM sleep
dreaming, and in particular changes in top-down frontoparietal connectivity, would be
valuable.
25
4. Brain lesions and lucid dreaming
In the neurological literature, to our knowledge only one paper has reported changes
in lucid dreaming as a result of neurological insult or brain lesions (Sagnier et al.,
2015). The paper describes two case reports of young patients who reported lucid
dreams following unilateral ischemic stroke to the left mediodorsal thalamus. The first
patient was a 26-year-old female with a left anterior and mediodorsal thalamic stroke in
an area supplied by the premamillary artery, as revealed by MRI scans. She reported
frequent lucid dreams in the early morning hours, along with increased nightmares and
nocturnal awakenings. She also reported that her lucid dreams mostly involved the
hospital and medical staff that she encountered during her hospitalization, and included
catastrophic events such as helicopter crashes and hyper-aggressive behaviors. The
second patient was a 36-year-old male with a left mediodorsal thalamic stroke in the
area supplied by the paramedian artery as revealed by MRI scans. He reported frequent
lucid dreams following the stroke, which also tended to occur in the early morning
hours, along with increased nocturnal awakenings, but without an increase in
nightmares. Lucid dreams subsided after one month for both patients.
Lucid dreams have not previously been reported following either unilateral or
bilateral thalamic stroke. However, loss of neurons in the anterior and dorsomedian
thalamic nuclei that occurs in familial fatal insomnia is associated with loss of nocturnal
sleep as well as oneiric ‘intrusions’ during wakefulness (Montagna, 2005; Raggi,
Cosentino, Lanuzza and Ferri, 2010). Furthermore, hypersomnia and irregular sleep are
frequently reported following paramedian thalamic stroke (Hermann et al., 2008;
Luigetti et al., 2011). These clinical features could be manifestations of disruption of the
intralaminar and midline thalamic nuclei located in the mediodorsal thalamus, which are
part of the brain’s arousal network (Van der Werf, Witter and Groenewegen, 2002).
Thus, one possibility is that lucid dreams in these patients could have partly resulted
from increased or abnormal functioning of the brain’s arousal systems during sleep,
which is consistent with the reported increase in nocturnal awakenings. Both patients
also reported that their dreams contained highly anxious and emotional content, which
could reflect abnormal connectivity between these thalamic nuclei and limbic structures
with which they are densely connected (Van der Werf et al., 2002). The highly
emotional or disturbing content may have also contributed to lucid dreaming, as these
types of experiences could induce individuals to question whether the explanation for
26
such surprising or frightening experiences is that they are dreaming (LaBerge and
Rheingold, 1990).
A systematic study of the incidence of lucid dreams following thalamic strokes is
lacking. Particularly given that the lucid dreams subsided after a relatively short
duration (one month) for both patients, it is possible that lucid dreaming as a
consequence of thalamic strokes has been under-reported and/or overlooked. More
generally, a large-scale study of the occurrence of lucid dreaming in neuropsychological
cases has not been undertaken and would be a welcome addition to the literature. The
converse case, of a brain lesion causing loss of lucid dreaming in an individual who
regularly experiences lucid dreams has also to our knowledge not been reported. Such a
case would likely be very unusual, particularly considering the small percentage of
individuals who experience lucid dreams spontaneously with high frequency. However,
if such a case were identified it could be informative to the neurobiology of lucid
dreaming.
27
5. Pharmacological induction of lucid dreaming
A main target of research is to develop methods to make the lucid dream state more
accessible. Indeed, reliable techniques to induce lucid dreams are needed for it to be
effectively used in both clinical and scientific applications. Evidence suggests that lucid
dreaming is a learnable skill (LaBerge, 1980a) that can be developed by training with
various induction strategies (LaBerge and Rheingold, 1990; Price and Cohen, 1988;
Stumbrys, Erlacher, Schädlich and Schredl, 2012). These include training in prospective
memory techniques (LaBerge and Rheingold, 1990), which can be further aided by
application of external sensory cues (LaBerge and Levitan, 1995; LaBerge, Levitan,
Rich and Dement, 1988; LaBerge, Owens, Nagel and Dement, 1981b) and/or
interrupting sleep with short periods of wakefulness (Aspy, Delfabbro, Proeve and
Mohr, 2017; LaBerge, 1980a; LaBerge, Phillips and Levitan, 1994; Stumbrys et al.,
2012). Within cognitive neuroscience, studies have evaluated pharmacological as well
as non-invasive brain stimulation approaches to lucid dream induction. In this section,
we review studies that have taken a pharmacological approach to lucid dream induction
and in the next section we review electrical brain stimulation studies.
5.1 Effects of Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors (AChEIs) on lucid dreaming
As discussed above, lucid dreaming is associated with increased cortical activation
(LaBerge et al., 1981a), which reaches its peak during phasic REM sleep. Given the
relationship between phasic REM sleep and lucid dreaming, as well as the role of
Acetylcholine (ACh) in REM sleep regulation (e.g., Amatruda, Black, McKenna,
McCarley and Hobson, 1975; Velazquez-Moctezuma, Shalauta, Gillin and Shiromani,
1991), agents acting on the cholinergic system have received particular interest. In an
initial pilot study, LaBerge (2001) evaluated the effect of donepezil (Aricept), an
Acetylcholinesterase inhibitor (AChEI), on lucid dreaming in a small group of
participants (N=10) who reported prior experience with lucid dreaming. On each night,
participants received either 0 mg (placebo), 5 mg, or 10 mg of donepezil, with the dose
order counterbalanced across the three nights of the experiment. Nine of the ten
participants (90%) reported at least one lucid dream on donepezil, while only one
participant reported a lucid dream on the placebo dose.
28
Following on these results and additional pilot research, LaBerge, LaMarca and
Baird (2018b) conducted a double blind, placebo-controlled study in a large group of
participants (N=121) with high dream recall and an interest in lucid dreaming. The first
goal of the study was to quantify the size and reliability of the effect of AChEI on lucid
dreaming. The second goal was to test the effectiveness of an integrated lucid dream
induction protocol that combined cholinergic stimulation with other methods, including
sleep interruption and the Mnemonic Induction of Lucid Dreams (MILD) technique,
which trains participants to use prospective memory to induce lucid dreams (LaBerge,
1980a; LaBerge and Rheingold, 1990). Participants were randomly assigned
counterbalanced orders of three doses of galantamine (0 mg=placebo, 4 mg, and 8 mg),
an AChEI that is readily accessible, fast acting and has a mild side effect profile. On
three consecutive nights, participants awoke approximately 4.5 hours after lights out
(after approximately the 3rd REM cycle), recalled a dream, ingested the capsules and
stayed awake for at least 30 minutes. Participants then returned to sleep practicing the
MILD technique. After each subsequent awakening, participants rated their dreams on a
range of variables including lucidity, recall, vividness, bizarreness, complexity, affect,
cognitive clarity, metacognition and control. Full reports of lucid dreams were also
collected.
Galantamine was found to significantly increase the frequency of lucid dreaming in a
dose-related manner. Increased incidence of lucid dreaming was observed for both 4 mg
(27% of participants) and 8 mg (42% of participants) doses compared to 14% of
participants for the active placebo procedure (which included sleep interruption and
MILD). Galantamine was also found to be associated with significantly increased
sensory vividness and environmental complexity, which might be expected given the
general intensification of REM sleep, and associated dreaming, triggered by cholinergic
stimulation (Riemann, Gann, Dressing, Müller and Aldenhoff, 1994).
Another recent double blind, placebo-controlled study conducted by Sparrow, Hurd,
Carlson and Molina, 2018) also tested the effect of galantamine on lucid dreaming. 35
participants completed an eight-night study that tested the effect of 8 mg of galantamine
paired with 40 minutes of sleep interruption (termed “Wake-Back-to-Bed” (WBTB) in
the study). The study additionally tested combining galantamine with middle-of-the-
night meditation and the imaginary reliving of a distressing dream (termed meditation
and dream reliving or MDR; Sparrow, Thurston and Carlson, 2013). The study included
29
pre- and post-baseline nights and six conditions: 1) WBTB; 2) WBTB + placebo; 3)
WBTB + galantamine; 4) MDR; 5) MDR + placebo; and 6) MDR + galantamine. MDR
conditions matched the 40 minutes of sleep interruption in the WBTB conditions. Lucid
dreams were measured with self-reports on the dream lucidity scale (DLS; Sparrow et
al., 2013), which in this study included three categories: 0=non-lucid, 1=pre-lucid
(which included either “questioning things in the dream without actually concluding
that you were dreaming” or “doing things that are ordinarily impossible to do”), or
2=lucid.
Both galantamine conditions (WBTB + galantamine; MDR + galantamine)
significantly increased self-ratings of lucidity on the DLS compared to the other
conditions. However, no significant difference was observed between WBTB +
galantamine and MDR + galantamine. The number of participants who had a lucid
dream in each condition was not reported, only the conditional means for the DLS,
which reflects the effect collapsed across both pre-lucid and lucid dreams. We therefore
contacted the authors for this information in order to compare the results of this study to
the study by LaBerge et al. (2018b). 9% of participants reported a lucid dream in the
WBTB + placebo condition and 11% in the MDR + placebo, whereas 40% of
participants reported a lucid dream in the WBTB + galantamine and 34% in the MDR +
galanatamine (S. Sparrow, personal communication, December 17, 2018). Overall,
therefore, these results are comparable to the effect of 8 mg galantamine observed by
LaBerge and colleagues.
5.2 Effects of L-alpha glycerylphosphorylcholine (α-GPC) lucid dreaming
In contrast to the positive findings for AChEIs, a double blind randomized placebo-
controlled study found no significant effect of 1200 mg of the ACh precursor L-alpha
glycerylphosphorylcholine (α-GPC) on the frequency of lucid dreams in 33 participants
with varying degrees of lucid dreaming experience (Kern, Appel, Schredl and Pipa,
2017). One interpretation of this result is that, in contrast to AChEIs, α-GPC is not
effective for inducing lucid dreams, perhaps due to differences in the neurobiological
effects of choline—an ACh precursor—and AChEIs. However, participants in this
study appear to have received no training in mental set, such as recalling and attending
to dreams in an effort to become lucid. Thus, an alternative, not mutually exclusive,
interpretation is that training in at least the minimal mental set for lucid dream induction
30
is needed for pharmacological (and other) interventions to effectively increase the
frequency of lucid dreams.
5.3 General discussion of pharmacological induction of lucid dreaming
Overall, these data provide strong initial evidence that cholinergic enhancement with
AChEls, and galantamine in particular, facilitates a state of the brain favorable to lucid
dreams. However, a limitation of all pharmacological studies on lucid dreaming
performed to date is that they were not conducted in a sleep laboratory and there was
therefore no validation of lucid dreams with eye-signaling methods. It will be important
to follow up these results with sleep laboratory studies to objectively validate lucid
dreams with polysomnographic recording. Such studies would also be valuable to
investigate the physiological effects of galantamine on the brain during REM sleep, and
which effects are predictive of lucidity. Given the high success rate of lucid dreams in
the combined induction protocol with cholinergic stimulation reported by LaBerge,
LaMarca and Baird (2018) and Sparrow et al. (2018), studies of galantamine have the
potential to enable efficient collection of large sample sizes in electrophysiological and
neuroimaging studies of lucid dreaming.
The mechanism by which AChEls facilitate lucid dreams remains unclear. There are
several, not mutually exclusive, possibilities, including increasing REM sleep
intensity/phasic activation, influencing the brain regions/networks associated with lucid
dreaming, and influencing cognitive processes associated with becoming lucid. In
general, it is known that AChEls inhibit the metabolic inactivation of ACh by inhibiting
the enzyme acetylcholinesterase (AChE), leading to accumulation of ACh at synapses.
Furthermore, ACh and its agonists as well as AChE and its antagonists are involved in
the generation of REM sleep (Amatruda et al., 1975; Gillin et al., 1985; Velazquez-
Moctezuma et al., 1991). For example, evidence suggests that REM sleep is controlled
by cholinergic neurons in the brainstem (Baghdoyan, 1997; Hernandez-Peon, Chavez-
Ibarra, Morgane and Timo-Iaria, 1963), and studies have observed that microinjection
of the ACh agonist carbachol in the pontine area of the brainstem results in REM sleep
in both humans and animals (Amatruda et al., 1975; Baghdoyan, 1997). Administration
of galantamine has been associated with increased phasic activity and shortened REM
latency (Riemann et al., 1994). The increased frequency of lucid dreams associated with
AChEls could therefore plausibly be related to its effects on cholinergic receptors
31
during REM sleep, leading to longer, intensified REM periods with increased phasic
activity, which, as noted above, has been found to be associated with lucid dreams
(LaBerge et al., 1981a).
Beyond intensification of REM sleep/phasic activation, AChEls might also directly
act on cognitive processes associated with lucidity and their neural underpinnings. One
key question is whether AChEIs could facilitate lucid dreaming through increasing
activation within the network of frontopolar-temporo-parietal areas observed in the
neuroimaging studies of Dresler et al. (2012) and Baird et al. (2018a). The relationship
between cholinergic modulation and frontoparietal activation is complex and depends
on the task context and population under study (see Bentley, Driver and Dolan, 2011 for
a review). However, pro-cholinergic drugs in general tend to increase frontoparietal
activity in conditions in which these areas show low baseline activation, which is
thought to reflect increased attentional-executive functions (Bentley et al., 2011). Given
that frontoparietal activity is typically suppressed during REM sleep (Braun et al., 1997;
Maquet et al., 1996), it is plausible that AChEIs could increase activation within this
network during REM sleep dreaming.
Notably, AChEls are also prescribed to manage the cognitive symptoms of
Alzheimer's disease (Koontz and Baskys, 2005). Theoretically, another way that
AChEls could facilitate lucid dreams is therefore through their effect on memory. For
example, AChEIs could enhance the ability to remember to recognize that one is
dreaming (a form of prospective memory), which is the core of the MILD technique for
lucid dream induction that participants engaged in during the study by LaBerge,
LaMarca and Baird (2018b). However, evidence for cognition enhancing effects of
AChEls in healthy subjects, particularly for single doses, is sparse (Dresler et al., 2018),
rendering the theoretical possibility of a memory-mediated effect on dream lucidity
unlikely. Overall, it remains unclear whether galantamine also exerts a direct influence
on cognitive processes associated with lucidity, and the MILD technique in particular,
or whether it merely optimizes the physiological conditions for such techniques.
One last possibility is that AChEIs could influence lucidity indirectly by affecting
other neuromodulators. For example, evidence suggests that AChEls also increase
systemic norepinephrine and dopamine (Cuadra, Summers and Giacobini, 1994;
Giacobini, Zhu, Williams and Sherman, 1996). It is therefore possible that the increase
in lucid dreams associated with AChEI might instead be directly linked to aminergic
32
modulation that occurs as a result of the increases in ACh. Additional research will be
needed to understand how the neuromodulatory changes caused by AChEIs stimulate
lucid REM sleep dreaming. Studies using Positron Tomography (PET) or pharmaco-
fMRI would be valuable to address this issue.
Several other pharmacological substances have been suggested to increase the
frequency of lucid dreams, including various types of supplements and drugs (e.g.,
Yuschak, 2006). For instance, recreational drugs such as alcohol, cocaine and cannabis
have been reported to be associated with lucid dreaming. These substances suppress
REM sleep (Roehrs and Roth, 2001; Schierenbeck, Riemann, Berger and Hornyak,
2008), which leads to a phenomenon referred to as REM rebound, in which longer REM
sleep periods occur following REM sleep suppression (Vogel, 1975). Intensified,
prolonged REM sleep as a result of REM rebound could potentially increase lucid
dream frequency, particularly in individuals predisposed to have lucid dreams or with
prior experience in lucid dreaming. Another example is LSD, since there is some
evidence that it can prolong REM sleep periods at some doses (Muzio, Roffwarg and
Kaufman, 1966), which could potentially be favorable to lucid dreaming. To our
knowledge, however, no studies have systematically evaluated whether these
substances, or other substances not reviewed here, increase the incidence of lucid
dreams. Furthermore, it is prudent to remain cautious about such claims given the
placebo effect. Placebo-controlled studies will be essential to substantiate any purported
effects of pharmacological substances on lucid dreaming.
33
6. Induction of lucid dreams with transcranial electrical brain stimulation
Two studies have attempted to induce lucid dreams through transcranial electrical
stimulation of the frontal cortex during REM sleep. Stumbrys, Erlacher and Schredl
(2013b) tested direct current stimulation and Voss et al. (2014) tested alternating current
stimulation. We review each of these studies in turn.
6.1 Effects of frontal transcranial direct current stimulation on lucid dreaming
In an investigation of the effect of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on
lucid dreaming, Stumbrys et al. (2013b) applied either tDCS or sham stimulation
(counterbalanced across nights) over a frontolateral scalp region in 19 participants.
Stimulation was delivered through two pairs of electrodes with anodes at positions F3
and F4, respectively, and cathodes at the supraclavicular area of the same side.
Stimulation was applied during all REM sleep periods except the first for two
consecutive nights in the sleep laboratory. In total, 109 stimulations were performed,
after which participants were awakened to complete a dream report.
Compared to sham stimulation, tDCS resulted in a small numerical increase in self-
ratings of the unreality of dream objects as assessed by the Dream Lucidity
Questionnaire (DLQ; Stumbrys et al., 2013b). Post-hoc analyses revealed that this effect
was seen only in subjects with a high baseline frequency of lucid dreams, but not in
participants with little or no lucid dreaming experience. However, tDCS did not
significantly increase the number of dreams rated by judges to have a clear indication of
lucidity: seven dreams in total, three from sham stimulation and four from tDCS
stimulation. Furthermore, only one lucid dream in total was confirmed by eye signaling,
which was in the stimulation condition. Given a difference of only one lucid dream
between stimulation and sham conditions for either of these two assessment criteria,
overall frontal stimulation with tDCS as tested in this study does not appear to be a
reliable method for inducing lucid dreams.
6.2 Effects of frontal transcranial alternating current stimulation on lucid dreaming
A study by Voss et al. (2014) reported a more pronounced increase in lucid dreaming
by applying transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) in the gamma frequency
34
band over the frontal cortex. The study tested 27 participants for up to four nights in the
sleep laboratory. tACS was applied for 30 seconds after two minutes of uninterrupted
REM sleep to frontolateral scalp regions at several different frequencies (2, 6, 12, 25,
40, 70 or 100 Hz or sham stimulation). Stimulation was delivered through two pairs
electrodes with anodes at positions F3 and F4, respectively, and cathodes over the
mastoids close to TP9 and TP10, respectively. Participants were then awakened and
completed a dream report and the Lucidity and Consciousness in Dreams (LuCiD) scale
(Voss et al., 2013). The LuCiD scale measures dream content on several different
factor-analytically derived dimensions, including insight, control, thought, realism,
memory, dissociation and affect.
The authors reported they were able to induce lucid dreams with a success rate of
58% with 25 Hz stimulation and 77% with 40 Hz stimulation. However, it is important
to note how lucid dreams were classified in this study: instead of assessment of lucid
dreams with eye signaling, self-report, or through statistical analysis of judges’ ratings
of dream reports, as in Stumbrys et al. (2013b), dreams were assumed to be lucid if
subjects reported “elevated ratings (>mean + 2 s.e.) on either or both of the LuCiD scale
factors insight and dissociation”. While dissociation scores (i.e., “seeing oneself from
the outside” or a “3rd person perspective”) have been found to be increased in lucid
dreams before (Voss et al., 2013), dissociation in the sense of adopting a 3rd person
perspective has never been considered a defining feature of lucid dreams per se (e.g.,
DeGracia and LaBerge, 2000; Gackenbach and LaBerge, 1988; Green, 1968; LaBerge,
1985, 1990). It is therefore controversial to classify dreams as lucid based solely on
higher ratings of dissociation.
The insight subscale corresponds more closely to the standard definition of lucid
dreams adopted by other researchers as well as the general public. Mean ratings in the
insight subscale increased from approximately 0.1-0.2 in the sham stimulation to 0.5-0.6
in the 25 Hz or 40 Hz stimulation conditions, similar to the effects in the dissociation
subscale, which was approximately 0.9-1.2. However, the scale anchors ranged from 0
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree), indicating that, on average, in both the 25 Hz
and 40 Hz stimulation conditions, participants disagreed that their dreams had increased
insight. Moreover, in the original validation of the LuCiD scale, non-lucid dreams
scored on average at 0.3 or 0.8 on the insight scale (depending on whether the
assessment was laboratory or survey based), whereas lucid dreams scored at 3.2 or 3.5
35
(Voss et al., 2013). Thus, though significantly stronger in relation to sham simulation,
mean responses of 0.5-0.6 in the 25 and 40 Hz stimulation conditions were much lower
than the values reported for lucid dreams in the validation study, and still on the non-
lucid (disagree that the dream contains insight) end of the scale spectrum in absolute
values. Overall, therefore, the results of this study appear to be comparable to the tDCS
finding of Stumbrys et al. (2013b) in that prefrontal tACS gamma band stimulation
induced numerical increases in some measures of dream content, but does not appear to
have lead to increases in the number of lucid dreams when defined in the traditional
sense of being aware of dreaming while dreaming.
6.3 General discussion of induction of lucid dreaming with electrical brain stimulation
In summary, studies examining induction of lucid dreams with electrical brain
stimulation (tDCS and tACS) have thus far observed intriguing effects on dream
cognition, but a method for reliably inducing lucid dreams by electrical stimulation of
the brain is still yet to be identified. We think this is a particularly interesting direction
for upcoming work. Future studies might consider stimulating a wider number of brain
areas and different types of stimulation. For example, transcranial magnetic stimulation
(TMS) is another method of noninvasive brain stimulation that could be used to attempt
to induce lucid dreams that has not yet been examined (Noreika, Windt, Lenggenhager
and Karim, 2010; Mota-Rolim et al., 2010). In contrast to tACS or tDCS, high-
frequency repetitive TMS (rTMS) stimulation sequences can be used to increase
neuronal excitability in focal areas of the cortex (e.g., Hallett, 2007). Several practical
challenges with application of TMS during sleep include the auditory artifacts produced
by the TMS coil as well as tactile sensations on the scalp during stimulation, which may
lead to awakenings. However, noise masking has been used to prevent subjects from
hearing the clicks associated with TMS pulses (e.g., Nieminen et al., 2016), and scalp
sensations could be decreased, for example, by reducing stimulation intensity or
potentially by application of a topical anesthetic cream.
Studies of electrical brain stimulation have also thus far only tested a small part of
the parameter space and there are many other stimulation protocols that appear to be
worth evaluating. For example, given the neuroimaging results reviewed above,
synchronous frontoparietal tACS—in which synchrony is increased between frontal and
36
parietal regions by simultaneous stimulation of these regions (e.g., Violante et al.,
2017)—is another stimulation method that would be interesting to examine. Finally, as
noted above, these methods are likely to maximize their effect when participants are
trained in at least the minimal mental set for lucid dream induction, which was not done
in either the study by Voss et al. (2014) or Stumbrys et al. (2013b).
37
7. Lucid dreaming as a methodology for the cognitive neuroscience of
consciousness
Psychophysiological studies of non-lucid dreams have been used to study neural
correlates of conscious experiences during sleep (Perogamvros et al., 2017; Siclari et al.,
2017). However, there are significant limitations to this approach. Specifically, while
methods such as “serial awakenings” are useful for contrasting the global presence vs.
absence of dream experience during sleep (Siclari et al., 2017), it is an inefficient
method to collect data on specific conscious contents during sleep. For instance, given
that non-lucid dreamers usually have no control over their dream content, these studies
typically employ a shot-in-the-dark approach, in which large numbers of sleep
recordings are made and small subsets of data in which the content of interest appears
by chance are extracted. Additionally, and perhaps most importantly, establishing
temporally precise correlations between retrospective dream reports and physiological
measurements is a substantial challenge. Lucid dreaming provides ways to overcome
several of these challenges. For example, as mentioned above, lucid dreamers can
conduct specific tasks within REM sleep dreams, enabling experimental control over
dream content. Furthermore, as noted, participants can be trained to time-stamp the
onset and offset of particular content or actions with eye movement signals, which
provides a way to obtain more precise correlations between conscious experiences and
physiological measurements during sleep (Dresler et al., 2011; LaBerge, 1990;
LaBerge, Greenleaf and Kedzierski, 1983). In this section we briefly summarize several
recent studies that illustrate how lucid dreaming can be used as a methodology in the
cognitive neuroscience of consciousness.
7.1 Activation of sensorimotor cortex during dreamed movement in REM sleep
Dresler et al. (2011) used lucid dreaming to test whether a motor task performed
during dreaming elicits neuronal activation in the sensorimotor cortex. Participants
made a series of dreamed left and right hand clenches in their lucid dreams and marked
the start and end of the sequences of clenches with LRLR eye movements. Specifically,
participants clenched their left hand in the dream ten times, then signaled LRLR, then
clenched their right hand in the dream ten times, then signaled LRLR, and continued
repeating this sequence for as long as possible until awakening. Additionally,
38
participants performed an executed hand-clenching task as well as imagined hand-
clenching task during wakefulness for comparison. Six participants with prior
experience in lucid dreaming participated in the study. Of these, two participants
succeeded in performing the task during lucid REM sleep: one under simultaneous
EEG-fMRI (in two different signal-verified lucid dreams) and one under combined
EEG-near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) recording (again in two different signal-
verified lucid dreams).
Contrasting left vs. right fist clenches, increased BOLD signal in the contralateral
sensorimotor cortex was observed in both lucid dreams of the fMRI experiment, as well
as in waking and imagination conditions. Compared to executed hand clenches during
wakefulness, however, BOLD signal increases during dreaming in contralateral
sensorimotor cortex were more localized, which could indicate either weaker activation
or more focal activation of hand areas only. BOLD signal fluctuations during dreaming
were approximately 50% of those observed for executed hand clenches during
wakefulness. Correspondingly, the NIRS data showed increased oxygenated
hemoglobin and decreased deoxygenated hemoglobin in the contralateral sensorimotor
cortex during dreamed hand clenching. This hemodynamic response was also observed
in the supplementary motor area, which is involved in the timing, monitoring and
preparation of movements (Goldberg, 1985). This differed with the fMRI data in which
no significant differences in the supplementary motor area were found. Interestingly,
during dreamed hand clenching, hemodynamic responses were smaller in the
sensorimotor cortex but of similar amplitude in the supplementary motor area when
compared to overt motor performance during wakefulness.
Overall, these data suggest that the pattern of brain activation observed during
dreamed motor execution overlaps with motor execution during wakefulness. However,
given the different patterns of activation, the data may also suggest that the interactions
between the supplementary motor area, somatosensory and sensorimotor cortex differs
between REM sleep dreaming and waking. The authors suggest that the reduced
activation in sensorimotor cortex could be due in part to the lack of sensory feedback as
a result of REM sleep atonia. However, given that this study consisted of two case
reports—one for each imaging modality—the data should be interpreted cautiously.
Clarification of the neural correlates of dreamed motor activity, as well as clarification
of any differences in this network compared to overt motor performance, awaits larger-
39
scale group-level studies. Nevertheless, this experiment provides a proof-of-concept that
neuroimaging of specific dream content can be accomplished using lucid dreaming as a
methodology.
7.2 Voluntary control of central apneas during REM sleep dreaming
A similar type of experiment was recently undertaken by Oudiette et al. (2018) to
examine respiratory behavior during REM sleep dreaming. Specifically, the researchers
used lucid dreaming to investigate whether irregular breathing during REM sleep has a
cortical origin and whether such breathing patterns can reflect the mental content of
dreams. This research follows up on a study from LaBerge and Dement (1982) which
observed tachypnea during volitional rapid breathing and apnea during voluntary breath
holding during lucid REM sleep dreaming. Polysomnography and respiration were
recorded during early morning naps from 21 patients with narcolepsy who reported
frequent lucid dreams. Participants were instructed to modify their dream scenario so
that it involved vocalizations or an apnea (e.g., diving under water)—two respiratory
behaviors that purportedly require a cortical control of respiration (McKay, Adams,
Frackowiak and Corfield, 2008). Participants signaled the onset of lucidity as well as
the start and end of the respiration tasks with LRLR eye movement signals. Participants
also performed the task during waking by actually executing the respiratory task as well
as during waking imagination. In total, 32 lucid REM sleep naps were included in the
analysis. Physiological data was scored for the presence of central apneas, which were
defined as “cessation of nasal flow for more than 10 s in the absence of cyclic thoracic
and abdominal movements” as well as preparatory breaths preceding the central apnea.
In 16 out of the 32 signal-verified lucid dreams, the physiological data showed the
expected respiratory behavior (e.g., a central apnea, in some cases including preparatory
breaths), which was appropriately marked by LRLR signals and confirmed by dream
reports of becoming lucid and executing the task. In the remainder of cases, participants
either failed to control the dream (N=2) or appropriately execute the LRLR signals
(N=2), or there was incongruence between the report and the data (participants either
did not recall performing the task (N=8) or there was no physiological evidence of the
task despite a report of completing the task (N=4)). As the authors discuss, the
incongruences between reports and physiological data may in part have been
40
attributable to the fact that reports were obtained at the end of the 30-minute nap
sessions rather than awakening subjects directly after completing the task. Thus, in
some cases this resulted in a long delay between the dream task and the report. Despite
the presence of these ambiguous cases, overall the data show that voluntarily control of
central apneas during REM sleep occurred in a majority of participants.
As the authors discuss, the pons is hypothesized to regulate cessation of breathing,
which assimilates input from supra-brainstem structures and inhibits medullary
respiratory neurons (McKay et al., 2008). Thus, they conclude that the fact that
voluntary control of central apneas is possible during REM sleep suggests that this
cortico-pontine drive is preserved during REM sleep. However, it remains unclear from
this study to what extent non-lucid dreams containing this type of mental content (for
example, diving under water), would result in central apneas. Indeed, as the authors
note, some participants reported that they voluntarily held their breath when diving
under water in the dream even though they did not feel that they had to. In general, it
remains unclear whether central apneas as observed in the study are linked to the
voluntary control of respiration enabled during lucid dreams in particular or whether
they systematically occur in dreams with this type of content/scenario. In either case,
however, the data support the conclusion that voluntarily control of central apneas is
possible during REM sleep.
7.3 Smooth pursuit eye movements during visual tracking in REM sleep dreaming
One last recent example of the use of lucid dreaming as methodology in cognitive
neuroscience is provided by LaBerge, Baird and Zimbardo (2018a), who addressed the
question of whether smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEMs) occur during tracking of a
slowly moving visual target during lucid REM sleep dreaming. Seven participants with
high reported dream recall and frequency of lucid dreams participated in the study.
Participants marked the moment of lucidity as well as the initiation and completion of
the tracking tasks with LRLR eye movement signals. After making the LRLR signal,
participants performed one of two eye-tracking tasks: circle tracking or one-dimensional
movement tracking on the horizontal meridian. For each tracking task, participants
followed the tip of their thumb with their gaze as they traced the pattern. Participants
performed the tracking tasks in three conditions: 1) lucid REM sleep dreaming
41
(“dreaming”), 2) awake with eyes open (“perception”) and 3) tracking the imagined
movement while awake with eyes closed (“imagination”). Eye movements were
recorded with direct current EOG and subjected to a validated algorithm that classifies
saccades, fixations and smooth pursuit eye movements (Komogortsev and Karpov,
2013).
The results revealed that intentional slow tracking of visual motion (of both circles
and lines) during lucid REM sleep dreams results in SPEMs. Pursuit eye movements in
REM sleep dreams did not significantly differ from pursuit during waking perception,
and both were characterized by high pursuit ratios and low saccade rates. In contrast,
tracking in imagination was characterized by low pursuit with frequent saccadic
intrusions. A Bayesian classification model that included pursuit ratio and saccade rate
discriminated both REM sleep dreaming and perception from imagination with greater
than 98% accuracy.
Together, these findings help to address several broad questions within cognitive
neuroscience and sleep research. First, the data provide empirical evidence for a
difficult to test question that has been asked at least since Aristotle: “Are dreams more
like perception or imagination?” (Nir and Tononi, 2010). Based on the smooth tracking
behavior, the findings of this study suggest that, at least in this respect, the visual
quality of REM sleep dream imagery is more similar to waking perception than
imagination. Second, the findings help to address a longstanding question in the
psychophysiology of sleep since the discovery of REM in the 1950’s: whether the eye
movements of REM sleep track the gaze of the dreamer—the so-called “scanning
hypothesis” (for a review, see Arnulf, 2011). By demonstrating that individuals can
trace circles and lines with their gaze while in EEG-verified REM sleep, which can be
recorded with EOG, the data provide unique evidence that shifts in the perceived gaze
direction in dreams give rise to the appropriate corresponding eye movements. This is
consistent with the view that a subset of eye movements during REM sleep are linked to
the direction of subjective gaze during dreams. Lastly, the results also provide a novel
source of data for a central research question on the topic of smooth pursuit in humans
and non-human primates that dates back at least forty years. Specifically, an enduring
question has been whether a physical stimulus and/or retinal image motion is necessary
to drive the neural circuitry of smooth pursuit (Spering and Montagnini, 2011). By
demonstrating that sustained SPEMs can be elicited in the absence of visual input to the
42
cortex, as is the case during REM sleep, the findings provide strong evidence that
neither a physical motion stimulus nor readout of retinal image motion are necessary for
SPEMs.
7.4 General discussion of lucid dreaming as experimental methodology
Altogether, these studies illustrate the potential of lucid dreaming as an experimental
methodology for the study of consciousness in general and REM sleep dreaming in
particular. The fact that lucid dreamers can exercise volitional control over their actions
while dreaming and conduct experiments from within EEG-verified REM sleep dreams
opens up the ability to perform experiments that would otherwise be difficult or
impossible to conduct. This methodology also has the potential to more efficiently
target specific research questions. As discussed above, lucid dreaming enables both
experimental control over the content of dreams as well as a way to establish precise
psychophysiological correlations between the contents of consciousness during sleep
and physiological measures in a way that is not possible in studies of non-lucid
dreaming. In sum, lucid dreaming provides a methodology that allows for new ways of
studying the relationship between consciousness and neurophysiological processes, and,
as the above studies highlight, shows emerging potential for research on consciousness
in sleep science.
43
8. Clinical cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming
Research on lucid dreaming is not only relevant to the neuroscience of consciousness
but could also have clinical implications. While a detailed treatment of the clinical
applications of lucid dreaming is beyond the scope of this review (see e.g., Garfield,
Fellows, Halliday and Malamud, 1988), here we briefly note several interesting
applications and future directions with particular relevance for cognitive neuroscience.
8.1 Lucid dreaming in the treatment of persistent nightmares
The most researched application of lucid dreaming is to treat recurring nightmares
(Abramovitch, 1995; Holzinger, Klösch and Saletu, 2015; Spoormaker and Van Den
Bout, 2006; Tanner, 2004; Zadra and Pihl, 1997). Lucid dreaming was included by the
American Academy of Sleep Medicinein their therapy suggestions for nightmare
disorder (Morgenthaler et al., 2018). Conceptually, the idea is that becoming lucid
during a nightmare should allow the dreamer to realize the content of the nightmare is
not real and thus has no reason to be afraid, and to be able to exert control over the
dream and/or work with the psychological content of the nightmare (LaBerge and
Rheingold, 1990; Mota-Rolim and Araujo, 2013). Neurocognitive models of nightmare
generation suggest a hyper-responsivity of the amygdala coupled with a failure of
prefrontal regions to dampen this activation (Levin and Nielsen, 2007). Studying the
influence of lucidity on nightmare resolution could therefore provide an interesting
opportunity to study top-down regulation of amygdala activation, perhaps through
reactivation of prefrontal regions (Dresler et al., 2012).
A case study (Zadra and Pihl, 1997), and one small, controlled pilot-study
(Spoormaker and Van Den Bout, 2006) have found that lucid dreaming therapy was
effective in reducing nightmare frequency. Also a study of 32 patients who suffer from
frequent nightmares found a slight advantage of lucid dreaming as add-on to Gestalt
therapy compared to the latter alone (Holzinger et al., 2015). In contrast, a larger online
study did not find any additional effect of lucid dreaming therapy as an add-on to other
cognitive-behavioral techniques, such as imagery rehearsal therapy (Lancee, Van Den
Bout and Spoormaker, 2010), although low power and high dropout rates (>70%)
limited the scope of the conclusions. Controlled experiments with larger sample sizes
are needed to further evaluate the potential usefulness of lucid dreaming for the
44
treatment of recurring nightmares and related disorders, as well as to examine the neural
mechanisms of these interactions.
8.2 Lucid dreaming and narcolepsy
Consistent with a potential beneficial role of lucid dreaming in recurring nightmares,
patients with narcolepsy report that becoming lucid provides psychological relief (Rak
et al., 2015). Generally, narcolepsy patients experience longer, more complex, and more
vivid dreams and more frequent nightmares than healthy subjects (Mazzetti et al.,
2010). Moreover, patients with narcolepsy report higher reflective consciousness within
dreams (Fosse, 2000) and accordingly a considerably increased lucid dreaming
frequency (Dodet et al., 2014; Rak et al., 2015). The mechanisms for this increased
lucid dreaming frequency are not entirely understood yet: nightmares often lead to the
insight into the current dream state, however it is currently unclear if an increased
nightmare frequency causes an increased lucid dreaming frequency in narcolepsy, or if
the strongly increased frequency of REM sleep in narcolepsy affects nightmares and
lucid dreaming independently. In particular sleep onset REM (SOREM) episodes appear
to prompt lucid dreams (Dodet et al., 2014), which is in line with the dream lucidity-
enhancing effects of the early morning “Wake-Back-to-Bed” procedure in healthy
subjects. Of note, patients with narcolepsy make a clear distinction between their
experiences of lucid dreaming and sleep paralysis (Dodet et al., 2014), and the increased
lucid dreaming frequency in this patient group does not appear to be associated with
medication use (Rak et al., 2014). It has been speculated whether, potentially as a result
of a considerably increased lucid dreaming experience, narcoleptic patients might
exhibit different neurophysiological correlates of lucid dreaming (Dodet et al., 2014).
8.3 Lucid dreaming as a model of insight in psychiatric conditions
Another area in which research on lucid dreaming might be applied is in psychiatric
disorders in which patients suffer from lack of insight into their state. Indeed, there is
considerable overlap between preliminary findings of brain regions related to insight
into the dream state and brain regions impaired in psychotic patients with disturbed
insight into their pathological state (Dresler et al., 2015; Mota, Resende, Mota-Rolim,
45
Copelli and Ribeiro, 2016). The concept of insight is becoming an increasingly
important area in schizophrenia research (e.g., Baier, 2010), for example, where
between 50 and 80% of patients diagnosed with schizophrenia have poor insight into the
presence of their disorder (Lincoln, Lüllmann and Rief, 2007). Evidence suggests that
poor insight in turn leads to more relapses, re-hospitalizations and poorer therapy
success (Mintz, Dobson and Romney, 2003). With regard to dreaming, lack of insight
into the current state characterizes almost all dream experiences, with the exception of
lucid dreaming. Thus, lucid dreaming could potentially be used as a model for at least
some cognitive dimensions of insight (David, 1990). In this context it is interesting to
note that historical approaches to psychosis used the term lucid to denote the awareness
of the patient into his or her illness (Berrios and Markova, 1998). At the current time,
these ideas remain highly speculative, but this appears to us to be an area worthy of
additional research.
8.4 Using lucid dreaming to establish brain activity markers of self-awareness
Another potential clinical application of research on lucid dreaming is in the
development of neuroimaging-based diagnostic markers of awareness. Such measures
have the potential to improve the diagnosis and monitoring of patients who are
unresponsive due to traumatic injury, aphasia, motor impairment, or other physical
limitations. Neurobiological studies of lucid dreaming could provide information
relevant to development of these brain activity markers since, in addition to assessing a
patient’s capacity for primary consciousness (i.e., if a patient can see, hear or experience
pain), an important clinical goal is to assess whether patients, who may nevertheless be
unresponsive via behavioral assessment, are aware of themselves and their state
(Laureys, Perrin and Brédart, 2007). This information is critical to making appropriate
therapeutic choices and determining prognosis (Laureys et al., 2007). Research indicates
that the bedside behavioral assessment of such patients is challenging and has a high
rate of misdiagnosis (over 40% according to some studies) (Laureys, Owen and Schiff,
2004; Schnakers et al., 2009). Accordingly, identification of a reliable brain activity
marker of a patients’ capacity for self-awareness—which could be informed by studying
the changes in brain activity between lucid and non-lucid REM sleep dreaming—is an
important goal. Research on lucid dreaming has the potential to contribute a valuable
source of data to this question since, as noted above, the contrast between lucid and
46
non-lucid REM sleep is perhaps the only one currently known in which a global loss
and recovery of self-awareness can be contrasted within the same vigilance state in
healthy individuals.
47
9. The measurement of lucid dreaming in cognitive neuroscience research
In this last section, we review methodological issues and strategies in the measurement
of lucid dreaming in cognitive neuroscience research. We review the validation of lucid
dreaming both physiologically and with questionnaires, and discuss best-practice
procedures to investigate lucid dreaming in the sleep laboratory.
9.1 Eye signaling methodology
The eye signaling methodology is the gold standard in lucid dreaming research, as it
allows for objective confirmation of lucid dreams through the execution of pre-agreed
upon sequences of eye movements recorded with the EOG during EEG-verified REM
sleep (Figure 1). While there are some types of research studies (e.g., field studies)
where eye signaling is not possible or not applicable, we recommend considering this
method as the standard practice for laboratory-based studies of lucid dreaming. In
general, most recent studies have adhered to this convention; however, there is some
confusion about how to properly instruct participants in executing the eye movements,
which has resulted in studies implementing several different variations of the method.
For instance, while some experimenters instruct participants to move their eyes left and
right (e.g., Dresler et al., 2012), in other cases participants have been instructed to “scan
the horizon” from left to right (Dodet et al., 2014).
Differences in eye signaling instructions could partly account for the varying degrees
of effectiveness in objectively identifying the eye signals across studies. For example,
Dodet et al. (2014) reported that while three control participants and twelve narcoleptic
patients reported making the eye signal in overnight EEG recordings, none of these
could be objectively identified, and only about half of reported signals could be
unambiguously identified from nap recordings. While it is possible that some of these
instances represent cases where participants misremembered or misreported a lucid
dream, it is likely that this high rate of ambiguous eye movement signals is partly
attributable to the specific instructions that were given. Similarly, while the exact
instructions provided to participants was not described, Voss et al. (2009) stated that
they were unable to obtain reliable eye signals using the standard signaling method and
therefore attempted to develop a novel signaling method employing two sets of eye-
signals separated by a pause. Again, suboptimal results could plausibly be due to the
48
instructions provided and the way the eye signals were executed by participants in the
study.
One reason for the confusion and diverse instructions given for the eye signaling
methodology is that a standardized set of instructions for making the signals has never
been published. Here we report a simple set of instructions adapted from LaBerge et al.
(2018a) that has been reported to yield nearly 100% correspondence between subjective
reports of eye signals and objective EOG recordings. The instruction is as follows:
When making an eye movement signal, we would like you to look all the way to
the left then all the way to the right two times consecutively, as if you are looking
at each of your ears. Specifically, we would like you to look at your left ear, then
your right ear, then your left ear, then your right ear, and then finally back to
center. Make the eye movements without moving your head, and make the full
left-right-left-right-center motion as one rapid continuous movement without
pausing.
Looking at the ears is one of the critical aspects of the instruction, as it encourages full-
scale eye movements without corresponding head movements. These extreme and rapid
consecutive eye movements make the signals easy to discern in the horizontal EOG
time series. The signals are of higher amplitude than typical REMs, and have a
distinctive shape (Figure 1) which can be identified with template matching (LaBerge et
al., 2018a). To optimize execution of the eye signals, participants are given the
opportunity to practice the signals in the laboratory while awake and connected to the
EOG, with an opportunity to view the signals and receive feedback from the
experimenter. Additionally, participants practice making the eye signals in any lucid
dreams they have at home in the weeks leading up to the sleep laboratory visit, in order
to gain experience with executing the signals.
9.2 Questionnaire assessment of lucid dreaming
A related issue concerns how the phenomenology of lucid dreaming should
optimally be assessed in cognitive neuroscience research. Several questionnaires have
been developed to assess changes in conscious experience during dreaming. For
instance, the Metacognitive, Affective, Cognitive Experience questionnaire (MACE;
Kahan, LaBerge, Levitan and Zimbardo, 1997; Kahan and Sullivan, 2012) was designed
49
to assesses the monitoring and regulation of both cognitive and affective experience
during both wakefulness and sleep. In its latest form, it consists of ten items, including
four self-monitoring questions, three questions on self-reflective consciousness and
three questions that target self-regulatory behaviors. Another measure, the Dream
Lucidity Questionnaire (DLQ; Stumbrys et al., 2013b), is designed to assess different
aspects of lucidity within dreams. It consists of ten items, scored on a 5-point scale: 0 –
not at all, 1 just a little, 2 moderately, 3 pretty much, 4 very much. The DLQ
evaluates different types of awareness (awareness of dreaming, awareness that the
physical body is asleep, awareness that dream characters and objects are not real,
awareness of different possibilities) and control (deliberately choosing an action,
changing dream events, dream characters, dream scenes, breaking physical laws). In a
similar manner, the Lucidity and Consciousness in Dreams scale (LuCiD, Voss et al.,
2013) includes eight subscales, derived from factor analysis of a sample of lucid and
non-lucid dream reports, which assess various dimensions of dreaming experience and
cognition: 1) Insight, 2) Control, 3) Thought, 4) Realism, 5) Memory, 6) Dissociation,
7) Negative emotion, and 8) Positive emotion.
All three questionnaires provide measures for evaluating how some content
dimensions might differ across lucid and non-lucid dreams. However, as mentioned,
questionnaires such as these are not sufficient to objectively establish whether a
participant had a lucid dream. It is worth noting that only the DLQ in its first question
(“I was aware that I was dreaming”) directly queries whether the dreamer was lucid, and
the MACE was never intended for this purpose. Most studies using the DLQ and LuCiD
questionnaires have collected a dream report from participants in addition to the
questionnaire responses. We would like to emphasize the importance of this point:
Instead of relying on the inference of lucidity solely from questionnaire measures of
dream content, accurate assessment of lucidity can be facilitated by, in addition to using
the eye signaling method, collecting a full dream report from participants. In making the
dream report, participants are typically asked to describe in as much detail as possible
the narrative of the dream, including the sequence of events and any thoughts, feelings
or sensations that they experienced. In dream reports for lucid dreams, the participant is
also asked to include a specific description of how they became lucid in the dream (for
example, by noticing an oddity of an event, action or person in the dream, which is also
50
known as “dream sign”) and also to explicitly note any eye signals made at the
appropriate instances in the dream narrative.
Following the collection of a full open-ended dream report, further confirmation of
lucidity and eye signaling can be accomplished with simple follow-up questions,
including, for example: 1) “Were you aware of the fact that you were dreaming while
you were dreaming?” (YES/NO), If YES: 2) “How confident are you that you became
lucid?” (0-4 scale), 3) “Did you have a wake-initiated lucid dream (WILD) or a dream
initiated lucid dream (DILD)?” (DILD/WILD), 4) “Did you make the eye movement
signal to indicate that you became lucid?” (YES/NO), 5) “Please briefly describe how
you became lucid.” These example questions are not meant to be exhaustive, but they
illustrate the types of questions that in our view are useful to obtaining an accurate and
thorough assessment of lucid dreams following collection of a full dream report.
9.3 Measurement of individual differences in lucid dreaming
Another related question facing researchers is how to measure individual differences
in lucid dream frequency, which has been done in inconsistent ways, and could be
improved in future research. A method used in several studies consists of an 8-point
scale that asks participants to self-rate the frequency with which they experience lucid
dreams, ranging from “never” to “several times per week” (Schredl and Erlacher, 2004;
Mota-Rolim et al., 2013). While this method provides a straightforward coarse
assessment of an individual’s estimated frequency of lucid dreams that has shown high
test-retest reliability (Stumbrys, Erlacher and Schredl, 2013a), a limitation is that it does
not measure lucid dream frequency greater than several times per week. The scale could
be improved by including additional categories on the higher end of the measure,
including, for example, “Every night” and “Multiple times per night.” While individuals
who experience lucid dreams on a nightly basis represent a very small percentage of
respondents (according to Baird et al. (2018a) approximately one in one thousand),
optimally this kind of instrument would enable a researcher to distinguish respondents
that have lucid dreams once or several times a week from those that have them every
night or multiple times per night. Indeed, these “virtuoso” lucid dreamers represent
perhaps one of the most interesting populations for cognitive neuroscience studies of
individual differences in lucid dreaming (Baird et al., 2018a).
51
A related method is to query the number of lucid dreams reported in a given period
of time (i.e., the last 6 months), which has the advantage of asking participants to report
a specific number rather than a coarse estimate. In principle, this method has the
potential to be more accurate and to capture increased variance. However, some
participants, particularly those with high lucid dream rates, may not be able to recall all
instances of their lucid dreams within the requested time interval, and may therefore
resort to heuristics when answering the question, making it akin to simply asking
participants to report frequency using a multiple-choice scale. Furthermore, this method
may not accurately assess lucid dream frequency over longer periods of time i.e., an
individual may normally experience lucid dreams frequently, but has not experienced
them as frequently during the last queried interval of time. If using this approach, it is
therefore advisable to also collect additional estimates of frequency, including the most
lucid dream episodes experienced in any 6-month period (e.g., LaBerge et al., 2018b).
A limitation of the above methods is that they do not measure variation in the length
or degree of lucid dreams. Indeed, lucid dreams can range from a mere fleeting thought
about the fact that one is dreaming followed by an immediate loss of lucidity or
awakening, to extended lucid dreams in which an individual is able to engage in
sequences of actions (e.g., LaBerge and Rheingold, 1990). Distinguishing between these
different “levels” or subtypes of lucid dreams will likely be valuable to understanding
observed differences (or lack of differences) in brain structural or functional measures
associated with lucid dream frequency. Along these lines, several recent questionnaires
have taken first steps to measure individual differences in specific characteristics of
lucid dreaming. For example, the Lucid Dreaming Skills Questionnaire (LUSK)
measures participants’ frequency of several different but inter-related aspects of
awareness and control in lucid dreams (Schredl, Rieger and Göritz, 2018). Another
questionnaire, the Frequency and Intensity Lucid Dreaming Questionnaire (FILD)
queries participants regarding the duration of their lucid dreams and various aspects of
dream control, as well as the frequency with which they deliberately attempt to induce
lucid dreams (Aviram and Soffer-Dudek, 2018). In addition to providing data for
individual differences studies, such questionnaires could also potentially be useful in
selecting participants for sleep laboratory experiments of lucid dreaming, for example
by selecting participants who report high levels of dream control in addition to frequent
52
lucid dreams (in order to select participants who are more likely to be able to effectively
make the eye signals or engage in experimental tasks during lucid dreams).
For all the above methods, important steps need to be taken to minimize
measurement error, particularly to ensure that participants have a clear understanding of
the meaning of lucid dreaming (Snyder and Gackenbach, 1988). These include
providing participants with a written definition of lucid dreaming, asking participants to
provide written examples of their own lucid dreams to ensure clear understanding, as
well as additional vetting through participant interviews (Baird et al., 2018a).
Ultimately, however, basing the measurement of individual differences in lucid
dreaming solely on self-report is not optimal. One way to further validate participant
questionnaire responses would be to attempt to physiologically validate at least one
lucid dream in the sleep laboratory for each participant. While additional validations
such as this could potentially be valuable to incorporate in future studies, it is important
to note that the estimated frequency of lucid dreaming would still depend on
questionnaire assessment. Thus, approaches such as this do not obviate the reliance on
questionnaire assessment.
An intriguing, though ambitious, method for deriving a measure of lucid dream
frequency not dependent on questionnaire assessment would be to utilize home-based
EEG recording systems to collect longitudinal sleep polysomnography data, from which
estimates of lucid dreaming frequency could be derived from the frequency of signal-
verified lucid dreams collected over many nights. However, this approach would only
measure the frequency of signal-verified lucid dreams, and instances in which
participants achieved lucidity but did not make the eye signal due to factors such as
awakening, forgetting the intention, or lack of dream control would be missed by this
procedure. There are many more points that could be addressed on the topic of
questionnaire assessment of lucid dreaming frequency, but an extended analysis of this
issue is beyond the scope of the present review.
9.6 General discussion of the measurement of lucid dreaming in cognitive neuroscience
In summary, cognitive neuroscience studies of lucid dreaming have at their disposal
a unique set of rigorous methodological tools, including in particular the eye movement
signaling method, which allows for the objective validation of lucid dreaming as well as
53
precise time-stamping of physiological data. However, refinement of the instructions
given to participants, as described above, could help further increase the reliability of
the technique. Accurate phenomenological assessment of lucidity has been more mixed.
In sleep laboratory studies using the eye signaling method, phenomenological reports
are important but less critical due to the presence of an objective marker of lucidity. In
contrast, in studies without eye signaling, accurate phenomenological assessment of
whether an individual was lucid becomes essential, and inadequate or ambiguous
measurement of lucidity can undermine the interpretability of the results. Questionnaire
measures such as the DLQ (Stumbrys et al., 2013b) or LuCiD scale (Voss et al., 2013)
by themselves do not provide an unambiguous assessment of whether an individual had
a lucid dream. Cognitive neuroscience research on lucid dreaming would greatly benefit
from the further development of improved questionnaire measures for the validation of
lucidity and the cognitive and experiential changes that accompany it, as well as further
development of standardized measures for quantifying both the frequency and degree of
lucidity. Overall, this brief discussion highlights the need for a set of standard operating
procedures for both the phenomenological and objective sleep laboratory assessment of
lucid dreaming.
54
10. Conclusion
Despite having been physiologically validated for approximately four decades, the
neurobiology of lucid dreaming is still incompletely characterized. Most studies
conducted to date have relied on small sample sizes, which limits the generalizability of
the findings. Not surprisingly, the results of such underpowered studies are not
consistent: almost every EEG study reports changes in spectral power in a different
frequency band or brain area. Neuroimaging data on lucid dreaming is even sparser.
Currently, there is only one fMRI study contrasting lucid and non-lucid REM sleep and
it is a case study. Nevertheless, the results of this study converge with MRI studies that
have evaluated individual differences in lucid dreaming frequency. Together, this
preliminary evidence suggests that regions of anterior prefrontal, parietal and temporal
cortex are involved in lucid dreaming. The involvement of these brain regions in
metacognitive processes during the waking state is also in line with these findings.
A primary goal is to develop reliable strategies for making lucid dreaming more
accessible. As reviewed above, several studies have explored methods for non-invasive
electrical stimulation of the brain as well as pharmacological approaches to lucid dream
induction. Electrical stimulation of prefrontal brain areas has resulted in statistically
significant but weak increases of dream “insight” ratings, but so far it has not resulted in
significant increases in the frequency of lucid dreams. Currently it remains too early to
tell how effective (if at all) electrical stimulation of the frontal cortex, or other brain
areas, could be for lucid dream induction. Pharmacological induction with agents acting
on the cholinergic system, in particular the AChEI galantamine, has shown promising
results; however, these findings need to be replicated systematically, and lucid dreams
objectively confirmed with polysomnography. Other approaches to lucid dream
induction not discussed in the current review, and that do not directly target neural
mechanisms, such as cognitive/psychological approaches, also appear promising
(Stumbrys et al., 2012). For example, advances in the research of targeted memory
reactivation via olfactory or acoustic stimuli during sleep (e.g., Oudiette, Antony,
Creery and Paller, 2013) might lead to new strategies for lucid dream induction,
together with continued research on stimulating lucid dreams with visual or auditory
cues (LaBerge and Levitan, 1995; LaBerge et al., 1988; LaBerge et al., 1981b).
In conclusion, additional studies with larger sample sizes, for example large-scale
group-level high-density EEG, MEG, and concurrent EEG/fMRI studies, will be
55
important next steps toward characterizing the neural functional changes associated with
lucid dreaming. For now, a more detailed understanding of the neurobiological basis of
lucid dreaming remains an open question for ongoing research. Lucid dreaming shows
promise as a useful methodology for psychophysiological studies of REM sleep, with
potential applications in both clinical and basic research domains. Perhaps the largest
potential of research on lucid dreaming is that it provides a unique method to investigate
the neurobiology of consciousness, which remains one of the largest lacunas in
scientific knowledge.
56
Acknowledgements
We thank Stephen LaBerge for helpful discussion.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Funding
BB was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) under Ruth L. Kirschstein
National Research Service Award F32NS089348 from the NINDS. SAM-R was
supported by the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior
(CAPES), Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq),
Financiadora de Estudos e Projetos do Ministério da Ciência e Tecnologia (FINEP), and
Fundação de Apoio à Pesquisa do Estado do Rio Grande do Norte (FAPERN). MD was
supported by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO) through a
Vidi fellowship (016.Vidi.185.142). The content is solely the responsibility of the
authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH, CAPES,
CNPq, FINEP, FAPERN or NWO.
57
References
Abramovitch,H.,1995.Thenightmareofreturninghome:Acaseofacuteonsetnightmare
disordertreatedbyluciddreaming.Israeljournalofpsychiatryandrelatedsciences32,
140‐145.
Amatruda,T.T.,Black,D.A.,McKenna,T.M.,McCarley,R.W.,Hobson,J.A.,1975.Sleepcycle
controlandcholinergicmechanisms:Differentialeffectsofcarbacholinjectionsat
pontinebrainstemsites.BrainRes.98,501‐515.
Antrobus,J.S.,Antrobus,J.S.,1967.Discriminationoftwosleepstagesbyhumansubjects.
Psychophysiology4,48‐55.
Aristotle,1941.OnDreams,in:McKeon,R.(Ed.),ThebasicworksofAristotle.RandomHouse,
NewYork,NY,pp.618‐625.
Arnulf,I.,2011.The'scanninghypothesis'ofrapideyemovementsduringREMsleep:Areview
oftheevidence.Arch.Ital.Biol.149,367‐382.
Aspy,D.J.,Delfabbro,P.,Proeve,M.,Mohr,P.,2017.RealitytestingandtheMnemonic
InductionofLucidDreams:FindingsfromthenationalAustralianluciddreaminduction
study.Dreaming27,206‐231.
Aviram,L.,Soffer‐Dudek,N.,2018.Luciddreaming:Intensity,butnotfrequency,isinversely
relatedtopsychopathology.Front.Psychol.9,384.
Baghdoyan,H.A.,1997.CholinergicmechanismsregulatingREMsleep,in:Schwartz,W.(Ed.),
Sleepscience:Integratingbasicresearchandclinicalpractice.Karger,Basel,pp.88‐
116.
Baier,M.,2010.Insightinschizophrenia:Areview.Curr.PsychiatryRep.12,356‐361.
Baird,B.,Castelnovo,A.,Gosseries,O.,Tononi,G.,2018a.Frequentluciddreamingassociated
withincreasedfunctionalconnectivitybetweenfrontopolarcortexand
temporoparietalassociationareas.Sci.Rep.
Baird,B.,Castelnovo,A.,Riedner,B.A.,Lutz,A.,Ferrarelli,F.,Boly,M.,Davidson,R.J.,Tononi,
G.,2018b.Humanrapideyemovementsleepshowslocalincreasesinlow‐frequency
oscillationsandglobaldecreasesinhigh‐frequencyoscillationscomparedtoresting
wakefulness.eNeuro5.
Baird,B.,Riedner,B.A.,Boly,M.,Davidson,R.J.,Tononi,G.,2018c.Increasedluciddream
frequencyinlong‐termmeditatorsbutnotfollowingmindfulness‐basedstress
reductiontraining.PsychologyofConsciousness:Theory,ResearchandPractice.
Baird,B.,Smallwood,J.,Gorgolewski,K.J.,Margulies,D.S.,2013.Medialandlateralnetworks
inanteriorprefrontalcortexsupportmetacognitiveabilityformemoryandperception.
J.Neurosci.33,16657‐16665.
Bentley,P.,Driver,J.,Dolan,R.J.,2011.Cholinergicmodulationofcognition:Insightsfrom
humanpharmacologicalfunctionalneuroimaging.Prog.Neurobiol.94,360‐388.
Berrios,G.E.,Markova,I.S.,1998.Insightinthepsychoses:Aconceptualhistory,in:Amador,
X.,David,A.(Eds.),Insightandpsychosis.OxfordUniversityPress,NewYork,NY,pp.
33‐46.
Berryhill,M.E.,Phuong,L.,Picasso,L.,Cabeza,R.,Olson,I.R.,2007.Parietallobeandepisodic
memory:Bilateraldamagecausesimpairedfreerecallofautobiographicalmemory.J.
Neurosci.27,14415‐14423.
Blumenfeld,H.,2008.Epilepsyandconsciousness,in:Laureys,S.(Ed.),Theneurologyof
consciousness.ElsevierAcademicPress,Oxford,pp.247–260.
Boly,M.,Garrido,M.I.,Gosseries,O.,Bruno,M.‐A.,Boveroux,P.,Schnakers,C.,Massimini,M.,
Litvak,V.,Laureys,S.,Friston,K.,2011.Preservedfeedforwardbutimpairedtop‐down
processesinthevegetativestate.Science332,858‐862.
Boly,M.,Moran,R.,Murphy,M.,Boveroux,P.,Bruno,M.‐A.,Noirhomme,Q.,Ledoux,D.,
Bonhomme,V.,Brichant,J.‐F.,Tononi,G.,2012.Connectivitychangesunderlying
58
spectralEEGchangesduringpropofol‐inducedlossofconsciousness.J.Neurosci.32,
7082‐7090.
Braun,A.,Balkin,T.,Wesenten,N.,Carson,R.,Varga,M.,Baldwin,P.,Selbie,S.,Belenky,G.,
Herscovitch,P.,1997.Regionalcerebralbloodflowthroughoutthesleep‐wakecycle.
AnH2(15)OPETstudy.Brain120,1173‐1197.
Braun,A.R.,Balkin,T.J.,Wesensten,N.J.,Gwadry,F.,Carson,R.E.,Varga,M.,Baldwin,P.,
Belenky,G.,Herscovitch,P.,1998.Dissociatedpatternofactivityinvisualcorticesand
theirprojectionsduringhumanrapideyemovementsleep.Science279,91‐95.
Brylowski,A.,Levitan,L.,LaBerge,S.,1989.H‐reflexsuppressionandautonomicactivation
duringlucidREMsleep:Acasestudy.Sleep12,374‐378.
Cavallero,C.,Cicogna,P.,Natale,V.,Occhionero,M.,Zito,A.,1992.Slowwavesleepdreaming.
Sleep15,562‐566.
Cavanna,A.E.,Trimble,M.R.,2006.Theprecuneus:Areviewofitsfunctionalanatomyand
behaviouralcorrelates.Brain129,564‐583.
Christoff,K.,Ream,J.M.,Geddes,L.,Gabrieli,J.D.,2003.Evaluatingself‐generatedinformation:
Anteriorprefrontalcontributionstohumancognition.Behav.Neurosci.117,1161‐
1168.
Cuadra,G.,Summers,K.,Giacobini,E.,1994.Cholinesteraseinhibitoreffectson
neurotransmittersinratcortexinvivo.J.Pharmacol.Exp.Ther.270,277‐284.
Dane,J.,VandeCaslte,R.,1984.Acomparisonofwakinginstructionandposthypnotic
suggestionforluciddreaminduction.LucidityLetter3.
David,A.S.,1990.Insightandpsychosis.TheBritishJournalofPsychiatry156,798‐808.
DeGracia,D.J.,LaBerge,S.,2000.Varietiesofluciddreamingexperience,in:Kunzendorf,R.G.,
Wallace,B.(Eds.),Individualdifferencesinconsciousexperience.JohnBenjamins,
Amsterdam,pp.269‐307.
Dement,W.,Wolpert,E.A.,1958.Therelationofeyemovements,bodymotility,andexternal
stimulitodreamcontent.J.Exp.Psychol.55,543‐552.
Dixon,M.L.,DeLaVega,A.,Mills,C.,Andrews‐Hanna,J.,Spreng,R.N.,Cole,M.W.,Christoff,K.,
2018.Heterogeneitywithinthefrontoparietalcontrolnetworkanditsrelationshipto
thedefaultanddorsalattentionnetworks.Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.,E1598–E1607.
Dodet,P.,Chavez,M.,Leu‐Semenescu,S.,Golmard,J.,Arnulf,I.,2014.Luciddreamingin
narcolepsy.Sleep38,487–497.
Donner,T.H.,Siegel,M.,2011.Aframeworkforlocalcorticaloscillationpatterns.TrendsCog.
Sci.15,191‐199.
Dresler,M.,Eibl,L.,Fischer,C.F.,Wehrle,R.,Spoormaker,V.I.,Steiger,A.,Czisch,M.,
Pawlowski,M.,2014.Volitionalcomponentsofconsciousnessvaryacrosswakefulness,
dreamingandluciddreaming.Front.Psychol.4,987.
Dresler,M.,Koch,S.P.,Wehrle,R.,Spoormaker,V.I.,Holsboer,F.,Steiger,A.,Sämann,P.G.,
Obrig,H.,Czisch,M.,2011.Dreamedmovementelicitsactivationinthesensorimotor
cortex.Curr.Biol.21,1833‐1837.
Dresler,M.,Sandberg,A.,Bublitz,C.,Ohla,K.,Trenado,C.,Mroczko‐Wąsowicz,A.,Kühn,S.,
Repantis,D.,2018.Hackingthebrain:Dimensionsofcognitiveenhancement.ACS
ChemicalNeuroscience.
Dresler,M.,Wehrle,R.,Spoormaker,V.I.,Koch,S.P.,Holsboer,F.,Steiger,A.,Obrig,H.,
Sämann,P.G.,Czisch,M.,2012.Neuralcorrelatesofdreamlucidityobtainedfrom
contrastinglucidversusnon‐lucidREMsleep:AcombinedEEG/fMRIcasestudy.Sleep
35,1017‐1020.
Dresler,M.,Wehrle,R.,Spoormaker,V.I.,Steiger,A.,Holsboer,F.,Czisch,M.,Hobson,J.A.,
2015.Neuralcorrelatesofinsightindreamingandpsychosis.SleepMed.Rev.20,92‐
99.
Engel,A.K.,Fries,P.,2010.Beta‐bandoscillations—signallingthestatusquo?Curr.Opin.
Neurobiol.20,156‐165.
59
Engel,J.,Kuhl,D.E.,Phelps,M.E.,1982.Patternsofhumanlocalcerebralglucosemetabolism
duringepilepticseizures.Science218,64‐66.
Erlacher,D.,Schredl,M.,LaBerge,S.,2003.Motorareaactivationduringdreamedhand
clenching:ApilotstudyonEEGalphaband.SleepHypnosis5,182‐187.
Filevich,E.,Dresler,M.,Brick,T.R.,Kühn,S.,2015.Metacognitivemechanismsunderlyinglucid
dreaming.J.Neurosci.35,1082‐1088.
Fleming,S.M.,Weil,R.S.,Nagy,Z.,Dolan,R.J.,Rees,G.,2010.Relatingintrospectiveaccuracy
toindividualdifferencesinbrainstructure.Science329,1541‐1543.
Fosse,R.,2000.REMmentationinnarcolepticsandnormals:Anempiricaltestoftwo
neurocognitivetheories.Conscious.Cogn.9,488‐509.
Funk,C.M.,Honjoh,S.,Rodriguez,A.V.,Cirelli,C.,Tononi,G.,2016.Localslowwavesin
superficiallayersofprimarycorticalareasduringREMsleep.Curr.Biol.26,396‐403.
Gackenbach,J.,Cranson,R.,Alexander,C.,1986.Luciddreaming,witnessingdreaming,andthe
transcendentalmeditationtechnique:Adevelopmentalrelationship.LucidityLetter5,
34–40.
Gackenbach,J.,LaBerge,S.,1988.Consciousmind,sleepingbrain:Perspectivesonlucid
dreaming.PlenumPress,NewYork,NY.
Garfield,P.,Fellows,P.,Halliday,G.,Malamud,J.R.,1988.Clinicalapplicationsoflucid
dreaming,in:Gackenbach,J.,LaBerge,S.(Eds.),ConsciousMind,SleepingBrain.
PlenumPress,NewYork,NY,pp.289‐319.
Giacobini,E.,Zhu,X.‐D.,Williams,E.,Sherman,K.,1996.Theeffectoftheselectivereversible
acetylcholinesteraseinhibitorE2020onextracellularacetylcholineandbiogenicamine
levelsinratcortex.Neuropharmacology35,205‐211.
Gillin,J.C.,Sitaram,N.,Janowsky,D.,Risch,C.,Huey,L.,Storch,F.I.,1985.Cholinergic
mechanismsinREMsleep,in:Wauquier,A.,Gaillard,J.,Monti,J.,Radulovacki,M.
(Eds.),Sleep:Neurotransmittersandneuromodulators.RavenPress,NewYork,NY,pp.
29‐42.
Goldberg,G.,1985.Supplementarymotorareastructureandfunction:Reviewand
hypotheses.Behav.Brain.Sci.8,567‐588.
Gratton,G.,Coles,M.G.,Donchin,E.,1983.Anewmethodforoff‐lineremovalofocular
artifact.Electroencephalogr.Clin.Neurophysiol.55,468‐484.
Green,C.E.,1968.Luciddreams.Hamilton,London.
Hallett,M.,2007.Transcranialmagneticstimulation:Aprimer.Neuron55,187‐199.
Hearne,K.M.,1978.Luciddreams:Anelecro‐physiologicalandpsychologicalstudy
(Unpublisheddoctoraldissertation).LiverpoolUniversity,Liverpool,UK.
Hermann,D.M.,Siccoli,M.,Brugger,P.,Wachter,K.,Mathis,J.,Achermann,P.,Bassetti,C.L.,
2008.Evolutionofneurological,neuropsychologicalandsleep‐wakedisturbancesafter
paramedianthalamicstroke.Stroke39,62‐68.
Hernandez‐Peon,R.,Chavez‐Ibarra,G.,Morgane,P.,Timo‐Iaria,C.,1963.Limbiccholinergic
pathwaysinvolvedinsleepandemotionalbehavior.Exp.Neurol.8,93‐111.
Hipp,J.F.,Siegel,M.,2013.Dissociatingneuronalgamma‐bandactivityfromcranialandocular
muscleactivityinEEG.Front.Hum.Neurosci.7,338.
Hobson,J.A.,Pace‐Schott,E.F.,2002.Thecognitiveneuroscienceofsleep:Neuronalsystems,
consciousnessandlearning.Nat.Rev.Neurosci.3,679‐693.
Hobson,J.A.,Pace‐Schott,E.F.,Stickgold,R.,2000.Dreamingandthebrain:Towardacognitive
neuroscienceofconsciousstates.Behav.Brain.Sci.23,793‐842.
Hodes,R.,Dement,W.C.,1964.Depressionofelectricallyinducedreflexes(“H‐reflexes”)in
manduringlowvoltageEEG“sleep”.Electroencephalogr.Clin.Neurophysiol.17,617‐
629.
Holzinger,B.,Klösch,G.,Saletu,B.,2015.Studieswithluciddreamingasadd‐ontherapyto
Gestalttherapy.ActaNeurol.Scand.131,355‐363.
60
Holzinger,B.,LaBerge,S.,Levitan,L.,2006.Psychophysiologicalcorrelatesofluciddreaming.
Dreaming16,88‐95.
Joseph,R.,1999.Frontallobepsychopathology:Mania,depression,confabulation,catatonia,
perseveration,obsessivecompulsions,andschizophrenia.Psychiatry62,138‐172.
Kahan,T.L.,LaBerge,S.,Levitan,L.,Zimbardo,P.,1997.Similaritiesanddifferencesbetween
dreamingandwakingcognition:Anexploratorystudy.Conscious.Cogn.6,132‐147.
Kahan,T.L.,Sullivan,K.T.,2012.Assessingmetacognitiveskillsinwakingandsleep:A
psychometricanalysisoftheMetacognitive,Affective,CognitiveExperience(MACE)
questionnaire.Conscious.Cogn.21,340‐352.
Keren,A.S.,Yuval‐Greenberg,S.,Deouell,L.Y.,2010.Saccadicspikepotentialsingamma‐band
EEG:Characterization,detectionandsuppression.NeuroImage49,2248‐2263.
Kern,S.,Appel,K.,Schredl,M.,Pipa,G.,2017.NoeffectofαGPConluciddreaminductionor
dreamcontent.Somnologie21,180‐186.
Kjaer,T.W.,Nowak,M.,Lou,H.C.,2002.Reflectiveself‐awarenessandconsciousstates:PET
evidenceforacommonmidlineparietofrontalcore.NeuroImage17,1080‐1086.
Komogortsev,O.V.,Karpov,A.,2013.Automatedclassificationandscoringofsmoothpursuit
eyemovementsinthepresenceoffixationsandsaccades.Behav.Res.Methods45,
203‐215.
Koontz,J.,Baskys,A.,2005.Effectsofgalantamineonworkingmemoryandglobalfunctioning
inpatientswithmildcognitiveimpairment:Adouble‐blindplacebo‐controlledstudy.
Am.J.Alzheimer'sDis.OtherDementias20,295‐302.
Koshino,Y.,Niedermeyer,E.,1975.Enhancementofrolandicmu‐rhythmbypatternvision.
Electroencephalogr.Clin.Neurophysiol.38,535‐538.
LaBerge,S.,1980a.Luciddreamingasalearnableskill:Acasestudy.Percept.MotorSkills51,
1039‐1042.
LaBerge,S.,1980b.Luciddreaming:Anexploratorystudyofconsciousnessduringsleep
(Unpublisheddoctoraldissertation).StanfordUniversity,Stanford,CA.
LaBerge,S.,1985.Luciddreaming:Thepowerofbeingawakeandawareinyourdreams.
JeremyP.Tarcher,LosAngeles.
LaBerge,S.,1988a.Luciddreaminginwesternliterature,in:LaBerge,J.G.S.(Ed.),Conscious
Mind,SleepingBrain.PlenumPress,NewYork,NY,pp.11‐26.
LaBerge,S.,1988b.Thepsychophysiologyofluciddreaming,in:Gackenbach,J.,LaBerge,S.
(Eds.),Consciousmind,sleepingbrain:Perspectivesonluciddreaming.PlenumPress,
NewYork,NY,pp.135‐153.
LaBerge,S.,1990.Luciddreaming:PsychophysiologicalstudiesofconsciousnessduringREM
sleep,in:Bootzin,R.R.,Kihlstrom,J.F.,Schacter,D.L.(Eds.),SleepandCognition.
AmericanPsychologicalAssociation,WashingtonDC,pp.109‐126.
LaBerge,S.,2001.Theparadoxandpromiseofluciddreaming.Researchupdate:Cholinergic
stimulationofluciddreaming;voluntarycontrolofauditoryperceptionduringREM
luciddreams,InternationalAssociationfortheStudyofDreams,Berkeley.
LaBerge,S.,2003.Luciddreamingandtheyogaofthedreamstate:Apsychophysiological
perspective,in:Wallace,B.A.(Ed.),Buddhism&science:Breakingnewground.
ColumbiaUniversityPress,NewYork,NY,pp.233‐258.
LaBerge,S.,2010.Signal‐verfiedluciddreamingprovesthatREMsleepcansupportreflective
consciousness.Int.J.DreamRes.3,26‐27.
LaBerge,S.,2015.Luciddreaming:Metaconsciousnessduringparadoxicalsleep,in:Glucksman,
M.K.M.(Ed.),Dreamresearch:Contributionstoclinicalpractice.Routledge,NewYork,
NY,pp.198‐214.
LaBerge,S.,Baird,B.,Zimbardo,P.G.,2018a.Smoothtrackingofvisualtargetsdistinguishes
lucidREMsleepdreamingandwakingperceptionfromimagination.Nat.Comm.9,
3298.
61
LaBerge,S.,Dement,W.C.,1982.VoluntarycontrolofrespirationduringREMsleep.SleepRes.
11,107.
LaBerge,S.,Greenleaf,W.,Kedzierski,B.,1983.Physiologicalresponsestodreamedsexual
activityduringlucidREMsleep.Psychophysiology,454‐455.
LaBerge,S.,LaMarca,K.,Baird,B.,2018b.Pre‐sleeptreatmentwithgalantaminestimulates
luciddreaming:Adouble‐blind,placebo‐controlled,crossoverstudy.PLoSONE13,
e0201246.
LaBerge,S.,Levitan,L.,1995.ValidityestablishedofDreamLightcuesforelicitinglucid
dreaming.Dreaming5,159‐168.
LaBerge,S.,Levitan,L.,Dement,W.C.,1986.Luciddreaming:Physiologicalcorrelatesof
consciousnessduringREMsleep.J.MindBehav.7,251‐258.
LaBerge,S.,Levitan,L.,Rich,R.,Dement,W.C.,1988.Inductionofluciddreamingbylight
stimulationduringREMsleep.SleepRes.17,104.
LaBerge,S.,Nagel,L.,Taylor,W.,Dement,W.,Zarcone,V.,1981a.Psychophysiological
correlatesoftheinitiationofluciddreaming.SleepRes.10,149.
LaBerge,S.,Owens,J.,Nagel,L.,Dement,W.C.,1981b."Thisisadream”:Inductionoflucid
dreamsbyverbalsuggestionduringREMsleep.SleepRes.10,150.
LaBerge,S.,Phillips,L.,Levitan,L.,1994.Anhourofwakefulnessbeforemorningnapsmakes
luciditymorelikely.NightLight6,1‐4.
LaBerge,S.,Rheingold,H.,1990.Exploringtheworldofluciddreaming.BallantineBooksNew
York,NY.
LaBerge,S.P.,Nagel,L.E.,Dement,W.C.,Zarcone,V.P.,1981c.Luciddreamingverifiedby
volitionalcommunicationduringREMsleep.Percept.MotorSkills52,727‐732.
Lachaux,J.P.,Fonlupt,P.,Kahane,P.,Minotti,L.,Hoffmann,D.,Bertrand,O.,Baciu,M.,2007.
Relationshipbetweentask‐relatedgammaoscillationsandBOLDsignal:Newinsights
fromcombinedfMRIandintracranialEEG.Hum.BrainMapp.28,1368‐1375.
Lancee,J.,VanDenBout,J.,Spoormaker,V.I.,2010.Expandingself‐helpimageryrehearsal
therapyfornightmareswithsleephygieneandluciddreaming:Awaiting‐list
controlledtrial.Int.J.DreamRes.3,111‐120.
Laureys,S.,Owen,A.M.,Schiff,N.D.,2004.Brainfunctionincoma,vegetativestate,and
relateddisorders.LancetNeurol.3,537‐546.
Laureys,S.,Perrin,F.,Brédart,S.,2007.Self‐consciousnessinnon‐communicativepatients.
Conscious.Cogn.16,722‐741.
Lee,U.,Ku,S.,Noh,G.,Baek,S.,Choi,B.,Mashour,G.A.,2013.Disruptionoffrontal‐parietal
communicationbyketamine,propofol,andsevoflurane.Anesthesiology118,1264‐
1275.
Levin,R.,Nielsen,T.A.,2007.Disturbeddreaming,posttraumaticstressdisorder,andaffect
distress:Areviewandneurocognitivemodel.Psychol.Bull.133,482.
Lincoln,T.M.,Lüllmann,E.,Rief,W.,2007.Correlatesandlong‐termconsequencesofpoor
insightinpatientswithschizophrenia.Asystematicreview.SchizophreniaBulletin33,
1324‐1342.
Lou,H.C.,Luber,B.,Crupain,M.,Keenan,J.P.,Nowak,M.,Kjaer,T.W.,Sackeim,H.A.,Lisanby,
S.H.,2004.Parietalcortexandrepresentationofthementalself.Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.
101,6827‐6832.
Luigetti,M.,DiLazzaro,V.,Broccolini,A.,Vollono,C.,Dittoni,S.,Frisullo,G.,Pilato,F.,Profice,
P.,Losurdo,A.,Morosetti,R.,2011.Bilateralthalamicstroketransientlyreduces
arousalsandNREMsleepinstability.J.Neurol.Sci.300,151‐154.
Malcolm,N.,1959.Dreaming.Routledge,London,England.
Maquet,P.,Péters,J.‐M.,Aerts,J.,Delfiore,G.,Degueldre,C.,Luxen,A.,Franck,G.,1996.
Functionalneuroanatomyofhumanrapid‐eye‐movementsleepanddreaming.Nature
383,163‐166.
62
Mashour,G.A.,2014.Top‐downmechanismsofanesthetic‐inducedunconsciousness.Front.
Sys.Neurosci.8,115.
Mason,L.I.,Orme‐Johnson,D.W.,2010.Transcendentalconsciousnesswakesupindreaming
anddeepsleep.Int.J.DreamRes.3,28‐32.
Mazzetti,M.,Bellucci,C.,Mattarozzi,K.,Plazzi,G.,Tuozzi,G.,Cipolli,C.,2010.REM‐dreams
recallinpatientswithnarcolepsy‐cataplexy.BrainRes.Bull.81,133‐140.
McCaig,R.G.,Dixon,M.,Keramatian,K.,Liu,I.,Christoff,K.,2011.Improvedmodulationof
rostrolateralprefrontalcortexusingreal‐timefMRItrainingandmeta‐cognitive
awareness.NeuroImage55,1298‐1305.
McKay,L.C.,Adams,L.,Frackowiak,R.S.,Corfield,D.R.,2008.Abilateralcortico‐bulbarnetwork
associatedwithbreathholdinginhumans,determinedbyfunctionalmagnetic
resonanceimaging.NeuroImage40,1824‐1832.
Mintz,A.R.,Dobson,K.S.,Romney,D.M.,2003.Insightinschizophrenia:Ameta‐analysis.
SchizophreniaResearch61,75‐88.
Montagna,P.,2005.Fatalfamilialinsomnia:Amodeldiseaseinsleepphysiopathology.Sleep
Med.Rev.9,339‐353.
Morgenthaler,T.I.,Auerbach,S.,Casey,K.R.,Kristo,D.,Maganti,R.,Ramar,K.,Zak,R.,Kartje,
R.,2018.Positionpaperforthetreatmentofnightmaredisorderinadults:An
AmericanAcademyofSleepMedicinepositionpaper.JournalofClinicalSleep
Medicine14,1041‐1055.
Mota,N.B.,Resende,A.,Mota‐Rolim,S.A.,Copelli,M.,Ribeiro,S.,2016.Psychosisandthe
controlofluciddreaming.Front.Psychol.7,294.
Mota‐Rolim,S.,Pantoja,A.,Pinheiro,R.,Camilo,A.,Barbosa,T.,Hazboun,I.,Araujo,J.,Ribeiro,
S.,2008.Luciddream:Sleepelectroencephalographicfeaturesandbehavioral
inductionmethods,FirstCongressIBRO/LARCofNeurosciencesforLatinAmerica,
CaribbeanandIberianPeninsula.Búzios,Brazil.
Mota‐Rolim,S.A.,Araujo,J.F.,2013.Neurobiologyandclinicalimplicationsofluciddreaming.
Medicalhypotheses81,751‐756.
Mota‐Rolim,S.A.,Brandão,D.S.,Andrade,K.C.,deQueiroz,C.M.T.,Araujo,J.F.,deAraujo,D.B.,
Ribeiro,S.,2015.NeurophysiologicalfeaturesofluciddreamingduringN1andN2
sleepstages:Twocasereports.SleepSci.4,215.
Mota‐Rolim,S.A.,Erlacher,D.,Tort,A.B.,Araujo,J.F.,Ribeiro,S.,2010.Differentkindsof
subjectiveexperienceduringluciddreamingmayhavedifferentneuralsub‐strates.Int.
J.DreamRes.25,550‐557.
Mota‐Rolim,S.A.,Targino,Z.H.,Souza,B.C.,Blanco,W.,Araujo,J.F.,Ribeiro,S.,2013.Dream
characteristicsinaBraziliansample:Anonlinesurveyfocusingonluciddreaming.
Front.Hum.Neurosci.7,836.
Muzio,J.N.,Roffwarg,H.P.,Kaufman,E.,1966.Alterationsinthenocturnalsleepcycle
resultingfromLSD.Clin.Neurophysiol.21,313‐324.
Nieminen,J.O.,Gosseries,O.,Massimini,M.,Saad,E.,Sheldon,A.D.,Boly,M.,Siclari,F.,Postle,
B.R.,Tononi,G.,2016.Consciousnessandcorticalresponsiveness:Awithin‐statestudy
duringnon‐rapideyemovementsleep.Sci.Rep.6,30932.
Nir,Y.,Fisch,L.,Mukamel,R.,Gelbard‐Sagiv,H.,Arieli,A.,Fried,I.,Malach,R.,2007.Coupling
betweenneuronalfiringrate,gammaLFP,andBOLDfMRIisrelatedtointerneuronal
correlations.Curr.Biol.17,1275‐1285.
Nir,Y.,Tononi,G.,2010.Dreamingandthebrain:Fromphenomenologytoneurophysiology.
TrendsCog.Sci.14,88‐100.
Norbu,N.,Katz,M.,1992.Dreamyogaandthepracticeofnaturallight.SnowLion
Publications,Ithaca,NY.
Noreika,V.,Windt,J.M.,Lenggenhager,B.,Karim,A.A.,2010.Newperspectivesforthestudy
ofluciddreaming:Frombrainstimulationtophilosophicaltheoriesofself‐
consciousness.Int.J.DreamRes.3,36‐45.
63
Ogilvie,R.,Hunt,H.,Kushniruk,A.,Newman,J.,1983.Luciddreamsandthearousal
continuum.LucidityLetter2.
Ogilvie,R.,Hunt,H.,Sawicki,C.,McGowan,K.,1978.Searchingforluciddreams.SleepRes.7,
165.
Ogilvie,R.,Vieira,K.,Small,R.,1991.EEGactivityduringluciddreaming.LucidityLetter10.
Ogilvie,R.D.,Hunt,H.T.,Tyson,P.D.,Lucescu,M.L.,Jeakins,D.B.,1982.Luciddreamingand
alphaactivity:Apreliminaryreport.Percept.MotorSkills55,795‐808.
Oudiette,D.,Antony,J.W.,Creery,J.D.,Paller,K.A.,2013.Theroleofmemoryreactivation
duringwakefulnessandsleepindeterminingwhichmemoriesendure.J.Neurosci.33,
6672‐6678.
Oudiette,D.,Dodet,P.,Ledard,N.,Artru,E.,Rachidi,I.,Similowski,T.,Arnulf,I.,2018.REM
sleeprespiratorybehavioursmentalcontentinnarcolepticluciddreamers.Sci.Rep.8,
2636.
Padmasambhava,1998.Naturalliberation:Padmasambhava’steachingsonthesixbardos.
Wisdom,Boston,MA.
Perogamvros,L.,Baird,B.,Seibold,M.,Riedner,B.,Boly,M.,Tononi,G.,2017.The
phenomenalcontentsandneuralcorrelatesofspontaneousthoughtsacross
wakefulness,NREMsleep,andREMsleep.J.Cogn.Neurosci.29,1766‐1777.
Pfurtscheller,G.,Stancak,A.,Neuper,C.,1996.Post‐movementbetasynchronization.A
correlateofanidlingmotorarea?Electroencephalogr.Clin.Neurophysiol.98,281‐293.
Pigorini,A.,Sarasso,S.,Proserpio,P.,Szymanski,C.,Arnulfo,G.,Casarotto,S.,Fecchio,M.,
Rosanova,M.,Mariotti,M.,Russo,G.L.,2015.Bistabilitybreaks‐offdeterministic
responsestointracorticalstimulationduringnon‐REMsleep.NeuroImage112,105‐
113.
Price,R.F.,Cohen,D.B.,1988.Luciddreaminduction:Anempiricalevaluation.Consciousmind,
sleepingbrain:Perspectivesonluciddreaming,105‐134.
Purdon,P.L.,Pierce,E.T.,Mukamel,E.A.,Prerau,M.J.,Walsh,J.L.,Wong,K.F.K.,Salazar‐Gomez,
A.F.,Harrell,P.G.,Sampson,A.L.,Cimenser,A.,2013.Electroencephalogramsignatures
oflossandrecoveryofconsciousnessfrompropofol.Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.110,E1142‐
E1151.
Raggi,A.,Cosentino,F.I.,Lanuzza,B.,Ferri,R.,2010.Behaviouralandneurophysiologic
featuresofstatedissociation:Abriefreviewoftheliteratureandthreedescriptive
casestudies.Behaviouralneurology22,91‐99.
Rak,M.,Beitinger,P.,Steiger,A.,Schredl,M.,Dresler,M.,2015.Increasedluciddreaming
frequencyinnarcolepsy.Sleep38,787‐792.
Rechtschaffen,A.,Kales,A.,1968.Amanualofstandardizedterminology,techniquesand
scoringsystemforsleepstagesofhumansubjects.GovernmentPrintingOffice
NationalInstitutesofHealth,Washington,DC.
Rees,G.,Wojciulik,E.,Clarke,K.,Husain,M.,Frith,C.,Driver,J.,2002.Neuralcorrelatesof
consciousandunconsciousvisioninparietalextinction.Neurocase8,387‐393.
Riemann,D.,Gann,H.,Dressing,H.,Müller,W.E.,Aldenhoff,J.B.,1994.Influenceofthe
cholinesteraseinhibitorgalanthaminehydrobromideonnormalsleep.PsychiatryRes.
51,253‐267.
Roehrs,T.,Roth,T.,2001.Sleep,sleepiness,sleepdisordersandalcoholuseandabuse.Sleep
Med.Rev.5,287‐297.
Sagnier,S.,Coulon,P.,Chaufton,C.,Poli,M.,Debruxelles,S.,Renou,P.,Rouanet,F.,Olindo,S.,
Sibon,I.,2015.Luciddreams,anatypicalsleepdisturbanceinanteriorand
mediodorsalthalamicstrokes.Revueneurologique171,768‐772.
Saunders,D.T.,Roe,C.A.,Smith,G.,Clegg,H.,2016.Luciddreamingincidence:Aqualityeffects
meta‐analysisof50yearsofresearch.Conscious.Cogn.43,197‐215.
Schierenbeck,T.,Riemann,D.,Berger,M.,Hornyak,M.,2008.Effectofillicitrecreationaldrugs
uponsleep:Cocaine,ecstasyandmarijuana.SleepMed.Rev.12,381‐389.
64
Schmitz,T.W.,Rowley,H.A.,Kawahara,T.N.,Johnson,S.C.,2006.Neuralcorrelatesofself‐
evaluativeaccuracyaftertraumaticbraininjury.Neuropsychologia44,762‐773.
Schnakers,C.,Vanhaudenhuyse,A.,Giacino,J.,Ventura,M.,Boly,M.,Majerus,S.,Moonen,G.,
Laureys,S.,2009.Diagnosticaccuracyofthevegetativeandminimallyconsciousstate:
Clinicalconsensusversusstandardizedneurobehavioralassessment.BMCNeurol.9,
35.
Schooler,J.W.,2002.Re‐representingconsciousness:Dissociationsbetweenexperienceand
meta‐consciousness.TrendsCog.Sci.6,339‐344.
Schredl,M.,Erlacher,D.,2004.Luciddreamingfrequencyandpersonality.Pers.Individ.Dif.37,
1463‐1473.
Schredl,M.,Rieger,J.,Göritz,A.S.,2018.Measuringluciddreamingskills:Anewquestionnaire
(LUSK).Int.J.DreamRes.
Siclari,F.,Baird,B.,Perogamvros,L.,Bernardi,G.,LaRocque,J.J.,Riedner,B.,Boly,M.,Postle,
B.R.,Tononi,G.,2017.Theneuralcorrelatesofdreaming.Nat.Neurosci.20,872‐878.
Siclari,F.,LaRocque,J.J.,Postle,B.R.,Tononi,G.,2013.Assessingsleepconsciousnesswithin
subjectsusingaserialawakeningparadigm.Front.Psychol.4.
Snyder,T.J.,Gackenbach,J.,1988.Individualdifferencesassociatedwithluciddreaming,in:
LaBerge,S.,Gackenbach,J.(Eds.),Consciousmind,sleepingbrain.Plenum,NewYork,
NY,pp.221‐259.
Sparrow,G.,Hurd,R.,Carlson,R.,Molina,A.,2018.Exploringtheeffectsofgalantaminepaired
withmeditationanddreamrelivingonrecalleddreams:Towardanintegratedprotocol
forluciddreaminductionandnightmareresolution.Conscious.Cogn.63,74‐88.
Sparrow,G.S.,Thurston,M.,Carlson,R.,2013.Dreamrelivingandmeditationasawayto
enhancereflectivenessandconstructiveengagementindreams.Int.J.DreamRes.6,
84‐93.
Spering,M.,Montagnini,A.,2011.Dowetrackwhatwesee?Commonversusindependent
processingformotionperceptionandsmoothpursuiteyemovements:Areview.
VisionRes.51,836‐852.
Spoormaker,V.I.,Czisch,M.,Dresler,M.,2010.Lucidandnon‐luciddreaming:Thinkingin
networks.Int.J.DreamRes.3,49‐51.
Spoormaker,V.I.,VanDenBout,J.,2006.Luciddreamingtreatmentfornightmares:Apilot
study.Psychother.Psychosom.75,389‐394.
Steriade,M.,Timofeev,I.,Grenier,F.,2001.Naturalwakingandsleepstates:Aviewfrom
insideneocorticalneurons.J.Neurophysiol.85,1969‐1985.
Stickgold,R.,Malia,A.,Fosse,R.,Hobson,J.A.,2001.Brain–mindstates:I.Longitudinalfield
studyofsleep/wakefactorsinfluencingmentationreportlength.Sleep24,171‐179.
Stumbrys,T.,Erlacher,D.,2012.LuciddreamingduringNREMsleep:Twocasereports.Int.J.
DreamRes.5,151‐155.
Stumbrys,T.,Erlacher,D.,Malinowski,P.,2015.Meta‐awarenessduringdayandnight:The
relationshipbetweenmindfulnessandluciddreaming.Imagin.Cogn.Pers.34,415‐
433.
Stumbrys,T.,Erlacher,D.,Schädlich,M.,Schredl,M.,2012.Inductionofluciddreams:A
systematicreviewofevidence.Conscious.Cogn.21,1456‐1475.
Stumbrys,T.,Erlacher,D.,Schredl,M.,2013a.Reliabilityandstabilityofluciddreamand
nightmarefrequencyscales.Int.J.DreamRes.6,123‐126.
Stumbrys,T.,Erlacher,D.,Schredl,M.,2013b.Testingtheinvolvementoftheprefrontalcortex
inluciddreaming:AtDCSstudy.Conscious.Cogn.22,1214‐1222.
Tanner,B.A.,2004.Multimodalbehavioraltreatmentofnonrepetitive,treatment‐resistant
nightmares:Acasereport.Percept.MotorSkills99,1139‐1146.
Tononi,G.,2011.Integratedinformationtheoryofconsciousness:Anupdatedaccount.Arch.
Ital.Biol.150,56‐90.
65
Tononi,G.,Boly,M.,Massimini,M.,Koch,C.,2016.Integratedinformationtheory:From
consciousnesstoitsphysicalsubstrate.Nat.Rev.Neurosci.17,450‐461.
Tononi,G.,Massimini,M.,2008.Whydoesconsciousnessfadeinearlysleep?Ann.N.Y.Acad.
Sci.1129,330‐334.
Tyson,P.D.,Ogilvie,R.D.,Hunt,H.T.,1984.Lucid,prelucid,andnonluciddreamsrelatedtothe
amountofEEGalphaactivityduringREMsleep.Psychophysiology21,442‐451.
VanderWerf,Y.D.,Witter,M.P.,Groenewegen,H.J.,2002.Theintralaminarandmidlinenuclei
ofthethalamus.Anatomicalandfunctionalevidenceforparticipationinprocessesof
arousalandawareness.BrainRes.Rev.39,107‐140.
VanEeden,F.,1913.Astudyofdreams,ProceedingsoftheSocietyforPsychicalResearch,pp.
431‐461.
vanGaal,S.,Ridderinkhof,K.R.,Scholte,H.S.,Lamme,V.A.,2010.Unconsciousactivationofthe
prefrontalno‐gonetwork.J.Neurosci.30,4143‐4150.
Vanni,S.,Portin,K.,Virsu,V.,Hari,R.,1999.Murhythmmodulationduringchangesofvisual
percepts.Neuroscience91,21‐31.
Velazquez‐Moctezuma,J.,Shalauta,M.,Gillin,J.C.,Shiromani,P.J.,1991.Cholinergic
antagonistsandREMsleepgeneration.BrainRes.543,175‐179.
Violante,I.R.,Li,L.M.,Carmichael,D.W.,Lorenz,R.,Leech,R.,Hampshire,A.,Rothwell,J.C.,
Sharp,D.J.,2017.Externallyinducedfrontoparietalsynchronizationmodulates
networkdynamicsandenhancesworkingmemoryperformance.Elife6,e22001.
Vogel,G.W.,1975.AreviewofREMsleepdeprivation.Arch.Gen.Psychiatry32,749‐761.
Voss,U.,Holzmann,R.,Hobson,A.,Paulus,W.,Koppehele‐Gossel,J.,Klimke,A.,Nitsche,M.A.,
2014.Inductionofselfawarenessindreamsthroughfrontallowcurrentstimulationof
gammaactivity.Nat.Neurosci.17,810‐812.
Voss,U.,Holzmann,R.,Tuin,I.,Hobson,J.A.,2009.Luciddreaming:Astateofconsciousness
withfeaturesofbothwakingandnon‐luciddreaming.Sleep32,1191‐1200.
Voss,U.,Schermelleh‐Engel,K.,Windt,J.,Frenzel,C.,Hobson,A.,2013.Measuring
consciousnessindreams:Thelucidityandconsciousnessindreamsscale.Conscious.
Cogn.22,8‐21.
Wagner,A.D.,Shannon,B.J.,Kahn,I.,Buckner,R.L.,2005.Parietallobecontributionsto
episodicmemoryretrieval.TrendsCog.Sci.9,445‐453.
Wallace,B.A.,2013.Buddhismandscience:Breakingnewground.ColumbiaUniversityPress.
Wallace,B.A.,Hodel,B.,2012.Dreamingyourselfawake:LuciddreamingandTibetandream
yogaforinsightandtransformation.ShambhalaPublications,Boston,MA.
Windt,J.M.,2010.Theimmersivespatiotemporalhallucinationmodelofdreaming.
Phenomenol.Cogn.Sci.9,295‐316.
Windt,J.M.,2015.Dreaming:Aconceptualframeworkforphilosophyofmindandempirical
research.MITPress,Cambridge,MA.
Windt,J.M.,Metzinger,T.,2007.Thephilosophyofdreamingandself‐consciousness:What
happenstotheexperientialsubjectduringthedreamstate?,in:Barrett,D.,
McNamara,P.(Eds.),Thenewscienceofdreaming:Culturalandtheoretical
perspectives.Praeger,Westport,CT,pp.193‐247.
Windt,J.M.,Nielsen,T.,Thompson,E.,2016.Doesconsciousnessdisappearindreamless
sleep?TrendsCog.Sci.20,871‐882.
Yeo,B.,Krienen,F.M.,Sepulcre,J.,Sabuncu,M.R.,Lashkari,D.,Hollinshead,M.,Roffman,J.L.,
Smoller,J.W.,Zöllei,L.,Polimeni,J.R.,2011.Theorganizationofthehumancerebral
cortexestimatedbyintrinsicfunctionalconnectivity.J.Neurophysiol.106,1125‐1165.
Yuschak,T.,2006.Advancedluciddreaming:Thepowerofsupplements.LuluEnterprises,
Raleigh,NC.
Yuval‐Greenberg,S.,Deouell,L.Y.,2009.Thebroadband‐transientinducedgamma‐band
responseinscalpEEGreflectstheexecutionofsaccades.BrainTopogr.22,3‐6.
66
Yuval‐Greenberg,S.,Tomer,O.,Keren,A.S.,Nelken,I.,Deouell,L.Y.,2008.Transientinduced
gamma‐bandresponseinEEGasamanifestationofminiaturesaccades.Neuron58,
429‐441.
Zadra,A.L.,Pihl,R.O.,1997.Luciddreamingasatreatmentforrecurrentnightmares.
Psychother.Psychosom.66,50‐55.
67
Figure 1
Fig 1. Lucid REM sleep eye movement signaling paradigm. Exemplary left-right-
left-right-center (LRLR) eye movement signal during polysomnographcally-verified
REM sleep. Participants signal when they realize they are dreaming by rapidly looking
all the way to the left (as if looking at their ear) then all the way to the right two times
consecutively then back to center without pausing. The LRLR signal is readily
discernable in the HEOG, which exhibits a distinctive shape of four consecutive full-
scale eye movements of higher amplitude compared to typical REMs. Note high-
frequency electroencephalogram (EEG) with theta rhythm (~5 Hz) and lack of alpha at
OZ as well as minimal electromyogram (EMG) amplitude due to muscle atonia
characteristic of REM sleep (left) compared to wakefulness (right).
68
Figure 2
Figure 2. a) Blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) activation in fMRI case study
of lucid dreaming (Dresler et al., 2012). Clusters show regions with significantly
increased BOLD signal during lucid REM sleep (pFDR < 0.005) in the left lateral
hemisphere view (left) and right lateral hemisphere view (right). Increased activity was
observed in anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC), medial and lateral parietal cortex,
including the supramarginal and angular gyrus and inferior/middle temporal gyrus
during lucid REM sleep contrasted with non-lucid REM sleep. b) Seed-based resting-
state functional connectivity differences between frequent lucid dreamers and
controls (Baird et al., 2018a). To estimate connectivity, spherical regions-of-interest
were defined in aPFC based on the peak voxel reported in Dresler et al. (2012) (red
circle). Frequent lucid dreamers had increased resting-state functional connectivity
between left aPFC and bilateral angular gyrus, bilateral middle temportal gyrus and
right inferior frontal gyrus. All clusters are significant at p<0.05, corrected for multiple
comparisons at the cluster level.
... Hence, meditation practices cultivate specific mental skills, including enhanced attentional stability and the monitoring of one's mental state (i.e., meta-awareness) [18]. Meta-awareness is an essential cognitive capacity discussed as the primary cognitive component for the state shift of non-lucid to lucid dreams [19]. Non-propositional metaawareness is considered particularly relevant to lucid dreaming [7]. ...
... Several conceptual links have been made between lucid dreaming and meditation practices, postulated to be influenced by regulating attention and meta-awareness [19,28,33]. A meta-analysis looked at meditation practices across various traditions and backgrounds and extracted the various effects of different meditation styles and their influence on cognitive capacities [72]. ...
... However, it has to be noted that a mediation model with this study design cannot imply a causal relationship since the variables were assessed simultaneously, and there might be other variables and cognitive mechanisms involved that were not captured. Nonetheless, the findings are consistent with the hypothesis that higher meta-awareness is associated with more lucid dreams [7,19]. Consistent with previous findings by Stumbrys et al. [33] and Baird et al. [28], the results suggest that lucid dreaming is associated with specific aspects of mindfulness that differ depending on whether an individual has experience with meditation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Lucid dreaming involves becoming aware that one’s current experience is a dream, which has similarities with the notion of mindfulness—becoming aware of moment-to-moment changes in experience. Additionally, meta-awareness, the ability to explicitly notice the current content of one’s own mental state, has also been proposed to play an important role both in lucid dreaming and mindfulness meditation practices. However, research has shown conflicting strengths of associations between mindfulness, meditation, and lucid dreaming frequency, and the link between lucid dreaming and meta-awareness has not yet been empirically studied. This study evaluated the associations between lucid dreaming frequency and different meditation practice styles, mindfulness traits, and individual differences in meta-awareness through an online survey (n = 635). The results suggest that daily frequent meditators experience more lucid dreams than non-frequent meditators. However, weekly frequent meditators did not have a higher lucid dreaming frequency. A positive association was observed between open monitoring styles of meditation and lucid dreaming. The findings also indicate that meta-awareness is higher for meditators and weekly lucid dreamers. Furthermore, frequent lucid dreaming was commonly associated with a non-reactive stance and experiencing transcendence. Overall, the findings suggest a positive relationship between specific meditation practices and lucid dreaming as well as the importance of meta-awareness as a cognitive process linking meditation, mindfulness, and lucid dreaming.
... More specifically, the most adopted definition of OBEs includes three phenomenological characteristics: (i) disembodiment (i.e., location of the self outside one's body); (ii) the impression of seeing the world from an elevated, distanced, egocentric, and extracorporeal perspective; and (iii) the impression of seeing one's own body from this perspective (Bünning andBlanke 2005, Blanke 2012). In contrast, a LD is defined as a dream during which the subject has the awareness that they are dreaming (Baird, Mota-Rolim et al. 2019), i.e., the subject retains their consciousness during the dream and may have control over their actions or the dream's content. ...
... Furthermore, we acknowledge that OBEs and LDs may exhibit additional distinctions, given the potential differences in their respective brain signatures (Rogo 1985). However, while some progress has been made particularly in the field of LD neurophysiology (Erlacher and Schredl 2004, Voss, Holzmann et al. 2009, Dresler, Wehrle et al. 2012, Erlacher, Schädlich et al. 2013, Baird, Mota-Rolim et al. 2019 there is still insufficient knowledge for a comprehensive comparison between the two experiences. Additional research on the neural correlates of OBEs, in particular, will be necessary to further enhance our understanding of the distinct brain signatures underlying these phenomena. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Despite extensive discussions, a consensus is lacking on whether out-of-body experiences (OBEs) and lucid dreams (LDs) should be considered as distinct categories of experience, and if so, what differentiates them. To address this issue, we first discussed the implications of OBEs and LDs as the same type of phenomena. We then compiled a comprehensive list of experiential features associated with OBEs and LDs, aiming to identify a distinguishing criterion between them. We conclude that neither the previously proposed features nor the interpretation of the experience can reliably differentiate them. We propose that disembodiment, or existing without a physical body, should be the key phenomenological feature of OBEs, regardless of other aspects of the experience. Our argument is supported by first-person reports included as supplemental material, indicated by superscript numbers. By focusing on this crucial element, we can better understand and characterize an OBE, distinguishing it from other related experiences.
... Drawing inspiration from the literature on lucid dreaming (Baird, Mota-Rolim, & Dresler, 2019;La Berge, Nagel, Dement, & Zarcone, 1981;Voss, Holzmann, Tuin, & Hobson, 2009), we used horizontal eye movements as a potential non-disruptive marker for the initiation of voluntary OBEs. This technique, often referred to as eye movement signaling, proves valuable in lucid dreaming research because it leverages the fact that eye muscles remain active and unaffected by the muscular atonia typically associated with REM sleep. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
In this pilot study, we delved into the experiences of three individuals who claimed the ability to induce Out-of-Body Experiences (OBEs) at will. Our primary objective was to ascertain the feasibility of replicating self-induced OBEs within a controlled laboratory setting. Additionally, we aimed to explore the potential of utilizing voluntary horizontal eye movements, monitored through electro-oculography (EOG), as an indicator of OBE onset. During the study, we observed how room layout and environmental conditions influenced the participants’ ability to induce OBEs. Notably, two participants successfully induced OBEs during the experiment. Significantly, our findings suggest that horizontal eye movements, as detected by EOG, could serve as a reliable marker for the initiation of OBEs. This study paves the way for further research and investigation into the intriguing realm of OBEs.
... Despite new developments (Konkoly et al., 2021), the technique of lucid dreaming does not provide a safe way out. For a review of electroencephalographic, neuroimaging, brain injury, pharmacological, and brain stimulation studies in the field of lucid dreaming, see Baird et al. (2019). New approaches to stimulus control are needed (Bulkeley, 2017;Domhoff, 2007Domhoff, , 2022Dresler et al., 2011;Scarpelli et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Compared to ordinary memories, dream memories still have something mysterious about them and invite interpretation. The aim of this study was the simple but fundamental question whether this (perceived, presumed) emotional–mental specificity of dream memories is associated with something specific at the level of brain physiology. Subjects were n = 30 healthy volunteers, aged 21–62 years, who had experienced a significant high-intensity dream or had repeatedly experienced such a dream. Using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and a recognition memory paradigm, we investigated the neural correlates of recognition of “dream words” compared to “story words.” Thirty dream words that best represented the dream were extracted during a qualitative dream interview with intensive dream reliving. Thirty story words were learned through self-directed effort from a written short story. These items, along with 60 additional distractor words from the dictionary, were randomly presented on the scanner in an old/new decision task. When dream words were statistically contrasted with story words in terms of their blood-oxygen-level-dependent effects, the difference was significant (p < .001, effect size dz = 1.2). By superimposing the mean contrast image on a reference brain, we were able to localize the specificity of dream activation in the medial orbitofrontal cortex/medial prefrontal cortex, a central part of the default mode network. The results suggest that dream memories are differentially processed in these areas. A novel experimental design was demonstrated that allows precise control of dream stimuli for future functional magnetic resonance imaging studies.
... Lucid dreaming in this model is explained by nudging the higher cortical FF neurons with the target activity set by the FB pathway (as in NREM and REM), but the discriminator neurons are now nudged to encode dreams, corresponding to the true (non-adversarial) state of the sensory activity (Figure 5d). The teaching signal dreamed for the discriminator neurons may give the subject the awareness that its current state is the dream, and is in fact happening in lucid dreaming [61]. To keep the functional benefit of lucid dreaming, the plasticity in the FB pathway has to be turned off, as in NREM sleep (Figure 4), so that the FB pathway is not unlearned by reverting the discriminator target. ...
Article
Full-text available
The importance of sleep for healthy brain function is widely acknowledged. However, it remains unclear how the internal generation of dreams might facilitate cognitive processes. In this perspective, we review a computational approach inspired by artificial intelligence that proposes a framework for how dreams occurring during rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep can contribute to learning and creativity. In this framework, REM dreams are characterized by an adversarial process that, against the dream reality, tells a discriminator network to classify the internally created sensory activity as real. Such an adversarial dreaming process is shown to facilitate the emergence of real-world semantic representations in higher cortical areas. We further discuss the potential contributions of adversarial dreaming beyond learning, such as balancing fantastic and realistic dream elements and facilitating the occurrence of creative insights. We characterize non-REM (NREM) dreams, where a single hippocampal memory is replayed at a time, as serving the complementary role of improving the robustness of cortical representations to environmental perturbations. We finally explain how subjects can become aware of the adversarial REM dreams, but less of the NREM dreams, and how content- and state-awareness in wake, dream, and lucid dreaming may appear.
... Lucid dreaming in this model is explained by nudging the higher cortical FF neurons with the target activity set by the FB pathway (as in NREM and REM), but the discriminator neurons are now nudged to encode dreamed, corresponding to the true (non-adversarial) state of the sensory activity (Figure 5d). The teaching signal dreamed for the discriminator neurons may give the subject the awareness that its current state is the dream, as in fact happening in lucid dreaming [65]. To keep a functional benefit of lucid dreaming, the plasticity in the FB pathway has to be turned of, as in NREM sleep (Figure 2), so that the FB pathway is not unlearned by reverting the discriminator target. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The importance of sleep for healthy brain function is widely acknowledged. However, it remains unclear how the internal generation of dreams might facilitate cognitive processes. In this perspective we review a computational approach inspired by artificial intelligence that proposes a framework for how dreams occurring during rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep can contribute to learning and creativity. In this framework, REM dreams are characterized by an adversarial process that, against the dream reality, tell a discriminator network to classify the internally created sensory activity as real. Such an adversarial dreaming process is shown to facilitate the emergence of real-world semantic representations in higher cortical areas. We further discuss the potential contributions of adversarial dreaming beyond learning, such as balancing fantastic and realistic dream elements, and facilitating the occurrence of creative insights. We characterize non-REM (NREM) dreams, where a single hippocampal memory is replayed at a time, as serving a complementary role of improving the robustness of cortical representations to environmental perturbations. We finally explain how subjects can become aware of the adversarial REM dreams, but less of the NREM dreams, and how the awareness phenomenon of wake, dream and lucid dreaming may appear.
Article
Perceptual reality monitoring refers to the ability to distinguish internally triggered imagination from externally triggered reality. Such monitoring can take place at perceptual or cognitive levels-for example, in lucid dreaming, perceptual experience feels real but is accompanied by a cognitive insight that it is not real. We recently developed a paradigm to reveal perceptual reality monitoring errors during wakefulness in the general population, showing that imagined signals can be erroneously attributed to perception during a perceptual detection task. In the current study, we set out to investigate whether people have insight into perceptual reality monitoring errors by additionally measuring perceptual confidence. We used hierarchical Bayesian modeling of confidence criteria to characterize metacognitive insight into the effects of imagery on detection. Over two experiments, we found that confidence criteria moved in tandem with the decision criterion shift, indicating a failure of reality monitoring not only at a perceptual but also at a metacognitive level. These results further show that such failures have a perceptual rather than a decisional origin. Interestingly, offline queries at the end of the experiment revealed global, task-level insight, which was uncorrelated with local, trial-level insight as measured with confidence ratings. Taken together, our results demonstrate that confidence ratings do not distinguish imagination from reality during perceptual detection. Future research should further explore the different cognitive dimensions of insight into reality judgments and how they are related.
Article
Full-text available
A common objection to Sosa’s epistemology is that it countenances, in an objectionable way, unsafe knowledge. This objection, under closer inspection, turns out to be in far worse shape than Sosa’s critics have realised. Sosa and his defenders have offered two central response types to the idea that allowing unsafe knowledge is problematic: one response type adverts to the animal/reflective knowledge distinction that is characteristic of bi-level virtue epistemology. The other less-discussed response type appeals to the threat of dream scepticism, and in particular, to the idea that many of our everyday perceptual beliefs are unsafe through the nearness of the dream possibility. The latter dreaming response to the safety objection to Sosa’s virtue epistemology has largely flown under the radar in contemporary discussions of safety and knowledge. We think that, suitably articulated in view of research in the philosophy and science of dreaming, it has much more going for it than has been appreciated. This paper further develops, beyond what Sosa does himself, the dreaming argument in response to those who think safety (as traditionally understood) is a condition on knowledge and who object to Sosa’s account on the grounds that it fails this condition. The payoffs of further developing this argument will be not only a better understanding of the importance of insights about dreaming against safety as a condition on knowledge, but also some reason to think a weaker safety condition, one that is relativised to SSS (i.e., skill/shape/situation) conditions for competence exercise, gets better results all things considered as an anti-luck codicil on knowledge.
Article
Full-text available
In an increasingly complex information society, demands for cognitive functioning are growing steadily. In recent years, numerous strategies to augment brain function have been proposed. Evidence for their efficacy (or lack thereof) and side effects has prompted discussions about ethical, societal and medical implications. In the public debate, cognitive enhancement is often seen as a monolithic phenomenon. On a closer look, however, cognitive enhancement turns out to be a multifaceted concept: There is not one cognitive enhancer that augments brain function per se, but a great variety of interventions that can be clustered into biochemical, physical and behavioral enhancement strategies. These cognitive enhancers differ in their mode of action, the cognitive domain they target, the timescale they work on, their availability and side effects, and how they differentially affect different groups of subjects. Here we disentangle the dimensions of cognitive enhancement, review prominent examples of cognitive enhancers that differ across these dimensions, and thereby provide a framework for both theoretical discussions and empirical research.
Article
Full-text available
Abstract Humans typically lack awareness that they are dreaming while dreaming. However, at times a remarkable exception occurs and reflective consciousness can be regained while dreaming, referred to as lucid dreaming. While most individuals experience lucid dreams rarely there is substantial variance in lucid dream frequency. The neurobiological basis of lucid dreaming is unknown, but evidence points to involvement of anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC) and parietal cortex. This study evaluated the neuroanatomical/neurofunctional correlates of frequent lucid dreams and specifically whether functional connectivity of aPFC is associated with frequent lucid dreams. We analyzed structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging from an exceptional sample of fourteen individuals who reported ≥3 lucid dreams/week and a control group matched on age, gender and dream recall that reported ≤1 lucid dream/year. Compared to controls, the frequent lucid dream group showed significantly increased resting-state functional connectivity between left aPFC and bilateral angular gyrus, bilateral middle temporal gyrus and right inferior frontal gyrus, and higher node degree and strength in left aPFC. In contrast, no significant differences in brain structure were observed. Our results suggest that frequent lucid dreaming is associated with increased functional connectivity between aPFC and temporoparietal association areas, regions normally deactivated during sleep.
Article
Full-text available
Strong conceptual and theoretical connections have been made between meditation practice, mindfulness, and lucid dreaming. However, only a handful of empirical studies have evaluated the relationship between lucid dreaming and meditation, and conclusions remain tempered by methodological limitations. In this study, we evaluated the relationship between meditation, mindfulness, and lucid dream frequency using several complementary methods. First, using a cross-sectional design, we evaluated differences in lucid dream frequency between long-term meditators and meditation-naïve individuals. Second, we evaluated the relationship between lucid dream frequency and specific facets of trait mindfulness in both meditators and nonmeditators. Third, using a blinded, randomized controlled design, we evaluated the effect of an 8-week mindfulness course on lucid dreaming frequency. Our results show that lucid dreaming is more frequent in long-term meditators than in meditation-naïve individuals. In addition, lucid dream frequency in meditation-naïve individuals was associated with a tendency to verbalize experience, whereas lucid dream frequency in long-term meditators was associated with observational and decentering facets of trait mindfulness. However, an 8-week mindfulness course did not increase the frequency of lucid dreams. Together, these results support a continuity between increased awareness of waking and sleeping states, provide a novel form of evidence linking meditation training to meta-awareness, and support an association between meditation practice and lucid dreaming, but leave open the specific nature of this connection.
Article
Full-text available
It is often assumed that during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep the cerebral cortex homogenously shows electroencephalogram (EEG) activity highly similar to wakefulness. However, to date no studies have compared neural oscillatory activity in human REM sleep to resting wakefulness with high spatial sampling. In the current study, we evaluated high-resolution topographical changes in neural oscillatory power between both early and late naturalistic REM sleep and resting wakefulness in adult humans. All-night recordings with 256-channel high-density EEG (hd-EEG) were collected in healthy volunteers (N = 12). Topographic analysis revealed that, compared to wake, both the first and last cycle of REM sleep were associated with increased low-frequency oscillations in local central and occipital regions. In contrast, high-frequency activity in both α and β bands (8–20 Hz) was globally decreased during both early and late REM sleep cycles compared to wakefulness. No significant differences in topographic power in any frequency band were observed between REM sleep cycles occurring early and late in the night. We replicated these findings in an independent dataset (N = 33). Together, our findings show that human REM sleep shows consistent topographical changes in oscillatory power across both early and late sleep cycles compared to resting wakefulness.
Article
Full-text available
Humans are typically unable to engage in sustained smooth pursuit for imagined objects. However, it is unknown to what extent smooth tracking occurs for visual imagery during REM sleep dreaming. Here we examine smooth pursuit eye movements during tracking of a slow-moving visual target during lucid dreams in REM sleep. Highly similar smooth pursuit tracking was observed during both waking perception and lucid REM sleep dreaming, in contrast to the characteristically saccadic tracking observed during visuomotor imagination. Our findings suggest that, in this respect, the visual imagery that occurs during REM sleep is more similar to perception than imagination. The data also show that the neural circuitry of smooth pursuit can be driven by a visual percept in the absence of retinal stimulation and that specific voluntary shifts in the direction of experienced gaze within REM sleep dreams are accompanied by corresponding rotations of the physical eyes.
Article
Full-text available
Lucid dreaming is a remarkable state of consciousness in which one is aware of the fact that one is dreaming while continuing to dream. Based on the strong relationship between physiological activation during rapid eye-movement sleep and lucid dreaming, our pilot research investigated whether enhancing cortical activation via acetylcholinesterease inhibition (AChEI) would increase the frequency of lucid dreams and found AChEI to be a promising method for lucid dream induction. In the current study we sought to quantify the size and reliability of the effect of AChEI on lucid dreaming, dream recall and dream content as well as to test the effectiveness of an integrated lucid dream induction protocol which combined cholinergic stimulation with other methods for lucid dream induction. Participants (N = 121) with high dream recall and an interest in lucid dreaming were randomly assigned counterbalanced orders of 3 doses of galantamine (0, 4 and 8 mg). On 3 consecutive nights, they awoke approximately 4.5 hours after lights out, recalled a dream, ingested the capsules and stayed out of bed for at least 30 minutes. Participants then returned to bed and practiced the Mnemonic Induction of Lucid Dreams technique while returning to sleep. The percentage of participants who reported a lucid dream was significantly increased for both 4 mg (27%, odds ratio = 2.29) and 8 mg doses (42%, odds ratio = 4.46) compared to the active placebo procedure (14%). Galantamine also significantly increased dream recall, sensory vividness and complexity (p
Article
An experimental home study examined the impact of a pre-sleep protocol for enhancing self-awareness, lucidity, and responsiveness in dreams. It included ingesting the cholinesterase inhibitor galantamine--which is widely reported to increase the frequency of lucid dreaming--prior to engaging in middle-of-the-night meditation and the imaginary reliving of a distressing dream while exercising new responses. Thirty-five participants completed an eight-night study, which included pre- and post-baseline nights and six conditions: waking for 40 min before returning to bed, called Wake-Back-to-Bed (WBTB); Wake-Back-to-Bed plus placebo (WBTB + P); Wake-Back-to-Bed plus galantamine (WBTB + G); meditation and dream reliving (MDR); meditation and dream reliving plus placebo (MDR + P); and meditation and dream reliving plus galantamine (MDR + G). The outcome measures included lucidity, reflectiveness, interactive behavior, role change, constructive action, and fear and threat, as measured by the participants' self-ratings. The results support the use of this protocol in further studies of lucid dream induction and nightmare/trauma resolution.
Article
Introduction: Nightmare disorder affects approximately 4% of adults, occurring in isolation or as part of other disorders such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and can significantly impair quality of life. This paper provides the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) position regarding various treatments of nightmare disorder in adults. Methods: A literature search was performed based upon the keywords and MeSH terms from the Best Practice Guide for the Treatment of Nightmare Disorder in Adults that was published in 2010 by the AASM. The search used the date range March 2009 to August of 2017, and sought to find available evidence pertaining to the use of behavioral, psychological, and pharmacologic therapies for the treatment of nightmares. A task force developed position statements based on a thorough review of these studies and their clinical expertise. The AASM Board of Directors approved the final position statements. Determination of position: Positions of "recommended" and "not recommended" indicate that a treatment option is determined to be clearly useful or ineffective/harmful for most patients, respectively, based on a qualitative assessment of the available evidence and clinical judgement of the task force. Positions of "may be used" indicate that the evidence or expert consensus is less clear, either in favor or against the use of a treatment option. The interventions listed below are in alphabetical order within the position statements rather than clinical preference: this is not meant to be instructive of the order in which interventions should be used. Position statements: • The following therapies are recommended for the treatment of PTSD-associated nightmares and nightmare disorder: image rehearsal therapy. • The following therapies may be used for the treatment of PTSD-associated nightmares: cognitive behavioral therapy; cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia; eye movement desensitization and reprocessing; exposure, relaxation, and rescripting therapy; the atypical antipsychotics olanzapine, risperidone and aripiprazole; clonidine; cyproheptadine; fluvoxamine; gabapentin; nabilone; phenelzine; prazosin; topiramate; trazodone; and tricyclic antidepressants. • The following therapies may be used for the treatment of nightmare disorder: cognitive behavioral therapy; exposure, relaxation, and rescripting therapy; hypnosis; lucid dreaming therapy; progressive deep muscle relaxation; sleep dynamic therapy; self-exposure therapy; systematic desensitization; testimony method; nitrazepam; prazosin; and triazolam. • The following are not recommended for the treatment of nightmare disorder: clonazepam and venlafaxine. • The ultimate judgment regarding propriety of any specific care must be made by the clinician, in light of the individual circumstances presented by the patient, accessible treatment options, and resources.
Article
Lucid dreaming is the phenomenon of dreaming while knowing that one is dreaming but the skills within the lucid dream, e.g., carrying out successfully pre-arranged tasks, vary considerably from dreamer to dreamer - even in frequent lucid dreamers. In order to measure inter-individual differences in lucid dreaming skills, a ten-item questionnaire was developed. The internal consistency was high. Substantial correlations with the skills score, age, and lucid dream frequency were found whereas gender did not affect lucid dreaming skills in the present sample (N = 675). The next steps would be to validate the questionnaire, i.e., demonstrating that high lucid dreaming skills (LUSK) scores are predicting better performance in sleep laboratory experiments, e.g., performing pre-arranged eye movements or specific tasks.