ArticlePDF Available

The reaction in counter-action: how Meisner technique and active analysis complement each other

Authors:

Abstract

This article is an investigation into the difference between reaction and counter-action. The question arose during my experience of Active Analysis at the Stanislavski Acting Laboratory in California Riverside University. In the Meisner technique the emphasis lies on instinctive reaction, whereas in Stanislavski’s Active Analysis the action and counter-action are emphasised. Counter-action can be seen as the force working against the main action of the scene creating conflict. Having extensive knowledge of reaction, and experience of counter-action, it became important to understand the difference between the two concepts and the importance of both in actor training and application to text. Through research into Action-Perception theory, self-regulation and motivation, I attempt to dissect the fundamental discrepancies between the two principles. My findings show that reaction stems from impulse and instinct, whereas counter-action is rooted in motivation. When motivation and instinct are in conflict, self-regulation will attempt to supress the impulse and over-ride it with an alternative counter-action more suited to the overall motivation. As self-regulation is a limited resource, prolonged use will make this harder to control. Emotion control draws on the same limited resource as self-regulation. Suggesting that emotion regulation would be affected by a conflict in instinct and motivation. These conclusions have a strong impact on how emotions are manifested and produced in actors and warrant a re-evaluation of how actors reach emotional connection to the given circumstance, as well as how emotion is viewed and engaged with in actor training in general.
The reaction in counter-action: How Meisner technique and Active Analysis
complement each other.
Philippa Strandberg-Long
Biography:
Philippa is an actor trainer, director and researcher specialising in the Meisner
technique. She teaches acting technique at several London conservatoires, coaches
professional actors on and off set, and run workshops in the Meisner technique around
the UK and Europe. Apart from this is also currently working on a PhD in Meisner
technique and Cognition at the University of Kent.
Keywords: Action, Counter-action, Reaction, Meisner technique, Active Analysis
Abstract:
This article is an investigation into the difference between reaction and counter-
action. The question arose during my experience of Active Analysis at the
Stanislavski Acting Laboratory in California Riverside University. In the Meisner
technique the emphasis lies on instinctive reaction, whereas in Stanislavski’s Active
Analysis the action and counter-action are emphasised. Counter-action can be seen as
the force working against the main action of the scene creating conflict. Having
extensive knowledge of reaction, and experience of counter-action, it became
important to understand the difference between the two concepts and the importance
of both in actor training and application to text. Through research into Action-
Perception theory, self-regulation and motivation, I attempt to dissect the fundamental
discrepancies between the two principles. My findings show that reaction stems from
impulse and instinct, whereas counter-action is rooted in motivation. When
motivation and instinct are in conflict, self-regulation will attempt to supress the
impulse and over-ride it with an alternative counter-action more suited to the overall
motivation. As self-regulation is a limited resource, prolonged use will make this
harder to control. Emotion control draws on the same limited resource as self-
regulation. Suggesting that emotion regulation would be affected by a conflict in
instinct and motivation. These conclusions have a strong impact on how emotions are
manifested and produced in actors and warrant a re-evaluation of how actors reach
1
emotional connection to the given circumstance, as well as how emotion is viewed
and engaged with in actor training in general.
During spring 2018 I travelled to The University of California for the international
Stanislavski Acting Laboratory. My time at Riverside was spent working in a group
consisting of theatre and acting practitioners from around the world, led by
Stanislavski and Active Analysis expert Sharon-Marie Carnicke. Our quest, as a
group, was to explore the difference between an improvisation1 and an improvisatory
etude such as in Stanislavski’s Active Analysis. The difference between these two
aspects was an interesting concept and making this distinction would also be key for
someone teaching and researching Meisner technique – a process that would also
qualify as a series of improvisatory exercises, distinct from improvisations. Meisner
himself opposed the use of improvisations that were based on dialogic word games,
like the ones The Group Theatre had engaged in and which were later popularised by
Keith Johnstone2 Meisner instead looked to create exercises that took the actors away
from being the intellectual creators of a script and emphasised the listening and
reacting to the partner instead.
…in the early days of the Group Theatre, the actors used to do what they called
“improvisations.” . . . These were general verbalizations of what we thought was an
approximation of our situation in the play. I came to the realization that this was all
intellectual nonsense. A composer doesn’t write down what he thinks would be effective; he
works from the heart. I decided I wanted an exercise for actors where there is no
intellectuality... to take away all the mental manipulation and get to where the impulses come
from.3
Meisner believed that intellectuality created a sense of predictability that hindered real
immediacy.4 He defined his improvisations as the actors co-creating a spontaneous,
unpredictable, affective engagement in the moment.5 The most significant contrast to
dialogical improvisations was that Meisner’s did not seek to further the storyline in
the way other improvisations did.6
Although Active Analysis uses paraphrasing, it is similar to Meisner technique in that
it ultimately tries to “break down the wall between analysis and embodiment”7 to find
a less intellectual and more experiential way of doing this based on the interactions
2
between the scene partners.8 Although my own experience of one particular exercise
will be highlighted, this article will not be dissecting our exploration of Improvisation
and Improvisatory exercises as such, but rather one particular aspect of Active
Analysis and its relation to Meisner technique: Counter-action.
What is counter-action and how does it differ from reaction?
For Meisner acting was reacting, with a clear motivation to keep the actor
spontaneous.9 In Meisner’s process the students are encouraged to base their
behaviour and actions on their instinctive reaction to the present moment. The driving
force being their own, and the reading of their partner’s, emotional impulses10. This
kind of reaction is meant to be acted upon immediately and continuously throughout
the interaction, allowing the impulses to bring about discoveries in the present
moment, and not be suppressed. However, focusing on the notion of counter-action
during the acting laboratory, raised questions about how an instinctive reaction differs
from a counter-action.
At the heart of Stanislavski’s improvisatory rehearsal tool, Active Analysis, lies the
notion of action and counter-action. Stanislavski created this technique in the latter
stages of his career, with the intention of fostering a freer and more reactive way to
approach a script. His earlier work on Method of Physical Actions had made him
question whether actions alone were sufficient, as well as whether the notion of
counter-action is the crucial element in producing conflict and dynamic
performances11. Counter-action can be seen as the force working against the main
action of the scene, creating the contention, and struggle, which is ultimately integral
to a dramatic situation.
Conflict [is] the clash produced when an impelling action meets a counteraction. Conflict may
be direct (as in a fist fight) or oblique (as happens in the subtle plays of Anton Chekhov or in
passive-aggressive encounters).12
Throughout our sessions with Carnicke it became clear that it was important to try to
clarify the differences between an instinctive reaction and a counter-action. Since the
ultimate application of actor training would be to take on a character through text this
raises questions about the validity of training the initial reaction and its usefulness to
the actor’s scene work. To be able to answer this, it is necessary to explore how these
3
approaches differ and the effect of one on the other. If a reaction is based on impulse,
what would be the basis for a counter-action? Given that the action and counter-
action in Active Analysis are based on the information from the text, the main focus
in determining the counter-action would be the objective of the character in
question.13 As the characters’ needs and wants play out in corresponding actions, the
main difference would seem to be the factor of the motivation. In an email
interview14, Carnicke agrees that, although she doesn’t use the word objective
specifically, counter-action and action is based on the given circumstances of the text
– where we find what the characters want. The question then becomes: how does
motivation affect instinctive reaction, and why train impulsivity if the application to
text would require a counter-action, which might go against the initial feeling?
Impulse versus motivation
Impulses or urges can be seen as the immediate response to a situation, which people
sometimes look to change or not act upon, whereas motivation would refer to a drive
to do something in order to serve a particular want – or the need to achieve a goal.15
Depending on the strength of our motivation, we might deem our impulsive reaction
to be an unsuitable course of action and the way to change this would be through self-
regulation.
Self-regulation refers to the capacity of [human beings] to override and alter their responses. It
is the process by which people attempt to constrain unwanted urges in order to gain control of
the incipient response. Regulation means change, especially change to bring behavior (or
other states) into line with some standard such as an ideal or goal.16
There is not always a discrepancy between our instinct and objective. If a person’s
instinctive reaction would be in line with the motivation – for example if a building
was on fire and the instinctive need was to get out – there would be no conflict
between the reaction and the want and hence no need for self-regulation. It is only in
the circumstances where the instinct and motivation are in conflict with each other
that we adjust our responses. These conflicts can occur in a multitude of situations but
are often linked to how we want to be perceived by others. We might view our
impulsive response to not be socially acceptable or make us look less favourable in
the eyes of others, so we regulate our behaviour accordingly.17 In Active Analysis we
see these regulations in the form of actions and counter-actions: of course a counter-
4
action could be based on a reaction, but it is the motivation of the character that
ultimately drives it. So, how does Meisner’s notion of training impulse apply to
regulated actions and counter-actions?
Meisner and Stanislavski both based the development of their processes18 on the
notion of Communion19– Stanislavski’s concept of communication and energy flow
between actors during interaction. Active Analysis is similar to Meisner technique
(and different to Method of Physical Actions) in terms of the attention being placed
outwards, towards the partner, with the intention to enhance adaptability and presence
in the actors.20 Although I acknowledge that my work with students on Meisner
technique has enhanced listening, instinct and action selection, I was curious to find
out how it would affect situations in which we regulate our actions. In order to
understand how action selection works I decided to build on my MA thesis research
into Action-Perception theory and the idea that reactions stem from our perceived
environment21.
What is Action-Perception theory?
In my MA research I discovered that Action-Perception is ultimately a survival
mechanism used by humans and animals in order for them to navigate the
environment. We perceive by interpreting the information we receive from our
immediate surroundings, and act on this knowledge with the intention of putting
ourselves in a more advantageous place. This creates an uninterrupted feedback-loop
where the environmental stimuli influence the action we take, which in turn create
new perceived stimuli, and so forth. Learning theorist Renate Caine explains how this
daily process works:
As a result of acting, the organism gets feedback - new information from the world and from
itself. That feedback provides the guidance about how the organism needs to act next time. [...] at
its most basic, people observe, act and get feedback.22
Although this makes it sound as if there are three parts to this cognitive process -
observing, acting and getting feedback – later research suggests that action and
perception follow each other instantaneously, and possibly simultaneously23. It was
ecological psychologist James J. Gibson who coined the term Action-Perception in
the 1960s. His research involved our interaction with the world visually and explored
how that visual perception of our surroundings influences our physical actions. Some
5
of Gibson’s later work was focused on the notion of affordances; the way we afford
certain values and judgments to what we see around us. In turn these judgment values
are instrumental when deciding on an action. As my earlier research highlighted,
Gibson argued that the perception and judgment are not two components separated in
a linear process – but that they happen at the same time – hence our actions are
instinctive reactions to visual stimuli. This was strongly contested by the philosophers
of mind and cognitive scientists at the time.24 The belief was that the mind separated
action and perception and was the main decider on which action to take. The cycle, as
it was seen at the time, was perception– cognition–action. However, the view of this
has in latter years changed in favour of Gibson's model.
Over the last years several neurophysiological studies have offered us a very different
description of brain architecture and functions. These researches have changed the standard
view of sensory motor system and the motor system, shedding light on the mutual relationship
between action and perception.25
This would indicate that training our actors’ perceptions of their surroundings, and
how we evaluate those perceptions in the moment, would be integral to our way of
deciding on an action. The foundation of Meisner’s work is focused on letting the
instinctive reactions form the basis of our interaction, however a large part of the
initial training emphasizes the placing of attention outside of ourselves onto our
partner, in order to move away from the self-initiated action and allow the reaction to
come from something outside of ourselves.
As the research into neuroscience and cognition has evolved, Action-Perception
cycles have been reclaimed through the lens of social cognition26. Professor of
psychology and neuroscientist Jean Decety, applies Action-Perception cycles to his
work on social interaction. His work emphasises the intentions behind our actions and
how this is influenced by other people’s behaviour towards us. His view is in line with
Gibson’s discoveries as he describes the action-perception cycles and functionally
intertwining action and cognition: “perception is a means to action and action is a
means to perception”.27
There is a difference between interacting with other humans and interacting with
inanimate objects. The interaction between humans is more intricate and is referred to
as natural action perception dynamic28. The dynamic, instant, continuous feedback
6
loop between people happens in the present moment – simply put: we observe, we get
feedback and we act. Referring to this, Decety states that the Action-Perception cycle
creates the internal stimulus that leads to both our perception of someone else’s action
and our resulting reaction:
…these [internal] representations not only guide our own behaviour, but are also used to
interpret the behaviour of others, because they are shared across individuals.29
Looking at this research, one can argue that focusing on training actors to stay present
would be of importance. What the study of Action-Perception theory tells us is that
these continuous feedback loops instigate our initial reaction and consequently
determine our action, which happens in the moment. When the mind wanders from
that present moment it enters a cognitive realm outside the Action-Perception cycle.
Naturally the cycle does not stop during this time, but some internal representations
formed will not be true to the moment – due to being imagined or remembered – and
for actors the consequence is that some actions may not appear authentic to the
audience as they cannot be adjusted according to the present interaction. Meisner
technique teaches the importance of paying close attention to your scene partner’s
actions in order to keep you focused and open to spontaneous responses.
Meisner based his process on the idea that “What you do does not depend on you; it
depends on the other fellow30. This notion closely follows the science of the Action-
Perception cycle – his exercises capitalising on the natural way humans interact. The
repetition exercise trains the actors to base their actions on their impulsive reaction to
their partner. The exercise also hones their skills in reading other people’s behaviour
whilst evaluating the reasons behind it. Thus, arguably, Meisner technique can be
seen to train perception and by starting out in an environment free from other stimuli,
we can learn to focus our attention on the dynamic force of the other actor. However,
at this point we have to ask the question, what really lies behind our behaviour, and
what we do to alter other people’s behaviour?
Behaviour is adaptive, designed to allow the organism to prevent or accomplish something,
while negotiating obstacles, i.e. it is action in the actor’s sense.31
Blair is referring to the fact that the basis for our behaviour and actions is ultimately
our objective, or our need to accomplish something. Now adding intention into the
7
equation – the basis for counter-action – it would be of relevant to explore how the
notion of goal-oriented behaviour would fit into the Action-Perception cycle.
What about the counter-action?
Patrick Haggard, from the Institute for Cognitive Neuroscience, differentiates
between actions that are the result of an immediate reaction and actions that are driven
by a clear intention. Besides the feeling of ownership with regard to initiating an
intentional action, the brain processes engaged during these types of actions are set
apart from those engaged during responsive actions32. Recognizing the difference in
brain activity during intentional action and responsive action is vital in order to
investigate my question fully – how reaction and counteraction differ and what part
training each of them plays in actor training. Even though my main focus as a Meisner
teacher has been to bring about spontaneity and impulsivity in the actors’ work, one
cannot dismiss the fact that human interaction is often bound by intentions and
calculation and the neurological differences separating these types of action may be of
significance for the teaching and directing of actors.
Further, Haggard flags up that neuroscientific studies into intentional behavior is a
less travelled path than that of conscious perception. Nevertheless a consensus has
been reached within the field that two types of action exist:
Human actions comprises of a spectrum extending from direct responses to immediate stimuli,
to much longer ranging actions. The former often called ‘automatic’, whereas the latter may
be called ‘intentional’.33
For my research question it would be appropriate here to make the distinct link
between automatic action and reaction, and intentional action and counter-action.
However, it would seem that there is some contention here between the neurobiology
and Action-Perception theory, given that automatic and intentional actions are seen as
separate processes within the brain, although Action-Perception theory considers them
as one. Gibson asserted that action was an integral part of perception; hence the
information processing and the following decision-making would not be seen as
separate processes. However, intentional action would appear to be following a more
goal-oriented path, where final desired outcome would be part of the calculation.
Most desires need a string of actions in order to be achieved– even more so when
8
involving the dynamic interactions of other humans. The action-sequence would start
off with an intention – something that you want or need – the next actions can only be
determined by continuously evaluating the response of the other people in the
interaction. Will they help you achieve your goal? Will they stand in your way? What
other obstacles occur whilst you are interacting? Will the goal take a long or short
time to achieve? When does my action stop working? This is one way of the reaction
and counter-action working together to create drama. The audience can most likely
tell the desires of the actors, they know where each actor want to get to, but the
journey there is explored in the moment, creating dynamic performances. Since these
two actions do work together, are they both part of the Action Perception cycle?
Neuroscientist Joaquin Fuster argues that intention is not just part of the cycle – but
the Action-Perception cycle depends on it:
The Perception-Action cycle is the circular flow of information from the environment to
sensory structures, to motor structures, back again to the environment, to sensory structures,
and so on, during the processing of goal-directed behaviour.34
Fuster continues by adding that automatic actions – what I am referring to as a
reactions– are situated in a different part of the brain cycle, to the more complex
intentional actions, or counter-actions. However, both types of actions are connected
by long fibers in the brain, making them equivalent in the executive rank and meaning
that they are of equal importance during action selection35.
Reaction + Counter-action = Affect?
Looking at this information from the perspective of Meisner technique and Active
Analysis, I can see the importance of both reaction and counter-action within an
actor’s approach to the script. Furthermore, during the workshops with Carnicke I had
an experience, which made me question the notion of affect and how emotion can be
generated through conflict of motivation and impulse. During the second stage of our
improvisational laboratory36 we had put our improvisation into the Active Analysis
structure, which consisted of an action and a counteraction as well as a verb or tactic
to play throughout the scene. The scene concerned a sensitive and highly emotional
subject, something my character had experienced. I considered whether I would be
required to engage in any kind of emotional preparation in order for me to access
those kinds of emotions, or even connect to the stakes of this situation, but for the
9
sake of the laboratory I decided to rely wholly on the outlined structure and process.
During the etude, my inner reaction to Character 1’s action was completely different
to my chosen tactic, which in turn created a strong conflict inside me. The more she
pushed me to tell her the harder it became to make light of the situation. This struggle
in turn created a strong emotional connection to the situation and at one point I felt
physical pain in my body due to this inner turmoil and how difficult Character 1 made
it for me to maintain this facade. There was a strong sense of self-awareness in this
scene which was not hindering me as an actor, if anything it aided the situation. I was
aware of everything I felt, as if I was really in this situation rather than as an actor
playing a part. Self-consciousness became character consciousness, as I felt self-
conscious as the person in this situation being watched and judged within the given
circumstances – not by an audience. Character consciousness was a term used by both
Stanislavski and Chekhov and referred to what happens when our imagination takes
us from empathy or compassion to actually experiencing the feelings of our character
due to the predicament of the situation. Chekhov referred to this as “a third I” – where
we think, feel and pursue the same things as the character.37 In my experience at
Riverside I felt extremely self-aware of my own behaviour, however it fully related to
the circumstance and not to the fact that I was being watched. At no point did my fear,
panic, sadness or awkwardness relate to my performance, but only to how my partner
was observing and behaving towards me. My feelings came out of the three-way
struggle between my impulse, my motivation and my partner’s action.
(Fig 1)
Partner's
action
Partner's
action
Motivation
Motivation
Instinctiv
e reaction
Instinctiv
e reaction
10
When asking Carnicke about her thoughts in regard to this she told me that in his later
work Stanislavski never asked actors to work on specific emotions. He increasingly
deemed emotion to be a sixth sense that would appear as a result of what we do. She
added that Active Analysis presumes that if you are fully committed to a verb
(provided it is well chosen in terms of the given circumstances and facts) emotional
connection will follow – as had happened in our etude.38 Meisner also had thoughts
along these lines when he stated that the motivation behind your actions should be the
source of your concentration, which would generate the emotions by itself.39 This was
clearly demonstrated to me in this exercise as emotions in turn create behaviour,
which we observe in our partners and ourselves and evaluate from the perspective of
our desired outcome. My experience reflected the integral connection between
reaction and counteraction and in what manner they are strongly linked to affect.
Behaviour is the outcome of an internal struggle among many mental modules, and it is played
out on the chessboard of opportunities and constraints defined by other people’s behaviour.40
What I experienced as ‘character consciousness’ related to the interaction between my
partner and me coupled with how well I was doing in achieving my goal. This created
a feedback loop influencing my behaviour and decisions and consequently generating
emotions. I suggest that this is different to an actor experiencing self-consciousness if
the changes in behaviour are the result of the motivation and the present
communication that are firmly rooted in the imaginary circumstances.
At this point I return to the concept of self-regulation, as it also effects emotion. As
stated earlier, when our instinctive reaction goes against what we believe to be the
best action to achieve our goal, or the standard we have set for ourselves, we deploy
self-regulation.41 Self-regulation needs some form of monitoring from the self in order
to be initiated; hence the feedback-loop brings about a sense of self-awareness that is
key to the interaction.42 As self-regulation refers to our ability to control our urges
rather than acting on them,43 motivation plays a crucial part in deciding whether we
achieve this or not. The stronger the motivation to achieve something, the harder the
person will work to over-ride instinctive responses that would counteract that goal.
This also relates to what is at stake, as the higher the stakes of the situation the more
effort we devote to overcoming the obstacles. If the instinctive response however is
very strong there will be a struggle between urges and impulses, which could lead to a
11
decreased level of self-control. Self-control draws on a limited resource pool
encompassing a wide variety of executive functions (attention, inhibition, working
memory44). As this is a limited capacity the more it is being used the more it will be
depleted.45 When this resource becomes depleted it is called ‘ego depletion’ and will
affect our ability to change our behaviour.46
An interesting point here is that this depletion also affects our emotion-regulation
ability. As humans we have a primary initial emotional response, which is followed
by a secondary emotional response driven by emotion-regulation.47 Generally, the
emotions regulated would be the less desirable ones like anxiety, anger, sadness or
jealousy, which in turn would be more likely to occur in higher stakes situations.
Social psychologists Gailliot, Zell and Baumeister have tested and confirmed the
hypothesis that those involved in situations that require the use of self-regulation do
find it harder to subsequently regulate their emotions. Their findings were based on a
study in which participants were required to use self-control to engage in focused
attention, change their writing style or over-ride a habit. The participants were then
presented with situations that would normally induce emotions such as anxiety,
sadness or jealousy. The results showed that, compared to those that had not been
involved in self-regulating activities, participants that had, felt these emotions to a
greater extent.48 Whilst those that had exercised self-control felt the emotions in a
stronger way, further studies also suggest that emotions that are being regulated or
worked against – using self-control to change how we are being perceived – will
increase in strength as well as being harder to manage the longer the situation
progresses. These emotions are also known to transmute into different kinds of
emotions due to the suppression of them.49
When discrepancies between feelings and feeling rules exist, the discrepancy generates a new
kind of negative emotional arousal, above and beyond the emotions initially experienced.
Thus, a person who feels sad in a situation demanding the expression of happiness may also
become angry at having to appear happy, thus racketing up the emotional intensity.50
This would explain the intense emotions I was experiencing during our Active
Analysis exercise, where my instinctive reaction and primary emotions were being
suppressed by my counter-action, whilst continuously working against an obstacle
(my partner’s action). This continuous three-way conflict, in a high stakes situation,
12
would take a lot of effort to uphold and hence be very likely to cause ego-depletion
and reduced self and emotional control ability.
Conclusion
In this article I have attempted to investigate the main differences between the
reaction in Meisner technique and the counter-action in Active Analysis. Meisner
technique is foremost an actor training process and Active Analysis a rehearsal tool, I
therefore wanted to understand how the two concepts work together on text and the
validity of training impulse. Having had a particularly strong emotional connection to
an Active Analysis etude I questioned whether this was due to the inner reaction and
counter-action working together and resulting in emotion.
Since reaction is based on immediate instinct I turned to Action-Perception theory in
order to explore how natural action selection occurs. Action-Perception theory
suggests that actions are instantaneously decided on, based on our perception of our
environment, which relates to Meisner’s notion of acting on impulse. However, given
that the counter-action is seemingly based on motivation, this suggests that we do not
always follow our instinct if we believe that our goal can be better achieved in a
different way. Following this idea, I discovered that within Action-Perception theory
there is an acknowledgement of two types of action, automatic and intentional,
intentional being more goal motivated and less impulsive. Interestingly, these two
actions are still linked inside Action-Perception feedback cycles and impact on one
another.
During my own encounter with Active Analysis at the acting laboratory at Riverside –
where both my partner’s action and my inner reaction worked against my ability to
perform my counter-action – I experienced the effect of this triangle of effort (see
fig.1) and the emotional connection that it manifested. As my investigation continued
I discovered that when we alter our impulsive reaction to another action – depending
on motivation – we employ self-regulation, which is a limited resource. It uses the
same resource as emotion-regulation, which is the way we regulate mainly negative
feelings as they occur. The discovery that these two regulatory functions use the same
resource, and can become depleted, gave me an idea of what had happened during that
exercise. My emotion regulation resource had become depleted due to using it to alter
my immediate reaction to fit my counter-action, resulting in a stronger manifestation
13
of emotion and the subsequent inability to keep playing my counter-action.
In conclusion this research has shown the importance of training impulse in actors and
how instinctive reaction works in conjunction with action on text, and more
specifically Active Analysis. Moreover, I would posit that the idea of the action-
triangle - inner reaction – counter-action – partner’s action - can present actors with
an effective way of accessing emotions which are wholly based on the present, the
given circumstances and attention to the partner.
14
1End notes:
Improvisations are often created spontaneously and without preparation, the actors become the creators of the script
and are encouraged not to block each other’s inputs. Improvisation practitioners like Keith Johnstone often work from
game structure in order to further the creative dialogue between the actors (Gray, 2015:727). For this laboratory we
defined an improvisation as an interaction between the actors where the lines are improvised and the structure might be
as loose as a set of given circumstances, or in the case of our group stem solely from the inspiration of an object.
2 Gray, Living Truthfully, 727.
3 Meisner, Meisner on Acting, 36
4 Gray, Living Truthfully, 727.
5 Esper & DiMarco, The Actor’s Arts and Craft; Meisner, Meisner on Acting.
6 Gray, Living Truthfully, 729.
7 Carnicke, Stanislavski in Focus, 195.
8 ibid, 196; Merlin, Konstantin Stanislavski, 42.
9 Zarrilli, The Psychophysical Actor, 17.
10 Stinespring, Principles of Truthful Acting, 119.
11 Carnicke, Stanislavski in Focus, 200.
12 ibid, 212.
13 Merlin, Konstantin Stanislavski, 34
14 Email interview with Sharon Carnicke 07/06/2018:
Q: My interpretation of it is that it is aiding something you want/need - rooted in the text?
A: Yes, Philippa, you are correct. If we were working on a text you would need to link your character’s action or
counteraction and also your choice of verb to the circumstances and facts of the text as written—what the character
wants is usually encoded in the circumstances and facts. In the case of our lab, the action/counteraction and verb (when
we had no text) were linked to what you want in terms of what your group set up as your circumstances. In early
Stanislavsky, he actually used the word “want” to help actors see their scenes in active terms.
15 Baumeister & Vohs, 2.
16 ibid
17 Baumeister & Vohs, Social and Personality Psychology, 5.
18 In this case I refer mainly to the creation and development of Active Analysis.
19 Merlin, Acting: The Basics, 183.
20 Merlin, Konstantin Stanislavski, 42.
21 The idea to look at Action-Perception theory in terms of the teaching of actioning in drama school was part of my
MA Dissertation “Reaction in Action” 2012. Here the main exploration was how Meisner technique could be used to
teach a foundation of actioning to drama school students.
22 Caine, Natural Learning, 2.
23 Decety, Current Opinion in Neurobiology; Giorelly & Sinigaglia, Perception in Action.
24 Giorelli & Sinigaglia, Perception in Action, 49
25 ibid
26 Social cognition is the encoding, storage, retrieval, and processing, of information in the brain, which relates to
members of the same species.
27 Decety, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 259.
28 Full quote: “In human beings the process is more complex and we therefore call it the natural perception-action
dynamic” (Caine, 2008: 2)
29 Decety, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 262
30 Meisner, Meisner on Acting, 36.
31 Blair, The Actor, Image, and Action, 41.
32 Haggard, Intentional Action, 695.
33 Haggard, Conscious Intention and Motor Cognition, 291
34 Fuster, Trends in Cognitive Science, 143
35 ibid
36 The first stage of the laboratory consisted of free improvisation stimulated from a chosen object. In the second stage
we took the situation the first improvisation generated and structured it into characters with either Action or Counter-
action and set verbs/tactics.
37 Gillett, Acting Stanislavski, 123.
38 Email interview with Carnicke 07/06/2018:
Q: The other question was regarding my experience of the verb working against each other so strongly that it caused an
emotional connection in me when the obstacle was too great to play the verb: all I was looking for her was a comment
from you on that etude, if you had any thoughts on what we experienced there.
A: Yes, the discovery that you made and what i said in our session was this: in his later years, Stanislavsky never asked
actors to work on specific emotions. He saw emotion (like our ability to see or touch) as a sixth sense that comes
naturally as a result of what we do. Active Analysis presumes that if you commit fully to your verb (provided that the
verb is well chose in terms of the scene’s circumstances and facts), emotion will result naturally (As it did for you in
your etude)!
39 Meisner, Meisner on Acting, 39.
40 Pinker, How The Mind Works, 42
41 Baumeister & Vohs, Social and Personality Psychology, 3
42 There is always a feedback loop present in our interactions, however if there is not strong motivation the feedback
revolves around the experience itself and it is evaluated from the perspective of past experiences and whether we want
to change it or keep it the same (Csikszentmihalyi & Nakamura, 2012:108-109)
43 Baumeister & Vohs, Social and Personality Psychology, 2
44 Diamond, Executive Functions, 136.
45 Muraven & Baumeister, Psychological Bulletin, 248.
46 Baumeister & Vohs, Social and Personality Psychology, 4.
47 Koole, Cognition and Emotion, 22
48 Gailliot, Zell & Baumeister, Open Access, 1.
49 Turner & Stets, Annual Revue Sociological, 31
50 ibid, 27.
Bibliography
Alfreds, M. (2007) Different Every Night. Nick Hern Books.
Baumeister, R. & Vohs, K. (2007) Self-Regulation, Ego Depletion, and Motivation. Social and Personality Psychology
Compass Vol.1 , p.115-128
Blair, R. (2007) The Actor, Image, and Action: Acting and Cognitive Neuroscience. Routledge.
Botvinick, M. (2007) Multilevel Structure in Behaviour and in the Brain: A Model of Fuster’s hierarchy,
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Vol 10, p.1615-1626
Caine, R. (2008) Natural Learning: The Basis for Raising and Sustaining High Standards of Real World
Performance, Position paper for The Natural Learning Institute.
Carnicke, S. (2008) Stanislavski in Focus: An Acting Master for the Twenty-First Century, Taylor and Francis.
Csikszentmihalyi, M & Nakamura, J. (2012) Emerging Goals and the Self-Regulation of Behavior in R. Wyer’s
Perspectives on Behavioural Self-regulation, Psychology Press.
Damasio, A .(2000) The Feeling Of What Happens: Body, Emotion and the Making of Consciousness. Vintage.
Decety, J. (2004) Motor Cognition: a New Paradigm to Study Self-Other Interactions, Current Opinion in
Neurobiology, Vol 14, p.259-263
Diamond, A. (2013) Executive Functions, The Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 64, p.136-168.
Durham, K. (2003) Acting on and off: Sanford Meisner reconsidered. Studies in Theatre & Performance, Vol. 23
Issue 3, p.151-163
Esper, W. & D. DiMarco (2009) Actor’s Art and Craft: William Esper Teaches The Meisner Technique. Anchor Books.
Frith, C. (2007) Making Up the Mind: How the Brain Creates Our Mental World. Wiley-Blackwell.
Fuster, J. (2004) Upper Processing Stages of Perception-Action Cycle, Trends in Cognitive Science, p.143-145
Gailliot, M, Zell, A. & Baumeister, R. (2014) Having Used Self-Control Reduces Emotion Regu- lation—Emotion
Regulation as Relying on Interchangeably Used “Self-Control Energy”. Open Access Library Journal, Vol 1, p.1-14
Giorelli, G. & Sinigaglia, C. (2007) Perception in Action, ACTA Biomed Vol 78, p.49-57
Gillett, J. (2014) Acting Stanislavski: A Practical Guide to Stanislavski’s Approach and Legacy. Bloomsbury
Publishing.
Gray, A. (2015) Living Truthfully Under Imaginary Circumstances: Improvisation in Psychoanalysis, Psychoanalytic
Dialogues, Vol 25, p.725-742
Haggard, P. (2005) Conscious Intention and Motor Cognition, Trends in Cognitive Science, Vol 9, p.290-295
Haggard, P. (2003) Intentional Action: Conscious Experience and Neural Prediction, Consciousness and Cognition,
Vol 12, p. 695-707
Ingold, T., ed. (2000) The Perception of the Environment: Essays on Livelihood, Dwelling and Skill. Routledge.
Koole, S.L (2009) The Psychology of Emotion Regulation: An Integrative Review, Cognition and Emotion, Vol 23,
p.4-41
Meisner, S. & Longwell, D. (1987) Meisner on Acting. Vintage Books.
Merlin, B. (2001) Beyond Stanislavsky. Nick Hern Books.
Merlin. B (2003) Konstantin Stanislavski. Routledge.
Merlin. B (2010) Acting: the Basics. Routledge.
Moseley, N. (2005) Acting and Reacting. Nick Hern Books.
Muraven, M. & Baumeister, R. F. (2000) Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control
resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, Vol 126, p. 247-259.
Noe, A. (2006) Action in Perception. MIT Press.
Pinker, S. (1997) How The Mind Works. Penguin.
Rizzolatti, G. & Sinigaglia, C. (2007) Mirrors in the Brain: How our minds share actions and emotions. OUP Oxford.
Stanislavski, K. (1980) An Actor Prepares. Methuen Drama.
Stinespring, L. (1999) Principles of Truthful Acting: A Theoretical Discourse on Sanford Meisner. PHD Dissertation
in Fine Arts, Texas University.
Thomas, J. (2016) Stanislavski’s Active Analysis. Methuen Drama.
Turner, J. & Stets, J. Sociological theories of human emotions, Annual Revue Sociological. 2006. Vol 32, p.25–52
Vohs, K. & Baumeister, R. (Eds.). (2010) Handbook of Self Regulation Volume 2. Guilford Press
Zarrilli, P. (2008) The Psychophysical Actor at Work. Routledge.
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Improvisation in psychoanalysis has heretofore been defined by a theater games model in which the emphasis is on a creative exchange of dialogue. That dialogue focuses on maintaining an established story line. In this paper, I expand the concept of improvisation by drawing on the contributions of Sanford Meisner. In his model, two improvisers through mutual influence maintain affective engagement. Dialogue emerges unpredictably out of that affective engagement and facilitates it. Meisner described this process as "living truthfully under imaginary circumstances." Clinical case material illustrates how a Meisner improvisation can be applied to circumstances in which the analyst is pushed beyond familiar analytic comfort zones. Such an improvisation provides the analyst with an additional resource with which to respond to these challenging moments in ways that facilitate increasing intimacy between patient and analyst. I spell out how a Meisner improvisation impacts therapeutic action in psychoanalysis.
Article
Full-text available
The neural substrate for behavioral, cognitive and linguistic actions is hierarchically organized in the cortex of the frontal lobe. In their methodologically impeccable study, Koechlin et al. reveal the neural dynamics of the frontal hierarchy in behavioral action. Progressively higher areas control the performance of actions requiring the integration of progressively more complex and temporally dispersed information. The study substantiates the crucial role of the prefrontal cortex in the temporal organization of behavior
Article
Full-text available
The present article reviews modern research on the psychology of emotion regulation. Emotion regulation determines the offset of emotional responding and is thus distinct from emotional sensitivity, which determines the onset of emotional responding. Among the most viable categories for classifying emotion-regulation strategies are the targets and functions of emotion regulation. The emotion-generating systems that are targeted in emotion regulation include attention, knowledge, and bodily responses. The functions of emotion regulation include satisfying hedonic needs, supporting specific goal pursuits, and facilitating the global personality system. Emotion-regulation strategies are classified in terms of their targets and functions and relevant empirical work is reviewed. Throughout this review, emotion regulation emerges as one of the most far-ranging and influential processes at the interface of cognition and emotion.
Article
Full-text available
Executive functions (EFs) make possible mentally playing with ideas; taking the time to think before acting; meeting novel, unanticipated challenges; resisting temptations; and staying focused. Core EFs are inhibition [response inhibition (self-control-resisting temptations and resisting acting impulsively) and interference control (selective attention and cognitive inhibition)], working memory, and cognitive flexibility (including creatively thinking "outside the box," seeing anything from different perspectives, and quickly and flexibly adapting to changed circumstances). The developmental progression and representative measures of each are discussed. Controversies are addressed (e.g., the relation between EFs and fluid intelligence, self-regulation, executive attention, and effortful control, and the relation between working memory and inhibition and attention). The importance of social, emotional, and physical health for cognitive health is discussed because stress, lack of sleep, loneliness, or lack of exercise impairs EFs. That EFs are trainable and can be improved with practice is addressed, including diverse methods tried thus far. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Psychology Volume 64 is November 30, 2012. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/catalog/pubdates.aspx for revised estimates.
Article
Full-text available
Over the past three decades, five general theoretical approaches to understanding the dynamics of human emotions have emerged in sociology: dramaturgical theories, symbolic interactionist theories, interaction ritual theories, power and status theories, and exchange theories. We review each of these approaches. Despite the progress made by these theories, several issues remain unresolved: the nature of emotions, feeling, and affect; the degree to which emotions are biologically based or socially constructed; the gap between social psychological theories on emotions and macrostructural theorizing; and the relatively narrow range of emotions theorized, coupled with an equally narrow focus on the structural and cultural conditions producing these emotions.
Chapter
We agree with the general thrust of Carver and Scheier’s position on self-regulation and applaud their important effort to integrate complexity models in their synthesis.
Article
The Actor, Image and Action is a 'new generation' approach to the craft of acting; the first full-length study of actor training using the insights of cognitive neuroscience. In a brilliant reassessment of both the practice and theory of acting, Rhonda Blair examines the physiological relationship between bodily action and emotional experience. In doing so she provides the latest step in Stanislavsky's attempts to help the actor 'reach the unconscious by conscious means'. Recent developments in scientific thinking about the connections between biology and cognition require new ways of understanding many elements of human activity, including: imagination, emotion, memory, physicality, reason. The Actor, Image and Action looks at how these are in fact inseparable in the brain's structure and function, and their crucial importance to an actor's engagement with a role. The book vastly improves our understanding of the actor's process and is a must for any actor or student of acting.
Article
This is a re-evaluation of the contribution of the Sanford Meisner approach to psychophysical actor training. It explores its distinctive refocusing of the Stanislavski system, and investigates, theoretically and culturally, the reasons for its unique emphases. Through the writer's observation and practice at the Robert Modica Studio in New York, and with particular reference to its core ‘Repetition exercises’, the essay critically examines, and draws conclusions about, the efficacy of the Meisner approach as a training method.