ArticlePDF Available

Antisocial behaviour during the teenage years: Understanding developmental risks

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Individuals are far more likely to engage in antisocial behaviour during adolescence than any other period of their life. This paper presents selected results from two studies which used secondary data analysis to provide a theoretically informed picture of youths' decision-making process in relation to delinquency. Study 1 focused on changes in adolescents' perceived rewards and delinquency involvement over four years. Results showed that high levels of perceived rewards go hand in hand with high levels of delinquency, but perceived antisocial rewards 'topped out' by age 14, suggesting that the best time to intervene is during early adolescence or late childhood. Study 2 focused on anger control. Youth who were highly delinquency-involved were especially likely to report surges in anger on days when they experienced a stressor, pointing to a need for delinquency prevention programs aimed at emotion control, including cognitive reappraisal. Improving these skills should enhance youths' ability to navigate risk during the teenage years. © 2018 Australian Institute of Criminology. All rights reserved.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Trends & issues in crime and criminal jusce
ISSN 0817-8542
Antisocial behaviour during
the teenage years:
Understanding
developmental risks
Kathryn Lynn Modecki, Bep Uink and
Bonnie L Barber
Adolescent ansocial behaviour, including delinquency, illegal
substance use and violence carry heavy social and economic
costs in Australia (Williams et al. 2005). Individuals are far more
likely to engage in ansocial behaviour during adolescence than
any other period of their life. In fact, in Australia, the oending
rate for adolescents is almost three mes the rate of all other age
groups (AIC 2013). Society’s challenges in reducing adolescent
ansocial behaviour underscore a fundamental reality: in order
to eecvely prevent these illegal and dangerous behaviours, we
must adequately understand their causes (Mo 2005).
Theorecal advances
Recent advances in the psychological sciences point to a
convergence of factors that exacerbate risk for iniaon and
escalaon of ansocial behaviour during adolescence (Modecki
& Uink 2017). Illustravely, signicant up-cks in problem
behaviours during the teenage years may be at least parally
aributable to developmental decits in what psychologists term
‘execuve funcon’ capacies (Luciana 2013). These capacies
are innately ed to decision making, and allow youth control
over impulses and behaviour.
Abstract | Individuals are far
more likely to engage in ansocial
behaviour during adolescence than
any other period of their life. This
paper presents selected results
from two studies which used
secondary data analysis to provide
a theorecally informed picture of
youths’ decision-making process in
relaon to delinquency.
Study 1 focused on changes in
adolescents’ perceived rewards
and delinquency involvement over
four years. Results showed that
high levels of perceived rewards
go hand in hand with high levels
of delinquency, but perceived
ansocial rewards ‘topped out’ by
age 14, suggesng that the best
me to intervene is during early
adolescence or late childhood.
Study 2 focused on anger control.
Youth who were highly delinquency-
involved were especially likely to
report surges in anger on days when
they experienced a stressor, poinng
to a need for delinquency prevenon
programs aimed at emoon control,
including cognive reappraisal.
Improving these skills should
enhance youths’ ability to navigate
risk during the teenage years.
No. 556 July 2018
Trends & issues in crime and criminal justice
Australian Institute of Criminology
2
No. 556 July 2018
Yet adolescents’ execuve systems can easily become overwhelmed because of the considerable
demands placed on these systems. For instance, adolescents are oen aracted to novel and risky
sengs and they idenfy sizable social and emoonal rewards from problem behaviour engagement.
More generally, adolescents are suscepble to uctuang emoons and oen wrestle with intense
emoonal reacvity as they encounter setbacks and challenges. These burdens on adolescents’
execuve systems, in the form of disproporonate perceived rewards for ansocial behaviour and
intense emoons, hamper adolescents’ ability to modulate their ‘internal trac’. As described below,
this may make it especially dicult for adolescents to rein in their impulses to engage in ansocial
behaviours, including behaviours related to delinquency—dened here as illegal and ansocial
behaviours of youth under the age of 18 (Luciana 2013; Modecki, Zimmer-Gembeck & Guerra 2017).
Arguably as a result of such burdens, adolescents are more suscepble to acng on their impulses
and taking part in aggressive, illegal and risky behaviours, relave to either children or adults
(Fine & Sung 2014; Luciana 2013). In environments featuring developmentally novel stressors,
adolescents are at a disadvantage, because they lack a repertoire of skills with which they might
prosocially navigate challenge. For instance, during the teenage years, crical skills such as decision-
making (including opmal weighing of rewards versus risks) and emoonal regulaon remain under
construcon (Modecki 2017).
Strain and ansocial behaviour
Because youth with underdeveloped decision-making and less-controlled emoons lack the
psychological resources to successfully resolve issues through convenonal strategies, these stressful
situaons can be especially strong catalysts for problems (Simons et al. 2003). As a result, pursuing
violence and illegal behaviours may be one way youth cope with the challenges of day-to-day life
(Chassin et al. 2010).
Notably, youth living in economically disadvantaged sengs experience added strains and
stressors as they navigate day-to-day life. Stressors including family dicules, perceived injusce,
neighbourhood disorganisaon, and less-eecve social instuons, amplify the common
developmental challenges with which youth must cope (Uink et al. 2018). These and other cumulave
strains may trigger youth to act out in the form of violence and other ansocial behaviours (Agnew
2001; Simons et al. 2003). In fact, criminologists have long pointed to the experience of ‘strain’ as a
salient explanaon for crime.
Adolescent ansocial behaviour and heterogeneity
Among adolescents, who are developmentally at risk for criminal engagement, there exists a subset
of young people who connue these behaviours into adulthood. These youth, somemes termed
‘life-course persistent oenders,’ also tend to begin ansocial acvies at a very early age (Mo et
al. 2002). During the teen years, life-course persistent oenders can be dicult to dierenate from
youth whose engagement is limited to adolescence, because their behaviours during this period are
relavely similar, except for violence (Mo 1993). A body of work has sought to disentangle those
who connue to oend from those who do not, and to do so earlier in the life course. Among the
disnguishing factors associated with more persistent involvement are negave emoonality at a
Trends & issues in crime and criminal justice
Australian Institute of Criminology
3
No. 556 July 2018
young age and ‘state dependence’, in which early involvement in problem behaviour leads to further
problems (Nagin & Paternoster 2000). In addion, young people who persist in ansocial behaviours
are more likely to come from low socio-economic backgrounds, given the numerous stressors and
associated lack of supports endemic to these environments.
Indeed, one of the more intractable risk factors for persistence with crime beyond adolescence is
being raised within circumstances of socio-economic adversity (Mo et al. 2002). Among other
hazards, adverse social environments amplify risks for negave interacons with family, peers and
school sengs, which can lead young people onto developmental pathways of risk rather than
resilience (Aguilar et al. 2000).
Individual characteriscs: rewards and negave emoons
That said, scholars have also idened a number of individual risk factors for adolescent involvement
in ansocial behaviour which exist across socio-economic gradients but exacerbate the risk associated
with situaonal strains and early disadvantage. In parcular, reward percepons and negave emoons
have received growing aenon from developmental psychologists, because they are closely linked to
teens’ involvement in ansocial behaviour and because the development of these characteriscs aligns
with age–crime trends (eg Steinberg et al. 2009). Indeed, substanal evidence supports what many
juvenile jusce praconers already suspect (Modecki 2017)—that a heightened focus on rewards
and negave emoonality is associated with ansocial behaviour, and that these factors contribute to
youths’ crime to a degree that disnguishes them from adult oenders (Sco & Steinberg 2008).
Given that these individual risks—reward-bias and the tendency to experience intense negave
emoons—appear to develop over me and are ed to ansocial choices, they represent risk factors
that may be modiable to prevent crime (Modecki 2009). As a result, understanding the development
of these factors, and how they relate to involvement in ansocial behaviour across the teenage years,
can inform intervenon and prevenon eorts. This report explores these factors in the context of
unique data from Australian youth living in sengs of economic disadvantage.
This report focuses on two studies which together provide a picture of factors that contribute to
escalang problems during the teenage years. These factors are explored among a parcularly high-
value group for criminologists and policymakers—young adolescents in economically disadvantaged
sengs, a subset of whom could require signicant me and resources due to their risk for engaging
in ansocial behaviour. Both studies provide brief snapshots of how changes in these factors are
linked with involvement in ansocial behaviour, and suggest novel ways that delinquency might be
prevented among high-risk youth.
Study 1: How rewarding is delinquency?
Increasingly, adolescents’ disproporonate involvement in ansocial behaviour has been aributed
to their heightened sensivity to rewards. That is, adolescents are more behaviourally disposed
towards aaining emoonal and social rewards from crime than any other age group (Shulman &
Cauman 2013) and a growing literature has idened the rewards of problem behaviour as having a
parcularly strong inuence on youth delinquency (Modecki 2009; Smith et al. 2011).
Trends & issues in crime and criminal justice
Australian Institute of Criminology
4
No. 556 July 2018
Indeed, past research indicates perceived rewards may be a stronger predictor of oending than
perceived risks, at least among juvenile oenders (Loughran et al. 2009). In prior research, Loughran
and colleagues ploed average perceived rewards over three years, and showed that mean levels
of perceived rewards remained relavely stable among incarcerated youth. Importantly, however,
reward levels connued to correspond with levels of oending, in that youth who engaged in high
levels of oending also perceived high rewards from crime, medium-level oenders perceived
moderate rewards, and so on.
That said, previous research has only looked at averages at dierent points in me, and has not
yet mapped developmental trajectories of reward percepons, nor examined how such changes in
reward percepon may be linked to delinquency. Further, serious juvenile oenders may experience
delinquency as dierenally rewarding than community-based youth, because by the me young
individuals have become incarcerated, they will have accumulated a broad range of benets and costs
from their crimes.
Thus, previous data do not answer quesons about the developmental progression of perceived
rewards and delinquency during adolescence, nor do they necessarily generalise to risks specic to
youth sll living in their communies. With a focus on disadvantaged youth in community sengs,
Study 1 explores a key queson for criminal jusce programs and policy: how do rewards drive
behaviour (and vice versa) among these youth?
This report addresses some of the methodological challenges inherent in probing the link between
perceived rewards and ansocial behaviour, by examining whether early levels of perceived
rewards predict changing involvement in delinquency over four years. Importantly, the reverse is
also examined: whether high levels of perceived rewards earlier in adolescence predict changes in
delinquency over four years. In other words, this approach applies a developmental criminology lens
to ansocial rewards and delinquency among low socio-economic status Australian youth during the
teenage years.
Sample
Data were derived from mulple waves of a large-scale annual self-report survey of Western
Australian youth, the Youth Acvity Parcipaon Survey (YAPS), funded by the Australian Research
Council. Further details regarding data collecon, the range of measures collected, and samples
over me can be found in Modecki, Barber and Vernon (2013); Modecki, Barber and Eccles (2014);
and Drane, Modecki and Barber (2017). Among YAPS parcipants, longitudinal data on ansocial
rewards were available for one cohort of youth across four years (from grades 9–12, corresponding
approximately with ages 13–18 years); see Table 1.
YAPS recruited schools across the state which covered a range of the socio-economic index computed
annually by the Western Australian Department of Educaon. The Index of Community
Socio-Educaonal Advantage (ICSEA) is calculated with data from the Australian Bureau of Stascs,
based on the addresses of all students aending each school. This study includes students with
average or below ICSEA, represenng the boom half of the spectrum of educaonal background
(n=480). Thus, Study 1 draws on a sample of youth from average to extremely educaonally
disadvantaged backgrounds and uses annual longitudinal self-report data to examine perceived
rewards and delinquency over me.
Trends & issues in crime and criminal justice
Australian Institute of Criminology
5
No. 556 July 2018
Measures
Perceived ansocial rewards were assessed using items from a valid construct which has successfully
measured decision-making in adolescents (Parsons, Seigel & Cousins 1997). Youth were presented
with the following:
Below is a list of behaviours that are illegal and/or dangerous. Some people might think that they have
advantages or benets. We are interested in whether you think they have advantages or benets.
Four items were used to assess the perceived benets of four illegal behaviours, such as shopliing
and illegal drug use. Youth responded on a Likert scale ranging from 0 (no benets), to 4 (moderate
benets) to 8 (a lot of benets). Internal reliability in this sample was good, and ranged from
α=0.78–0.94 across waves. This construct is referred to as ‘ansocial rewards’ or ‘perceived ansocial
rewards’ throughout this report.
Delinquency was measured with a reliable (α ranged from 0.77–0.91 across waves) and valid
construct that was adapted from a larger delinquency scale (Modecki, Barber & Vernon 2013)
to assess key behaviours of interest, including damaging public property, police contact, physical
ghng, and stealing. An example item includes:
In the past 6 months, how oen have you goen in a physical ght with another person?
Items were measured on an eight-point scale from 1 (none) to 8 (31 or more mes).
Pubertal ming was also controlled in these analyses, given its links to the development of ansocial
behaviours (eg Modecki, Barber & Eccles 2014). Pubertal ming was assessed using one item, taken
from Dubas, Graber and Petersen (1991). This item assessed self-reported physical development
relave to peers, with responses ranging from 1 (much later) to 5 (much earlier).
Table 1: Demographic characteriscs of Study 1 parcipants
Age at grade 9: M(SD) 14.42 (0.38)
Gender (% female) 57.3
School(s) socio-economic range (ICSEA) 815–1,000
Source: YAPS collecon 2011 [data le]
Analyses
Analyses involved modelling an uncondional latent growth curve of perceived ansocial rewards
across grades 9–12, followed by a condional model in which covariates (gender, pubertal ming)
and predictors (early, grade 9 delinquency) were added. Next, the same models were run with
delinquency as a latent growth curve and early (grade 9) perceived rewards as the predictor.
All models were run in Mplus version 7.1 both with maximum likelihood esmaon and then with
Bayes esmaon. For more detailed analyses and comparisons with alternave analyc approaches,
see Modecki and Uink forthcoming.
Trends & issues in crime and criminal justice
Australian Institute of Criminology
6
No. 556 July 2018
Results
First, perceived rewards were relavely stable across grades 9–12; however there was signicant
inter-individual variaon in early levels of rewards in grade 9. Adding covariates and predictors
to the model showed high early perceived rewards was associated with higher early delinquency
involvement (intercept, p<0.001). Importantly, other factors were also associated with subsequent
change in perceived rewards over four years.
That is, early puberty was associated with subsequent increases in ansocial rewards (intercept,
p=0.04) and high levels of early delinquency involvement were associated with subsequent declines
in rewards (linear slope, p<0.001). As described in Figure 1, this eect of delinquency on rewards
represents a ‘bouncing back’ eect, as youth who engaged in high levels of delinquency early on
appear to experience a ceiling eect. Even so, these youth with high levels of early delinquency
involvement sll perceived ansocial behaviour as most rewarding, at a rank-level, across the four
years. Even by the end of high school, the dierent categories of youth failed to converge in their
perceived ansocial rewards.
Figure 1: Interacon between maturaon (me) and delinquency engagement predicng perceived rewards
Source: YAPS collecon 2011 [data le]
Second, delinquency underwent curvilinear change across four years, following a u-shaped curve.
There was also signicant inter-individual variaon in early levels of delinquency in grade 9.
Adding covariates and predictors to the model showed high early perceived rewards were associated
with high early levels of delinquency involvement (intercept, p<0.001).
High levels of perceived ansocial rewards were also associated with subsequent declines in
delinquency over the rst few years (linear slope, p<0.001) followed by increases over the last few
years (quadrac slope, p<0.001).
Trends & issues in crime and criminal justice
Australian Institute of Criminology
7
No. 556 July 2018
Figure 2 demonstrates the interacon between maturaon (me) and early perceived rewards
predicng delinquency involvement. Youth who perceived high levels of ansocial rewards early
on, were already engaging in high levels of delinquency. These youth declined in their delinquency
involvement over the next few years of high school, followed by a slight upturn in grade 12.
Again, despite declines in delinquency over four years of high school, the sub-set of youth who
perceived many rewards from crime in 9th grade remained the most delinquency involved, at a rank-
level, across all years.
Figure 2: Interacon between maturaon (me) and perceived rewards predicng delinquency
involvement
Source: YAPS collecon 2011 [data le]
Overall, Study 1 ndings highlight that early adolescents’ percepons of delinquency’s rewards
are not necessarily enduring. Rather, for those adolescents whom police and jusce personnel are
most likely to encounter (who are already engaging in relavely high levels of delinquency early
in adolescence), these rewards diminished over me. That is, for these youth, perceived rewards
appeared to hit a ceiling by 9th grade. This suggests that delinquency may ‘lose its shine’, and these
young people gradually desist from ansocial behaviour. Indeed, adolescents high in early perceived
rewards also reported rapid declines in delinquency from grade 9–11, such that grade 9 represented
a developmental peak for engagement in ansocial behaviours. Although these young people were
the most frequent oenders at each me point, this subset of adolescents are likely to be largely
representave of ‘adolescent-limited’ oenders, given their overall paern of desistance.
That said, early oenders represent a high-value target for law enforcement. As described further in
the conclusion, addressing ancipated benets from crime as a prevenve strategy early on (prior to
the transion to high school) could prove useful.
Trends & issues in crime and criminal justice
Australian Institute of Criminology
8
No. 556 July 2018
For youth low in early delinquency, however, perceived rewards tended to increase over me,
perhaps a reecon of perceived es between social status and rule-violang behaviours (Rebellon
2006). Although adolescents did not nominate specic percepons related to the benets of illegal
behaviour, previous work suggests that impressing peers is indeed a salient idened reward from
crime (Modecki 2009). Those adolescents who were low in early perceived rewards reported increased
delinquent behaviour across the remainder of high school, reecve of a developmental norm of at
least some low level problem behaviour engagement (Modecki 2017). That said, these adolescents
remained lowest in delinquency—in terms of rank—at each me point, so that early low levels of
perceived reward as well as early low levels of delinquency involvement appear to characterise youth
on a fairly auspicious developmental trajectory, at least in terms of averng crime involvement.
Study 2: Are emoonal responses and adolescent
delinquency linked?
Not only are rewards of crime especially salient to adolescents’ ansocial decisions, but emoon also
plays a role. The emoonal variability of adolescence is well documented and this developmental
period is characterised by relavely poor emoonal control (Cauman & Steinberg 2000).
However, signicant variability between young people also exists. That is, some adolescents are
beer able to temper their emoons than others are and some youth are less emoonally reacve
than others when encountering setbacks and challenges (Uink, Modecki & Barber 2017).
More specically, adolescents who are delinquency-involved tend also to be disnguished by
especially intense emoonal responses to aggravaons and annoyances, and can show large
deviaons (in terms of highs and lows) in their emoons (Planer et al. 2007; Uink et al. 2018).
Indeed, previous survey research shows that adolescents who are beer able to temper their
emoons also make fewer ansocial decisions and engage in fewer delinquent acts (Cauman
& Steinberg 2000; Modecki 2008, 2009). Moreover, among juvenile oenders, developmental
improvements in emoonal control are associated with subsequent decreases in and desistance from
ansocial behaviour (Chassin et al. 2010; Monahan et al. 2009).
This link between emoonal control and delinquency is important for a number of reasons.
Understanding young people’s responses to strains and hassles is highly germane to delinquency
prevenon, because these can trigger emoonal and behavioural responses associated with ‘acng-
out’. Thus, learning to be less reacve to aggravaons may help to diminish adolescents’ aggressive and
ansocial reacons to setbacks. More broadly, if at-risk youth are to steer away from involvement in the
jusce system, they will need the skills necessary to successfully navigate challenges in day-to-day life.
Sample
Study 2 takes a dierent approach to understanding ansocial behaviour during the teenage years,
and examines a second developmental risk—emoonal valence, here in relaon to stress. Taking
advantage of exisng data from an intensive ecological momentary assessment (EMA) study with
at-risk youth, this study examines how changes in emoons relave to typical daily emoons are
linked to delinquent behaviour. By describing how delinquent youth (fail to) temper their emoonal
responses in the real world, ndings provide praccal insight for delinquency prevenon and
intervenon programs and services (Modecki & Mazza 2017).
Trends & issues in crime and criminal justice
Australian Institute of Criminology
9
No. 556 July 2018
Thus, the second approach was to use intensive EMA data from cohort 1 of the Young and
Well Cooperave Research Centre supported ‘How do you feel?’ study. Data were collected via
smartphones from 109 low socio-economic status Australian youth (see Uink, Modecki & Barber
2017 for details of the study; see Table 2 for ‘How do you feel?’ cohort 1 parcipant details). Youth
were texted ve mes a day for seven days and asked to provide their current emoon and whether
they had experienced a recent hassle, among other details. Before and aer EMA, the parcipants
reported their recent delinquency involvement and completed other wellbeing indices to provide a
picture of overall mental health.
Table 2: Demographic characteriscs of ‘How do you feel?’ cohort 1 parcipants
Age: M(SD) 14.7 (0.92)
Gender (% female) 66.9
Socio-economic range (ICSEA) 900–1,000
Source: ‘How do you feel?’ data collecon cohort 1 2013–14 [data le]
Measures
Delinquency was measured before and aer EMA using 15 items that assessed how oen parcipants
had engaged in ansocial or aggressive behaviour and substance use. This measure has been used
in previously published research on adolescent ansocial behaviour (Fredricks & Eccles 2006), and
example items include: ‘About how oen in the last 6 months have you used drugs?’and ‘About how
oen…have you goen in a physical ght with another person?’, where responses ranged from
0 (none) to 7 (31 or more mes). Internal reliability at before and aer EMA was excellent (α=0.85,
α=0.90) and test-retest reliability was high (r=0.88). Parcipants’ scores from both surveys were
averaged to create an ansocial behaviour score.
Daily hassles were measured during the EMA poron of the study by asking parcipants via
smartphones, ‘Since you were last messaged has anything bad happened to you?’ at each sampling
moment. The format of this queson meant that parcipants reported on events that had occurred
within the last two to ve hours. A dummy variable was created based on this informaon, coded
so that 0 means no bad events (hassles) that day, and 1 means one or more moderate to severe
hassle(s) that day. Emoon was also measured during the EMA poron of the study, by asking via
smartphones ‘Right now, how are you feeling?’ Parcipants rated how angry (among other emoons)
they were feeling on a ve-point scale, from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). Emoon was averaged
across the day for this report.
Analyses
Cross-level random slopes models were run within a hierarchical linear modelling framework in
Mplus version 7.0, with delinquency at level 2, daily hassle (yes/no) as the level 1 predictor and day-
level anger as the level 1 outcome variable.
Trends & issues in crime and criminal justice
Australian Institute of Criminology
10
No. 556 July 2018
Results
There was a signicant cross-level interacon of hassles × delinquency for anger. As Figure 3
illustrates, youth who engaged in high levels of delinquency reported surges in anger on days they
experienced a hassle (b=0.614, p<0.001). But low-delinquency youth did not change in their relave
anger on days they ran into a hassle (b=–0.176, p>0.05). Thus, as expected, youth delinquency
involvement was ed to surges in anger in relaon to hassles.
Figure 3: Relaons between experiencing of a hassle and daily level of reported anger (ranging from 1–5)
for high and low delinquency youth
***
Source: ‘How do you feel?’ data collecon cohort 1 2013–14 [data le]
That adolescents who engage in high levels of delinquency experience surges in anger on days when
they experience a roadblock or challenge is not surprising at an intuive level. But empirical support
for this phenomenon represents useful evidence for researchers and praconers alike. Daily links
between experience of strain and surges in anger for young people who engage in delinquency
mean that helping adolescents to beer navigate hassles and strains may represent an important
mechanism for improving resilience and coping. Importantly, too, high reacvity to strain suggests that
these adolescents require beer ‘life skills’ for eecvely responding to stress. Given that adolescence
is a crical developmental period for accumulang these skills, helping youth to improve emoonal
control and decision-making in response to dicules should contribute to reducons in ansocial
behaviours and more posive funconing overall (Modecki, Zimmer-Gembeck & Guerra 2017).
Trends & issues in crime and criminal justice
Australian Institute of Criminology
11
No. 556 July 2018
Conclusions
All told, ndings across these two studies describe key developmental features of delinquency that
could be further targeted in intervenon and prevenon programs to reduce adolescent crime.
First, programs and policies should work to reduce the salience of rewards for teenagers. One way
to do so would be to highlight the temporary nature of ansocial rewards (such as impressing peers
and an emoonal ‘rush’), while simultaneously highlighng crime’s serious and long-term negave
consequences (Modecki 2016, 2009). That said, eorts to counter teens’ reward percepons should
be deployed early on, well before 9th grade (age 14), because by this stage youth appear to have
already idened crime as especially rewarding.
Second, programs and services should focus on emoon regulaon and anger control in parcular,
to improve young people’s resilience and prevent delinquency (Landenberger & Lipsey 2005). Holisc
approaches may be best suited to improving self-regulaon and coping skills, including approaches
that balance law enforcement with assisng youth and reconnecng them with supports from
families, schools and communies.
These types of supports may be most eecvely delivered in partnership with respected community
members, by oering a range of support services, and by oering these within an open-door
framework. In all, because oending among those in mid to late adolescence tends to be especially
costly for society (Piquero, Jennings & Farrington 2013), these types of front-end intervenons to
divert youth from ansocial pathways early on should reap monetary and social benets.
Acknowledgements
These data were collected with support from two sources (the Australian Research Council and the
Young and Well Cooperave Research Centre). Neither project was originally funded to examine
juvenile delinquency. Special thanks to members of the project’s advisory board, including Sergeant
Victoria Lewis and Inspector Corey Allen.
Trends & issues in crime and criminal justice
Australian Institute of Criminology
12
No. 556 July 2018
References
Agnew R 2001. Building on the foundaon of general strain theory: Specifying the types of strain most likely to
lead to crime and delinquency. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 38: 319–361
Aguilar B, Sroufe LA, Egeland B & Carlson E 2000. Disnguishing the early-onset/persistent and adolescence-
onset ansocial behavior types: From birth to 16 years. Development and Psychopathology 12: 109–132
Australian Instute of Criminology (AIC) 2014. Australian crime: Facts & gures: 2013. Canberra: Australian
Instute of Criminology. hps://aic.gov.au/publicaons/facts/2013
Berkowitz L 1990. On the formaon and regulaon of anger and aggression: A cognive-neoassociaonisc
analysis. American Psychologist 45: 494–503
Broidy LM 2001. A test of general strain theory. Criminology 39: 9–36
Cauman E & Steinberg L 2000. (Im)maturity of judgment in adolescence: Why adolescents may be less
culpable than adults. Behavioral Sciences & the Law 18: 741–760
Chassin L, Dmitrieva J, Modecki KL, Steinberg L, Cauman E, Piquero AR et al. 2010. Does adolescent alcohol
and marijuana use predict suppressed growth in psychosocial maturity among male juvenile oenders?
Psychology of Addicve Behaviors 24: 48–60
Drane CF, Modecki KL & Barber BL 2017. Disentangling development of sensaon seeking, risky peer aliaon,
and binge drinking in adolescent sport. Addicve Behaviors 66: 60–65
Dubas JS, Graber JA & Petersen AC 1991. A longitudinal invesgaon of adolescents’ changing percepons of
pubertal ming. Developmental Psychology 27: 580–586
Fine JG & Sung C 2014. Neuroscience of child and adolescent health development. Journal of Counseling
Psychology 61: 521–527
Fredricks JA & Eccles JS 2006. Is extracurricular parcipaon associated with benecial outcomes? Concurrent
and longitudinal relaons. Developmental Psychology 42: 698–713
Landenberger NA & Lipsey MW 2005. The posive eects of cognive–behavioral programs for oenders: A
meta-analysis of factors associated with eecve treatment. Journal of Experimental Criminology 1: 451–476
Loughran TA, Piquero AR, Fagan J & Mulvey EP 2009. Dierenal deterrence: Studying heterogeneity and
changes in perceptual deterrence among serious youthful oenders. Crime & Delinquency 58: 3–27
Luciana M 2013. Adolescent brain development in normality and psychopathology. Development and
Psychopathology 25: 1325–1345
Modecki KL 2017. Adolescent development and clinical and legal implicaons, in Righthand S & Murphy WD
(eds) The Safer Society handbook of assessment and treatment of adolescents who have sexually oended.
Brandon VT: Safer Society Press: 1–24
Modecki KL 2016. Do risks maer? Variable and person-centered approaches to adolescents’ problem behavior.
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 42: 8–20
Modecki KL 2009. “It’s a rush”: Psychosocial content of ansocial decision making. Law and Human Behavior 33:
183–193
Modecki KL 2008. Addressing gaps in the maturity of judgment literature: Age dierences and delinquency. Law
and Human Behavior 32: 78–91
Modecki KL, Barber BL & Eccles JE 2014. Binge drinking trajectories across adolescence: Extra-curricular
acvies are protecve for youth with early pubertal development. Journal of Adolescent Health 54: 61–66
Modecki KL, Barber BL & Vernon L 2013. Mapping developmental precursors of cyber-aggression: Trajectories
of risk predict perpetraon and vicmizaon. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 42: 651–661
Modecki KL & Mazza G 2017. Are we making the most of ecological momentary assessment data? A comment
on Richardson, Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, O’Donnell, Ling, & Staiger, 2017. Health Psychology Review 11: 295–297
Trends & issues in crime and criminal justice
Australian Institute of Criminology
13
No. 556 July 2018
Modecki KL & Uink B 2018. Understanding ansocial behaviour during the teenage years: Developmental
pathways of ansocial decision making among disadvantaged youth. Report to the Criminology Research
Advisory Council. Canberra: Australian Instute of Criminology
Modecki KL & Uink B 2017. How can developmental psychopathology inuence social and legal policy?
Adolescence, mental health, and decision making, in Cenfan LC & Williams D (eds), The Wiley Handbook of
Developmental Psychopathology. Wiley-Blackwell
Modecki KL, Zimmer-Gembeck M & Guerra N 2017. Emoon regulaon, coping and decision making: Three
linked skills for prevenng externalizing problems in adolescence. Child Development 88: 417–426
Mo TE 2005. The new look of behavioral genecs in developmental psychopathology: Gene-environment
interplay in ansocial behaviors. Psychological Bullen 131: 533–554
Mo TE 1993. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent ansocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy.
Psychological Review 100: 674–701
Mo TE & Caspi A 2001. Childhood predictors dierenate life-course persistent and adolescence-limited
ansocial pathways among males and females. Development and Psychopathology 13: 355–375
Mo TE, Caspi A, Harrington H & Milne BJ 2002. Males on the life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited
ansocial pathways: Follow-up at age 26 years. Development and Psychopathology 14: 179–207
Monahan K , Steinberg L, Cauman E & Mulvey EP 2009. Trajectories of ansocial behavior and psychosocial
maturity from adolescence to young adulthood. Developmental Psychology 45: 1654–1668
Nagin D & Paternoster R 2000. Populaon heterogeneity and state dependence: State of the evidence and
direcons for future research. Journal of Quantave Criminology 16: 117–144
Parsons JT, Siegel AW & Cousins JH 1997. Late adolescent risk-taking: Eects of perceived benets and perceived
risks on behavioral intenons and behavioral change. Journal of Adolescence 20: 381–392
Piquero AR, Jennings WG & Farrington D 2013. The monetary costs of crime to middle adulthood: Findings from
the Cambridge study in delinquent development. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 50: 53–74
Planer B, Karnik N, Booil J, Hall RE, Schallauer A, Carrion V, Feucht M & Steiner H 2007. State and trait
emoons in delinquent adolescents. Child Psychiatry and Human Development 38: 155–169
Rebellon CJ 2006. Do adolescents engage in delinquency to aract the social aenon of peers? An extension and
longitudinal test of the social reinforcement hypothesis. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 43: 387–411
Sco ES & Steinberg L 2008. Adolescent development and the regulaon of youth crime. The Future of Children
18: 15–33
Shulman EP & Cauman E 2013. Reward-biased risk appraisal and its relaon to juvenile versus adult crime.
Law and Human Behavior 37: 412–423
Simons RL, Chen YF, Stewart EA & Brody GH 2003. Incidents of discriminaon and risk for delinquency: A
longitudinal test of strain theory with an African American sample. Jusce Quarterly 20: 827–854
Smith AB, Halari R, Giampetro V, Brammer M & Rubia K 2011. Developmental eects of reward on sustained
aenon networks. Neuroimage 56: 1693–1704
Steinberg L, Graham S, O’Brien L, Woolard J, Cauman E & Banich M 2009. Age dierences in future orientaon
and delay discounng. Child Development 80(1): 28–44
Uink B, Modecki KL, Barber BL & Correia H 2018. Socioeconomically disadvantaged adolescents with elevated
externalizing symptoms show heightened emoon reacvity to daily stress: An experience sampling study. Child
Psychiatry and Human Development. DOI: 10.1007/s10578-018-0784-x
Williams J, Toumbourou JW, McDonald M, Jones S & Moore T 2005. A sea change on the island connent:
Frameworks for risk assessment, prevenon and intervenon in child health in Australia. Children & Society 19:
91–104
General editor, Trends & issues in crime and criminal jusce series: Dr Rick Brown, Deputy
Director, Australian Instute of Criminology. Note: Trends & issues in crime and criminal jusce
papers are peer reviewed. For a complete list and the full text of the papers in the Trends &
issues in crime and criminal jusce series, visit the AIC website at: aic.gov.au
ISSN 0817-8542
©Australian Instute of Criminology 2018
GPO Box 1936
Canberra ACT 2601, Australia
Tel: 02 6268 7166
Disclaimer: This research paper does not necessarily
reect the policy posion of the Australian Government
Trends & issues in crime and criminal justice
Australian Institute of Criminology
aic.gov.au
Kathryn Modecki is a Senior Lecturer
at Grith University and Menzies
Health Instute Queensland; data
were collected while at Murdoch
University.
Bep Uink is a PhD candidate at
Murdoch University.
Bonnie Barber is Professor and
Director of the Building Healthy
Communies program in the Menzies
Health Instute Queensland.
... Rule-breaking constituted the most common form of antisocial behaviour, in line with some previous research (e.g., Becht, Prinzie, Deković, van den Akker & Shiner, 2016). It is worth noting that some studies reported physical aggression as the most common form of antisocial behaviour in adolescents (Cuervo, Villanueva, Born & Gavray, 2018;Modecki, Uink & Barber, 2018). The obtained results are in relatively good agreement with the findings of earlier domestic studies. ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous studies have shown that exposure to adverse life events is positively associated with a tendency towards antisocial behavior. However, results concerning relationships between antisocial behaviour and internal factors, such as personality traits, are not so consistent. The present study aimed to examine the effects of negative life events and Dark Tetrad traits (narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy, and sadism) on different antisocial behaviors, including norm-breaking, aggression, vandalism, and drug use. The sample included 221 high school students (66% females) from three cities in Serbia. Results showed that negative life events significantly predicted norm-breaking, aggression, and vandalism, while such life events’ power to predict drug use ceased once the dark traits were added to the regression model. In the case of dark traits, narcissism, psychopathy, and sadism positively predicted norm-breaking and aggression, while sadism also predicted vandalism. Machiavellianism did not significantly predict any of the antisocial behaviors. Тhe results suggest that norm-breaking is largely explained by both negative life events and personality traits. Aggression, on the other hand, is better explained by personality traits. Finally, the examined factors contribute much less to the explanation of vandalism and drug use. The results are discussed in the context of possible prevention of antisocial behavior in adolescents.
Article
Full-text available
The fact that youth widely engage with the online space in order to improve their emotional health has been lost amongst the debate surrounding adolescents' technology use and associated well-being. Two studies focused on adolescents’ use of technology to cope with stressors in daily life. Focus groups (Study 1; n = 16) indicated that youth readily identify using technology to cope and perceive it as helpful for emotional relief. Experience sampling participants (Study 2; n = 156) completed a baseline assessment of online coping and mental health and one week of reports, five times daily, on their technology use, stress, and emotions. Multilevel regression results indicated that across their daily lives, youth who widely endorsed using online strategies to cope responded more negatively to stressors and displayed difficulties in recovering from worry and jealousy. Results held even controlling for use of technology, typical number of stressors, and mental health. Findings highlight the potential shortcomings and comorbid vulnerabilities for youth who frequently turn to the online environment to seek support, information, or distraction.
Article
Full-text available
Numerous theories assert that youth with externalizing symptomatology experience intensified emotion reactivity to stressful events; yet scant empirical research has assessed this notion. Using in-vivo data collected via experience sampling methodology, we assessed whether externalizing symptoms conditioned adolescents’ emotion reactivity to daily stressors (i.e. change in emotion pre-post stressor) among 206 socioeconomically disadvantaged adolescents. We also assessed whether higher externalizing symptomology was associated with experiencing more stressors overall, and whether adolescents’ emotional upheavals resulted in experiencing a subsequent stressor. Hierarchical linear models showed that adolescents higher in externalizing symptoms experienced stronger emotion reactivity in sadness, anger, jealously, loneliness, and (dips in) excitement. Externalizing symptomatology was not associated with more stressful events, but a stress-preventative effect was found for recent upheavals in jealousy among youth low in externalizing. Findings pinpoint intense emotion reactivity to daily stress as a risk factor for youth with externalizing symptoms living in socioeconomic disadvantage. Access online: http://rdcu.be/HIwb
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter briefly outlines some of the contextual elements that are both risk factors for psychopathology in childhood and adolescence and may represent hindrances to adolescents' beneficial choices. Next, it discusses how adolescents with psychopathology, and externalizing in particular, may be especially ill-equipped for decision making both in antisocial contexts and in the adjudication process. The chapter then provides some starting points for future research in relation to psychopathology and juvenile criminal culpability and legal treatment. It also provides a brief consideration of some of the known familial and parental risk factors in the development of child and adolescent psychopathology, to underscore how these contextual risks place youth with mental health issues in a double bind in terms of problematic outcomes. Finally, the chapter describes how executive function (EF) skills develop across the course of adolescence among normative youth and highlight a burgeoning literature linking deficits in EF to adolescent psychopathology broadly.
Article
Full-text available
Research on executive control during the teenage years points to shortfalls in emotion regulation, coping, and decision making as three linked capabilities associated with youth's externalizing behavior problems. Evidence gleaned from a detailed review of the literature makes clear that improvement of all three capabilities is critical to help young people better navigate challenges and prevent or reduce externalizing and related problems. Moreover, interventions can successfully improve these three capabilities and have been found to produce behavioral improvements with real-world significance. Examples of how successful interventions remediate more than one of these capabilities are provided. Future directions in research and practice are also proposed to move the field toward the development of more comprehensive programs for adolescents to foster their integration.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: To describe simultaneous within-person change in adolescent binge drinking, sensation seeking and risky peer affiliation for adolescents in sport across grades 8-10. Methods: Longitudinal data from 502 adolescent sport participants were analyzed. A multivariate growth curve model tracked early levels and concurrent change in binge drinking, sensation seeking, and risky peers in sports across three years. Results: Findings revealed that early levels of sensation seeking and risky peers in sport were positively and significantly associated with early levels of binge drinking. Further, early high levels of sensation seeking predicted increases in risky peer associations and binge drinking. Importantly, developmental increases in risky peer associations paralleled developmental increases in binge drinking, even after accounting for development of sensation seeking. Conclusions: It is important to identify factors that place adolescents at increased risk for escalations in binge drinking, especially within settings known to be problematic. Sensation seeking is a particularly potent driver of adolescent binge drinking within the extracurricular sport context. Further, accelerations in binge drinking travel hand-in-hand with affiliating with risky peers in sport.
Article
Full-text available
Two limitations in research examining adolescents' risk cognitions have been the absence of developmental age group comparisons on a breadth of cognitions and the need to better characterize how cognitions influence behavior. To address these limitations, this study compared adolescent (n=205; 52% female) and young adult (n=274; 58% female) risk cognitions (risk probability, risk identification, risk tolerance, risk salience, and risk preference) and used variable- and person-centered approaches to explore how cognitions affect problem behavior. Adolescents generally reported lower risk-related cognitions than young adults. Further, risk probability, the cognition typically assessed in research, did not exert an independent effect on behavior. Adolescents and young adults were characterized by two similar cognition profiles, but only adolescents were characterized by a third, maladaptive profile, Low Identification/High Preference, reflecting low risk identification and risk salience and high risk preference. Interventions should arguably target these three cognitions within at-risk youth.
Article
Full-text available
Recent advances in technology and neuroscience have increased our understanding of human neurodevelopment. In particular, research on neuroplasticity and psychosocial genomics lends compelling support to a biopsychosocial perspective by elucidating mechanisms through which psychosocial forces and environments shape neurobiology. This article summarizes selected results from recent investigations of neuroplasticity and psychosocial genomics, which demonstrate complex interaction between genes, epigenetic processes, and environmental experience that confers neural growth into adulthood. Counseling psychologists working with children and adolescents need to be familiar with recent literature to be more effective in their work so that they can provide developmentally appropriate counseling services. Social cognitive theory and resilience theory are introduced to illustrate how counseling psychologists can incorporate neuroscience research findings in a counseling context and hypotheses are proposed for future counseling psychology research. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2014 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
To describe adolescent binge drinking trajectories across grades 8-11 and examine their associations with pubertal timing, socioeconomic status (SES), and structured activity and sport involvement. Longitudinal data were analyzed from the Youth Activity Participation Study (YAPS), an annual survey of youth in 39 schools across Western Australia (N = 1,342). Latent class growth analysis revealed four binge drinking trajectory groups: Accelerating (early onset, increased frequency), Steep Increase (delayed onset, rapid escalation), Slow Growth (delayed onset, gradual increase) and Stable Low (abstinence). Accelerating was characterized by early pubertal timing, low SES, and more sport involvement in grade 8, relative to Stable Low. The groups did not significantly differ in their grade 8 activity participation. However, for early maturers, greater grade 8 activity participation was associated with a decreased probability of belonging to Steep Increase relative to Stable Low. Early pubertal timing and sports participation increased the odds of belonging to a problematic binge drinking trajectory. For youth at-risk due to early pubertal timing, structured activities appear to be protective against a problematic developmental course of binge drinking.
Chapter
Who we “are” in adolescence does not foretell our adulthood. Because adolescence is a time of immense physiological, cognitive, and social change, teenagers confront daily challenges in navigating their shifting internal and external landscapes. These developmental shifts, and the resources needed to successfully pilot them, strain adolescents’ capacity for adaptive and prosocial decision making in the context of risky or antisocial opportunities they confront (Centifanti, Modecki, MacLellan, & Gowling, 2016; Chein, Albert, O’Brien, Uckert, & Steinberg, 2011). As a result of these and other age-based demands, the risks for engaging in behaviors such as violence, sexual risk taking, substance use, school dropout, and other antisocial activities is higher during adolescence relative to any other developmental period (Bongers, Koot, Van Der Ende, & Verhulst, 2004). That said, maturation during adolescence around self-identity, cognitive abilities, and moral reasoning also provide a solid foundation for intervention during this time. In fact, neurological and psychosocial changes are tightly paired during adolescence, and this can set the stage for malleability in some key factors that contribute to antisocial behavior, such as affective decision making, emotional valence, and susceptibility to peer influence (Modecki, 2008; 2009; Monahan, Steinberg, Cauffman, & Mulvey, 2009). For this reason, prevention of adolescents’ antisocial behaviors and settings and programs that diminish their occurrence, is of sizable interest to researchers, policy makers, practitioners. In this chapter, I briefly outlay theory and empirical research across major areas of psychology to help explicate the unique role of “adolescence” in the occurrence of risky and antisocial behaviors. I then tie our developmental understanding of adolescents’ decision making and behavior to the legal arena, where considerations of teenagers’ normative development should arguably impact their legal treatment. Finally, I briefly discuss policy and practice implications for adolescent antisocial behavior, broadly speaking, with a final note on areas for future research.
Article
Since this journal's inception, the field of adolescent brain development has flourished, as researchers have investigated the underpinnings of adolescent risk-taking behaviors. Explanations based on translational models initially attributed such behaviors to executive control deficiencies and poor frontal lobe function. This conclusion was bolstered by evidence that the prefrontal cortex and its interconnections are among the last brain regions to structurally and functionally mature. As substantial heterogeneity of prefrontal function was revealed, applications of neuroeconomic theory to adolescent development led to dual systems models of behavior. Current epidemiological trends, behavioral observations, and functional magnetic resonance imaging based brain activity patterns suggest a quadratic increase in limbically mediated incentive motivation from childhood to adolescence and a decline thereafter. This elevation occurs in the context of immature prefrontal function, so motivational strivings may be difficult to regulate. Theoretical models explain this patterning through brain-based accounts of subcortical-cortical integration, puberty-based models of adolescent sensation seeking, and neurochemical dynamics. Empirically sound tests of these mechanisms, as well as investigations of biology-context interactions, represent the field's most challenging future goals, so that applications to psychopathology can be refined and so that developmental cascades that incorporate neurobiological variables can be modeled.
Article
Objectives: Monetary cost estimates of criminal careers have been limited to specific samples, specific ages, and focused on the United States. This article is the first to examine the costs of a life course of crime in the United Kingdom. Method: This study uses longitudinal data from 411 South London males from the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development (CSDD) to derive costs-of-crime estimates from childhood to middle adulthood (ages 10 to 50). Additional features include a calculation of cost estimates across distinct offending trajectories and centering on costs per offender. Results: Offending over the life course imposes a considerable amount of economic and social costs and these costs are differentially distributed across offending trajectories. The cost of high-rate chronic offending is nearly two and a half to ten times greater than the cost of high adolescence peaked offending, very low-rate chronic offending, and low adolescence peaked offending, respectively. It is estimated that a male high-rate chronic offender on average would impose an annual cost of £18 ($29) per U.K. citizen or a lifetime cost of £742 ($1,185) per U.K. citizen. Conclusions: As the average and total costs of crime were significantly different across offending trajectories, with high-rate chronics imposing the most financial burden, adopting prevention and intervention strategies aimed at reducing the number of high-rate chronics and/or speeding up their eventual desistance will offer many savings to the public and perhaps turn those negative costs into positive contributions.