ArticlePDF Available

Behavioural responses of white and bronze turkeys (Meleagris gallopavo) to tonic immobility, gait score and open field tests in free-range system

Taylor & Francis
Journal of Applied Animal Research
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study was carried out to investigate the behavioural responses of white and bronze turkeys to tonic immobility (TI), gait score (GS) and open field (OF) tests in a free-range system. 144 female turkeys (72 white and 72 bronze) were studied for 23 weeks. They were 32 weeks old. The stocking density was 2 birds/m² indoors and 0.66 birds/m² outdoors. Both bird genotypes were fed on a diet containing 16% crude protein and 11.7 ME MJ/kg. The birds were weighed in the 32nd, 35th, 48th and 55th week. The turkeys’ behaviour was determined by TI, GS and OF tests. Behavioural parameters were established for each applied test. Although the mortality rates of white and bronze turkeys during the study were 6% and 3%, respectively, the white turkeys showed better results in the TI and OF tests suggesting that are more native breed than bronze ones. The results indicate that bronze turkeys are more suited for use in free-range systems than white turkeys with respect to GS and the consequent mortality rates in latter ones.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Behavioural responses of white and bronze turkeys (Meleagris gallopavo) to tonic
immobility, gait score and open eld tests in free-range system
Atilla Taskin , Ufuk Karadavut and Huseyin Çayan
Department of Animal Science, Faculty of Agriculture, Ahi Evran University, Kırsehir, Turkey
ABSTRACT
This study was carried out to investigate the behavioural responses of white and bronze turkeys to tonic
immobility (TI), gait score (GS) and open eld (OF) tests in a free-range system. 144 female turkeys (72
white and 72 bronze) were studied for 23 weeks. They were 32 weeks old. The stocking density was 2
birds/m
2
indoors and 0.66 birds/m
2
outdoors. Both bird genotypes were fed on a diet containing 16%
crude protein and 11.7 ME MJ/kg. The birds were weighed in the 32nd, 35th, 48th and 55th week. The
turkeysbehaviour was determined by TI, GS and OF tests. Behavioural parameters were established for
each applied test. Although the mortality rates of white and bronze turkeys during the study were 6%
and 3%, respectively, the white turkeys showed better results in the TI and OF tests suggesting that are
more native breed than bronze ones. The results indicate that bronze turkeys are more suited for use
in free-range systems than white turkeys with respect to GS and the consequent mortality rates in
latter ones.
ARTICLE HISTORY
Received 29 December 2015
Accepted 27 June 2018
KEYWORDS
Free-range; genotype; open
eld activity; tonic
immobility; Turkey
1. Introduction
The interactions between housing conditions, management
and animals are extremely complex. The impact of environ-
mental conditions on animal welfare has to be considered in
detail (Tuyttens et al. 2008). Numerous factors inuence the
ranging behaviour of poultry. Changing the environment of
an animal to support its physical activity can fortify the struc-
tural integrity of its body (Balog et al. 1997). Environmental
changes can contribute to the welfare of animals as well as
their body health. There may be negative eects on animal
health and welfare in intensive production (Maurice et al.
1990). In such environments, animals may be distressed,
which in turn is associated to depressed growth (Schutz et al.
2004), increased injury (Reed et al. 1993), lower production
(De Haas et al. 2013), feather pecking (De Haas et al. 2010)
and a large number of similar negative eects related to pro-
ductivity and welfare. Free-range systems are benecial for
animals in terms of their health and welfare. In this system,
the outdoor shelter provides a wide free eld with sunlight
where animals are able to display their natural behaviour
(Thiele and Pottgüter 2008; Turkoglu and Sarica 2009). Consu-
mers are showing interest in poultry production systems that
are semi-intensive, extensive or free-range. The products from
these systems are more natural and healthy. They are produced
in accordance with the accepted standards of animal welfare.
Their popularity has, therefore, increased year by year (Sarica
and Yamak, 2010).
Various indicators are available to assess the welfare con-
ditions of animals, such as tonic immobility (TI), gait score
(GS) and open eld test (OF). Tonic immobility is connected
to disposition and anti-predator behaviour, being a measure
of courage toward predators (Edelaar et al. 2012). The longer
a bird stays still, the higher its level of fear is (Moller and Szép
2011). Tonic immobility (TI) is a connatural reaction of animals
in times of fear, manifesting itself with temporal petrication
or paralysis. Tonic immobility can be observed in a variety of
animals including domestic fowl (Gallup et al. 1972). In a
study by Taskin (2009), adding thyme powder to the basal
diets of broilers lowered the duration of tonic immobility (172
s) to a statistically signicant extent (P< .05) when compared
to the control group. It was evidenced that pharmacological
activity of thyme powder had expectorant, antimicrobial, anti-
septic, antioxidative, antivirotic, antihelminthic, sedative, anti-
spasmodic, carminative, diaphoretic and antifungal eects (El-
Hack et al. 2016). In a study conducted on free and intermittent
feeding of turkeys, their tonic immobility duration was found at
327.6 and 427.3 s, respectively (Konca et al. 2004).
Improvements in detection of lameness bring about
enhanced clinical results (Alawneh et al. 2012; Leach et al.
2012). Visual inspection of walking ability oers the advantage
of allowing noninvasive evaluation of large numbers of birds in
a short period of time (Webster et al. 2008). Customary pro-
cedure to assign gait scoring includes the manual scoring of
animal behaviour in the poultry house (Aydin et al. 2010). Dom-
estic fowls have been evaluated mostly by using the gait
scoring system. Birds with a 3- or over gait scores experienced
pain (Kestin et al. 1992; Ferket et al. 2009). Comprehensibility of
the 3-point system currently used in commercial farms in the
United States may encourage observer trustworthiness for
gait scoring in commercial poultry ocks (Webster et al. 2008).
© 2018 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distri-
bution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
CONTACT Atilla Taskin ataskin@ahievran.edu.tr Department of Animal Science, Faculty of Agriculture, Ahi Evran University, Kırsehir 40100, Turkey
JOURNAL OF APPLIED ANIMAL RESEARCH
2018, VOL. 46, NO. 1, 12531259
https://doi.org/10.1080/09712119.2018.1495642
In a study carried out on turkeys, the proportion of turkeys with
a normal gait was found 56.67% and 64.71% in freely and inter-
mittently fed groups. In turkeys with medium-level diculties of
walking, the rate was found as 20.0% and 20.59%. The percen-
tage of the free feeding group with symptoms of severe walking
diculties was 23.33. This ratio was found as 18.75% in the
intermittent feeding group (Konca et al. 2004).
The open-eld test was used to investigate the emotional
responsiveness and incentives of laboratory and poultry
animals (Candland and Nagy 1969). Spontaneous locomotor
activity of turkeys was assessed by the open eld (OF) test (Bel-
viranli et al. 2012). Activity of animals during the open-eld tests
is used to detect behavioural genetic dierences between
selected lines or strains (Koolhaas et al. 1999; Wahlsten et al.
2003; Lalonde and Strazielle 2008). Common fear-avoidance is
associated with escaping, jumping and ight behaviours in an
open eld test (Melik et al. 2006;OBrien and Sutherland
2007). Estimates of heritability (h
2
) in open eld behaviours
have been observed in several studies and found to range
from 0.08 to 0.49 for overall locomotion and from 0.06 to 0.10
for defecation (Boyer et al. 1970; Faure 1981; Webster and
Hurnik 1989). Ambulation in the open eld is also heritable
(Forkman et al. 2007).
As they are more native breeds, bronze turkeys are more
suitable for extensive production methods than white ones
(Camci and Sarica 1991; Cevher and Turkyilmaz 1999; Turkoglu
et al. 2005). For this reason, bronze turkeys are used as stocks in
pasture-based, semi-intensive and extensive turkey farms.
Exogenous factors like feeding, housing and management or
endogenous factors like genetic tendency (Hafez, 1999) have
been propounded to explain the extremely aggressive beha-
viours of domesticated turkeys under commercial rearing con-
ditions (Buchwalder and Huber-Eicher 2004). Fast-growing
strains of turkeys such as B.U.T. BIG 6 are usually housed in
large farms at stocking densities of up to 60 kg/m
2
(FAWC 1995).
There is a huge dierence between white and bronze
turkeys in terms of line weight. This aects their behavioural
responses in a free-range system. It may be speculated or
hypothesized that bronze turkeys feel themselves more comfor-
table than white ones since they have got bigger body sizes.
Unlike chickens and quails, turkeys have been under
researched in terms of their welfare (Marchewka et al. 2013).
There is not sucient research as well to compare the beha-
viours and welfare of white and bronze turkey hens. As
female turkeys are always in large numbers in breeding ocks,
we felt the need to investigate their behavioural responses in
a free-range housing condition. The present study, therefore,
aimed at comparing white and bronze turkey hens in terms
of their responses to TI, GS and OF tests. Comparisons include
the advantage or disadvantage of body weight.
2. Material and methods
2.1. Animals and experimental design
The study was carried out with 144 white (72) and bronze (72)
turkey pullets. They were 32 weeks old and each of them was
designated with a numbered leg ring. It was conducted at the
Poultry Unit of Agricultural Faculty of Ahi Evran University in
Kirsehir province (39° 80′′ N, 34° 100′′ E) of Turkey. Meteoro-
logical data in experimental unit were collected by using data
loggers (HOBO, Onset, Pocasset, MA). Outdoor climate data
were obtained from the Kirsehir Meteorological Oce. Environ-
mental conditions of the free-range system are shown in Table
1. According to the table, average indoor temperature was
between 17.9 °C and 22.5 °C during the 32nd and 55th weeks.
Indoor relative humidity ranged between 57.1 and 69.9%.
In the free-range system, stocking density was 2 birds/m
2
indoors and 0.66 birds/m
2
outdoors. Feed and water were pro-
vided ad libitum indoors. The pens (indoors) were 2.0 × 6.00 m
in size and bedded with fresh wood shavings. Possible inci-
dences of mortality and any other abnormalities were observed
daily. Both animal genotypes were fed on a diet containing 16%
crude protein and 11.7 ME MJ/kg.). The pullets were housed in
pens sized 2 × 6x2 m (height x length x width) indoors and
having free access to the open eld area sized 2 × 18 × 2 m
(height x length x width). In open areas, the land is used in its
natural state. In this study, round type plastic feeders and drin-
kers were used. All the birds were weighed on the 32nd, 35th,
48th and 55th weeks. For determining live weight changes of
turkeys and collecting gait and open eld test scores, all
animals were used while only 12 animals from each genotype
were used for TI, in order not to distress all animals.
2.2. Tonic immobility test
A tonic immobility (TI) test was conducted in the 32nd and 55th
week according to the modied methods described by Noble
et al. (1996). To measure TI, turkeys were gently taken from
their pens at random and tested individually only once in a sep-
arate room isolated from other environmental events. Within a
few seconds after the bird was caught, it was laid on a at stand
with fabric lamina. The observer restrained the turkey on its left
side by placing his left hand over its right wing and tenderly
grasped its legs with his right hand. He gradually kept his
hands othe turkey nearly 15 s later. The duration of its
laying position was counted in seconds with a chronometer.
The turkey was observed from 1-m distance.).
The percentage of one induction (OI, percentage of animals
exhibiting tonic immobility reaction in the rst implementation
of the test), vocalizations (V, percentage of sound-making
during the test), defecations (D, percentage of animal feces
during the test), full durations (FD, staying 600 s without stand-
ing up) and tonic immobility duration (TID, standing up willingly
in 600 s without any enforcement) were registered in this study.
These measurements were taken as values of behaviours exhib-
ited by each animal (totally, twelve birds for each genotype). As
the animals exhibited multiple behaviours, each behaviour was
calculated in percentile values.
2.3. Open eld test
For open eld test, each turkey was singly transferred to an
empty compartment bordering to the compartment in which
they were housed for testing. The open eld site consisted of
a square land (3 m x 3 m). The testing site was covered by a
black polycarbonate plate (1.50 m height). Blue plastic strips
were used to form a net of 100 squares (each 0.09 m
2
) on the
1254 A. TASKIN ET AL.
ground. The birds were put in the centre of the site for 10 min
and their behaviours were observed. The behaviours of stand-
ing, sitting, ambulation, vocalization, defecation, and escape
were recorded. Since animals showed multiple behaviours,
each behaviour was calculated in % values. The ethogram is
described in Table 2.
2.4. Gait scores
The scoring system was established by using the system
demonstrated by Ferket et al. (2009) with scores systematically
arranged as following: 0 = no noticeable leg irregularities, 1 =
mild lameness or wobbly leg, 2 = signicant lameness and 3 =
lame and lacking ability to move. Gait scores were determined
using a scale ranging from 0 to 3. Gait scoring included the birds
at the age of 32nd and 55th weeks. Their walking abilities were
individually assessed by two estimators while they were
walking within the pen. Each estimator scored the birds freely
and an average score was computed in the sequel.
2.5. Statistical analysis
Bartletts and Levenes tests were used to examine homogen-
eity of variance, and AndersonDarling and Kolmogorov
Smirnov tests were used to examine normal distribution.
These tests showed that the assumption of normality was met
for the distribution of live weights. The repeated measures
design was used, therefore, as a parametric statistical test. It
was based on a two-factor experimental design in which one
of the factors involved repeated measurement of levels. The
turkeys were divided into groups on the basis of live weights
and measurement times. Live weights were measured at ve
dierent times.
In what follows, y
ijm
: m refers to the measurement value
obtained from the experimental unit at the i
th
level of factor
A (group) and the j
th
level of factor B (time). Considering the var-
iance elements that could aect this measurement value, the
following linear model was created (Gurbuz et al. 1999).
yijm :
m
+
a
i+
p
m(i)+
b
j+
ab
ij +
bp
jm(i)+1
µ: Overall mean value obtained from turkeys,
α
i
: The eect of the ith level of live weight,
π
m(i)
: The random eect of the mth experimental unit with a live
weight of i,
β
j
: The eect of the jth level of time,
αβ
ij
: The eect of the interaction between live weight and time,
βπ
jm(i)
: The interaction between time and the experimental unit
at the ith level of live weight,
ε
l(ijm)
: The eect of random error.
To identify which group or groups were responsible for the
inter-group dierences found, we used Duncans multiple com-
parison test (Gomez and Gomez 1984).
To see whether pretest and posttest measurements of inde-
pendent variables, such as tonic immobility, gait score and open
eld test results diered from each other, the Mann Whitney U
Test, which is a non-parametric statistical test, was used
(Gamgam and Altunkaynak 2013). All data collected were sub-
jected to Analysis by the Statistical Analysis System Institute
(SAS, 1999).
3. Results and discussion
3.1. Live weight changes
Weekly body weights of the turkey hens are given in Figure 1.As
expected, there was a signicant dierence between white and
bronze turkeys (P< .05). The variance analysis was carried out for
bronze and white turkeys in accordance with the multiple com-
parison test; the average body weight changed according to the
week (P< .05). In white turkeys, the average live weight was
Table 1. Climate data of indoors and outdoors.
Months
Indoors Outdoors
Temperature (
o
C) Relative humidity (%) Temperature (
o
C) Relative humidity % Areal rains (mm) Max. Wind (m/s) Sunbathe duration (hour)
April 18.1 66.3 13.0 58.1 20.2 19.2 247.7
May 19.6 68.9 16.4 60.8 46.6 20.7 266.7
Jun 19.6 65.4 20.0 53.6 36.0 18.1 294.9
July 22.4 57.1 26.3 38.4 13.0 18.8 362.7
August 22.5 59.4 26.8 39.4 17.0 23.6 339.5
September 19.6 63.1 19.9 51.2 30.4 16.4 261.0
October 17.9 69.9 13.5 67.0 31.6 14.6 218.6
Table 2. Description of the behaviours of turkeys.
Behaviours Description
Standing (ST) Standing, feet or legs, but not belly, on the oor
Sitting (SI) Sitting with breast and belly on the oor
Ambulation (A) Two or more treads in swift progression.
Flying (F) Flapping wings, no contact with oor
Vocalization (V) Production of sounds by birds
Defecation (D) Defecating of the animals during the test
Escape (E) Endeavoring to leap out of the test stage
Figure 1. The changes in body weight (kg) of white and bronze turkeys during 32
55 weeks (n= 72). Capital letters show signicant dierences between genotypes
while small letters show signicant dierences between ages at a signicance level
of P< .05.
JOURNAL OF APPLIED ANIMAL RESEARCH 1255
13.85 kg in week 32, and 12.83 kg in week 55. In bronze turkeys,
on the other hand, the initial weight was 6.11 kg, and the average
weight in the nal period was 5.65 kg. However, these dier-
ences were not found to be statistically signicant within each
genotype. Considering the average rearing period, the average
weights of the white turkeys were 13.17 kg while those of the
bronze turkeys were 5.89 kg. This dierence between the geno-
types was found to be statistically signicant. Their live weights
also are of signicance when they are compared according to the
weeks (P< .05).
3.2. Tonic immobility test
Some physiological changes observed throughout TI suggest
that the autonomic nervous system is powerfully involved in
this process (Alboni et al. 2008). Values for TI (OI, V, IFD and
D) at 32 and 55 weeks of age in turkeys reared in a free-range
system are presented in Figure 2.
In the rst measurement (week 32), white turkeys66.66%
showed OI behaviour (P< .05), 33.33% V behaviour (P< .01),
25% IFD behaviour (P< .05), and 75% D behaviour (P< .01). In
the last measurement (week 55), these birds58.83% exhibited
OI behaviour (P> .05), 41.66% V behaviour (P< .01), 16.66%
IFD behaviour, and 58.33% D behaviour. The signicant
changes (P< .05) were observed in these behaviours. Of the
bronze turkeys, 66.66% exhibited OI behaviour in the rst
measurement, 75% V behaviour, 41.66% IFD behaviour, and
91.66% D behaviour. In the last measurement, on the other
hand, 50% of them exhibited OI behaviour, 58.33% V behaviour,
25% IFD behaviour, and 83.33% D behaviour. In bronze turkeys,
also, the signicant decreases were observed in these beha-
viours (P< .05). In terms of vocalization behaviour, they
showed less reaction than white turkeys. This could be a geno-
type-related characteristic. In this case, the white turkeys with
an increasing age showed more vocalization behaviour during
the TI test (when incurring the fear condition) compared to
the bronze ones. The signicance (P< .05) regarding those
properties (OI, V, IFD and D) when the genotypes are compared
in general and in weeks indicate its relevance to the genotypic
dierences.
Initial and nal TI durations are shown in Figure 3.A signi-
cant dierence was observed between the white and bronze
turkeys in terms of TI durations. The bronze ones showed a sig-
nicantly shorter duration than the white ones for 55th week.
Considering TI behaviours, signicantly lower TID (about 61 s)
(P< .01) was observed in the white turkeys in the 32nd week.
However, TID of bronze turkeys was signicantly lower (approxi-
mately 33 s) (P< .05) than the white ones in the 55th week. A
signicant decrease in TID of both genotypes indicates that
they were accustomed to free-range system. Signicant
decreases (P< .05) in TI in both genotypes from the 32nd to
the 55th week may point to the positive eects of free-range
system on animal welfare.
Bronze turkeys show less fear reactions, which can be inter-
preted as being less aected from free-range conditions. Fear
reactions of turkeys are moderately correlated between days
and weeks (Erasmus and Swanson 2014). Our study is in agree-
ment with these ndings. At 32-week old, the bronze turkeys
showed more fear responses than the whites, but the older
bronze turkeys lost this state of fear when compared to the
white ones (P< .01). This might be attributed to the fact that
white turkey is more domesticated than bronze one.
However, it can be speculated that bronze turkeys are accus-
tomed to handling at 55-week old. That was why they
showed less fear. TI is used as a criterion to appraise fear (Villa-
gra et al. 2011). A long duration of TI is mostly considered to be
a sign of high levels of fearfulness (Reese et al. 1984). TI is invo-
luntary and a reexive state characterized by physical immo-
bility, muscular rigidity, and suppressed vocal behaviour when
confronted with inevitable and fear-inducing situations (Marx
et al. 2008).
3.3. Open eld test
Initial and nal behavioural elements (ST, S, A, V, E, and D) of the
bronze and white turkeys in the open eld test are shown in
Figure 2. Behaviours of female turkeys (mean ± SE) during tonic immobility test
change, (n= 12). OI (One induction); V (Vocalization); IFD (Immobile for full dur-
ation); D (Defecation). Capital letters show signicant dierences between geno-
types while small letters show signicant dierences between ages at a
signicance level of P< .05.
Figure 3. The average tonic immobility durations (s) of female turkeys (n= 12). t=
5.16 for white, t= 8.91 for bronze in 3255 weeks, and t=4.5 for between white
and bronze in 32 week, t= 2.83 for between white and bronze in 55 week.
Capital letters show signicant dierences between genotypes while small
letters show signicant dierences between ages at a signicance level of P< .05.
1256 A. TASKIN ET AL.
Figure 4.ST and SI values were found to be statistically non-
signicant in the white turkeys. A, V, E and D were found signi-
cant (P< .05). As for the bronze ones, A, V and D values were
found nonsignicant while ST, S and E values were found to
be signicant (P< .05).
In the open eld test, 76.38% of the white turkeys exhibited
ST behaviour in the rst test (week 32), 11.11% SI behaviour,
9.72% A behaviour, 27.77% V behaviour, 11.11% E behaviour,
and 77.77% D behaviour. Conversely, in the last test (week
55), 79.41% of them displayed ST behaviour, 14.7% SI behav-
iour, 5.88% A behaviour, 44.11% V behaviour, 7.35% E behav-
iour, and 66.17% D behaviour. The dierence between ST and
SI behaviours was not statistically signicant; however, there
was a signicant increase in V behaviour over time (P< .01)
while the signicant decrease was observed (P< .05) in the
other behaviours in open eld test. In the rst measurement,
76.38% of the bronze turkeys exhibited ST behaviour, with
23.61% SE behaviour, 13.88% A behaviour, 62.5% V behaviour,
19.44% E behaviour, and 69.44% D behaviour. In the last
measurement, however, 85.71% of them exhibited ST behav-
iour, 14.28% SI behaviour, 12.85% A behaviour, 57.14% V behav-
iour, 34.28% E behaviour, and 68,57% D behaviour. As for the
bronze turkeys, dierences between ST, SI and E behaviours
were found to be both large and signicant (P< .01);
nevertheless, no signicant dierences were observed in
terms of A, V and D behaviours (P> .05). The open eld test
scores of turkeys diered in terms of both weeks and geno-
types. Both genotypes scored dierently in the last week in pro-
portion to the rst week. As it can be seen in the groupings of
turkeys according to the multiple comparison tests (Figure 4),
those dierences are statistically signicant (P< .05), which
shows that the reactions of both genotypes changed over
time. Positive changes are important in terms of rearing. The
most striking aspect here was the noise the turkeys made
when they got outdoors. The bronze genotypes made more
noise than the white ones did, possibly due to the fact that
the former ones genetically have a more erce temperament.
These dierences between white and bronze turkeys are
thought to stem from the relevant genotypes. In a study on
enriched and non-enriched pens, there was no treatment
eect on latency for birds to move out of the starting square
in which they were placed, and no eect on the frequency of
escape attempts or number of defecations (Hartcher et al.
2015).
3.4. Gait scores
Gait scores (%) of the bronze and white turkeys at the begin-
ning and the end of the study (GS0, GS1, GS2 and GS3) are
shown in Figure 5.Those scores (%) were signicant (P< .05)
in the 32nd and 55th weeks.
In the rst measurement (week 32), 62.5% of the white
turkeys displayed GS0, with 34.94% GS1, 5.56% GS2, and 0%
GS3. In the last measurement (week 55), however, 55.88% of
them displayed GS0, with 19.11% GS1, 13.24% GS2, and
11.77% GS3. Dierences between behaviours were found to
be large and signicant (P< .01; P< .05 for GS0). In the bronze
genotypes, 74.99% displayed GS0 in the rst measurement,
20% displayed GS1, 5.71% displayed GS2, and 0% displayed
GS3. In the last measurement, on the other hand, 85.71% dis-
played GS0, 11.43% displayed GS1, 1.43% displayed GS2, and
1.34% displayed GS3. Dierences between the gait scores of
bronze turkeys were found to be large and signicant (P< .01;
P< .05 for GS0). This indicates that the free-range system has
eventually generated a positive impact on the gaits of bronze
turkeys. In terms of gait scoring, the turkeys diered signi-
cantly in GS0, GS1, GS2, GS3 according to their genotypes.
The fact that GS0 was higher among the bronze genotypes
show that they did not have walking diculties. GS1, GS2 and
GS3 were also statistically lower among them, which indicates
that the bronze turkeys can reap the benets of free-range
systems more actively than the white ones can. It needs to be
further elaborated as this property also makes it possible to
use feeding and housing in a more ecient way.
Exercises in free-range systems increase the metabolic
activity and circulation, which may contribute to reducing the
animals stress (Kjaer 2004). It has been seen that there are
fewer incidences of lameness in free-range chickens (Kestin
et al. 1992). The increasing age negatively aected the gait of
white turkeys, on the other hand. This eect stems from the
high body weights depending on the genotype. Gait scores
of the bronze turkeys improved in general. Examination of
the heavier B.U.T. T9 and Big 6 strains revealed incidences of
Figure 4. Open eld scores (%) of female turkey (n= 72). ST (Standing); SI (Sitting);
A (Ambulation); V (Vocalization); E (Escape); D (Defecation). Capital letters show
signicant dierences between genotypes while small letters show signicant
dierences between ages at a signicance level of P< .05.
Figure 5. Gait scores (%) of female turkey (n= 72). Capital letters show signicant
dierences between genotypes while small letters show signicant dierences
between ages at a signicance level of P< .05.
JOURNAL OF APPLIED ANIMAL RESEARCH 1257
tibial dyschondroplasia with 88.2% and 90.5%, respectively
(Reinmann 1999). The white turkeys were heavier than the
bronze ones. At sexual development (2734 weeks), all female
turkeys of a heavy breeding line exhibit cartilage lesions
(Hocking and Lynch 1991). This situation (genetic predisposi-
tion) can account for the worse gait scores of the white
turkeys as well. Keeping commercial breeds under free-range
conditions reduced, but did not eliminate lameness (Kestin
et al. 1992). In some cases, the frequency of leg disorders can
be high and are connected to elevated rates of mortality
(Sanotra et al. 2002). In the present study, mortality rates were
found higher among the white turkeys. The white turkeys
showed a 6-percent mortality while that of the bronze ones
was 3%. The results may be related to these rates.
4. Conclusions
The bronze female turkeys showed lower mortality compared
to the white turkeys. The bronze ones responded with more
natural behaviours to TI and OF tests, albeit not to GS. The
results suggest that bronze turkeys are more comfortable
than white ones in free-range systems. Considering their behav-
ioural responses to ambient conditions, bronze turkeys may be
more suitable than white turkeys for rearing in free-range
systems.
Disclosure statement
No potential conict of interest was reported by the authors.
Funding
This study was supported by the Scientic Research Projects Coordinator-
ship of Ahi Evran University in Turkey with AEU-PYO-ZRT.4001.12.016
project number.
ORCID
Atilla Taskin http://orcid.org/0000-0001-5897-2062
References
Alawneh JI, Laven RA, Stevenson MA. 2012. Interval between detection of
lameness by locomotion scoring and treatment for lameness: a survival
analysis. Vet J. 193(3):622625.
Alboni P, Alboni M, Bertorelle G. 2008. The origin of vasovagal syncope: to
protect the heart or to escape predation? Clin Auton Res. 18:170178.
Aydin A, Cangar O, Ozcan SE, Bahr C, Berckmans D. 2010. Application of a
fully automatic analysis tool to assess the activity of broiler chickens
with dierent gait scores. Comp Elect Agri. 73:194199.
Balog JM, Bayyari BR, Rath NC, HuWE, Anthony NB. 1997.Eect of intermit-
tent activity on broiler production parameters. Poult Sci. 76:612.
Belviranli M, Atalik KEN, Okudan N, Gokbel H. 2012. Age, sex and anxiety
aect activity in rats. In Spink AJ, Grieco F, Krips OE, Loijens LWS,
Noldus LPJJ, Zimmerman PH. Proceedings of Measuring Behavior.
Utrecht, The Netherlands. p. 500503.
Boyer JP, Melin JM, Ferre R. 1970.Dierences genetiques de comportement
exploratoire experimental chez le poussin. Premiers resultats.
Proceedings 14th Worlds Poultry Congress, Madrid. WPSA, Madrid,
2125.
Buchwalder T, HuberEicher B. 2004.Eect of increased oor space on
aggressive behaviour in male turkeys (Meleagris gallopavo). Appl Anim
Behav Sci. 89:207214.
Camci O, Sarica M. 1991. Intensive rearing of turkeys. Tigem J. 36:519.
Candland DK, Nagy ZM. 1969. The open eld: some comparative data. Ann
NY Acad Sci. 159:831851.
Cevher Y, Turkyilmaz MK. 1999. Turkey meat and its importance in Turkey. J
Turkish Veter Med Soci. 70:34.
De Haas EN, Kemp B, Bolhuis JE, Groothuis T, Rodenburg TB. 2013. Fear,
stress, and feather pecking in commercial white and brown laying hen
parentstock ocks and their relationships with production parameters.
Poult Sci. 92:22592269.
De Haas EN, Nielsen BL, Buitenhuis AJ, Rodenburg TB. 2010. Selection on
feather pecking aects response to novelty and foraging behaviour in
laying hens. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 124:9096.
Edelaar P, Serrano D, Carrete M, Blas J, Potti J, Tella JL. 2012. Tonic immobility
is a measure of boldness toward predators: an application of Bayesian
structural equation modeling. Behav Ecol. 23:619626.
El-Hack MEA, Alagawany M, Farag MR, Tiwari R, Karthik K, Dhama K,
Zorriehzahra J, Adel M. 2016. Benecial impacts of thymol essential oil
on health and production of animals, sh and poultry: a review. J
Essent Oil Res. 28(5):365382.
Erasmus M, Swanson J. 2014. Temperamental turkeys: Reliability of behav-
ioural responses to four tests of fear. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 157:100108.
Farm Animal Welfare Council. 1995. Report on the Welfare of turkeys.
Tolworth, FAWC.
Faure JM. 1981. Bidirectional selection for openeld activity in young
chicks. Behav Genet. 11:135144.
Ferket PR, OviedoRondon EO, Mente PL, Bohorquez DV, SantosJrAA, Grimes
JL, Richards JD, Dibner JJ, Felts V. 2009. Organic trace minerals and 25
hydroxycholecalciferol aect performance characteristics, leg abnormal-
ities, and biomechanical properties of leg bones of turkeys. Poult Sci.
88:118131.
Forkman B, Boissy A, MaunierSalaun MC, Canali E, Jones RB. 2007. A critical
review of fear tests used on cattle, pigs, sheep, poultry and horses.
Physiol Behav. 92:340374.
Gallup GG, Rosen TS, Brown CW. 1972.Eect of conditioned fear on tonic
immobility in domestic chickens. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 78:2225.
Gamgam H, Altunkaynak B. 2013. Parametrik Olmayan Yontemler SPSS
Uygulamalı. Ankara, Turkey: Gazi Kitabevi.
Gomez AK, Gomez AA. 1984. Statistical procedure for agricultural research.
2nd ed. New York: Wiley.
Gurbuz F, Baspinar E, Keskin S, Mendes M, Tekindal B. 1999. Path analysis
technique. 4. National Biostatistics Meeting, 2324 September 1999,
Ankara.
Hafez HM. 1999. Gesundheitsstörungen bei Puten im Hinblick auf die
tierschutzrelevanten und wirtschaftlichen Gesichtspunkte. Arch.
Geügelk. 63:7376.
Hartcher KM, Trana MKTN, Wilkinsona SJ, Hemsworthc PH, Thomsona PC,
Cronina GM. 2015. Plumage damage in free-range laying hens: behav-
ioural characteristics in the rearing period and the eects of environ-
mental enrichment and beak-trimming. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 164:6472.
Hocking P, Lynch M. 1991. Histopathology of antitrochanteric degeneration
in adult female turkeys of four strains of dierent mature size. Res Veter
Sci. 51:327331.
Kestin SC, Knowles TG, Tinch AE, Gregory NG. 1992. Prevalence of leg weak-
ness in broiler chickens and its relationship with genotype. Veter Rec.
131:190194.
Kjaer M. 2004. Role of extracellular matrix in adaptation of tendon and skel-
etal muscle to mechanical loading. Physiol Rev. 84:649698.
Konca Y, Ozkan S, Cabuk M, Yalcin S. 2004. The eect of intermittent feeding
on the performance and stress related parameters of turkey toms. J Ege
Univ Fac Agri. 41:133143.
Koolhaas JM, Korte SM, De Boer SF, Van Der Vegt BJ, Van Reenen CG, Hopster
H, De Jong IC, Ruis MAW, Blokhuis HJ. 1999. Coping styles in animals:
current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci Biobehav
Rev. 23:925935.
Lalonde R, Strazielle C. 2008. Relations between openeld, elevated plus
maze, and emergence tests as displayed by C57/BL6J and BALB/c mice.
J Neurosci Meth. 171:4852.
Leach KA, Tisdall DA, Bell NJ, Main DCJ, Green LE. 2012. The eects of early
treatment for hindlimb lameness in dairy cows on four commercial UK
farms. Vet Jour. 193:626632.
1258 A. TASKIN ET AL.
Marchewka J, Watanabe TTN, Ferrante V, Estevez I. 2013. Review of the social
and environmental factors aecting the behavior and welfare of turkeys
(Meleagris gallopavo). Poult Sci. 92:14671473.
Marx BP, Forsyth JP, Gallup GG, Fuse T, Lexington JM. 2008. Tonic immobility
as an evolved predator defense: implications for sexual assault survivors.
Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 15:7490.
Maurice DV, Jones JE, Lightsey SF, Rhoades JF. 1990. Response of male
poults to high levels of dietary niacinamide. Poult Sci. 69:661668.
Melik E, Babar E, Ozen E, Ozgunen T. 2006. Hypofunction of the dorsal hip-
pocampal NMDA receptors impairs retrieval of memory to partially pre-
sented foreground context in a singletrial fear conditioning in rats.
Eur Psychopharmacol. 16:241247.
Moller AP, Szép T. 2011. The role of parasites in ecology and evolution of
migration and migratory connectivity. J Ornithol. 152:141150.
Noble DO, Krueger KK, Nestor KE. 1996. The eect of altering feed and water
location and of activity on growth, performance, behavior, and walking
ability of hens from two strains of commercial turkeys. Poult Sci.
75:833837.
Obrien J, Sutherland RJ. 2007. Evidence for episodic memory in a Pavlovian
conditioning procedure in rats. Hippocampus. 17:11491152.
Reed HJ, Wilkins LJ, Austin SD, Gregory NG. 1993. The eect of environ-
mental enrichment during rearing on fear reactions and depopulation
trauma in adult caged hens. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 36:3946.
Reese WG, Angel C, Newton JE. 1984. Immobility reactions. a modied
classication. Pavlov J Biol Sci. 19:137143.
Reinmann M. 1999. Probleme in der Putenhaltung am Beispiel der tibialen
Dyschondroplasie eine kleine Chronologie. DGVReferatesammlung,
56. Fachgespräch, Hannover.
Sanotra GS, Lund JD, Vestergaard KS. 2002.Inuence of lightdark schedules
and stocking density on behaviour, risk of leg problems and occurrence
of chronic fear in broilers. Br Poult Sci. 43:344354.
Sarica M, Yamak US. 2010. Developing slow growing meat chickens and
their properties. Anadolu J Agric Sci. 25:6167.
SAS Institute . 1999. SAS/GRAPH Software: Reference, Version 8. Cary, NC:
SAS Institute Inc.
Schutz KE, Kerje S, Jacobsson L, Forkman B, Carlborg O, Andersson L, Jensen
P. 2004. Major growth QTLs in fowl are related to fearful behavior: poss-
ible genetic links between fear responses and production traits in a red
junglefowl × white leghorn intercross. Behav Genet. 34:121130.
Taskin A. 2009. The eects of aromatic plants on broiler meat quality and
tonic immobility reaction [Ph.D thesis]. Hatay, Turkey: University of
Mustafa Kemal.
Thiele HH, Pottgüter R. 2008. Management recommendations for laying hens
in deep litter, perchery and free range systems. Lohmann Inf. 43(1):4353.
Turkoglu M, Sarica M. 2009. Poultry science breeding, nutrition, diseases. 3rd
ed. Ankara: Bey Ofset Printing Company.
Turkoglu M, Sarica M, Eleroglu H. 2005. Turkey production. Uğurer Tarım
Kitapları. Samsun: Otak Form Ofset.
Tuyttens F, Heyndrickx M, De Boeck M, Moreels A, Van Nuel A, Van Poucke
E, Van Coillie E, Van Dongen S, Lens L. 2008. Broiler chicken health,
welfare and uctuating asymmetry in organic versus conventional pro-
duction systems. Livest Sci. 113:123132.
Villagra A, Olivas I, Benitez V, Lainez M. 2011. Evaluation of sludge from
paper recycling as bedding material for broilers. Poult Sci. 90:953957.
Wahlsten D, Metten P, Crabbe JC. 2003. Survey of 21 inbred mouse strains in
two laboratories reveals that BTBR T/+ tf/tf has severely reduced hippo-
campal commissure and absent corpus callosum. Brain Res. 971:4754.
Webster AB, Fairchild BD, Cummings TS, Stayer PA. 2008. Validation of a
threepoint gaitscoring system for eld assessment of walking ability
of commercial broilers. J Appl Poult Res. 17:529539.
Webster AB, Hurnik JF. 1989. Genetic assessment of the behavior of White
Leghorn type pullets in an open eld. Poult Sci. 68:335343.
JOURNAL OF APPLIED ANIMAL RESEARCH 1259
... Erasmus and Swanson (2014) also observed that number of vocalisations is not a reliable component in open field test. Taskin et al. (2018) examined the behavioural responses of white and bronze turkeys using tonic immobility and open field tests and concluded that white turkeys are less fearful than bronze turkeys based on the two fear tests. Durosaro et al. (2021) examined the association between plumage colour and fear behaviour in young Nigerian indigenous turkeys using five fear tests (tonic immobility, emergence, open field, inversion and attention bias) and concluded that lavender turkeys are the most fearful compared with black and white turkeys. ...
... Our results on tonic immobility are also similar to result obtained by Noble et al. (1996) who reported highest tonic immobility duration in an F line turkey selected over 28 generations for increased body weight at 16 weeks of age compared with lowest tonic immobility duration observed in randombred turkeys developed in 1966 and 1986. Our results on tonic immobility also agree with the report of Taskin et al. (2018) who observed differences in tonic immobility duration between white and bronze turkey genotypes. ...
... So, more time spent vocalizing, a higher number of escape attempts and a higher percentage of floor squares visited by NIT indicate a pronounced exploration (Campbell et al., 2019) and less fearfulness (Campler et al., 2009). Our results from the open field behaviour of the birds are in agreement with the report of Taskin et al. (2018) who also observed differences in open field behaviours between white and bronze turkey genotypes. ...
Article
Characterisation of fear response in turkey breeds could bring important beneficial information for future breeding, animal welfare and preservation of breeds. The two major turkey breeds raised presently in Nigeria are Hybrid Converter turkey (HCT) and Nigerian indigenous turkey (NIT). The HCT has been selected for fast growth and can adapt to different environmental and management conditions. In contrast, NIT is an unselected slow-growing bird that is hardy and well-adapted to the tropical climate of Nigeria. This study examined fear behaviour in turkey poults of fast and slow growing breeds to know how genetic selection for fast growth has shaped fear response in young turkeys. Twenty-five each of white NIT and HCT poults were used for this study. Fear tests such as tonic immobility, emergence, open field, inversion and attention bias tests were used to assess fear responses in the turkey poults during the first 16 days of life. A Wilcoxon Two-Sample test was used to determine the effect of the turkey breed on the poults’ fear responses. There was a significant breed effect on the duration of tonic immobility (Ws=482.00, z = −3.0138, p = 0.003) and latency to emerge from a dark box (Ws=382.00, z = −4.9521, p = 0.000). The HCT stayed in tonic immobility longer than NIT, while NIT emerged faster than HCT from the dark box. There was also a significant breed effect on vocalization duration (Ws=664.50, z = 3.65, p = 0.000) in the open field arena, with longer duration of vocalization in NIT compared with HCT. More escape attempts (Ws=598.00, z = 3.05, p = 0.004) and more explored floor squares (Ws=606.50, z = 2.43, p = 0.015) were observed in NIT compared with HCT during the open field test. The results obtained from this study showed significant breed differences in the fear responses of the turkey poults, with HCT poults being more fearful than NIT. The increased fear response observed in HCT poults appears to be an unintended consequence of selection for fast growth. Genetic selection programmes aimed at improving growth in turkeys should also consider their fear behaviour.
... Thus, there are reasonable opportunities to further select for growth without additional deterioration of walking ability. Several studies showed a decline in walking ability with the increase in turkeys' age and weight [19][20][21][22]. Kappel et al. [14] reported small unfavorable genetic correlations between body weight and valgus and varus deformities, and gait scores. ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to investigate the potential non-linear relationship between growth and walking ability (WA). The phenotypic data included body weights at 12 and 20 weeks and WA at 20 weeks of age measured on 276,059 male turkeys. The growth rate at three age periods (0 to 12, 12 to 20 and 0 to 20 weeks) was calculated. Each bird was assigned to one of the quartiles of the growth rate distribution for each age period. Between the first and fourth quartiles, the incidence of score 1 (bad WA) increased by 31, 18, and 33% for the first, second, and third age periods, respectively. For good WA (scores 4, 5, and 6), the incidence decreased by 55, 66, and 72% between the first and fourth quartiles for the first, second, and third age periods, respectively. Estimates of heritability of WA ranged between 0.18 and 0.26. The genetic correlations between adjacent growth rate quartiles were high and decayed as the interval between quartiles increased. The magnitude of the variation in the incidence of walking scores and genetic correlations across the growth rate quartiles point towards a non-linear relationship between growth and mobility suggesting other factors may affect walking ability.
... Several studies have documented a decrease in walking ability (worsening gait) as turkeys get older and heavier (e.g. [46,[48][49][50]), and our results are consistent with these reports as we observed a decline in gait with age in all treatment groups. Gait scores worsened in T and C turkeys from 16 wk onward and in turkeys in all other treatment groups at 19 wk; however, gait was not assessed between 16 and 19 wk. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated age-related changes in turkey welfare measures (wounds, feather quality (FQ), feather cleanliness, and footpad condition (FCON)) and walking ability (gait) as influenced by different types of environmental enrichment (EE). Tom turkeys (n = 420) were randomly assigned to: straw bale (S), platform (P), platform + straw bale (PS), pecking block (B), tunnel (T) or control (C; no enrichment) group. Welfare measures and gait were assessed at 8, 12, 16 and 19 wk and analyzed using PROC LOGISTIC with Firth bias-correction. Better wing FQ with age was observed in turkeys in S and T groups. Turkeys in the S group had better wing FQ at 16 (P = 0.028) and 19 wk (P = 0.011) vs. 8 wk. Wing FQ (P = 0.008) was better at 19 vs. 8 wk for T turkeys. FCON worsened over time for turkeys in all treatment groups except for the S group. FCON was worse at 19 vs.8 wk for P (P = 0.024), PS (P = 0.039), B (P = 0.011), T (P = 0.004) and C (P = 0.014) turkeys and was worse at 19 vs. 12 wk for B (P = 0.038), T (P = 0.015) and C (P = 0.045) turkeys. FCON was worse at 19 vs. 16 wk for T (P = 0.007) and C (P = 0.048) turkeys. FCON was also worse at 16 vs. 8 wk for B (P = 0.046) turkeys. Gait worsened with increasing age in all treatment groups. Gait was worse at 19 wk for S (P < 0.001), P (P < 0.001), PS (P < 0.001) and B turkeys (P < 0.001) vs. earlier ages, while gait in T (P < 0.001) and C turkeys (P < 0.001) worsened starting at 16 wk.
... In an indigenous turkey breed, more aggressive behavior has been reported in black-and lavender-feathered individuals when compared to white individuals (86). In line with these results, as aggression can frequently be initiated by a fear-producing stimulus, (87) Frontiers in Veterinary Science frontiersin.org . /fvets. . ...
... In an indigenous turkey breed, more aggressive behavior has been reported in black-and lavender-feathered individuals when compared to white individuals (86). In line with these results, as aggression can frequently be initiated by a fear-producing stimulus, (87) Frontiers in Veterinary Science frontiersin.org . /fvets. . ...
Book
Full-text available
“Early-life programming” describes how exposure to certain conditions during embryonic development or the early post-hatching period can change the normal development process, permanently altering how a bird’s body looks and functions. Early-life programming can affect a bird for the rest of its life, altering its growth performance, tolerance to harsh environmental conditions, resistance to diseases like enteric infection and inflammation, immune function, metabolic disorders, and overall production. Considerable evidence suggests that early-life programming can alter the phenotype and performance of chicks in significant ways by modifying the expression of specific genes. In both the pre- and post-hatching phases, these treatments alter the environment and the diet of the organisms involved. This Research Topic is aimed at collecting papers suitable for improving our understanding of early life programming and its long-term role in minimizing environmental and health challenges. Its goal is also to share our knowledge on nutritional and environmental factors involved in early-life programming in poultry, such as in-ovo feeding, inadequate conditions, nutrient deficiencies, and sexing methods.
... In an indigenous turkey breed, more aggressive behavior has been reported in black-and lavender-feathered individuals when compared to white individuals (86). In line with these results, as aggression can frequently be initiated by a fear-producing stimulus, (87) . /fvets. . ...
Article
Full-text available
Early sex determination methods are not only crucial in the worldwide massive poultry industry, but also for small-holder producers. The profitability of sexing techniques must be accounted for when aiming to boost management, nutrition, and conservation practices in endangered poultry breeds. This becomes pivotal when the local breed dealt with belongs to an understudied species, such as the turkey. So, the main objective of this study is to identify which method combination may report a higher likelihood of successful sex determination in poults across the three-pattern varieties of the Andalusian turkey breed. A total of 84 one to two days old Andalusian turkey poults (42 black, 28 black-roan, and 14 bronze-roan) were evaluated in this study. Sex determination was performed using 15 methods, which included testing external egg metrics and eggshell color, poult morphological appraisal and phaneroptics, and behavioral traits. Possible differences across plumage varieties and the interaction between sex and plumage were observed when external egg quality was measured. Sex determination through behavioral methods in black base feathered (black and black-roan) male sex individuals showed seven times higher sensitivity when compared to the rest of the studied individuals (χ² = 7.14, df = 1, P < 0.01). In contrast, for the black-roan plumage females, the method based on the color of down feathers was approximately four times more sensitive (χ² = 3.95, df = 1, P ≤ 0.05). For the bronze-roan pattern, none of the sexing techniques was reported to efficiently predict sex itself. However, the most proper method combination to determine sex, independent of plumage color, was physical external egg characteristics, the color of down feathers, and behavioral approaches (“English method” and “slap technique”). The specificity values were found to be 49.12, 93.33, and 100%, while the sensitivity values were observed to be 74.64, 91.03, and 100%, which translated into accuracy of 63.10, 92.26, and 100% in black, black-roan, and bronze-roan poults, respectively. Our results suggest that the method combination tested in this study could be considered a highly accurate, simple, and affordable alternative for sex determination in turkeys. This could mean a pivotal advance for small producers of turkeys, as early sex detection can help to plan timely conservational management strategies, which is of prominent importance in the context of endangered poultry breeds.
... If no TI reaction occurred after 5 repeated interventions, the relevant goose was scored as 0. In addition, geese were evaluated in three groups according to their responses for TI stimulation. Of the geese, those that only showed TI reaction, those that showed TI+ sound, and those that TI+ defecation were grouped as non-response (NR), medium-response (MR), and high-response (HR), respectively and they were expressed as percent (Taskin et al., 2018). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The aim of the study is to examine the performance and carcass characteristics and some behavioral parameters of geese reared in the areas created by using some objects for environmental enrichment (EE) purpose. For this purpose, a total of 72 one-day-old goose goslings of both sexes were used. Geese were accommodated collectively for the first four weeks, then they were divided in three groups in 3 repetitions according to the EE applications and these were expressed as control (C), broom (B), and mirror (M). At the end of 14th week, live weight (LW), beak length (BL), wing length (WL), head diameter (HD), foot width (FW), body temperature (BT), warm carcass (WC), and cold carcass (CC) weights as well as warm and cold pH values (WpH&CpH), and tonic immobility duration (TID) of the groups were measured. Responses presented during the TI test were expressed as non-responses (NR), medium responses (MR), and high responses (HR). Among the groups, group M had the lowest value (110.08±10.69 s) in terms of TID (P > 0.05). Differences in terms of NR, MR, and HR were significant (P > 0.05). The differences between the groups were not statistically significant in terms of the other characteristics (P < 0.05). As a result, it was determined that geese reared by using the EE objects were affected by such environments, however, there were differences between the groups in terms of fear and stress parameters without having any negative effects on yield.
Article
The effects of plumage colour on different behaviours have been reported in several avian species, but there are only few studies for Nigerian indigenous turkeys (NIT). Fear is a negative affective state that is indicative of suffering and associated with adverse effects on welfare and productivity of livestock species. This study compared fear behaviours in NIT with different plumage colours during early life (first 16 days of life). Seventy-five birds (25 each of white, black and lavender) were used in this study. The three colour phenotypes were derived from the same genetic line. Tonic immobility, emergence, open field, inversion and attention bias tests were done at 7, 9, 12, 14 and 16 days old, respectively. Data were analysed with Kruskal-Wallis tests. The durations of tonic immobility and freezing during the attention bias test were not significantly associated with plumage colour (χ² =1.7592, df=2, p = 0.42 and χ² =3.9421, df=2, p = 0.14, respectively). The latency to emerge from a dark box was significantly related to plumage colour (χ² =8.9229, df=2, p = 0.01), with the shortest emergence latency observed in black NIT. There was a significant plumage colour effect on the duration of ambulation (χ² =9.6013, df=2, p = 0.01) and a significant effect on the percentage of floor squares explored by the birds in the open field test (χ² =6.1191, df=2, p = 0.05). The least ambulation time was observed in black NIT (p = 0.02). Lavender turkeys explored fewer floor squares compared to black and white NIT (p = 0.04). The frequency of escape attempts was significantly associated with the plumage colour of the birds (χ² =11.6810, df=2, p = 0.00). The highest frequency of escape attempts during the open field test was observed in black birds, while there was no significant difference in the frequency of escape attempts of white and lavender birds. There were no plumage colour effects on the duration of preening (χ² =1.5896, df=2, p = 0.45), wall pecking (χ² =3.3346, df=2, p = 0.19) and floor pecking (χ² =1.7483, df=2, p = 0.42) of NIT in the open field test. The results from this study applied to the early life stage. It can be concluded that plumage colour influenced the level of fear in young NIT, with lavender turkeys being the most fearful in three out of the four statistically significant fear indicators (longer emergence duration, lower number of escape attempts and lower percentage of floor squares explored in an open field test), followed by white, whereas black turkeys were least fearful.
Article
Full-text available
The thymol is an herbal food additive used to improve performance indices, feed utilization, immune system, and preventing from infectious diseases. Feeding animal, fish and poultry with diets supplemented with medicinal plants containing effective compounds and natural antioxidants in thymol demonstrated valuable potential of thymol to enhance the productive and reproductive performances, nutrient bioavailability, immunity and general health of livestock as well as lowering the problems of different animal diseases, side effects of chemical drugs, syntactic materials and cancer. These properties may be attributed to thymol’s capability to work as antispasmodic, antioxidant, antimicrobial, immunomodulatory, anticancer and anti-inflammatory agent by suppressing harmful compounds/free radicals from interacting with cellular biological compounds, ability to alter the gut microbiota, and increasing digestion, absorption and metabolism of nutrients. This review illustrates the multiple beneficial applications and various aspects of thymol including its chemical structure and physical proprieties, metabolism and excretion, mechanisms of action, natural sources and practical applications in animal, fish and poultry nutrition for increasing production, immunity and safeguarding health.
Book
Full-text available
kutulan “parametrik olmayan yöntemler” dersine uygun olarak hazırlanmıştır. Ancak konunun bir çok bilim dalıyla da ilgili olması nedeniyle başta; eğitim, ziraat, tıp, diş hekimliği, eczacılık,… vb. alanlardaki derslerde de kullanılmaktadır. Kitabın, 4. baskısında öne çıkan iki yeni özellik kitabı daha kullanışlı hale getirmiştir. Bunlardan ilki; kitapta yer alan örneklerin hem elde (veri ortamında) hem de SPSS çözümlerine yer verilmesidir. Diğer özellik ise hipotez testlerinde p-değerinin, SPSS çıktılarında yer almasının yanında, ilgili tablolar aracılığıyla yaklaşık olarak bulunmasını da içermesidir. Ayrıca SPSS’in yeni versiyonu ile birlikte gelen parametrik olmayan çoklu karşılaştırma testlerinin uygulamalarına da kitapta yer verilmiştir. Kitapta konuların daha iyi anlaşılması için 100’ü çözümlü 150 örnek olaya yer verilmiştir.
Article
Full-text available
Severe feather-pecking, whereby birds peck at and pull out the feathers of other birds, is one of the greatest welfare concerns and the most prevalent behavioural problem in laying hens. It can be extremely difficult to control, especially in non-cage laying flocks. Despite a multitude of studies on the topic, the principal underlying causes remain unclear and not much is known about why certain birds are affected more than others. Literature suggests that rearing is an important period for the development of behaviours later in life. Although severe feather-pecking is not usually a welfare concern in the rearing period, behavioural tests when performed early in life may be predictive of plumage damage due to severe feather-pecking in adulthood. This experiment aimed to investigate whether behavioural tests during the rearing period could be predictive of plumage damage later in life. Sixteen pens of 50 ISA Brown laying hens were used, with four birds per pen selected at random as focal birds. Focal birds were subjected to behavioural tests during the rearing period including the open-field test, tonic immobility test and tests for a novel food reward. Two treatments, beak-trimming and environmental enrichment, were applied in a 2 × 2 factorial arrangement in rearing. The non-trimmed birds vocalised more (P = 0.02, 91.5 vs. 83.6%) and at louder volumes (P = 0.02, 71.4 vs. 47.0% of vocalisations categorised as loud rather than soft or silent) in the open-field test at 5 weeks of age. There was no difference between treatments in duration of tonic immobility (P = 0.99). Non-trimmed birds exhibited more plumage damage at 43 weeks of age (P < 0.001, 5.2 vs. 72.9% of birds with feather loss or wounds). Ordinal regression with treatments and treatment interactions as fixed effects and pens and blocks (sides of the shed) as random effects indicated no significant associations between behavioural test reactions and plumage damage (all P > 0.1). Thus while beak-trimmed birds made fewer vocalisations in an open-field test and had less plumage damage in adulthood as expected, there is no evidence that reactions to the behavioural tests were predictive of plumage damage. Instead, results indicate that environmental enrichment affected bird behaviour during the rearing period but did not affect plumage damage due to severe feather-pecking later in life. The test responses including more vocalisations in the open-field test, but no difference in the tonic immobility responses, indicate that the differences may be due to motivation for social reinstatement rather than fearfulness.
Article
Full-text available
Little is known about the relationship between welfare traits and production in laying hen parent stock (PS). In commercial laying hens and pure lines, it is known that aspects associated with reduced welfare such as high fear, stress, and feather pecking can have negative effects on production. Because PS hens are housed under different conditions than commercial laying hens, the relationship between welfare traits and production may differ. We therefore studied the fear response to a stationary person (SP) and novel object (NO), basal plasma corticosterone (CORT) and whole-blood serotonin levels (5-HT), and feather damage as a proxy for feather pecking in 10 Dekalb White (DW) and 10 ISA Brown (ISA) commercial PS flocks and related these to production data. Because the relationship between welfare traits and production may differ by genetic origin and group size, we also assessed genotype and group size effects. Dekalb White birds were more fearful of a SP, and had more feather damage and lower 5-HT levels than ISA birds. Genotypes did not differ in CORT. A large group size (n > 5,000) was associated with low feed intake and better feed conversion for ISA flocks. For DW flocks, high fear of the NO was associated with low BW, low egg weight, and low feed intake. For ISA flocks, high fear of the SP was associated with high mortality. For both lines, high CORT was related to low egg weight. This is the first study to associate levels of fear and CORT to production in commercial PS flocks. Management of PS flocks should take into account breed differences, group size effects, and effects of human-bird interactions. Further research is needed to determine the effects of fear, CORT, 5-HT, and feather damage in commercial PS flocks on the development of their offspring.
Article
Full-text available
Monitoring the activity of broilers is a potential way for determining gait score level at commercial farms. In this study, a fully automatic monitoring technique was developed to measure the activity of broiler chickens with different gait score levels. We carried out two experiments in order to assess the relationship between gait scores obtained by human experts and activity levels quantified by an automatic image monitoring system. The chickens were scored for their degree of lameness by experts according to the method of Kestin et al. (1992). For each experiment, a total of 30 birds, on day 32 were selected from a local commercial farm. Five birds were selected in each of six gait score groups (GS0–GS5). The activity levels were obtained for all gait scores by using an automatic image monitoring system. For this purpose, video surveillance images of broilers with six different predefined gait scores were analysed. In both experiments, there was a significant relation between gait score by experts and activity monitored by image analysis. The broilers with gait score 3 (GS3) showed significantly higher activities than the other gait scores in both experiments (P
Article
Full-text available
A 3-point gait-scoring system used to evaluate broiler walking ability in welfare audits of commercial flocks in the United States was compared with the 6-point Kestin system. In 2 university trials, market-age broilers of 2 commercial varieties were gait-scored by 2 observers for each scoring system. Subsamples of birds were rescored and evaluated in latency-to-lie (LTL) tests. Too few birds had significant walking difficulties in these trials to allow for a good comparison of the 2 scoring systems across all the gait score categories, but the data were encouraging despite sampling limitations. There was a significant association between the 2 systems, and both had substantial between-observer agreement. Both scoring systems had significant correlations with LTL, but the variation of LTL was too high to give gait score or LTL much predictive value for each other. In the field observations, 2 teams of observers scored broilers 47 to 61 d of age on 5 commercial farms each. Two pairs of observers on each team scored the same birds, each pair using the 3-point system or the Kestin system. Broilers with walking problems were oversampled to obtain an adequate number in each gait score category. Weighted. statistics showed substantial between-observer agreement in each system but more so in the 3-point system, suggesting that the application of the 3- point system was more consistent between observers. Spearman correlations between 3- point and Kestin scores for individual birds indicated good correspondence between the 2 systems. The simplicity of the 3-point gait-scoring system appears to facilitate between-observer agreement, making it preferable to more complex systems for use in commercial animal welfare audits. The correspondence between the gait-scoring systems validates the 3-point system in light of the 6-point Kestin system.
Article
Open field (OF), tonic immobility (TI), voluntary approach (VA) and novel object (NO) tests are used to assess fear responses, activity levels and coping styles of poultry. Fear tests are also used as part of welfare assessment programs. Little is known about fear responses of turkeys. Specifically, it is unknown whether turkeys’ fear responses are reliable which is important when developing tests for assessing welfare. This study examined the short- (between days) and long-term (between weeks) changes in, and test-retest reliability of, turkeys’ fear responses. Male commercial turkeys were housed in groups of four to six in 16 pens. Turkeys were individually tested in OF (n = 60) and TI (n = 66) tests. VA and NO tests were administered to groups of turkeys in their pens (n = 16). Turkeys were tested three times in each test. The first (Period 1) and second (Period 2) tests were administered on consecutive days between 4 and 6 weeks, and the third test (Period 3) between 8 and 10 weeks. Other than increased sitting and reduced standing during OF testing and more birds approaching and pecking the observer during VA testing (Period 2 vs. 1), frequencies of responses did not differ between test periods. However, test responses (e.g. latencies to ambulate, vocalize, approach and peck) differed between periods depending on the fear test used. All VA test measures differed between periods, whereas most TI test measures did not. Most OF and NO test measures differed between weeks, but not between days. Except for the number of vocalizations (rS = 0.39), most OF test responses were moderately reliable (rS > 0.40). The latency to vocalize (rS ≥ 0.51) and number of vocalizations (rS ≥ 0.59) were the most reliable TI test measures, whereas reliability of TI duration (rS ≤ 0.31) was low. All VA test measures were moderately to highly repeatable between days and weeks (e.g. latency to peck: rS ≥ 0.67). Reliability was lowest for the NO test, where only the latency to peck was moderately repeatable (rS = 0.61). These findings suggest that although some fear responses of turkeys change over time and with repeated testing, most OF, TI and VA responses are reliable between days and weeks. However, few NO test measures were reliable under the conditions of this study. Further research is needed to assess the validity of OF, TI, VA and NO tests for assessing fear responses of turkeys.
Article
Under commercial rearing conditions, domestic turkeys are often aggressive towards “pen-mates”, and this may lead to serious injuries or even death. We hypothesized that restricted space, as a consequence of high stocking density, might prevent the attacked bird from retreating from its opponent to avoid serious consequences from an encounter. This study examines the aggressive response in 10 groups of five familiar turkey toms, comparing behaviour to an unfamiliar conspecific that is introduced to two pens of different sizes. Groups were tested both in a small (2m × 3m) and in a large (6m × 13m) pen for 30min. Aggressive interactions and distances between the newly introduced bird and test-group members, and additionally distances among test-group members, were recorded.In the small pen we observed significantly more aggressive pecks and threats to the introduced bird. In the large pen introduced birds kept a greater distance from the group than in the small pen. However, they did not make use of all of the available floor space in the large pen. The mean number of fights and chases per test was no different between the two pens.We conclude that the aggressive response of the group was reduced with an increase in floor space. To summarise, the present study shows that in small groups of turkeys, an increase in floor space reduces the number of aggressive pecks and threats aimed at an introduced unfamiliar conspecific. Additionally, we found evidence that there might be a critical distance below which retreating from an opponent is not successful in avoiding aggressive encounters.