ArticlePDF Available

Understanding the Role of Negative Emotions in Adult Learning and Achievement: A Social Functional Perspective

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The role of emotions in adult learning and achievement has received increasing attention in recent years. However, much of the emphasis has been on test anxiety, rather than the wider spectrum of negative emotions such as sadness, grief, boredom and anger. This paper reports findings of a qualitative study exploring the experience and functionality of negative emotions at university. Thirty-six academic staff and students from an Australian university were interviewed about emotional responses to a range of learning events. Data analysis was informed by a prototype approach to emotion research. Four categories of discrete negative emotions (anger, sadness, fear, boredom) were considered by teachers and students to be especially salient in learning, with self-conscious emotions (guilt, embarrassment, shame) mentioned by more students than staff. While negative emotions were frequently viewed as detrimental to motivation, performance and learning, they were also construed under some circumstances as beneficial. The findings are discussed in relation to the value of social functional approaches for a better understanding of the diverse roles of negative emotions in learning and achievement.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27; doi:10.3390/bs8020027
www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci
Article
Understanding the Role of Negative Emotions in
Adult Learning and Achievement: A Social
Functional Perspective
Anna D. Rowe
1,
* and Julie Fitness
2
1
Professional and Community Engagement
,
Macquarie University, North Ryde 2109, Australia
2
Psychology Department
,
Macquarie University, North Ryde 2109; Australia; julie.fitness@mq.edu.au
* Correspondence: anna.rowe@mq.edu.au; Tel.: +61-2-9850-1937
Received: 15 January 2018; Accepted: 9 February 2018; Published: 20 February 2018
Abstract: The role of emotions in adult learning and achievement has received increasing attention
in recent years. However, much of the emphasis has been on test anxiety, rather than the wider
spectrum of negative emotions such as sadness, grief, boredom and anger. This paper reports
findings of a qualitative study exploring the experience and functionality of negative emotions at
university. Thirty-six academic staff and students from an Australian university were interviewed
about emotional responses to a range of learning events. Data analysis was informed by a prototype
approach to emotion research. Four categories of discrete negative emotions (anger, sadness, fear,
boredom) were considered by teachers and students to be especially salient in learning, with self-
conscious emotions (guilt, embarrassment, shame) mentioned by more students than staff. While
negative emotions were frequently viewed as detrimental to motivation, performance and learning,
they were also construed under some circumstances as beneficial. The findings are discussed in
relation to the value of social functional approaches for a better understanding of the diverse roles
of negative emotions in learning and achievement.
Keywords: negative emotions; achievement; higher education; perceptions; qualitative research;
functional theory; prototype
1. Introduction
The role of emotions in learning has received increasing empirical and theoretical attention in
recent years [1–3]. However, much of the emphasis has been on test anxiety and (more recently)
achievement emotions, that is, emotions directly related to achievement activities and outcomes [1,4].
Even so, a growing number of researchers also recognize that students experience a range of other
emotions in academic settings including topic (i.e., subject area), epistemic and social emotions [5–7].
Epistemic emotions are experienced in response to the knowledge-generating qualities of cognitive
activities [8] while social emotions arise in response to social concerns, e.g., status, power and
attachment [9]. Discrete negative emotions found to be important in academic contexts include
anxiety, fear, frustration, anger, boredom, sadness, shame, hopelessness, guilt and embarrassment
[1,7,10,11]. Such emotions have been linked to assessment and testing [12,13], receiving grades and
feedback on performance [7,14], teacher behavior [15], personal study [16] and satisfaction with the
learning experience [11]. Understanding the impact of these and other negative emotions is important
for informing developments in practice, as well as promoting strategies for managing such reactions,
which can in turn lead to improved learning outcomes (for a review see [1,2]).
Evidence on the impact of negative emotions within academic environments suggests that they
are detrimental to motivation, performance and learning in many situations, although findings are
variable. For example, test anxiety, the most studied emotion in education, has been found to impact
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 2 of 19
negatively on academic achievement [4] as well as motivate effort to avoid failure [13]. The impact of
other negative emotions on learning and achievement is less well known, although literature is
starting to emerge particularly on boredom [3,13,17–19]. Similarly, there is interest in the roles of fear
in avoidance behavior and in prioritizing information processing under particular circumstances [20],
and the role of anger in undermining motivation and task-irrelevant thinking [13]. In general,
negative emotions are held to be detrimental to the pursuit of achievement goals, investment of effort,
cognitive processes (such as attention and memory), motivation, self-regulation and self-efficacy
[16,18,21–23]. Even so, there are some circumstances in which negative affect can potentially be
adaptive; for example, in motivating students to attain goals and reduce error making or to recover
from a negative performance evaluation [1,2,4,24]. The precise effects of emotions on academic
achievement are thought to depend on the interactions among various mechanisms (e.g.,
achievement goals, cognitive resources, learning strategies), as well as requirements of the task being
undertaken [2,3]. A limitation of much of the available empirical evidence is that it has been
undertaken with school students and therefore remains to be tested with college learners.
The overall aim of the current study was to further explore the perceived consequences of
negative emotions through interviewing teachers and students in a University context. In particular,
we sought to build on prior work on academic emotions [1,3,6,16] and to ascertain the extent to which
students and teachers perceived negative emotions to have potentially useful functions in the context
of learning and teaching.
1.1. Theoretical Perspectives
The guiding theoretical approach to this research is social-functionalist [25]. Social-functionalist
approaches to emotions provide a useful framework for understanding why particular emotions are
likely to be experienced in different contexts such as academic settings. This perspective conceives
emotions as felt responses to appraisals, or interpretations, of events [26,27]. They are relatively short
lasting and can be categorized into discrete emotion families such as fear, joy, sadness, and anger.
Emotions are thought to differ from moods which are generally viewed as more diffuse affective
states (e.g., feeling up or down) with a less identifiable cause [28]. Research on general mood states
theoretically treats all negative and positive discrete emotions as functionally the same, and therefore
does not recognize the diverse roles emotions play. Hence, as Pekrun and Perry, and others have
observed, it is insufficient to simply differentiate between positive and negative states in educational
contexts; the functional differences between diverse types of emotions need to be considered [3].
Discrete emotion categories are often termed basic emotions because they are thought to serve a
primarily evolutionary function, underpinning coping strategies and adaptation [25,29,30].
Specifically, emotions are held to motivate potentially adaptive behaviors that enable individuals to
resolve particular types of interpersonal and intrapersonal difficulties. The experience of emotion
elicits and coordinates changes in cognition, physiology, behavior and subjective experience
[29,31,32]. Anger, for example, is typically elicited in the context of perceived goal obstruction and
motivates attempts to remove the obstruction. The experience of anger includes increased attention
to goals and the actions of others, higher levels of physiological activity (e.g., increased heart rate,
respiration) and potentially, behaviors such as aggression. Emotions thus motivate behaviors such as
approach or withdrawal from the event or situation that has triggered the emotional reaction.
Further, it is individuals’ perceptions (appraisals) of triggering events/situations that determine
which emotions are experienced (sadness, anger, etc.), with around 70% of emotions predictable from
the goal-relevant events that precede them [33].
Within educational contexts, negative emotions are thought to support tasks that require
thinking within the boundaries of externally provided rules (accommodation), while positive
emotions support tasks that depend on exploration and the creation of new ideas beyond given rules
(assimilation) [2]. Piaget’s (1954) notions of assimilation and accommodation—complementary
processes of adaptation through which awareness of the outside world is internalized—provide a
useful theoretical lens for understanding emotions related to completing tasks at university [2].
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 3 of 19
However, this framework does not account for the specific functions of discrete emotions, or address
how emotions resolve other issues such as interpersonal problems.
Pekrun’s control-value theory of achievement addresses the functional mechanisms of discrete
emotions, distinguishing emotions according to three dimensions: valence (positive and negative);
the object focus (activity and outcome) and degree of activation (activating and deactivating) [3,6].
Negative activating emotions include anger, frustration, anxiety, and shame while deactivating
emotions include boredom, sadness, disappointment and hopelessness. The theory posits that two
types of appraisals—control and value—are particularly relevant to achievement emotions. That is,
the extent to which students feel they have control over achievement activities and their outcomes,
as well as the extent to which these activities and outcomes are perceived to be important. Our work
has established that other appraisals are also important, particularly in relation to situations
involving giving and receiving of feedback on assessments, e.g., deservingness, agency and
expectedness [34]. While Pekrun and colleagues acknowledge the importance of social and epistemic
emotions, the control-value theory focuses on academic achievement, with less emphasis on other
areas of learning [1,8]. Further work is thus needed to develop more wholistic theories to account for
the multiple types of emotions in educational contexts. Our study seeks to extend Pekrun’s work by
exploring a broader range of negative emotions and their potential functions across a range of
learning situations, and to compare the perspectives of teachers and students in their emotion
knowledge and understanding.
1.2. Emotion Knowledge (Prototypes and Schemas)
Prototype theory allows for the investigation of specific functions played by discrete emotions,
and is compatible with the empirical findings of basic emotion researchers who conceptualize
emotions as differing along a positive/negative dimension, as well as categories of discrete emotions
(e.g., sadness, anger) [30]. Emotion prototypes are held to be mental representations of emotion
concepts and categories, which are typically organized around the important elements (or exemplars)
of a particular emotion. A set of defining features thus distinguish one emotion concept from other
(sometimes similar) concepts—with separation often considered a matter of degree (i.e., the extent to
which an emotion has characteristics deemed to be typical of that category) [35]. Prototypes are
conceptualized along a hierarchy with superordinate (e.g., emotion), middle (negative emotion), and
subordinate (e.g., sadness, fear, regret) levels.
The findings of prototype studies (e.g., [36,37]) show that peoples’ ‘emotion knowledge’, that is,
the way they understand the “expressions, feelings, and functions of discrete emotions” [30] (p. 44),
has “considerable impact” on their perceptions, expectations and memories of emotion events [38]
(p. 196). Thus, people draw on emotion prototypes to label and interpret their experiences of emotion,
as well as to choose courses of action [39]. A student’s cognitive representations of anger (in response
to perceptions of injustice over a grade) for example, will influence whether and how this emotion is
expressed toward their teacher, and equally, how the teacher’s representation of anger will influence
their behavior towards the student. Emotions schemas (prototypes) therefore play a critical role in
shaping people’s perceptions, expectations, interpretations, memories and behaviors.
Interestingly, there are an extensive number of studies on emotion knowledge in young school
aged children but not for university/college students [40,41]. The prototype approach offers an
alternative to examining scientific accounts of emotions in educational contexts, which while
valuable, do not necessarily shed light on the underlying mechanisms of how people perceive,
interpret, remember and act upon their emotions. Exploring student and faculty (i.e., lay persons’)
theories of emotion, provides a fruitful avenue of investigation of this complex area, with the
usefulness and applicability of prototype theory to the study of emotions and learning/achievement
recently established [34].
1.3. Emotions in Academic Settings
The experiences of students and teachers (including the emotions experienced by each) impact
on the quality of student learning, achievement outcomes and relationships [42,43]. While not an
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 4 of 19
explicit focus in this paper, the nature of the student-teacher relationship differs from other types of
interpersonal relationships (e.g., romantic; workplace) where a large amount of emotion research has
taken place. It follows that the emotions experienced in academic contexts are likely to differ in
response to different issues and therefore need to be considered. Firstly, the student/teacher
relationship is hierarchical with unequal power structures/status, which is itself an important factor
shaping how laypeople understand the way emotion episodes play out (e.g., emotion
schemas/scripts) [44]. Given the structural power imbalances in university settings it might be
expected negative emotions such as anger to be experienced by students because of perceptions of
unfairness/injustice (over grades, feedback, unmet expectations etc.) or hopelessness, distress or
depression over a perceived lack of control regarding academic outcomes [6,26,44,45].
Secondly, the supportive/nurturing element of teaching means that emotions such as anger,
shame, guilt, boredom, hurt, hopelessness and anxiety might be experienced in response to a
perceived lack of caregiving [15]. The importance of quality student-teacher relationships to students’
success at university is widely recognized [43], and understanding the interplay of social emotions
experienced by students and faculty within educational settings is crucial, given interpersonal
communication is a key trigger for emotional responses [9,46]. Thirdly, the achievement focus of
higher education and potential for success and failure means that self-conscious emotions such as
shame and guilt would likely be salient experiences among students, as well as anxiety, unhappiness,
disappointment, hurt and sadness related to poor performance or negative feedback [6,24,47]. Fourth,
the processes of learning and generating knowledge are likely to elicit epistemic emotions such as
frustration, uncertainty, anxiety, confusion and boredom, which can impact on students’ use of
cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies [8,48,49]. Finally, evidence suggests that some
emotions (and their appraisals) can be domain specific, that is, are experienced in relation to specific
discipline areas (e.g., math-related anxiety) [50].
The impact of emotional experiences on motivation and behavior within academic settings are
variable. Negative activating emotions (e.g., hopelessness and boredom) are typically (although not
always) associated with avoidance and therefore can undermine achievement motivation due to low-
control appraisals (hopelessness) or perceived lack of incentives to perform academic activities
(boredom) [3,18,19]. Negative activating emotions can lead to approach or avoidance behaviors
depending on the underlying appraisals. For example, anger and anxiety are both negative activating
emotions, but anger is approach-related—often triggered by appraisals of obstruction and unfairness
[51], whereas anxiety is associated with avoidance of situations perceived to be threatening [4].
In summary, it was expected in the current study a range of achievement, epistemic and social
emotions such as boredom, fear/anxiety, frustration, guilt and shame would be reported. Secondly,
the perceived impact of such emotions would vary—in some cases prompting approach and in others
avoidance behavior, which would have varying consequences for achievement and learning
outcomes, in line with previous research and theory [1,3,6]. However, the overall approach to the
research was exploratory, given that we still know little about the adaptive functions of emotions in
learning and achievement, and the factors that mediate emotional responses in academic settings.
1.4. Qualitative Approaches to the Study of Emotions
Qualitative exploratory analysis is considered an effective approach for developing insights into
the range and phenomenology of an individual’s emotion knowledge and the generation of
hypotheses and theory which can inform the construction of quantitative measures (i.e., to draw
generalizations and analyze more precise causes and effects) [1,16]. A qualitative approach was
chosen for this research because of the need for exploratory research this area [1]. As Pekrun and
Stephens note, such research is “necessary to judge the relative importance of different emotions as
experienced by different students, and in different types of academic situations … [and] to generate
more comprehensive conceptions of the contents and functions of student emotions, beyond
hypotheses that can deductively be derived from existing theories” [1] (p. 296). Our aim was to obtain
an in-depth understanding of student and teacher perspectives, given the extant focus on student
perspectives [13,16]. Qualitative research can provide “in-depth contextualized understandings of
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 5 of 19
human behavior and accounts of personal experience and meaning that may not be possible with
quantitative methods” [52] (p. 29). The importance of investigating student and teacher perceptions
is well established—both have views about the factors that enhance and hinder learning and such
perspectives are valuable for understanding diverse factors that drive the learning process and its
outcomes [53,54]. Importantly, it is also likely that students and teachers do not always understand
their own or each other’s emotions in congruent ways, leading to the possibility of conflict over which
emotions are more or less appropriate to be feeling, regulating, or expressing, in particular kinds of
teaching and learning contexts.
1.5. Aims and Research Question
In line with the social functional approach discussed above, this paper focuses on students’ and
teachers’ perceptions and understandings of negative emotions in a University context. Findings
presented form part of a broader qualitative study exploring the experience and functionality of
emotions at University, with analyses of positive emotions previously reported [10]. Specifically, the
research investigates the following exploratory question: What are the features, functions and
consequences, according to University students and teachers, of discrete negative emotions on
learning and achievement (i.e., the impact on educational outcomes)?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
Interviews were undertaken with 36 university staff (students = 21, faculty = 15) at an Australian
university, with participants (female = 22, male = 14) located across a range of disciplines including
business, science and humanities (Appendix A). The average age of students was 22 years. Age
demographics were not collected for staff as it was not deemed relevant (it could be assumed Faculty
were older). A decision was made to interview students and faculty as few emotion studies have
reported on the perspectives of both groups. Additionally, we wanted to gauge the extent to which
their views aligned. All participants were allocated a pseudonym.
2.2. Materials and Procedure
Participants were recruited via email, with invitations sent via various staff and student groups
in keeping with a non-random (self-selecting) sampling approach, whereby participants volunteered
to take part in the research, i.e., no particular individuals or groups were targeted. The diverse sample
enabled the exploration of emotions and learning in a broader sense, rather than investigating
potential demographic or disciplinary differences. Interviews were conducted on campus by two
research assistants and participants received one movie ticket for their contribution. Semi-structured
interview questions were developed by the authors following a review of the literature, and were
supplemented with the results of a previous study [55]. Questions were intended to encourage
participants’ recollections of their emotional responses (and in the case of Faculty, the emotional
responses of students) to a range of learning situations and events under six broad themes: key
emotions in learning; positive emotions; negative emotions; motivation; feedback and
student/teacher relationships. While some specific areas were covered (e.g., feedback), in most cases
the academic situations and events were not specified, as it was of interest to the researchers what
types of situations/events would be reported.
Both groups were interviewed around the same theme areas, however questions focused on the
student experience, not that of Faculty. There were some exceptions to this, e.g., a starter question
asked students to describe a time when they felt bad about the learning experience, while faculty
were asked to describe a time they felt bad about teaching. Generally, however, students were asked
about their experiences of emotions in learning, while Faculty were asked about their perceptions of
student emotional responses, and their role as teachers in facilitating and/or managing such
responses (e.g., calming anxious students). This distinction was intended to allow for a comparison
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 6 of 19
of perspectives in light of power differences between the two groups. Some faculty however, did
speak of their own affective reactions in the course of the interviews.
Starter questions for themes most relevant to this paper (e.g., excluding positive emotions) were
as followsthese were designed to prompt rich accounts, with probing questions used if initial
questions did not lead to developed responses:
What emotions do you think are important in the learning process? (Participants were asked to
give examples of when they felt bad about learning/teaching, reporting the emotions
experienced and why it was a negative experience);
What strategies do students use to manage negative emotions (e.g., felling stressed, upset) in
order to successfully complete tasks/focus on their work? (Probing questions asked participants
to comment on whether these were successful and what factors helped, e.g., peers, staff);
What motivates students to learn? Do you think negative emotions are ever motivating?
What emotions do you experience when you receive feedback? (Students) How do students feel
about receiving feedback on their assignments? (Faculty) (Probing questions focused on
identifying the impact this has on student motivation, if any, and strategies to mitigate negative
emotional reactions triggered by feedback).
Despite the small sample size, data saturation was reached and the sample was considered
sufficient for an exploratory qualitative study [56]. All participants gave their informed consent for
inclusion before being interviewed. Ethics approval for this research was granted by Macquarie
University (ethical approval code: HE26SEP2008-D06071).
2.3. Coding and Analysis
Coding of data was undertaken using QSR International NVivo 11 software (Doncaster, VIC,
Australia) and informed by inductive and deductive methods of analysis. Predetermined coding
categories were used to code and group emotions. This was to allow for an analysis of basic and
discrete emotions, rather than broad negative affect. Coding (that is the classification and
organization) of emotions adopted a combination of categorical (e.g., discrete categories such as
anger, sadness) and dimensional (e.g., positive, negative) approaches to emotion measurement
informed by a prototype approach [37,57]. The use of prototype approaches to emotion knowledge,
concepts and scripts is well supported by empirical findings [36,37] and is compatible with
dimensional and categorical approaches to emotion measurement as “it addresses both the contents
of individual categories (e.g., the category of sadness episodes) and the hierarchical relations among
categories (e.g., loneliness is a type of sadness, which itself is a type of negative emotion)” [58] (p.
186).
Emotions were initially coded using participants’ own words, then as a second step, they were
coded according to valence (positive/negative). Finally, they were grouped into basic emotion
categories following previous research on emotion prototypes (largely drawing on Shaver et al.’s
1987 model) (Figure 1). Note in Figure 1, only select examples of discrete emotions are provided and
disgust was removed due to its relative absence from the data. The allocation of discrete emotions to
these broader dimensions was based on previous research, including Shaver and colleagues’
hierarchical organization of emotion knowledge [37], and the findings of basic emotion researchers
such as Izard [32,59,60]. Across the data set, six negative ‘basic’ emotion dimensions were identified:
anger, fear, sadness, disgust, boredom and self-conscious emotions (this category included some
positive emotions as well as negative ones, i.e., guilt, shame, embarrassment and pride). Given the
explorative nature of the research, having positive and negative emotions contained within one
category was not considered problematic.
Remaining coding categories emerged from the data through elemental (e.g., descriptive) and
thematic methods of coding and were revised over several cycles. The process of theme identification
followed a similar procedure to that outlined by Braun and Clarke where the essence of each theme
was identified and considered in relation to other codes and themes [61]. Parent or primary coding
categories for the subset of data analyzed in the present paper were: social functioning
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 7 of 19
(communication), cognition, motivation and drive, performance and productivity. Sub-categories of
themes were coded hierarchically under these broad headings in accordance with their relationship
to the parent category (e.g., complaining, assistance seeking were coded under ‘Social Functioning’;
conscientiousness, exploration and curiosity, persistence were coded under ‘Productivity, Effort and
Achievement Potential’ (Table 2). This follows the same procedure undertaken for our analysis of
positive emotions [10]. Coding was initially conducted by the lead author and refined over several
cycles, with reliability of coding categories verified through face to face meetings and email
communication with other members of the research team [62]. For the study more broadly, data
source triangulation (i.e., drawing on the voices of students and lecturers, the literature, and multiple
theories), was used to enhance the validity of the research [63]. Triangulation of participants, for
example, allowed for the comparison and crosschecking of consistency of information, as well as
justification of coding themes, thereby enhancing the credibility of findings [63,64].
Matrix coding queries were generated in NVivo to cross reference parts of the data where
emotions were reported in relation to their impact on learning and achievement. Interestingly, with
the exception of motivation, interview questions did not explicitly ask participants for their thoughts
about the impact of emotions on cognition, performance and other areas of functioning. Rather these
were revealed in responses to other questions and identified through the coding process. Anger,
boredom and fear were identified as key emotions, as measured by the higher frequency of
mentions—others were cited less regularly by participants, suggesting these three emotions may play
a more prominent role in enhancing and inhibiting learning. However, care must be taken in
interpreting this finding as one of the interview questions specifically asked participants about
managing stress (which might be associated with anxiety and related emotions in some people’s
minds). Hence, the focus of this analysis is on the range of emotional experiences reported and their
potential functions, rather than the importance (or not) of particular emotions.
Figure 1. Negative emotion ‘family’ dimensions based on prototype analysis.
3. Results
The research question aimed to identify perceived features and consequences (if any) of negative
emotions on learning and achievement. It was anticipated that findings would shed light on specific
functions served by negative emotions in educational contexts, thus extending previous theoretical
work by emotion scholars. While the focus of the study was to identify the range of ideas and views
reported, frequency data (generated through matrix queries) is also presented.
Discrete emotions
Emotion dimensions
(basic emotions)
Valence Negative
emotions
anger
frustration
irritation
upset
fear
anxiety
intimidation
confusion
sadness
disappoint-
ment
hurt
regret
boredom
boredom
disinterest
self-
conscious
guilt
shame
embarr-
assment
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 8 of 19
3.1. Comparison of Student and Faculty Perceptions
As expected, anger, boredom and fear were reported frequently by both groups, although as
mentioned earlier, care must be taken in interpreting this finding (Table 1). Interestingly, students
also mention sadness and self-conscious emotions more than Faculty. This could be an artifact of the
data, due in part to the focus on student emotions in the interview questions. Alternatively, the
discrepancy could be related to the respective roles of each participant group. Given intense/powerful
emotional experiences are remembered longer and recalled with greater vividness [65], it follows that
the types of negative memories for each group might also differ, e.g., for teachers’, salient negative
experiences could include dealing with angry students dissatisfied by grades/feedback as well as
observing students bored in class, while for students it might be emotions tied to unmet achievement
goals.
Participants reported that negative emotions can have either a positive or negative impact on
different areas of functioning, suggesting a complex relationship between negative affect and
learning (Figure 2). Interestingly, compared to students who generally viewed negative emotions as
detrimental (with the exception of fear), teachers were more likely to emphasize the functional aspect
of negative emotions. In particular, Faculty appeared to have a wider appreciation of the potential
usefulness of emotions such as anger and frustration in motivating students to engage in a deeper
way with their learning and complete assessment tasks. This was also reflected by the overall number
of comments referring to the impact of emotions on learning and achievement, with almost double
the number of coded remarks from teachers, despite the higher number of student participants (Table
2). Note that coding references are not a definitive measure but provide a general indication of the
importance or prevalence of particular ideas/themes (as reflected by a higher number of coding
references). The majority of student comments concerned increases and decreases in motivation,
while Faculty comments ranged across a broader range of areas including behaviors (common ones
being immobilization, complaining, assistance seeking, and increases in motivation/drive).
Figure 2. Perceived impact of negative emotions on learning.
Table 1. Percentage of participants who reported discrete emotions contained within basic negative
emotion categories.
Student (%) Faculty (%)
Anger 95 94
Boredom 81 75
Fear 100 94
Sadness 81 44
Self-conscious 81 56
Promotes
learning
• promotes assistance seeking
behaviour
• enhances cognition
• increases motivation drive
• enhances
productivity/performance
Mediating
factors
• intensity of emotion
•age
• life experience/maturity
• gender
• personality type
•nature of task
Inhibits
Learning
• hinders communication
• prevents engagement and
reduces motivation
• impaires cognition
• diminishes
produtivity/performance
Negative
emotions
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 9 of 19
According to Faculty, the extent to which emotions promoted or hindered learning was
mediated by several factors including gender, age and life experience (with mature age students
perceived as being more able to cope with adversity), the intensity of emotions experienced,
personality factors, and the nature of learning tasks. Emotion intensity was thought to be particularly
important, with a “balance” needed between milder negative emotions (perceived to enhance
learning) and more intense emotions (“too much stress and anxiety [is] detrimental to their [students]
learning” Hilary, Accounting Faculty).
If they worry sometimes, at least it means they respect the difficulty of the task. But if it goes to an
extreme and their anxiety is at a level where they can’t function, it becomes destructive. (Sheldon,
Accounting Faculty)
Individual personality differences were considered important both in terms of learners’ being
predisposed to react and behave in certain ways, as well as possessing the capacity to cope (or not)
when experiencing negative emotions (i.e., individual resilience). Specifically, certain types of
students were “stress resistant and [able to] … cope very well by their nature” (Benjamin, Business
Faculty). As further explained by a faculty member:
There are people who just freeze and those who act. So the responses are different… Some of them
would respond to it as if—it completely immobilizes them and they can’t function anymore and that
is not a good way. And I imagine that those are the students that really need to consult with their
tutors or their lecturers or would probably need to seek counseling or some support group that would
enable them to actually manage that fear in a positive way… I don’t think we can just say fear is
disabling. (Frances, Accounting Faculty)
“Anxiety and stress” were considered by some to be “an important part of being a high achiever
… But [was] detrimental if it’s too extreme” (Hilary, Accounting Faculty). Benjamin noted that gender
might play a role, suggesting that male students maynot be able to be willing to express their
frustration and come and talk to you [the teacher]”; rather they were prone to aggressiveness,
whereas females were more able to ask for help. Finally, the nature of the task was also important,
e.g., some students find public speaking extremely fear provoking, more so than for other tasks, such
as private study.
Table 2. Student and Faculty perceptions of the impact of negative emotions on learning as measured
by number of coding references.
Perceived Impact Students Faculty
Promotes learning (generally) 0 1
Enhances cognition 0 4
Enhances social functioning (assistance seeking) 1 8
Increases motivation and drive 11 12
Increases productivity, effort and achievement potential 3 5
Heightens conscientiousness 0 2
Promotes exploration and curiosity 1 5
Promotes persistence 3 2
Inhibits learning (generally) 2 8
Lowers performance and productivity (wasted time) 2 2
Impairs social functioning (communication) 1 0
Complaining/trying to change unfair outcome 1 10
Prevents assistance seeking 0 3
Reluctance to provide feedback 0 1
Impairs cognition 6 8
Closes off mind to ideas or people 1 1
Immobilizes 0 10
Loss of motivation, avoidance and procrastination 16 5
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 10 of 19
3.2. Cognitive and Social Functioning
Both participant groups perceived cognition to be impaired by negative emotions (especially
fear and to a lesser extent, boredom and anger). These emotions reportedly contributed to difficulties
in concentrating, focusing, paying attention to detail (with the exception of fear), absorbing and/or
understanding information and instructions:
It made me unhappy and more stressed, because I really wanted to get some studying done but I
found myself just being distracted by the emotion. So I’d find myself doing things like going to a tute
[tutorial] for an hour, then the next day I wouldn’t have a clear understanding again. (Nadine,
Science Student)
In terms of cognitive engagement, negative emotions were thought to close the mind off to ideas
and connections with others (“some [students] will not respond and will just reject me again because
I’m offering a hard solution”), as well as inhibit learning through freezing or immobilizing so “they
[students] can’t function any more” (Frances, Accounting Faculty). Interestingly, only Faculty
commented on the immobilization function, suggesting students may not be aware, or have the
language to articulate this process yet. Typical comments were similar to those expressed by
Benjamin, a business/marketing lecturer, who observed that with some forms of anxiety “you start
to feel emotionally switched off, and you’re no longer open to engaging with the material.”
Social functioning was viewed as being both enhanced and impaired by negative emotions.
Assistance seeking behaviors were prompted by feelings of frustration, fear and confusion. For
example, students approached teaching staff, peers, friends and family to clarify issues they were
unsure about or to ask for help, including application for special consideration in times of hardship.
Special consideration is the formal name of the process at the university, whereby students can
request compassion for their study/performance during periods of hardship, illness etc.
I’ll never leave something unresolved if I’m not sure with what they’re [teachers] trying to say or if
I’m really unhappy with what they’ve written. (Luke, Science Student)
At the same time, fear and anger were also thought to impair assistance seeking behavior and
communication more broadly. For example, Faculty considered that students might feel too
“intimidated to come to the teacher” while angry students approached teachers directly to complain
or pressure them to change “unfair” outcomes such as poor grades. While such behaviors could be
considered adaptive, they were not perceived by lecturers to be cooperative or helpful to the learning
process:
There are different ways of dealing with frustration and it’s not always channeled the right way, as
in asking for help or assistance. (Benjamin, Business Faculty)
Embarrassment could also hinder assistance seeking through avoidance: “so you sort of felt
embarrassed to ask a few questions because you didn’t know how they [the teachers] were going to
react” (Wendy, Chiropractic Student).
3.3. Motivation, Productivity and Performance
According to both participant groups, negative emotions had the capacity to increase as well as
decrease motivation and direct effort into study/task completion, resulting in varied levels of
productivity and achievement. Like cognitive engagement, there was a general sentiment that “a
certain amount of stress is good; like not to be too relaxed. [That] You need to be a little bit stressed
to really achieve perhaps your potential” (Hilary, Accounting Faculty). Similarly,
The fear of failure is a reality and therefore those sorts of negative emotions are usually present and
can be motivating and I think that we have to recognize that while the fear of failure is not what you
want to be a dominating sort of emotional motivator, it’s part of the package. (Cameron, Business
Faculty)
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 11 of 19
The experience of frustration encouraged exploration and curiosity, suggesting epistemic
emotions may have an important role to play here:
…frustration is part of curiosity, in a sense, that I want to know more and I can’t. You have to sort
of shut against a brick wall for a while before it falls down. There’s a degree of frustration that’s quite
healthy because it fuels your own desire to push forward. You know you’ve got something to push
against. (Michelle, English Faculty)
Frustration, in particular, was held to motivate students to understand concepts, as well as
challenge their assumptions: “If they’re frustrated because they don’t understand it, that motivates
them to take the steps needed to overcome their lack of understanding” (Sheldon, Accounting
Faculty). Increased persistence, especially on undesirable tasks, was also associated with negative
emotions such as dislike, uncertainty and unhappiness. Thus, they appear to play a role in promoting
diligence and the commitment to follow through, especially on difficult or unpleasant tasks. Despite
the perceived detrimental impact of negative emotions on attention broadly, interestingly fear was
associated with increased conscientiousness, suggesting a role in promoting quality outcomes:
[fear] makes sure you cross your Ts and dot your Is. It ensures that you cover every little detail
because you’re so terrified that you’re going to, if it’s a public speaking thing, stuff up, get it wrong;
make a mistake in an exam, miss an issue. (Hilary, Accounting Faculty)
Despite some of the benefits for motivation, on the flip side negative emotions (particularly
boredom and fear) could equally promote avoidance behaviors such as withdrawing from courses,
swapping classes, procrastinating, sleeping in, and the general urge to “run away.”
I’ve seen several times in three years … people withdrawing because they felt so stressed that they
just couldn’t cope with coming in class, fulfilling their work and everything. (Genevieve, French
Faculty)
Boredom was viewed as strongly hindering motivation and cognition, and one of the more
challenging emotions to manage as a teacher:
If a student’s bored, you know, in the long run, no matter what you do as a teacher, no matter how
much you try and engage them in the most innovative creative ways you can possibly think of, if
they don’t want to be there… I don’t think there’s anything you can do. (Angelina, Cultural
Studies Faculty)
Only three participants held the view that boredom could be motivating and productive in some
circumstances. Negative emotions were also associated with encouraging surface learning
approaches, such as rote learning materials the day before an exam, and other strategies considered
undesirable by teaching staff. Sadness appears to play a role in both inhibiting and promoting
motivation, particularly around experiences of failure and/or poor performance evaluations, but little
else. Negative self-conscious emotions (shame, grief and embarrassment) were seldom mentioned in
terms of their impact on learning or achievement, but were dominant elsewhere in the data,
particularly in relation to the antecedents (appraisals) of emotions—notably experiences of social
exclusion/ostracism. Where reported, feelings of guilt and inadequacy were linked to an increased
drive to succeed and/or investment of effort:
…it really did impact on my self-esteem. It really did just make me feel quite inadequate as a student
… being told that I’m not smart enough to do something, I now have this constant drive to want to
be just smart enough to do something. (Kevin, International Studies Student)
Finally, a strong theme of “waste” was evident in participant’s accounts of the lower
productivity and achievement resulting from negative emotions.
Uncertainty, fear, regret; like maybe I thought I was better able to do this work than I am so maybe
I’ve wasted my time and my money and her [the teachers] time. Yeah, doubt, total doubt. Fear, like
oh my gosh, this is where I thought my career was going, this is my study, and this is the postgrad
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 12 of 19
so it’s kind of the culmination of my undergrad and study, and all of these years of—so has that all
been wasteful. (Phoebe, Arts Student)
In summary, a range of impacts were reported by participants, which provide clues as to the
potential functions of particular negative emotions in learning and achievement at university.
4. Discussion
4.1. The Functions of Negative Emotions
Patterns were observed in the data in relation to the effects of anger, fear, sadness and boredom
across four main areas of functioning (Table 1). Unlike positive emotions, which seem to have a
mostly positive impact on learning (e.g., [10,66,67]), participants reported that negative emotions can
have either a positive or negative impact. This suggests a more complex relationship between
negative affect and learning, a view supported by existing literature (e.g., [1]). Interestingly, adaptive
functions were recognized more by Faculty, with students focusing on motivational consequences.
The view that a certain level of negative emotion was needed to enhance engagement resonates with
the Yerkes-Dodson Law that proposes a certain degree of stress or arousal can actually improve
performance [68]. Anger and fear are clearly perceived to be both enablers and inhibitors of learning
and achievement, impacting across all areas of functioning. Anger and anxiety (coded here as part of
Fear) are both considered activating emotions [3], however they likely serve different functions, with
anger generally approach-related (e.g., students’ complaining to lecturers about grades) while
anxiety is avoidant (e.g., students’ not attending an exam because of a fear of failing). Pekrun and
Perry note the motivational effects of negative activating emotions such as anger and fear are
complex, as the outcomes can be so variable [3]. Our findings are in line with previous theory and
research that has found anger functions primarily to remove obstacles to goal achievement, often
through securing a better outcome by forcing a change in another person’s behavior [69]. Fear serves
to address threats, by removing oneself from a perceived danger, supporting previous scholarship
which shows fear is elicited in response to appraisals of immediate and specific threats, and narrows
thoughts and actions [27,67]. The research also supports existing work on anxiety, which has been
found to impair performance in testing situations [14]. The key to better understanding the nuanced
effects of these emotions may be to look more closely at the underlying appraisals of each [7].
In the present data set, frustration at not understanding a concept or feeling dissatisfied with the
current state of knowledge appeared to be associated with increased motivation, while anger or
frustration attributed to teachers and/or the curriculum was linked with impaired motivation in
participants’ minds. This may reflect personality variables, but it is interesting that frustration
directed toward external factors (e.g., teachers, curricula) appear (at least in this instance) to be more
detrimental to motivation than when directed at self. It points to potential differences in the role of
frustration as an epistemic vs. an achievement emotion. Emotions can share affective properties with
other categories of emotion, but differ in terms of their object focus [8]. Frustration could be
considered an epistemic emotion if the focus is on cognitive incongruity resulting from an unsolved
problem, or an achievement emotion if the focus is on the curriculum or teacher as a perceived
obstacle to academic success. In both cases frustration is an activating emotion which could in some
circumstances promote strategy use (i.e., as an epistemic emotion) or persistence (as an achievement
emotion), and is consistent with other evidence that unpleasant emotions can promote learning and
do not always require remediation [70,71].
The finding that boredom hindered motivation and cognition, supports recent research
suggesting boredom has a uniformly negative effect on learning and achievement outcomes [19].
However, recent perspectives suggest boredom may play a role in encouraging the pursuit of
alternative goals and experiences, by signaling to a person that it is time to try something new [17].
Mixed findings in the literature could be due in part, to a lack of clarity and agreement around the
arousal levels of boredom—like frustration, boredom is considered a deactivating emotion [3],
however recent research suggests there may be subtypes of boredom with differing levels of arousal
and valence [72]. Another factor may be perceptions of agency. In the present study, boredom was
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 13 of 19
associated primarily with two external factors—the curriculum (content) and its delivery (i.e.,
teaching style), with Faculty investing considerable effort to minimize boredom and its effects (e.g.,
disengagement, distraction). This suggests appraisals of agency are potentially an important
antecedent to experiences of boredom, in addition to value and control which have already been
theoretically and empirically established [18]. Further work is needed on this under researched
emotion to determine more precisely its antecedents and impact.
All mentions of sadness were in relation to its intrapersonal effects, an area of research that
remains largely untested [73]. There was insufficient data to draw any other conclusions from our
data and this emotion requires further investigation in educational contexts. In the scholarly
literature, sadness is a deactivating emotion elicited by appraisals of irreparable loss and therefore
likely has implications for how students cope in the face of adversity, e.g., persistence, dropping out
[27]. It may also play a role in helping students make sense of past failures in order to prevent future
ones [74].
It is not clear why negative self-conscious emotions were not as strongly articulated as having
an effect on learning, but were reported more in relation to emotional triggers. Existing literature on
self-conscious emotions suggests they play an important role in achievement [47], so it could be that
this is an artifact of the data given the majority of comments coded for negative self-conscious
emotions were made by students (and students do not appear to recognize the adaptive benefits of
emotions to the same extent as teachers). Future research would benefit from empirically
investigating different types of shame, embarrassment etc. in tertiary settings, along with the self-
evaluations that underpin such emotions, in order to better understand their variable outcomes on
achievement and learning. Such findings could be used to inform instructional strategies to prevent
and manage shame and related responses [75], which may include developing students emotion
knowledge.
4.2. Mediating Factors
Several mediating factors were identified by Faculty as affecting the direction of impact
(promoting or inhibiting) on learning and achievement following the experience of negative
emotions. These were mostly mentioned in relation to fear, although it should be noted that the
overall number of comments was small and should not be over emphasized. Demographic variables
of age, gender and personality attributes were deemed to influence how people react, as well as
hinder their ability to cope with and/or regulate negative feelings. Intensely felt emotions were
thought be more detrimental, perhaps because they are harder to manage and regulate. Typically,
(although not always) emotion regulation strives to increase positive emotions, and decrease negative
ones [1] and a person’s capacity to mentalize will impact on their ability to regulate emotions,
pointing again to the importance of individual differences [76]. While the nature of academic tasks
was also mentioned, this received less emphasis than personal variables, which is surprising as task
demands are emphasized in the literature as a major contributing factor to emotions experienced in
educational settings (e.g., [2,6]).
It is beyond the scope of the present paper to discuss all these mediating factors in depth,
however a couple are worth a mention. Gender differences in experiences of affect for example, have
been reported in relation to domain specific areas such as mathematics, with adolescent girls
reporting significantly more anxiety than boys [77]. Benjamin’s account of gender differences in
dealing with frustration is suggestive of gender differences in emotion knowledge, that is, how
female and male students understand frustration. In both cases described, the same emotion
(frustration) is experienced, but different approach behaviors are enacted that are likely to lead to
very different outcomes. Personality traits are another area worthy of further investigation. While
they have been linked to academic performance and achievement some findings are mixed. For
example, one study found neuroticism was negatively associated with academic achievement [78]
while another reported it was not a strong predictor of GPA [79]. It could be that there is an inverted
relationship, which means some amount of neuroticism may be beneficial but too much detrimental.
This and other trait emotions are deserving of further follow up. In our study, while some faculty
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 14 of 19
commented on personality characteristics of students, few mentioned trait emotions. One lecturer
observed that chronic predispositions to experience particular emotions such as anxiety were
problematic and required professional treatment. While not examined here, individual differences
relating to gender, culture etc. are important as it is recognized that emotions are not necessarily
similarly experienced or expressed across groups [58,80].
The dominant voice of teachers’ views of the impact of emotions on learning, as well as their
wider appreciation of the benefits of negative affect, could be attributed to their age, in that they have
a longer history of experiences with emotions in learning. Further, older adults are generally better
able to differentiate positive and negative sources of emotion knowledge [81]. Given emotion
knowledge “operates as a major (though not necessarily the only) factor in emotion regulation” [30]
(p. 45) it could be assumed that that participants with a stronger emotion knowledge are better able
to regulate their emotions, and therefore more readily see the benefits of negative affect. Such an
assumption is supported by a recent study which found that while chronic negative affect
(unhappiness) over the duration of a students’ degree program was detrimental to performance, the
academic success of ‘happy’ students’ seems to originate from their ability to adaptively manage the
motivational benefits of bouts of heightened negative affect [82].
4.3. Limitations
Findings reported in the present study are of perceptions of emotions, not induced or observed
emotions. They provide a snapshot in time of the experiences of students and academics at one
university, and cannot be generalized beyond that context. However, they can inform larger studies
investigating more precise effects and causes (e.g., systematic measures of educational outcomes), as
well as contribute to development of teaching strategies aimed at harnessing emotions to promote
rather than hinder learning, and teaching students how to recognize and regulate negative affect. The
sample was comprised of students and academics from diverse disciplines, which may affect their
understanding of emotions, and therefore potentially influence the data. Other limitations include
the general confines of using interviews as a data collection method (e.g., social desirability effects,
memory bias, lack of openness on the part of participants) and of using interviews in emotion
research (e.g., the potential to under report less intense emotion experiences over intense ones,
willingness to disclose emotions) [31]. This is important as subtle affective cues can impact on
cognition, i.e., it does not need to be an enduring, intensively felt emotion to have an influence on
achievement, motivation or behavior [2]. Finally, we acknowledge the limitations of basic emotions
as a construct (e.g., [83]) and of coding data in this way (particularly in light of debate around the
status of some emotion categories, e.g., whether confusion should be defined as an emotion within
its own right, as opposed to a sub-category of other more ‘basic’ emotions) [84]. For the purposes of
this exploratory study, a prototype approach was deemed an effective and simple way of coding and
analyzing responses in order to make meaningful inferences.
4.4. Implications for Practice and Future Research
There are practical lessons to be learnt from this data on how to both prevent and manage the
fallout of negative emotions. A number of strategies were offered by participants to reduce the onset
of negative emotions, most of which fell within the responsibility of teaching staff. They included the
use of humor and other teaching methods to foster interest in the classroom (particularly when the
content was considered ‘boring’), showing care and concern, encouraging students to form
connections with peers and seek assistance in times of stress, providing frequent and timely feedback
to reduce anxiety and confusion, being available to students, providing clear instructions and
dispersing assessment tasks throughout semester to prevent student overload. Students’ ability to
cope with and regulate negative emotions is also paramount, and reflective practice, cognitive
reappraisal and other pedagogical strategies may offer useful approaches to promoting self-
regulation [85,86].
There are a number of areas in need of further investigation. In addition to those already
mentioned, the relationship between different categories of emotion (achievement, social, epistemic
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 15 of 19
etc.) and co-occurring emotions (such as frustration/curiosity and fear/excitement in the present
study) warrants additional research, as these can be overlooked in experimental paradigms of basic
emotions which tend to focus study on the discrete boundaries that differentiate one emotion from
another [83]. The antecedents of negative emotions in learning (both in terms of the types of
events/situations and appraisals); power dynamics and discrete negative emotions within the context
of student-teacher relationships, and more theorizing and empirical work around academic emotions
not related to achievement (i.e., social and epistemic emotions) is also needed. Finally, future
quantitative research needs to empirically examine our study’s assumptions, as well as building on
existing work testing variations of emotional experience in educational contexts by gender, class,
culture and other demographic variables to allow a richer contextual understanding of the experience
of emotions in education (e.g., [87]).
5. Conclusions
The research presented is a contribution for advancing the field of emotion research in learning
and teaching. We have reported on emotions identified by students and faculty as being important
to learning and achievement in higher education, as well as explored the meaning of those emotions
across a range of learning situations. This offers an alternative perspective to scientific accounts of
the role of academic emotions, with lay peoples’ knowledge about the nature and course of emotions
such as anger, sadness, fear and boredom a valuable source of data about the functions these
emotions play in educational contexts. Overall findings reveal that negative affect is perceived to
both enhance and hinder learning, supporting previous empirical and theoretical work. Given the
complexity of interactions between emotions and variables such as task requirements, interpersonal
relationships, achievement goals and cognitive resources, and the lack of agreement over the precise
mechanisms by which some emotions are able to bring about very different effects, perhaps
researchers and theorists should modify the types of questions being asked. As Fielder and Beier
observe, rather than asking for a “simplified, one-sided answer to the question of whether
achievement and motivation profit from positive or negative mood”, perhaps we should ask what
kinds of achievement, learning and motivation are enhanced by negative emotions? [2] (p. 51).
Further research is needed to uncover the nuances of these interactions to better understand
emotions, which have the potential to both enhance and inhibit learning, and add knowledge about
the experiences of co-occurring emotions in the learning environment.
Acknowledgments: This research was funded by an Australian Postgraduate Award Scholarship (APA) with
supplementary funding from the Faculty of Business and Economics at Macquarie University.
Author Contributions: This paper was conceived by Dr Anna Rowe based on work undertaken as part of her
doctoral thesis under the supervision of Professor Leigh Wood and Professor Julie Fitness. Julie provided input
on theory and data analysis and both authors contributed to writing the manuscript.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Appendix A
Table A1. Participant discipline areas.
Discipline Groups Disciplines Represented in the Sample Students Faculty
Humanities Asian studies, cultural studies, international studies, English, French 6 4
Social sciences Anthropology 1
Education Education, early childhood studies 3
Sciences Science 3
Health sciences Chiropractic 4
Administration, business,
economics, law Accounting, business, business-law, economics 5 10
Note: Discipline groups are based on the Australian Standard Classification of Education (ASCED)
higher education discipline groupings (2001) retrieved from: http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/
abs@.nsf/0/F500513319ABDD4ACA256AAF001FCA75?opendocument.
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 16 of 19
References
1. Pekrun, R.; Stephens, S.J. Achievement emotions in higher education. In Higher Education: Handbook of
Theory and Research; Smart, J.C., Ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2010; Volume 25, pp. 257–306.
2. Fiedler, K.; Beier, S. Affect and cognitive processes in educational contexts. In International Handbook of
Emotions in Education; Alexander, P.A., Pekrun, R., Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK,
2014; pp. 36–55.
3. Pekrun, R.; Perry, R.P. Control-value theory of achievement emotions. In International Handbook of Emotions
in Education; Alexander, P.A., Pekrun, R., Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2014; pp.
120–141.
4. Zeidner, M. Anxiety in education. In International Handbook of Emotions in Education; Alexander, P.A.,
Pekrun, R., Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2014; pp. 265–288.
5. Pekrun, R.; Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. Introduction to emotions in education In International Handbook of
Emotions in Education; Pekrun, R., Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Eds.; Taylor and Francis: Florence, Italy, 2014;
pp. 1–10.
6. Pekrun, R. The Control-Value Theory of Achievement Emotions: Assumptions, corollaries, and
implications for educational research and practice. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 18, 315–341.
7. Rowe, A.; Fitness, J.; Wood, L. The role and functionality of emotions in feedback at university: A
qualitative study. Aust. Educ. Res. 2014, 41, 283–309.
8. Pekrun, R.; Vogl, E.; Muis, K.R.; Sinatra, G.M. Measuring emotions during epistemic activities: The
Epistemically-Related Emotion Scales. Cogn. Emot. 2017, 31, 1268–1276.
9. Hareli, S.; Parkinson, B. What’s social about social emotions? J. Theory Soc. Behav. 2008, 38, 131–156.
10. Rowe, A.D.; Fitness, J.; Wood, L.N. University student and lecturer perceptions of positive emotions in
learning. Int. J. Qual. Stud. Educ. 2015, 28, 1–20.
11. White, C.J. Higher education emotions: A scale development exercise. High. Educ. Res. Dev. 2013, 32, 287–
299.
12. Vogl, E.; Pekrun, R. Emotions that matter to achievement: Student feelings about assessment. In Handbook
of Human and Social Conditions in Assessment; Brown, G.T.L., Harris, L.R., Eds.; Routledge Handbooks
Online: New York, NY, USA, 2016.
13. Pekrun, R.; Goetz, T.; Perry, R.P.; Kramer, K.; Hochstadt, M.; Molfenter, S. Beyond test anxiety:
Development and validation of the test emotions questionnaire (TEQ). Anxiety Stress Coping 2004, 17, 287–
316.
14. Pekrun, R.; Cusack, A.; Murayama, K.; Elliot, A.J.; Thomas, K. The power of anticipated feedback: Effects
on students’ achievement goals and achievement emotions. Learn. Instr. 2014, 29, 115–124.
15. Mazer, J.P.; McKenna-Buchannan, T.P.; Quinlan, M.M.; Titsworth, S. The dark side of emotion in the
classroom: Emotional processes as mediators of teacher communication behaviors and student negative
emotions. Commun. Educ. 2014, 63, 1–20.
16. Pekrun, R.; Goetz, T.; Titz, W.; Perry, R.P. Academic emotions in students’ self-regulated learning and
achievement: A program of qualitative and quantitative research. Educ. Psychol. 2002, 37, 91–105.
17. Bench, S. The Role of Boredom in The Pursuit of Negative Experience. Ph.D. Thesis, Texas A&M University,
College Station, TX, USA, 2014.
18. Pekrun, R.; Goetz, T.; Daniels, L.M.; Stupnisky, R.H.; Perry, R.P. Boredom in achievement settings:
Exploring control–value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. J. Educ. Psychol.
2010, 102, 531–549.
19. Pekrun, R.; Hall, N.C.; Goetz, T.; Perry, R.P. Boredom and academic achievement: Testing a model of
reciprocal causation. J. Educ. Psychol. 2014, 106, 696–710.
20. Kuhbandner, C.; Spitzer, B.; Pekrun, R. Read-out of emotional information from iconic memory. Psychol.
Sci. 2011, 22, 695–700.
21. Pekrun, R.; Elliot, A.J.; Maier, M.A. Achievement goals and achievement emotions: Testing a model of their
joint relations with academic performance. J. Educ. Psychol. 2009, 101, 115–135.
22. Pekrun, R.; Elliot, A.J.; Maier, M.A. Achievement goals and discrete achievement emotions: A theoretical
model and prospective test. J. Educ. Psychol. 2006, 98, 583–597.
23. Kuhbandner, C.; Pekrun, R. Affective state influences retrieval-induced forgetting for integrated
knowledge. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e56617.
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 17 of 19
24. Lyubomirsky, S.; Boehm, J.K.; Kasri, F.; Zehm, K. The cognitive and hedonic costs of dwelling on
achievement-related negative experiences: Implications for enduring happiness and unhappiness. Emotion
2011, 11, 1152–1167.
25. Keltner, D.; Gross, J.J. Functional accounts of emotions. Cogn. Emot. 1999, 13, 467–480.
26. Roseman, I.J. Appraisal in the emotion system: Coherence in strategies for coping. Emot. Rev. 2013, 5, 141–
149.
27. Lazarus, R.S. Emotion and Adaptation; Oxford University Press: New York, UK, 1991.
28. Lench, H.C.; Bench, S.W.; Darbor, K.E.; Moore, M. A functionalist manifesto: Goal-related emotions from
an evolutionary perspective. Emot. Rev. 2015, 7, 90–98.
29. Frijda, N.H. The psychologists’ point of view. In Handbook of Emotions, 2nd ed.; Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones,
J.M., Eds.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2000; pp. 59–74.
30. Izard, C.E.; Woodburn, E.M.; Finlon, K.J.; Krauthamer-Ewing, E.S.; Grossman, S.R.; Seidenfeld, A. Emotion
knowledge, emotion utilization, and emotion regulation. Emot. Rev. 2011, 3, 44–52.
31. Nielsen, L.; Kaszniak, A.W. Conceptual, theoretical, and methodological issues in inferring subjective
emotion experience. In Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment; Coan, J.A., Allen, J.J.B., Eds.; Oxford
University Press: Oxford, UK, 2007; pp. 361–375.
32. Izard, C.E. Emotion theory and research: Highlights, unanswered questions, and emerging issues. Annu.
Rev. Psychol. 2009, 60, 1–25.
33. Oatley, K.; Duncan, E. The experience of emotions in everyday life. Cogn. Emot. 1994, 8, 369–381.
34. Rowe, A.D. Feedback in Higher Education: Personal, Relational and Emotional Dimensions. PhD Thesis,
Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia, 2013.
35. Fehr, B.; Russell, J.A. Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1984,
113, 464–486.
36. Fitness, J.; Fletcher, G.J. Love, hate, anger, and jealousy in close relationships: A prototype and cognitive
appraisal analysis. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1993, 65, 942–958.
37. Shaver, P.; Schwartz, J.; Kirson, D.; O’Connor, C. Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype
approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1987, 52, 1061–1086.
38. Fitness, J. Emotion knowledge structures in close relationships. In Knowledge Structures in Close
Relationships: A Social Psychological Approach; Fletcher, G.J.O., Fitness, J., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates:
Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1996; pp. 195–217.
39. Planalp, S.; Fitness, J.; Fehr, B. Emotion in theories of close relationships. In The Cambridge Handbook of
Personal Relationships; Vangelisti, A.L., Perlman, D., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK,
2006; pp. 369–384.
40. Eisenberg, N.; Sadovsky, A.; Spinrad, T.L. Associations of emotion-related regulation with language skills,
emotion knowledge, and academic outcomes. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2005, 109, 109–118.
41. Denham, S.A.; Bassett, H.H.; Brown, C.; Way, E.; Steed, J. “I know how you feel”: Preschoolers emotion
knowledge contributes to early school success. J. Early Child. Res. 2015, 13, 252–262.
42. Frenzel, A.C.; Pekrun, R.; Goetz, T.; Daniels, L.M.; Durksen, T.L.; Becker-Kurz, B.; Klassen, R.M. Measuring
teachers’ enjoyment, anger, and anxiety: The Teacher Emotions Scales (TES). Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2016,
46, 148–163.
43. Hagenauer, G.; Volet, S.E. Teacher-student relationship at university: An important yet under-researched
field. Oxf. Rev. Educ. 2014, 40, 370–388.
44. Fitness, J. Anger in the workplace: An emotion script approach to anger episodes between workers and
their superiors, coworkers and subordinates. J. Organ. Behav. 2000, 21, 147–162.
45. Feather, N.T. Deservingness and emotions: Applying the structural model of deservingness to the analysis
of affective reactions to outcomes. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 17, 38–73.
46. Metts, S.; Planalp, S. Emotion experience and expression: Current trends and future directions in
interpersonal relationship research. In The SAGE Handbook of Interpersonal Communication; Knapp, M.L.,
Daly, J.A., Eds.; SAGE: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2011; pp. 283–316.
47. Oades-Sese, G., V. ; Matthews, T.A.; Lewis, M. Shame and pride and their effects on student achievement.
In International Handbook of Emotions in Education; Alexander, P.A., Pekrun, R., Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Eds.;
Routledge: London, UK, 2014; pp. 246–264.
48. Carruthers, P. Are epistemic emotions metacognitive? Philos. Psychol. 2017, 30, 58–78.
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 18 of 19
49. Muis, K.R.; Pekrun, R.; Sinatra, G.M.; Azevedo, R.; Trevors, G.; Meier, E.; Heddy, B.C. The curious case of
climate change: Testing a theoretical model of epistemic beliefs, epistemic emotions, and complex learning.
Learn. Instr. 2015, 39, 168–183.
50. Goetz, T.; Frenzel, A.C.; Pekrun, R.; Hall, N.C. The domain specificity of academic emotional experiences.
J. Exp. Educ. 2006, 75, 5–29.
51. Carver, C.S.; Harmon-Jones, E. Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychol.
Bull. 2009, 135, 183–204.
52. Povee, K.; Roberts, L.D. Qualitative research in psychology: Attitudes of psychology students and academic
staff. Aust. J. Psychol. 2014, 66, 28–37.
53. Birenbaum, M. Assessment and instruction preferences and their relationship with test anxiety and
learning strategies. High. Educ. Int. J.High. Educ. Educ. Plan. 2007, 53, 749–768.
54. Lahtinen, A.M. University teachers’ views on the distressing elements of pedagogical interaction. Scand. J.
Educ.l Res. 2008, 52, 481–493.
55. Rowe, A. The personal dimension in teaching: Why students value feedback. Int. J. Educ. Manag. 2011, 25,
343–360.
56. Guest, G.; Bunce, A.; Johnson, L. How many interviews are enough? Field Methods 2006, 18, 59–82.
57. Oatley, K.; Johnson-Laird, P.N. Cognitive approaches to emotions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014, 18, 134–140.
58. Shaver, P.R.; Wu, S.; Schwartz, J.C. Cross-cultural similarities and differences in emotion and its
representation. In Review of Personality and Social Psychology, Clark, M.S., Ed. Sage Publications: Thousand
Oaks, CA, USA, 1992; Volume 13, pp. 175–212.
59. Izard, C.E. Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspect. Psychol. Sci.
2007, 2, 260–280.
60. Ortony, A.; Turner, T.J. What’s basic about basic emotions? Psychol. Rev. 1990, 97, 315–331.
61. Braun, V.; Clarke, V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual. Res. Psychol. 2006, 3, 77–101.
62. Saldaña, J. The Coding Manual for Qualitative Researchers, 3rd ed.; SAGE: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2016.
63. Patton, M.Q. Qualitative Research and Evaluation Methods, 3rd ed.; SAGE Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA,
USA, 2002.
64. Creswell, J.W. Research Design: Qualitative, Quantitative and Mixed Method Approaches, 2nd ed.; SAGE
Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2003.
65. Talarico, J.; LaBar, K.; Rubin, D. Emotional intensity predicts autobiographical memory experience. Mem.
Cogn. 2004, 32, 1118–1132.
66. Fredrickson, B.L.; Cohn, M.A. Positive emotions. In Handbook of Emotions, 3rd ed.; Lewis, M., Haviland-
Jones, J.M., Barrett, L.F., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 777–796.
67. Fredrickson, B.L. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2004,
359, 1367–1378.
68. Yerkes, R.M.; Dodson, J.D. The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habitformation. J. Comp.
Neurol. Psychol. 1908, 18, 459–482.
69. Fischer, A.H.; Roseman, I.J. Beat them or ban them: The characteristics and social functions of anger and
contempt. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 93, 103–115.
70. Baker, R.S.J.D.; Mello, S.K.; Rodrigo, M.M.T.; Graesser, A.C. Better to be frustrated than bored: The
incidence, persistence, and impact of learners’ cognitive–affective states during interactions with three
different computer-based learning environments. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2010, 68, 223–241.
71. D’Mello, S. A selective meta-analysis on the relative incidence of discrete affective states during learning
with technology. J. Educ. Psychol. 2013, 105, 1082–1099.
72. Thomas, G.; Nathan, C.H. Academic boredom. In International Handbook of Emotions in Education;
Alexander, P.A., Pekrun, R., Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2014; pp. 311–330.
73. Lench, H.C.; Tibbett, T.P.; Bench, S.W. Exploring the toolkit of emotion: What do sadness and anger do for
us? Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2016, 10, 11–25.
74. Andrews, P.W.; Thomson, J.A. The bright side of being blue: Depression as an adaptation for analyzing
complex problems. Psychol. Rev. 2009, 116, 620–654.
75. Turner, J.E.; Husman, J. Emotional and cognitive self-regulation following academic shame. J. Adv. Acad.
2008, 20, 138–173.
76. Fonagy, P. The mentalization-focused approach to social development. In Handbook of Mentalization-Based
Treatment; Allen, J.G., Fonagy, P., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons Ltd.: Chichester, UK, 2006; pp. 53–100.
Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 27 19 of 19
77. Bieg, M.; Goetz, T.; Wolter, I.; Hall, N. Gender stereotype endorsement differentially predicts girls’ and
boys’ trait-state discrepancy in math anxiety. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1404.
78. Chamorro-Premuzic, T.; Furnham, A. Personality predicts academic performance: Evidence from two
longitudinal university samples. J. Res. Personal. 2003, 37, 319–338.
79. Vedel, A. The Big Five and tertiary academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Personal.
Individ. Differ. 2014, 71, 66–76.
80. Fischer, A.H.; Rodriguez Mosquera, P.M.; Van Vianen, A.E.M.; Manstead, A.S.R. Gender and culture
differences in emotion. Emotion 2004, 4, 87–94.
81. Ready, R.E.; Robinson, M.D.; Weinberger, M. Age differences in the organization of emotion knowledge:
Effects involving valence and time frame. Psychol. Aging 2006, 21, 726–736.
82. Barker, E.T.; Howard, A.L.; Galambos, N.L.; Wrosch, C. Tracking affect and academic success across
university: Happy students benefit from bouts of negative mood. Dev. Psychol. 2016, 52, 2022–2030.
83. Posner, J.; Russell, J.A.; Peterson, B.S. The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective
neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 2005, 17, 715–734.
84. Sidney, K.D.M.; Arthur, C.G. Confusion. In International Handbook of Emotions in Education; Routledge:
London, UK, 2014; pp. 289–310.
85. Gross, J.J.; John, O.P. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect,
relationships, and well- being. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 85, 348–362.
86. Willen, S. Confronting a “big huge gaping wound”: Emotion and anxiety in a cultural sensitivity course
for psychiatry residents. Culture Med. Psychiatry 2013, 37, 253–279.
87. Frenzel, A.C.; Thrash, T.M.; Pekrun, R.; Goetz, T. Achievement emotions in Germany and China: A cross-
cultural validation of the Academic Emotions Questionnaire—Mathematics. J. Cross-Cultural Psychol. 2007,
38, 302–309.
© 2018 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
... The associate editor coordinating the review of this manuscript and approving it for publication was Jeffrey Jonathan Yackley . impact on motivation, performance, and learning [2]. On the contrary, when experiencing emotions such as happiness or optimism, students' performance tends to improve [3]. ...
... course. 2 The first activity, unplugged, consists of sorting numbers using the pairwise ordering algorithm, while in the second exercise, plugged, the sections in the course related to conditionals were done. ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotions are essential drivers of the learning process, influencing motivation, performance, and problem-solving abilities. In the field of Computer Science, students often struggle with negative emotions during programming activities, impacting their performance and project quality. To address this challenge, there is a growing need to introduce Computational Thinking skills at the pre-university level. This study focuses on understanding the emotions experienced by primary and secondary school students during Computational Thinking activities, involving both plugged and unplugged tools. A significant performance difference was observed between primary and secondary education levels, with the latter outperforming the former. The research identifies specific associations between concepts and emotions, highlighting age-related differences with younger students exhibiting more positive emotions. While gender-based disparities in Computer Science perception exist in secondary education, there are no corresponding distinctions in emotional responses. The study reveals a gender-based effect, with girls showing reduced emotional responses and lower Computational Thinking performance than boys. In summary, this research underscores the profound role of emotions in learning, providing essential insights for tailoring educational strategies considering gender-specific and programming concept-related factors. It also connects lower emotional reactions to inferior results, emphasizing the importance of heightened emotional engagement.
... Several interventions have been successful in improving students' appreciation for feedback, or ability to take action (Carless, 2020;Carless & Winstone, 2020;Hoo et al., 2022;Little et al., 2023;Man et al., 2022;Winstone et al., 2019); however, few studies investigated the impact of their intervention on managing affect with respect to feedback (Little et al., 2023). It is especially important to support a student's ability to manage affect while training them in feedback literacy, as feedback-associated stress (feelings of anxiety, confusion, inadequacy, hopelessness, or injustice) may act as a barrier to developing other feedback literacy components (Lindsey & Cresswell, 2017;Lipnevich & Smith, 2018;Rowe & Fitness, 2018;Ryan & Henderson, 2018). As such, investigating if and how students already manage affect is first required to inform the development of appropriate training in feedback literacy. ...
... Mindful practices enable students to pay attention, accept a range of emotions associated with feedback, develop a sense of gratitude for learning opportunities, and increase their self-compassion, allowing students to use feedback to re-appraise their understanding. This may allow students to be open-minded and curious, which increases cognitive engagement (Rowe & Fitness, 2018) and can help students make judgments about what actions they need to take to increase their learning. ...
Article
Full-text available
Addressing feedback-associated stress as a barrier to learning is increasingly relevant to student success and well-being. Mindfulness practices support stress management for students during the academic feedback process. Even if students receive high-quality feedback, the receiving end of feedback can be stressful, perhaps raising feelings of anxiety, confusion, or inadequacy. Feedback literacy and mindfulness practices complement one another. Mindfulness can potentially support feedback literacy by focusing one’s attention on the tasks needed to address feedback, instead of being distracted by emotions triggered by feedback. This study, comprised of an online survey (n = 237) and focus groups (n = 6), assesses post-secondary students’ perceptions concerning feedback literacy, mindfulness, and stress, and their thoughts about digital mindfulness tools intended to support students experiencing feedback-associated stress. Recruitment of students was from courses in Health Sciences, Medical Sciences, Media Studies, and Law. The survey data demonstrate that students with greater mindfulness have significantly greater feedback literacy as well as lower stress. Focus group data shows that a broad range of affective and behavioral responses are shaped by students’ perceptions of their abilities, circumstances, and feedback itself. Although students expressed familiarity with mindfulness practices, few considered explicitly linking mindfulness to their feedback process. Nevertheless, students expressed interest regarding the development of digital mindfulness tools to alleviate feedback-associated stress and offered recommendations for implementation.
... Kebingungan merupakan sebuah emosi negatif yang berkaitan dengan perkembangan kognitif seseorang yang mana dapat menutup atau ide, menghambat proses belajar dan melumpuhkan proses berpikir dalam menemukan solusi terhadap suatu masalah (Rowe & Fitness, 2018). ...
Article
The COVID-19 pandemic requires teachers to conduct online learning. Monotonous online learning using the lecture method causes a lack of interaction between teachers and students, causing confusion. Thus we need a method that can facilitate the teacher to explain the material by involving students in learning. Thus, researchers use the lecture method plus question and answer as one way to overcome student confusion in online learning. In conducting research, researchers use descriptive qualitative methods that emphasize literature review. Giving the lecture method plus question and answer to overcome student confusion is a form of teacher responsibility to God. Through the provision of this method the teacher can show his dedication to God by being responsible for overcoming students' confusion. The lecture plus question and answer method can overcome student confusion by providing space for students to ask questions or express their confusion in both synchronous and asynchronous learning. Researchers suggest teachers to continue to encourage students not to feel ashamed and afraid to be considered stupid when students express their confusion. The teacher must also give appreciation in the form of applause, words of praise, and so on as a form of motivation to students so that they can express their confusion.Keywords: confusion, responsibility, lecture method plus question and answer
... Our findings indicate that the three-step digitization approach significantly reduces these negative emotions, aligning with literature that underscores the importance of minimizing such emotional experiences to enhance learning outcomes. While there is some discussion in the literature about the potential constructive roles of negative emotions under certain conditions [18,38], the primary focus of our study -and the broader consensus in educational research -is on the detrimental impact of these emotions on learning processes. By effectively reducing negative emotions, our digitization approach contributes to creating a more positive and conducive learning environment, which is crucial for academic success. ...
Article
Full-text available
This research work focuses on evaluating the development of our three-step digitization approach, designed to transform traditional lectures into engaging digital learning experiences. Aimed at offering an easy-to-use solution for educators, this approach addresses the pressing challenges of modern educational environments by enhancing accessibility, engagement, and effectiveness of learning through digital means. Quantitative analysis demonstrated a notable increase in knowledge gain for students engaged with the digital format (t(230) = -2.795, p = 0.006) over traditional methods. Additionally, it was observed that the online setting significantly improved concentration levels (t(230) = -5.801, p < .001) and reduced distractions (t(230) = 2.848, p = 0.005). Emotional assessments, based on the Achievement Emotions Questionnaire (AEQ), indicated an elevation in enjoyment (t(230) = -4.717, p < .001) and a reduction in anxiety (t(230) = 9.446, p < .001) within the digital learning environment. A substantial preference for the digital course format was expressed by 61.0% of participants, with 71.4% acknowledging its superior efficiency compared to 14.3% for traditional lectures. Qualitative feedback underscored the digital format's flexibility, comprehensive learning experience, and immediate feedback as key benefits. Nevertheless, nuances such as a preferred understanding in face-to-face interactions and the absence of a social component in digital settings were noted. To conclude, the findings from this study illuminate the significant advantages of the three-step digitization approach in meeting contemporary educational challenges. By facilitating an enhanced knowledge acquisition and fostering a supportive emotional climate, this approach signifies a promising direction for the future of medical education and beyond, fusing the convenience of digital solutions with the depth and engagement of traditional learning methodologies.
... Det är därför varken möjligt eller önskvärt att skala bort de känslomässiga aspekterna av undervisningen (Cox, 2022). Ett par studier visar att det kan vara en positiv upplevelse att gå igenom situationer som väcker negativa känslor (Ekström et al., 2021;Rowe & Fitness, 2018). Det kan påverka motivationen positivt och man kan lära sig något om sig själv och sina reaktioner. ...
Article
Full-text available
Syftet med denna forskningsöversikt är att bidra till fördjupade kunskaper om studenters upplevelser i känslomässigt påfrestande situationer i olika undervisningssammanhang. Litteratursökningen gjordes i flera olika databaser: ERIC, Web of Science och Google Scholar. Studier som publicerats mellan 2010 och 2021 och som utgörs av empiriska studier inkluderades, totalt 65 artiklar. Forskningsöversikten är en narrative review där fokus har varit att analysera artiklarnas resultat tematiskt. Detta har skett i två steg: först en kategorisering av vilka undervisningssituationer och ämnesinnehåll som väcker starka känslor och sedan en tolkande analys för att undersöka vad som kännetecknar de utmaningar studenter upplever i samband med dessa situationer. Resultaten visar att de undervisningssituationer och ämnesinnehåll som väcker negativa känslor och av studenter upplevs som känslomässigt påfrestande kan delas in i fyra övergripande kategorier: dissektioner, upplevelser under verksamhetsförlagd utbildning (VFU), ångest i samband med vissa ämnen och känsligt ämnesinnehåll. De utmaningar som studenter ställs inför att hantera rör deras egna emotionella och fysiska reaktioner, balansen mellan akademiskt och känslomässigt engagemang, professionella utmaningar och existentiella reflektioner. Studenter behöver stöd att hantera dessa utmaningar. Genom att lärare blir mer medvetna om vad som kan utgöra känslomässiga utmaningar för studenter kan vi stötta dem bättre och ge dem utrymme att bearbeta sina upplevelser. ENGLISH ABSTRACT When learning becomes difficult: A narrative review of students’ experiences of emotional challenges in teaching situations The purpose of this review is to deepen our knowledge about students’ experiences in emotionally challenging situations in different teaching contexts. The literature search was made in several different databases: ERIC, Web of Science and Google Scholar. Studies published between 2010 and 2021 and consisting of empirical studies were included, a total of 65 articles. The research overview is a type of narrative review where the focus has been to analyze the results of the articles thematically. The analysis has been conducted in two steps: first a categorization of which teaching situations and subject content that was emotionally distressful and then an interpretive analysis to investigate what characterizes the challenges students experience in connection with these situations. The results show that the teaching situations and subject content that are experienced by students as emotionally stressful can be divided into four general categories: dissections, experiences during work-based education (VFU), anxiety in connection with certain subjects and sensitive topics. The challenges that students are faced with relate to: their own emotional and physical reactions, the balance between academic and emotional engagement, professional challenges, and existential reflections. Students need support to deal with these challenges. By making teachers more aware of what may be emotionally challenging and distressful for students, we can better support them and give them space to process their experiences.
... We mentioned above that negotiation is considered a complex competitive procedure that can generate negative emotions (Van Kleef, 2008); however, in goal-driven situations specific negative emotions are experienced in order to help the participants achieve their goals. For example, anger motivates individuals to remove any obstructions and increases attention to goals and the actions of others, while fear increases concentration to a specific problem and absorbing information and instructions (Rowe & Fitness, 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Negotiation is omnipresent in people’s lives, whether the context be arguing with a spouse, making a business deal, or buying a new car. This study aimed to investigate the role of emotions in both economic and social-psychological outcomes of negotiation. Thirty-eight undergraduate psychology students formed dyads and were randomly assigned to either the experimental or the control group. In the experimental condition, positive emotions were induced through loving-kindness meditation (LKM) to both parties in each dyad, while in the control condition participants were not exposed to any emotional manipulation. Then, all participants were engaged in a cell phone negotiation simulation. The results showed that LKM led to increased levels of positive emotions and decreased levels of negative emotions. Nevertheless, the levels of experimental group’s negative emotions increased after the negotiation. In addition, the present study provided evidence that positive emotions are linked to increased cooperation potential and reduced possibilities for impasse, whilst it was found that the higher the levels of positive emotions, the less time is needed for negotiators to make a deal. Furthermore, it was found that negative emotions are associated with worse economic outcomes, while positive emotions experienced after the negotiation relate to participants’ perceptions about their self, the economic outcomes, and the overall satisfaction derived from the negotiation. Finally, it was found that distinct positive and negative emotions serve different functions. Practical implications for designing and implementing interventions focusing on cultivating positive negotiation skills in several contexts, such as workplace, close relationships, and parenting, are discussed.
Article
Numerous studies have explored internal factors such as cultural values and acquired knowledge accounting for how people engage with science. However, it remains unclear how external factors embedded in science texts relate to audience engagement. A content analysis of 298 text-based popular science news articles and their following 5852 reader comments was conducted in two Israeli audiences, science-minded and general readers, to explore how the accessibility strategies embedded in these articles relate to cognitive, affective, and behavioral engagement. Findings indicated similar patterns of relationship between accessibility strategies and engagement expressions for both audiences; however, the relationships were more pronounced for general readers compared to science-minded readers. Using jargon and a narrative writing style increased the odds of positive emotions in reader comments, whereas addressing socio-scientific issues increased the odds of cognitive expressions. These results may contribute to a fuller understanding of ways to drive meaningful public engagement.
Article
Full-text available
Recent work on natural categories suggests a framework for conceptualizing people's knowledge about emotions. Categories of natural objects or events, including emotions, are formed as a result of repeated experiences and become organized around prototypes (Rosch, 1978); the interrelated set of emotion categories becomes organized within an abstract-to-concrete hierarchy. At the basic level of the emotion hierarchy one finds the handful of concepts (love, joy, anger, sadness, fear, and perhaps, surprise) most useful for making everyday distinctions among emotions, and these overlap substantially with the examples mentioned most readily when people are asked to name emotions (Fehr & Russell, 1984), with the emotions children learn to name first (Bretherton & Beeghly, 1982), and with what theorists have called basic or primary emotions. This article reports two studies, one exploring the hierarchical organization of emotion concepts and one specifying the prototypes, or scripts, of five basic emotions, and it shows how the prototype approach might be used in the future to investigate the processing of information about emotional events, cross-cultural differences in emotion concepts, and the development of emotion knowledge.
Article
Full-text available
We examined how positive and negative affect covary within individuals over time and how patterns of association between affective traits and states relate to academic success across 4 years of university. Participants were 187 full-time first-year students at a large Canadian university who completed questionnaires about recent affective experiences in 6 waves across 4 years. Grade point average for each year of study was provided by the registrar’s office. Our analysis identified an adaptive pattern characterized by the maintenance of high positive affect (“chronic happiness”) and the cooccurrence of time-limited bouts of negative affect. Our results are consistent with findings showing productive consequences of experiencing positive and negative affect in tandem and the development of emotion regulation capacity across the transition to adulthood.
Article
Full-text available
Measurement instruments assessing multiple emotions during epistemic activities are largely lacking. We describe the construction and validation of the Epistemically- Related Emotion Scales, which measure surprise, curiosity, enjoyment, confusion, anxiety, frustration, and boredom occurring during epistemic cognitive activities. The instrument was tested in a multinational study of emotions during learning from conflicting texts (N = 438 university students from the United States, Canada, and Germany). The findings document the reliability, internal validity, and external validity of the instrument. A seven-factor model best fit the data, suggesting that epistemically-related emotions should be conceptualised in terms of discrete emotion categories, and the scales showed metric invariance across the North American and German samples. Furthermore, emotion scores changed over time as a function of conflicting task information and related significantly to perceived task value and use of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies.
Article
Full-text available
The emotions of teachers are considered relevant not only for their own well-being but also for the functioning of classrooms. Nevertheless, research on teacher emotions has been slow to emerge, and scales for their assessment via self-report are generally lacking. In the present research we developed four-item scales for three emotions considered most relevant in the context of teaching: enjoyment, anger, and anxiety (Teacher Emotions Scales, TES). Based on data of 944 teachers, we tested German and English language versions of the TES for reliability, internal and external validity, and cross-language equivalence, while exploring the utility of both a general and a student-group specific variant. All scales proved to be highly reliable, and confirmatory factor analysis supported internal validity by showing that three-factor models (enjoyment, anger, and anxiety) were superior to single-factor or two-factor (positive vs. negative affect) models. The external validation analyses provided consistent evidence for theoretically meaningful relations with teachers’ general affect, burnout, job satisfaction, and teacher self-efficacy. These findings were robust across multiple studies. In addition, consistent relationships with student ratings of teaching behaviors were found. Analyses of measurement invariance revealed that the English and the German language versions were fully structurally equivalent und displayed metric invariance.
Article
Emotion research has become a mature branch of psychology, with its own standardized measures, induction procedures, data-analysis challenges, and sub-disciplines. During the last decade, a number of books addressing major questions in the study of emotion have been published in response to a rapidly increasing demand that has been fuelled by an increasing number of psychologists whose research either focus on or involve the study of emotion. Very few of these books, however, have presented an explicit discussion of the tools for conducting research, despite the facts that the study of emotion frequently requires highly specialized procedures, instruments, and coding strategies, and that the field has reached a place where a large number of excellent elicitation procedures and assessment instruments have been developed and validated. The Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment corrects this oversight in the literature by organizing and detailing all the major approaches and instruments for the study of emotion. It is the most complete reference for methods and resources in the field, and will serve as a pragmatic resource for emotion researchers by providing easy access to a host of scales, stimuli, coding systems, assessment tools, and innovative methodologies. This handbook will help to advance research in emotion by encouraging researchers to take greater advantage of standard and well-researched approaches, which will increase both the productivity in the field and the speed and accuracy with which research can be communicated.
Article
This article addresses the question whether epistemic emotions (such as surprise, curiosity, uncertainty, and feelings of knowing) are in any sense inherently metacognitive. The paper begins with some critical discussion of a recent suggestion made by Joelle Proust, that these emotions might be implicitly or procedurally metacognitive. It then explores the theoretical resources that are needed to explain how such emotions arise and do their work. While there is a perennial temptation to think that epistemic emotions are somehow about the cognitive states of the person undergoing the emotion, we will see that such views can and should be resisted.