ArticlePDF Available

Ju/'hoan Women's Tracking Knowledge and Its Contribution to Their Husbands' Hunting Success

Authors:
  • Kalahari Peoples Fund

Abstract

1995 fieldwork in the Nyae Nyae area of northeastern Namibia indentified substantial contribution to their husbands' hunting success by Ju/'hoan women. This contribution came from a more acitve use of tracking knowledge than previously reported in anthropological literature on San and other hunter-gatherers. A mixed-gender team composed of an experienced hunter/tracker (Barclay) and an anthropologist/Ju/'hoan translator (Biesele) recorded in detail 1) the events of several collaborative hunting trips with Ju/'hoan husband-wife teams and 2) interview information about the kind and frequency of such collaboration. This team also recorded linguistic and technical details of hunting paraphernalia and techniques hitherto unelaborated in the literature, focusing on snares used by both men and women (to be presented elsewhere). The current paper presents the 1995 findings in the context of relevant information on gender and hunting from other societies. It invites colleagues to share ideas and information on this topic. In particular, it poses the question: does the current observed frequency of spousal cooperation in hunting Nyae Nyae 1)reflect very recent circumstances, or 2)does it, as some Ju/'hoan statements suggest, have substantial time-depth? The conclusion reached is that 1) and 2) are not necessarily opposing propositions, and that further investigation is needed.
Title Ju/'hoan Women's Tracking Knowledge and Its Contribution to
Their Husbands' Hunting Success
Author(s) BIESELE, Megan; BARCLAY, Steve
Citation African study monographs. Supplementary issue (2001), 26:
67-84
Issue Date 2001-03
URL https://dx.doi.org/10.14989/68407
Right
Type Departmental Bulletin Paper
Textversion publisher
Kyoto University
African Study Monographs, Suppl.26: 67-84, March 2001 67
JU/’HOAN WOMEN’S TRACKING KNOWLEDGE AND ITS
CONTRIBUTION TO THEIR HUSBANDS’ HUNTING SUCCESS
Megan BIESELE
Kalahari Peoples Fund and Texas A & M University
Steve BARCLAY
Kalahari Peoples Fund
ABSTRACT 1995 fieldwork in the Nyae Nyae area of northeastern Namibia identified
a substantial contribution to their husbands’ hunting success by Ju/’hoan women. This
contribution came from a more active use of tracking knowledge than previously reported
in anthropological literature on San and other hunter-gatherers. A mixed-gender team
composed of an experienced hunter/tracker (Barclay) and an anthropologist/Ju/’hoan
translator (Biesele) recorded in detail 1) the events of several collaborative hunting trips
with Ju/’hoan husband-wife teams and 2) interview information about the kind and fre-
quency of such collaboration. This team also recorded linguistic and technical details of
hunting paraphernalia and techniques hitherto unelaborated in the literature, focusing
on snares used by both men and women (to be presented elsewhere). The current paper
presents the 1995 findings in the context of relevant information on gender and hunting
from other societies. It invites colleagues to share ideas and information on this topic.
In particular, it poses the question: does the current observed frequency of spousal co-
operation in hunting in Nyae Nyae 1)reflect very recent circumstances, or 2)does it, as
some Ju/’hoan statements suggest, have substantial time-depth? The conclusion reached
is that 1) and 2) are not necessarily opposing propositions, and that further investigation
is needed.
Key Words: Africa; San; Indigenous knowledge; Hunting; Tracking; Gender
INTRODUCTION
I. Introduction by Biesele
In 1971 I had the good fortune to be present on a gathering trip with Ju/’hoan
San women when a wildebeest was spotted grazing with a herd of Herero cattle not
far from Dobe, Botswana. Several women hurried back to Dobe to inform hunters,
who soon arrived and began to track the animal. I was excited to witness this
example of what one of my teachers, Pat Draper, had referred to as !Kung women
reporting to men on “the state of the bush.” During the 1970s and 1980s I had
a chance to participate in a number of hunting trips in Botswana, which, besides
myself, involved only men. In the late 1980s, I took part in a trip which involved
both men and women, near /Aotcha, Namibia–a gathering trip which was also a
68 M. BIESELE AND S. BARCLAY
joint tracking expedition for a giraffe which had been wounded with a poison arrow
but whose spoor had been lost.
It was not until 1995, however, that I was asked to accompany a husband and
wife as they tracked together (also near /Aotcha). One factor which made this
participation possible was the presence of my husband, Steve Barclay, whose own
tracking and hunting knowledge had been honed on his family’s South Texas ranch
conservation projects, and in hunting for the cooking pot. Another factor was that
I was recovering, at the time, from a broken ankle, and was still using a cane to
walk. Steve and I had requested to join a hunter, N!ani, on a tracking trip, and
because of my ankle his wife Di//xao had offered to go along, to walk back to camp
with me in case I couldn’t keep up. This set of circumstances allowed us to make
the discovery that Di//xao was fully as competent as her husband in tracking, and
was his valued collaborator. (Luckily, I also discovered that I could keep up their
pace, and Di//xao didn’t have to take me back to camp. Afterwards I threw away
my cane.)
Steve and I spent most of a July winter’s day trotting closely behind N!ani
and Di//xao as they headed rapidly away from /Aotcha. They were making for
an area south of /Aotcha pan where many kudus, warthogs, and other animals
spend daytime. N!ani and Di//xao tracked parallel to each other and about 30
feet apart, close enough to share information but far enough apart to take in
a swath of ground which afforded multiple options in trails to follow. We saw
them communicate, often wordlessly, with hand signs, about the most promising
tracks to follow. They changed tracks at least 20 times in four hours of fast-paced
movement, acting as a smoothly communicating unit to choose the most recent
and potentially productive tracks in a welter of animal spoor of many kinds. They
stooped and felt faeces to see how close the animal might be, weighed variables of
the size of animals versus the difficulty of getting close enough to them for a shot,
and generally processed, together, immense amounts of current information while
covering many kilometers of ground. When we did come up close to the family of
warthogs we were then tracking, Di//xao fell back so that her husband could get
a good shot with his bow and poisoned arrow. But it was clear that the cerebral
part of the stalking had been very much a joint activity, one made immensely
more profitable by the multiplier effect of two observers working in parallel who
communicated and made decisions seemingly as one.
I kept thinking that it was as if they had been doing this together all their lives.
In fact, these middle-aged people told us later that they HAD been doing it most
of their married lives. I knew, too, that N!ani had a trusted male hunting partner,
/Kunta, as well, and that the two men worked together in tracking much as did
N!ani and Di//xao. Seeing this married couple in action, however, was the impetus
which led me and Steve to refocus our inquiry into Ju/’hoan hunting techniques
and terminology. We learned from talking to other couples that what we had
observed with N!ani and Di//xao was far from rare. We also had the opportunity
to accompany another couple, /’Angn!ao and N!aq’e, as they tracked together, and
to interview them later about this kind of teamwork between Ju/’hoan spouses.
What we learned caused us to look back at the ethnographic literature on the
Ju/’hoansi-!Kung and other hunter-gatherers with new eyes. Did other groups
Ju/’hoan Women’s Tracking Knowledge 69
report anything like what we had seen on women’s participation in tracking? Were
colleagues’ earlier statements that they had no information on Ju/’hoan women
hunting to be taken as definitive, i.e. were Steve and I witnessing something new
in Ju/’hoan life, linked perhaps to sedentization and other recent changes, or had
this kind of collaboration previously just gone unobserved? (After all, few of my
colleagues were themselves skilled trackers, and the white hunters who did have
the knowledge and went out with San men not only did not speak the language
but likely never dreamed of asking women to accompany their husbands on such a
hunt.) With our combination of tracking experience and expertise in the Ju/’hoan
language, however, along with our own status as partners and my game leg, maybe
Steve and I had stumbled–literally–on the chance to see a partner activity with
some time-depth in Ju/’hoan communities.
Accordingly, we focused our limited period of Namibian fieldwork (a few weeks in
the southern winter of 1995) on questions about women’s tracking skills and their
contribution to male hunting success. We also studied the specifics of hunting
techniques used by both men and women, adding to the Ju/’hoan ethnographic
literature a bit of new vocabulary. This work was possible thanks both to Steve,
who knew the right questions to ask, and to !Xuma, my longtime language teacher,
who traveled from Dobe, Botswana to Namibia to help us pin down linguistic
details from the hunting narratives we had put on tape. Much of our success, we
feel, was due to our being a mixed-gender team ourselves.
So we offer the following experiences now to our colleagues for commentary,
hoping we may make some contribution to what Agnes Estioko-Griffin and P. Bion
Griffin called for in 1981 in “Woman the Hunter: the Agta.” Commenting on the
need to reexamine available ethnographic literature concerning women and hunting
worldwide, they point out that “We may have overlooked off-the-cuff comments
which suggest that women were hunting in many situations...Part of the problem is
in the definition of hunting itself and in an ill-defined separation of big from small
game...(For instance, in Mbuti data from the Ituri forest) no hint was given that
women use bow and arrow or spear, or that they kill buffalo or elephant. However,
one might ask again...That (women) also collect plant foods is beside the point.
Men do, too.” (1981: 140-41).
II. Introduction by Barclay
The purpose of tracking in south Texas brush is to study patterns of animal
movement, using evidence like tracks, trails, hog wallows, scrapes, etc. All physical
evidence is used to know where to set blinds, the most common form of hunting
in this area. The brush is too thick to stalk animals, as they will hear and smell
a person long before one can get a clear shot. Blinds need to be set according to
the terrain and the height of brush and tree lines, for visibility, as overgrazing and
previous cultivation have caused bush encroachment. Though some do hunt with
bow and arrow in south Texas, the majority use guns, in contrast to the poisoned
arrows and spears used in the Kalahari.
70 M. BIESELE AND S. BARCLAY
The San’s tracking, also in contrast, is actual stalking of a moving animal. This
is done in order to get close enough to put the shaft of a small, poisoned arrow into
contact with its bloodstream. Thereafter there is more leisured tracking, observing
the signs of poisoning and eventual death, with the aim of catching up to the animal
in time either to dispatch it with knives, clubs and spears or to take possession of
its meat before other carnivores do. While making the original stalk, San hunters
change from one track to another ceaselessly, looking for the freshest spoor. When
they get close they choose one, weapons come out and the hunting partner, when
there is one, falls back for the hunter to get the shot. If both are shooting they
circle, alternate, and otherwise enable each other to get advantageous shots.
Sometimes (rarely), two or three hunters crouch in a shallow depression blind
near a pan where the animals come to drink. They stay in the blind all night,
with a small fire for safety and warmth, as the animals drink in the morning.
Blind hunting is now quite rare: Ju/’hoan people today regard it as ineffective.
Alison Brooks (1978), and Aron Crowell and Robert Hitchcock (1978) write that
blind hunting used to be quite common. Polly Wiessner told us that “With the
intrusion of cattle into the area, game became more shy of settlements and (blind
hunting) became more infrequent. One can see the remains of game blinds around
all waterholes and Ju/’hoansi can tell you much about hunting from blinds. Still,
animals shot from blinds must be tracked until they die.” (Wiessner, P. pers.
comm., December, 1999).
The Ju/’hoansi never use bait, as we do in Texas (deer corn, etc) to slow animals
down and to keep them in open areas long enough to give opportunities for a shot,
partly because they do not have surplus food to use as bait. But also this is because
they must slip up closely (to within 25-75 feet of an animal) due to the shooting
range of their light arrows. Whether stalking or using blinds, they use persistent
teamwork and constant calculation and recalculation of evidence. We observed,
as did Louis Liebenberg, the three distinct levels of tracking he characterizes as
simple, systematic, and speculative (1990: 29ff).
Simple tracking involves following a trail of clear track prints. This situation is
not frequent in the dry Kalahari, so systematic tracking must be used to interpret
signs and conditions if track prints are not easy to follow. For example, a bent-
over grass leaf in a track print which has not yet straightened up may indicate
the animal is near, or one may see blood from the arrow-wound on leaves, or may
be able to determine the closeness of the animal by seeing and feeling whether
the faeces are dry, wet, warm, etc. If the animal is walking at its ease, it will be
scattering faeces occasionally. If it is scared and running, it will hold its faeces
until it reaches a stopping point. And there are many other clues which can be
used to inform the progress of more difficult tracking.
Speculative tracking, the next level, involves a whole new way of thinking, with
the creation of a working hypothesis, involving knowledge of animal behavior and
terrain. It’s here that San people excel, with a combination of inductive and
deductive reasoning outsiders have yet to understand. The main point to stress
is that this hypothetical, speculative activity is done equally well by men and
women, and this is why Ju/’hoan women join men as efficient hunting partners
though, according to them, they rarely carry weapons.
Ju/’hoan Women’s Tracking Knowledge 71
Liebenberg, the only researcher on San hunting whose work supports our obser-
vation that women do some tracking, wrote to us that he has always wanted to go
hunting with women hunters he knows personally, but has not yet had the chance.
He mentions the role of women in passing in The Art of Tracking (1990: 4, 55, 73),
but says “in a sense it does not do justice to the fundamental role I believe tracking
plays in hunter-gatherer understanding of nature. Both men and women have a
scientific understanding of animals and plants based on the interpretation of signs
in nature. The interpretation of signs forms the basis of any theoretical science,
including modern physics ... Only by showing that women play an indispensable
role in the collective tracking knowledge of hunter-gatherers can one demonstrate
the fundamental role of tracking in their culture.” (pers. comm., July, 1997).
After the day we spent with Di//xao and N!ani and another with /’Angnao and
his wife N!aq’e, I had one full day of tracking as a non-weapon-carrying partner
for /’Angn!ao, when Megan was busy on another project. Though I knew little of
the Ju/’hoan language, that experience spoke volumes to me about the interactive
nature of Ju/’hoan hunting partnerships, whether with women or with other men.
I began to see how extremely valuable the already-honed communicative skills
of a married couple could be in this context. Interviews with other Ju/’hoan
men and women reinforced the impression Megan and I were getting that spousal
collaboration in tracking is (at least today) far from rare, and may even be fairly
routine. Yet to my knowledge at that time, the only previous researcher who had
remarked upon it was Liebenberg. (Unfortunately, his book, The Art of Tracking:
The Origin of Science, based on extensive first-hand experience in the Kalahari,
to this day remains little known outside of southern Africa.)
So Megan and I asked ourselves 1) whether the collaborative tracking might
reflect a big change in Ju/’hoan life just over the last few years or 2) whether this
kind of collaboration might be a fundamental part of longterm Ju/’hoan history.
Realizing that we were not in a position to formulate or test rigorous hypotheses
concerning this topic, we nevertheless wished to draw attention to the fact that
there might be modern reasons for cooperative hunting.
Thus with this paper we would like to invite interested colleagues to share ideas
and information on spousal cooperation in hunting among foragers. In what fol-
lows, we present a brief survey of other ethnographic literature, including that
on San peoples, which mentions foraging women’s participation in hunting. We
then present the remainder of our observations from our 1995 interviews and hunt-
ing trips, comment on them, and offer some preliminary thoughts on the above
question.
ANTHROPOLOGICAL LITERATURE ON WOMEN AND HUNITING
I. “Man the Hunter” and the CHAGS Conferences
There is an extensive, fairly recent literature on sex roles and egalitarianism on
record with regard to hunter-gatherers which has relevance for this paper (see for
example Barnard, 1983; Begler, 1978; Bernt, 1978; Biesele, 1993; Endicott, K.,
72 M. BIESELE AND S. BARCLAY
1981; Gale, 1978; Guenther, 1983-4; Imamuru-Hayaki, 1996; Kent, 1993; Testart,
1986). There is also a great deal of published material on the ecology of hunting and
gathering to provide background on knowledge and use of a range of environments
by hunter-gatherers (Bicchieri, 1969; Hawkes, 1991; Hewlett, 1996; Hitchcock et
al., 1996; Kitanishi, 1995; Lee & DeVore, eds., 1976; Nonaka, 1996; O’Connell et
al., 1983; Yellen & Lee, 1976). Some of the literature on settlement, sedentism, and
change has relevance to the sociological study of subsistence foraging (eg. Draper
& Kranichfeld, 1990, Hitchcock, 1995; Hitchcock, 1998; Osaki, 1984; Sugawara,
1991; Wilmsen, 1989). Two recent papers which focus from different angles on
traditional San hunting may also be mentioned (Ikeya, 1989; Thomas, 1990).
But neither the “Man the Hunter” nor the successor CHAGS Conferences had
much of a literature on which to draw in considering the question of women’s
hunting–if this question was considered at all. Aside from a few comments on
the importance of women’s roles in the preservation of fish and meat, for instance
by Gould for the Northern Territory of Australia (Lee & DeVore, 1968: 64), the
conference on Man the Hunter held in Chicago in 1965 touched not at all on
foraging women’s possible roles in hunting. In fact it barely acknowledged women’s
economic role in such societies. By 1981 scholars had reacted to this omission by
publishing papers which showed the substantial contribution of women’s gathering
(and food-processing) to many foraging economies (Hunn, 1981; Dahlberg, 1981;
Hawkes & O’Connell, 1981).
Also in that year, Agnes Estioko-Griffin and P. Bion Griffin published “Woman
the Hunter: the Agta” (1981) describing the predominating role in hunting of some
groups of Agta women in northeastern Luzon, Philippines. They called attention
in that paper to several earlier works which discussed female hunting, such as
Jenness, 1922; Flannery, 1932; 1935; Fox, 1952; Goodale, 1971; Briggs, 1974;
Hammond & Jablow, 1976. They also suggested a critical reexamination of other
ethnographic literature, where clues to women’s hunting may have been missed
due to the prevailing academic model of men as hunters and women as gatherers.
Burch (1975) and Meehan (1982) showed that in fishing and shellfishing, respec-
tively, women often play major roles, even if they do not hunt in the usual sense of
the term. By 1990 and the VIth International Conference on Hunting and Gath-
ering Societies, women’s hunting, in the context of gender awareness, had become
a “key issue” in hunter-gatherer studies (Burch and Ellanna, 1994, reporting on
the 1990 CHAGS held in Alaska). This conference was immediately followed by
the 1990 Inuit Studies Conference, also held in Fairbanks. In that conference, Ann
Fienup Riordan chaired a special session on Inuit ethnography and anthropologi-
cal theory. That session resulted in a special issue of Etudes/Inuit/Studies (1990,
14[1-2]) in which both Riordan and Barbara Bodenhorn looked specifically at the
ways in which they thought their own material contributed to gender theorizing.
Bodenhorn’s seminal article, “I am not the great hunter: my wife is”: Inupiat
and anthropological models of gender“ appeared in that special issue. Marion Mc-
Creedy’s paper in the Burch and Ellanna volume, “The Arms of the Dibouka”
(1994), first presented at Alaska, outlined Biaka Pygmy women’s roles in doing
most of the hunting for their group.
Ju/’hoan Women’s Tracking Knowledge 73
As Burch and Ellanna stated (1994: 12): “In a few societies, females have hunted
big game, in others they have cooperated with males in the pursuit of big game, and
in quite a few they have hunted small game and have fished, with or without male
assistance. Even where they have not actively participated in hunting, females
often have played a major part in the rituals that have helped to ensure hunting
success...One cannot really make sense of the division of labor along gender lines
in a given society without reference to the allocation of power and responsibility,
ritual, symbolism, communication, and emotional expression. The straightforward
focus of many early gender studies on the amount of time males and females spent
in different activities is no longer sufficient.”
In short, Burch and Ellanna called for more nuanced and contextualized ap-
proaches to the question of gender roles. Accordingly, they began the anthology
they drew from the Alaska conference with two papers focused on gender roles
and power in hunting, in gathering, and in foraging economies as wholes (Mc-
Creedy’s paper mentioned above, and Henry S. Sharp’s paper on the Chipewyan,
both 1994). Taken together, McCreedy’s and Sharp’s papers are a study in the
extreme flexibility of gender definition in foraging economies and social systems,
ranging from a high degree of egalitarianism (the Biaka) to high male dominance
(the Chipewyan). If Biaka women hunt so much and Chipewyan women do not do
so, what might this mean in terms of the relationship between this valued activity
and women’s status relative to men?
II. Diversity and Women’s Hunting: the Agta and Ache
In 1995, Robert L. Kelly’s survey of hunter-gatherer diversity posed the status
question another way: “If men acquire higher status by virtue of the fact that they
hunt, why, then, do women not hunt more than they apparently do?” He dismissed
the earlier assumption that women simply do not have the strength and endurance
to hunt large game by citing a number of well-documented cases of their doing
just that, and pointed out as well that hunting requires knowledge and patience
as much as it does strength. Among the cases he cited was one in the Peruvian
Amazon (Romanoff, 1983); two in the Arctic (Landes, 1938: 137; Watanabe, 1968:
74) and several among Pygmy women who participate in communal game drives
(Turnbull, 1965; Bailey & Aunger, 1989). These and other cases, notably those of
the Agta and Ache, he used in a contextualized discussion of the roles of childcare
and breastfeeding practices in determining division of labor in regard to hunting.
Estioko-Griffin and Griffin (1981; 1985) and Estioko-Griffin (1986) present what
Kelly calls “the exception that proves the rule” in ethnographic studies of women
hunters. About 85% of Philippine Agta women hunt, and they hunt the same
quarry as men. Agta women hunt in groups and with dogs, and have a 31%
success rate as opposed to 17% for men. Their rates are even better when they
combine forces with men: mixed hunting groups have a full 41% success rate among
the Agta. M. Goodman and her co-workers (including Estioko-Griffin) conclude
in their 1985 book that Agta women are able to hunt because of their childcare
practices: they can leave their children with others, and they tend to hunt within
74 M. BIESELE AND S. BARCLAY
10 km. of camp so they can return at the end of the day. Breastfeeding children
are taken along in a sling. But most of the serious hunting by women is done by
those who have come to the end of childbearing. Bion Griffin told Kelly that while
younger women with children may hunt if the opportunity arises, it is the older
women who set out to hunt intentionally (Kelly, 1995: 269).
Ache women of Venezuela, similarly, show less efficiency as hunters if they are
still breastfeeding. Hurtado et al (1985) point out that while hunting for these
women is not more strenous than it would be for men, it IS more strenuous for
their children, who put constraints on the lengths to which their mothers will go
to bring down an animal. Gathering can be interrupted by children’s needs, but
following an animal, overnight if need be, is much less compatible with childcare
and breastfeeding.
Kelly makes a final point of relevance to our discussion of women’s hunting in
referring to the choices made by some older women among the Hadza, Mbuti,
and Ju/’hoan. Citing Kristen Hawkes et al (“Hardworking Hadza Grandmothers,”
1989) he surveys alternatives to older women’s hunting: if women are sometimes
prevented (by childrearing) from learning to hunt earlier in their lives, they may
opt to forage for their daughters so they can do so (Hadza) or take care of their
children (Ju/’hoansi). Mbuti mothers decide whether to participate in hunting
based on comparative returns from hunting and wage labor. All these strategies,
importantly, can become socially defined and passed on symbolically to further
generations (Kelly, 1995: 269).
III. Women and the Technology of Net Hunting
Several writers have remarked upon the prominence of women’s roles in Mbuti
net hunting as opposed to that with bow and arrow. Tanno (1976) and Harako
(1976) described this net hunting in some detail, and Ichikawa summarized its basic
principles in the same year (1976: 28). Mbuti women’s participation is critical, as
they serve as beaters and also carry the captured animals. Using this information
in contrast with his own observations in the Central Kalahari, Tanaka (1980) posits
a continuum between extremes of individualistic male bow hunting in the dry, open
Kalahari, with its sparse but large-size game populations, vs. the communal, men-
women-children net hunting of medium-to-small game in the lush, limited-vision
environment of Central Africa inhabited by Pygmy groups and their prey.
Taking a center point along this continuum, Tanaka suggests, are the Mbote
(Bambote) of the western shore of Lake Tanganyika in the current Democratic
Republic of Congo, reported upon by Terashima (1980). Three environmentally-
linked characteristics of Mbote hunting distinguish it from that of the Mbuti: 1)
irregularities in the Mbote environment mean that hunting can only take place in a
few areas, so more territory is required, 2) sparse tree cover dictates that the Mbote
carry along poles to support the nets, so they use only half-circle net arrangements
rather than full circles as do the Mbuti. This in turn puts more stress on the role
of the beaters, so that 3) men as well as women act as beaters, and “the entire
group participates as a single unit,” according to Tanaka (1980: 167).
Ju/’hoan Women’s Tracking Knowledge 75
This three-point continuum locates Kalahari San groups, partly due to their
terrain, far from both communal hunting and from participation in hunting by
women. Tanaka (pers.comm., 1996) stated that he had no report of Kade San
women hunting or helping their husbands to hunt, and others, eg. Osaki, who
work in the Central Kalahari of Botswana, said the same thing (pers. comm.,
1996)
In contrast are our recent observations on Ju/’hoan women’s involvement in
hunting, identified both within and outside their society as chiefly the work of
men. Our experience suggests that Ju/’hoan women play informational, commu-
nicational, and cognitive as well as ritual roles in enabling successful hunting. We
begin the discussion by outlining relevant comments in earlier literature on San
and Ju/’hoan women. Though this is surely not an exhaustive listing of references,
it does give an idea of what have been the generally accepted views about San
gender roles in regard to hunting and the division of labor.
IV. Earlier Literature on San and other Women’s Hunting
Much of the earlier San literature emphasized that women’s roles in regard to
hunting were mostly ritual ones. The prevailing point of view was that hunting
proscriptions involving women in these societies, emphasizing the need not to dam-
age men’s hunting prospects, were of primary importance. Less often, the positive
effects of female ritual participation were brought out. Lorna Marshall (1976: 130)
states that !Kung (Ju/’hoan) women never hunt, and stresses the people’s feeling
that femaleness must be kept apart from male hunting activities. In Marshall 1999
(xxxiv, 188, 192, 200-201), she outlines the positive ritual role of girls at menarche
in conferring hunting luck and prowess on the young men of the group. Similarly,
Heinz (1975: 8) describes !Ko (!Xoo) girls at menarche shooting an arrow into a
gemsbok-brow-skin target, for the purpose of “bringing luck to the weapons of the
men.” Many clues in different Khoisan societies, in fact, link the power of the
newly adult woman to the health of the land and to abundance of hunted and
gathered food.
These ritual clues liken San attitudes to women and hunting to a number of
other world examples. Barbara Bodenhorn’s 1990 article, “I am not the great
hunter: my wife is,” describes the ritual role of Inupiat women in attracting the
animals so that their husbands can kill them. So appreciative are the men of this
role, that they categorize the women as hunters (1990: 5). Similarly, Baka women
as described by Veronique Joiris play important ritual, though not physical roles
in elephant hunting. They act as diviners and as mediators with the world of the
animals on behalf of the men (1990).
But over the last few decades practical as well as ritual roles of women in hunt-
ing have been discussed. After hearing the version of the present paper presented
at CHAGS 8, for example, Barbara Bodenhorn concurred with Ernest Burch that
Inupiat women’s knowledge of their environment might have to be even greater
than Inupiat men’s, as the women are the ones who locate the animals and then
drive them to the men (pers. comm., October, 1998). Ernest Burch recalled to us
76 M. BIESELE AND S. BARCLAY
historical writings on northern Alaska confirming the roles of women in tracking
their men to the sites of seal kills in order to drag the seals home (pers. comm.,
October, 1998: see also Bockstoce, ed., in Bibliography). In Bender’s 1993 book
“Landscape: Politics and Perspectives,” Bodenhorn includes a chapter (“Public
spaces, private places: public and private revisited on the North Slope of Alaska”)
which expands one aspect of her 1990 paper with particular reference to Mauss’
seasonal variations essay. She explores there the extent to which the marital re-
lationship is constructed as a hunting relationship. Tuvaqati, for instance, which
is the non-gendered term for spouse, literally means companion on a hunting trip.
Spring hunting in particular is a time when families are meant to hunt together.(B.
Bodenhorn, pers. comm., May 24, 2000).
The practical involvement of women in San hunting has also been mentioned
by a number of authors. In 1975 Patricia Draper stated categorically that !Kung
women do not hunt, but stressed their importance to male hunters in bringing
back news of animals and signs observed in the bush (1975: 82-88). H.-J. Heinz
and M. Lee (1978) mentioned that !Xoo women hunted. Richard Lee in his 1979
book, while stating that “basically (Ju/’hoan women) leave hunting to the men”
(1979: 235), nevertheless acknowledged that women participate actively in hunting
preparation discussions, sharing information gleaned from gathering trips (1979:
210).
Lee’s work, and Lorna Marshall’s work, also bring up questions about the rela-
tionship between gender and the ownership of the means of production among the
Ju/’hoansi. Women can, like anyone else, be owners of arrows, which fact allows
them to become distributors of meat from animals shot with those arrows. Boden-
horn points out that, similarly, in Barrow, a woman can own whaling guns–or can
give guns to quite wide-flung kin–which generates rights to shares in the take in
which those means of production played a part (pers. comm., May 24, 2000).”
Where cooperative hunting among Ju/’hoansi is concerned, some extremely rel-
evant fieldwork carried out by Alison Brooks and Aron Crowell in the mid-1970s
must be mentioned. They followed husbands and wives along the same track about
ten minutes apart, and compared notes of what tracks were identified and what
information derived from them. Wives of excellent hunters identified and drew
information from tracks of important food animals, whereas wives of non-hunters
commented on mouse tracks and toad tracks, and missed many economically im-
portant ones (A. Crowell and A. Brooks, pers. comm., January, 2000).
Polly Wiessner additionally commented that, in her experience, couples who
cooperate are only those who engage in traditional bow and arrow hunting. Women
do not assist men in non-traditional forms of hunting like hunting with guns or
with spears on horseback.” However, she continued, “in southern communities
of Nyae Nyae (Namibia), it is interesting that men have many arrows in their
quivers from their wives–their wives do not make them but receive them in xaro
(ritual gift-giving) from male relatives, or, more frequently, from their husbands.
If the hunter kills an animal with an arrow that he or somebody else has given
his wife, the meat belongs to his wife. When I photographed arrows at //Aru
and other southern Nyae Nyae communities in 1997, I was surprised how many
had been made by husbands and given in xaro to their wives, a practice that
Ju/’hoan Women’s Tracking Knowledge 77
transferred the ownership of the meat of the animal killed with that arrow to
the hunter’s wife. She would then have the right to distribute it. I had not
encountered this practice frequently in northern Nyae Nyae communities nor in
Ju/’hoansi communities on the Botswana side of the border. When I asked if this
was a new or old practice, I was told that people’s grandparents had done the same.
However, arrow xaro between spouses is not strict convention. Some men chose
to xaro arrows to their wives and others did not. Unfortunately I did not ask if
arrow xaro between spouses was correlated with cooperation in other phases of the
hunt.”(pers. comm., December, 1999 and January, 2000). Accompanying figures
provided by Polly Wiessner showed that fully one-third (33 of 98) of the arrows
contained in the quivers of five actively hunting men at //Aru in 1997 belonged to
their wives.
Marjorie Shostak’s 1981 book Nisa: The Life and Words of a !Kung Woman
reported a first-hand account of a young girl killing first a baby steenbok by herself
(1981: 94) and later a young kudu (1981: 102). One of the authors (Biesele) also
heard accounts of young Ju/’hoan girls during the 1970s and 1980s in Botswana
hunting, usually small or immature animals. George Silberbauer reported (1981:
214) that adolescent G/ui boys and girls alike set traps for gallinaceous birds
and small mammals. Susan Kent (1993: 489-90) described women at Kutse, in
eastern Botswana, hunting small animals with snares or clubbing them with digging
sticks, though they “do not hunt with bows and arrows.” Kutse women also
routinely make and set bird snares. If their husbands are away, women may check
traps and butcher any animals captured. Also, “some husbands and wives forage
together, both collecting wild plants and checking traps.” Of great interest is
Kent’s observation that after 1988, when the idea of using plastic threads from
mealie-meal bags to make snares for traps caught on among Kutse women, animals
caught in such traps belonged to the women, not the men. From then on, ritual
scarifications believed to bring animals into these traps could be performed for
womenaswellasformen.
Liebenberg (1990: 4, 73) provides, to our minds, the most persuasive perspective
on San women’s participation in hunting. With his permission, we quote in full
two paragraphs from his chapter on hominid evolution in The Art of Tracking:
The Origin of Science:
It has been pointed out that the traditional “Man the Hunter” formulation of
human evolution, with the emphasis on hunting and male dominance, is an out-
come of male bias on the part of anthropologists working in a male-dominated
world. These anthropologists have played down the evolutionary importance of the
woman’s role: their economic contribution in gathering and sharing food; their role
as primary socialisers and pivotal members of the family unit; and their role as
information “bankers” in the success of male hunters. Earlier anthropological bias
also denies women’s direct involvement in hunting (Dahlberg, 1981). As far as the
art of tracking is concerned, we should remember that both men and women were
involved.
The art of tracking, as practised by contemporary trackers of the Kalahari, is
a science that requires fundamentally the same intellectual abilities as modern
78 M. BIESELE AND S. BARCLAY
physics and mathematics. It may well have been the first creative science practised
by the earliest members of anatomically modern Homo sapiens who had modern
intellects. Natural selection for an ability to interpret tracks and signs may have
played a significant role in the evolution of the scientific intellect.
This perspective provides an admirable transition to the last few observations
we wish to report from our work with Ju/’hoansi in 1995. Following our initial
tracking trips with two Ju/’hoan couples, we did interviews with women, men,
and mixed groups. As is usual with Ju/’hoansi, many answers came in the form
of stories. Thus we heard contextualized information, data embedded reliably in
mnemonically-powerful narratives–information, thus, with perhaps more substance
than mere brief answers to a questionnaire.
We heard that “many women,” including those of the previous generation, had
“for a long time” accompanied their husbands on hunting trips. Some took nursing
infants along, as they do when gathering. These women are said to be excellent
trackers. They share signals with the men but the men are “in charge”–in the
sense that they carry the weapons, that the poisoned arrow has the power, in the
last analysis, to bring the animal down, and that both sexes respect that.
While gathering, as well, women often find priority tracks and spoor, as well as
baby animals that require immediate response by the hunters to obtain access to
the mother. Baby animals are used, often by women, to lure the mothers to men
so they can shoot them. The babies are tied or their legs are broken or bitten
to make them cry out to call the mother. Men and women alike cautioned that
this strategy is never used with gemsboks, as the females have horns that are as
formidable as those of males. Kudu cows and calves are preferred, as kudu cows
are hornless and relatively non-aggressive.
Women sometimes lead the hunt and the tracking/stalking until the game is
closely approached. When the hunter draws the arrows from his quiver, he takes
the lead. Women (as do other male hunting partners) drop back to be out of the
way, and all use hand signs from then on to be as silent as possible.
From hands-on demonstrations by both men and women of snaring techniques,
we learned how extremely terrain-specific are the various sorts of snares. It was
clear that women knew as well as men how to set snares. In one memorable
interview at Baraka, Namibia, /’Angn!ao’s ten year old daughter, //Ukxa, raced
to collect the materials for a demonstration snare as her father described them to
us. She put them together expertly as he talked: it was clear that she knew as
well as her father and brother how to construct, set, and check this snare.
It was clear also that both men and women Ju/’hoansi felt that hunting in mixed
groups was likely to be productive. There was a good feeling about their positive
descriptions, one that went beyond the occasional jokes made about husbands and
wiveshavingtogotosuchgreatlengthstohavetimealonetogether. Instead,
women’s sagacity and judgment and strength were celebrated by all. There was
just no male arrogance noted: the appreciation of women’s contributions here
appeared absolutely genuine.
These observations, though brief, point some very interesting directions for fur-
ther study. Our evidence is admittedly scanty, and we suggest further study by
ourselves or others whenever possible. Our observations bear similarity to those
Ju/’hoan Women’s Tracking Knowledge 79
of Steven Romanoff in the Peruvian Amazon, who wrote on “Women as Hunters
among the Matses” there (1983: 339-43). Matses couples working together provide
more meat than men alone. Men and women alike capture a collared peccary, a
large catch. Women accompany their husbands on about half of their hunting days.
Women gather on these trips but also take an active role in hunting. They spot
game, take part in the chase, retrieve arrows, bring water to flood armadillo holes,
encourage dogs, strike animals with sticks or machetes, participate in orienting the
party, and carry meat home. In the case of polygynous men, only one wife at a
time hunts with her husband, a fact which seems to reinforce the idea of clear,
well-honed communication contributing to the success of the hunt.
Like the situation with the Agta, this description of the Matses attests to the
flexibility of hunter-gatherer economic arrangements, and particularly to the op-
portunism of both men and women in response to various environmental options.
We propose, therefore, to quote a suggestion by Estioko-Griffin and Griffin, that
Ju/’hoan data as well as Agta data “already deny the universality of the woman-
the-gatherer model, and go far to advance the concept of hunter-gatherers as in-
credibly flexible in all their organizational characteristics.” (1981: 143). Whether
the observed collaboration of men and women trackers among Ju/’hoansi is shal-
low or deep in time is an historic fact which can possibly still be ascertained. But
world data on women’s hunting in other societies, as well as many nuanced clues
embedded in time-tested areas of Ju/’hoan practice and belief, would not at this
point seem to argue AGAINST either 1) the “recent change” hypothesis, or 2) the
possibility that spousal collaboration may have been an ancient, long-lived option.
As an opener to a more general discussion of these issues, we would like to offer
Louis Liebenberg’s answer to an early version of our question. “My answer to
the question you pose would be that spousal collaboration in tracking/hunting has
substantial time-depth,” he wrote (July 14, 1997). His reasons included:
1. A tracker does not work in isolation - tracking involves the interpretation of
specific signs and predictions on the basis of extensive knowledge of animal
behavior and the environment as a whole. This knowledge is based not only
on the tracker’s own observations, but also those of other members of the
community, including women.
2. Most information is shared in the context of storytelling, which involves
men and women. The tracker’s understanding of nature is based on an oral
tradition that involves generations of hunter-gatherers.
3. Hunter-gatherers can identify each others’ footprints in the same way that
you recognize the handwriting of someone you know well. The interpretation
of tracks and signs is as fundamental to men and women in hunter-gatherer
societies as reading and writing is for you. To say that women are not trackers
is like saying that you cannot read or write because you are a woman.
4. For hunter-gatherers the interpretation of tracks and natural signs in nature
is equivalent to the interpretation of artificial signs in the city environment
(advertising signs, road signs, words in newspapers and books, adverts on
TV that use metaphors to symbolize desired lifestyles, economic indicators,
etc.) Whether you live in nature or in a city, your survival depends on your
ability to interpret signs in your environment, irrespective of whether you
are a man or a woman.
80 M. BIESELE AND S. BARCLAY
Our tentative conclusion leans well in the direction pointed by Liebenberg’s re-
marks, i.e. that spousal collaboration in hunting may have substantial time-depth.
These remarks support in a most comprehensive way the apparently long-tenured
relationships between Ju/’hoan knowledge and its communication through expres-
sive culture that one of us (Biesele) has explored in numerous research projects
and publications, and that both of us have experienced in talking and tracking
with Ju/’hoan people. However, it does not seem that the “recent change” and
the “ancient, long-lived” hypotheses are necessarily opposing ones. It appears to
us that spousal cooperation in hunting may have deep roots in the past, with the
degree of cooperation worked out individually by each couple according to the cir-
cumstances at hand. Whether circumstances have encouraged or discouraged such
cooperation today and why is a matter that needs further investigation.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors acknowledge the help of the following in prepara-
tion of this paper: /’Angn!ao /’Un, //Ukxa /’Angn!ao, Di//xao =Oma, N!ani Kxao, and
!Xuma /Kunta; Louis Liebenberg, Robert Hitchcock, P. Bion Griffin and Agnes Estioko-
Griffin; Susan Kent, Lewis Binford, and Nancy Stone; Lesley Beake, Jiro Tanaka, and
Masakazu Osaki; Steve Black, Michael Collins, and Thomas Hester of the Texas Ar-
chaeological Research Laboratory, University of Texas, Austin; Barbara Bodenhorn; and
Ernest “Tiger” Burch. Polly Wiessner’s help in commenting extensively and construc-
tively on early drafts and providing additional field data is especially appreciated. We
also gratefully acknowledge Gary “Butch” Barclay’s painstaking and expert review for us
of many hours of video footage of recent Ju/’hoan hunting.
ENDNOTE
In a personal comment (Sept. 20, 2000), Edwin Wilmsen mentioned the impor-
tance in this paper of the voice of an author (Barclay) informed about subsistence
hunting in his own area of the world. This voice allows the paper, he said, to
begin to demystify San knowledge and to emphasize its practical application to
the conditions under which the San live.
Wilmsen also noted that Herero pastoralists in the same environment are equally
good at reading spoor. In his experience at /Kae/kae, Botswana, this expertise
could be found among both old and young Herero women as well (though as they
live today they seldom travel far on foot so have little need to use it). This obser-
vation, along with instances Wilmsen recorded of Ju/’hoan San women tracking
game with their husbands, and with other (eastern Botswana) pastoralists tracking
cattle, underscores the point made by Barclay in this paper that all peoples who
depend on animals need to acquire tracking skills.
Ju/’hoan Women’s Tracking Knowledge 81
AUTHORS’ QUALIFICATIONS
Megan Biesele has done anthropology and human rights work in the Ju/’hoan
areas of Botswana and Namibia since 1970, and has published in a number of areas
touched on in this paper, including environmental knowledge, gender issues, and
language. Steve Barclay learned tracking and hunting from his father and uncles,
subsistence hunters and sportsmen in South Texas, where the environment has
many similarities with that of the Kalahari. With his son Butch, he has made
a lifelong study of animal behavior there. Steve and Megan’s collaboration as
a husband-wife team, combining technical hunting knowledge with experienced
Ju/’hoan translation, enabled them to observe husband-wife tracking and hunting
expeditions and to place their details in the context of information from other
world hunting traditions.
REFERENCES
Bailey, R. & R. J. Aunger 1989. Net hunters vs. archers: Variations in women’s subsistence
strategies in the Ituri forest. Human Ecology, 17: 273-297.
Barnard, A. 1983. Sex roles among the Nharo Bushmen of Botswana. Africa, 50: 115-124.
Begler, E. 1978. Sex, status, and authority in egalitarian society. American Anthropologist,
80: 571-588.
Berndt, C. 1978. Digging sticks and spears, or, the two-sex model. In (F. Gale,
ed.) Woman’s Role in Aboriginal Society, pp.64-80. Institute of Aboriginal Studies,
Canberra.
Bicchieri, M. 1969. The differential use of identical features of physical habitatat in con-
nection with exploitative, settlement, and community patterns: The Bambuti case. In
(D. Damas, ed.) Contributions to Anthropology: Ecological Essays, pp.65-72. National
Museum of Canada Bulletin 230. National Museums of Canada, Ottawa.
Biesele, M. 1993. Women Like Meat: The Folklore and Foraging Ideology of the Kalahari
Ju/’hoan,Witwatersrand University Press and Indiana University Press, Johannesburg
and Bloomington.
Bockstoce, J. (ed.) 1988. The Journal of Rochefort Maguire, 1852-1854: Two years at
Point Barrow, Alaska, aboard HMS Plover in the search for Sir John Franklin.Vol.II.
The Hakluyt Society, London.
Bodenhorn, B. 1990. I am not the great hunter: My wife is: Inupiat and anthropological
models of gender. Etudes/Inuit/Studies, 14(1-2): 55-74.
1993. Gendered spaces, public places: Public and private revisited on the north
slope of Alaska. (In B. Bender, ed.) Landscape: Politics and Perspectives, pp.169-203.
Berg Press, Oxford.
Briggs, J. 1974. Eskimo women: Makers of men. In (C. J. Matthiasson, ed.) Many Sisters:
WomeninCross-CulturalPerspective, pp.261-304. Free Press, New York.
Brooks, A. 1978. A note on the late stone age features at =Gi: Analogies from historic
San hunting practices. Botswana Notes and Records, 10: 1-3.
Burch, E. & L. Ellanna 1994. Key Issues in Hunter-Gatherer Research. Berg, Oxford and
Washington.
Crowell, A. & R. Hitchcock 1978. Basarwa ambush hunting in Botswana. Botswana Notes
and Records, 10: 37-51.
Dahlberg, F. (ed.) 1981. Woman the Gatherer. Yale University Press, New Haven.
Draper, P. 1975. !Kung women: Contrasts in sexual egalitarianism in foraging and seden-
tary contexts. In (R. Reiter, ed.) Towa rd an Anthro po logy of Wom en, pp.77-109.
MonthlyReviewPress,NewYork.
82 M. BIESELE AND S. BARCLAY
Draper, P. & M. Kranichfeld 1990. Coming in from the bush: Sttled life by the !Kung
and their accommodation to Bantu neighbors. Human Ecology, 18 (4): 363-384.
Endicott, K. 1981. The conditions of egalitarian male/female relationships in foraging
societies. Can be rra An thropo l ogy, 4: 1-10.
Estioko-Griffin, A. 1986. The Ethnography of Southeastern Cagayan Agta Hunting.Un-
published MA Thesis in author’s files. University of the Philippines, Dept. of Anthro-
pology.
Estioko-Griffin, A. & P. B. Griffin 1981. Woman the Hunter: The Agta. In (F. Dahlberg,
ed.), Woman the Gatherer, pp.121-151. Yale University Press, New Haven.
Estioko-Griffin, A. & P. B. Griffin 1985. Women hunters: The implications for pleistocene
prehistory and contemporary ethnography. In (M. Goodman, ed.) WomeninAsiaand
the Pacific: Towards an East-West Dialogue, pp.61-81. University of Hawaii Press,
Honolulu.
Flannery, R. 1932. The position of women among the Mescalero-Apache. Primitive Man,
10: 26-32.
Flannery, R. 1935. The position of women among the eastern Cree. Primitive Man, 12:
81-86.
Fox, R. B. 1952. The Pinatubo Negritos, their useful plants and material culture.
Philippine Journal of Science, 81: 113-414.
Gale, F. (ed.) 1978. Woman’s Role in Aboriginal Society. Institute of Aboriginal Studies,
Canberra.
Goodale, J. C. 1971. Tiwi Wives: A Study of the Women of Melville Island, North
Australia. University of Washington Press, Seattle, Washington.
Goodman, M., P. Griffin, A. Estioko-Griffin & J. Grove 1985. The compatibility of hunting
and mothering among the Agta hunter-gatherers of the Philippines. Sex Roles, 12:
1199-1209.
Gould, R. 1966. The wealth quest among the Talowa Indians of northwestern California.
Proceedings of t he American Philo sophical Society, 110(1): 67-89.
Griffin, P. B. 1989. Hunting, farming, and sedentism in a rain forest foraging society. In (S.
Kent, ed.), Farmers as Hunters: The Implications of Sedentism, pp.60-70. Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge.
Griffin, A. E. & P. B. Griffin 1981. Woman the hunter: The Agta. In (F. Dahlberg, ed.)
Woman the Gatherer. Yale University Press, New Haven.
Guenther, M. 1983-4. Bushwoman: The position of women in San society and ideology.
Journal of Comparative Sociology and Religion, 10-11: 12-31.
Hammond, D. & A. Jablow 1976. WomeninCulturesoftheWorld.Benjamin/
Cummings, Menlo Park, California.
Harako, R. 1976. The Mbuti as hunters – a study of ecological anthropology of the Mbuti
Pygmies. Kyoto University African Studies, 10: 37-99.
Hawkes, K. 1991. Showing off: Tests of a hypothesis about men’s foraging goals. Ethology
and Sociobiology, 12: 29-54.
Hawkes, K. & J. F. O’Connell 1981. Affluent hunters? Some comments in light of the
Alyawara case. American Anthropologist, 83(3): 622-626.
Hawkes, K., J. O’Connell & N. B. Jones 1989. Hardworking Hadza grandmothers. In
(V. Standen & R. Foley, eds.) Comparative Socioecology: The Behavioral Ecology of
Humans and Other Mammals, pp.341-366. Blackwell Scientific, Oxford.
Heinz, H.-J. 1975. !Ko-Buschmaenner (Suedafrika, Kalahari): Maedchen-Initiation. In
(G. Wolf, ed.) Encyclopaedia Cinematographica, Institut f¨ur den Wissenschaftlichen
Film, Goettingen.
Heinz, H.-J. & M. Lee 1978. Namkwa: Life among the Bushmen. Jonathan Cape, London.
Hewlett, B. 1996. Cultural diversity among African Pygmies. In (S. Kent, ed.) Cultural
Diversity among Twentieth Century Foragers: An African Perspective, pp.245-277.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Hitchcock, R. 1995. Subsistence hunting and special game licenses in Botswana. Botswana
Note s and Records, 27. Botswana Society, Gaborone, Botswana.
Ju/’hoan Women’s Tracking Knowledge 83
1998. ‘Hunting is our heritage’: The struggle for hunting and gathering rights
among the San of Southern Africa. Paper prepared for CHAGS 8, Conference on
Hunting and Gathering Societies, Osaka, Japan, October, 1998.
Hitchcock, R., J. Yellen, D. Gelburd, A. Osborn & A. Crowell 1996. Subsistence hunting
and resource management among the Ju/’hoansi of northwestern Botswana. African
Study Monographs, 17(4): 153-219.
Hunn, E. 1981. On the relative contribution of men and women to subsistence among
hunter-gatherers of the Columbia Plateau: A comparison with ethnographic atlas
summaries. Journal of Ethnobiology, 1: 124-134.
Hurtado, A., K. Hawkes, K. Hill & H. Kaplan 1985. Female subsistence strategies among
Ache hunter-gatherers of Eastern Paraguay. Human Ecology, 13(1): 1-28.
Ichikawa, M. 1976. Hunting life of Bambuti Pygmies. Shizen 31(4): 26-35.
Ikeya, K. 1989. Hunting activity of the central Kalahari San. Kikan Jinruigaku, 20(4):
284-332.
Imamura-Hayaki, K. 1996. Gathering activity among the central Kalahari San. African
Study Monographs Supplementary Issue 22: 47-66.
Jenness, D. 1922. The life of the Copper Eskimos. Report of the Canadian Arctic Expedi-
tion 1913-1918, vol. XII, pt. 9. Acland, Ottawa.
Joiris, V. 1990. Ritual Participation of Baka Women of Southern Cameroon in Activities
Considered Exclusively Masculine. Paper presented at the Sixth International Confer-
ence on Hunting and Gathering Societies, Fairbanks, Alaska.
Kelly, R. 1995. The Foraging Spectrum: Diversity in Hunter-Gatherer Lifeways.
Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington.
Kent, S. 1993. Sharing in an egalitarian Kalahari community. Man, 28(3): 489-490.
1999. Why the Mbuti and Ache hunt so much: Diversity in cross-cultural hunting
yields among contemporary foragers. Unpublished ms. in possession of author.
Kitanishi, K. 1995. Seasonal changes in the subsistence activities and food intake of the
Aka hunter-gatherers in northeastern Congo. African Study Monographs, 16(2): 73-
118.
Landes, R. 1938. The Ojibwa Woman. Columbia University Press, New York.
Lee. R. 1979. The !Kung San: Men, Women, and Work in a Foraging Society. Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge, England.
Lee, R. & I. DeVore (eds.) 1968. Man the Hunter. Aldine, Chicago.
1976. Kalahari Hunter-Gatherers: Studies of the !Kung San and Their Neigh-
bors. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Liebenberg, L. 1990. The Art of Tracking: the Origin of Science. David Philip, Cape
Town.
Marshall, L. 1976. The !Kung of Nyae Nyae. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
1999. Nyae Nyae !Kung Beliefs and Rites. Peabody Museum of Archaeology
and Ethnology, Cambridge, MA.
McCreedy, M. 1994. The arms of the Dibouka. In (E. Burch & L. Ellanna, eds.) Key
Issues in Hunter-Gatherer Research. Berg, Dulles, VA.
Meehan, B. 1982. Shell Bed to Shell Midden. Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies,
Canberra.
Nonaka, K. 1996. Ethnoentomology of the Central Kalahari San. African Study Mono-
graphs, Supplementary Issue, 22: 29-46.
O’Connell, J., K. Hawkes & N. B. Jones 1983. Hadza scavenging: Implications for Plio-
Pleistocene hominid subsistence. Current Anthropology, 29: 356-363.
Osaki, M. 1984. The social influence of change in hunting technique among the Central
Kalahari San. African Study Monographs, 5: 49-62.
Romanoff, S. 1983. Women as hunters among the Matses of the Peruvian Amazon. Human
Ecology, 11: 339-343.
Sharp, H. 1994. Asymmetric equals? Gender equality among the Chipewyan. In (L.
Klein & L. Ackerman, eds.) Gender and Power in Native North America.University
of Oklahoma Press, Norman.
84 M. BIESELE AND S. BARCLAY
Shostak, M. 1981. Nisa: The Life and Words of a !Kung Woman.HarvardUniversity
Press, Cambridge, MA.
Silberbauer, G. 1981. Hunter and Habitat in the Central Kalahari Desert. Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge, England.
Sugawara, K. 1991. The economics of social life among the Central Kalahari San
(G//anakhwe and G/wikhwe) in the sedentary community at !Koi!kom. Senri Eth-
nological Studies, 30: 91-116.
Tanaka, J. 1980. The San, Hunter-Gatherers of the Kalahari: A Study in Ecological
Anthropology. University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo.
Tanno, T. 1976. The Mbuti net-hunters in the Ituri Forest, eastern Zaire. Kyoto University
African Studies, 10: 101-135.
Terashima, H. 1980. Mota and other hunting activities of the Mbuti archers: A socioeco-
nomic study of subsistence technology. African Study Monographs, 3: 71-85.
Testart, A. 1986. La Femme et la Chasse. La Recherche, 181, vol. 17: 1194-1201.
Thomas, E. M. 1990. Reflections: The old way. The New Yorker October, pp.78-110,
New York, NY.
Turnbull, C. 1965. Wayward Servants: The Two Worlds of the African Pygmies.The
Natural History Press, Garden City, NY.
Wilmsen, E. 1989. Land Filled With Flies: A Political Economy of the Kalahari.Univer-
sity of Chicago Press, Chicago.
Watanabe, H. 1968. Subsistence and ecology of northern food gatherers with special
reference to the Ainu. In (R. B. Lee & I. DeVore, eds.) Man the Hunter, pp.69-77.
Aldine, Chicago.
Yellen, J. & R. B. Lee 1976. The Dobe-/Du/Da environment: Background to a hunting
and gathering way of life. In (R. B. Lee & I. DeVore, eds.) Kalahari Hunter-Gatherers:
Studies of the !Kung San and their Neighbors, pp.27-46. Harvard University Press.
Cambridge, MA.
Accepted September 25, 2000
Author’s Name and Address: Megan BIESELE, Kalahari Peoples Fund and Department
of Anthropology, Texas A & M University College Station, Texas, 3107 Greenlawn Park-
way, Austin, TX 78757 USA. Stuve BARCLAY, Kalahari Peoples Fund, Austin, Texas,
3107 Greenlawn Parkway, Austin, TX 78757 USA
... Cluster 237 (West Papuans) posed a slightly different problem: the Mimika are the only culture represented in this cluster, but the only source we found for them (Zegwaard et al., 2000) included their neighbors, the Asmat. Due to "often-striking similarities in the myths and legends of Turnbull, 1959, 1965a, Turnbull, 1965b Africa Khoisan B Biesele, 1993;Biesele & Barclay, 2001;Bleek, 1929;Blurton Jones & Konner, 1976;Guenther, 1989;Hewitt, 1986;Shostak, 1981;Thomas, 1959 Densmore, 1929;Hilger, 1951aHilger, , 1951bLandes, 1937 (continued on next page) Sugiyama and K.J. Reilly Evolution and Human Behavior xxx (xxxx) & Hallowell, 2018;Copway, 1851;Hallowell & Brown, 1992;Speck, 1915;Vennum, 1988283 Chipewyan N America N Athapaskan S Hearne, 1958Jarvenpa, 1980Jarvenpa, , 1982Lowie, 1925;Sharp, 1988;Smith, 1985284 Kaska N America N Athapaskan S Honigmann, 1954McClellan, 1987;Teit & Helm, 1956285 Gwich'in N America N Athapaskan S McClellan, 1987 286 Dena N America N Athapaskan S Attla, 1983Attla, , 1990Chapman & Goddard, 1914;de Laguna, 1995;Jette, 1908 Dauenhauer, 1987;de Laguna, 1972;Jones, 1914;Kan, 1989;McClellan, 1987 , -27, Adamson, 1934Elmendorf, 1961Elmendorf, , 1993Goertz, 2018 these people" (Zegwaard et al., 2000:7), the collection examines Mimika and Asmat storytelling practices as a whole; thus, we followed suit. The majority of our sample (67%) consists of North American cultures (Fig. 1). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
For most of human evolution, accumulated cultural knowledge has been stored in memory and transmitted orally. This presents a daunting information management problem: how to store and transmit this knowledge in a portable format that resists corruption. One solution--widespread among foragers--is to encode knowledge in narrative. However, this strategy depends on accurate performance of the story. Significantly, some forager cultures have rules regulating myth performance, although the extent of this phenomenon is unknown. We hypothesize that these rules subserve high-fidelity transmission across generations. Accordingly, we predicted that, across forager cultures, myth-telling rules will mandate: (P1) transmission by the most proficient storytellers (P2) under low-distraction conditions with (P3) multiple individuals and (P4) multiple generations present, and the application of measures that (P5) prevent, identify, and/or correct errors, (P6) maintain audience attention, (P7) discourage rule violations and/or (P8) incentivize rule compliance. To test these predictions, we searched the forager ethnographic record for descriptions of myth performance and coded them for prescriptions/proscriptions regarding narrator age, performance context, audience composition, narrative delivery, and audience comportment, as well as sanctions associated with rule transgression or compliance. Results indicate that rules regulating myth performance are widespread across forager cultures and are characterized by features that reduce the likelihood of copy errors. These findings help elucidate the role that anthropogenic ratchets played in the emergence of cumulative culture.
... Device monitoring is compatible with other seasonal and high-input/reliable-return activities in forests, such as honey-hive construction and management (Blackburn, 1982). Snares, traps, nets, and pitfalls can be made, set, and tended by diverse group members: men and women, children and elders (Biesele and Barclay, 2001;Kent, 1993;Lupo and Schmitt, 2002;Silberbauer, 1981). ...
... From the Klamath and Modoc rule summary, we derived a total of Turnbull, 1959, 1965a, Turnbull, 1965b Africa Khoisan B Biesele, 1993;Biesele & Barclay, 2001;Bleek, 1929;Blurton Jones & Konner, 1976;Guenther, 1989;Hewitt, 1986;Shostak, 1981;Thomas, 1959 20 Densmore, 1929;Hilger, 1951aHilger, , 1951bLandes, 1937 282 Ojibwa N America Algonkian S Bigmouth & Hallowell, 2018;Copway, 1851;Hallowell & Brown, 1992;Speck, 1915;Vennum, 1988 283 Chipewyan N America N Athapaskan S Hearne, 1958;Jarvenpa, 1980Jarvenpa, , 1982Lowie, 1925;Sharp, 1988;Smith, 1985Smith, , 1995Smith, , 1998 Attla, 1983Attla, , 1990Chapman & Goddard, 1914;de Laguna, 1995;Jette, 1908 Dauenhauer, 1987;de Laguna, 1972;Jones, 1914;Kan, 1989;McClellan, 1987 , -27, Adamson, 1934Elmendorf, 1961Elmendorf, , 1993Goertz, 2018 Dempsey, 2003;Ewers, 1958;Grinnell, 1962;Wissler & Duvall, 1909;Hungry Wolf, 1980 314 (Table 2). We then parsed our predictions in terms of these rule types. ...
Article
For most of human evolution, accumulated cultural knowledge has been stored in memory and transmitted orally. This presents a daunting information management problem: how to store and transmit this knowledge in a portable format that resists corruption. One solution--widespread among foragers--is to encode knowledge in narrative. However, this strategy depends on accurate performance of the story. Significantly, some forager cultures have rules regulating myth performance, although the extent of this phenomenon is unknown. We hypothesize that these rules subserve high-fidelity transmission across generations. Accordingly, we predicted that, across forager cultures, myth-telling rules will mandate: (P1) transmission by the most proficient storytellers (P2) under low-distraction conditions with (P3) multiple individuals and (P4) multiple generations present, and the application of measures that (P5) prevent, identify, and/or correct errors, (P6) maintain audience attention, (P7) discourage rule violations and/or (P8) incentivize rule compliance. To test these predictions, we searched the forager ethnographic record for descriptions of myth performance and coded them for prescriptions/proscriptions regarding narrator age, performance context, audience composition, narrative delivery, and audience comportment, as well as sanctions associated with rule transgression or compliance. Results indicate that rules regulating myth performance are widespread across forager cultures and are characterized by features that reduce the likelihood of copy errors. These findings help elucidate the role that anthropogenic ratchets played in the emergence of cumulative culture.
... In the past, hunting was seen by the Ju/'hoansi as an important activity, one that provided not only food but also goods and materials crucial to the local economy. Women contributed to men's hunting success through their extensive tracking knowledge and by sharing information they had obtained during the course of gathering trips (Biesele and Barclay 2001). One of the concerns expressed to us by a number of older Ju/'hoansi was the fact that members of the younger generation often did not have the desire to learn how to hunt. ...
... Chimpanzee diets are composed of ~95% predictable and easy to collect foods such as leaves and fruits; in contrast, hunter-gatherers rely on unpredictable resources, which require both high skill and good luck to obtain (Kaplan et al., 2000). For instance, in some foraging societies, successful acquisition of meat occurs on fewer than 20% of hunting trips (Biesele & Barclay, 2001). However, when it does occur it produces large nutrient dense food packages, which provide more meat than the hunter's household can consume before it rots. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
For most of human evolutionary history our species lived as hunter-gatherers; hence, much of our cognition and behaviour is adapted to this way of life. Given the magnitude of the sociocultural, economic and lifestyle changes experienced by Homo sapiens over the last 10,000 years, in particular the last several hundred years, aspects of human psychology may be maladapted to modern ways of life. This process of maladaptation following changes in the physical or social environment is referred to as ‘evolutionary mismatch’ and has been hypothesised to contribute to the high prevalence of mental disorders in industrialised societies. However, very few studies have examined the prevalence of these pathologies among contemporary hunter-gatherer populations; thus, empirical support for such diseases of modernity hypotheses is lacking. In this chapter, we review the limited existing research and theorise about the key differences between hunter-gatherer and industrialised societies that are likely to have profound implications for mental health. Specifically, we contrast the strong social support networks, egalitarianism, explorative modes of learning, sensitive child-rearing practices and present orientation of hunter-gatherers with corresponding features of industrialised populations. We argue that mismatches in these domains are partially responsible for of a vast array of mental illnesses, ranging from common mood disorders to behavioural pathologies and psychotic spectrum disorders. We hope that this chapter stimulates the generation and testing of mismatch hypotheses and, eventually, trials of interventions based on mismatch reduction. We end by offering suggestions for methodological approaches to this future research.
Chapter
As we have seen, Social Metabolism, in its different versions, has been used to study the structure, functioning and material dynamics of human societies throughout history, i.e., as an explanatory theory of socioenvironmental change.
Article
Full-text available
Women participate in hunting in some foraging societies but not in others. To examine the socioecological factors that are conducive to women’s hunting, we conducted an ethnographic survey using the Human Relations Area Files and other selected sources authored in the past 200 years. Based on life history theory and behavioural ecology, we predicted that women should engage in hunting when: i) it poses few conflicts with childcare, ii) it is associated with few cultural restrictions around the use of hunting technology, iii) it involves low-risk game within range of camp, with the aid of dogs, and/or in groups, and, iv) women fulfil key logistical or informational roles. We systematically reviewed ethnographic documents across 64 societies and coded 242 paragraphs for the above variables. The data largely support theoretical expectations. When women hunted, they did so in a fundamentally different manner than men, focusing on smaller game and hunting in larger groups near camp, often with the aid of dogs. There was little evidence to suggest that women only participated in hunting during non-reproductive years; instead, allocate networks were a prominent strategy for mitigating trade-offs between hunting and childcare responsibilities. Women commonly fulfilled crucial informational, logistical and ritualistic roles. Cultural restrictions limited women’s participation in hunting, but not to the extent commonly assumed. These data offer a cross-cultural framework for making inferences about whether and how women’s hunting occurred in the past.
Article
Full-text available
Namibia is rich in hunter-gatherer rock art from the Later Stone Age (LSA); this is a tradition of which well-executed engravings of animal tracks in large numbers are characteristic. Research into rock art usually groups these motifs together with geometric signs; at best, therefore, it may provide summary lists of them. To date, the field has completely disregarded the fact that tracks and trackways are a rich medium of information for hunter-gatherers, alongside their deeper, culture-specific connotations. A recent research project, from which this article has emerged, has attempted to fill this research gap; it entailed indigenous tracking experts from the Kalahari analysing engraved animal tracks and human footprints in a rock art region in central Western Namibia, the Doro! nawas Mountains, which is the site of recently discovered rock art. The experts were able to define the species, sex, age group and exact leg of the specific animal or human depicted in more than 90% of the engravings they analysed (N = 513). Their work further demonstrates that the variety of fauna is much richer in engraved tracks than in depictions of animals in the same engraving tradition. The analyses reveal patterns that evidently arise from culturally determined preferences. The study represents further confirmation that indigenous knowledge, with its profound insights into a range of particular fields, has the capacity to considerably advance archaeological research.
Article
This article explores the skilled arts of tracking and gathering as methods for noticing and theorizing multispecies landscapes in the Kalahari Desert, Botswana. Tracking is typically used to describe a practice of following animals, usually for hunting, whereas gathering primarily refers to the collection of plant and fungal materials. The author presents a case in which these terms have been scrambled during long-term ethnographic field research. The author and his interlocutors tracked the Kalahari desert truffle, an experience that demonstrates how aspects of tracking extend to gathering, but also how the practices are attentive to the movements of landscapes more broadly. This form of tracking attends to multiple spatial and temporal movements that include nonanimals and other nonhumans. It represents a way of noticing the assemblages of more-than-human relations that make up landscapes. These convergences, first identified through tracking, are then explored through the more distributed analytic of gathering. Inspired by Ursula LeGuin’s call to describe stories of gatherers and collectives in her “Carrier Bag Theory of Fiction,” the article argues that thinking tracking through the gathering analytic helps articulate a “carrier bag approach” for understanding landscapes through the gatherings of relations with which they emerge.
Article
G/wi society and culture have been shaped by the rugged natural environment. The volume focusses on the interrelationships, the socio-cultural system and habitat of the hunter-gatherer G/wi bushmen of the central Kalahari Desert of Botswana. Drawing on ten years of field-experience, the author sets out the foundations of G/wi society, with descriptions of their social, political and economic organisation, living patterns, subsistence technology, and seasonal adaptations. -John Sheail
Book
In 1845 Sir John Franklin's expedition left England, searching for a northwest passage, and vanished into the Arctic forever. Three years later HMS Plover's was the first departure of 21 expeditions searching for Franklin. Although most of the analyses of the Franklin Search have focused on the large expeditions in the eastern Arctic, the smaller western expeditions also produced significant geographical and ethnographical information. The Plover's voyage of 1848 to 1854 was the first constant presence of Europeans in the western Arctic, and Rochfort Maguire's journal is the earliest account of a sustained foreign association with the Eskimos of northern Alaska. Maguire's journal is far more than an important historical document; it is a fascinating account of Europeans and Eskimos learning to cope with one another. Maguire's narrative is introduced by a detailed discussion of the history, strategy and logistics of the Franklin Search in the western Arctic. Appendices include accounts of the Search's five boat expeditions near Point Barrow as well as Dr John Simpson's seminal essay on the Eskimos of northern Alaska. The main pagination of this and the following volume (Second series 170) is continuous. This is a new print-on-demand hardback edition of the volume first published in 1987.