ArticlePDF Available

Psychology goes molecular. Epigenetics of learning

Authors:

Figures

Content may be subject to copyright.
NAUKA 4/2016 • 35-52
JAN BARCISZEWSKI, WŁADYSŁAW JACEK PALUCHOWSKI, DOROTA GURDA,
ELŻBIETA HORNOWSKA, ALEKSANDRA JASIELSKA*
Psychology goes molecular.
Epigenetics of learning
Introduction
An organism’s ability to learn and to memorize is essential for its behavioral adap-
tation to environmental changes. On a cellular level, learning and memory is mediated
by structural and functional changes of in protein synthesis in the nervous system.
These variations, also termed plasticity, modulate a neuron’s response to external sti-
muli. Despite advances in methodologies applied, there are no objective methods to
monitor learning and memory, mostly because psychology relies on symptoms as the
basis of a diagnosis.
Process of learning
Learning is a result of past experience, permanent changes in the behavioral poten-
tial, thoughts and emotions of human being or an animal (Anderson, 2000; Sternberg,
2015). The foundation of that process is a lasting record of experiences, defined as
memory, which allows to store products or effects of learning. Up to now, the process
of learning has been analyzed from two perspectives. The first, nonmentalistic one con-
centrates on behavioral changes that are an effect of stimulating situations, and the
second one, cognitive approach, which assume that learning is a reorganization of cog-
nitive scheme in the individual that takes place due to active processing of information.
There are many approaches to describe the process of learning. The type of know-
ledge acquired by the student, the degree of engagement in the process, contextual
circumstances of learning and other variables should be considered. Taking into a con-
sideration the form of acquired knowledge, one can separate semantic knowledge (de-
clarative, e.g., school knowledge) from acquired skills (the so-called procedural know-
ledge, e.g., manipulation activities). Analysis of the number of repetitions and the
length of time between them, enables to identify cumulated learning (memorizing the
* Prof. dr hab. Jan Barciszewski, dr Dorota Gurda – Institute of Bioorganic Chemistry, Polish
Academy of Sciences, Poznań, Poland;
prof. dr hab. Władysław Jacek Paluchowski, prof. dr hab. Elżbieta Hornowska, prof. dr hab.
Aleksandra Jasielska – Institute of Psychology, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań, Poland
J. Barciszewski, W.J. Paluchowski, D. Gurda, E. Hornowska, A. Jasielska36
entire material without breaks) and time distribution (memorizing the material which
is exposed in several sessions of learning). When one considers the organization and the
scope of the material, it is possible to identify holistic (total) learning (learning pertains
to the entire material at once) and partial learning (dividing the material into smaller
pieces and learning them one after the other). With respect to student’s attitude, there
are difference between voluntary learning (the student consciously concentrates on the
process of learning) and involuntary learning (when the student comes in contact with
the material, but has no intention of learning it), (Chlewiński, 1997).
The most up-to-date psychological dictionary claims that the definition and manner
of applying the term learning has not acquired too much controversy among theoreti-
cians, and it has been used with a relatively small ballast (in developmental, educatio-
nal, cognitive psychology, behaviorism, et al.). Usually, it appears as a key word in titles
of chapters in that meaning that does not diverge from its commonly accepted under-
standing. A problem arises, trying to define theoretical processes and mechanisms of
learning (Reber, 2000). Currently, there are two issues, implicit and emotional learning
(Kirsner, 1998: Elias, 2004). Implicit learning is a basic way of acquiring implicit know-
ledge (Reber, 1989). It enables building a deep representation of knowledge that takes
into consideration abstract relationships between elements of the surrounding (envi-
ronment) world. It functions without the participation of conscious learning strategies
and without the intent to gain knowledge, and it is effortless. On the other hand, emo-
tional learning is an extension of the construct of emotional intelligence (Mayer, Salo-
vey, Caruso, & Sitarenios 2004), which is defined as an ability to perceive and express
emotions, to understand and manage them in order to support the development of an
individual. The authors describe emotional intelligence in terms of a collection of skills,
wherein perception, appraisal and expression of emotions is considered to be the most
basic skill, and within it, the ability to identify emotion in other people (Salovey, 2008).
Emotional learning is gaining by the learning individual knowledge, attitudes and skills
necessary to understand and manage emotions. The widely perceived emotional
learning encompasses a set of 15 socio-emotional abilities, one of which is “the ability
to identify emotion in oneself and in other people” (Jasielska, 2009). There are data
that assume that, for example, the genetically conditioned attention and memory span
can have an influence on the processes of learning (Colombo, 2004; Richards, 2004,
Bauer, 2004). It has been shown that the temperamentally conditioned tendency to
retreat in new situations is treated as one of the mechanisms that reduce the diversity
of stimuli with which the individual comes into contact, and this, in turn, has an influen-
ce on the pace of learning. Fagen (1987) have demonstrated that tearfulness and
associated with it lower level of exploratory activity in children, have a negative in-
fluence on the performance in cognitive tasks, whereas Colom (2007) has suggested
Psychology goes molecular. Epigenetics of learning 37
that impulsiveness, sensation seeking, and a lower level of anxiety are connected with
the level of an academic performance. In line with the theory of activation (Gray, 1964),
people differ from one another in terms of the level of activation (arousal) that
determines both the direction and intensity of behavior. According to Gray (1964),
differences in the level of activation depend on an individual trait that can be coined
arousability, i.e. the level of activation characteristic of the particular person, depends,
in turn, on the individually diversified reactivity of biological mechanisms that underlie
particular temperament traits. Arousability is considered to be a more or less stable
tendency to react to stimuli, which is conditioned by adequate biological mechanisms
that are responsible for its level (Strelau, 2014). Irrespective of the manner of ope-
rationalization of the construct of arousability (extraversion and emotional stability in
the concept of H. J. Eysenck, sensation seeking in M. Zuckerman’s concept, or novelty
seeking, harm avoidance and reward dependence in the concept of R. Cloninger), it can
be assumed that “there exist more or less stable individual differences in physiological
and/or biochemical mechanisms, which explain individual differences in respect of tem-
perament traits” (Stelau, 2014). These mechanisms can also effect the process of
learning. At the bottom of the psychobiological model of personality developed by
Cloninger (1993,1994a, 1994b, Hauser, 2003; Hornowska, 2003a,b, 2006) there is an
assumption about an influence of the genetically controlled neurotransmitters on the
expression of particular traits of human personality. In line with that model, personality
is composed of the genetically conditioned temperament and environmentally de-
termined character. It is worth to add, that phenotypical manifestation of the persona-
lity structure may differ from its biogenetical basis due to the process of interaction
that takes place between genes and environmental influences (Cloninger, 1993).
Temperament, which is to a large extent conditioned genetically and can be understood
as an individual set of emotional reactions and skills manifested in response to stimuli
present in the surrounding environment, constitutes the biological foundation for the
development of personality traits (Cloninger, 1993). Individual differences in tempe-
rament are associated with minor differences in the structure and connections within
cerebrum, such as amygdala, hypothalamus, striatum and structures of the limbic sys-
tem (Cloninger, 1994a,b). Particular dimensions of temperament are also connected
with the diversification of procedural memory functioning. Procedural memory is
limited to simple, unconscious cognitive functions. It refers to our life experience in
respect of operational conditioning of habits with the use of presemantic processes that
code particular spatio-visual information and affective states. When it comes to dimen-
sions that describe temperament in the discussed model, there are, novelty seeking
(NS), i.e., the tendency to react actively to new stimuli, harm avoidance (HA), i.e., the
tendency to inhibit actions in response to negative stimuli, reward dependence (RD),
J. Barciszewski, W.J. Paluchowski, D. Gurda, E. Hornowska, A. Jasielska38
i.e., the tendency to maintain behavior in response to positive reinforcement, and per-
sistence(P), i.e., the ability to independently maintain the given kind of activity (Hor-
nowska, 2011). According to Cloninger (1993) three out of four dimensions of tem-
perament, i.e., sensation seeking, harm avoidance and reward dependence, possess
a precisely determined biological foundation. The author assumes that the novelty of
seeking remains in a relation with the dopaminergic system. Harm avoidance is con-
nected with the serotonin synthesis, and reward dependence with noradrenaline pro-
duction.
Psychologists are strung ling to establish a simple and direct predictor of academic
success. Instead of many small factors, one big thing can explain everything is urgently
needed. After excitement with a potential changes of gene expression induced by social
processes, it turned out that such fast processes as learning and memory do not
provide a good perspective for this type of studies (Hornowska, 2013).
Molecular psychology
Early studies of long-term storing in memory showed that both the protein syn-
thesis and gene transcription are the key players of that process (Barondes, 1964;
Cohen, 1966). Further studies have shown that the formation of long-term memory is
a complex process that requires the engagement of many independent signal paths and
regulation of numerous genes (Robertson, 1999; Selcher, 2002; Levenson, 2004). How-
ever, they are not so clear and are difficult to interpret.
It is possible to access a genetic factors like single DNA nucleotide polymorphisms
(SNPs). Although the largest effect sizes of the associations between SNPs and beha-
vioral traits are very small, it is possible to aggregate the effects of thousands of SNP
associations, ranked by effect size, into a SNP genotypic score for a particular trait or
as a genome-wide polygenic score (GPS). Many different labels have been ascribed to
polygenic scores that usually include the word risk. It highlights the genome-wide natu-
re of these polygenic scores and encompasses positive as well as negative effects im-
plied by the normal distribution of polygenic scores. Recently a genome-wide polygenic
score (GPS) has been used to predict behavior all traits from DNA alone. Some GPS
are more strongly associated with a particular trait, but others are less strongly asso-
ciated. The effects of these variants are weighted by the strength of association and
then summed to a score, so that people with many variants related to academic achie-
vement will have a higher polygenic score and higher academic achievement, whereas
people with fewer associated variants will have a lower score and lower level of acade-
mic achievement or success. To get that, one should carry out genome-wide association
studies (GWAs), which identify specific genetic variants linked to particular traits. Over
the last several years genome wide association (GWA) research across the life sciences
Psychology goes molecular. Epigenetics of learning 39
has revealed that there are almost no genetic variants with large effects on complex
traits and common disorders. This suggests that the heritability of behavioral traits is
due to many genetic variants of small effect. GWA studies of behavioral traits began to
be successful as their sample sizes increased sufficiently to detect associations of very
small effect size between single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and outcome.
GWAs look at traits such as capability to learning, as a means of defining perso-
nalized educational achievement. Identifying the genetic variants responsible for the
ubiquitous heritability of behavioral dimensions and disorders is transforming genetic
research in the social and behavioral sciences by making it possible to predict genetic
strengths and weaknesses of individuals from DNA alone. Assessment of individuals
educational strengths and weaknesses from their DNA, provide information about
whether a student may develop learning problems (Selzam, 2016).
Currently physiology is seeking for new approaches to analyze and evaluate learn-
ing and memory processes. It seemed that molecular biology could provide an answer.
It seems that at the molecular level, the acquisition and maintenance of memory re-
quire not only genetic component but also epigenetic variations as DNA methylation,
histone modifications, non-coding RNAs and chromatin remodeling. It is understand-
able that beyond the two-dimensional genetic universe there is the three dimensional
epigenetic universe which is subtler, shiftier, but still chartable. In fact, this is a 3D
object, folded in hierarchical way, and affect how to think about many aspects of human
development, health and disease.
Epigenetics
In the last decade one can observe an exponential increase in the study of epigene-
tics, which is stable chemical modifications that occur on, over, near, or at (but not in)
to the gene (DNA) primary structure (nucleotide sequence) by both heritable and non-
heritable influences that does not alter the gene sequence. It serves to activate and
silence genes, and to incorporate a layer of information onto the backbone of DNA,
thus controlling gene expression and behavior traits. The DNA sequence of the human
genome is identical in all cells of the body, but cell types, such as heart, brain or skin
cells have specific characteristics and are uniquely susceptible to various diseases. By
guiding how genes are expressed, epigenomes allow cells carrying the same DNA to
differentiate into the more than 200 types in the human body. The term epigenetics
has been in common usage since 1942 when the Conrad Waddington (1905-1975) de-
fined it as a part of molecular biology that studies the causal interactions between
genes and their products, which ultimately bring the phenotype into being. More
recently, the term has gained varied additional meanings dependent upon whether it
is used in developmental, molecular or evolutionary biology. The underlying concept
J. Barciszewski, W.J. Paluchowski, D. Gurda, E. Hornowska, A. Jasielska40
is that across these disciplines, those biological processes may be described as epige-
netic if they are not the result of direct changes to the sequence of DNA, but to
changes in how genes are expressed. A good example is cellular differentiation. Every
cell in an organism contains the same genetic material, but different cell lines become
specialized as development proceeds, becoming part of the liver, skin, heart etc. This
is achieved through the epigenetic silencing or activation of specific genes within each
cell line, with these modifications inherited by daughter cells. Indeed, another common
usage of epigenetic is the inheritance of characteristics of an individual from one gene-
ration to the next through nongenetic mechanisms. Epigenetics is the study of traits
inherited by daughter cells that are not directly caused by DNA sequence alterations.
Epigenetics is concerned with modifications to genetic material that affect whether
genes are expressed (Loi, 2013; McGuinness, 2012). The role of epigenetics is functio-
nary and the biological study is dual. It is about gene regulation (histone modification
through acetylation and methylation, DNA methylation, and non-coding RNAs); and
about transgenerational inheritance – cellular memory. Epigenetics describes heritable
changes affected by the environment. What is surprising that after all, for decades we
have all been told, you are what you eat, you are what you drink, you are how much, or
how little, you exercise, you are whatever toxic material you imbibe or inhale. But as
health experts have cautioned us repeatedly, we are a product of our own lifestyle choi-
ces. Environment signals come from nutrition, exercise, toxins, ecological conditions,
stress, trauma, nurturance, and personal beliefs. A biomarker should be a quantitative
compound, specifically relevant that can be applied to monitor of a problem studied.
There is a wealth of data which exemplify that some biomolecules are on different level
relative to their normal counterparts and their altered level could be measured in order
to find a correlation with current organism state. Such markers, can have high prognos-
tic and prediction values.
Selective biomarkers in medicine can identify susceptibility risks and be helpful for
diagnosis and for proper time therapeutic interventions and effective treatment. To
date, there are no good biomarkers in blood and serum for detection and follow up.
Therefore new markers are needed for more detailed human characterization, better
prognosis and selection of efficient treatment. It has been known for long time that
genetic variants associated with subjective psychological wellbeing have a genetic
component and only a few specific genetic variants related to this traits have been
identify. However it seems that genetics is only one factor that influences psychological
traits. The environment (epigenetics) is at least as important, and it interacts with the
genetic effects.
Epigenetic mechanisms play a central role in the regulation of cellular processes
by influencing genomic activity. DNA methylation, defined as the covalent bonding of
Psychology goes molecular. Epigenetics of learning 41
Fig. 1. DNA as basis for learning
and memory assessment
a methyl group to a cytosine in the context of a CpG dinucleoside, is an important
component of these mechanisms in mammals. Basically DNA methylation represses
transcription, which can occur by inhibiting the binding of transcription factors or by
recruting binding proteins that remodel chromatin structure. The establishment and
maintenance of DNA methylation patterns are crucial for normal cellular function
and developmental processes and, these patterns are highly heterologous at different
life stages and between different tissue types.
Genome scale DNA methylation can be considered to be a large set of traits in
which variation can arise from environmental, stochastic or genetic changes and there
is welch of data that DNA methylation could mediate the links between these processes
in influencing complex diseases (Gaunt et al. 2016).
So, now comprehensive appreciation of the integrated genomics and epigenomics
is urgently needed for better understanding of the multiple cellular pathways involved
in development, and establish markers of resistance to traditional therapies as well as
contributing to the development of new treatment modalities. It is now clear that epi-
genetic changes in histone and DNA modifications can alter gene expression, affect
their function and contribute to well-being, learning and memory.
The assessment of epigenetic alterations is one of the most promising means of
marker identification for the early detection of current status of an individual. The best
characterized epigenetic mark is a methyl group at the fifth position of cytosine (m5C).
The methylation of gene promoters is associated with gene silencing and thus the dis-
tribution of methyl groups within the genome defines regions of varying transcriptional
J. Barciszewski, W.J. Paluchowski, D. Gurda, E. Hornowska, A. Jasielska42
potential. It may lead to genomic instability, affect tissue specific differentiation and
finally influences well-being. The genome of cell individuals under different kind of ex-
ternal stimuli are generally characterized by a global loss of methylation (hypomethy-
lation). That can happen through oxidative stress. It results from a cellular imbalance
in the production of reactive oxygen species (ROS) and antioxidant enzyme activities.
It is generally accepted that environmental influence on a human genome has been
linked to the disruption of red-ox balance. Furthermore stressed cells are characterized
by enhanced ROS generation, which deregulate the red-ox homeostasis and promote
an aberrant induction of signaling networks that effect of well-being. ROS are formed
during normal metabolic processes. Under physiological conditions the balance exists
among ROS production and scavenging, oxidative alteration of cellular components and
their repair. An imbalance between ROS production and scavenging potential (oxidative
stress), leads to an accumulation of oxidative damaged cellular macromolecules that is
associated with abnormal behavior. Among these macromolecules that are covalently
modified by ROS are nucleic acids. ROS, particularly hydroxyl radical (·OH), may cause
a wide range of DNA lesions including canonical and odd bases, deletions, strand
breakage, and chromosomal rearrangements. Oxidative damaged DNA has been blamed
for the physiological changes associated with as cancer. One of the best studied DNA
damage product is 8-oxo-7,8-dihydroguanine (8-oxoGua), a marker of oxidative stress,
which is formed in DNA via a direct reaction of guanosine with ·OH. The same random
radical reaction can take place with all normal and modified DNA constituents. Euka-
ryotic DNA contains only 5-methyldeoxycytosine (m5C) as a modified nucleoside. It is
assumed that ca. 5% of all cytosine residues or 1% of bases in the mammalian genomes
are methylated. Although DNA methylation has been viewed as a stable epigenetic
mark, studies in the past decade have revealed that is not the case. m5C demethylation
occurs through action of ROS. 5-methylcytosine (m5C), along with other DNA bases are
targets for hydroxyl radical (·OH), the most reactive ROS. Radical oxidation of m5C
leads to its modification and degradation. The oxidized derivatives are unstable and
release formaldehyde, what results in loss of the methyl group from m5C and decrease
of global (genomic) m5C contents in DNA (hypomethylation), (Haigis, 2012). To deter-
mine a minute content of mC in limited amount of blood, we will apply the postlabeling
with 32P] ATP of DNA hydrolysate and identification of m5C with two dimensional thin
layer chromatography (TLC). We expect to find changes of m5C and direct correlation
of global m5C content in blood DNA with well-being. We have already found hypo-
methylation in DNA isolated from peripheral blood of patient with pain. That observa-
tion prompted us to use m5C demethylation as a probe for psychological studies.
Oxidative damage can lead to the formation of a variety of modified bases in DNA, in-
cluding oxidation of guanine to 8-oxoguanine and the oxidation of m5C to 5-hydroxy-
Psychology goes molecular. Epigenetics of learning 43
methylcytosine, which leads to the demethylation of m5C. Oxidative deamination of
m5C was also observed. For aberrant DNA methylation various reasons have been pro-
posed, mostly changes in DNA methyltransferases, but never radical demethylation.
Methylation of cytosine is established in epiblast and maintained in the later stages of
the cell development. It creates a pattern specific and characteristic to every organism
(Esteller, 2007). This is the only one modification involving the formation of covalent
carbon-carbon bond between the C5 of cytosine and methyl group. 5-methyl cytosine
(m5C) is present in the genomes of all vertebrates and flowering plants and some fungi,
invertebrates and bacteria. The total amount of m5C in the genome of mammals is -1%
which means that about 5% of all cytosines are methylated. Methylated are primarily
dinucleotides CpG, CpNpG and CpNpN (where N – adenosine, cytidine or thymidine).
In the stem cells of embryos almost 25% of all methylated cytosines are located within
the last two sequences which are lost as a result of differentiation of embryonic stem
cells. CpG sequences are not evenly distributed throughout the genome, they create
so-called “CpG islands” consisting 0.5-4 kbp and are located mainly within the gene pro-
moters made in 60-70% of these dinucleotides. In the remainder part of the genome,
their number is estimated at less than 20%. The promoters of human genes are divided
into two distinct classes of CpG content. The first which is hypermethylated has a low
CpG content and a second group of genes undergoing continuous transcription has a
high content of CpG and low methylation levels. There are three groups of epigenetic
enzymes as DNA methyltransferases, histone methylase and histone deacetylase.
Cytosine methylation causes silencing, while demethylation, activation of gene expres-
sion. Methylated cytosines bind methyl-binding proteins (MBD), which prevent the
transcription factors accessing the gene promoter or interact with histone deacetyla-
ses, which induce chromatin condensation. Thus cytosine methylation determines spe-
cific at a given stage of the development, gene expression leading to tissue differen-
tiation. DNA methylation in mammalian cells is catalysed by four enzymes: DNMT1,
and DNMT3b and DNMT3a DNMT3L (Goll, 2008; Enlago, 2008; Yoo, 2006; Cheng,
2008; Brown, 2007). DNA methyltransferases 3a, 3b and 3L catalyse methylation de
novo during early embryonic development, while DNMT1 maintains the methylation
pattern in subsequent DNA replication. Methylation of the daughter strand of DNA
during the synthesis according to the pattern from maternal strand takes place in the
S phase of the cell cycle. Lack of DNMT1 in the replication complex causes an imba-
lance leading cell to the one of the many pathways of apoptosis. For example, in colon
cancer cell line with a knockout of DNMT1 gene a reduct ion in total methylation of the
genome was observed, which decreased more in the case of a double knockout of
DNMT1 and 3b. Deletion of the coding sequence or DNMT3a DNMT1 and 3b leads to
the lethality of murine embryos. DNMT1 activity allows the progenitor cells to multiple
J. Barciszewski, W.J. Paluchowski, D. Gurda, E. Hornowska, A. Jasielska44
divisions, by maintaining the proliferative activity and inhibition of differentiation of
these cells, thus providing the ability to self-renewal of tissue. Loss of methylation
during differentiation leads to the activation of inhibitors of cyclin-dependent kinases
– p15INK4B and p16INK4A, belonging to the tumour suppressor protein and regu-
lating the cell cycle. Mechanism of active DNA methylation was quite satisfactory
explained. But the reverse process – DNA demethylation very important on the stage
of cleaning the genomic slate during embryogenesis still become mysterious. Moreover
the mechanism of DNA demethylation proposed in response to rapid activation of
silence genes, which requires the multistage oxidation methyl group catalysed Tet
proteins also raises reasonable doubts. According to that mechanism methyl group is
oxidized in three step reaction through subsequent formation of 5-hydroxymethyl-,
5-formyl- and 5-carboxycytosine catalysed by Tet1-3 protein followed by base excision.
Other method of removing of methyl group proceed through cytosine deamination,
leading to uracil formation with subsequent base excision with repair enzymes (Ito,
2010). The other two genes which methylone will be studied are histone methyltrans-
ferase and histone deacetylase. In epigenetic analysis of individuals we will measure
global DNA methylation level as well as specific methylome DNA methyltransferase.
Psychology epigenetics
Psychology, social psychology, economics and sociology represent the scientific
disciplines dealing with human behavior, but epigenetics helps to explain how nurture
shapes nature, where nature refers to biological heredity, but nurture refers to virtually
everything that occurs during the life-span (e.g., social-experience, diet and nutrition,
and exposure to environment). Epigenetics in psychology can provide a framework for
understanding how the expression of genes is influenced by experiences and the
environment to produce individual differences in behavior, cognition, personality, and
mental health.
The goal of Epigenetics Psychology is the study and practical application of know-
ledge gained from epigenetics and from behavioral standpoints. It asks what can be
done by man to influence or obtain the desired functions? The effective management
of any human behavior requires a diagnosis at an early stage, which specifies the need
for specific and sensitive biomarkers.
The recent studies (Levenson, 2004) show that same processes that lead to the for-
mation of long-term memory also lead to the epigenetic characterization of the genome.
It has been assumed that changes in methylation are a response of the organism to
acquired experiences, and consequently transformations within long-term memory
(Lester, 2011). The studies on learning in animals suggested that DNA methylation is
dynamically regulated within the central nervous system (CNS) of adult individuals
Psychology goes molecular. Epigenetics of learning 45
upon the influence of experience, and that molecular mechanism is crucial for memory
formation (Blaze, 2013). In laboratory animal models, where the classical paradigms
of fear conditioning by an aversive stimulus were applied, it has been proven that the
epigenetic molecular mechanism that involves DNA methylation and demethylation
leads to long-term changes in behavior through active transcription of genes in CNS
(e.g., Lattal, 2013).
One should remember that learning or memorizing is differentiated from acquiring
knowledge (Nęcka, 2006). If we look at the process of learning in terms of a mechanism
thanks to which a human being is able to adjust to the dynamically changing envi-
ronment, then the critical element of this process will be the application of the acquired
knowledge in new circumstances. A characteristic trait of this process is detaching the
knowledge from the primary context of learning and applying it in many situations. It is
believed that only such a procedure makes it possible to check the actual adaptiveness
of the process of learning. Studies indicate that remembering the knowledge created
as a result of solving problems is the more difficult, the more time has passed since the
moment of its processing (Phyne, 1990).
For that reason, it is believed that the essence of the process of learning is trans-
ferring the acquired knowledge or skill to situations outside the situation in which it
has been acquired. For this process to take place, there has to occur the phenomenon
of transfer, i.e., the application of the acquired knowledge in new situations (Greeno,
1996). The probability of transfer increases due to de contextualization of learning
which consists in loosening the relationship between the content of learning and the
non-significant aspects of the context of learning (Perkins, 1989; Salomon, 1989).
Additionally, it has been assumed that emotional memory, which is created as
a result of fear conditioning in animals, (Day, 2010) can contribute to the explanation
of PTSD in humans (Zovkic, 2012). Learning of coping with stress in a situation of
sudden danger in the form of a defense reaction, specifically the reaction of conditioned
defensive burying (CDB) has also been investigated (Ahmadiyeh, 2005). Also, studies
of recognizing new objects and the formation of spatial memory e.g., with the use of
Morris water maze, (Sweat, 2009) have been conducted. Very interesting in this res-
pect are studies of psychopathology that take into consideration the mediational in-
fluence of epigenetic factors in schizophrenia (Roth, 2009; Toyokawa, 2012). In the
process of learning we can observe a change of behavior that corresponds to an alleged
mental change. Thus, mental change is identified with a memory record, which is
identified with changes in neuronal activity (Kandel, 1991). It has been assumed that
short-term memory is probably based upon slight changes in the strength of synaptic
connections, which result from the amount of the released neurotransmitter, whereas
long-term memory requires structural changes in synapses. These processes are ac-
J. Barciszewski, W.J. Paluchowski, D. Gurda, E. Hornowska, A. Jasielska46
companied by various molecular phenomena: in the case of short-term memory – modi-
fication of proteins that already exist, and in the case of long-term memory–synthesis
of new proteins thanks to gene expression (Squire, 2000). For many decades now, it
has been assumed that protein synthesis is crucial for the formation of long-term
memory through strengthening synaptic connections (Davis, 1984). With reference to
that, perhaps the epigenetic mechanism is engaged in the regulation of synaptic va-
riability that is required for the formation of long-term memory (Bird, 2007; Dulac,
2010; Levenson, 2005). In psychology, there has prevailed an opinion that learning is
to a large extent a well-investigated area, described in a way that does not raise too
many doubts (Anderson, 2000; Jagodzińska, 2008).
There are evidences that epigenetic modifications, specifically histone acetyla-
tion and methylation as well as DNA methylation, might be a molecular correlate of
long-term memory by modulating the stimulus dependent activation of learning-relevant
genes in memory-forming cellular networks. In addition, epigenetic modifications might
be functionally important during cellular and systems consolidation. Although this
theoretical framework highlights the potentially dual role of chromatin modifications
in learning and memory processes, the extent of DNA methylation and histone changes
and their functional implications remain unclear. So, DNA methylation plays a roles in
learning and memory.
DNA modification is changing during memory acquisition and correlated with varia-
tions in gene expression. Although long-lasting changes were almost exclusive to
neurons, learning-related epigenetic changes can occur in non-neuronal cell types,
suggesting a functional role for non-neuronal cells in epigenetic learning. In a molecular
framework of memory acquisition and maintenance, DNA methylation alters the ex-
pression and splicing of genes involved in functional plasticity and synaptic wiring.
Epigenetic changes, especially during memory consolidation, strongly suggest
a functional role of non-neuronal cells during memory formation. Although the exact
role of these changes in learning and memory processes requires further investigation,
it is quite unlikely that they represent a cellular correlation of memory, as their timing,
specificity and location. Nevertheless, these results imply that chromatin modification
changes are not necessarily of neuronal origin and provide further rationale for the cell
type-specific analysis of epigenetic modifications in the brain, especially in the emerg-
ing field of neuroepigenetics.
Learning as one of many social and individual issues has significant sociological,
economical, psychological as well as biological dimensions. In the past scholars tended
to focus on the simple and direct interaction between the genome and the environment
in the etiology of such phenomenon. What is highlighted nowadays it is the need for
considering and study the meditating role of the epigenome. It is postulated that diffe-
Psychology goes molecular. Epigenetics of learning 47
rent environmental exposures can impact epigenetic pattern, with important impli-
cations for individuals’ well-being (McGowan, 2010). For example; in his ‘4Rs’ model
of nutritional epigenomics John Mathers shows how dietary experience over time
is received and recorded by the human body, remembered during tissue regeneration
and revealed in patterns of gene expression. In other words, each individual has a fixed
genotype, but human’s phenotype is plastic in response to environmental factors,
and this plasticity is meditated, at least to some extent, by epigenetics mechanism
(Mathers, 2015). Only in recent years scholars appreciated that individuals’ life expe-
riences affect DNA methylation pattern, which is only one a kind of epigenetic mecha-
nism that modulates gene expression. Epigenetics is a study of semi-stable molecular
states that are sensitive to environmental clues rather than being exclusively endo-
genously determined and that have the capability to “learn” (Loi, 2013). Although there
are some striking examples of the ways in which environment modulate phenotype via
epigenetic mechanism, the epigenetics-based research is in its infancy (Mathers, 2015)
Perspectives
We would like to coin a new scientific discipline psychology epigenetics. It is lo-
cated on the border of social sciences, particularly psychology and molecular biology.
Two general questions are of special interest in psychology of epigenetics: i) how va-
rious, environmental conditions affect individual’s biologically embedded learning and
ii) whether the epigenetic markers reflect change of different conditions that affect indi-
vidual’s learning and memory capacity? It would be of special interest to:
1) Combining the global DNA methylation changes in human epigenome with variety
of learning.
2) Understanding the epigenetic mechanisms of how individuals’ react to different mo-
des of learning.
3) Identifying the differences in methylation level in individuals’ reactions to different
learning modes.
4) Understanding the limitations of experimental and survey studies of learning.
5) Identifying the biological mechanisms that affect various factors of subjective learn-
ing.
We think that it is possible to combine the methods of analysis of psychological stu-
dies with epigenetic markers as global DNA methylation changes to get better under-
standing of the human being. This is a very first application of epigenetics in psycholo-
gical studies.
Generally, by linking social with molecular life sciences it is possible to establish
the constructive and a perspective interdisciplinary dialogue. Shortly, we can say that
molecular biology meets social sciences.
J. Barciszewski, W.J. Paluchowski, D. Gurda, E. Hornowska, A. Jasielska48
References
Ahmadiyeh N., Churchill G.A., Solberg L.C., et al. (2005)
Lineage is an Epigenetic Modifier of
QTL Influencing Behavioral Coping with Stress.
Behav. Genet. 35, 189-198.
Anderson J.R. (1982)
Acquisition of cognitive skill
. Psychol. Rev. 89, 369-406.
Anderson J.R. (2000)
Learning and Memory: An Integrated Approach
, 2nd Ed. Hoboken, NJ,
John Wiley & Sons.
Barciszewska A.M., Murawa D., Gawronska I., et al. (2007b)
Analysis of 5-methylcytosine in
DNA of breast and colon cancer tissues
. IUBMB Life 59: 765-70.
Barciszewska A.M., Nowak S., Gawronska I., Barciszewska, M.Z. (2009).
Diagnosis of brain tu-
mors through global specific DNA methylation analysis.
[in:]
Therapeutic Ribonucleic Acids
in Brain Tumors
, Springer Verlag, Heidelberg, (V.A. Erdmann, G. Reifenberger, J. Barcisze-
wski Eds.) 141-156.
Barciszewska M.Z., Barciszewska A.M., Rattan S.I.S. (2007a)
TLC-based detection of methyla-
ted cytosine: application to aging epigenetics.
Biogerontology 8, 673-678.
Barondes S.H. Jarvik M.E. (1964).
The influence of actinomycin-D on brain RNA synthesis and
on memory
. J. Neurochem. 11, 187-195.
Bauer P.J. (2004).
Getting explicit memory off the ground: Steps toward construction of a neuro-
developmental account of changes in the first two years of life
. Develop’ Rev. Special Me-
mory Development in the New Millennium 24, 347-373.
Berry D.C. (1996) (Ed.).
How Implicit is Implicit Learning
. Oxford University Press.
Bird A. (2007).
Perceptions of epigenetics
. Nature 447, 396-398.
Blaze J., Roth T.L. (2013).
Epigenetic mechanism in learning and memory
. WIREs Cognitive
Science, 4, 105-115.
Brown K.D., Robertson K.D. (2007).
DNMT1 knockout delivers a strong blow to genome sta-
bility and cell viability
. Nat. Genet. 3, 289-90.
Chlewiński Z., Hankała A., Jagodzińska M., Mazurek B. (1997).
Psychologia pamięci. Leksykon
Omegi
. Warszawa: Wiedza Powszechna.
Cloninger C.R. (1994a).
Temperament and personality
. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 4/2, 266-273.
Cloninger C.R. (1994b).
The genetic structure of personality and learning: A phylogenetic mo-
del.
Clin. Genet. 46, 124-137.
Cloninger C.R., Svrakic D.M., Przybeck T.R. (1993).
A psychobiological model of temperament
and character.
Archiv. Gen. Psych. 50, 975-990.
Cohen H.D., H. Barondes S.H. (1966).
Further studies of learning and memory after intra-
cerebral actinomycin-D
. J. Neurochem. 13, 207-211.
Colom R., Escorial S., Shih P.Ch., Privado J. (2007).
Fluid intelligence, memory span, and tem-
perament difficulties predict academic performance of young adolescents. Personal
. Individ.
Differ. 42, 1503-1514.
Colombo J. (2004).
Visual attention in infancy: Process and product in early cognitive develop-
ment
. [In:] M.I. Posner (Ed.),
Cognitive neuroscience of attention
. New York: Guilford
Press.
Davis H.P. Squire L.R. (1984).
Protein synthesis and memory: A review
. Psychol. Bull. 96, 518-
559.
Day J.J., Sweatt J.D. (2010).
DNA methylation and memory formation
. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1319-
1323.
Dulac C. (2010).
Brain function and chromatin plasticity
. Nature 465, 728-735.
Psychology goes molecular. Epigenetics of learning 49
Elias M.J. (2004).
The connection between social-emotional learning and learning disabilities:
Implications for intervention
. Learn. Disab. Quart. 27, 53-63.
Enlago N., Yi S.V. (2008).
DNA methylation and structural and functional bimodality of verte-
brate promoters
. Mol. Biol. Evol. 25, 1602-1608.
Esteller M. (2007).
Cancer epigenomics: DNA methylomes and histone – modification maps
.
Nat. Rev. Genet. 8, 286-298.
Fagen J.W., Ohr P.S., Singer J., Fleckenstein L.K. (1987).
Infant temperament and subject loss
due to crying during parent conditioning
. Child Develop. 58, 497-504.
Fagnou D.D. Tuchek J.M. (1995).
The biochemistry of learning and memory
. Molecular and
Cell. Biochem. 149, 279-286
Gagne R.M., Yekovich C., Yekovich F. (1993).
The cognitive psychology of school learning
. New
York; Harper Collins.
Gaunt T.R., et al. (2016)
Systematic identification of genetic influences
. Genome Biology 17, 61
.
Goll M.G. Bestor T.H. (2005).
Eukaryotic cytosine methyltransferases
. Ann. Rev. Biochem. 74,
481-514.
González-Pardo H. Álvarez M.P. (2013).
Epigenetics and its implications for Psychology.
Psico-
thema 25, 3-12.
Gordon P.C., Holyoak K.J. (1983).
Implicit learning and generalization of the ”mere exposu-
re”effect.
J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 45, 492-500.
Gray J.A. (1964).
Strenght of the nervous system and levels of arousal: A reinterpretation
. [In:]
J.A. Gray (ed.).
Pavlov’s typology
(p. 289-364). Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Greeno J.G., Collins A.M., Resnick L.B. (1996).
Cognition and learning
. [In:] D.C. Berliner, R.C.
Calfee (ed.),
Handbook of educational psychology
(p. 15-46). New York: Simon & Schuster
Macmillan.
Haigis M.C., Deng C.X., Finley L.W., et al. (2010).
SIRT3 is a mitochondrial tumor suppressor:
a scientific tale that connects aberrant cellular ROS, the Warburg effect, and carcinogenesis
.
Can. Res. 72, 2468-2472.
Hauser J., Hornowska E., Rybakowski F., et al. (2003).
Polska wersja kwestionariusza Tempera-
ment and Character Inventory R.C. Cloningera
. Studia Psychol. 41, 159-179.
Hornowska E. (2003).
Temperamentalne uwarunkowania zachowania. Badania z wykorzysta-
niem skali TCI R.C. Cloningera
. Poznań, Bogucki Wydawnictwo Naukowe.
Hornowska E. (2006).
Temperament a zachowania ryzykowne. Wokół temperamentalnego czyn-
nika ryzyka uzależnień
. Przegl. Psychol. 3, 243-259.
Hornowska E. (2011).
Cloninger’s Psychobiological Model of Personality and Strelau’s Regula-
tive Theory of Temperament – analysis of their associations in a Polish sample
. Pol. Psy-
chol. Bull. 42, 2, 71-80.
Hornowska E., Kaliszewska K. (2003).
Neurogenetyczna koncepcja osobowości R.C. Cloningera
– związki z teorią PEN H.J. Eysencka oraz Modelem Wielkiej Piątki w ujęciu P.T. Costy
i R.R. McCrae
. Czasop. Psychol. 1, 7-15.
Cheng X. (2008). Blumenthal RM. Mammalian DNA methyltransferases: a structural perspec-
tive. Structure 16, 341-50.
Chlewiński Z., Hankała A., Jagodzińska M., Mazurek B. (1992).
Psychologia pamięci
. Warszawa:
Wiedza Powszechna.
Ito S., D'Alessio A.C., Taranova O.V., et al. (2010).
Ro le of Tet protei ns in 5mC to 5h mC conver-
sion, ES-cell self-renewal and inner cell mass specification
. Nature 466, 1129-1133.
Jagodzińska M. (2008).
Psychologia pamięci. Badania, teorie, zastosowania
. Gliwice: Helion.
J. Barciszewski, W.J. Paluchowski, D. Gurda, E. Hornowska, A. Jasielska50
Jasielska A. (2009).
Analiza wpływu stereotypu płci w obszarze emocji na edukację emocjonalną
.
Forum Oświatowe 1, 71-87.
Kandel E.R., Schwartz J.H., Jessell T (1991).
Principles of neural science
. New York: Elsevier.
Kirsner K., Speelman C. Maybery M., et al. (1998).
Implicit and Explicit Mental Processes.
New
York, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Krumhuber E.G., Tamarit L., Roesch E.B., Scherer K.R. (2012)
FACS Gen 2.0 animation soft-
ware: Generating three-dimensional FACS-valid facial expressions foremotion research.
Emotion 12, 351-363.
Lattal K.M., Marcelo A., Wood M.A. (2013).
Epigenetics and persistent memory: implications
for reconsolidation and silent extinction beyond the zero
. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 124-129.
Lester B.M., Tronick E., Nestler E., et al. (2011).
Behavioral epigenetics
. Ann. New York Acad.
Sci. 1226, 14-33.
Levenson, J. M, O'Riordan, K. J., Brown, K.D. et al. (2004).
Regulation of histone acetylation
during memory formation in the hippocampus
. J. Biol. Chem. 279, 40545-59.
Levenson J.M., Sweatt J.D. (2005).
Epigenetic mechanism in memory formation
. Nat. Rev. Neu-
rosci. 6, 108-118.
Levenson J.M., Choi S., Lee S.Y., et al. (2004) A
bioinformatics analysis of memory consolida-
tion reveals involvement of the transcription factor c-rel
. J. Neurosci. 24, 3933-3943.
Loi M., Del Savio, L, Stupka E. (2013).
Social Epigenetics and Equality of Opportunity
, Pub.
Health Ethics 6, 142-153.
Matczak A., Piekarska J., Studniarek A. (2005).
Skala Inteligencji Emocjonalnej – Twarze.
SIE-T.
Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych PTP.
Mathers J. (2015)
Epigenetics
, [In:] (eds) J.A. Lovegrove, L. Hodson, S. Sharma, S.A. Lanham
New Nutrition Research Methodologies,
John Wiley & Sons; p. 212-224.
Mayer J.D., Salovey P., Caruso D.R. (2004).
Emotional Intelligence: Theory, Findings, and Impli-
cations.
Psychol. Inquiry 15, 197-215.
Mayer J.D., Salovey P., Caruso D.R., Sitarenios G. (2003).
Measuring emotional intelligence
with the MSCEIT V 2.0
. Emotion 3, 97-105.
Mayer R.E. (2001).
Changing conceptions of learning: A century of progress in the scientific
study of education
. [In:] L. Corno (Ed.),
Education across a century
(pp. 34-75). Chicago;
University of Chicago Press.
McGowan P.O., Szyf M. (2010).
The Epigenetics of Social Adversity in Early Life: Implications
for Mental Health Outcomes
. Neurobiol. Dis. 39, 66-72.
McGuinness D., McGlynn L.M., Johnson P.C.D., et al. (2012).
Socioeconomic status is asso-
ciated with epigenetic differences in the pSoBid cohort
. Int. J. Epidemiol. 41, 151-60.
Nęcka E., Orzechowski J., Szymura B. (2006).
Psychologia poznawcza
. Warszawa: Academica.
Nicolosi G., Ruivenkamp G. (2012).
The epigenetic turn. Some notes about the epistemological
change of perspective in biosciences.
Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy 15, 309-319.
Nummenmaa T. (1992).
Pure and Blended Emotion in the Human Face. Psychometric experi-
ments.
Finnish Academy of Science and Letters Annales Academiae Scientiarum Fennicae:
Jyväskylä.
Perkins D.N., Solomon G. (1989).
Are cognitive skills context-bound?
Edu. Res. 18, 16-25.
Phye G.D. (1990)
Inductive problem solving: schema inducement and memory-based transfer
.
J. Educ. Psychol. 82, 826-831.
Putnam A.L. Ozubko J.D., MacLeod C.M., Roediger H.L. (2013).
The production effect in paired
associate learning.
Memory and Cognition.
Psychology goes molecular. Epigenetics of learning 51
Reber A.S. (1989).
Implicit Learning and Tacit Knowledge
. J. Exp. Psychol. CXVIII, 219-235.
Reber A.S. (2010).
Dictionary of psychology
. London: Penguin/Viking.
Richards J.E. (2004).
The development of sustained attention in infants
. [In:] M.I. Posner (Ed.),
Cognitive neuroscience of attention; cognitive neuroscience of attention
(p. 342-356). New
York, NY, USA: Guilford Press.
Roberson E.D., Sweatt J.D. (1999).
A biochemical blueprint for long-term memory
. Learn. Mem.
6, 381-388.
Roth T.L., Lubin F.D., Sodhi M., Kleinman J.E. (2009).
Epigenetic mechanisms in schizophre-
nia
. Biochem. Biophys. Acta 1790, 869-877.
Rybakowski F., Slopien A., Zakrzewska M., et al. (2004).
Temperament and Character Inventory
(TCI) in adolescents with anorexia nervosa
. Acta Neuropsych. 16, 169-174.
Salovey P., Detweiler-Bedell B.T., Detweiler-Bedell J.B., Mayer J.D. (2008).
Emotional Intelli-
gence.
[In:] M. Lewis, J.M. Haviland-Jones, & L.F. Barrett (Eds.),
Handbook of emotions
(p. 533-547). New York, NY: Guilford.
Sarason I.G., Johnson J.H., Siegel J.M. (1978).
Assessing the impact of life changes: Develop-
ment of the Life Experiences Survey
. J. Consul. Clin. Psychol. 46, 932-946
Seifert L.S. (1993).
Real-world knowledge facilitates interpretation of paintings by naive art ob-
serves
. J. Psychol. 127, 573-575.
Selcher J.C., Weeber E.J., Varga A.W. et al. (2002).
Protein kinase signal transduction cascades
in mammalian associative conditioning
. Neuroscientist 8, 122-131.
Smolarek I., Wyszko E., Barciszewska A.M., et al. (2010)
Global DNA methylation changes in
blood of patients with essential hypertension
. Med. Sci. Monit. 16: CR149-155.
Sobolewski A., Strelau J., Zawadzki B. (1999).
Kwestionariusz Zmian Życiowych (KZŻ): Polska
adaptacja kwestionariusza “Recent Life ChangesQestionnaire” (RLCQ) R.H. Rahe’a.
Przeg.
Psychol. 42, 27-49.
Solomon G., Perkins D.N. (1989).
Transfer of cognitive skills from programming: When and
how?
J. Edu. Comp. Res. 3, 149-169.
Squire L.R., Kandel E.R. (2000).
From mind to molecules
. New York, Scientific American.
Sternberg R.J., Sternberg K. (2015).
Cognitive psychology
, 7th Ed. Boston: Cengage Lerning.
Strelau J. (2014).
żnice indywidualne
. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe Scholar.
Sweatt J.D. (2009).
Experience-dependent epigenetic modifications in the CNS
. Biol. Psych. 65,
191-197.
Szewczuk W. (red.) (1985).
Ćwiczenia eksperymentalne z psychologii ogólnej
. Warszawa: PWN.
Toyokawa S., Uddin M. Koenen K.C., Galea S. (2012).
How does the social environment “get
into the mind”? Epigenetics at the intersection of social and psychiatric epidemiology
. Soc.
Sci. Med. 74, 67-74.
Wolfe C.D., Bell M.A. (2004).
Working memory and inhibitory control in early childhood: Con-
tributions from physiology, temperament, and language
. Develop. Psychobiol. 44, 68.
Yoo C.B., Jones P.A. (2006).
Epigenetic therapy of cancer: past, present and future
. Nat. Rev.
Drug Discov. 5, 37-50.
Zovkic I.B., Sweatt J.D. (2012).
Epigenetic Mechanisms in Learned Fear: Implications for
PTSD.
Neuropsychopharm. Rev. 1-17
Predicting educational achievement from DNA,
S.
Selzam, et al.
Molecular Psychiatry (2016) 00, 1-6.
J. Barciszewski, W.J. Paluchowski, D. Gurda, E. Hornowska, A. Jasielska52
Abstract
The challenge for psychology is to integrate findings from genetics and environ-
mental (social, biological, chemical) factors, into the study of human behavior and
deep understanding of the emergence of different changes in the anatomy, phy-
siology, and chemistry of the nervous system that influence the mental health.
Currently, cognitive abilities associated with learning and memory, reasoning,
problem solving, and developing relationships are in scope of molecular psycho-
logy, which is the study of behavior and its underlying brain systems using the
tools of molecular biology. However, studies have demonstrated that DNA se-
quence variations and rare mutations account for only a small fraction of the risk
for inheritance of personality traits and mental illness. The large unaccounted
heritability of personality traits and mental health suggest that additional mole-
cular and cellular mechanisms are involved. Various complex gene-environment
interactions can lead to different phenotypes. These structural changes may be
crucial for the development of mature neural networks that support emotional,
cognitive, and social behavior. The generation of different morphology, physiolo-
gy, and behavioral outcomes from a single genome in response to changes in the
environment forms the basis for phenotypic plasticity, which is fundamental to
the way organisms cope with environmental variation, navigate the present
world, and solve future problems. Epigenetics has major implications for psycho-
logy and gives the new answer for the old question- what is the biochemical basis
of learning. It is bringing back the leading role of environment and behavior, by
including their effects on genome function. In addition, it opens up the possibility
of memory being stored in the epigenome, so that our experiences may be em-
bedded in our genome by epigenetic mechanisms. Epigenetics can be described
as the study of the complex interactions underlying the development of an orga-
nism over its lifetime.
Key words: behavioral epigenetics, cognitive functions, DNA methylation, epige-
netic inheritance, epigenetics, gene-environment interplay, learning, memory
Article
Loneliness is a negative and distressing emotional state that arises from a discrepancy between one's desired and achieved levels of social connectedness. The evolutionary theory of loneliness (ETL) posits that experiencing loneliness is an inherited adaptation that signals that salutary social relations are endangered or damaged and prompts people to reconnect to significant others. The basic tenets of the ETL has led researchers to examine the genetic underpinnings of loneliness. The current review provides an updated overview of genetic studies on loneliness and discusses the importance of genetic research for the ETL. The most recent studies suggest that the many genes that contribute to a small degree to differences in loneliness partially overlap with genes that contribute to neuroticism, but not with depression. In addition, the genetic studies discussed in this review show that genes are unlikely to have a direct effect on loneliness. Instead, environmental factors determine in a dynamic fashion how genes that contribute to loneliness are expressed. Future research on epigenetic processes, such as DNA methylation, can further elucidate the dynamic interplay between genes and the environment and how this interplay contributes to loneliness.
Article
Full-text available
Background: The influence of genetic variation on complex diseases is potentially mediated through a range of highly dynamic epigenetic processes exhibiting temporal variation during development and later life. Here we present a catalogue of the genetic influences on DNA methylation (methylation quantitative trait loci (mQTL)) at five different life stages in human blood: children at birth, childhood, adolescence and their mothers during pregnancy and middle age. Results: We show that genetic effects on methylation are highly stable across the life course and that developmental change in the genetic contribution to variation in methylation occurs primarily through increases in environmental or stochastic effects. Though we map a large proportion of the cis-acting genetic variation, a much larger component of genetic effects influencing methylation are acting in trans. However, only 7 % of discovered mQTL are trans-effects, suggesting that the trans component is highly polygenic. Finally, we estimate the contribution of mQTL to variation in complex traits and infer that methylation may have a causal role consistent with an infinitesimal model in which many methylation sites each have a small influence, amounting to a large overall contribution. Conclusions: DNA methylation contains a significant heritable component that remains consistent across the lifespan. Our results suggest that the genetic component of methylation may have a causal role in complex traits. The database of mQTL presented here provide a rich resource for those interested in investigating the role of methylation in disease.
Article
Full-text available
Cloninger's Psychobiological Model of Personality and Strelau's Regulative Theory of Temperament - analysis of their associations in a Polish sample The present study explores the relationship between Cloninger scales and Strelau's Regulative Theory of Temperament (RTT) traits. Cloninger's psychobiological model identifies four dimensions of temperament (Novelty seeking, Harm Avoidance, Reward Dependence and Persistence) and three dimensions of character (Self-Directedness, Cooperativeness and Self-Transcendence). RTT proposes the traits of Briskness, Perseveration, Sensory Sensitivity, Emotional Reactivity, Endurance and Activity as the basic dimensions underlying individual differences. The relationships between the dimensions of Cloninger's Temperament and Character and Strelau's Regulative Theory of Temperament are investigated in a sample of 282 participants. Data analysis demonstrated some significant correlations between the two models. The strongest associations were found between the dimension of Harm Avoidance from Cloninger's concept and Strelau's RTT traits (positive correlations from r = .73 for Emotional Reactivity to r = .48 for Perseveration, and negative correlations from r = -.57 for Endurance to r = -.51 for Briskness). However, there is not enough evidence to suggest that the two models offer an alternative way of explaining individual differences. According to RTT, temperamental traits are expressed in formal characteristics of behavior (energetic and temporal) and every kind of behavior (irrespective of its content) can be described in the same formal categories. In the Psychobiological Model of Personality the traits are characterized rather by the content or goals of behavior. In this model, in addition to underlining the biological variation of heritable traits, the social, cultural and phenotypical levels of behavior are reflected.
Article
Nature Reviews Drug Discovery 5, 37–50 (2006); 10.1038/nrd1930In Table 1 (p. 40
Article
Recent discoveries have associated epigenetic mechanisms, including DNA methylation, histone modifications, and microRNA (miRNA) processing, with activity-dependent changes in gene expression necessary to drive long-term memory formation. Here, we discuss the current interpretation of epigenetic mechanisms in the context of memory and sustained behavioral change. One of the two emerging viewpoints is that epigenetic mechanisms subserve information storage in the central nervous system (CNS), a notion supported by rodent studies of fear, recognition and spatial memories, and stress. The second viewpoint is that epigenetics serves as a mechanism for passing on acquired information across generations, a provocative notion now supported by several lines of work using developing and adult rodents. Continued research on such mechanisms promises to advance our understanding of biological pathways linking experiences to long-term and even multigenerational trajectories in neurobiology and behavior. WIREs Cogn Sci 2013, 4:105-115. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1205 For further resources related to this article, please visit the WIREs website. Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.