ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Emo youth subculture bases its subcultural identification mostly on symbols of death, dying, suicide and other kinds of morbid content. The main goal of the present study was to explore attitudes toward suicidal behaviour and self-injury in emo adolescents. Semistructured in-depth qualitative interviews were conducted with 14 emo adherents, accompanied by an analysis of emo Web discussion forums. The results show a subculture-specific display of rules, i.e. norms of how one should express emotions. These unusual display rules enabled emo community members to express any actual emotional experience in public spaces without any inhibition. Emo participants reported that the main philosophy of the emo subculture is “to experience and express actual emotions without any restraint”. Furthermore, participants reported attitudes that included high acceptance of suicidal behaviour and self-injury. The escalation of strong emotions, for example, strong depression in the form of suicide, was perceived as something quite common in emo communities. Identification with the emo youth subculture is considered to be a factor strengthening vulnerability towards risky behaviours. Keywords: Risky Behaviors, Risky Behaviours, Problematic Behaviors, Problematic Behaviours, Suicide, Suicidal Behavior, Suicidal Behaviour, Suicidal Ideation, Self-Injury, Self-Harm, Deliberate Self-Harm, Non-Suicidal Self Injury, Nonsuicidal Self Injury, Self-Injurious Behavior, Self-Destructive Behavior, Self Mutilation, Klonsky Affect-Regulation Model, Youth Subcultures, Music-Based Subcultures, Youth, Teens, Adolescents, Adolescence, Peer Contagion, Developmental Psychology, Adolescent Psychology, Death, Dying. MeSH Headings: Suicide, Dangerous Behavior, Suicidal Ideation, Self-Injurious Behavior, Self Mutilation, Adolescent, Psychology, Developmental
Content may be subject to copyright.
Death Studies, 2018, Vol. 42, No. 6, 337-345
Understanding death, suicide and self-injury among adherents of the
emo youth subculture: A qualitative study
Radek Trnka a,b, Martin Kuška a,b, Karel Balcar a, and Peter Tavel b
a Science and Research Department, Prague College of Psychosocial Studies, Prague, Czech Republic
b Social Health Institute - OUSHI, Palacký University, Olomouc, Czech Republic
Note: This is the preprint version of the manuscript, the published article is available at
https://doi.org/10.1080/07481187.2017.1340066
Abstract
Emo youth subculture bases its subcultural identification mostly on symbols of death, dying, suicide,
and other kinds of morbid content. The main goal of the present study was to explore attitudes toward
suicidal behavior and self-injury in emo adolescents. Semistructured in-depth qualitative interviews
were conducted with 14 emo adherents, accompanied by the analysis of emo Web discussion forums.
Participants reported attitudes including high acceptance for suicidal behavior and self-injury. The
identification with the emo youth subculture is considered to be a factor strengthening vulnerability
towards risky behaviors.
Introduction
EMO youth subculture bases its subcultural identification on symbols of death, dying, suicide
and the mutilation of body parts. Adherents of this subculture are mostly in adolescence, which is a
critical period otherwise highly vulnerable for engaging in risky behaviors such as suicide (Anderson,
Lowry, & Wuensch, 2015; Miers, Abbott, & Springer, 2012; Wood, Byram, Gosling, & Stokes, 2012)
or self-injury (Anestis & Selby, 2015; Heilbron & Prinstein, 2008; Swannell, Martin, Page, Hasking, &
St John, 2014). Research on suicidal behavior in adolescence is undoubtedly very important (see e.g.,
Andriessen, Draper, Dudley, & Mitchell, 2016). There is a topical and strong need to explore not only
cultural factors relating to the suicidal behavior of adolescents (e.g., Chu, Goldblum, Floyd, & Bongar,
2010; Langhinrichsen-Rohling, Friend, & Powell, 2009), but also the influence of peers, for example,
on the level of youth subcultures. In contrast to the influences of gender (e.g. Allooh, Rummell, &
Levant, 2013; Ryalls, 2013), culture (Eskin, Schild, Öncü, Stieger, & Voracek, 2015), race (Anderson,
Lowry, & Wuensch, 2015; Mortara & Ironico, 2013) or ethnicity (Range et al., 1999; Chu, Goldblum,
Floyd, & Bongar, 2010), less attention has thus far been paid to the role of subcultural identification in
adolescent attitudes toward suicidal behavior.
Given that adolescence is a period during which peers are especially influential (e.g., Dishion
& Tipsord, 2011; Spear, 2000), adhering to some youth subcultures may be related to a specific kind of
peer influence. Young et al. (2006) found an influence of identification with several youth subcultures
on the risk of suicide attempts and self-injury. Further, the study of Bowes et al. (2015) showed that
young people identifying with Goth subculture might be at increased risk for depression and self-harm.
Stack, Lester, and Rosenberg (2012) even refer to some music-based subcultures as suicidogenic. Goth
subculture shares some aspects with EMO subculture, such as a morbid visual aesthetic, and one may
seriously ask about the possible relationship between youth subcultural identification and attitudes
towards self-harming or suicide.
Figure 1. Adherents of the EMO youth subculture. (Photo: Radek Trnka)
Given the above-mentioned findings, a detailed, in-depth investigation of the influence of
adhering to EMO youth subculture on suicidal behavior in adolescents is desirable. When seeking a link
between the social influence of peers and problematic adolescent behavior, the theory of peer contagion
(Dishion & Tipsord, 2011) offers a suitable theoretical basis. Generally, the concept of peer contagion
describes "a mutual influence process that occurs between an individual and a peer and includes
behaviors and emotions that potentially undermine one’s own development or cause harm to others"
(Dishion & Tipsord, 2011, p. 190). The mechanism of peer contagion is not sufficiently clear, but Bowes
et al. (2015) suggest that interpersonal processes and peer contagion may be related to an increased risk
for self-harm in adherents of Goth youth subculture. Also, the interpersonal theory of suicide emphasizes
that the suicidal ideation is interpersonal in nature (Lockman & Servaty-Seib, 2016).
As mentioned at the beginning of this paper, EMO subcultural identification is based on the
interiorization of symbols of death, dying or suicide, and these symbols as well as other kinds of morbid
aesthetic are shared in adherents of EMO youth subculture. There is evidence that public opinion
generally links the EMO youth subculture with self-harming and suicidal behavior (Strauss, 2012). EMO
adolescents are generally perceived as being emotionally oversensitive, easily hurt, often frustrated,
depressed and also highly vulnerable to committing suicide (Definis-Gojanovic, Gugic, & Sutlovic,
2009). Although this public opinion may be biased in part due to EMO’s morbid aesthetics, self-
destructive behavior and suicidal tendencies definitely do occur in EMO communities (Johnson &
Cloonan, 2009).
The EMO subculture is a highly individualized one, but it is possible to distinguish several
features shared by its adherents. The most striking feature of the EMO subculture is the visual
appearance of EMO adolescents (Bailey, 2006; Kuška, Formankova, & Kolarova, 2010), which also
serves as an identifying marker of their interiorized EMO lifestyle (see Figure 1). Their clothing is
usually tuned to dark colors, often with brightly colored accessories in girls, while boys’ clothing evokes
an overall impression of shabbiness (Kuška, Formankova, & Kolarova, 2010). They wear canvas
sneakers and put dark make up around the eyes. The hairstyle is especially distinctive, often dyed to
dark shades with a quiff covering one or both eyes. The lyrics of EMO music groups as well as EMO
comic books are saturated with death, suicides, blood, images of pain, sorrow, suffering as well as with
bodily mutilations (Baker & Bor, 2008; de Boise, 2014). There are images such as cut wrists, hollowed-
out eyes, a heart extracted from the chest, penetration of the skull, and so on. It is reasonable to ask
about what kind of attitudes toward suicide and self-injury may be found in adolescent adherents of
EMO subculture.
Adherents of EMO communities represent an example of a hard-to-reach population. EMO
adherents are closed or even inaccessible to people outside of the EMO subculture. For this reason,
EMO is a quite rarely studied subculture, and quantitative research of suicidal behavior and ideation in
EMO adolescents is lacking. Several papers that have been published on EMO subculture were based
on virtual analysis from Internet resources, without personal contact of researchers with EMO
adolescents (e.g., Chernoff & Widdicombe, 2015; Seganti & Smahel, 2011; Zdanow & Wright, 2012).
Other recent research on EMO subculture has been conducted in more general field of popular culture
(e.g., Red, 2014), lifestyle (Rohde, 2016; Mortara & Ironico, 2013) or specific aspects, like crisis of
masculinity (de Boise, 2014; Ryalls, 2013) or gender in general (Allooh, Rummell, & Levant, 2013;
Rohde, 2016). The purpose of the present study is to explore attitudes toward suicide and self-injury in
adolescent adherents of the EMO youth subculture. We focus on the ways EMO adolescents understand
and interpret suicidal and self-injuring behavior and analyze the meanings that self-injury and suicidal
behavior have for adolescent adherents of EMO subculture.
Methods
Participants and Procedure
All participants were caucasian Czechs living in the Czech Republic. They participated
voluntarily without any financial reimbursement. The adherents of the EMO subculture were contacted
personally at public events, like the concerts of EMO music groups, EMO events in music clubs and
outdoor EMO meetings. Adolescent adherents of the EMO subculture behaved very distantly. They
appeared self-contained, shy, inaccessible and leery of the researchers. We contacted 186 EMO
adherents in total, but 149 of them strictly refused the invitation for an interview immediately, and
communication with those who did agree to the interview was very delicate. Twenty-three interviews
were later canceled, and many contacts with participants were not successful in the starting stage of the
research. The semi-structured qualitative interviews were mostly conducted in music clubs before their
program started, or in bars, fast food restaurants and cafés following the initial contact and negotiation
with the potential EMO participants. Several interviews were conducted at EMO outdoor meetings in a
park in the center of Prague. All interviews were recorded on a Dictaphone, and all participants agreed
with the recording of their interviews and provided signed informed consent with participation in the
study. Signed parental informed consent was obtained in cases of participants younger than 18 years.
The average duration of a recorded interview was 59 minutes, and a total of 14 semi-structured in-depth
interviews were included in the final sample (6 EMO boys; 8 EMO girls; aged 15 to 19).
The research design was approved by the institutional ethics committee. We always respected
the adolescents’ refusal to participate or continue in their participation (CIOMS, 2002). We assured
participants about protecting the confidentiality of the research data. The true names of the participants
were changed when presenting their testimonies in the following text. Although a recent review of
Dazzi, Gribble, Wessely, and Fear (2014) suggests that talking about suicide in fact reduces, rather than
increases, suicidal ideation, we were aware that we were investigating potentially vulnerable
participants, and thus contacting as well as interviewing them was done very sensitively.
Measures The structure of the in-depth interviews included seven broader areas: family atmosphere,
personal values and identity, attitudes toward suicide and self-harming, circumstances of joining EMO,
EMO commercial products, identification features of the EMO community, and future life plans. The
presentation of the whole data set far exceeds the focus and extent of the present study; therefore,
attention will be primarily paid to attitudes towards suicide and self-injury and the ways the participants
make sense of phenomena like suicide and self-injury. Data concerning these issues will be presented
in this study.
The battery of questions in the second half of the interview reflected the participants’ attitudes
and understanding of suicide and self-injury and included questions on personal experiences with suicide
or self-injury (both their own and their EMO friends) and personal opinions on what self-injury and
suicide mean to them. We also asked additional questions based on the actual course of the particular
interview. We remained open to new issues and dimensions that emerged during the interview.
Transcriptions of all interviews were made at the end of the interviewing stage of the project.
The qualitative research was accompanied by supplementary content analysis of Web
discussion forums in the Czech language. The utilization of strategies based on samples drawn from
Web pages dedicated to the music subculture is also recommended by Stack, Lester, and Rosenberg
(2012). We found and analyzed eight online discussions from two public EMO portals and contents and
discussions from nine EMO blogs.
Analytical Strategy
The interviews were analyzed using the inductive content analysis that has been proved to be
a suitable analysis strategy in qualitative descriptive studies (Rissanen, Kylma, & Laukkanen, 2009;
Sandelowski, 2000; Sullivan-Bolyai, Knafl, Tamborlane, & Grey, 2004). The analytical strategy was
further inspired by the method of interpretative phenomenological analysis (Smith, Flowers, & Larkin,
2009) in the sense that we focused on understanding the experiential world of the participants. More
specifically, we focused on insights into how the participants make sense of phenomena like suicide and
self-injury.
The transcripts of the interviews were subjected to multiple close readings. All the statements
in which a participant told something about self-injury or suicide were coded independently by two
researchers (M.K., R.T.) and validated by another researcher (K.B.). The process of validation was based
on the comparison of codes from two independent coders (M.K., R.T.) by a third researcher (K.B.), who
also recommended various improvements for coding. The codes were grouped into categories based on
their similarities and differences, and the categories were named according to their content. All four
authors agreed on the selection of representative quotations. The contents from the supplementary
analysis of Web discussion forums were analyzed in a similar way.
Results
Suicide as a Natural Outcome of Strong Emotions
EMO participants expressed high acceptance of suicide, suicidal behavior and deliberate self-
injury. Suicide was not perceived as something strange or pathological, but rather as a way that “serious
problems” can be solved. Testimonies often included notions about “serious problems”, “escape from
reality”, “great passions” or “big emotions”. Emotions occupy a central position in the general
philosophy of the EMO subculture, as reported by our EMO participants. The main philosophy of the
EMO subculture is “to experience and express your actual emotions without any restraint”. During the
contact stage of the research, we often observed at public events that EMO adolescents overtly expressed
intensive emotions like crying or angry screaming, i.e. those emotions whose overt expression is
otherwise mostly restricted by cultural display rules in public spaces. Thus, the escalation of, for
example, strong depression in the form of suicide is perceived as something quite common in EMO
communities. EMO participants in the present study also reported stories about the successful suicides
of their past EMO friends as well as about their own suicide attempts. One participant, Viktor, had
already attempted suicide three times unsuccessfully:
Researcher: Have you ever considered the idea that you would try to harm yourself?
Viktor: No, I loathe the fact that I have veins and arteries and blood, etc. … I cannot stand seeing blood
… even if I cut my finger; it's crazy, isn't it? And mainly, why should I have any scars? Should I have a
big problem, I'd just do it … and it would be really freaky … and it would be really mad, that it would
simply be the end, I would just kill myself, but cut myself? Why?
Researcher: What would have to happen for you to make you to cut yourself?
Viktor: What would happen? Well, I'd cut myself, because I want to die. And it would become necessary,
for example, I don't know, if I had a girlfriend for five years and she died. In this case I would think
about it, because I would be sad, but I think maybe it's not necessary to kill myself because of
something like this. I think … I would do it … I don't know … only if I just would be threatened by an
awful danger and nothing could be done about it … Just when "they" would come for me in my house
and someone would want to burn or beat or kill me, I don't know, murder, mafia, in such a case, I would
do it, but otherwise I find it unnecessary to erase myself from the world.
Viktor belonged to a group of EMO adherents with a high occurrence of completed suicides.
It is likely that his own attitudes towards suicidal behavior may also be related to the influence of suicidal
behavior of his previous EMO friends. To the question “Do you have any EMO friends?” he answered:
Viktor: Not any more. I just look for new friends everywhere I do not know I just want such a
friend … I have always needed such friend, and I had … but all of the people died. And those who were
here at the top … on the peak, the style … simply awful people … everyone knew them and talked about
them. When their nickname was said, everyone knew who it was. But … they just stopped … None of my
friends remain; everyone said that I am the last, and this is true no one is like that among the people
I know.
Suicide as a Common Solution to Life Problems
Suicide is perceived as a one of the acceptable solutions to serious life problems by EMO
adolescents. Some testimonies include expressions of indifference to the possibilities of living or to
dying:
Alex: Suicide is a certain solution. I agree, too. Until I was 13, I lived with an alcoholic father. He could
just get drunk and came home, wake me, beat me, wake my sister, forced us to go into the living room
and watch as he beat my mother ... I do not have enough strength to kill myself, but I don´t care if I die.
I don’t mind. If I die, so it was just meant to be. It didn´t bother me to such an extent that once I doused
myself with gasoline and set on fire. I did it just because I was bored, so I took the camera, doused
myself and shot it. I don´t really care if I die.
Alex presented his story on an EMO Web discussion forum. The reactions of several other EMO
adherents illustrate that they understand suicide as one of commonly perceived possibilities when
encountering difficult life situations:
Evelina: Everyone has it different, some manage better, some worse, and when it's all too much, I do
not see any other way than to leave ... and it may not be that they would not want to suffer anymore, but
it may be that they think that without them other people will feel better.
Bernard: Simply to kill oneself. Two seconds are enough and the suffering may end and you can finally
breathe normally and can feel like a full human being. It is not a bad prospect
Radim: From my perspective, suicide is a last rescue. I'm not saying that I‘ve thought about it, but I
know about it, and I live as fully as I can, because I know that when the time comes, I can do it with a
clear conscience and not regret anything. At best it should stop, right?
Patricie: As it has already been mentioned hereI feel and experience so much that it hurts. I'm not
strong enough to do it (suicide) alone. But I strongly wish to be at the end. Let it happen...
The reactions of other EMO adherents to online suicidal self-disclosure as well as public
suicidal self-disclosure may itself be related to interpersonal sharing of attitudes and possibly also to
online peer contagion. Aside from this, the interviewed participants often reported other instances of
interpersonal influences, for example:
Researcher: Have you ever met someone of EMO people who cut himself/herself?
Anastazie: Of course! I met a girl who did not look like an EMO girl, indeed, but she presented herself
as EMO. And ... she cut her legs...here, above the knee...and she wore miniskirts so others could see...and
she boasted about it.
Strange Excitement after Self-Injury
Various experiences with self-injuring behavior were reported by the participants.
Testimonies about deliberate self-injury and the experience of pain included several interesting aspects.
Although deep skin damage may elicit pain in the self-injuring person, such feelings were not often
reported, at least not when describing the moment of committing self-injury. On the contrary, positive
feelings like relief were reported:
Researcher: How would you describe the feeling before, during and after cutting?
Adam: During: a strange excitement ... and after: Damn, my foot hurts!
Researcher: Another characteristic?
Adam: It is such satisfaction, but so terribly special ... It is a relief, but at the same time, one knows it
just does not make sense. That it is useless. But at the same time one feels relieved. One knows that it
will not solve anything, but I still do it again.
Adam learned when he was thirteen that his parents are adoptive. Then, in his own words, “his
life was changed forever”. He described this event as a very traumatic experience. He then started to
respond aggressively to his father and became an adherent of the EMO subculture. Adam and his
girlfriend Liliana (also an adherent of the EMO subculture) both reported experimenting with practices
of black magic, telepathy and astral travelling. Both have already committed self-injury, and Adam also
thinks about committing suicide in the future. For him, astral travelling is considered to be a preparation
for real death. Similar feelings accompanying self-injury were described by Boris:
Researcher: So you're ... Do you sometimes have the feeling that you have to cut yourself?
Boris: No feelings to cut, but when I'm really very upset and I do not have cigarettes in hand, and I'm
lazy to buy them ... so ... I need to cut myself a few times, on the surface only ... because I need to feel
relief.
Researcher: How many times do you have to cut yourself like this per month?
Boris: Now, not so many times. I have got antidepressants prescribed by my doctor again.
Participant Kristian reported decreased awareness of the pain, or partial analgesia in the course
of his intentional self-injury:
Researcher: How would you describe the feeling before, during and after cutting?
Kristian: When you are angry a lot, or nervous, or are strongly depressed, then I cannot feel the pain,
at least until I am O.K. again. Then my hand starts to hurt. But otherwise, it is ... I do it basically also
with a “scientific” intent, because when you are bleeding, the endorphins should get released and you
feel a little bit happier. So when I am strongly depressed and do not have any antidepressant pills at
hand ... sometimes I cut myself ...a little bit.
Kristian did not feel pain when he cut his forearm. The changed affective state, as predicted by
the affect-regulation model (Klonsky, 2007), often appeared in the testimonies of participants involving
the subjective experience of intentional self-injury. Participants reported depression, distress, feeling
upset, anger, or nervousness before they commit an act of self-injury.
Self-Injuries as a Mark of Affiliation to EMO Subculture
The analysis of Web discussion forums and the researchers’ experiences from the contact
stage of the research revealed that EMO adherents overtly present their self-injuries during face-to-face
encounters as well as online. During the contact stage of the project, we saw many EMO adolescents
with already healed self-injuries at public events, like concerts of EMO music groups, EMO events in
music clubs and outdoor EMO meetings. The scars were mostly on their forearms, and they wore
clothing that left the scars clearly visible. For example, they selected appropriate sleeve-lengths so as
not to conceal the scars. For many of them the overt demonstration of self-injury in the community of
EMO adherents is a mark of affiliation to EMO subculture. They aim to affirm that they are real insiders,
that they are “true EMOs”.
The analysis of Web discussion forums revealed that many EMO adherents are motivated
even to show photos of their self-injuries on the Internet. Most self-injuries were of skin damage caused
by cutting with knives, needles or razor-blades. Some of the photos published on the Web were taken
with almost professional lighting, composition, etc., and present the self-injuries in an artistic manner.
This indicates that at least some EMO adherents even perceive their self-injury as a form of art. The
artistic presentation of self-injuries is a part of the morbid aesthetics considered to be one of the typical
features of EMO subculture.
The contents of online discussion forums provided us with further opinions of EMO adherents
regarding their self-injury. Many of reactions on the Internet justified the individual choice of engaging
in deliberate self-injury:
Gabriel: If you have never cut yourself and gotten to the other side of the barricade, I dare
say with all regards, you do not know what you're talking about ... I have totally sawed my whole left
hand, for example. Yes, it is quite visible. And not hide it forever! I'm not talking about it with anybody,
apart from the psychologist I visit... someone is depressed and cuts himself he has real problems.
Benedikt: I hate people who hassle us about the fact that we cut. So what? It's our thing and
cutting is the only escape from this fucking reality…
Diana: Hey, I write it like this. I cut myself for a long time; it calms me and overrides the
emotional pain by this physical pain and it doesn't hurt so much… !! In all, there are a million reasons
not to do it, but there are still infinite reasons why we are doing it!!!
Antonie: Hi, hey EMO is a style... These people must be different... if we are not something
different, then everyone could be EMO and it would not be something exceptional, as it is now... EMO
is about cutting and it also is not… It is not a rule to cut yourself…I will not cut when I am O.K... but
EMOs are people with great passion and emotions… they feel and experience everything much more
strongly than other people… therefore, they cut themselves… they are exceptional in this, …for this
reason people connect us with cutting and so we are exceptional ... so if you want to cut, let’s cut…but
be careful, I once did it and then I’d like to do it again and again... when my blood is flowing from my
veins…I love it…but I hate it at the same time.
Discussion
The results of the present study contribute to current knowledge about attitudes of adolescents
toward suicide and deliberate self-injury, reflecting the identification of youth with a subculture based
on morbid aesthetics. First, the EMO adolescents in our study showed high tolerance for suicide, suicidal
behavior and deliberate self-harm (see also Segal, Connella, Miller, & Coolidge, 2016). Suicide is
understood to be something relatively common, or an acceptable solution to a difficult life situation. It
is possible to consider that the attitudes towards suicide and deliberate self-injury in adolescent adherents
of the EMO subculture are shaped by the opinions and attitudes of other EMO peers with whom they
are, or previously were, in contact. Thus, their understanding of self-injury and suicide may be shaped
by belonging to a community of other EMO adherents as well as by an online perception of values and
symbols related to the EMO subculture. The participants reported high acceptance for suicidal behavior
and deliberate self-injury. Such attitudes toward suicidal behavior and self-injury may be influenced by
mutual communication among EMO community members as well as by online sharing, i.e. using blogs,
social networks and online discussions. These findings provide new impetus for the theory of peer
contagion (Dishion & Tipsord, 2011) and also support previous findings in Goth subculture (Bowes et
al., 2015). Also, other scholars considered social contagion to be a risk factor for engagement in self-
injury (Jarvi, Jackson, Swenson, & Crawford, 2013). Social contagion was suggested to influence the
transmission of self-injury and increase the risk of initial engagement in deliberate self-injury among
individuals (Jarvi, Jackson, Swenson, & Crawford, 2013). The peer-contagion effect may be present in
the transfer of risky attitudes towards both suicidal behavior and deliberate self-injury between EMO
adherents. Therefore, we consider the identification with EMO youth subculture to be a factor
strengthening vulnerability towards risky behaviors, especially suicidal behavior and deliberate self-
injury.
Second, the researchers’ experiences with EMO adherents and the analysis of Web discussion
forums revealed that EMO adherents are intensely exposed to morbid images and contents, including
death, suicides, blood, suffering, self-injury and other injuries, self-mutilation, etc. Such themes also
occur in the lyrics of musical interpreters favored by EMO adherents. EMO adherents meet with these
themes expressed visually in the form of symbols both online and during face-to-face encounters with
their EMO friends. These morbid symbols are involved in commercially designed EMO clothes, T-
shirts, sweatshirts, trousers, etc. and also in the graphical designs of EMO Web forums and blogs.
Morbid images are shared both online and via face-to-face interactions. Indeed, the frequent perception
of morbid symbols may represent an important influence for mental development of adolescents. Baker
and Lewis (2013) reported that exposure to graphic stimuli, including self-injury, reinforced and
encouraged self-harming. Analogically, if we think about heightened exposure to images, including
suicidal behavior, a similar effect on adolescents’ own suicidal behavior and suicidal ideation may be
hypothesized. The study of McMahon, Corcoran, Keeley, Perry, and Arensman (2013) brought evidence
that adolescents exposed to suicidal behavior in the past were eight-times more likely to report self-harm
than adolescents not exposed to suicidal behavior. Further, a meta-analysis on risk-glorifying media
exposure determined that exposure to risk-glorifying music tended to be associated with risky behaviors,
cognitions and emotions (Fischer et al., 2011). Taking into account the effect of peer contagion (Dishion
& Tipsord, 2011), the intense exposure of EMO adherents to morbid symbols and images can also be
considered a risk factor for initial engagement in suicidal behavior.
Third, the present study contributes to current knowledge about self-injury in adolescents. Our
results support Klonsky’s (2007) affect-regulation model in part using a non-clinical, community sample
of adolescents. The affect-regulation model posits the idea that self-injury is preceded by negative
emotions. Participants in the present study reported that the execution of self-injury often takes place
following an experience of depression, distress, being upset or nervousness. These results, based on
testimonies emerging from structured interviews, are in line with the empirical study of Klonsky (2009).
Undergraduates reported similar affect-states as frustrated, anxious, sad and overwhelmed or hurt
emotionally.
We consider the identification with EMO youth subculture to be a risk factor strengthening
vulnerability towards problematic behavior, such as suicidal behavior or deliberate self-injury.
However, it is not clear how strong the influence of EMO peers on attitudes of newcomers joining the
EMO subculture is. What is contentious is the influence of personality and previous attitudes of
adolescents that are attracted to join the EMO youth subculture. Bowes et al. (2015) posited that a degree
of self-selection is present, where thinking about young people who are attracted to join the Goth
subculture. We may consider the possibility that some adolescents attracted by a subculture based on
morbid aesthetics may be more personally susceptible to depression, self-harming or suicidal behavior.
Such personal predispositions may influence the probability that an adolescent will decide to become
an adherent of some youth subculture that is in accordance with his/her personality. Taking personal
predispositions and vulnerability factors for risky behavior into consideration may be very fruitful for
future research on the peer contagion effect (Dishion & Tipsord, 2011) relating to death and suicide.
A limitation of the present study is that qualitative data have a limited potential for making
any broader generalizations. Therefore, our conclusions represent rather new hypotheses and new
questions that may be inspiring for future research. A further limitation is that the transcriptions of all
interviews were made at the end of the interviewing stage, which is not conventional in qualitative
research. Future research should consider the influence of EMO music on mental distress and
depressivity of EMO adherents (see, e.g. Peterson, Safer, & Jobes, 2008; Rustad, Snall, Jobes, Safer, &
Peterson, 2003). We hypothesize that listening to the lyrics of EMO music may potentially elicit feelings
of alienation, pessimism or hopelessness, and hopelessness was previously considered to be a key risk
factor for suicide (e.g. Stack, 1998; Stack et al., 1994). Further, the role of autobiographical self-
disclosure on suicidal ideation (Kovac & Range, 2002) could also be taken into account when analyzing
the effects of blogging and online self-disclosure of adherents of some suicidogenic subcultures.
Funding This research has been financed by the EEA and Norway Grants and institutionally supported
by the Czech Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports under Reg. No. 7F09122.
References
Allooh, N. N., Rummell, C. M., & Levant, R. F. (2013). "Emo" culture and gender norms in late
adolescents and young adults. Boyhood Studies, 7(1), 21-42. doi:10.3149/thy.0701.21
Anderson, L. M., Lowry, L. S., & Wuensch, K. L. (2015). Racial differences in adolescents’ answering
questions about suicide. Death Studies, 39(10), 600-604.
doi:10.1080/07481187.2015.1047058
Andriessen, K., Draper, B., Dudley, M., & Mitchell, P. B. (2016) Pre-and postloss features of adolescent
suicide bereavement: A systematic review. Death Studies, 40(4), 229-246.
doi:10.1080/07481187.2015.1128497
Anestis, M. D., & Selby, E. A. (2015). Grit and perseverance in suicidal behavior and non-suicidal self-
injury. Death Studies, 39(4), 211-218. doi:10.1080/07481187.2014.946629
Bailey, B. (2006). Emo music and youth culture. In S. Steinberg, P. Parmar, & B. Richard, (Eds.),
Encyclopedia of Contemporary Youth Culture (pp. 338-343). Westport, CT: Greenwood
Press.
Baker, F., & Bor, W. (2008). Can music preference indicate mental health status in young people?
Australian Psychiatry, 16(4), 284288. doi:10.1080/10398560701879589
Baker, T. G., & Lewis, S. P. (2013). Responses to online photographs of non-suicidal self-injury: A
thematic analysis. Archives of Suicide Research, 17(3), 223-235.
doi:10.1080/13811118.2013.805642
Bowes, L., Carnegie, R., Pearson, R., Mars, B., Biddle, L., Maughan, B., Lewis, G., Fernyhough, C., &
Heron, J. (2015). Risk of depression and self-harm in teenagers identifying with goth
subculture: a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Psychiatry, 2(9), 793-800. doi:10.1016/S2215-
0366(15)00164-9
CIOMS (2002). International ethical guidelines for biomedical research involving human subjects.
Prepared by the the Council for International Organizations of Medical Sciences (CIOMS) in
collaboration with the World Health Organization (WHO), CIOMS: Geneva.
Chernoff, N., & Widdicombe, S. (2015). ‘I was bored so…’: motivational accounts of participation in
an online emo group. Journal of Youth Studies, 18(3), 305-321.
doi:10.1080/13676261.2014.944115
Chu, J. P., Goldblum, P., Floyd, R., & Bongar, B. (2010). Review: The cultural theory and model of
suicide. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 14(1), 25-40. doi:10.1016/j.appsy.2011.11.001
Dazzi, T., Gribble, R., Wessely, S., & Fear, N. T. (2014). Does asking about suicide and related
behaviours induce suicidal ideation? What is the evidence? Psychological Medicine, 44(16),
3361-3363. doi:10.1017/S0033291714001299
de Boise, S. (2014). Cheer up emo kid: rethinking the ‘crisis of masculinity’in emo. Popular Music,
33(2), 225-242. doi: 10.1017/S0261143014000300
Definis-Gojanovic, M., Gugic, D., & Sutlovic, D. (2009). Suicide and emo youth subculture: A case
analysis. Collegium Antropologicum, 33(2), 173-175.
Dishion T. J., & Tipsord J. M. (2011). Peer contagion in child and adolescent social and emotional
development. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 189-214.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100412
Eskin, M., Schild, A., Öncü, B., Stieger, S., & Voracek, M. (2015). A cross-cultural investigation of
suicidal disclosures and attitudes in Austrian and Turkish university students. Death Studies,
39(10), 584-591. doi:10.1080/07481187.2015.1037971
Fischer, P., Greitmeyer, T., Kastenmuller, A., Vogrinic, C., & Sauer, A. (2011). The effects of risk-
glorifying media exposure on risk positive cognitions, emotions, and behaviors: A meta-
analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 137(3), 367-390. doi:10.1037/a0022267
Heilbron, N., & Prinstein, M. J. (2008). Peer influence and adolescent nonsuicidal self-injury: A
theoretical review of mechanisms and moderators. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12(4),
169-177. doi:10.1016/j.appsy.2008.05.004
Jarvi, S., Jackson, B., Swenson, L., & Crawford, H. (2013). The impact of social contagion on non-
suicidal self-injury: A review of the literature. Archives of Suicide Research 17(1), 1-19.
doi:10.1080/13811118.2013.748404
Johnson, B., & Cloonan, M. (2009). Dark side of the tune: Popular music and violence. Farnham:
Ashgate.
Klonsky, E. D. (2007). The functions of deliberate self-injury: A review of the evidence. Clinical
Psychology Review, 27(2), 226-239. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.08.002
Klonsky, E. D. (2009). The functions of self-injury in young adults who cut themselves:
Clarifying the evidence for affect-regulation. Psychiatry Research, 166(2-3), 260-268.
doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.02.008
Kovac, S. H., & Range, L. M. (2002). Does writing about suicidal thoughts and feelings reduce them?
Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 32(4), 428-440. doi:10.1521/suli.32.4.428.22335
Kuška, M., Formankova, P., & Kolarova, K. (2010). Subkultura emo kids jako fenomen soucasne
konzumni spolecnosti. In K. Zabrodska & I. Cermak (Eds.), Kvalitativni pristup a metody ve
vedach o cloveku IX. Individualita a jedinecnost v kvalitativnim vyzkumu (pp. 370-374). Brno:
Psychologicky ustav AV CR.
Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J., Friend, J., & Powell, A. (2009). Adolescent suicide, gender, and culture: A
rate and risk factor analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14(5), 402-414.
doi:10.1016/j.avb.2009.06.010
Lockman, J. D., & Servaty-Seib, H. L. (2016). College student suicidal ideation: Perceived
burdensomeness, thwarted belongingness, and meaning made of stress. Death Studies, 40(3),
154-164. doi:10.1080/07481187.2015.1105325
McMahon, E. M., Corcoran, P., Keeley, H., Perry, I. J., & Arensman, E. (2013). Adolescents exposed
to suicidal behavior of others: Prevalence of self-harm and associated psychological, lifestyle,
and life event factors. Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, 43(6), 634645.
doi:10.1111/sltb.12045
Miers, D., Abbott, D., & Springer, P. R. (2012). A phenomenological study of family needs following
the suicide of a teenager. Death Studies, 36(2), 118-133. doi:10.1080/07481187.2011.553341
Mortara, A., & Ironico, S. (2013). Deconstructing Emo lifestyle and aesthetics: a netnographic research.
Young Consumers, 14(4), 351-359. doi:10.1108/YC-03-2013-00355
Peterson, R. J., Safer, M. A., & Jobes, D. A. (2008). The impact of suicidal rock music lyrics on youth:
An investigation of individual differences. Archives of Suicide Research, 12(2), 161169.
doi:10.1080/13811110701857533
Range, L. M., Leach, M. M., McIntyre, D., Posey-Deters, P. B., Marion, M. S., Kovac, S. H., Baños, J.
H., & Vigil, J. (1999). Multicultural perspectives on suicide. Aggression and Violent
Behavior, 4(4), 413-430. doi:10.1016/S1359-1789(98)00022-6
Red, M. (2014). Who are the “Emos” anyway? Youth violence in Mexico City and the myth of the
revolution. Journal of Popular Music Studies, 26(1), 101-120. doi:10.1111/jpms.12062
Rissanen, M.L., Kylma, J., & Laukkanen, E. (2009). Helping adolescents who self-mutilate: Parental
descriptions. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 18(12), 1711-1721. doi:10.1111/j.1365-
2702.2008.02672.x
Rohde, A. (2016). Gays, cross-dressers, and Emos. Journal of Middle East Women's Studies, 12(3), 433-
449. doi:10.1215/15525864-3637642
Rustad, R., Snall, J. E., Jobes, D. A., Safer, M., & Peterson, R. (2003). The impact of rock videos and
music with suicidal content on thoughts and attitudes toward suicide. Suicide and Life-
Threatening Behavior, 33(2), 120131. doi:10.1521/suli.33.2.120.22776
Ryalls, E. (2013). Emo angst, masochism, and masculinity in crisis. Text and Performance Quarterly,
33(2), 83-97. doi:10.1080/10462937.2013.764570
Sandelowski, M. (2000). Whatever happened to qualitative description? Research in Nursing Health,
23(4), 334340. doi:10.1002/1098-240X(200008)23:4<334::AID-NUR9>3.0.CO;2-G
Segal, D. L., Connella, A., Miller, T., & Coolidge, F. L. (2016). Deliberate self-harm among younger
and older adults. Death Studies, 40(7), 440-444. doi:10.1080/07481187.2016.1171265
Seganti, F. R., & Smahel, D. (2011). Finding the meaning of emo in youths' online social networking:
A qualitative study of contemporary Italian emo. First Monday, 16(7). Retrieved from
http://ojphi.org/ojs/index.php/fm/article/view/3197/3021
Spear, L. P. (2000). Neurobehavioral changes in adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 9(4), 11114. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00072
Smith, J. A., Flowers, P., & Larkin, M. (2009). Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis: Theory,
Method and Research. London: Sage.
Stack, S. (1998). Heavy metal, religiosity and suicide acceptability. Suicide and Life-Threatening
Behavior, 28(4), 388394. doi:10.1111/j.1943-278X.1998.tb00974.x
Stack, S., Gundlach, J., & Reeves, J. (1994). The heavy metal subculture and suicide. Suicide & Life-
Threatening Behavior, 24(1), 1523. doi:10.1111/j.1943-278X.1994.tb00659.x
Stack, S., Lester, D., & Rosenberg, J. (2012). Music and suicidality: A quantitative review and
extension. Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, 42(6), 654-671. doi:10.1111/j.1943-
278X.2012.00120.x
Strauss, E. (2012). Early Adolescent Boys' Perceptions of the Emo Youth Subculture. Dissertation
Thesis. Potchefstroom (South Africa): North-West University. Retrieved from
http://dspace.nwu.ac.za/bitstream/handle/10394/9209/Strauss_E.pdf
Sullivan-Bolyai, S., Knafl, K., Tamborlane, W., & Grey, M. (2004). Parents’ reflections on managing
their children’s diabetes with insulin pumps. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 36(4), 316323.
doi:10.1111/j.1547-5069.2004.04058.x
Swannell, S. V., Martin, G. E., Page, A., Hasking, P., & St John, N. J. (2014). Prevalence of nonsuicidal
self-injury in nonclinical samples: Systematic review, meta-analysis and meta-regression.
Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, 44(3), 273-303. doi:10.1111/sltb.12070
Wood, L., Byram, V., Gosling, A. S., & Stokes, J. (2012). Continuing bonds after suicide bereavement
in childhood. Death Studies, 36(10), 873-898. doi:10.1080/07481187.2011.584025
Young, R., Sweeting, H., & West, P. (2006). Prevelance of deliberate self harm and attempted suicide
within contemporary Goth youth subculture: Longitudinal cohort study. British Medical
Journal, 332, 1058-1061. doi:10.1136/bmj.38790.495544.7C
Zdanow, C., & Wright, B. (2012). The representation of self injury and suicide on Emo social
networking groups. African Sociological Review, 16(2), 81-101.
... All these studies included strong sets of confounders that did not render goth's effects insignificant. In a small scale, qualitative study, a relationship was reported between belonging to the emo rock subculture and high tolerance towards self-harm, suicide ideation and suicidal behaviors (Trnka et al., 2018). These results prompted all these researchers to conclude that members of the goth and emo rock scene are vulnerable young people. ...
... Our results provide evidence that adolescents in the non-mainstream groups of all-out rock fans and rock/metal fans indeed show more problems compared to their peers, particularly when compared to the pop fans group. Our results corroborate the conclusions of earlier qualitative and cross-sectional studies that loud, rebellious music indicates adolescent problem behaviors (Arnett, 1996;Lozon & Besimon, 2014;Trnka et al., 2018;Weinstein, 1991). However, the approach in the current study further nuances the relationship between music and problem behaviors. ...
Article
Full-text available
Adolescent preferences for non-mainstream types of rock music can be markers of adolescent problem behaviors, but no study has ever investigated whether this relationship continues into adulthood. In a six-wave study, 900 Dutch adolescents were followed from ages 12 to 21 (Mage T1 12.4, 51.1% girls), while reporting on depressive symptoms, mental well-being, aggression and drug use. A latent class growth analysis on their preferences for specific types of rock music revealed four fan groups. When these fan groups were compared to one another, in adolescence, the all-out rock fans displayed the highest peak in depressive symptoms and the lowest dip in well-being and the rock/metal fans reported the most aggression. And for both these groups, drug use increased at the onset of adulthood. Pop fans displayed a profile characterized by low depressive symptoms and aggression, and high in mental well-being. Finally, the popular rock fans held an in-between position between pop fans, on one side, and the all-out rock fans and rock/metal fans, on the other side. Thus, music preferences can be markers of problems, not only in adolescence but also in young adulthood. Still, music can enhance mood, helps to cope with problems, and peers in fan groups can provide support. This research focuses on the relationship between music and problem behaviors, specifically among members of the all-out rock fans and rock/metal fans, but many of these young people might have had more personal problems if they had not had their music and their fan-group peers.
... 154). In their study of emo, Trnka et al. (2018) observed that 'the main philosophy of emo subculture is "to experience and express your actual emotions without any restraint," and that at public events emos outwardly expressed such emotional unguardedness through crying and angry screaming ' (p. 340). ...
... With their hearts on their sleeves, emo vocalists perform the hegemony of masculinity through screaming, however effeminate it might seem to outsiders. noteS 1. Emo has also been linked to broader issues about suicide and self-harming behaviour (Trnka et al. 2018). Media tend to frame such issues around a moral panic, and this discourse often focuses on young women. ...
Chapter
Focusing first on the discourse surrounding emo, Mack argues that ‘emo’ was not used solely to identify an emotive variant of hardcore punk but to also mark a subordinate expression of masculinity rooted in feminine stereotypes. The chapter then turns to the act of screaming. By way of two brief examples, Mack posits that screaming in emo is affected by the emotional intensity of the lyrics and that when screams are pushed to the point of breakage they express the loss of emotional control. The chapter concludes by demonstrating that the affected scream in emo violates the truism that men must practise emotional restraint while simultaneously functioning as a sonic expression of male dominance from its own subordinate position.
... Research on suicide scripts has used both quantitative (e.g., [30,36,48,49]) and qualitative methods. A qualitative approach is most common and appropriate when there is limited information about the suicide scripts of a culture or social group [24][25][26][27][28][29][31][32][33][34]37,38,[50][51][52][53][54][55][56][57][58][59][60][61]. As argued by Staples and Widger, to understand suicidality, we need to learn "how suicidal behaviours are imagined, talked about, and practiced; how they relate to other kinds of behaviours and other kinds of institutions; when and under what possibilities different people in the communities we study think suicide might arise and when it might not, when it might be 'acceptable' and when it might not; and how suicidal behaviour does not begin with the 'precipitating factor' and end with the 'suicidal act,' but extends deep into individual and collective pasts and futures" [62] (p. ...
Article
Full-text available
Information about suicidal behavior in Nepal is limited. According to official records, suicide rates were high until the year 2000 and declined thereafter. Official records are considered unreliable and a gross undercounting of suicide cases, particularly female cases. Suicide research in Nepal has been mostly epidemiologic and hospital-based. Little is known about how suicide is understood by Nepali people in general—including dominant suicide attitudes and beliefs in Nepal. Suicide attitudes and beliefs, which are elements of a culture’s suicide scripts, predict actual suicidality. Drawing on suicide-script theory, we developed and used a semi-structured survey to explore Nepali beliefs about female and male suicide. The informants were adult (Mage = 28.4) university students (59% male). Female suicide was believed to be a response to the society-sanctioned oppression and abuse that women are subjected to, in their family and community. The prevention of female suicide was viewed as requiring dismantling ideologies, institutions, and customs (e.g., child marriage, dowry) that are oppressive to women, and ensuring that women are protected from violence and have equal social and economic rights and opportunities. Male suicide was believed to be a symptom of societal problems (e.g., unemployment) and of men’s psychological problems (e.g., their difficulties in managing emotions). The prevention of male suicide was viewed as requiring both societal (e.g., employment opportunities) and individual remedies (e.g., psychological counseling). This study’s findings suggest that a semi-structured survey can be a fruitful method to access the suicide scripts of cultures about which there is limited research.
... Adolescents who identify with the "emo" youth culture are reported in the literature to engage in a range of self-harming and negative behaviors (anxiety, depression, low self-esteem) and are considered as self-destructive people. The "emo" subculture gathers youngsters between 12 and 20 years old, a critical and highly vulnerable period to engage in dangerous behaviors such as suicide and self-harm (Anestis & Selby, 2015;Trnka, Kuska, Balcar, & Tavel, 2017). Information concerning NSSI is shared by "emo" adolescents and young people through internet and it is present in movies and music (Lewis, Heath, St Denis, & Noble, 2011), suggesting that NSSI and self-harming are spread as a social contagion (Young et al., 2014). ...
... After the suicide of two Australian female students, Jodie Gater and Stephanie Gestier, in 2017, emo kids were not viewed only as subculture members or fans of modern punk music, but folk devils [13], [14]. They used social network and allowed emo texts to alter their thoughts to associate with self-injury and suicidal idea [8], [15]. This contradiction against social norm is the cause that public marked them as deviants rather than a subculture [16], [17]. ...
Article
In the context of "Traffic is King ", a large number of videos and content with empty meaning, lack of information and striving for traffic are flooded with the network. In the social platform, the audience has created many language expressions rich in "meme", such as "meaningless words". As an important part of youth subculture, "meaningless words" mainly creates characters through collages, spoofs, parodies and other forms, and its expression is more direct and simple. It is an important way of satirizing and roast on the social media platform. Why do contemporary youth produce "meaningless words"? Why does "meaningless words" spread so widely? This paper will explore the production mechanism of "meaningless words" from the perspective of emotional communication.
Article
Background: Online self-harming and suicidal behavior is a novel and rapidly increasing phenomenon warranting comprehensive mapping of used research methods. Aim: To identify and map how knowledge on online self-harming and suicidal behavior is gathered, including how data are collected e.g. questionnaires and interviews. Methods: The review follows the Joanna Briggs Institute Manual for Scoping Reviews in tandem with Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews. A keyword search of three electronic databases was conducted on two occasions, yielding 5422 records. Following duplicate removal, the records were screened based on the following inclusion criterions; (1) in English or Nordic language and published between 2011-2022, (2) presenting results for self-harming and/or suicidal behavior on social media and (3) using tools for either interview or questionnaire aiming at assessment of the experience of online self-harming and suicidal behavior from the perspective of the person who engages in the behavior. A total of 64 articles were included. Results: 45 used questionnaires, 17 used interviews, and two studies mixed the two approaches. 17% of the studies had made some effort to ensure validity within the questionnaires and 15.8% gave full access to the interview guide. Conclusion: Research into online self-harming and suicidal behavior is characterized by a lack of validated measurements and methodological transparency. The results emphasize a need for further development, testing, and validation of questionnaires and greater openness and reflexivity in qualitative methodology to enable cross-study comparison and advance knowledge of this complex phenomenon.
Thesis
Full-text available
ASSESSMENT OF THE CORRELATION BETWEEN PREFERRED MUSIC GENRE AND ANGER STYLES / AGGRESSION LEVELS In this study, it was aimed to provide a wide source of information about the neuroscience of music, not only to measure the relationship between the music genres and aggression levels with anger expression styles. The Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire and the Trait Anger and Anger Expression Styles Scales were applied to 359 participants as an online survey also with the questions about their socio-demographic information, music listening habits and preferred music genres. The findings showed that there was a significant difference only in the physical aggression dimension among the aggression and anger expression styles and preferred music genres. It has been determined that physical aggression behavior is seen more in those who listen to arabesque and Turkish protest music than those who listen to classical music genre and jazz/blues/r&b genre group.
Article
Social media posts expressing mental illnesses such as depression are trending in today’s society. The aesthetics of dark and depressing posts are highlighted in blogs or social media accounts of young individuals. Consequently, this study investigated why young people use mental illnesses as an aesthetic on social media. On top of that, this study contributes to the limited body of research on this subject. This study applied a method known as netnography, an analysis of social interactions online. Furthermore, the Actor-Network Theory (ANT) was employed as an analytical tool to explore insights and draw conclusions. Seven mediators were identified as possible causes that prompted mental illness aesthetic use, including Tumblr, media, attention seekers, coping mechanism, victimhood, emo culture, and mental health awareness. These findings provided insights into developing interventions and systematic methods to discuss mental illness properly. This study also identified current challenges in social media research regarding mental illness detection.
Article
Full-text available
This essay explores ruptures and continuities in organized violence against sex or gender nonconformity in recent Iraqi history. Nonnormative gendered and sexual practices and identities are long-standing in Iraq and other parts of the region and coexist with hostility to same-sex relations and gender and sexual nonconformity. Violence against nonnormative gender and sexual behaviors, enactments, and identities can be attributed to the general erosion of the Iraqi state and the normalization of violence after decades of war, embargo, and societal conflict. Despite armed and well-financed attempts to repress diversity, Iraqi society remains multiple and vital in its gendered and sexual practices.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined self-harming behaviors among younger and older adults who completed the Self-Harm Inventory (SHI). A 2 (age group) × 2 (gender) ANOVA showed a significant main effect for age such that younger adult students (M = 3.42, SD = 3.86) had higher SHI total scores than community-dwelling older adults (M = 1.58, SD = 2.35). Younger adults endorsed "Yes" responses significantly more frequently than older adults on 13 of 22 items. Future research should examine specific behaviors of older adults who self-harm more frequently and explore relationships between self-harming behaviors and other risk and resiliency factors for elder suicide.
Article
Full-text available
Despite increasing clinical and research interest in suicide bereavement the impact of suicide on adolescents is still poorly understood. This critical review aims to disentangle pre- and post-loss features that affect response to grief in this age group. The literature was analysed after a systematic search and hand-check of retrieved articles. The impact of suicide is affected by (1) pre-loss features related to personal/family history of mental health, family life, suicidal behavior, and type and emotional closeness of relationship; and (2) post-loss issues such as quality of remaining relationships. Future research and bereavement support should consider these broader contexts.
Article
Full-text available
Background Previous research has suggested that deliberate self-harm is associated with contemporary goth subculture in young people; however, whether this association is confounded by characteristics of young people, their families, and their circumstances is unclear. We aimed to test whether self-identification as a goth is prospectively associated with emergence of clinical depression and self-harm in early adulthood. Methods We used data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, a UK community-based birth cohort of 14 541 pregnant women with expected delivery between April 1, 1991, and Dec 31, 1992. All children in the study were invited to attend yearly follow-up visits at the research clinic from age 7 years. At 15 years of age, participants reported the extent to which they self-identified as a goth. We assessed depressive mood and self-harm at 15 years with the Development and Wellbeing Assessment (DAWBA) questionnaire, and depression and self-harm at 18 years using the Clinical Interview Schedule-Revised. We calculated the prospective association between goth identification at 15 years and depression and self-harm at 18 years using logistic regression analyses. Findings Of 5357 participants who had data available for goth self-identification, 3694 individuals also had data for depression and self-harm outcomes at 18 years. 105 (6%) of 1841 adolescents who did not self-identify as goths met criteria for depression compared with 28 (18%) of 154 who identified as goths very much; for self-harm, the figures were 189 (10%) of 1841 versus 57 (37%) of 154. We noted a dose–response association with goth self-identification both for depression and for self-harm. Compared with young people who did not identify as a goth, those who somewhat identified as being a goth were 1·6 times more likely (unadjusted odds ratio [OR] 1·63, 95% CI 1·14–2·34, p<0·001), and those who very much identified as being a goth were more than three times more likely (unadjusted OR 3·67, 2·33–4·79, p<0·001) to have scores in the clinical range for depression at 18 years; findings were similar for self-harm. Associations were not attenuated after adjustment for a range of individual, family, and social confounders. Interpretation Our findings suggest that young people identifying with goth subculture might be at an increased risk for depression and self-harm. Although our results suggest that some peer contagion operates within the goth community, our observational findings cannot be used to claim that becoming a goth increases risk of self-harm or depression. Working with young people in the goth community to identify those at increased risk of depression and self-harm and provide support might be effective.
Article
The general view of descriptive research as a lower level form of inquiry has influenced some researchers conducting qualitative research to claim methods they are really not using and not to claim the method they are using: namely, qualitative description. Qualitative descriptive studies have as their goal a comprehensive summary of events in the everyday terms of those events. Researchers conducting qualitative descriptive studies stay close to their data and to the surface of words and events. Qualitative descriptive designs typically are an eclectic but reasonable combination of sampling, and data collection, analysis, and re-presentation techniques. Qualitative descriptive study is the method of choice when straight descriptions of phenomena are desired.
Article
Written against the academically dominant but simplistic romanticization of popular music as a positive force, this book focuses on the 'dark side' of the subject. It is a pioneering examination of the ways in which popular music has been deployed in association with violence, ranging from what appears to be an incidental relationship, to one in which music is explicitly applied as an instrument of violence. A preliminary overview of the physiological and cognitive foundations of sounding/hearing which are distinctive within the sensorium, discloses in particular their potential for organic and psychic violence. The study then elaborates working definitions of key terms (including the vexed idea of the 'popular') for the purposes of this investigation, and provides a historical survey of examples of the nexus between music and violence, from (pre)Biblical times to the late nineteenth century. The second half of the book concentrates on the modern era, marked in this case by the emergence of technologies by which music can be electronically augmented, generated, and disseminated, beginning with the advent of sound recording from the 1870s, and proceeding to audio-internet and other contemporary audio-technologies. Johnson and Cloonan argue that these technologies have transformed the potential of music to mediate cultural confrontations from the local to the global, particularly through violence. The authors present a taxonomy of case histories in the connection between popular music and violence, through increasingly intense forms of that relationship, culminating in the topical examples of music and torture, including those in Bosnia, Darfur, and by US forces in Iraq and Guantánamo Bay. This, however, is not simply a succession of data, but an argumentative synthesis. Thus, the final section debates the implications of this nexus both for popular music studies itself, and also in cultural policy and regulation, the ethics of citizenship, and arguments about human rights.
Article
The interpersonal theory of suicide (ITS) posits that suicidal ideation is interpersonal in nature. More specifically, in the ITS, perceived burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness are dynamic cognitive-affective interpersonal states that engender suicidal ideation. However, intrapersonal (i.e., within the self) existential protective factors for suicidal ideation, such as the ability to make meaning from stressful life events, remain relatively unexplored. We examined the degree to which interpersonal and intrapersonal variables contribute to the variance of suicidal ideation in college students (n = 165). Results indicated that students' meaning made of stress was negatively and uniquely associated with suicidal ideation after controlling for ITS interpersonal contributions.
Article
The present purpose was to examine racial differences in response rate and serious behavioral suicide risk based on the national Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance Survey (YRBS). Data from 15,245 adolescents (YRBS 2011) were included. Survey items pertaining to making suicidal plans and attempting suicide were included. Significant differences in responding and content emerged, especially with regard to suicide attempts. Racial minority adolescents are at elevated risk for serious suicidal behaviors and are more likely to omit items pertaining to suicide attempts. African American adolescents rarely reported having attempted suicide, but they also frequently failed to respond to that question.