ChapterPDF Available

Assessing the fit of each item of the ‘Picture-Based Value Survey for Children’ into the theoretical structure of values

Authors:
ASSESSING THE FIT OF EACH ITEM OF THE ‘PICTURE-BASED
VALUE SURVEY FOR CHILDREN’ INTO THE THEORETICAL
STRUCTURE OF VALUES
WOLFGANG BILSKY, WWU Münster, Germany
ANNA K. DÖRING, University of Westminster London, UK
PATRICK J.F. GROENEN, Erasmus University Rotterdam, The Netherlands
ABSTRACT
The Picture-Based Value Survey (PBVS-C) is based on Schwartz’s structural theory of values
and measures value preferences of young children. It has been applied in several studies around
the world. Due to some recurring deviations of single items from theoretical expectations we
took a closer look at their fit into the theoretical structure. We re-analysed the data from former
studies by using a strong confirmatory
MDS that enforces theory-based regional restrictions
onto the data (Bilsky, Borg, Janik & Groenen, 2015). To better understand how much each
single item affects the overall model fit, Stress-per-Point (SPP) coefficients were computed for
every item. Since the global MDS model and the external regional restrictions imposed by con-
firmatory MDS are theoretically grounded, SPP scores are diagnostically relevant with respect
to item and construct validity. The results of our re-analyses are presented and discussed with
regard to the observed misfit of items. Consequences for further research are outlined.
INTRODUCTION
The empirical analysis of children’s values does not have a long tradition. This is partly due to
the lack of adequate instruments that do take the still developing literacy of young children into
account. The Picture Based Value Survey for Children (PBVS-C; Döring et al., 2010) closed this
gap only a few years ago. This instrument is based on Schwartz’s (1992) theory on universals in
the content and structure of values.
Schwartz’s theory identifies ten basic values (i.e., value types) which differ with respect to
motivational content: Universalism (UN), Benevolence (BE), Tradition (TR), Conformity (CO),
Security (SE), Power (PO), Achievement (AC), Hedonism (HE), Stimulation (ST), and Self-
Direction (SD). These motivational differences have consequences for the value structure, which
takes the form of a circumplex (Borg & Shye, 1995) in two-dimensional space: According to
Schwartz’s theory, values of the same value type should form sectors with boundaries emanating
from a common origin; furthermore, adjacent sectors are expected to be motivationally more
similar than sectors farther apart. On a more general level, basic values are subsumed under four
higher-order values (HOVs): “Self-Transcendence” (UN, BE) opposed to Self-Enhancement”
(PO, AC), and “Openness-to-Change” (HE, ST, SD) opposed to “Conservation” (TR, CO, SE;
see Appendix).
The PBVS-C was designed to measure value preferences of children aged 6 to 11. It was
constructed in close analogy to Schwartz’s Portrait Values Questionnaire (PVQ) and comprises
twenty items, each depicting one value-related situation. Every basic value is represented by two
items which are printed on removable stickers. Children are asked to paste them on a standard
response sheet with five graded alternatives from “very important” to “not at all important” in
my life (see Döring et al., 2010).
Until today the PBVS-C has been administered to children in various countries around the
world (Bilsky et al., 2013; Döring et al., 2010, 2015). Exploratory and weak confirmatory Multi-
dimensional Scaling (MDS; Borg & Groenen, 2005) were employed to compare the empirical
value structure with Schwartz’s structural model. Overall, the validity and the cross-cultural ap-
plicability of the instrument were supported. Some items, however, deviated repeatedly from the
theoretical structure. It is these deviations the present paper focuses upon.
METHOD
The present study builds on and extends a former one, in which value data from a Brazilian
sample of young children (Roazzi et al., 2011) were re-analysed by strong confirmatory ordinal MDS
(Bilsky et al., 2015). As in this former study, strong confirmatory MDS is accomplished by imposing
theory based regional restrictions on value data (Borg & Groenen, 2005).
Data
We analyse data that were collected with the PBVS-C from 10 samples of children aged 6 to
12. These samples were from eight different countries, five European and three extra-European1:
Germany (DE; N1=221, N2=294), Bulgaria (BU; N=439), France (FR; N=306), United Kingdom
(UK; N=352), Italy (IT; N=380), Turks living in Germany (gTR; N=117), Brazil (BR; N=182),
New Zealand (NZ; N=104), and United States (USA; N=69).
Facetization of Values
Former facetizations of values (Schwartz & Bilsky, 1987; Schwartz, 1992) considered only
one motivational facet. In this manuscript, we use two independent facets for analysing value
structure (Bilsky et al., 2015). They derive from a recent reframing of Schwartz’s two “basic
value dimensions” (Schwartz, 2006; see also Schwartz et al., 2012). This reframing implies dy-
namic principles which go beyond congruence and conflict among values (cf. Schwartz, 2006,
947f). The first principle differentiates between social and personal interests, the second con-
trasts anxiety and growth related objectives: the prevention of loss and the promotion of gain (see
Table A1 in the appendix). The following mapping sentence results from this distinction:
p considers value X that refers to his/her {social/personal} interests and the {promotion of
gain/prevention of loss} → {very important … not important} as a guiding principle for p’s life.
Strong Confirmatory MDS
The regional restrictions imposed on our data follow from the above mapping sentence
(Bilsky et al., 2015): Thus, every basic value (and the respective PBVS-items) can be character-
ized independently by the two facets “interest” and “objective” (see Figure 1 and Table A1, col-
umns 2, 4 and 5). In accordance with this facetization, our structural hypotheses suggest a split
into four quadrants in two-dimensional space, i.e. a duplex, as indicated in Figure 1.
1We thank our former students for their collaboration and for collecting these data in the context of their final theses:
K. Aryus, L. Drögekamp, V. Glatzel, N. Janowicz, M. Nyagolova, A. Petersen, E.R.Scheefer, T.S.Şimşek, and J.
Sindermann. The Brazilian data were first published in Roazzi et al. (2011).
Objective
promotion: gain
prevention: loss
Interest
social
1/1
Self-Transcendence:
Universalism
Benevolence
1/2
Conservation:
Security
Conformity
Tradition
personal
2/1
Openness to Change:
Hedonism
Stimulation
Self-Direction
2/2
Self-Enhancement:
Achievement
Power
Figure 1 Classification of the ten basic human values (Schwartz, 1992) according to the two
facets “Interest” and “Objective”
There exists no standard software until today which would allow testing such a structure by
confirmatory MDS. Therefore we used an experimental program written in MatLAB by Groenen
and already used for similar purposes in earlier studies (Borg et al., 2011; Bilsky et al., 2015). In
order to minimize the risk of running into local minima, we used a starting configuration derived
from Schwartz’s original model of value structure (Schwartz, 1992; see Bilsky & Janik, 2010,
for more detailed information).
To answer our central research question which items of the PBVS-C show recurring devia-
tions from the theoretical structure and to what extent, we compute two indices separately for all 10
samples: Stress-1 and Stress per Point (SPP). While Stress-1 informs about the mismatch between
the correlations and the corresponding distances of an MDS solution, SPP indicates how much
every single item contributes to this mismatch. MDS plots complement our computations.
RESULTS
Table 1 summarizes the results of our confirmatory MDS. For a quick overview, SPP-
coefficients that are equal to or bigger than the respective mean Stress per Point are marked by grey
underlay, those that are equal to or bigger than the third quartile by bold print. The contribution of
AC1 to overall stress, for example, is below-average in all samples; the contribution of CO2 and SD1,
in contrast, is above-average in nine of ten samples.
DISCUSSION
A strong confirmatory MDS enforces theory-based regional restrictions onto the resulting MDS
plot. In the present case, these restrictions are specified by the two facets “interest” and “objective”
(Figure 1). They force the PBVS-items into one of four quadrants in two-dimensional space accord-
ing to their motivational content. These quadrants correspond to the four higher-order values (HOVs)
“self-enhancement vs. self-transcendence”, and “openness-to-change vs. conservation”. They charac-
terize value structure as supposed by Schwartz (1992) value theory on the most general level.
Table 1 PBVS-C; ordinal MDS with theory-based Starting Configuration and Regional Restrictions:
Stress per Point (SPP)
PBVS-
Item
DE1
DE2
BU
IT
NZ
USA
N
221
294
439
380
104
69
UN1
0.036
0.020
0.042
0.022
0.053
0.070
UN2
0.031
0.024
0.012
0.051
0.044
0.099
BE1
0.017
0.008
0.020
0.018
0.064
0.072
BE2
0.055
0.052
0.032
0.055
0.057
0.083
TR1
0.049
0.029
0.017
0.044
0.056
0.035
TR2
0.054
0.086
0.014
0.063
0.087
0.049
CO1
0.029
0.005
0.007
0.027
0.055
0.050
CO2
0.079
0.056
0.057
0.070
0.099
0.026
SE1
0.064
0.038
0.033
0.031
0.079
0.031
SE2
0.075
0.086
0.066
0.058
0.086
0.061
PO1
0.026
0.020
0.027
0.015
0.058
0.047
PO2
0.038
0.021
0.037
0.032
0.054
0.078
AC1
0.049
0.021
0.008
0.033
0.027
0.054
AC2
0.101
0.028
0.045
0.069
0.057
0.073
HE1
0.035
0.047
0.055
0.029
0.029
0.060
HE2
0.106
0.020
0.053
0.094
0.043
0.101
ST1
0.036
0.023
0.030
0.029
0.055
0.055
ST2
0.046
0.014
0.026
0.019
0.041
0.075
SD1
0.073
0.074
0.114
0.067
0.048
0.077
SD2
0.045
0.029
0.064
0.021
0.059
0.081
Mean
0.052
0.035
0.038
0.042
0.058
0.064
Stress-1
0.228
0.187
0.195
0.206
0.240
0.253
3rd Quar-
tile
0.067
0.048
0.053
0.059
0.060
0.077
Note. Grey underlay: SPP >= Mean; bold figures: SPP >= 3rd Quartile
Stress-1 informs about the overall mismatch between the correlations of the 20 PBVS- items and
the corresponding distances of the MDS solution. This mismatch is smallest for the sample from
Great Britain (GB: 0.184) and highest for that from Brazil (BR: 0.282). Despite the observed range of
stress coefficients, however, only the coefficient of the Brazilian sample challenges the hypotheses of
non-randomness (Spence & Ogilvie, 1973).
When calculated in the context of a confirmatory MDS, Stress per Point indicates how well the
respective item fits the underlying theoretical model. Singular instances of above-average SPPs may
be attributable to random error, to a poor adaptation of the item to the target population, or to cultural
differences. Which of these alternatives comes true can only be answered by systematic replications.
Figure 2 Ordinal MDS of PBVS-Items with Starting-Configuration and Regional Restrictions;
PBVS-C above and PBVS-10 (short version) below
Finding above-average SPPs repeatedly within and/or across different cultures, however, chal-
lenges the validity of the respective item. As can be seen from Table 1, the number of above-average
SPPs differs considerably between the 20 PBVS-items. For the present it appears too early to draw
far-reaching conclusions from these findings. First, the size of the individual samples and the number
of samples per country is still very small despite the internationally growing number of studies using
the PBVS-C. Second, their representativeness is not warranted. The observed pattern of SPPs found
in the present data base should however be traced in future studies. In the case of recurring high
SPP-coefficients in future and more representative studies, revising or substituting the respective
items would be a reasonable consequence for ameliorating the PBVS-C in the long run.
While the revision or replacement of items is not an option for now, we used our knowledge of
SPPs for another purpose: item selection. This is possible because every basic value is represented by
two items in the PBVS-C. As can be seen from Table 1, for most values one of the two items showed
repeatedly below-average SPPs. Therefore we chose these items to construct a short version of the
Picture-Based Value Survey. The respective items of this PBVS-10 are: UN2, BE1, TR1, CO1, SE1,
PO1, AC1, HE1, ST1, and SD2 (see Figure A1 in the appendix). Such a short version could be useful
as part of a more comprehensive survey on different developmental topics or in cohort studies.
For illustrative purposes, we conducted two ordinal MDS with regional restrictions and these ten
items. Data were from the two German samples (see Table 1).The results for the standard and for the
short form of the Picture-Based Value Survey are summarized in Figure 2. As can be seen, items can
be partitioned in four quadrants according to Schwartz’s (1992) theory. However, their sequence
partly deviates from Schwartz’s structural model. Whether these deviations are random or systematic
needs additional analyses and further studies.
REFERENCES
Bilsky, W., Borg, I., Janik, M., & Groenen, P. (2015). Children's value structures - Imposing
theory-based regional restrictions onto an ordinal MDS solution. In A. Roazzi, B.C. de
Souza & W. Bilsky (Eds.), Searching for Structure in Complex Social, Cultural and Psy-
chological Phenomena (pp. 23-37). Recife: Editora Universitária – UFPE.
Bilsky, W., Döring, A.K., van Beeck, F., Rose, I., Schmitz, J., Aryus, K., Drögekamp, L., & Sin-
dermann, J. (2013). Investigating children's values and value preferences - testing and ex-
panding the limits. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 72(3), 123-136.
Bilsky, W. & Janik, M. (2010). Investigating value structure: Using theory-based starting con-
figuration in Multidimensional Scaling. Revista de Psicología Social, 25(3), 341-349.
Borg, I. & Groenen, P. (2005). Modern Multidimensional Scaling. Berlin: Springer.
Borg, I., Groenen, P.J.F., Jehn K.A., Bilsky W., & Schwartz, S.H. (2011). Embedding the organ-
izational culture profile into Schwartz's theory of universals in values. Journal of Personnel
Psychology, 10(1), 1-12.
Borg, I. & Shye, S. (1995). Facet Theory. Form and content. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Döring, A.K., Blauensteiner, A., Aryus, K., Drögekamp, L., & Bilsky, W. (2010). Assessing
values at an early age: the picture-based value survey for children (PBVS-C). Journal of
Personality Assessment, 92(5), 439-448.
Döring, A.K., Schwartz, S.H., Cieciuch, J., Groenen, P.J.F., Glatzel, V., Harasimczuk, J., Jan-
owicz, N., Nyagolova, M., Scheefer, E.R., Allritz, M., Milfont, T.L., & Bilsky, W. (2015).
Cross-cultural evidence of value structures and priorities in childhood. British Journal of
Psychology, 106, 675-699.
Roazzi, A., Döring, A.K., Gomes, Y.A., Souza, B.C. & Bilsky, W. (2011). The emergence of a
value structure at an early age. In Y. Fisher & I.A. Friedman (Eds.), New horizons for Facet
Theory (pp. 99-112). Israel: FTA Publication.
Schwartz, S.H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances
and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, Vol. 25 (pp. 1-65). New York: Academic Press.
Schwartz, S.H. (2005). Human values. European Social Survey Education Net. Retrieved from:
http://essedunet.nsd.uib.no/cms/topics/1/5/, 31.10.2008.
Schwartz, S.H. (2006). Les valeurs de base de la personne: Théorie, mesures et applications. Re-
vue Française de Sociologie, 47(4), 929-968.
Spence, I., & Ogilvie, J. C. (1973). A table of expected stress values for random rankings in
nonmetric multidimensional scaling. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 8, 511–517.
Appendix
Table A1: Human Values (Schwartz, 1992), classified according to “Interest” (social vs. per-
sonal) and “Objective” (promotion: gain vs. prevention: loss); see Schwartz (2006; Bilsky et
al., 2015)
Higher-Order
Values
(HOV)
Basic Values
and
PBVS-Items
Central
Motivational Goal a
Facets
and
Elements
Interest
Objective
Self-Tran-
scendence
Universalism
UN1, UN2
Understanding, appreciation,
tolerance, and protection for
the welfare of all people and
for nature
social promotion:
gain
Benevolence
BE1, BE2
Preserving and enhancing the
welfare of those with whom
one is in frequent personal
contact (the 'in-group')
social promotion:
gain
Conservation
Tradition
TR1, TR2
Respect, commitment, and
acceptance of the customs and
ideas that traditional culture or
religion provide the self
social prevention:
loss
Conformity
CO1, CO2
Restraint of actions, inclina-
tions, and impulses likely to
upset or harm others and vio-
late social expectations or
norms
social prevention:
loss
Security
SE1, SE2
Safety, harmony, and stability
of society, of relationships,
and of self
social prevention:
loss
Self-
Enhancement
Power
PO1, PO2
Social status and prestige,
control or dominance over
people and resources
personal prevention:
loss
Achievement
AC1, AC2
Personal success through
demonstrating competence
according to social standards
personal prevention:
loss
Openness
to
Change
Hedonismb
HE1, HE2
Pleasure and sensuous gratifi-
cation for oneself
personal
promotion:
gain
Stimulation
ST1, ST2
Excitement, novelty, and chal-
lenge in life
personal
promotion:
gain
Self-Direction
SD1, SD2
Independent thought and ac-
tion; choosing, creating, ex-
ploring
personal promotion:
gain
a Central motivational goals (Schwartz, 2005; http://essedunet.nsd.uib.no/cms/topics/1/)
b Hedonism shares elements of both self-enhancement and openness to change, but is closer to
openness in most cases (Schwartz, 2005).
Figure A1 Short Version of the Picture-Based Value Survey for Children (PBVS-10)
Universalism
Benevolence
Tradition
Security
Power
Achievement
Hedonism
Stimulation
Selfdirection
Conformity
... A discriminant analysis revealed differences in value preferences between girls and boys. The fit of each PBVS-item into the theoretical structure of values is compared to the respective data from 10 other countries (Bilsky, Döring, & Groenen, 2016). ...
... Only confirmatory MDS was run with both, smacof constraint from the R package and an experimental program written in MatLab by Patrick Groenen. This latter program was used to ensure comparability with the 6 results from previous studies (Bilsky, Borg, Janik & Groenen, 2015;Bilsky, Döring & Groenen, 2016). ...
... Classification of the ten basic human values according to "Focus" and "Goals"(Schwartz, 2006;Bilsky, Döring & Groenen, 2016) Picture-Based Value Survey for Children (PBVS-C): Sample items and response alternatives ...
Conference Paper
This study examines value structures of Russian children (N=227) aged 7-12. It extends Döring's (2018) cross-cultural adaptation of the Picture-Based Value Survey for Children (PBVS-C). Based on Schwartz's (1992) value theory, the following assumptions are tested: The 20 items of the PBVS-C are valid indicators of the 10 Basic (BV) and four Higher-Order Values (HOVs) postulated by Schwartz. The BVs show the expected circular structure, the HOVs the hypothesized opposite positions. These assumptions hold when tested across (based on BVs' intercorrelations) and within (based on the data matrix) Russian children. Exploratory, confirmatory, and spherical MDS and two-dimensional Unfolding confirmed our assumptions. Results were validated using permutation tests and bootstrapping. A discriminant analysis revealed differences in value preferences between girls and boys. The fit of each PBVS-item into the theoretical structure of values is compared to the respective data from 10 other countries (Bilsky, Döring, & Groenen, 2016).
... Sie plädieren für eine längsschnittliche Untersuchung von Wertorientierungen bei noch jüngeren Kindern, um die Frage zu klären, ob es sich bei den Strukturen um Universalien handelt, oder ob sie einem Entwicklungsprozess unterliegen (Döring et al., 2015, S. 25 f.). Bilsky, Döring & Groenen (2016) verringern die Itemanzahl des PBVS-C von 20 auf 10 Items, indem sie sich induktiv an der Messgüte der einzelnen Items des Gesamtinstruments orientieren. Auch mit diesem Kurzinstrument, in dem jeder Wertetyp nur noch durch ein Item repräsentiert wird, können die vier übergeordneten Wertebereiche der Theorie der universellen Werte bei 6-12-Jährigen nachgewiesen werden. ...
... 2006; Bilsky, Döring & Groenen, 2016). Diese Achsen können, zumindest auf der Ebene der übergeordneten Wertedimensionen, schon bei den jüngsten Probanden stabil bestätigt werden (Döring et al., 2015, S. 19). ...
... Mit der später in die Forschungstradition der universellen Werte eingeführten Strukturierungsachse der Zielausrichtung an angstbasierter Verlustvermeidung vs. Zuwachs (Schwartz, 2006;Bilsky et al. 2015;Bilsky, Döring & Groenen, 2016) kann man sich eventuell der systematischen Erklärung der Ausdifferenzierung innerhalb der idiozentristischen und allozentristischen Wertebereiche nähern. Eine Ausdifferenzierung der Dimension Verlustvermeidung vs. Zuwachs ist in der Vorschule noch nicht zu beobachten (Vogelbacher, 2015). ...
Book
Die Untersuchung der Entstehung von Wertorientierungen bei Kindern stellt in der soziologischen Forschung ein Nischenthema dar. In der Sozial- und Persönlich-keitspsychologie existiert zu diesem Gebiet dagegen ein dynamisches Forschungs-feld. Allerdings zeigt die psychologische Werteforschung zwei Mankos. Die Wertestrukturen von Kindern werden hier meist querschnittlich untersucht, was keine Erkenntnisse über ihre Entwicklung im Zeitverlauf zulässt. Dazu vernachlässigt die psychologische Forschung die soziale Bedingtheit der Werteentstehung bei Kindern. Die vorliegende Arbeit schließt diese Forschungslücken und versucht somit eine soziologische Wiedereinbettung der Werteforschung in der Kindheit. Hierfür werden in einer Stichprobe von Kindern in der ersten und zweiten Klasse Wertepräferenzen über Selbstauskunftsverfahren erhoben. In einem ersten Schritt werden die vorliegenden Wertestrukturen identifiziert. In einem nächsten Schritt wird die Entwicklung der Wertestruktur von der ersten zur zweiten Klasse längsschnittlich untersucht. Im letzten Teil der Arbeit wird der Einfluss familiärer Struktur- und Prozessmerkmale auf die Wertorientierungen und Wertestruktur der Kinder überprüft. Die Ergebnisse der Analysen zeigen bereits in der ersten Klasse eine plausible Wertestruktur. Sie beruht auf einer Grunddifferenzierung in individualistische und kollektivistische Werte. Während sich in der ersten Klasse nur für den kollektivistischen Wertebereich Substrukturen nachweisen lassen, zeigen sich in der zweiten Klasse auch im Bereich der individualistischen Werte Ausdifferenzierungen. Unter den familiären Bedingungen lassen sich besonders für den sozioökonomischen Status der Eltern, ihre religiösen Wertorientierungen wie auch ihren Erziehungsstil Einflüsse auf die Wertorientierungen der Kinder nachweisen. Die Erklärung der Strukturen von Wertorientierungen bei Kindern, ihrer Entwicklung und der familiä-ren Einflüsse bezieht theoretische Ansätze aus der Bedürfnisforschung, der Entwicklungspsychologie und der ungleichheitsorientierten Sozialisationsforschung mit ein.
... Past research has shown that girls tend to prioritize conservation values more than boys (e.g., Bilsky et al., 2015;Döring et al., 2015). Further, the school environment tends to promote and reward specific values, such as conformity, which encourage compliance with rules and the smooth functioning of social relations. ...
Article
Full-text available
Personal values are broad motivational goals that have been found to have systematic relations with subjective wellbeing in adults. Values that promote higher subjective wellbeing are considered healthy while those that hamper it are considered unhealthy (Schwartz & Sortheix, 2018). However, little is known about these relations in children. This pre-registered study examined (1) whether the values of children (6 to 12 years of age) relate to their subjective wellbeing and (2) whether these relations are moderated or mediated by perceived social support from parents, teachers, classmates, and close friends. These research questions were examined with a sample of 738 primary school students (50% female). Our results show that healthy growth values were positively related to subjective wellbeing overall, and for the subgroups of girls and children 9 to 12 years but not boys and children 6 to 8 years; however, unhealthy anxiety values were only negatively associated with subjective wellbeing for girls. While perceived social support partially mediated relations between growth values and subjective wellbeing, the direct values-wellbeing relations accounted for over half the variance. Interestingly, this study also found that growth values positively, and anxiety values negatively, influenced perceived social support from all referents. While perceived social support did not moderate values-wellbeing relations in the overall sample, differences were found in the way perceived social support moderated these relations in some age and gender subgroups. Taken together, these findings suggest that healthy growth values positively influence subjective wellbeing in middle childhood, even after accounting for perceived social support.
Article
Full-text available
Basic human values : theory, measurement, and applications. Applying the values construct in the social sciences has suffered from the absence of an agreed-upon conception of basic values, of the content and structure of relations among these values, and of reliable methods to measure them. This article presents data from over 70 countries, using two different instruments, to validate a theory intended to fill part of this gap. It concerns the basic values that individuals in all cultures recognize. The theory identifies 10 motivationally distinct values and specifies the dynamics of conflict and congruence among them. These dynamics yield a structure of relations among values common to culturally diverse groups, suggesting an universal organization of human motivations. Individuals and groups differ in the priorities they assign to these values. The article examines sources of individual differences in value priorities and behavioral and attitudinal consequences that follow from holding particular value priorities. In doing so, it considers processes through which values are influenced and through which they influence action.
Article
Full-text available
Contrary to those of adults and adolescents, children’s value structures and value preferences have rarely been investigated. This paper bridges this gap by building on Schwartz’s (1992) value theory. We present two studies: one in which children from Germany, Portugal, and Chile, aged 10 to 12 years, completed the Portrait Values Questionnaire (Schwartz et al., 2001); and one in which children from Germany and France, aged 7 to 12 years, completed the Picture-Based Value Survey for Children (Döring, Blauensteiner, Aryus, Drögekamp, & Bilsky, 2010). Theory-based multidimensional scaling analyses revealed differentiated value structures in both studies. Findings on children’s value preferences converged with evidence from adult samples (Schwartz & Bardi, 2001). The results are discussed in light of the present state of knowledge and consequences for future research. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
The results of applying a nonmetric multidimensional scaling algorithm to sets of pseudo random data are used as the basis for constructing, by means of regression techniques, a table which will assist a user to decide whether empirically obtained data sets are the result of a random process. The table covers the range from 12 to 48 objects (points) for one to five recovered dimensions.
Article
Full-text available
Person-organization fit (P-O fit) is often measured by the congruence of a person’s values and the values that he or she ascribes to the organization. A popular instrument used in this context is the Organizational Culture Profile (OCP; O’Reilly, Chatman, & Caldwell, 1991). The OCP uses 54 items that form eight factors in exploratory factor analysis. We investigate the extent to which the OCP can be embedded into Schwartz’s Theory of Universals in Values (TUV) that is formulated in terms of a circumplex in a two-dimensional plane. To address this question, we develop a nonstandard multidimensional scaling (MDS) method that enforces a TUV-based axial regionality onto the solution space together with a permutation test that assesses the consistency of the side constraints with the MDS representation. We find that the OCP can indeed be embedded into the TUV. The practical implication is that P-O fit can be assessed more simply by the congruence of the person’s and the organization’s positions on two value dimensions: risk versus rules and results versus relations.
Article
Full-text available
Multidimensional Scaling (MDS) has been a central tool of psychological research into value structure for more than two decades. MDS is usually applied in an exploratory way because strict confirmatory analyses are still difficult to realize, due to the manifold problems of how to impose regional constraints on MDS solutions. "Weak confirmatory approaches" (Borg & Staufenbiel, 2007) which use a theory-based starting configuration could be an acceptable compromise. In this paper, we outline how such a configuration can be derived from Schwartz's (1992, p. 14) original model, which is characterized by a strict circular structure. Such a "parsimonious" structure seems particularly suited for multi-method studies. We also apply this configuration to the Spanish value data from the first three rounds of the European Social Survey to illustrate the procedure. Finally, we show how to test whether and to what extent regional adjustments of single items affect the overall fit of the respective MDS solution.