ChapterPDF Available

Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern.

Authors:

Abstract

Individuals, both within and between different countries, vary substantially in the extent to which they view climate change as a risk. What could explain such variation in climate change risk perception around the world? Climate change is relatively unique as a risk in the sense that it is difficult for people to experience directly or even detect on a purely perceptual or sensory level. In fact, research across the social and behavioral sciences has shown that although people might correctly perceive some changes in long-term climate conditions, psychological factors are often much more influential in determining how the public perceives the risk of climate change. Indeed, decades of research has shown that cognitive, affective, social, and cultural factors all greatly influence the public’s perception of risk, and that these factors, in turn, often interact with each other in complex ways. Yet, although a wide variety of cognitive, experiential, socio-cultural and demographic characteristics have all proven to be relevant, are there certain factors that systematically stand out in explaining and predicting climate change risk perception around the world? And even if so, what do we mean, exactly, by the term “risk perception” and to what extent does the way in which risk perception is measured influence the outcome? Last but certainly not least, how important is public concern about climate change in determining people’s level of behavioral engagement and policy-support for the issue?
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 1 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Summary and Keywords
Individuals, both within and between different countries, vary substantially in the extent
to which they view climate change as a risk. What could explain such variation in climate
change risk perception around the world? Climate change is relatively unique as a risk in
the sense that it is difficult for people to experience directly or even detect on a purely
perceptual or sensory level. In fact, research across the social and behavioral sciences
has shown that although people might correctly perceive some changes in long-term
climate conditions, psychological factors are often much more influential in determining
how the public perceives the risk of climate change. Indeed, decades of research has
shown that cognitive, affective, social, and cultural factors all greatly influence the
public’s perception of risk, and that these factors, in turn, often interact with each other
in complex ways. Yet, although a wide variety of cognitive, experiential, socio-cultural
and demographic characteristics have all proven to be relevant, are there certain factors
that systematically stand out in explaining and predicting climate change risk perception
around the world? And even if so, what do we mean, exactly, by the term “risk
perception” and to what extent does the way in which risk perception is measured
influence the outcome? Last but certainly not least, how important is public concern
about climate change in determining people’s level of behavioral engagement and policy-
support for the issue?
Keywords: risk perception, climate change, global warming, worry, concern, public opinion
The Nature of Human Risk Perception
Risk does not exist independent of our minds and culture.
— Paul Slovic (1992, p. 690)
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk
Perception, Worry, and Concern
Sander van der Linden
Subject: Climate Change Communication Online Publication Date: Mar 2017
DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190228620.013.318
Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Climate Science
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 2 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
The perception of risk is a mental construct (Sjöberg, 2000A) and human perception is
rather unique in the sense that it allows for a differentiation between the existence of
objective real-world threats, such as climate change, and the subjective perceptual
evaluation of those threats (Rosa, 2003). For example, although climate change is one of
the greatest existential threats to life on earth, risk judgments of global warming vary
greatly from one individual to another (e.g., Hine et al., 2013; Maibach et al., 2011; Metag,
Füchslin, & Schäfer, 2015; Whitmarsh, 2011).
Furthermore, there is considerable cross-cultural variation in both the intensity of
collective public concern as well as general willingness to address the issue (Bord,
Fisher, & Robert, 1998; Brechin & Bhandari, 2011; Capstick et al., 2015 Howe et al., 2015; Kim
& Wolinsky-Nahmias, 2014; Leiserowitz, 2007; Lee et al., 2015). For example, many large-
scale reviews and analyses of public opinion polls have shown that climate change has
consistently been perceived as a “very serious” problem in the United Kingdom,
Australia, and most of continental Europe (e.g., Bord et al., 1998; Lorenzoni & Pidgeon,
2006; Pidgeon, 2012; Reser et al., 2012) whereas concern, while waxing and waning, has
traditionally been lower in countries such as the United States, China, and Russia (e.g.,
Brechin & Bhandari, 2011; Lee et al., 2015; Leiserowitz, 2007).
Another relatively stable trend is that compared to many developed countries, climate
change is generally perceived as a much greater risk in most of the developing world
(Kim & Wolinsky-Nahmias, 2014; Lee et al., 2015; Leiserowitz, 2007). Last but not least,
although overall “awareness” and “concern” about climate change has generally
increased around the globe over the last quarter century (Capstick et al., 2015; Moser,
2010), the public still ranks climate change as a low priority compared to many other
societal issues, such as terrorism, health care, and the economy (Lorenzoni & Pidgeon,
2006; Nisbet & Myers, 2007; Motel, 2014). This low sense of urgency is partly due to the fact
that climate change is an abstract statistical concept that refers to long-term changes in
the variability of the earth’s climate (Weber, 2010). Unlike most ecological risks humans
have been exposed to for millions of years, human-caused climate change is unique: it is
global in nature and stretches over centuries (Breakwell, 2010). Moreover, the slow-
moving, cumulative, and unsituated nature of climate change makes it not only
evolutionarily novel (van Vugt, Griskevicius, & Schultz, 2014) but also difficult to directly
perceive and experience for people (Weber, 2010; Whitmarsh, 2008A). These characteristics
are important to understand because the subjective psychological nature of risk
perception is exactly what allows for substantial heterogeneity to exist across individuals
and nations.
Accordingly, the quantity of social and behavioral science research exploring what factors
shape public perceptions of climate change has increased exponentially over the last
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 3 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
decade (Moser, 2010; Weber, 2016). The goal of this chapter is to put the field’s intellectual
history in perspective as well as structure, organize, and synthesize the weight of
evidence on three important questions: (a) what social, psychological, cultural, political,
and physical factors have shown to consistently explain and predict public risk
perceptions of climate change around the world? (b) to what extent do these results
hinge upon how “risk perception” is measured and operationalized in the first place? and
(c) what is the evidence for a relationship between risk perception and concern about
climate change on one hand, and individual behavior change on the other, including
public support for climate change adaptation and mitigation policies?
The Factors That Shape Climate Change Risk
Perception Around the World
The study of risk perception grew out of the observation that when it comes to assessing
many technological and natural hazards, the views of the lay public often seem to diverge
(quite sharply) from expert assessments (Slovic, Fischhoff, & Lichtenstein, 1982; Starr,
1969). For many researchers, this divergence was both interesting and puzzling and
inspired the study of how people construe their mental representations of risks. Climate
change is a perfect case in point. For example, although many independent studies have
shown that over 97% of climate scientists agree that human-caused climate change is
happening (e.g., Anderegg et al., 2010; Cook et al., 2016; Powell, 2016), only about half of
Americans share this belief (Leiserowitz et al., 2016).
Fueled by the discovery of a number of cognitive heuristics people seem to use to
navigate an uncertain world, much early risk perception research was rooted in an area
of cognitive psychology known as “judgment and decision-making” (Kahneman, Slovic, &
Tversky, 1982). Specifically, the so-called “psychometric paradigm” pioneered the process
of identifying explanatory factors in risk perception (Slovic, 1987). Yet, following the
cognitive revolution, scholars increasingly began to criticize the overly “cognitive”
approach to the study of risk by highlighting the neglected yet important role of emotions
in shaping risk judgments. This development led to the conclusion that how people feel
about a particular risk often has a (more) powerful influence on their thinking
(Loewenstein et al., 2001; Slovic et al., 2004). Since then, so-called “dual-process” theories
have postulated that people comprehend risks in two fundamentally different ways;
analytically and experientially, and although these are often referred to as “two separate
modes of thinking,” they often operate in parallel (Chaiken & Trope, 1999; Epstein, 1994;
Kahneman, 2011; Marx et al., 2007; Sloman, 1996; van der Linden, 2014A).
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 4 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
The still predominantly “psychological” approach to studying risk was later criticized,
most notably by cultural anthropologist Mary Douglas and political scientist Aaron
Wildavsky, for neglecting the larger social, cultural, and political context in which risks
are framed and debated, and for depoliticizing the nature of risk. In other words, the
perception of risk was not solely to be seen as a matter of individual cognition and
emotion but also a function of deeply held worldviews and values about society and its
structural organization and functioning (Dake, 1992; Douglas & Wildavsky, 1982). This
development led to a third revolution in the study of risk perception, which is currently
still enjoying support through frameworks such as the Social Amplification of Risk
(Kasperson et al., 1988) and the Cultural Cognition Thesis—which aims to combine aspects
of the psychometric paradigm with the cultural theory of risk (Kahan, 2012).
All of these approaches have left a deep mark on the climate change risk perception
literature and although some attempts have been made to combine various schools of
thought (e.g., see Leiserowitz, 2006), when assessing the risk perception literature as a
whole, a severe lack of theoretical integration has been noted, with many of the
aforementioned dimensions often being assessed independently of each other (Wählberg,
2001; van der Linden, 2015A). In some sense, the field has become more theoretically
contested (Moser, 2016) with scholars disagreeing on the various approaches to the study
of risk perception (van der Linden, 2016A).
This complicates the process of “surveying the field.” To help advance and promote
further theoretical development in the literature, van der Linden (2015A) proposed an
integrated theory of risk perception that combines four key theoretical dimensions to
maximize explanatory power; “cognitive,” “experiential,” “socio-cultural” and “socio-
demographic” factors, also known as the “Climate Change Risk Perception
Model” (CCRPM). Empirically, these factors explained about 70% of the variation in risk
perception, which may well approximate the ceiling of the explanatory power of risk
perception models (Sjöberg, 2002). Accordingly, the framework is adopted here to help
organize, structure, and assess the empirical evidence for each of the major dimensions
that have shown to influence risk perceptions of climate change. The original formulation
of the Climate Change Risk Perception Model (CCRPM) included a fifth dimension,
entitled; “heuristics and biases” (Helgeson, van der Linden, & Chabay, 2012). This
dimension was later dropped from the model mainly for parsimony, as many heuristics
could reasonably be subsumed under one of the existing categories, but given the large
amount of heuristics and biases that have shown to influence global warming risk
perception in recent years, it warrants a separate discussion and I therefore reintroduce
the fifth dimension here for completeness.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 5 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
A conceptual representation of the model is provided in Figure 1. Note that these five
dimensions are not necessarily assumed to be independent, as will become clear from the
review, they can often be expected to interact in complex ways. For example, cognitive
and affective factors have shown to dynamically interact in shaping climate change risk
perception (Marx et al., 2007; van der Linden, 2014A). Additionally, the influence of socio-
demographic characteristics on risk perception and the use of heuristics and biases may
be conditional on cultural, affective, and cognitive factors. Thus, although Figure 1 is a
simplified representation of reality, it will be useful in organizing and synthesizing the
risk perception literature.
Cognitive Factors
The assumption that the public simply does not have enough information to accurately
evaluate societal risks has dominated the risk communication field for many years.
According to this view, if scientists would do a better job at explaining and
communicating climate science, then perhaps the public would be more concerned about
the issue. In the last two decades, the so-called “knowledge deficit” model of public
attitudes towards science has received fierce criticism (Sturgis & Allum, 2004), so much so
that polarization between “proponents” and “opponents” of education-based approaches
has increased substantially (Ranney & Clark, 2016). Yet, this dichotomy is somewhat
misleading. For example, what is meant by “knowledge” is often left undefined and the
wholesale dismissal of “knowledge” as a driver of risk perception begs the age-old
question of whether or not “cognition” is a necessary prerequisite for judgment formation
(Lai, Hagoort, & Casasanto, 2012). In other words, if one has no basic awareness of the
climate change problem, then how can a judgment about the issue be formed? Formally,
risk assessment is usually thought of as the product of two properties, namely; (a) the
probability with which an adverse event (e.g., climate change) is likely to occur and (b)
the severity of the negative consequences associated with that event (e.g., death,
damages). Thus, if the public understands that climate change is occurring, caused by
Click to view larger
Figure 1. Climate Change Risk Perception Model
adopted from van der Linden (2015A).
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 6 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
humans, and has negative consequences, they should (in theory) be concerned about the
issue. Yet, while varying substantially, deeper public understanding of climate change
around the world remains limited and is often rooted in influential misperceptions (Bord
et al., 1998; Brechin, 2003; Leiserowitz, 2007; Weber & Stern, 2011). Climate literacy is
especially low in the United States (e.g., Leiserowitz, Smith, & Marlon, 2010) while
substantially less is presently known about public understanding of climate change in the
non-Western world (Capstick et al., 2015).
Much early work on climate cognition was trying to understand the way in which
individuals process, classify, and organize new information, the study of so-called “mental
models,” that is, people’s intuitive and contextual understanding of how something works
(Kearney & Kaplan, 1997; Morgan et al., 2002). This line of research revealed that people
often have difficulty understanding the physical mechanisms underlying global climate
change, are unaware of the prevailing scientific consensus, and confuse climate change
with other environmental issues or hold misperceptions about the type of actions that are
effective in helping to reduce climate change (e.g., Bord et al., 1998; Bostrom et al., 1994;
Kempton, 1991; Nisbet & Myers, 2007; Read et al., 1994; Sterman & Booth Sweeney, 2007;
Sterman, 2008; Whitmarsh, 2009).
Although public awareness has generally increased since then (Capstick et al., 2015), many
of these deeper misperceptions continue to persist (e.g., see Brechin, 2003; Ranney &
Clark, 2016; Reynolds et al., 2010). When it comes to investing scarce resources in public
education about climate change, perhaps the more prudent and difficult question to
answer is how important cognitive knowledge about climate change is in shaping public
risk perception?
The answer, in part, depends on the method that is used to gauge the public’s
“knowledge” and understanding of climate change (Roser-Renouf & Nisbet, 2008; van der
Linden, 2015A). For example, there is a notable and important difference between an
individual’s subjective self-assessment of how much they believe they know about climate
change, and the actual level of correct knowledge people hold about the issue. Studies
using single-item measures, such as, “How much do you feel you know about global
warming?” have reported mixed results. For instance, Kellstedt et al. (2008) found that
knowledge is largely unrelated to concern. In contrast, Heath and Gifford (2006) report a
positive link whereas Malka, Krosnick, and Langer (2009) conclude that the knowledge-
concern relationship may be moderated by political ideology.
Although global self-assessments may sometimes provide a crude estimate of a latent
psychological disposition (van der Linden & Rosenthal, 2016), subjective, self-reported
climate knowledge measures are generally deemed unreliable (Roser-Renouf & Nisbet,
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 7 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
2008) and often prove inconsistent with more objective assessments (Hornsey et al., 2016;
Shi et al., 2016).
For example, Guy et al. (2014) found—using objective measures—that greater knowledge
actually attenuates the (negative) effect of ideological worldviews, resulting in a positive
relationship between more knowledge about climate change and public concern.
More generally, research that has attempted to objectively score and assess how much
people know about climate change typically finds a significant positive relationship
between more accurate knowledge about climate change and public risk perception
(Hidalgo & Pisano, 2010; Milfont, 2012; O’Connor, Bord, & Fisher, 1999; Sundblad et al., 2007;
Shi et al., 2016; van der Linden, 2015A). To further deconstruct the role of knowledge in risk
perception, scholars have proposed a conceptual distinction between three different
types of knowledge, including (declarative) knowledge about the causes and physical
mechanisms underlying climate change, knowledge about the impacts and consequences
of climate change, and (procedural) knowledge about how to respond and implement
potential solutions (Tobler et al., 2012; van der Linden, 2015A). In fact, a number of recent
large-scale studies have shed new and important light on the role of knowledge as a
predictor of risk perception. For example, in an unprecedented analysis of 119 countries,
Lee et al. (2015) find that both educational attainment and the understanding that climate
change is human-caused are important predictors of public risk perception worldwide.
Similarly, in another large study, Shi et al. (2016) find that across three continents,
different forms of climate knowledge are significant predictors of climate change risk
perception.
Although the weight of evidence is clearly in favor of a positive association between
knowledge about climate change and public concern, a logical next question is whether it
is possible to quantify how much knowledge matters? In an attempt to partition out the
unique variance explained by different forms of objective climate knowledge (while
controlling for other key constructs, such as norms and values), van der Linden (2015A)
estimates that knowledge about climate change explains roughly 10% of the variance in
public concern about climate change. In a cross-cultural follow-up study, Shi et al. (2016)
place this estimate between 2% and 18%, thus, there may be significant cross-cultural
variation in how much knowledge contributes (Lee et al., 2015; Shi et al., 2016). Overall, a
recent meta-analysis synthesizing 171 studies across 56 nations revealed that objective
knowledge shares a small to medium correlation with climate beliefs (r = 0.25),
explaining about 6.5% of the variance (Hornsey et al., 2016).
In conclusion, knowledge is likely a necessary but clearly not sufficient condition for
public concern. Having said this, it is reasonable to hypothesize that some forms of
1
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 8 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
knowledge may be more important than others (Kaiser & Fuhrer, 2003). Indeed, not all
types of knowledge about climate change exert an equal amount of influence on risk
perception (Shi et al., 2016; van der Linden, 2015A). To illustrate, perhaps better procedural
knowledge of what actions people can take to help reduce climate change is most
pressing (van der Linden, 2015A). On the other hand, understanding the human causes of
climate change is often a prerequisite for accepting the need for mitigatory action in the
first place (Guy et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2015). Accordingly, the potential synergy between
these different forms of climate knowledge should not be neglected or underestimated
(Kaiser & Fuhrer, 2003; van der Linden, 2015A). In short, researchers interested in studying
the role of climate knowledge in public risk perception are advised to take note of these
conceptual distinctions and implement objective assessments whenever possible.
Experiential Processing
Negative Affect
In addition to holding cognitive knowledge about a particular risk, people frequently
experience risks in affective and emotional terms as well. In fact, the “risk-as-feelings
hypothesis suggests that when cognitive and affective risk judgments diverge, affective
reactions are often more dominant in processing (Loewenstein et al., 2001). Early research
in affective neuroscience postulated that over time, through learning and experience,
people’s mental representations of objects and events become “tagged” with affective
associations that guide subsequent judgment formation. These instantaneous and
evaluative judgments of things people like, dislike, find positive or negative are known as
“somatic markers” (Damasio, 1994). Closely associated with a risk-factor known as
“dread,” this later formed the basis of what has become widely known as the “affect-
heuristic” (Slovic et al., 2004, 2007). In particular, people often rely on what is called an
“affective pool,” which essentially includes all the positive and negative associations that
people hold in memory with respect to a given risk object (Breakwell, 2010).
Yet, at the same time, some conceptual confusion over the meaning of the term “affect”
has led to a notable debate in the risk perception literature (e.g., see van der Pligt et al.,
1998; Wardman, 2006). For example, the concept of “affect” is theoretically distinct from
other, more discrete types of emotions such as fear or worry. Instead, affect is generally
described as a quick associative judgment or a “faint whisper of emotion” (Slovic &
Peters, 2006). Other scholars have noted that this definition is analogous to what is
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 9 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
generally considered to be the evaluative component of an individual’s attitude. In fact,
the concept of “attitude” has traditionally been defined as “the affect for or against a
psychological object” (Thurstone, 1931, p. 261).
Sjöberg has argued that if “affect” reflects an “attitude” and if “affect” is often falsely
equated in the literature with the term “emotion,” then we would mistakenly assume that
“emotions” play a crucial role in risk perception (Sjöberg, 2006). In addition, the
operational validity of the affect-heuristic and the explanatory power of the “dread”
factor in risk perception have both been questioned (e.g., Sjöberg, 2006, 2007). For
example, while negative affect is often inferred from self-reports in surveys, some implicit
association tests have revealed that affective judgments correlate more strongly with
explicit attitudes rather than quick associative reactions to stimuli (Townsend, Spence, &
Knowles, 2014). Other research has questioned the structure of the “risk-as-feelings”
model (Kobbeltved et al., 2005) and has suggested that some dimensions of the “affect-
heuristic,” e.g., the good-or-badness of an object (Slovic et al., 2007) likely conflates
affective with moral judgments (van der Linden, 2015A), at least to the extent that one is
interested in affective and not affective-based moral judgments (Roeser, 2009).
Nonetheless, it should be stressed that some of these conceptual objections, while valid,
do not weigh up against the depth and breadth of empirical evidence that has
documented the critical function of emotion in risk perception (Finucane, 2012; Wardman,
2006). For example, pioneering research examining global warming “affective imagery”
finds that the first thing that comes to mind for most people when thinking about global
warming are bleak and negative associations related to the impacts of climate change
(Leiserowitz, 2006; Leviston et al., 2014; Lorenzoni et al., 2006; Smith & Leiserowitz, 2012).
Accordingly, affective imagery and holistic negative affect have both shown to be
important predictors of global warming risk perception (Leiserowitz, 2006; Smith &
Leiserowitz, 2012; Sundblad et al., 2007; van der Linden, 2015A). In fact, explaining about
20%–30% of the variance by itself, negative affect often emerges as one of the single
most important determinants of global warming risk perception (Leiserowitz, 2006; van der
Linden, 2015A). Having said this, there is a much needed but notable lack of research
exploring the affective basis of concern about global warming in the non-Western world.
A final issue concerns the conceptual relationship between “affect” and “cognition,”
which is particularly important to the context of climate change. A large body of
converging research across social, cognitive, and clinical psychology has pointed towards
a complex “dual” or “parallel” process relationship between cognition and affect,
suggesting that the human brain processes information about risks in two fundamentally
different ways, with one system being slower, conscious, analytical, and rule-based,
whereas the other is faster, unconscious, associative, and automatic (Chaiken & Trope,
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 10 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
1999; Epstein, 1994; Kahneman, 2011; Loewenstein et al., 2001; Marx et al., 2007; Pessoa, 2008;
Sloman, 1996).
The primacy of “affect” as an independent force in shaping (risk) judgments has been
debated at length in psychology (Clore & Ortony, 2000; Lazarus, 1984; Loewenstein et al.,
2001; Zajonc, 1984), and although the human brain is fast and experienced in mapping
environmental cues directly into affective responses (Weber, 2006), it is increasingly
recognized that the “primacy” of affect versus cognition is context-dependent (Lai,
Hagoort, & Casasanto, 2012). For example, because the risk of climate change does not
automatically trigger the brain’s “affective” system (Weber, 2006), some cognitive
mediation is likely to occur. In other words, while the public may personally experience
the impacts of global warming, in order for people to form negative affective judgments
about climate change, such personal experiences need to be mentally associated with
climate change (Weber, 2010; van der Linden, 2014A). Accordingly, research has started to
reveal how the dynamic bi-directional relationship between cognitive and affective
processes shape public risk perceptions of global warming (van der Linden, 2014A).
This raises some important questions about the conceptual relationship between “affect”
and “risk perception.” In particular, it suggests that modeling affect solely as a
determinant of risk perception (and not risk perception also as a determinant of affect),
may fail to specify the true nature of the relationship between affect and cognition
(Jackson, Allum, & Gaskell, 2006). This issue is not particular to risk researchers, linear
models and linear thinking is widespread throughout the social sciences, but as tools are
being developed to allow for more complex, dynamic, and accurate representations of
reality, future research would be well-advised to focus on the link between cognition and
emotion in shaping global warming risk judgments. For example, it remains unclear
whether exposing people to vicarious imagery about global warming activates neural
substrates related to affective-based information processing, and, moreover, to what
extent such activation interacts with, or is mediated by, cognitive processes. With new
technological advances such as virtual reality simulations of climate change impacts
(Zaalberg & Midden, 2013) and emerging fields such as “communication
neuroscience” (Berkman & Falk, 2013), such integrated methodologies may become
increasingly accessible to social scientists.
Personal Experience
Akerlof et al. (2013) ask a crucial question; “Do people ‘personally experience’ global
warming, and if so, how, and does it matter?” Although experience can be a powerful
teacher, this is a difficult question to answer. From an indirect point of view, the answer
2
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 11 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
would appear “yes,” as it is through personal experiences that people form affective
associations, and affective judgments of future risks largely depend on the vividness with
which negative impacts can be imagined (Damasio, 1994; Risen & Critcher, 2011; Weber,
2006). Whether personal experience with climate change also has a direct impact on global
warming risk perception has become a source of debate and somewhat hinges upon how
“personal experience” is defined and measured.
As the literature and methodologies available to researchers examining this question has
grown over the years, it is useful to introduce a conceptual distinction between
“detection of environmental change” and personal experiences with “extreme weather.”
A common sense assumption among many climate scientists has been that as the average
global temperature continues to rise, people will eventually “catch on.” Yet, perceptual
detection of global warming is difficult because people only experience highly variable
local weather patterns, which are not always reflective of long-terms trends in the earth’s
climate (Pawlik, 1991). Nonetheless, in a large study covering 89 countries, Howe et al.
(2013) find that, on average, individuals living in places with rising temperatures are
indeed more likely than others to perceive local warming. Other studies also find that
public perceptions do broadly track with instrumental climate data, such as seasonal
weather, temperature, and precipitation change (Akerlof et al., 2013; Hamilton & Keim,
2009; Howe & Leiserowitz, 2013). In addition, a large body of work shows that the
experience and detection of heat and warm daily temperatures is associated with concern
about global warming (e.g., Brooks et al., 2014; Li, Johnson, & Zaval, 2011; Risen &
Critcher, 2011). In contrast, Marquart-Pyatt et al. (2014) argue against the theory that
changes in climatic conditions will produce noticeable shifts in public perception, as their
analyses suggests that objective climatic changes only have a negligible effect on concern
about climate change. McCright, Dunlap, and Xiao (2014) provide mixed evidence,
supporting the finding that actual temperature anomalies influence perceived warming,
but question how much this practically “matters.”
The relationship between global warming risks perception and (subjective) personal
experience with visceral extreme weather events, such as hurricanes, flooding, heat
waves, and droughts appears more robust, with a large body of evidence supporting a
significant association (Akerlof et al., 2013; Capstick & Pidgeon, 2014; Howe et al., 2014;
Krosnick et al., 2006; Myers et al., 2012; Reser et al., 2014; Spence et al., 2012; Taylor et al.,
2014; van der Linden, 2015A)—with only a few exceptions (Brulle et al., 2012; Whitmarsh,
2008A). Yet, in determining the importance of accurate detection and personal experience,
a major theme that has cropped up is the finding that the magnitude of the association
appears rather small in comparison to the role of political ideology (Marquart-Pyatt et al.,
2014; McCright et al., 2014; Shao & Goidel, 2016).
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 12 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
This finding particularly makes sense in countries where the issue of climate change is
highly politicized, such as the United States (McCright & Dunlap, 2011A). Accordingly,
studies have revealed a more complex and dynamic relationship between perceived
“local” experience and worldviews, so that personal beliefs about global warming color
one’s perception of local change and vice versa (Myers et al., 2012; Howe & Leiserowitz,
2013; Schuldt & Roh, 2014).
Other studies have noted that the association between personal experience and risk
perception is not attenuated by political ideology (Akerlof et al., 2013; Egan & Mullin, 2012),
although the magnitude of the remaining effect appears low (e.g., van der Linden, 2015A).
In their recent meta-analysis, Hornsey et al. (2016) classify the effect-size of local and
extreme weather experience as “low to medium.” Part of the issue in quantifying the
importance of personal experience is a lack of operational consistency in terms of what
qualifies as a significant “weather anomaly,” over what time period the change is
assessed, and whether people accurately recall their experiences. In addition, frequent
media use of the term “global warming” rather than “climate change” may limit the range
of experiences and weather phenomena that people associate with climate change and
thereby dampen its impact on risk perception (e.g., see Capstick & Pidgeon, 2014; Schuldt
& Roh, 2014). Other research suggests that the influence of (extreme) weather experiences
on public opinion decay rather quickly (Egan & Mullin, 2012) and so more longitudinal
assessments are therefore necessary (Reser et al., 2014). Lastly, it remains an open
question as to whether “personal experience” is best thought of as an indirect factor,
shaping people’s affective responses to climate change by making future impacts more
salient and easier to imagine (Risen & Critcher, 2011) or whether personal experience
should also be conceptualized as a direct predictor of global warming risk perception in
its own right.
Social and Cultural Influences
The Social Construction of Risk
In addition to both cognitive and affective theories, early sociological research criticized
existing approaches to the study of risk for the notable lack of consideration of social
influence processes (Dake, 1992; Douglas, 1978). This lack of attention for the social context
in which risks are framed and debated is indeed surprising, given that the way in which
people process and evaluate risks is clearly influenced by the thoughts, feelings, and
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 13 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
decisions of other people (Joffe, 2003). In response, two sociological approaches were
developed, including Social Representations Theory (Moscovici, 1984) and the Social
Amplification of Risk Framework (Kasperson et al., 1988; Pidgeon et al., 2003). Albeit
different theories, what both approaches have in common is a focus on how interpersonal
interactions, societal norms, and the mass media shape and circulate social
representations of a given risk in society. The process of how risk signals are received,
interpreted, and diffused is particularly relevant in understanding how the
communication of climate risks is impacted and moderated by social processes. For
example, qualitative studies have argued that although climate change risks have indeed
been societally amplified (e.g., Renn, 2010; Smith & Joffe, 2013), it remains difficult to
quantify what the impact of these processes are on concern about global warming.
Accordingly, both approaches have been criticized for their vague “meta-theoretical”
nature (Voelklein & Howarth, 2005; Wåhlberg, 2001), particularly because the “societal”
level of analysis makes it difficult to identify and quantify the causal impact of various
social influence processes on risk perception (Renn, 2010).
In turn, social psychologists have focused more specifically on the role of social and
group norms and generally distinguish between “descriptive” and “prescriptive” norms,
where the former simply describes the behavior of similar others while the latter
prescribes how one ought to think or behave (Cialdini, Kallgren, & Reno, 1991). More
generally, social norms can be thought of as “expectations of how people are supposed to
act, think, or feel in specific situations” (Popenoe, 1983, p. 5). Although norms are
generally studied in relation to behavior (Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004; Doherty & Webler,
2016), they influence perceptions too. In one study, van der Linden (2015A) showed that
both descriptive and prescriptive social norms exert a notable influence on global
warming risk perception, jointly contributing a substantial amount of the explained
variance (22%). In other words, the greater the extent to which climate change is viewed
as a serious risk by influential social referents, such as friends and family, the more it
amplifies and intensifies an individual’s own risk perception (van der Linden, 2015A). These
findings extend to communicating high social consensus about climate change among
influential out-groups too, such as scientists (Lewandowsky et al., 2013; van der Linden et
al., 2015). Other research has started to focus on the role of “network influence” and the
frequency with which people talk to or are influenced by close friends and family on the
issue of climate change (e.g., see Butts, 2016). Although this body of research is still
limited, studies have found that social network variables, such as homophily, network
size, and centrality have a significant influence on concern about global warming (Brody
et al., 2008; Leombruni, 2015). Nonetheless, unlike the rich behavioral literature, there
remains a substantial lack of research on the link between social influence processes and
climate change risk perception. For example, current studies infer network influence
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 14 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
from self-reported survey data (Brody et al., 2008; Leombruni, 2015) rather than analyzing
and constructing actual social networks. Yet, with the increasing spread and transmission
of risk information on social media, new theories and methods are being developed,
including “social contagion theories of risk” (Scherer & Cho, 2003), social tipping points
(Kinzig et al., 2013; van der Linden, 2017), “sentiment” analyses on Twitter (Cody et al.,
2015), and the role of network opinion leaders (Nisbet & Kotcher, 2009). In short, much
exciting research remains to be done on the topic of normative influence and its impact
on concern about global warming.
Culture, Values, and Worldviews
The notion that culture gives rise to socially constructed systems of beliefs, or so-called,
“worldviews” has gained increased attention over the last decades (Dake, 1992). Perhaps
the most well-known response to the criticism that cognitive and affective psychological
theories “depoliticize” the nature of risk by failing to take account of the competing socio-
cultural structures of societies was rooted in the development of the “Cultural Theory of
Risk” (Douglas, 1970; Douglas & Wildavsky, 1982). Originally based on anthropological
research, cultural theory proposes a conceptual typology of risk-culture, also known as
the “grid-group” system, where four overarching worldviews are delineated. These
include: egalitarianism, individualism, hierarchism, and fatalism. The relative position of
these cultural types is determined by the degree to which individuals feel bounded by a
sense of belonging and solidarity (group) and the amount of control and structure that
people maintain in their social lives (grid). Wildavsky and Dake (1990) later
operationalized these cultural types so that they could be measured and tested
empirically. Since then, the Cultural Theory of Risk has generated a fierce and long-
standing debate in the risk perception literature, polarizing “proponents” and
“opponents.”
On one hand, cultural worldviews, particularly the individualism and egalitarianism
dimensions, have shown to differentially influence global warming risk perception
(Akerlof et al., 2013; Kahan et al., 2012; Leiserowitz, 2006; Smith & Leiserowitz, 2012; Xue et
al., 2014). On the other hand, scholars have argued that the actual explanatory power of
the cultural worldview scales are very low (Boholm, 1996; Marris, Langford, & O’Riordan,
1998; Oltedal et al., 2004; Sjöberg, 1997, 1998, 2012; van der Linden, 2015A, 2016A), which has led
some scholars to conclude that “cultural theory is simply wrong” (e.g., Sjöberg, 1998, p.
150). Yet, leaving the explanatory power of the theory aside for a moment, the other point
of contention revolves around two major issues in the literature, namely: (a) the
conceptual and empirical validity of the cultural worldview scales themselves and (b)
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 15 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
whether it is appropriate or even informative, for that matter, to try to infer latent
cultural dispositions from individual-level data.
Starting with the more practical concern, the initial worldview scales were criticized for
having rather low scale reliability scores and for lacking basic construct and discriminant
validity (e.g., Boholm, 1996; Price, Walker, & Boschetti, 2014; Rippl, 2002; Sjöberg, 1998). This
issue is of particular relevance given that in practice, individuals often score high on
competing dimensions, which is problematic, because according to cultural theory,
individuals cannot be characterized by mutually incompatible worldviews (Kahan, 2012).
Moreover, even when scale reliabilities are improved, their explanatory power often
remains low (Rippl, 2002). In an attempt to combine research from the psychometric
paradigm with cultural theory, Kahan and colleagues advanced an alternative conception
of the cultural theory of risk known as the “cultural cognition thesis” (Kahan, 2012). The
basic premise of the cultural cognition thesis is that people are expected to credit or
dismiss empirical evidence about societal risks based on whether it coheres or conflicts
with their cultural values, a process described as “identity-protective cognition” (Kahan,
2012). The more recently developed cultural cognition scales have also shown to influence
climate change risk perception (e.g., Kahan et al., 2012).
In turn, the cultural cognition thesis has been heavily criticized, particularly for its
questionably low explanatory power (e.g., Boholm, 2015; Fremling & Lott, 2003; Sunstein,
2007; Swanson, 2010; van der Linden, 2016A). Although it may be argued that small effects
can still have important and practical consequences (Prentice & Miller, 1992), especially
when aggregating small changes across individual opinions, when the purpose is to
develop a theory of risk perception, the quality of the theory should be judged by its
overall explanatory power (Boholm, 2015; Sjöberg, 2012; van der Linden, 2015A). To this end,
recent meta-analyses do find some support for the cultural theory scales, but note that
consistent with prior research, the effect-sizes are often modest (Xue et al., 2014; Hornsey
et al., 2016).
One of the primary issues with both the original conception of cultural theory and its
successor, cultural cognition, is that the theory is tautological in its reasoning. In other
words, it is circular to suggest that “people of culture A habitually do X because they
share this particular culture A that prescribes that they do X” (Boholm, 1996, 2015). Indeed,
“by definition, the idea of cultural cognition is to illuminate risk perceptions only for
those risks that are culturally contested” (Sunstein, 2007, p. 17). In other words, to explain
public risk polarization on an issue such as climate change by (artificially) categorizing
the public into essentially two polarizing groups (individualists vs. egalitarians) is a so-
called “strange loop” (van der Linden, 2016A). Although such theoretical inconsistencies
are often overlooked, the consequences of pseudoscientific theorizing are serious
3
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 16 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
(Gigerenzer, 2000). For example, they may render the empirical predictions resulting from
cultural cognition theory suspect (van der Linden, 2016A).
Another major issue in the literature is rooted in the systematic conflation of concepts
such as culture, values, worldviews, and ideology. For example, cultural cognition
explores how different political groups in the United States perceive a select number of
contemporary societal issues. In fact, the cultural cognition scales feature the word
“government” over 10 times (van der Linden, 2016A) and so it is unclear to what extent
cultural cognition is conceptually distinct from partisan motivated reasoning (e.g., see
Bolsen, Druckman, & Cook, 2014; Hart & Nisbet, 2012).
Furthermore, research has shown that the cultural scales proposed by both Dake (1991)
and Kahan and colleagues do not translate well to other cultures, such as China (Xue et
al., 2015).
Part of the issue is rooted in a problematic conception of the term “culture,” what it
refers to, and how different levels of culture interact with each other (e.g., political vs.
national culture). Moreover, values are not the same as worldviews (Koltko-Rivera, 2004;
van der Linden, 2016A). Whereas worldviews are very broad, situation-invariant orienting
dispositions, values are usually defined as fundamental guiding principles that not only
precede but are also more stable and specific than worldviews (Rokeach, 1973; Schwartz &
Wolfgang, 1987; Stern et al., 1999). It has been noted that cultural worldviews and values
overlap conceptually (Corner, Markowitz, & Pidgeon, 2014; Koltko-Rivera, 2004), because
cultures are essentially comprised of and characterized by their underlying values
structures (Hofstede, 2001; Schwartz, 1992).
The difficulty lies in the argument that latent cultural worldviews may not be an innate
psychological tendency that can reliably be inferred from individual-level data (DeGroot,
Steg, & Poortinga, 2013; Rippl, 2002). This is mainly so because cultural differences are best
observed between different countries and not between individuals within the same
country, given that cultural variation decreases when people with different backgrounds
assimilate into the same culture (Oreg & Katz-Gerro, 2006). Mary Douglas herself noted
that the motivation behind the concept of “cultural bias” was to explain cross-cultural
differences in risk construal (Douglas, 1978), not conflicts between political groups within
the same country (with the same culture). Although it can therefore be argued that the
concept of “culture” cannot be reduced to a single variable, the large-scale aggregation
of value preferences within and between societies may offer a conceptually more stable
and direct way to “proxy” shared enculturation in models of risk perception (DeGroot et
al., 2013; Slimak & Dietz, 2006; van der Linden, 2015A, 2016A).
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 17 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
A prominent example is the conceptual distinction between so-called “egoistic,” “socio-
altruistic,” and “biospheric” value orientations (Stern, Dietz, & Kalof, 1993; Stern, 2000).
Some attractive properties of the values-approach are: (a) these value structures tend to
be the same in different countries (Schwartz & Sagiv, 1995), which makes standardization
and comparison easier and more meaningful; (b) values are not mutually exclusive, i.e.,
individuals can simultaneously express egoistic, socio-altruistic, and biospheric value
preferences but people (and therefore cultures) may prioritize these values differently
(Steg & De Groot, 2012); and lastly, (c) these value scales have been reliably validated in a
series of cross-cultural studies (De Groot & Steg, 2007, 2008, 2010; Schultz, 2001; Steg et al.,
2011; Stern & Dietz, 1994). Having said this, some scholars have noted that altruistic and
biospheric values tend to be strongly correlated (e.g., van der Linden, 2015A). Nonetheless,
it is possible that people’s concern for others and the environment could diverge, at
which point, the theoretical distinction may become more meaningful (DeGroot et al.,
2013). Although the application of values to the study of risk perception is relatively new in
comparison to cultural theory, biospheric or “environmental” values have shown to
reliably predict global warming risk perception (e.g., Brody et al., 2008; Milfont, 2012;
Hornsey et al., 2016; Slimak & Dietz, 2006; van der Linden, 2015A).
In conclusion, it should be acknowledged that any attempt to model individual risk
perception inevitably decontextualizes risk from the situation in which it arises.
Accordingly, there is some inherent difficulty in acknowledging the dynamic and
emergent nature of social practice on one hand, and the pursuit to try to represent the
“socio-cultural” as part of the individual, on the other. Nonetheless, over the last
decades, these questions have forced risk scholars to think harder and more carefully
about how culture shapes risk perception and the field would be well-served by further
attempts to bridge the “levels of analysis” divide (Jackson, Allum, & Gaskell, 2006). In part,
by clearly distinguishing and defining conceptual predictors (e.g., ideology, values,
culture, worldviews) so that better standardized comparisons can be conducted of the
various “cultural constructs” used to predict risk perceptions of climate change.
Socio-Demographic Characteristics
With a few exceptions, the weight of evidence on the influence of various socio-
demographic and social-structural factors on climate change risk perception is rather
mixed, as the results tend to vary from sample to sample and from study to study. For
example, while some studies find that higher education predicts stronger risk perceptions
of climate change (Hornsey et al., 2016; Lee et al., 2015; van der Linden, 2015A), other studies
find no education-effect (Akerlof et al., 2013; Brody et al., 2008; Kellstedt et al., 2008; Milfont,
2012; O’Connor et al., 1999 Sundblad et al., 2007) or even an inverse relationship between
4
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 18 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
higher education and concern about climate change (e.g., Malka et al., 2009; Slimak &
Dietz, 2006). Results are equally inconsistent for age, with some studies revealing a small
negative correlation between (older) age and global warming risk perception (Heath &
Gifford, 2006; Hornsey et al., 2016; Kellstedt et al., 2008; Malka et al., 2009; Milfont, 2012),
whereas others find no significant (Akerlof et al., 2013; O’Connor et al., 1999; Sundblad et
al., 2007) or a positive correlation (e.g., Slimak & Dietz, 2006).
It has been hypothesized that individuals with higher income and resources might have
an increased sense of perceived control and thus view themselves as less vulnerable to
the impacts of climate change. Yet, evidence for this hypothesis is also quite mixed, as
the impact of income on risk perception appears marginal (cf. Akerlof et al., 2013; Hornsey
et al., 2016; Kellstedt et al., 2008; Malka et al., 2009; Milfont, 2012; Smith & Leiserowitz, 2012).
The influence of religion also appears limited (Kellstedt et al., 2008; Milfont, 2012; Smith &
Leiserowitz, 2012) with some U.S. studies finding a small negative effect (Clements, Xiao,
& McCright, 2014; Hamilton & Keim, 2009; Smith & Leiserowitz, 2013) while little is
currently known about non-Christian denominations. One reason for these inconsistencies
is that cognitive, affective, social, and cultural influences generally trump or mediate
much of the initial effect of socio-demographic characteristics on risk perception (e.g.,
see Akerlof et al., 2013; Dietz et al., 1998; Leiserowitz, 2006; van der Linden, 2015A).
Accordingly, most studies generally reveal weak direct effects, with socio-demographics
typically explaining only a small amount of the (unique) variance in global warming risk
perception (Hornsey et al., 2016; Slimak & Dietz, 2006; van der Linden, 2015A).
Nonetheless, some stable patterns have emerged for at least three factors in particular,
namely gender, race, and political ideology. To start with the latter, one robust finding is
that both political ideology (Liberal vs. Conservative) and political identity (Republican
vs. Democrat) consistently predict global warming risk perception, with Conservatives
and Republicans expressing systematically less concern about climate change than
Liberals and Democrats (Dunlap & McCright, 2008; Hamilton, 2011; Hornsey et al., 2016;
Leiserowitz, 2006; McCright & Dunlap, 2011B).
In the United States, this trend is part of a larger growing political divide on
environmental issues (McCright, Xiao, & Dunlap, 2014), which is often thought to be driven
by “party sorting,” a theory which suggests that political party activists fuel a process of
conflict between political elites, which then leads to party sorting within the general
public (McCright & Dunlap, 2011A). Political ideology has also shown to interact with other
factors in shaping risk perceptions of climate change, including knowledge, media
attention, and (lower) trust in climate science (Malka et al., 2009; McCright & Dunlap,
2011B; Leiserowitz et al., 2013). Yet, how important are political views outside of the U.S.
context? Although political ideology has also shown to play some role in driving concern
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 19 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
in many European countries (e.g., McCright, Dunlap, & Marquart-Pyatt, 2016; van der
Linden, 2015A), much less is known about the importance of political beliefs in shaping risk
perception in the rest of the world (Lee et al., 2015).
Another relatively stable finding in the risk perception literature is known as the “white-
male” effect (Finucane et al., 2000), which refers to the finding that compared to white
females and ethnic minorities of both genders, white (conservative) males are generally
less concerned about a wide range of risks, including climate change (McCright &
Dunlap, 2011B). Indeed, studies show that females and nonwhites are generally more
worried about climate change than white males (Bord & O’Connor, 1997; Brody et al., 2008;
Hornsey et al., 2016; Leiserowitz, 2006; Malka et al., 2009; McCright, 2010; O’Connor et al.,
1999; van der Linden, 2015A). In fact, political polarization is also less pronounced among
nonwhites (Schuldt & Pearson, 2016). Aside from any cultural differences, racial minorities
are often thought to have higher risk perceptions because of their increased vulnerability
to negative environmental impacts, stress, and hardship (Mohai & Bryant, 1998; Vaughan
& Nordenstam, 1991). Yet, at the same time, the interaction between race and gender is
complicated, as some studies show that levels of concern between nonwhite males and
females are generally similar, which renders any biological explanations for the “white
male” effect rather unlikely (Flynn, Slovic, & Mertz, 1994).
Yet, strong evidence for gender socialization theories has also proven elusive. Competing
theories include the “Institutional Trust Hypothesis,” the “Social Roles” and the “Safety
Concern Hypothesis” (Davidson & Freudenburg, 1996). Whereas the first theory posits that
females are generally less trusting and place less confidence in technology and
institutions, the latter two suggest that social roles, such as nurturing and caregiving,
might lead to higher health and safety concerns among females. Although studies have
found some evidence for the Safety Concern Hypothesis (Davidson & Freudenburg, 1996;
Xiao & McCright, 2012), less evidence is found for the theory that females are less trustful
of institutions or that the performance of different societal roles account for gender
differences (Cutter et al., 1992; Xiao & McCright, 2012, 2013).
In short, although results vary, there is some evidence for a socio-demographic “risk
profile” where typically younger, female, higher educated, politically liberal, and racial
minorities express more concern about climate change. Yet, socio-demographics are often
included in models of risk perception without much theorizing as to what their conceptual
relevance is (Dietz et al., 1998). Much like the growing literature examining the interaction
between gender, race, and ideology, climate risk scholars are advised to constructively
add to the literature by more clearly explicating theoretical motivations to include socio-
demographic factors, as opposed to merely reporting on their “statistical significance” (or
lack thereof).
5
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 20 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Heuristics and Biases
In addition to cognitive, experiential, socio-cultural, and demographic factors, a number
of key heuristics and biases have also shown to influence climate change risk judgments
in predictable ways. Although the phrase “heuristics and biases” has come to have a
rather negative connotation, I should stress here that reliance on evolved cognitive
shortcuts (“heuristics”) can often be adaptive and lead to more accurate judgments than
more “rational” or deliberative processes (Gigerenzer & Brighton, 2009). However, when
there is a clear mismatch between the environment in which such heuristics evolved and
their application in modern (“global”) contexts, this can cause people to misperceive or
underestimate the risk of climate change in a number of important ways (Gifford, 2011;
van Vugt et al., 2014). I will review five heuristics and biases here that have arguably
proven most relevant to understanding how people form risks judgments about global
warming (van der Linden, Maibach, & Leiserowitz, 2015).
Optimism Bias, Judgmental Discounting, and Psychological Distance
Humans are optimistic about the future, which is generally a healthy state of mind. It is
often hypothesized that because humans evolved with a unique awareness of their own
mortality, it is adaptive to be unrealistically optimistic about the future (Varki, 2009). At
the same time, however, optimism bias often leads people to systematically overestimate
the likelihood of positive events while underestimating the probability of experiencing
negative life events (Sharot, 2011; Weinstein, 1989). For example, research across nearly 20
nations has revealed that people generally judge environmental risks and the impacts of
climate change to be much more likely and more serious for other people and places than
for themselves (Gifford et al., 2009; Leiserowitz, 2005; van der Linden, 2015A). Part of this
optimism stems from the fact that people tend to heavily discount uncertain future risks
(e.g., climate change impacts) a process known as “intertemporal discounting” (Berns,
Laibson, & Loewenstein, 2007). To some extent, temporal discounting is a natural by-
product of the way in which human psychology evolved; day to day concerns often take
precedent over planning for the future (van Vugt et al., 2014). Accordingly, people
mentally construe future risks differently from those in the present (Trope & Liberman,
2010), particularly as temporal distance increases, mental representations of risks tend to
become less concrete and increasingly abstract. This process is generally referred to as
the “psychological distance” of climate change (Spence, Poortinga, & Pidgeon, 2012). In
other words, people often underestimate the extent to which climate change is a serious
6
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 21 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
personal risk, believing it is more likely to happen in the distant future to other people in
other places.
The Local Warming Effect
It is difficult for people to detect global environmental change on a purely perceptual or
sensory level (Pawlik, 1991). Accordingly, for everyday survival, it made good sense for
humans to rely on daily and local weather patterns. Yet, the use of variation in local
weather as a heuristic for climate change is a form of “attribution-substitution,” that is,
individuals rely on simple available information, such as daily temperature, to make
judgments about a more complex and less accessible phenomenon, such as global
warming (Zaval et al., 2014). Accordingly, much research has shown that people are more
concerned about global warming on hot days than on cold days and when exposed to so-
called “heat primes” (Joireman et al., 2010; Lewandowski, Ciarocco, & Gately, 2012; Li,
Johnson, & Zaval, 2011; Risen & Critcher, 2011; Zaval et al., 2014; Schuldt & Roh, 2014). The
problem is that due to the high variation in short-term weather, the local warming
heuristic is an unstable inference tool for forming risk judgments about global warming,
with less concern on cold days and more concern on warmer days. Importantly, recent
research has indicated that the local warming effect may be eliminated by prompting
people to think about trends rather than current or ambient temperature (Druckman,
2015).
The Consensus-Heuristic: Perceived Scientific Agreement
Consensus describes the collective judgment of a group of individuals, such as experts.
People tend to rely on consensus cues when making judgments about social and political
issues (Mutz, 1998; Panagopoulos & Harrison, 2016; van der Linden, Clarke, & Maibach,
2015; van der Linden et al., 2017). In fact, in a complex and uncertain world, relying on
consensus cues is often adaptive because it reduces the cost of individual learning by
harnessing the “wisdom of the crowd” (Surowiecki, 2004), which is most pronounced
among experts, such as climate scientists (Maibach & van der Linden, 2016). Indeed, in
contrast to relying on the opinion of a single expert, people generally prefer to take cues
from the combined judgment of multiple experts (Mannes, Soll, & Larrick, 2014).
Accordingly, in light of the strong scientific consensus on human-caused climate change
(Cook et al., 2016), a growing body of research has found that the public’s perception of
the degree of scientific consensus acts as a so-called “gateway cognition”: influencing
“key” beliefs about climate change, including concern about global warming (Ding et al.,
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 22 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
2011; Hornsey et al., 2016; Malka, Krosnick, & Langer, 2009; McCright et al., 2013; van der
Linden et al., 2015). Moreover, while widespread public misperceptions of the degree of
scientific consensus dampen concern about global warming, recent research has found
that conveying the scientific consensus on human-caused climate change can increase
acceptance of and concern about climate change across the ideological spectrum (e.g.,
Lewandowsky et al., 2013; van der Linden et al., 2015, 2017).
System Justification and Motivated Science Denial
Although people are often biased in favor of the status quo, system justification theory
suggests that some people will not only defend and justify the status quo but also adopt
motivated perceptions to view the current system as stable, fair, just, and legitimate even
when the system may be disadvantageous to others (Jost & Hunyady, 2005). Systematic
justification is distinct from, but related to, free-market ideologies and political
conservatism (Jost et al., 2003). Because global warming and associated mitigation policies
strongly threaten the status quo, much research, particularly in the United States, has
shown that system justification, strong endorsement of free-market capitalism, and
conservatism predict motivated cognitions that result in reduced concern and widespread
climate change denial (Dunlap, 2013; Feinberg & Willer, 2010; Feygina, Jost, & Goldsmith,
2010; Heath & Gifford, 2006; Lewandowsky et al., 2013; van der Linden, 2015B).
Finite Pool of Worry
At the end of the day, people can only worry about so many things at the same time. In an
experimental study with Argentine farmers, Hansen et al. (2004) show that increasing
concern for one political risk (e.g., terrorism), typically reduces concern about another
societal risk (e.g., global warming) even although objectively, the nature of the risk has
not changed. Moreover, worry is often a draining emotional process. The cost of worry is
therefore likely cumulative so that the more people worry about an issue, the longer it
takes to regenerate (Marx et al., 2007). Unfortunately, many studies show that in light of
issues such as national security, the economy, health care, and other ecological issues
such as water scarcity, global warming generally remains a low priority for most people,
consistently occupying the lower ranks of the finite “pool of worry” (Leiserowitz, 2007;
Lorenzoni & Pidgeon, 2006; Nisbet & Myers, 2007; Motel, 2014).
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 23 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Measuring Public Risk Perception of Climate
Change
Risk perception is a multidimensional construct (Slovic, Fischhoff, & Lichtenstein, 1982)
and accordingly, a wide range of different items have been used to tap into and measure
how the general public perceives the risk of global warming. For example, some studies
have used “perceived seriousness” as an indicator of risk perception, whereas others
have asked how “concerned” the public is in general about the issue, how likely various
climate change impacts are to occur on varying timescales, how much people personally
“worry” about climate change while still others use a combination of all or some of these
measures (c.f., Akerlof et al., 2013; Brody et al., 2008; Ding et al., 2011; Hidalgo & Pisano,
2010; Malka et al., 2009; McCright, 2010; Milfont, 2012; Kellstedt et al., 2008; Leiserowitz, 2006;
Li et al., 2011; O’Connor et al., 1999; Spence et al., 2012; Sundblad et al., 2007; van der Linden,
2015A).
This notable lack of consensus on how to measure and operationalize a complex and
multidimensional construct such as global warming risk perception creates two main
challenges for scholars and practitioners. First, it is difficult to systematically quantify
how differences in risk perception measurement influence the observed relationship
between the various cognitive, experiential, socio-cultural, and demographic factors that
predict concern about climate change. For example, if it were the case that most risk
perception measures are highly correlated with each other (and thus “tap” into the same
latent “risk perception” factor), we would expect that differences in measurement would
not bear much on the relationship between the dependent and independent variables.
However, empirically, this is often not the case, as correlated measures can still
differentially relate to their predictors (Van Liere & Dunlap, 1981). Second, it is unclear
how different conceptualizations of concern subsequently relate to behavioral responses,
such as support for climate change adaptation and mitigation policies. What is known,
however, is that not all risk perception measures are created equal (van der Linden,
2014B) and indices that combine and tap into the various temporal, spatial, cognitive, and
affective bases of climate change concern are generally more reliable than single-items.
Particularly, because on average, multi-item measures cancel out item-specific variance
and measurement error (Epstein, 1983).
7
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 24 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
The Hierarchy of Concern (HoC) Model
To further elaborate on the observation that not all measures of risk perception are
created equal, I have developed a “hierarchy of concern” model that should help inform
future risk perception research (Figure 2). For example, there is a particularly notable
difference between generalized concern for an issue and personal worry. Worry is an
active emotional state that is often closely linked to adaptive behavioral responses aimed
at reducing a particular threat, whereas broad concern is not and can be expressed
without any particular motivational or emotional content (Leiserowitz, 2007; Smith &
Leiserowitz, 2014; van der Linden, 2014B). In fact, a logical “hierarchy of concern” can be
construed using similar reasoning. In short, an individual may think that climate change
(and associated impacts) are likely to occur, but that doesn’t mean that someone also
perceives climate change to be a serious issue. In turn, an individual can perceive climate
change to be a serious issue, but that doesn’t necessarily imply that they are concerned
about it. Finally, although the public may express generalized concern about climate
change, this often does not mean that people also personally worry about the issue or
think it is a high priority (Leiserowitz, 2007; Lorenzoni & Pidgeon, 2006; Nisbet & Myers,
2007; Motel, 2014).
In other words, concern may be a necessary but not sufficient condition for worry and
perceived seriousness and likelihood ratings are in turn components of generalized
concern (Levy & Guttman, 1976). Although public perception may of course not perfectly
abide by such a transitive axiom (likelihood < perceived seriousness < concern <
personal worry), it is a useful heuristic that can help guide researchers conceptualize
measures of risk perception.
Another key finding that
has been neglected in the
literature concerns the
distinction between
“societal” (i.e., other-
regarding) and “personal”-
level risk judgments (Tyler
& Cook, 1984). People are
generally optimistically
biased in the sense that
they believe that global
warming is a serious concern for others, society at-large, and non-human nature, whereas
personal concern and worry is typically much lower (e.g., Leiserowitz, 2005; van der
Click to view larger
Figure 2. “Hierarchy of Concern” (HoC).
8
9
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 25 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Linden, 2015A). A direct consequence of this conceptual distinction is that risk perception
measures which solely rely on people’s “global” or “societal” level risk judgments are
likely to seriously overestimate public concern. Although this distinction has been
implicitly acknowledged (Bord et al., 2000; Leiserowitz, 2005; Sjöberg, 2012), it has received
scant attention in the literature. In one study, van der Linden (2015A) provides empirical
support for the two-dimensional factor structure of risk perception and further shows
that each dimension may have different antecedents. For example, generalized
knowledge influenced societal but not personal-level risk judgments.
Accordingly, to help guide future research, I have delineated a three-step process of risk
perception measurement (Figure 3). Essentially, any multidimensional risk perception
scale should include both “global” societal-level as well as “personal”-level risk
judgments of global warming in order for researchers to be able to meaningfully compare
and differentiate the two. These two broad dimensions can in turn be broken out by the
different ways in which risk perception can be conceptualized, including likelihood
estimates, measures of perceived seriousness, generalized concern, and personal worry.
Of course, it may not always be feasible for risk scholars to construct multidimensional
risk scales that include a wide range of risk perception measures. For example, opinion
polls typically include single-items asking whether people broadly think that climate
change is a serious issue. Yet, Figure 3 would suggest that the next question to ask is: a
serious issue to whom? Sometimes the purpose of the research might come with practical
restrictions on how many questions or items can be included in a given survey. If the goal
of the research is to describe public opinion, then perceived seriousness or generalized
concern may both be appropriate measures. However, if the goal of the research is to
understand how concern about climate change relates to behavior or policy-support, then
personal worry might be a better indicator to use (e.g., Smith & Leiserowitz, 2014).
Similarly, if researchers only wish to tap into cognitive dimensions of risk, then perceived
“likelihood” is probably a better measure to use than personal “worry.” Nonetheless,
single-item measures are now generally discouraged (Epstein, 1983; Roser-Renouf &
Nisbet, 2008) and more careful consideration of personal vs. other-regarding risk
judgments, the inclusion of multiple items, and how they map onto specific research
questions will likely help improve and standardize future risk perception research.
10
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 26 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Risk Perception, Behavior Change, and
Support for Adaptation and Mitigation Policies
Although people are generally aware of and broadly concerned about the issue of climate
change, scholars have repeatedly noted that deeper behavioral engagement is often still
lacking (e.g., Whitmarsh, Lorenzoni, & O’Neill, 2012; van der Linden, 2014B). Yet, in
contrast to the rich literature on pro-environmental attitudes and behavior, much less is
known about the relationship between public concern about climate change and people’s
intentions and behaviors to address the issue. Having said this, evidence has increased
over the last decade, revealing a clear but inconsistent link between different measures
of climate change concern on one hand, and individual behaviors and support for
adaptation and mitigation policies, on the other. This discrepancy can be explained by
what I will refer to as the “measurement paradox” (Figure 4).
On one hand, studies find that public concern about climate change is broadly related to
adaptation and mitigation measures in consistent and important ways. For example,
Smith and Leiserowitz (2014) find that worry about climate change is one of the strongest
predictors of global warming policy support, such as regulating CO emissions, signing
international treaties, and increasing taxes on gasoline. Similarly, Brody, Grover, and
Vedlitz (2012), O’Connor et al. (1999), and Krosnick et al. (2006) all find that climate change
risk perceptions are predictive of general intentions to implement individual behavior
changes and/or broad policy-support to address the issue. Spence, Poortinga, Butler, and
Pidgeon (2011) also find that concern about climate change influences broad preparedness
to reduce energy use. More generally, there are numerous studies that find robust
Click to view larger
Figure 3. Three-step process of measuring and
operationalizing risk perception.
2
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 27 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
evidence for an association between risk perception, broad intentions to address climate
change, and self-reported policy support (e.g., see also Dietz, Dan, & Shwom, 2007; Ding et
al., 2011; Heath & Gifford, 2006; Hidalgo & Pisano, 2010; McCright et al., 2013; Semenza et al.,
2008; van der Linden et al., 2015; Zahran et al., 2006).
Yet, on the other hand, robust evidence for a significant link between risk perceptions of
climate change and specific behavioral actions is much less consistent. For example,
although it is often hypothesized that people might be more willing to implement
adaptation measures due to their greater personal relevance (e.g., Helgeson et al., 2012),
reviews of the role of climate change risk perception in decisions to purchase flooding
insurance (or other protective behaviors) suggest that the relationship is extremely weak
with most studies finding no effect (Bubeck, Botzen, & Aerts, 2012; Kreibich, 2011).
Similarly, in their meta-analysis, Hornsey et al. (2016) find that although climate beliefs
share moderate effect-sizes with broad support for climate policies and behavioral
intentions, the association between climate perceptions and more specific pro-
environmental behaviors is much weaker (about half the magnitude).
Part of the explanation for this discrepancy is rooted in the well-known “gap” between
stated intentions/concern and actual behavior (Sheeran, 2002) but also in an inherent
“measurement paradox.” In particular, there is a notable lack of studies specifically
exploring the role of risk perception in actual adaptation, mitigation, and voting decisions
and behaviors, either self-reported or observed. This is important because of the
conceptual relationship (or lack thereof) that researchers hypothesize between risk
perception and behavior. For example, how are risk perceptions of global warming
conceptually related to climate-friendly behaviors? Whitmarsh (2009) notes that many
specific energy conservation behaviors are generally not performed “out of concern” for
climate change. This makes sense; many personal behaviors, such as running the
dishwasher, changing light bulbs, or even decisions to purchase a fuel-efficient car are
probably driven by considerations specific to those behaviors and contextual
circumstances. For example, in a national study investigating over 20 (low- and high-cost)
climate-friendly behaviors, van der Linden (2016B) developed a causal model of climate
change mitigation behavior known as the Domain-Context-Behavior (DCB) model. The
DCB model reveals that specific actions that help reduce climate change are best
predicted by the specific attitudes, perceptions, beliefs, and barriers that are associated
with performing those behaviors.
The logic of the model is based on the notion of “measurement correspondence” (Ajzen &
Fishbein, 1977)—a principle which suggests that predictors of behavior (e.g., risk
perception) should be operationalized at the same level of specificity as the behavior
being predicted (e.g., purchasing green energy). For example, whether an individual
11
12
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 28 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
purchases green energy is probably best predicted by behavior-specific determinants,
such as that individual’s particular attitude toward or available resources to purchase
green energy. Nonetheless, van der Linden (2016B) shows that more distal predictors
such as people’s concern about climate change still play an important role by shaping a
general orienting intention to help curb climate change, which, when activated by a
specific decision-context, can in turn influence behavior-specific determinants to act
environmentally-friendly (van der Linden, 2016B).
The paradox arises from the fact that because many individual actions are often predicted
by the “power of the situation” (Nisbett & Ross, 1991), one way to try to relate a broad
construct such as risk perception to a specific behavioral measure, is by creating an
aggregate index of behavior and policy-support to help equalize the level of specificity
between the predictor and criterion. Aggregation has the desirable property of canceling
out situation-specific variance between different behaviors (Epstein, 1983; Weigel &
Newman, 1976). This allows researchers to examine common variation between public
concern about climate change and a broad range of behaviors. A drawback of this
approach is that it confounds the differential relationship that each individual behavior or
policy-item in the scale bears in relation to the model’s predictors (Van Liere & Dunlap,
1981; Roser-Renouf & Nisbet, 2008). Although this paradox is unlikely to be resolved, risk
researchers would benefit from being (more) mindful of this trade-off.
Nonetheless, a large body of research has established that public risk perception and
concern do consistently co-vary with “good intentions” and broad-stroke policy support.
Yet, at the same time, much less is known about how and in what ways people’s concern
about climate change drives them to adopt specific behaviors or vote for specific policies
in specific situations. In order to learn more about the complex relationship that public
risk perception plays in driving public engagement with climate change, future research
would benefit from more specific investigations, including examining the role of risk
perception in driving real-world adaptation and mitigation behaviors and decisions. In
addition, in order to not overestimate the relationship between concern about climate
change and behavior, researchers should explore the magnitude of the associations when
controlling for other key motivational factors that can be expected to influence specific
low-carbon behaviors and support for climate policy. As illustrated in Figure 4, this will
also further help evaluate whether risk perception primarily acts as a direct or indirect
driver of climate change response behaviors. Although the study of risk perception is
important in its own right, the field would benefit from becoming more decision-focused
(Arvai, 2014) and to this end, there is much work left to be done in terms of exploring risk-
behavior relations.
13
14
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 29 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Conclusion
Public risk perceptions of climate change are complex and influenced by a multitude of
cognitive, affective, social, cultural, and socio-demographic factors. Overall, experiential
and socio-cultural factors are most influential in driving public risk perceptions of climate
change with negative affect being one of the strongest determinants. Much of the
evidence comes from Western countries, however, and more research is needed from
other parts of the world. There is a notable inconsistency in the measures used to assess
public risk perception, which makes standardized comparisons difficult. Although public
concern is widespread and most people around the world view climate change as a
serious issue, personal worry is typically much lower. Research also shows that the way
in which people judge the risk of climate change for themselves and others frequently
diverge. Overall, while measures of risk perception have shown to influence self-reported
policy-support and general intentions to change behavior, the link between concern about
climate change and real-world adaptation and mitigation decisions remains less clear.
Suggested Readings
Leiserowitz, A. (2006). Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: The role of
affect, imagery, and values. Climatic Change, 77(1–2), 45–72.
van der Linden, S. (2015). The social-psychological determinants of climate change risk
perceptions: Towards a comprehensive model. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 41,
112–124.
Weber, E. U. (2010). What shapes perceptions of climate change? Wiley Interdisciplinary
Reviews: Climate Change, 1(3), 332–342.
Click to view larger
Figure 4. The conceptual relationship between risk
perception and behavior prediction.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 30 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
References
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and
review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84(5), 888–918.
Akerlof, K., Maibach, E. W., Fitzgerald, D., Cedeno, A. Y., & Neuman, A. (2013). Do
people “personally experience” global warming, and if so how, does it matter? Global
Environmental Change, 23(1), 81–91.
Anderegg, W. R., Prall, J. W., Harold, J., & Schneider, S. H. (2010). Expert credibility in
climate change. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(27), 12,107–
12,109.
Arvai, J. (2014). The end of risk communication as we know it. Journal of Risk Research,
17(10), 1,245–1,249.
Berkman, E. T., & Falk, E. B. (2013). Beyond brain mapping: Using neural measures to
predict real-world outcomes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(1), 45–50.
Berns, G. S., Laibson, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2007). Intertemporal choice–toward an
integrative framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(11), 482–488.
Boholm, Å. (1996). Risk perception and social anthropology: Critique of cultural theory.
Ethnos, 61(1–2), 64–84.
Boholm, A. (2015). Risk perception and social anthropology: The contribution from
cultural theory. In A. Boholm (Ed.), Anthropology and Risk (pp. 57–73). New York:
Routledge.
Bolsen, T., Druckman, J. N., & Cook, F L. (2014). The influence of partisan motivated
reasoning on public opinion. Political Behavior, 36(2), 235–262.
Bord, R. J., Fisher, A., & Robert, E. O. (1998). Public perceptions of global warming:
United States and international perspectives. Climate Research, 11(1), 75–84.
Bord, R. J., & O’Connor, R. E. (1997). The gender gap in environmental attitudes: The
case of perceived vulnerability to risk. Social Science Quarterly, 78(4), 830–840.
Bord, R. J., O’Connor, R. E., & Fisher, A. (2000). In what sense does the public need to
understand global climate change? Public Understanding of Science, 9(3), 205–218.
Bostrom, A., Morgan, M. G., Fischhoff, B., & Read, D. (1994). What do people know about
global climate change? Mental models. Risk Analysis, 14(6), 959–970.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 31 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Breakwell, G. M. (2010). Models of risk construction: Some applications to climate
change. WIREs: Climate Change, 1(6), 857–870.
Brechin, S. R. (2003). Comparative public opinion and knowledge on global climatic
change and the Kyoto Protocol: The U.S. versus the world? International Journal of
Sociology and Social Policy, 23(10), 106–134.
Brechin, S. R., & Bhandari, M. (2011). Perceptions of climate change worldwide. Wiley
Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 2(6), 871–885.
Brody, S., Grover, H., & Vedlitz, A. (2012). Examining the willingness of Americans to
alter behavior to mitigate climate change. Climate Policy, 12(1), 1–22.
Brody, S. D., Zahran, S., Vedlitz, A., & Grover, H. (2008). Examining the relationship
between physical vulnerability and public perceptions of global climate change in the
United States. Environment and Behavior, 41(1), 72–95.
Brooks, J., Oxley, D., Vedlitz, A., Zahran, S., & Lindsey, C. (2014). Abnormal daily
temperature and concern about climate change across the United States. Review of
Policy Research, 31, 199–217.
Brulle, R. J., Carmichael, J., & Jenkins, J. C. (2012). Shifting public opinion on climate
change: An empirical assessment of factors influencing concern over climate change in
the U.S., 2002–2010. Climatic Change, 114, 169–188.
Bubeck, P., Botzen, W. J., & Aerts, J. C. (2012). A review of risk perceptions and other
factors that influence flood mitigation behavior. Risk Analysis, 32(9), 1481–1495.
Butts, C. T. (2016). Why I know but don’t believe. Science, 354(6310), 286–287.
Capstick, S., Whitmarsh, L., Poortinga, W., Pidgeon, N., & Upham, P. (2015).
International trends in public perceptions of climate change over the past quarter
century. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 6(1), 35–61.
Capstick, S. B., & Pidgeon, N. F. (2014). Public perception of cold weather for and
against climate change. Climatic Change, 122(4), 695–708.
Carey, S. (1986). Cognitive science and science education. American Psychologist, 41(10),
1123–1130.
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York:
Guilford Press.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 32 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Cialdini, R. B., Kallgren, C. A., & Reno, R. R. (1991). A focus theory of normative conduct.
Advances in Experimental Psychology, 24, 201–234.
Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity.
Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591–621.
Clements, J. M., Xiao, C., & McCright, A. M. (2014). An examination of the “greening of
Christianity” thesis among Americans, 1993–2010. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 53(2), 373–391.
Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (2000). Cognition in emotion: Always, sometimes or never? In L.
Nadel, R. Lanc, & G. L. Ahern (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 24–61).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Cody, E. M., Reagan, A. J., Mitchell, L., Dodds, P. S., & Danforth, C. M. (2015). Climate
change sentiment on Twitter: An unsolicited public opinion poll. PloS ONE, 10(8),
e0136092.
Cook, J., Oreskes, N., Doran, P. T., Anderegg, W. R., Verheggen, B., Maibach, E. W., et al.
(2016). Consensus on consensus: A synthesis of consensus estimates on human-caused
global warming. Environmental Research Letters, 11(4), 048002.
Corner, A., Markowitz, E., & Pidgeon, N. F. (2014). Public engagement with climate
change: The role of human values. WIREs Climate Change, 5(3), 411–422.
Cutter, S. L., Tiefenbacher, J., & Solecki, W. D. (1992). En-gendered fears: Femininity and
technological risk perception. Organization & Environment, 6(1), 5–22.
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New
York: Grosset/Putnam.
Dake, K. (1991). Orienting dispositions in the perception of risk an analysis of
contemporary worldviews and cultural biases. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
22(1), 61–82.
Dake, K. (1992). Myths of nature: Culture and the social construction of risk. Journal of
Social Issues, 48(4), 21–37.
Davidson, D. J., & Freudenburg, W. R. (1996). Gender and environmental risk concerns a
review and analysis of available research. Environment and Behavior, 28(3), 302–339.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 33 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
De Groot, J. I. M., & Steg, L. (2007). Value orientations and environmental beliefs in five
countries: Validity of an instrument to measure egoistic, altruistic, and biospheric value
orientations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(3), 318–332.
De Groot, J. I. M., & Steg, L. (2008). Value orientations to explain environmental attitudes
and beliefs: How to measure egoistic, altruistic, and biospheric value orientations.
Environment and Behavior, 40(3), 330–354.
De Groot, J. I. M., & Steg, L. (2010). Relationships between value orientations, self-
determined motivational types, and pro-environmental behavioral intentions. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 30(4), 368–378.
De Groot, J. I. M., Steg, L., & Poortinga, W. (2013). Values, perceived risks and benefits,
and acceptability of nuclear energy. Risk Analysis, 33(2), 307–317.
Dietz, T., Dan, A., & Shwom, R. (2007). Support for climate change policy: Social
psychological and social structural influences. Rural Sociology, 72(2), 185–214.
Dietz, T., Stern, P. C., & Guagnano, G. A. (1998). Social structural and social
psychological bases of environmental concern. Environment and Behavior, 30(4), 450–
471.
Ding, D., Maibach, E. W., Zhao, X., Roser-Renouf, C., & Leiserowitz, A. (2011). Support
for climate policy and societal action are linked to perceptions about scientific
agreement. Nature Climate Change, 1(9), 462–466.
Doherty, K. L., & Webler, T. N. (2016). Social norms and efficacy beliefs drive the
Alarmed segment’s public-sphere climate actions. Nature Climate Change, 6, 879–884.
Douglas, M. (1970). Natural symbols: Explorations in cosmology. London, England: Barrie
and Rockliff.
Douglas, M. (1978). Cultural bias (Occasional Paper no. 35). London: Royal
Anthropological Institute. (Reprinted in: M. Douglas (1982). In the active voice (pp.183–
254). London: Routledge.)
Douglas, M., & Wildavsky, A. B. (1982). Risk and culture: An essay on the selection of
technical and environmental dangers. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Druckman, J. N. (2015). Eliminating the local warming effect. Nature Climate Change,
5(3), 176–177.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 34 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Dunlap, R. E. (2013). Climate change skepticism and denial: An introduction. American
Behavioral Scientist, 57(6), 691–698.
Dunlap, R. E., & McCright, A. M. (2008). A widening gap: Republican and Democratic
views on climate change. Environment: Science and Policy for Sustainable Development,
50(5), 26–35.
Egan, P. J., & Mullin, M. (2012). Turning personal experience into political attitudes: The
effect of local weather on Americans’ perceptions about global warming. The Journal of
Politics, 74, 796–809.
Epstein, S. (1983). Aggregation and beyond: Some basic issues on the prediction of
behavior. Journal of Personality, 51(3), 360–392.
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious.
American Psychologist, 49(8), 709–724.
Feinberg, M., & Willer, R. (2010). Apocalypse soon? Dire messages reduce belief in global
warming by contradicting just-world beliefs. Psychological Science, 22(1), 34–38.
Feygina, I., Jost, J. T., & Goldsmith, R. E. (2010). System justification, the denial of global
warming, and the possibility of “system-sanctioned change.” Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 36(3), 326–338.
Finucane, M. (2012). The role of feelings in perceived risk. In S. Roeser, R. Hillerbrand,
P. Sandin, & M. Peterson (Eds.), Handbook of risk theory (pp. 678–688). Dordrecht, the
Netherlands: Springer.
Finucane, M. L., Slovic, P., Mertz, C. K., Flynn, J., & Satterfield, T. A. (2000). Gender,
race, and perceived risk: The “white male” effect. Health, Risk, & Society, 2(2), 159–172.
Floyd, D. L., Prentice-Dunn, S., & Rogers, R. W. (2000). A meta‐analysis of research on
protection motivation theory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(2), 407–429.
Flynn, J., Slovic, P., & Mertz, C. K. (1994). Gender, race, and perception of environmental
health risks. Risk analysis, 14(6), 1101–1108.
Fremling, G. M., & Lott, J. R. (2003). The surprising finding that “cultural worldviews”
don’t explain people’s views on gun control. University of Pennsylvania Law Review,
151(4), 1341–1348.
Gifford, R. (2011). The dragons of inaction: Psychological barriers that limit climate
change mitigation and adaptation. American Psychologist, 66(4), 290–302.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 35 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Gifford, R., Scannell, L., Kormos, C., Smolova, L., Biel, A., Boncu, S., et al. (2009).
Temporal pessimism and spatial optimism in environmental assessments: An 18-nation
study. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29(1), 1–12.
Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Adaptive thinking: Rationality in the real world. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Gigerenzer, G., & Brighton, H. (2009). Homo heuristicus: Why biased minds make better
inferences. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(1), 107–143.
Goleman, D. (2006). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam.
Guy, S., Kashima, Y., Walker, I., & O’Neill, S. (2014). Investigating the effects of
knowledge and ideology on climate change beliefs. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 44(5), 421–429.
Hamilton, L. C. (2011). Education, politics, and opinions about climate change evidence
for interaction effects. Climatic Change, 104(2), 231–242.
Hamilton, L. C., & Keim, B. D. (2009). Regional variation in perceptions about climate
change. International Journal of Climatology, 29, 2348–2352.
Hansen, J., Marx, S., & Weber, E. U. (2004). The role of climate perceptions,
expectations, and forecasts in farmer decision making: The Argentine Pampas and South
Florida (IRI Technical Report 04-01). Palisades, NY: International Research Institute for
Climate Prediction.
Hart, P. S., & Nisbet, E. C. (2012). Boomerang effects in science communication: How
motivated reasoning and identity cues amplify opinion polarization about climate
mitigation policies. Communication Research, 39(6), 701–723.
Heath, Y., & Gifford, R. (2006). Free-market ideology and environmental degradation the
case of belief in global climate change. Environment and Behavior, 38(1), 48–71.
Helgeson, J., van der Linden, S., & Chabay, I. (2012). The role of knowledge, learning,
and mental models in public perceptions of climate change related risks. In. A. Wals & P.
B. Corcoran (Eds.), Learning for sustainability in times of accelerating change (pp. 329–
346). Wageningen, The Netherlands: Wageningen Academic Publishers.
Hidalgo, M. C., & Pisano, I. (2010). Determinants of risk perception and willingness to
tackle climate change. A pilot study. Psyecology: Bilingual Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 1(1), 105–112.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 36 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Hine, D. W., Reser, J. P., Phillips, W. J., Cooksey, R., Marks, A. D., Nunn, P., et al. (2013).
Identifying climate change interpretive communities in a large Australian sample. Journal
of Environmental Psychology, 36, 229–239.
Hofstede, G. H. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors,
institutions, and organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Hornsey, M. J., Harris, E. A., Bain, P. G., & Fielding, K. S. (2016). Meta-analyses of the
determinants and outcomes of belief in climate change. Nature Climate Change, 6,
622–626.
Howe, P., Markowitz, E. M., Ming-Lee, T., Ko, C.-Y., & Leiserowitz, A. (2013). Global
perceptions of local temperature change. Nature Climate Change, 3(4), 352–356.
Howe, P. D., Boudet, H., Leiserowitz, A., & Maibach, E. W. (2014). Mapping the shadow
of experience of extreme weather events. Climatic Change, 127, 381–389.
Howe, P. D., & Leiserowitz, A. (2013). Who remembers a hot summer or a cold winter?
The asymmetric effect of beliefs about global warming on perceptions of local climate
conditions in the U.S. Global Environmental Change, 23, 1,488–1,500.
Howe, P. D., Mildenberger, M., Marlon, J. R., & Leiserowitz, A. (2015). Geographic
variation in opinions on climate change at state and local scales in the USA. Nature
Climate Change, 5(6), 596–603.
Jackson, J., Allum, N., & Gaskell, G. (2006). Bridging levels of analysis in risk perception
research: The case of the fear of crime. Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 7(1), Art. 20.
Joffe, H. (2003). Risk: From perception to social representation. British Journal of Social
Psychology, 42(1), 55–73.
Joireman, J., Truelove, H. B., & Duell, B. (2010). Effect of outdoor temperature, heat
primes, and anchoring on belief in global warming. Journal of Environmental Psychology,
30(4), 358–367.
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as
motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339–375.
Jost, J. T., & Hunyady, O. (2005). Antecedents and consequences of system-justifying
ideologies. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(5), 260–265.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 37 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Kahan, D. (2012). Cultural cognition as a conception of the cultural theory of risk. In S.
Roeser, R. Hillerbrand, P. Sandin, & M. Peterson (Eds.), Handbook of risk theory (pp.
725–759). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.
Kahan, D. M., Peters, E., Wittlin, M., Slovic, P., Ouellette, L. L., Braman, D., & Mandel, G.
(2012). The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived climate
change risks. Nature Climate Change, 2(10), 732–735.
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics
and biases. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
Kaiser, F. G., & Fuhrer, U. (2003). Ecological behavior’s dependency on different forms of
knowledge. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 52, 598–613.
Kasperson, R. E., Renn, O., Slovic, P., Brown, H. S., Emel, J., Goble, R., et al. (1988). The
social amplification of risk: A conceptual framework. Risk Analysis, 8(2), 177–187.
Kearney, A. R., & Kaplan, S. (1997). Toward a methodology for the measurement of
knowledge structures of ordinary people: The conceptual content cognitive map.
Environment and Behavior, 29(5), 579.
Kellstedt, P. M., Zahran, S., & Vedlitz, A. (2008). Personal efficacy, the information
environment, and attitudes toward global warming and climate change in the United
States. Risk Analysis, 28(1), 113–126.
Kempton, W. (1991). Lay perspectives on global climate change. Global Environmental
Change, 1(3), 183–208.
Kim, S. Y., & Wolinsky-Nahmias, Y. (2014). Cross-national public opinion on climate
change: The effects of affluence and vulnerability. Global Environmental Politics, 14(1),
79–106.
Kinzig, A. P., Ehrlich, P. R., Alston, L. J., Arrow, K., Barrett, S., Buchman, T. G., et al.
(2013). Social norms and global environmental challenges: The complex interaction of
behaviors, values, and policy. BioScience, 63(3), 164–175.
Kobbeltved, T., Brun, W., Johnsen, B. H., & Eid, J. (2005). Risk as feelings or risk and
feelings? A cross‐lagged panel analysis. Journal of Risk Research, 8(5), 417–437.
Koltko-Rivera, M. E. (2004). The psychology of worldviews. Review of General
Psychology, 8(1), 3–58.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 38 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Kreibich, H. (2011). Do perceptions of climate change influence precautionary measures?
International Journal of Climate Change Strategies and Management, 3(2), 189–199.
Krosnick, J. A., Holbrook, A. L., Lowe, L., & Visser, P. S. (2006). The origins and
consequences of democratic citizens’ policy agendas: A study of popular concern about
global warming. Climatic Change, 77(1–2), 7–43.
Kruglanski, A. W., & Gigerenzer, G. (2011). Intuitive and deliberate judgments are based
on common principles. Psychological Review, 118(1), 97–109.
Lai, V. T., Hagoort, P., & Casasanto, D. (2012). Affective primacy vs. cognitive primacy:
Dissolving the debate. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 243.
Lazarus, R. S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. American Psychologist, 39(2), 124–
129.
LeDoux, J. E. (1989). Cognitive-emotional interactions in the brain. Cognition & Emotion,
3(4), 267–289.
Lee, T. M., Markowitz, E. M., Howe, P. D., Ko, C.-Y., & Leiserowitz, A. A. (2015).
Predictors of public climate change awareness and risk perception around the world.
Nature Climate Change, 5(11), 1014–1020.
Leiserowitz, A. (2006). Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: The role of
affect, imagery, and values. Climatic Change, 77(1–2), 45–72.
Leiserowitz, A. (2007). International public opinion, perception, and understanding of
global climate change (Human Development Report 2007/2008).
Leiserowitz, A., Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., Feinberg, G., & Rosenthal, S. (2016).
Climate change in the American mind: March, 2016. Yale University and George Mason
University. New Haven, CT: Yale Program on Climate Change Communication.
Leiserowitz, A., Smith, N., & Marlon, J. R. (2010). Americans’ knowledge of climate
change. Yale University, New Haven, CT: Yale Project on Climate Change
Communication.
Leiserowitz, A. A. (2005). American risk perceptions: Is climate change dangerous? Risk
analysis, 25(6), 1,433–1,442.
Leiserowitz, A. A., Maibach, E. W., Roser-Renouf, C., Smith, N., & Dawson, E. (2013).
Climategate, public opinion, and the loss of trust. American Behavioral Scientist, 57(6),
818–837.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 39 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Leviston et al. (2014). Imagining climate change: The role of implicit associations and
affective psychological distancing in climate change responses. European Journal of
Social Psychology, 44(5), 441–454.
Levy, S., & Guttman, L. (1976). Worry, fear, and concern differentiated. Israel Annals of
Psychiatry & Related Disciplines, 14(3), 211–228.
Lewandowski, G. W., Jr., Ciarocco, N. J., & Gately, E. L. (2012). The effect of embodied
temperature on perceptions of global warming. Current Psychology, 31(3), 318–324.
Lewandowsky, S., Gignac, G. E., & Vaughan, S. (2013). The pivotal role of perceived
scientific consensus in acceptance of science. Nature Climate Change, 3(4), 399–404.
Lewandowsky, S., Oberauer, K., & Gignac, G. E. (2013). NASA faked the moon landing—
therefore, (climate) science is a hoax: An anatomy of the motivated rejection of science.
Psychological Science, 24(5), 622–633.
Li, Y., Johnson, E. J., & Zaval, L. (2011). Local warming daily temperature change
influences belief in global warming. Psychological Science, 22(4), 454–459.
Leombruni, L. V. (2015). How you talk about climate change matters: A communication
network perspective on epistemic skepticism and belief strength. Global Environmental
Change, 35, 148–161.
Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, E. (2001). Risk as feelings.
Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 267–286.
Lorenzoni, I., Leiserowitz, A., Doria, M., Poortinga, W., & Pidgeon, N. (2006). Cross-
national comparisons of image associations with “global warming” and “climate change”
among laypeople in the United States of America and Great Britain. Journal of Risk
Research, 9(3), 265–281.
Lorenzoni, I., & Pidgeon, N. F. (2006). Public views on climate change: Europe and USA
perspectives. Climatic Change, 77(1–2), 73–95.
Maibach, E. W., Leiserowitz, A., Roser-Renouf, C., & Mertz, C. K. (2011). Identifying like-
minded audiences for global warming public engagement campaigns: An audience
segmentation analysis and tool development. PloS ONE, 6(3), e17571.
Maibach, E. W., & van der Linden, S. L. (2016). The importance of assessing and
communicating scientific consensus. Environmental Research Letters, 11(9), 091003.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 40 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Malka, A., Krosnick, J. A., & Langer, G. (2009). The association of knowledge with
concern about global warming: Trusted information sources shape public thinking. Risk
Analysis, 9(5), 633–647.
Mannes, A. E., Soll, J. B., & Larrick, R. P. (2014). The wisdom of select crowds. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 107(2), 276.
Marris, C., Langford, I. H., & O’Riordan, T. (1998). A quantitative test of the cultural
theory of risk perceptions: Comparison with the psychometric paradigm. Risk Analysis,
18(5), 635–647.
Marquart-Pyatt, S. T., McCright, A. M., Dietz, T., & Dunlap, R. E. (2014). Politics eclipses
climate extremes for climate change perceptions. Global Environmental Change, 29, 246–
257.
Marx, S. M., Weber, E. U., Orlove B. S., Leiserowitz, A., Krantz, D. H., Roncoli, C., et al.
(2007). Communication and mental processes: Experiential and analytic processing of
uncertain climate information. Global Environmental Change, 17(1), 47–58.
Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50(4), 370–
396.
McCright, A. M. (2010). The effects of gender on climate change knowledge and concern
in the American public. Population and Environment, 32(1), 66–87.
McCright, A. M., & Dunlap, R. E. (2011a). The polarization of climate change and
polarization in the American public’s views of global warming, 2001–2010. The
Sociological Quarterly, 52(2), 155–194.
McCright, A. M., & Dunlap, R. E. (2011b). Cool dudes: The denial of climate change
among conservative white males in the United States. Global Environmental Change,
21(4), 1,163–1,172.
McCright, A. M., Dunlap, R. E., & Marquart-Pyatt, S. T. (2016). Political ideology and
views about climate change in the European Union. Environmental Politics, 25(2), 338–
358.
McCright, A. M., Dunlap, R. E., & Xiao, C. (2013). Perceived scientific agreement and
support for government action on climate change in the USA. Climatic Change, 119(2),
511–518.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 41 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
McCright, A. M., Dunlap, R. E., & Xiao, C. (2014). The impacts of temperature anomalies
and political orientation on perceived winter warming. Nature Climate Change, 4, 1077–
1081.
McCright, A. M., Xiao, C., & Dunlap, R. E. (2014). Political polarization on support for
government spending on environmental protection in the USA, 1974–2012. Social Science
Research, 48, 251–260.
Metag, J., Füchslin, T., & Schäfer, M. S. (2015). Global warming’s five Germanys: A
typology of Germans’ views on climate change and patterns of media use and
information. Public Understanding of Science.
Milfont, T. L. (2012). The interplay between knowledge, perceived efficacy, and concern
about global warming and climate change: A one-year longitudinal study. Risk Analysis,
32(6), 1003–1020.
Milfont, T. L., Milojev, P., Greaves, L. M., & Sibley, C. G. (2015). Socio-structural and
psychological foundations of climate change beliefs. New Zealand Journal of Psychology,
44(1), 18–30.
Mohai, P., & Bryant, B. (1998). Is there a “race” effect on concern for environmental
quality? Public Opinion Quarterly, 62(4), 475–505.
Morgan, M., Fischhoff, B., Bostrom, A., & Atman, C. J. (2002). Risk communication: A
mental models approach. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
Moscovici, S. (1984). The phenomenon of social representations. In R. M. Farr & S.
Moscovici (Eds.), Social representations (pp. 3–69). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge
University Press.
Moser, S. C. (2010). Communicating climate change: History, challenges, process, and
future directions. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 1(1), 31–53.
Moser, S. C. (2016). Reflections on climate change communication research and practice
in the second decade of the 21st century: What more is there to say? Wiley
Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 7(3), 345–369.
Motel, S. (2014). Poll shows most Americans believe in climate change, but give it low
priority [Data set]. Pew Research Center [Distributor]. Retrieved from http://pewrsr.ch/
1DvMLej.
Mutz, D. C. (1998). Impersonal influence: How perceptions of mass collectives affect
political attitudes. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 42 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Myers, T., Maibach, E. W., Roser-Renouf, C., Akerlof, K., & Leiserowitz, A. (2012). The
relationship between personal experience and belief in the reality of global warming.
Nature Climate Change, 3(4), 343–347.
Nisbet, M. C., & Myers, T. (2007). The polls—trends: Twenty years of public opinion
about global warming. Public Opinion Quarterly, 71(3), 444–470.
Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. (1991). The person and the situation. New York: McGraw Hill.
Nisbet, M. C., & Kotcher, J. E. (2009). A two-step flow of influence? Opinion-leader
campaigns on climate change. Science Communication, 30(3), 328–354.
O’Connor, R. E., Bord, R. J., & Fisher, A. (1999). Risk perceptions, general environmental
beliefs, and willingness to address climate change. Risk Analysis, 19, 461–471.
Oltedal, S., Moen, B. E., Klempe, H., & Rundmo, T. (2004). Explaining risk perception: An
evaluation of cultural theory (Rotunde No. 85). Trondheim, Norway: Norwegian
University of Science and Technology, Department of Psychology.
Oreg, S., & Katz-Gerro, T. (2006). Predicting proenvironmental behavior cross-nationally:
Values, the theory of planned behavior, and value-belief-norm theory. Environment and
Behavior, 38(4), 462–483.
Panagopoulos, C., & Harrison, B. (2016). Consensus cues, issue salience, and policy
preferences: An experimental investigation. North American Journal of Psychology, 18(2),
405–417.
Pawlik, K. (1991). The psychology of global environmental change: Some basic data and
an agenda for cooperative international research. International Journal of Psychology,
26(5), 547–563.
Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 9(2), 148–158.
Pidgeon, N. F. (2012). Public understanding of, and attitudes to, climate change: U.K. and
international perspectives. Climate Policy, 12(1), 85–106.
Pidgeon, N., Kasperson, R. E., & Slovic, P. (2003). The social amplification of risk.
Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
Popenoe, D. (1983). Sociology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Powell, J. L. (2016). Climate scientists virtually unanimous anthropogenic global
warming is true. Bulletin of Science, Technology & Society, 35(5–6), 121–124.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 43 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1992). When small effects are impressive. Psychological
Bulletin, 112(1), 160–164.
Price, J. C., Walker, I. A., & Boschetti, F. (2014). Measuring cultural values and beliefs
about environment to identify their role in climate change responses. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 37, 8–20.
Ranney, M. A., & Clark, D. (2016). Climate change conceptual change: Scientific
information can transform attitudes. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(1), 49–75.
Read, D., Bostrom, A., Morgan, M. G., Fischhoff, B., & Smuts, T. (1994). What do people
know about global climate change? Survey studies of educated laypeople. Risk Analysis,
14(6), 971–982.
Renn, O. (2010). The social amplification/attenuation of risk framework: Application to
climate change. WIREs Climate Change, 2(2), 154–169.
Reser, J. P., Bradley, G. L., & Ellul, M. C. (2014). Encountering climate change: “Seeing”
is more than “Believing.” WIREs Climate Change, 5, 521–537.
Reser, J. P., Bradley, G. L., Glendon, A. L., Ellul, M. C., & Callaghan, R. (2012). Public risk
perceptions, understandings, and responses to climate change and natural disasters in
Australia, 2010 and 2011. Queensland, Australia: National Climate Change Adaptation
Research Facility.
Reynolds, T. W., Bostrom, A., Read, D., & Morgan, M. G. (2010). Now what do people
know about global climate change? Survey studies of educated laypeople. Risk Analysis,
30(10), 1520–1538.
Rippl, S. (2002). Cultural theory and risk perception: A proposal for a better
measurement. Journal of Risk Research, 5(2), 147–165.
Risen, J. L., & Critcher, C. R. (2011). Visceral fit: While in a visceral state, associated
states of the world seem more likely. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5),
777–793.
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free Press.
Roeser, S. (2009). The relation between cognition and affect in moral judgments about
risks. In L. Asveld & S. Roeser (Eds.), The ethics of technological risk (pp. 182–201).
London: Earthscan.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 44 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Rosa, E. A. (2003). The logical structure of the social amplification of risk framework
(SARF): Metatheoretical foundations and policy implications. In N. F. Pidgeon, R. E.
Kasperson, & P. Slovic (Eds.), The social amplification of risk (pp. 47–79). Cambridge,
U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
Roser-Renouf, C., & Nisbet, M. C. (2008). The measurement of key behavioral science
constructs in climate change research. International Journal of Sustainability, 3, 37–95.
Scherer, C. W., & Cho, H. (2003). A social network contagion theory of risk perception.
Risk Analysis, 23(2), 261–267.
Schuldt, J. P., Konrath, S. H., & Schwarz, N. (2011). “Global warming” or “climate
change”? Whether the planet is warming depends on question wording. Public Opinion
Quarterly, 75(1), 115–124.
Schuldt, J. P., & Pearson, A. R. (2016). The role of race and ethnicity in climate change
polarization: Evidence from a U.S. national survey experiment. Climatic Change, 136(3–
4), 495–505.
Schuldt, J. P., & Roh, S. (2014). Of accessibility and applicability: How heat-related cues
affect belief in “global warming” versus“ climate change.” Social Cognition, 32(3), 217–
238.
Schultz, W. P. (2001). The structure of environmental concern: Concern for self, other
people, and the biosphere. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21(4), 327–339.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical
advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1–65). New York: Academic Press.
Schwartz, S. H., & Sagiv, L. (1995). Identifying culture-specifics in the content and
structure of values. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26(1), 92–116.
Schwartz, S. H., & Wolfgang, B. (1987). Toward a universal psychological structure of
human values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(3), 550–562.
Semenza, J. C., Hall, D. E., Wilson, D. J., Bontempo, B. D., Sailor, D. J., & George, L. A.
(2008). Public perception of climate change: Voluntary mitigation and barriers to
behavior change. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 35(5), 479–487.
Shao, W., & Goidel, K. (2016). Seeing is believing? An examination of perceptions of
local weather conditions and climate change among residents in the US Gulf
Coast. Risk Analysis, 36(11), 2136–2157.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 45 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Sharot, T. (2011). The optimism bias. Current Biology, 21(23), R941–R945.
Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention—behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review.
European Review of Social Psychology, 12(1), 1–36.
Shi, J., Visschers, V. H., Siegrist, M., & Arvai, J. (2016). Knowledge as a driver of
public perceptions about climate change reassessed. Nature Climate Change, 6,
759–762.
Sjöberg, L. (1997). Explaining risk perception: An empirical evaluation of cultural theory.
Risk Decision and Policy, 2(2), 113–130.
Sjöberg, L. (1998). World views, political attitudes, and risk perception. Risk: Health,
Safety, and Environment, 137(9), 138–152.
Sjöberg, L. (2000a). The methodology of risk perception research. Quality and Quantity,
34(4), 407–418.
Sjöberg, L. (2000b). Factors in risk perception. Risk Analysis, 20(1), 1–12.
Sjöberg, L. (2002). Are received risk perception models alive and well? Risk Analysis,
22(4), 665–669.
Sjöberg, L. (2006). Will the real meaning of affect please stand up? Journal of Risk
Research, 9(2), 101–108.
Sjöberg, L. (2007). Emotions and risk perception. Risk Management, 9(4), 223–237.
Sjöberg, L. (2012). Risk Perception and societal response. In S. Roeser, R. Hillerbrand, P.
Sandin, & M. Peterson (Eds.), Handbook of risk theory (pp. 661–675). Dordrecht, The
Netherlands: Springer.
Slimak, M. W., & Dietz, T. (2006). Personal values, beliefs, and ecological risk perception.
Risk Analysis, 26(6), 1689–1705.
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological
Bulletin, 119(1), 3–22.
Slovic, P. (1987). The perception of risk. Science, 236(4799), 280–285.
Slovic, P. (1992). Perception of risk: Reflections on the psychometric paradigm. In S.
Krimsky & D. Golding (Eds.), Social theories of risk (pp. 117–152). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 46 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk
as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Analysis, 24(2),
311–322.
Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2007). The affect heuristic.
European Journal of Operational Research, 177(3), 1333–1352.
Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., & Lichtenstein, S. (1982). Why study risk perception? Risk
Analysis, 2(2), 83–93.
Slovic, P., & Peters, E. (2006). Risk perception and affect. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 15(6), 322–325.
Smith, N., & Joffe, H. (2013). How the public engages with global warming: A social
representations approach. Public Understanding of Science, 22(1), 16–32.
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2012). The rise of global warming skepticism: Exploring
affective image associations in the United States over time. Risk Analysis, 32(6), 1021–
1032.
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2013). American evangelicals and global warming. Global
Environmental Change, 23(5), 1009–1017.
Smith, N., & Leiserowitz, A. (2014). The role of emotion in global warming policy support
and opposition. Risk Analysis, 34(5), 937–948.
Spence, A., Poortinga, W., Butler, C., & Pidgeon, N. F. (2011). Perceptions of climate
change and willingness to save energy related to flood experience. Nature Climate
Change, 1(1), 46–49.
Spence, A., Poortinga, W., & Pidgeon, N. (2012). The psychological distance of climate
change. Risk Analysis, 32(6), 957–972.
Steg, L., & De Groot, J. I. M. (2012). Environmental values. In S. Clayton (Ed.), The
Oxford handbook of environmental and conservation psychology (pp. 81–92). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Steg, L., De Groot, J. I. M., Dreijerink, L., Abrahamse, W., & Siero, F. (2011). General
antecedents of personal norms, policy acceptability, and intentions: The role of values,
worldviews, and environmental concern. Society and Natural Resources, 24(4), 349–367.
Stern, P. C. (2000). New environmental theories: Toward a coherent theory of
environmentally significant behavior. Journal of Social Issues, 56(3), 407–424
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 47 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Sterman, J. D. (2008). Risk communication on climate: Mental models and mass balance.
Science, 322(5901), 532–533.
Sterman, J. D., & Sweeney, L. B. (2007). Understanding public complacency about
climate change: Adults’ mental models of climate change violate conservation of matter.
Climatic Change, 80(3–4), 213–238.
Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., & Kalof, L. (1993). Value orientations, gender, and environmental
concern. Environment and Behavior, 25(5), 322–348.
Stern, P. C., & Dietz, T. (1994). The value basis of environmental concern. Journal of
Social Issues, 50(3), 65–84.
Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., Abel, T., Guagnano, G. A., & Kalof, L. (1999). A value belief norm
theory of support for social movements: The case of environmental concern. Human
Ecology Review, 6(8), 1–97.
Starr, C. (1969). Social benefit versus technological risk. In T. S. Glickman & M. Gough
(Eds.), Readings in risk (pp. 183–194). New York: Resources for the Future.
Sturgis, P., & Allum, N. (2004). Science in society: Re-evaluating the deficit model of
public attitudes. Public Understanding of Science, 13(1), 55–74.
Sundblad, E. L., Biel, A., & Gärling, T. (2007). Cognitive and affective risk judgments
related to climate change. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 27, 97–106.
Sunstein, C. R. (2007). On the divergent American reactions to terrorism and climate
change. Columbia Law Review, 107(2), 503–557.
Surowiecki, J. (2004). The wisdom of crowds. New York: Anchor.
Swanson, J. (2010). What would Mary Douglas do? A commentary on Kahan et al.,
“Cultural cognition and public policy: The case of outpatient commitment laws.” Law and
Human Behavior, 34(3), 176–185.
Taylor, A., Bruin, W. B., & Dessai, S. (2014). Climate change beliefs and perceptions of
weather-related changes in the United Kingdom. Risk Analysis, 34, 1995–2004.
Thurstone, L. L. (1931). The measurement of social attitudes. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 26(3), 249–269.
Tobler, C., Visschers, V. H. M., & Siegrist, M. (2012). Consumers’ knowledge about
climate change. Climatic Change, 114(2), 189–209.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 48 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Townsend, E., Spence, A., & Knowles, S. (2014). Investigating the operation of the affect
heuristic: Is it an associative construct? Journal of Risk Research, 17(3), 299–315.
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance.
Psychological Review, 117(2), 440–463.
Tyler, T. R., & Cook, F. L. (1984). The mass media and judgments of risk: Distinguishing
impact on personal and societal level judgments. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 47(4), 693–708.
van der Linden, S. (2014a). On the relationship between personal experience, affect, and
risk perception: The case of climate change. European Journal of Social Psychology,
44(5), 430–440.
van der Linden, S. (2014b). Towards a new model for communicating climate change. In
S. Cohen, J. Higham, P. Peeters, & S. Gössling (Eds.), Understanding and governing
sustainable tourism mobility: Psychological and behavioural approaches (pp. 243–275).
London: Routledge.
van der Linden, S. (2015a). The social-psychological determinants of climate change risk
perceptions: Towards a comprehensive model. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 41,
112–124.
van der Linden, S. (2015b). The conspiracy-effect: Exposure to conspiracy theories (about
global warming) decreases pro-social behavior and science acceptance. Personality and
Individual Differences, 87, 171–173.
van der Linden, S. (2016a). A conceptual critique of the cultural cognition thesis. Science
Communication, 38(1), 128–138.
van der Linden, S. (2016b). The social-psychological determinants of climate change risk
perceptions, attitudes, and behaviours: A national study. Environmental Education
Research, 22(3), 434–435.
van der Linden, S. (2017). The nature of viral altruism and how to make it stick.
Nature Human Behaviour.
van der Linden, S., Maibach, E., & Leiserowitz, A. (2015). Improving public engagement
with climate change: Five “best practice” insights from psychological science.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(6), 758–763.
van der Linden, S., Leiserowitz, A., Rosenthal, S., & Maibach, E. (2017). Inoculating the
public against misinformation about climate change. Global Challenges.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 49 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
van der Linden, S., & Rosenthal, S. A. (2016). Measuring narcissism with a single
question? A replication and extension of the Single-Item Narcissism Scale (SINS).
Personality and Individual Differences, 90, 238–241.
van der Linden, S. L., Clarke, C. E., & Maibach, E. W. (2015). Highlighting consensus
among medical scientists increases public support for vaccines: Evidence from a
randomized experiment. BMC Public Health, 15(1), 1207.
van der Linden, S. L., Leiserowitz, A. A., Feinberg, G. D., & Maibach, E. W. (2015). The
scientific consensus on climate change as a gateway belief: Experimental evidence. PloS
ONE, 10(2), e0118489.
van der Pligt J., Zeelenberg, M., Van Dijk, W. W., De Vries, N. K., & Richard, R. (1998).
Affect, attitudes, and decisions: Let’s be more specific. European Review of Social
Psychology, 8(1), 33–66.
Van Liere, K. D., & Dunlap, R. E. (1980). The social bases of environmental concern: A
review of hypotheses, explanations, and empirical evidence. Public Opinion Quarterly,
44(2), 181–197.
Van Liere, K. D., & Dunlap, R. E. (1981). Environmental concern: Does it make a
difference how it’s measured? Environment and Behavior, 13(6), 651–676.
van Vugt, M., Griskevicius, V., & Schultz, P. (2014). Naturally green: Harnessing stone
age psychological biases to foster environmental behavior. Social Issues and Policy
Review, 8(1), 1–32.
Varki, A. (2009). Human uniqueness and the denial of death. Nature, 460(7,256), 684.
Vaughan, E., & Nordenstam, B. (1991). The perception of environmental risks among
ethnically diverse groups. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 22(1), 29–60.
Voelklein, C., & Howarth, C. (2005). A review of controversies about social
representations theory: A British debate. Culture and Psychology, 11(4), 431–454.
Wåhlberg, A. E. (2001). The theoretical features of some current approaches to risk
perception. Journal of Risk Research, 4(3), 237–250.
Wardman, J. K. (2006). Toward a critical discourse on affect and risk perception. Journal
of Risk Research, 9(2), 109–124.
Weber, E. U. (2006). Evidence-based and description-based perceptions of long-term risk:
Why global warming does not scare us (yet). Climatic Change, 77(1), 103–120.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 50 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Weber, E. U. (2010). What shapes perceptions of climate change? WIREs Climate Change,
1(3), 332–342.
Weber, E. U. (2016). What shapes perceptions of climate change? New research since
2010. WIREs Climate Change, 7(1), 125–134.
Weber, E. U., & Stern, P. C. (2011). Public understanding of climate change in the United
States. American Psychologist, 66(4), 315.
Weigel, R. H., & Newman, L. S. (1976). Increasing attitude-behavior correspondence by
broadening the scope of the behavioral measure. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 33(6), 793–802.
Weinstein, N. D. (1989). Optimistic biases about personal risks. Science, 246(4,935),
1232–1233.
Whitmarsh, L. (2008a). Are flood victims more concerned about climate change than
other people? The role of direct experience in risk perception and behavioural response.
Journal of Risk Research, 11(3), 351–374.
Whitmarsh, L. (2008b). What’s in a name? Commonalities and differences in public
understanding of “climate change” and “global warming.” Public Understanding of
Science, 18(4), 401–420.
Whitmarsh, L. (2009). Behavioural responses to climate change: Asymmetry of intentions
and impacts. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29(1), 13–23.
Whitmarsh, L. (2011). Skepticism and uncertainty about climate change: Dimensions,
determinants, and change over time. Global Environmental Change, 21(2), 690–700.
Whitmarsh, L., Lorenzoni, I., & O’Neill, S. (2012). Engaging the public with climate
change: Behaviour change and communication. London: Earthscan.
Wildavsky, A., & Dake, K. (1990). Theories of risk perception: Who fears what and why?
Daedalus, 119(4), 41–60.
Xiao, C., & McCright, A. M. (2012). Explaining gender differences in concern about
environmental problems in the United States. Society & Natural Resources, 25(11),
1,067–1,084.
Xiao, C., & McCright, A. M. (2013). Gender differences in environmental concern:
Revisiting the institutional trust hypothesis in the USA. Environment and Behavior, 47(1),
17–37.
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 51 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
Xue, W., Hine, D. W., Loi, N. M., Thorsteinsson, E. B., & Phillips, W. J. (2014). Cultural
worldviews and environmental risk perceptions: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 40, 249–258.
Xue, W., Hine, D. W., Marks, A. D., Phillips, W. J., & Zhao, S. (2015). Cultural worldviews
and climate change: A view from China. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 134–144.
Zaalberg, R., & Midden, C. J. (2013). Living behind dikes: Mimicking flooding
experiences. Risk Analysis, 33(5), 866–876.
Zahran, S., Brody, S. D., Grover, H., & Vedlitz, A. (2006). Climate change vulnerability
and policy support. Society and Natural Resources, 19(9), 771–789.
Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39(2), 117–123.
Zaval, L., Keenan, E. A., Johnson, E. J., & Weber, E. U. (2014). How warm days increase
belief in global warming. Nature Climate Change, 4, 143–147.
Notes:
(1.) A notable exception is Kahan et al. (2012). Although it should be mentioned that this
study assessed public science literacy in general rather than domain-specific knowledge
about climate change.
(2.) I should note that the “two systems of reasoning” model is mostly used as a metaphor
(Kahneman, 2011), given that the human brain does not literally have two distinct
“systems”—but since some scholars have proposed an alternative, unified model
(Kruglanski & Gigerenzer, 2011), I feel compelled to highlight this here.
(3.) A notable exception is Smith and Leiserowitz (2012).
(4.) These were derived from Schwartz’s (1992) self-enhancing vs. self-transcending value
clusters.
(5.) A related measure that is somewhat less U.S.-specific but produces similar results is
known as “free-market ideology” (e.g., see Heath & Gifford, 2006).
(6.) This is so because prediction error is a function of both bias and variance. Although
heuristics are necessarily “biased” by ignoring information, they typically capitalize on
having low variance (Gigerenzer & Brighton, 2009).
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 52 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
(7.) An additional debate revolves around the terms “climate change” vs. “global
warming” where use of the latter may elicit more public concern than the former (c.f.,
Schuldt, Konrath, & Schwarz, 2011; Whitmarsh, 2008b).
(8.) A “healthy” amount of worry is different from the experience of fear. Fear can often
result in so-called “amygdala hijack” (Goleman, 2006), which can interfere with risk
processing and produce maladaptive behavioral responses.
(9.) Of course, similar to criticisms of Maslow’s (1943) “hierarchy of needs,” it is possible
that knowing what the likely impacts of climate change are (bottom) can directly lead to
worry (top) about the issue as well.
(10.) The model is actually drawn in reverse order for conceptual clarity, in a modeling
sense, the societal and personal level variables would be latent factors with the four risk
perception items each being indicators. The two broad risk perception dimensions would
in turn be components of the latent multidimensional risk perception scale.
(11.) To the extent that this is due to differences in measurement, there is some evidence
to suggest that when risk perception is operationalized as personal worry, it bears a
stronger relationship to behavioral measures (Bubeck et al., 2012; Smith & Leiserowitz,
2014).
(12.) Notable exceptions include Semenza et al. (2008) and van der Linden (2016b).
(13.) In the health domain, the link between threat perception and behavior tends to be
more direct, because people are often motivated to “protect” themselves from visceral
health risks (e.g., see Floyd, Prentice-Dunn, & Rogers, 2000).
(14.) Climate beliefs and risk perception also tend to correlate less strongly with more
high-cost behaviors, as these are typically more difficult to implement for people due to
economic and structural barriers (van der Linden, 2016b).
Sander van der Linden
Princeton University
Determinants and Measurement of Climate Change Risk Perception, Worry, and Concern
Page 53 of 53
PRINTED FROM the OXFORD RESEARCH ENCYCLOPEDIA, CLIMATE SCIENCE (climatescience.oxfordre.com). (c) Oxford
University Press USA, 2016. All Rights Reserved. Personal use only; commercial use is strictly prohibited. Please see
applicable Privacy Policy and Legal Notice (for details see Privacy Policy).
Subscriber: Cambridge University Library; date: 03 April 2017
... Adaptation involves responding to the effects of climate change cautiously and reactively, often used in place of mitigation, which is a preventive approach aimed at reducing greenhouse gas emissions (Schipper, 2007). Reviewing existing literature, it can be observed that there are many studies attempting to predict behaviors related to mitigation/reduction through precursors such as environmental-friendly behaviors, carbon footprint, consumption behaviors, adaptation to and affinity with nature and ecological paradigms (Brügger, Morton, & Dessai, 2015;Chen, 2020;Dunlap, van Liere, Mertig, & Jones, 2000;O'Connor, Bard, & Fisher, 1999;van der Linden, 2015van der Linden, , 2017. Additionally, it should be noted that many studies have been conducted on different sample groups to measure levels of concern and anxiety (Aslan, Köçer, & Mizrak, 2023;Clayton & Karazsia, 2020). ...
... It is thought that the participants' feeling responsible for mitigating the negative effects of climate change will have an impact on their level of hope. In this context, a question consisting of a single statement was asked by analyzing similar studies in the literature (Brügger et al., 2015;Chen, 2020;Smith & Leiserowitz, 2012;van der Linden, 2017;van Valkengoed, Steg, & Perlaviciute, 2021). The statement "To what extent do you feel responsible for mitigating the negative impacts of climate change?" is a continuous variable and provides respondents with a 5-point Likert-type response. ...
Article
Full-text available
Climate change is one of the most important global problems of our time. For the solution of this problem, the need for awareness and taking responsibility at individual and social level is quite high. The aim of this study, which was conducted on social club members, is to examine the level of hope for the prevention of climate change according to sociodemographic characteristics, club factors and taking responsibility variable. Relational survey model, one of the quantitative research methods, was used in the study. The research data were collected from the students studying at Ardahan University in the 2023-2024 academic year through a questionnaire. The data obtained were analyzed using SPSS 27 and AMOS 24 statistical package programs. In this context, parametric tests such as Independent Samples T Test, One- Way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA), Pearson Correlation and Simple Linear Regression Analysis were applied due to the normal distribution of the data. When the results of the analysis are examined, it is seen that the mean scale scores of the sample group are high; there is a significant difference between the groups in terms of hope levels according to their education levels and club types; and the variable of taking responsibility has an effect of 26.7% on the level of hope. Thus, a one-unit increase in the level of responsibility causes a 0.301-unit increase in the level of hope for preventing climate change. In this context, club members' approaches to preventing climate change can be strengthened by conducting incentive studies to increase their sense of responsibility.
... This result also provides one potential explanation for why most studies (Leng et al., 2021;Verelst et al., 2018) have failed to detect free-riding effects in vaccination decisions. According to the theory of collective resistance, individuals are motivated to free-ride when they perceive lower risks associated with non-contribution (Linden, 2017). If descriptive norms do not shift risk perceptions as our findings suggest, there is no reason to expect descriptive norms to trigger free-riding motivations. ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite the proven effectiveness of influenza vaccines, vaccination coverage remains suboptimal globally. Descriptive norms messaging highlighting low influenza vaccination rates is often presented to the public. This study investigated the influence of descriptive norms on influenza vaccination intentions and assessed whether the interaction of value framing with herd-immunity threshold moderates this effect. Utilizing an online experiment, a 2 (descriptive norms: 20% vs. 80%) × 2 (value framing: individual immunity vs. herd immunity) × 2 (herd-immunity threshold: uncertain vs. 90%) between-subjects fractional design was conducted with 803 participants (female 62%, mean age 31 years). The results revealed that low descriptive norms decreased influenza vaccination intention by amplifying concerns about the vaccine’s safety and effectiveness. While framing the value of herd immunity increased vaccination intention when the herd-immunity threshold was uncertain, it did not mitigate the negative impact of low descriptive norms. These findings offer novel insights into the cognitive mechanisms underpinning norm-based social influence on vaccination intentions and suggest that public health officials should exercise caution when presenting vaccine coverage statistics to the public to avoid inadvertently discouraging vaccination.
Article
Full-text available
Under the turbulent environment of the 21st century, climate change emerges as a menacing and urgent crisis, especially in urban poverty-stricken areas, where its impacts are most acutely felt. Despite the importance of understanding public perceptions to foster effective climate change resilience, there still exists a notable gap in nuanced scholarship within the informal settlements of South Africa. This study employs a mixed-method approach to explore how cognitive, emotional, motivational and cultural factors affect these communities' resilience or vulnerability to climate-related hazards. The prevailing consensus in the literature suggests a general underestimation of climate change risks among residents. However, this paper contends that innovative, resource-constrained strategies observed in these communities suggest a significant degree of agency and preparedness to confront these challenges. This study sheds light on the interplay between individual actions, social networks, information channels, cultural practices, and power dynamics in shaping climate change perceptions. It recommends integrating local, cost-effective adaptation measures into wider policy frameworks. In conclusion, the study emphasises the importance of educating informal settlement residents, harnessing community participation and utilising local adaptation knowledge and sustainable development techniques to forge a resilient and equitable future for the inhabitants of Buffalo City Municipality.
Article
Full-text available
Climate change (CC) poses a significant threat to small-scale farmers in low-income countries, increasing vulnerability to food insecurity and requiring various methods to mitigate its impact. The current study assessed producers’ perceptions of CC and the adaption measures they adopt to mitigate the effect of CC in Builsa South district of Ghana. A generalized Poisson regression model was used to evaluate the factors affecting adoption of climate change adaptation strategies (CCASs) by the respondents. Farmers’ knowledge of the factors contributing to CC was analyzed by employing a 5-point Likert scale while producers’ perception of the effect of CC on maize cultivation was assessed using Kendell’s coefficient of concordance. The findings indicated that deforestation, bush burning, improper disposal of waste and greenhouse gases are the main activities contributing to CC in the district. The adaptation strategies used by farmers include early planting, adoption of disease resistant and drought-tolerant varieties, crop rotation, mixed cropping, and zero tillage. The study further revealed that years of education, farm size, radio ownership, and crop insurance significantly enhanced adoption of CCASs. The authors recommend more education and training on CC adaptation practices to equip farmers with the skills to alleviate the impact of CC.
Article
Full-text available
Understanding divergent perceptions of ethnic groups to climate change in mountainous regions home to multi-ethnic cultures and the factors influencing these perceptions is crucial for policymakers to predict the trending impacts of climate change and make long-term decisions. Based on the case of Southwest China, 1216 households were interviewed by questionnaire surveys to gain insight into the perceptions of local people on the dynamic evolution characteristics of climate events in the uplands of Yunnan, China, which is an area home to rich ethnic diversity, and also to determine the factors that influence these perceptions. Results indicated that climate events have now become important events for farmers’ livelihoods, ranking only after family diseases and livestock diseases. Drought, long-term drought, and erratic rainfall are the three kinds of climatic events with the most significant increase in frequency and severity in mountainous areas. Farmers’ perceptions on whether drought, long-term drought, and erratic rainfall occurred 10 years ago as well as changes in frequency and severity are significantly influenced by characteristics of respondents, ethnic culture, geographical environment of farmer residences, farmland characteristics, and sources of livelihood. Ultimately, taking ethnic differences into consideration for long-term planning will be an important part of the local response to climate change in the future.
Article
Full-text available
The term climate anxiety has increasingly appeared in the academic literature and popular discourse since 2019, typically when discussing young people's negative emotional responses to climate change. This paper reports results from a nationally representative survey of the Norwegian public (N = 2040) that investigated whether people respond differently to descriptions of young people “having climate anxiety”, compared with being “concerned” or “worried” about climate change. Results from the survey experiment showed stronger support for politicians taking young people's climate concern or climate worry into consideration when designing new climate policy as compared with young people's climate anxiety. Analyses of an open-ended question asking what people think of when they hear or read the term “climate anxiety” showed that most respondents (52%) provided neutral descriptions (e.g., worry about climate change impacts), 27% viewed climate anxiety as unfounded, irrational, or excessive, and equal proportions of respondents critiqued the term specifically for contributing to such negative associations (6%) or referred to climate anxiety as a reasonable and rational reaction (6%). These findings indicate that among some audiences, using the term climate anxiety may provoke reactance and be perceived as distracting from political actions to mitigate climate change. Our results give important insights into the potential consequences of the terms we use when reporting on climate distress.
Article
Full-text available
The ways in which people value wildlife, or “wildlife value orientations,” shape attitudes and behaviors and can even predict support for environmental issues. As such, wildlife value orientations can be used strategically to inform communication strategies. We used a national, experimental framing survey (n = 1,998) to investigate how the two most predominant wildlife value orientations in the US – domination and mutualism – affect people’s intentions to engage in conservation behaviors. Results showed that respondents on the mutualistic scale were more likely to express strong intentions to donate, engage in political activity, and adopt simple actions in support of conservation. In mutualistic-oriented respondents, pessimistically framed messages generally increased intentions to donate, but both optimistic and pessimistic-framed messages were likely to backfire and reduce intentions to adopt personal conservation actions. Our findings provide preliminary empirical evidence that wildlife value orientations could guide more effective communication approaches leading to behavior change.
Article
Full-text available
Public perceptions of energy choices will play a major role in the energy transition. Climate-related emotions, particularly concerns and worries, influence these perceptions, as they signal a heightened awareness of climate risks and greater personal salience of climate change. Here we conduct a series of meta-analyses to estimate whether climate worries and concerns influence energy preferences (k = 233; N = 85,285; 36 countries). Our findings reveal that climate worries and concerns translate into support for renewable energy, particularly solar and wind, and modest opposition to fossil fuels, particularly coal and gas. Climate worries and concerns are not associated with nuclear energy, albeit with a high degree of variance. Socio-demographic moderators, such as gender, education, and political orientation, did not influence these associations, while age and national energy supply attenuated these associations. These results suggest that climate concerns and worries translate into support for renewable energy, but not equal opposition to fossil fuels. More broadly, this meta-analysis underscores the role of climate-related emotions in shaping energy preferences, providing insights into the influence factors of energy policy support, the psychology of climate change, and climate change communication.
Article
Full-text available
Societal altruism is changing. Increased awareness and use of online social media is providing new ways of inspiring collective action and support for critical societal challenges. What makes some social causes go viral while others never seem to take off?
Article
Full-text available
Effectively addressing climate change requires significant changes in individual and collective human behavior and decision-making. Yet, in light of the increasing politicization of (climate) science, and the attempts of vested-interest groups to undermine the scientific consensus on climate change through organized “disinformation campaigns,” identifying ways to effectively engage with the public about the issue across the political spectrum has proven difficult. A growing body of research suggests that one promising way to counteract the politicization of science is to convey the high level of normative agreement (“consensus”) among experts about the reality of human-caused climate change. Yet, much prior research examining public opinion dynamics in the context of climate change has done so under conditions with limited external validity. Moreover, no research to date has examined how to protect the public from the spread of influential misinformation about climate change. The current research bridges this divide by exploring how people evaluate and process consensus cues in a polarized information environment. Furthermore, evidence is provided that it is possible to pre-emptively protect (“inoculate”) public attitudes about climate change against real-world misinformation.
Chapter
Full-text available
This book, a member of the Series in Affective Science, is a unique interdisciplinary sequence of articles on the cognitive neuroscience of emotion by some of the most well-known researchers in the area. It explores what is known about cognitive processes in emotion at the same time it reviews the processes and anatomical structures involved in emotion, determining whether there is something about emotion and its neural substrates that requires they be studied as a separate domain. Divided into four major focal points and presenting research that has been performed in the last decade, this book covers the process of emotion generation, the functions of amygdala, the conscious experience of emotion, and emotion regulation and dysregulation. Collectively, the chapters constitute a broad but selective survey of current knowledge about emotion and the brain, and they all address the close association between cognitive and emotional processes. By bringing together diverse strands of investigation with the aim of documenting current understanding of how emotion is instantiated in the brain, this book will be of use to scientists, researchers, and advanced students of psychology and neuroscience.
Book
This handbook is the first to comprehensively study the interdependent fields of environmental and conservation psychology. In doing so, it seeks to map the rapidly growing field of conservation psychology and its relationship to environmental psychology. The Oxford Handbook of Environmental and Conservation Psychology includes basic research on environmental perceptions, attitudes, and values; research on specific environments, such as therapeutic settings, schools, and prisons; environmental impacts on human well-being; and ways to promote a more sustainable relationship between people and the natural environment. This handbook presents an extensive review of current research and is a thorough guide to the state of knowledge about a wide range of topics at the intersection of psychology and the physical environment. Beyond this, it provides a better understanding of the relationship between environmental and conservation psychology, and some sense of the directions in which these interdependent areas of study are heading.
Article
Despite extensive efforts at public science education, polling over the past 30 years has consistently shown that about 40 to 45% of Americans believe that humans were supernaturally created in the past 10,000 years ( 1 ). A natural interpretation of this finding is that U.S. science education is failing to reach nearly half of the population, and that widespread belief in recent human origins reflects basic scientific illiteracy. However, the reality is more complex ( 2 ): Many of those who reject evolutionary theory are aware of the scientific consensus on the subject, and such rejection is not always associated with low scientific literacy. Similar results have been found for beliefs regarding anthropogenic climate change ( 3 ). On page 321 of this issue, Friedkin et al. ( 4 ) provide a key step toward understanding this phenomenon by introducing a simple family of models for social influence among individuals with multiple, interdependent beliefs.