ChapterPDF Available

Preventing repeat and near repeat crime concentrations

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Crime is highly concentrated: Most crime is a rehearsal for further crime against the same or similar targets, at the same or similar locations, and perhaps theft of the same type of products. The study of repeat victimization has evolved into that on crime hotspots and other forms of near repeat, and led to predictive policing. The F-B-I theory of crime concentration notes how some targets have characteristics that Flag them as attractive, offenders learn some targets or places are attractive which Boosts the chances of further crime, while the Interaction of potential offenders and suitable targets creates high crime locations. There is strong evidence that targeting crime concentrations with prevention resources can succeed but that it is not necessarily easy to implement appropriate tactics. Hence while there is great potential, much research remains to be undertaken in this rapidly evolving and important area.
Content may be subject to copyright.
PREVENTING REPEAT AND NEAR REPEAT CRIME CONCENTRATIONS
Graham Farrell1 and Ken Pease2
Forthcoming in N. Tilley and A. Sidebottom (Eds.). (2017) Handbook of Crime Prevention and Community
Safety, 2nd Edition.
Abstract
Crime is highly concentrated: Most crime is a rehearsal for further crime against the same or similar
targets, at the same or similar locations, and perhaps theft of the same type of products. The study of
repeat victimization has evolved into that on crime hotspots and other forms of near repeat, and led to
predictive policing. The F-B-I theory of crime concentration notes how some targets have characteristics
that Flag them as attractive, offenders learn some targets or places are attractive which Boosts the
chances of further crime, while the Interaction of potential offenders and suitable targets creates high
crime locations. There is strong evidence that targeting crime concentrations with prevention resources
can succeed but that it is not necessarily easy to implement appropriate tactics. Hence while there is
great potential, much research remains to be undertaken in this rapidly evolving and important area.
Keywords: crime concentration; repeat victimization; near repeats; hot spots; hot products; FBI theory;
crime concentration theory.
1Centre for Criminal Justice Studies, School of Law, University of Leeds
2Jill Dando Institute, University College London
Introduction
Burp! Crime repeats. This is one of the most important things about crime. Since the first cave dweller
stole food again! - from their neighbour, repeat victimisation has occurred. But why? And how does it
inform crime prevention? What do we do to stop it? This chapter answers these questions.
Consider a burglary. The rear door of a household is forced open and cash, jewellery and portable
electronics are stolen. Not only is that household now at greater risk of another burglary quite
soon, but so too are its neighbours. That is because the burglars literally have inside
information. They know how to get in and out. They know the layout. They know the low risk
and high payback. Let’s do it again! Or let’s do a similar house nearby.
Consider an online scam. Sending out hundreds of phishing emails brings only a handful of replies. But it
was cheap and easy to do. Some of the suckers sent money up front to pay for a bank transfer
they will not receive, so I will target them with another scam try to get their bank details and
PIN. I know they are gullible and now know more of their details. And I will target other victims
with similar profiles.
Consider terrorism: A military vehicle is blown up by an improvised explosive device near a military base.
It is a frequently patrolled road, and IEDs are easy to build. The roads cannot be constantly
watched so I will do the same thing again nearby as it is an efficient use of my limited staff and
resources. It maximises terror, publicity and disruption, which is our aim.
These are hypothetical scenarios but they draw on research about why repeat and near repeat
victimization occurs. They show how crimes that might appear random are related. Crime is never
random and is always concentrated on certain places, people, or other targets however defined: In fact,
it is highly concentrated in every dimension (more on this later). On the plus side, crime’s repetitive
nature provides a lot of information about where, when, and how it can be prevented.
In the United States, over three quarters of personal crimes were found to be repeats against persons
who already experienced personal crime that year. Table 1 shows rape and sexual assault, assault, and
theft (personal larceny). Four out of five rapes or sexual assaults (82.6%) are experienced by victims who
experienced more than one of them that year. Assault is experienced by 8.5% of persons (left column of
numbers) but there are 37 assaults per 100 people on average (middle column) so they are far from
evenly distributed. In fact most assaults (77.3% - right-hand column) are committed against persons
already assaulted.
Table 1: Personal crimes in the United States
Crime type
Victims per 100
persons
Crimes per 100
persons
% Repeats
Rape/sex assault
0.4
2.3
82.6
Robbery
1.3
3.6
63.9
Assault
8.5
37.4
77.3
Personal larceny
0.5
1.2
58.3
Total
10.6
44.6
76.2
Source: Farrell and Pease (2014) based on Planty and Strom (2007)
Generally speaking, most crime, and most variation in crime, is due to repeat and near repeat
victimisation. Figure 1 shows how, in England and Wales in recent years, crimes experienced by repeat
victims make up most crime plus most of the large rise and fall in common crimes including violence,
burglary, theft, vehicle crimes and vandalism (Britton et al 2012; Farrell and Pease 2014). But the real
proportion of repeat victimisation is even more than shown in Table 1 and Figure 1 because the surveys
count crimes each year whereas repeats occur over time (i.e. some in the year are linked to those before
or after that year). This is one of the reasons that surveys under-estimate repeats (Farrell, Sousa and
Weisel 2002). For Figure 1, the data are from the UK government’s Home Office, and for some reason
that institution only counts five crimes against a victim even if they experienced far more than that (see
Farrell and Pease 2007). How unfair! It is ironic but sadly not unusual that the most chronically
victimised members of society are ignored.
1
Figure 1: Repeat victimization in England and Wales
Source: Farrell and Pease (2014)
Another insightful indicator is the proportion of crime experienced by the most chronically victimised.
One study found 16% of the population experienced property crime and 8% experienced personal crime,
but among the most chronically victimised (Pease 1998):
2% of the population experience 44% of property crime
1% of the population experience 59% of personal crime
The picture is largely the same across high income countries and, to the extent we know from limited
information, for middle and low income countries as well (see e.g. Sidebottom 2011 on repeat burglary
in Malawi). The International Crime Victims Survey is the only methodologically standardised general
victim survey (meaning that its findings can be usefully compared across countries most crime data
cannot). It finds that rates of repeat victimization are remarkably similar in different countries (Farrell
and Bouloukos 2001, Mawby 2001; van Dijk 2001).
New Technology Crimes
While many common crimes have been in long-term decline, some crime types have increased. Some
seemingly ‘new crimes are variations on existing themes. Fraud and identity theft already existed but
have surged because the internet provides new crime opportunities - easy access to potential victims.
Fraudsters have always sought out existing victims who are potentially easy prey, and repeat fraud has
always been highly prevalent (see Titus and Gover 2001). While aspects of online fraud and identity
theft may have changed, the notion of targeting efforts to prevent repeats is likely to prove efficient.
Table 2: Computer system and internet-related repeats
With the widespread use of the Internet, e-commerce, business and other networks, the
security of such networks is increasingly important. Yet attacks and incidents against networks
are increasingly common. Potential crimes include fraud, theft (of funds, knowledge and
information, or other), account break-ins, malicious damage to users, institutions or networks.
Over a quarter (27%) of the 6684 computer sites studied experienced at least 3 attacks and a
mean of 12 attacks. The ten most victimized sites experienced an average of 369 attacks each!
Repeat attacks were far more likely to occur soon after a prior attack, particularly in the first
week. Some types of attack were likely to occur more quickly than others, and repeats were
more likely to be the same type of incident (perhaps suggesting the same offenders). Some
network domain types experienced more rapid repeats (those ending ‘.edu’ were fastest and
those ending ‘.com’ were slowest).
Though prevention was not the primary focus of the research, the potential is evident. Focusing
network security on sites already hacked could prevent a lot of hacking (and the displacement
literature suggests that, for various reasons, much of it will not simply move to other networks).
Security should be put in place quickly and certain types of domain such as educational
institutions (.edu sites) should be particularly proactive in prevention. There could exist the
potential to track and detect returning hackers who, in turn, may well be the most prolific and
serious hackers.
Source: Adapted from Moitra, S. D. and S.L. Konda. (2004)
Stemming crime opportunities provided by the internet is likely to be a mainstay of crime prevention
efforts for years to come. A focus upon repeats will promote efficiency. New technology crimes are also
highly concentrated: Network attacks, for instance, are highly concentrated upon particular domains
and upon particular networks (see Sidebottom and Tilley, this volume). Depending on the type of
preventive measure to be introduced there is likely to be efficiency in having a focus upon repeat
domains and repeatedly targeted networks the virtual equivalent of repeat victims and repeat places
(See Table 2).
Near Repeats Hotspots and Hot Products
One of the earliest studies of repeats led to the term ‘hotspots’. It examined repeat calls to the police
(Sherman et al. 1989), finding that half of calls to police came from only 3 percent of households. This
was repeat victimisation of the same persons or households which, on a map, appears as hotdots or
hotspots. This led to a growing interest in how crime concentrates in the same places, sometimes
termed ‘crime and place’ (Eck and Weisburd 1995; Curman et al. 2015; Andresen and Malleson 2011).
Figure 2 shows a simplified version of the relationship between victimization, repeat victimization, hot
spots and high crime areas.
Figure 2: Repeat and near repeats form hotspots and high crime areas
(Source: Adapted from Farrell and Sousa 2001)
In 1998, the term ‘virtual repeats’ was used to refer to crimes that are very similar (Pease 1998). The
term that has become more widely used is ‘near repeats’ (Morgan 2001). It has been used to refer to
how nearby neighbours are more likely to be burgled for a short period (Townsley et al. 2003; Bowers
and Johnson 2004; Bernasco 2008), armed robberies and shootings recur soon nearby (Ratcliffe and
Rengert 2008; Wells et al. 2008; Haberman and Ratcliffe 2012), and other types of crime (Youtsin et al.
2011) and terrorist activity repeat (Townsley et al. 2008), while repeat victimization locations are streets
or other places where crimes cluster (Levy and Tartaro 2010). Since repeats and near repeats show
where and when crimes occur, their predictive power has led to prospective hotspotting and predictive
policing (Bowers and Johnson 2004; Bowers, Johnson, and Pease 2004; Short et al. 2009; Pease and
Tseloni 2014). The spatial distribution of near repeats fall into what can be termed the victimology of
place.
Just as some victims and places experience repeat crimes, some types of consumer goods are stolen
much more than others. Particular makes and models of goods are targeted: some types of phones,
some types of cars, some laptops and so on. Such ‘hot products’ are attractive to thieves (Clarke 1999).
Two thefts of the same type of product are near repeats by dint of the similarity of the product, just as
two geographically similar crimes are near repeats because they are spatially close together. So the term
‘near repeat’ can be used to refer more broadly to different types of cluster or concentration of crime
(Farrell 2015). Even a burglary on the same street a few days later is similar in more than just spatial
and temporal terms it is probably the same offender who got into and out of similar households by the
same means and stole the same type of things (Everson and Pease 2001; Bernasco 2008). Repeat crimes
of different types are linked by their similarity: they are nearly the same. In practice the ways in which
crime concentrates usually overlap a lot: One victim had several phones stolen at different times from
their coat when they were at work: probably the same offender who stole a hot product from a repeat
victim at the same repeat place. Figure 3 shows a simplified version of the relationship between repeat
offending, repeat victimization, hot spots and hot products. The relative sizes of the circles and their
overlap are not to scale.
Figure 3: Overlapping repeat and near repeat phenomena
(Source: Adapted from Farrell and Sousa 2001)
In Figure 3, a crime at the centre is committed by a repeat offender stealing a hot product from a repeat
victim at a hotspot. Other crimes do not have to involve each of the elements. The next section looks at
why concentrations of crime occur.
Explaining Crime Concentrations: FBI Theory
How does crime get so concentrated? The repeat victimization literature identifies the ‘flag and boost’
explanations (Pease 1998) plus how repeats occur disproportionately in hot spots and high crime areas
due to interaction effects when multiple suitable targets and potential offenders converge (Farrell 1993
and Farrell et al. 1996, 2005 offer models based on the ideas of Cohen and Felson 1979). The
characteristics of some targets flag them as attractive to offenders. When some targets are victimized,
offenders learn they are good targets which boosts their chances of further victimization. When suitable
targets combine with potential offenders in locations that contain insufficient guardianship, the factors
interact to produce disproportionate crime rates.
The hotspot literature identifies the ‘generate and attract’ explanations (see e.g. Kurland et al. 2014).
Some places generate crime by the volume of interaction of potential targets and potential offenders.
Some places attract offenders because they are known to be good places to commit crime.
The mechanisms by which both repeats and hotspots occur their theories - have been compared and
shown to be the same, suggesting a more general theory of crime concentration (Farrell 2015). The
three overarching mechanisms are here identified as:
Flag some targets flag themselves such that they attract offenders
Boost the likelihood of further crime is boosted by experience with that target or place
Interaction some targets or places experienced disproportionate crime because the
interaction of multiple suitable targets and potential offenders generates a higher crime
rate
The flag-boost-interaction or ‘FBI’ explanation offers a general set of mechanisms that explains why
crime is always concentrated. A store may be repeatedly targeted because it provides visual cues a flag
- that it is attractive (for example it contains valuable goods). When the offender learns it is attractive
this boosts the chances of their return, which is why risk increases with each further crime. The
offenders peers may learn of the suitable target, and their interaction with potential targets nearby
results in additional crime.
Where, When, How and Why to Prevent Repeats
We know from experience that appropriate prevention tactics need to be put in place to prevent repeat
crimes (Grove et al. 2012). This means more than just telling victims they are now at greater risk
something concrete needs to be done. And that something needs to be properly implemented. So, if
windows and doors on a house need to be made more secure to prevent another burglary, then
somebody needs to make sure this gets done. As simple as it sounds, this is not always easy.
The first proper study of repeat victimization looked at hospital records (Johnson et al. 1973). It found
the same individuals kept returning to the hospital again and again as victims of violence though some
stopped returning when they were killed! This was just the tip of the iceberg. In the 1970s the clustering
of crime was not well recognised but, like much of what we now know, it was revealed by surveys that
ask people about their experience as victims of crime. Two famous studies using victim surveys - one in
the US and one in the UK - found remarkably similar patterns of extensive repeat victimization against a
small proportion of the population (Hindelang et al. 1978; Sparks et al. 1977). They showed that repeats
did not occur just by random chance (i.e. were not by ‘accident’), as many other studies have now also
shown.
The catalyst for a great deal of research into crime concentration was the Kirkholt burglary prevention
project (Forrester et al 1990, Pease 1991). Kirkholt is the name of an area of public housing near
Manchester, England. Burgled households were found to be more likely to experience further burglary,
so the project introduced measures to stop it. Two of the main things done were (1) improving security
to stop burglars entering households by the same means (more secure windows and doors), and (2)
encouraging nearby neighbours to watch out for burglars a localised version of neighbourhood watch
known as cocoon watch (neighbours forming a protective ‘cocoon’ for the victim). As a result, repeat
burglaries were wiped out and the overall burglary rate fell dramatically. The project has become one of
the world’s better known crime prevention projects and inspired much further crime concentration
research as well as developments in policy and practice. Follow-up research showed that repeat
victimization is more likely to be sooner rather than later (Polvi et al. 1991; Farrell and Pease 1993) and
that each further crime increases the risk of another. This implies that more resources should be
allocated to more frequent victims as a form of ‘graded’ response. A project in Huddersfield (UK)
developed this into the ‘Olympic model’ where first-time victims received a Bronze response, second
time victims a Silver response, and more frequent victims a Gold response, with the amount of
preventive resources increasing (Anderson et al. 1995).
The story of how early repeat victimization research and policy evolved is best told by Laycock (2001). In
the 1990s, repeat victimization was introduced as a performance indicator for all UK police forces (Tilley
1995, Bridgeman and Hobbs 1997, Farrell et al. 2000). This means that police had to show what they
were doing to prevent repeat crimes. Practical guides were developed by the US Department of Justice
(Pease and Laycock 1996; Herman et al. 2002; Weisel 2005). In recent years, staffing and other changes
in the civil service mean that preventing repeat crimes receives less attention than it used to,
particularly in the UK, even though evidence suggests that crime is, proportionally, even more
concentrated (Ignatans and Pease 2015; Hunter and Tseloni 2016). However, at the same time, renewed
policy interest in repeats has emerged via studies of near repeats under the banner of predictive
policing (Haberman and Ratcliffe 2012; Pease and Tseloni 2014).
Table 3: Top Ten Reasons to Prevent Repeats and Near Repeats
1. It gets the grease to the squeak: it is an efficient means of allocating, in time and space,
scarce resources to crime problems.
2. It is a form of ‘drip feeding’ resources gradually and routinely as crimes occur.
3. Since risk increases with each crime per target, resource allocation can be graded to risk.
4. Repeats occur quickly so special resources can be rotated between high-risk targets.
5. It avoids potentially divisive means of allocating resources such as to ‘all elderly people’.
6. It can enhance detection of repeat offenders who are more likely to commit repeats.
7. Chances of displacement are low and chances of a diffusion of benefits are high.
8. It can enhance detection of serious and prolific offenders.
9. It can be used to develop agency and individual performance indicators.
10. It is relevant to all crime types including organized crime and terrorism, e-crime, violent
and property crimes. Even murder can be the repeat of an attempt.
Source: The list has evolved and been adapted from Pease (1991), Laycock and Farrell (2003) and Grove
and Farrell (2012).
Key advantages to preventing repeats and near repeats have been identified (Pease 1991; Laycock and
Farrell 2003). A ‘top ten’ reasons for preventing repeats is shown as Table 3. At least one profit-making
company, PredPol (Prepol.com), appears to have evolved from the approaches described herein.
Does it work?
Since the Kirkholt project described above, over 30 projects to prevent repeat victimization have been
evaluated, mostly in Australia, the UK and the US. They have tended to focus on burglary so there is
plenty of scope for developments relating to other types of crime, which is important because there is
extensive evidence that all other types of crime recur. The evaluations show that crime can be reduced
when repeats are stopped (Farrell and Pease 2006; Grove et al. 2012; Grove and Farrell 2012). Figure 4
shows the impact of those projects that have sought to prevent repeat burglaries. Burglary fell in 70% of
projects (17 of 24 shown) although there is a lot of variation between projects. The most comprehensive
review of prevention projects to date found that on average over 20% or one in five crimes was
prevented (Grove et al. 2012).
Figure 4: Percent change in burglaries in different projects
(Source: Grove and Farrell 2012)
The reasons for the variation in success rates between projects are addressed in the next section. But a
key theme is that it is not as simple as just copying prevention tactics that are used elsewhere. It was
shown early on that replicating the impact of the Kirkholt project was not a straightforward case of just
copying the tactics (Tilley 1993). In short, however, there is still a lot of work to be done to develop
preventive measures to tackle frequently repeated violent crimes including sexual victimization,
domestic violence and child abuse.
What Works? What doesn’t?
Rather than list particular measures such as locks and bolts, ‘what works’ here focuses on the approach
and processes. Tables 4 and 5 list what works and what doesn’t respectively. These summaries draw
mostly on the reviews of repeat victimisation but they likely apply generally to efforts to prevent all
types of near repeats. There have also been efforts to stop crime at hotspots using police patrols by
making them more focused on where and when crime is likely to recur (Braga et al. 2012).
Table 4: What Works?
What works is:
1. A strong preventive mechanism. Specific prevention tactics need to be tailored to the
context and target because the nature of crime varies for the same type of crime.
2. Multiple tactics. Multiple tactics working together can produces an even stronger effect
(due to synergy). Opportunity-blocking aimed at preventing repeat victimisation by the
same modus operandi seems the most likely candidate for effectiveness.
3. Strong implementation. Some prevention efforts failed because the preventive tactic was
not introduced. Hardly surprising!
4. A focus on situations with high rates of repeats. Those crimes, times and places where
repeat rates are highest are clearly an appropriate focus for prevention efforts.
Table 5: What Doesn’t Work?
What doesn’t work, or causes effort to fail, is:
1. Weak or inappropriate preventive tactics fail to prevent crime. Note that the same
prevention tactic does not necessarily work everywhere because the nature of the crime
problem varies.
2. Poor implementation fails: In particular, education or advice for victims is well-meaning but
does not necessarily mean that security or other measures are implemented. Victims may
be unable or unwilling to spend money on crime prevention. Better sources of funding for
security and other equipment, and better motivation and incentives for victims and place-
managers (such as bar owners), are required.
3. Replicating tactics without attention to context does not necessarily work. The most
transferable aspects tend to be methods or strategies. For example, security upgrades to
prevent repeat burglary by the modus operandi of the prior burglary requires different
tactics to be adopted depending on how they enter different houses.
4. Overall impact is less where repeat victimisation rates are low. Attempting to prevent
repeats in circumstances where none is likely to be present cannot be said to fail per se,
because it is a non-starter.
Tricky Issues
The series of projects to date that have been evaluated suggest some problems are thornier than others.
The problems span the range from problem identification, the development of tactics, implementation,
replication, and sustainability (Table 6).
The existence of tricky issues mean that preventing repeats is not simple. It requires careful data
analysis, rigorous identification of prevention tactics, and overall good management practices with
respect to the development and implementation of prevention. In particular, for some crimes it is not
readily apparent what should be done to prevent their recurrence. For property crimes, additional
security appears a viable option in many instances. However for violent crime this is a less well-proven
tactic and there are relatively few clear options in the prevention repertoire. It is here that further
research is urgently needed.
Table 6: Tricky issues in preventing repeats
Evaluated efforts to prevent repeats suggest that it is tricky to:
1. measure repeats a problem specification issue
2. Tricky to know what to do to prevent repeats an intervention issue
3. Tricky to get victims or place managers to adopt preventive measures an
implementation issue
4. Tricky to adopt the same measures elsewhere because crime varies from one place
to the next a replication problem
5. Tricky to maintain prevention if short-term project funding dries up a
sustainability problem.
Conclusion
Successful crime is usually a rehearsal for further crime. The further crime tends to involve the same or
similar targets and places. The characteristics of some targets and places provide cues that Flag them to
offenders as somehow better easier or more rewarding. Offenders then learn which targets and places
are worth choosing again - this Boosts their chance of further crime. At some places the Interaction of
multiple potential targets and offenders makes for unusually high crime rate hotspots. This tripartite
flag-boost-interaction theory seems to apply to all types of concentration of crime.
There is strong evidence that crime can be prevented when it is most concentrated. Information about
where, when and how crime has occurred can be used to stop it recurring. But it is not always easy.
There is an urgent need for further research to examine how many types of repeat crime can be
prevented personal crimes, e-crimes and other new technology crimes in particular, but also organised
crime and terrorism since these also cluster in time and space against the same or similar targets.
Research and practice relating to repeats are evolving. The study of near repeats touches covers all
forms of crime concentration such as hotspots and hot products. The development of models of
predictive policing and predictive crime prevention have evolved from work on repeats and near
repeats. However it is clear that there remains much to do and great potential to exploit so that
societys level of crime can be reduced.
References
Anderson, David, Sylvia Chenery, and Ken Pease. 1995. Biting Back: Tackling Repeat Burglary and Car
Crime . Crime Detection and Prevention Series Paper 58. London: Police Research Group, UK Home
Office.
Andresen, M.A. and Malleson, N. (2011) Testing the stability of crime patterns: Implications for theory
and practice. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 48(1): 5882.
Bernasco, W. (2008) Them Again?: Same-Offender Involvement in Repeat and Near Repeat Burglaries,
European Journal of Criminology, 5 (4), 411431.
Bowers, Kate J., and Shane D. Johnson. 2004. “Who Commits Near Repeats? A Test of the Boost
Explanation.” Western Criminology Review 5:1224.
Bowers, K.J., Johnson, S.D., and Pease, K. (2004) Prospective Hot-spoting: The Future of Crime Mapping?
British Journal of Criminology, 44, 641-658.
Bridgeman, C. and Hobbs, L. (1997). Preventing repeat victimisation: The police officers’ guide. London:
Police Research Group
Britton, A., Kershaw, C., Osborne, S. and Smith, K. (2012) ‘Underlying Patterns within the England and
Wales Crime Drop.’ In J. van Dijk, A. Tseloni and G. Farrell (Eds.). The International Crime Drop: New
Directions in Research. UK: Palgrave MacMillan, pp.159-181.
Braga, Anthony A., Andrew V. Papachristos, and DavidM. Hureau. 2012. ‘The Effects of Hot Spot Policing
on Crime: An Updated Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis’, Justice Quarterly 31(4):63363.
Clarke, Ronald V. 1999. “Hot Products: Understanding, Anticipating and Reducing Demand for Stolen
Goods”, Police Research Series Paper 112. Policing and Reducing Crime Unit. Research Development and
Statistics Directorate, London: Home Office.
Cohen, L. and Felson, M. (1979) Social change and crime rate trends: A routine activity approach,
American Sociological Review, 44(4): 588608.
Curman, Andrea S.N., Martin A. Andresen and Paul J. Brantingham. 2015. ‘Crime and place: A
longitudinal examination of street segment patterns in Vancouver, B.C.’ Journal of Quantitative
Criminology, 31(1); 127-147.
Eck, J. E. and Weisburd, D. (1995) (eds) Crime and Place. Monsey NY: Willow Tree Press.
Everson, S. and Pease, K. (2001). Crime against the same person and place: Detection opportunity and
offender targeting. In G. Farrell and K. Pease (eds) Repeat victimization, 199220. Monsey, NY: Criminal
Justice Press.
Farrell, G. 1993. Repeat Criminal Victimsation. PhD Thesis. University of Manchester.
Farrell, G. (2015) ‘Crime concentration theory’ Crime Prevention and Community Safety: An International
Journal, 17(4); 233-248.
Farrell G., and A. C. Bouloukos. 2001. “A cross-national comparative analysis of rates of repeat
victimization” Crime Prevention Studies, 12, 5-25.
Farrell, G. and Pease, K. (1993) Once Bitten, Twice Bitten: Repeat Victimisation and its Implications for
Crime Prevention. Crime Prevention Unit Paper 46. London: Home Office.
Farrell, G. and K. Pease. 2006. ‘Preventing Repeat Residential Burglary: A Review’ in B. C. Welsh and D. P.
Farrington (Eds.) Preventing Crime: What Works for Children, Offenders, Victims, and Places Dordrecht:
Springer. (pp. 161-176).
Farrell, G. and K. Pease. 2007. ‘The sting in the British Crime Survey tail: Multiple victimisations’ in M.
Maxfield and M. Hough (Eds.) Surveying Crime in the 21st Century, volume 22 of Crime Prevention
Studies. Cullompton: Willan Publishing. pp.33-54.
Farrell, G. and K. Pease. 2014. ‘Repeat victimization’ in G. Bruinsma and D. Weisburd (Eds.) Encyclopedia
of Criminology and Criminal Justice. New York: Springer-Verlag. (NB: full length and peer-reviewed)
Farrell, G., and W. Sousa. 2001. ‘Repeat victimization and hot spots: the overlap and its implications for
crime control and problem-oriented policing’ Crime Prevention Studies, 12, 221-240.
Farrell, G., D. Ellingworth and K. Pease. 1996. "High crime rates, repeat victimization, and routine
activities" in T. Bennett (Ed.) Preventing Crime and Disorder. Cambridge: Institute of Criminology,
pp.276-296.
Farrell, Graham, Louise Hobbs, Alan Edmunds and Gloria Laycock. 2000. RV Snapshot: UK Policing and
Repeat Victimization. Crime Reduction Research Series. Policing and Reducing Crime Unit Paper 5.
London: Home Office.
Farrell, G., K. Clark, D. Ellingworth and K. Pease. 2005. ‘Of targets and supertargets: A routine activity
theory of high crime areas’ Internet Journal of Criminology.
Farrell, G., W. H. Sousa and D. Lamm Weisel. 2002. ‘The time-window effect in the measurement of
repeat victimization: a methodology for its measurement and an empirical study’ Crime Prevention
Studies, 13, 15-27. (ISSN: 1065-7029).
Forrester, David, Samantha Frenz, Martin O’Connell and Ken Pease. 1990. The Kirkholt “Burglary
Prevention Project: Phase II”. Crime Prevention Unit Paper 23. London: Her Majesty's Stationery Office.
Grove, L.E. and G. Farrell. 2012. ‘Once bitten, twice shy? Repeat victimization and its prevention’ in B. C.
Welsh and D. P. Farrington (eds) Oxford Handbook on Crime Prevention. Sage publishers.
Grove, Louise E., Graham Farrell, David P. Farrington, and Shane Johnson. 2012. Systematic Review of
Preventing Repeat Victimization. Stockholm: Swedish National Council for Crime Prevention.
Haberman, C. and J. H. Ratcliffe. 2012. The predictive policing challenges of near repeat armed street
robberies, Policing, 6(2); 151-166.
Herman, S., D. R. Anderson, D. Johnson, K. Dempsey, D. Weisburd, R. Greenspan, G. Farrell, and J. Ready.
2002. Bringing Victims into Community Policing. Washington D.C.: Department of Justice, Office of
Community Policing Oriented Policing Services.
Hindelang, M., M.R. Gottfredson and J. Garofalo (1978). Victims of Personal Crime: An Empirical
Foundation for a Theory of Personal Victimization. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
Hunter, James and Andromachi Tseloni. 2016. ‘Equity, justice and the crime drop: the case of burglary in
England and Wales’ Crime Science, 5(3); 1-13.
Ignatans, Dainis and Pease, Ken (2015) Distributive Justice and the Crime Drop. In: The Criminal Act: The
Role and Influence of Routine Activity Theory. Palgrave Macmillan, London, UK, pp. 77-87.
Johnson, J.H., Kerper, H.B., Hayes, D.D. and Killenger, G.G. (1973). The Recidivist Victim: A Descriptive
Study. Criminal Justice Monograph vol. IV, no. 1. Institute of Contemporary Corrections and the
Behavioral Sciences, Sam Houston State University: Huntsville, Texas.
Kurland, J., Johnson, S.D. and Tilley, N. (2014) Offences around stadiums: A natural experiment on crime
attraction and generation, Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 51(1): 528.
Laycock, Gloria. 2001. ‘Hypothesis-Based Research: The Repeat Victimization Story’ Criminal Justice
(London: Sage), 1, 1; 59-82.
Laycock, Gloria and Graham Farrell. 2003. ‘Repeat victimization: Lessons for implementing problem-
oriented policing’ in Johannes Knutsson (Ed.) Problem-Oriented Policing: from Innovation to
Mainstream, vol. 15 of Crime Prevention Studies, pp. 150-175.
Levy, M.P. and Tartaro, C. (2010) Repeat victimization: A study of auto theft in Atlantic city using the
WALLS variables to measure environmental indicators, Criminal Justice Policy Review, 21(3): 296318.
Mawby, R.I. (2001). The impact of repeat victimization on burglary victims in East and West Europe. In
Farrell, G. & Pease, K. (Eds.) Repeat Victimization. Monsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press. Pp .69-82.
Moitra, S.D. and Konda, S.L. (2004) ‘An empirical investigation of network attacks on computer systems’,
Computers and Security, 23: 4351.
Morgan, F. (2001) Repeat burglary in a Perth suburb: Indicator of short-term or long-term risk? In: G.
Farrell and K. Pease (eds.) Repeat Victimization. Monsey, New York: Criminal Justice Press, pp. 83118.
Pease, Ken. 1991. “The Kirkholt Project: preventing burglary on a British public housing estate”. Security
Journal, 2(2): 73-77. 411-414.
Pease, K. (1998) Repeat Victimisation: Taking Stock. London: Police Research Group, Home Office.
Pease, K. and Laycock, G. (1996) Revictimization: Reducing the heat on hot victims. Research in Action.
Washington DC: National Institute of Justice.
Pease, K. and Tseloni, A. (2014) Using Modeling to Predict and Prevent Victimization. New York:
Springer.
Planty, M. and Strom, K.J. 2007. Understanding the role of repeat victims in the production of annual US
victimization rates, Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 23(3); 179-200.
Polvi, Natalie, Terah Looman, Charlie Humphreys and Ken Pease. 1991. The time course of repeat
burglary victimization, British Journal of Criminology, 31(4);
Ratcliffe, J. H. and Rengert, G. F. (2008) Near repeat patterns in Philadelphia shootings. Security Journal,
21(1-2), 5876.
Sidebottom, A. 2011. ‘Repeat burglary victimization in Malawi and the influence of housing type and
area-level influence’ Security Journal, 25(3) 265-281.
Sidebottom, A. and Tilley, N. (2016). Designing systems against crime: introducing leaky systems. In N.
Tilley and A. Sidebottom (eds), Handbook of Crime Prevention and Community Safety (2nd ed).
Routledge.
Sherman, L.W., Gartin, P.R. and Buerger, M.E. (1989) Hot spots of predatory crime: Routine activities
and the criminology of place. Criminology, 27(1): 2755.
Short, M. B., M. R. D’Orsogna, P. J. Brantingham, and G. E. Tita. 2009. “Measuring and Modelling Repeat
and Near Repeat Burglary Effects.” Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 25:32539.
Sparks, R., Genn, H. and Dodd, D. (1977) Surveying Victims. Wiley: London.
Tilley, Nick 1993. After Kirkholt - Theory, Method and Results of Replication Evaluations. Crime
Prevention Unit Series Paper No. 47. London: Home Office Police Research Group.
Tilley, N. (1995) Thinking about Crime Prevention Performance Indicators, Crime Prevention
and Detection Series Paper 57, London: Home Office.
Titus, R. M. and A. R. Gover. 2001. ‘Personal fraud: The victims and the scams’ Crime Prevention Studies,
12; 133-151.
Townsley, Michael, Ross Homel and Janet Chaseling. 2003. Infectious repeats: A test of the near repeat
hypothesis. British Journal of Criminology. 43, 615-633.
Townsley, M., Johnson, S.D. and Ratcliffe, J.H. 2008. Space time dynamics of insurgent activity in Iraq,
Security Journal, 21(3); 139-146.
van Dijk, J.J.M. (2001). Attitudes of victims and repeat victims toward the police: Results of the
International Crime Victims Survey. In Farrell, G. & Pease, K. (Eds.), Repeat Victimization. Monsey, NY:
Criminal Justice Press. pp. 27-52.
Weisel, D.L. (2005) Analyzing Repeat Victimization. POP Center Tool Guide Number 4, Center for
Problem-Oriented Policing.
Wells W, Wu L, Ye X (2008) Patterns of near-repeat gun assaults in Houston. Journal of Research in Crime
and Delinquency, 49(2): 186212.
Youtsin TJ, Nobles MR, Ward JT, Cook CL. (2011) Assessing the generalizability of the near repeat
phenomenon, Criminal Justice and Behaviour, 38(10): 10421063.
Note
1
As this chapter goes to press, the Home Office is reviewing its counting procedures.
... Four recurrent findings are identified: (1) a small number of repeat victims typically account for a sizable proportion of all victimisations; (2) repeat victimisation occurs quickly in the wake of an initial victimisation; (3) repeats are highly prevalent in high crime areas; and (4) prior victimisation tends to be a reliable predictor of future victimisation (see e.g. Farrell et al., 1995;Farrell & Pease, 2017;Townsley et al., 2000). ...
... (also known as chronic victims) indicates that they accumulate a disproportionate volume of crime: 2% of the super targets account for 44% of property crime and 1% of them account for 59% of personal crime (Farrell & Pease, 2017). ...
... An extension of RV is known as near repeat victimisation (NRV), for which victimisation is linked to previous victimisation but not necessarily against the same targets (Farrell & Pease, 2014). NRV refers to the phenomenon in which crime clusters in space and time and follows a contagion-like process, whereby targets located close to a prior incident of victimisation show an elevated risk of victimisation in the short term (Farrell & Pease, 2017;Johnson et al., 2007;Townsley, 2008;Townsley et al., 2003). Like RV, 7 Researchers otherwise also argue that repeats do not have to be committed by the same offenders. ...
Thesis
Environmental criminology concerns the role of opportunities (both people and objects) existing in the environment that make crimes more likely to occur. Research consistently shows that opportunity perspectives (particularly with regard to individuals’ lifestyles and routines) help in explaining the prevalence and concentration of crimes. However, there is a paucity of studies investigating crime patterns from an opportunity perspective both outside western countries and in relation to cybercrimes. Hence, it is not clear whether non-Western and online contexts exhibit similar patterns of crime as would be predicted by an opportunity perspective. This thesis is concerned with criminal victimisation in Taiwan – a less researched setting in the field of environmental criminology. It covers both offline victimisation (with a focus on burglary) and online victimisation from the aforementioned opportunity perspective. The goal of this thesis is to identify individual- and area-level characteristics that affect the patterns of victimisation in Taiwan. To achieve this, the thesis draws on a range of secondary datasets, including police recorded crime statistics, the Taiwan Area Victimisation Survey, and the Digital Opportunity Survey for Individuals and Households. With the application of quantitative modelling, the thesis suggests that the generalisability the lifestyle-routine activity approach in explaining crime patterns in Taiwan should be taken with caution. The findings provide partial support for its applicability in relation to burglary and cybercrime in Taiwan. Furthermore, the findings reported here in relation to patterns of repeat and near repeat victimisation depart from those observed in the western literature. The thesis concludes by discussing the implications of the findings for academic research and practice in crime prevention.
... As a result, NRT is considered to be effective at identifying geographical areas for heightened police prevention activity. Since its emergence it has been demonstrated by numerous studies to be effective at predicting the location accuracy of future crimes (Bernasco, 2008;Chainey and Silva, 2016;Johnson andBowers, 2004 andJohnson et al., 2009;Farrell and Pease, 2017;and Wang, 2018). Specifically, the theory has been shown to be consistently accurate when analysed in the context of volume acquisitive crimes such as burglary and vehicle thefts (Pitcher and Johnson, 2011). ...
... Specifically, the theory has been shown to be consistently accurate when analysed in the context of volume acquisitive crimes such as burglary and vehicle thefts (Pitcher and Johnson, 2011). The concept 'flagging' (Farrell and Pease, 2017;Ferguson, 2011;Lui, 2008 andSagovsky andJohnson, 2007), which is outlined within NRT, indicates that an offender will target victims based on their perceived low level of risk. This theory is born from rational choice theory (RCT) (Cornish and Clarke, 1987) which effectively argues that criminals make calculated decisions whether to commit a crime or not based on a complex assessment of factors including the risk of apprehension, ease of commission, and financial reward. ...
Preprint
The use of crime mapping has been used by the police to inform deployment of resources for many decades. Such approaches are commonly used to underpin crime control strategies designed to prevent or reduce acquisitive crimes such as domestic burglary. In recent decades there has been a shift away from simple hot spot identification to more complex geospatial mapping methodologies, such as near repeat analysis which was developed through research regarding burglary victimisation. One of these newly emerging methodologies is built upon the ecological, optimal forager theory (OFT). Research using this theory to examine domestic burglary offending intimated potential for positive results in predicting areas at risk of future crime. This led to a number of police services using crime analysis methodologies built upon OFT to underpin their deployment of resources in an effort to prevent or reduce domestic burglary through increased capable guardianship. However, to date, there has been no detailed examination of how the police services implemented such approaches. As such, this study seeks to fill this gap by examining OFT strategies implemented within 5 police services. By interviewing participants directly involved in the programs the study gathers views and perspectives of its relative success. As a result, we identify that participants felt the strategies produced limited impact on recorded burglary crime. We discuss how despite some positive by-products of the strategies, failure to comprehensively apply the theoretical foundations of OFT, and a variety of implementation failures have undermined the various programs, ultimately impacting their effectiveness.
... As a result, NRT is considered to be effective at identifying geographical areas for heightened police prevention activity. Since its emergence it has been demonstrated by numerous studies to be effective at predicting the location accuracy of future crimes (Bernasco, 2008;Chainey and Silva, 2016;Johnson andBowers, 2004 andJohnson et al., 2009;Farrell and Pease, 2017;and Wang, 2018). Specifically, the theory has been shown to be consistently accurate when analysed in the context of volume acquisitive crimes such as burglary and vehicle thefts (Pitcher and Johnson, 2011). ...
... Specifically, the theory has been shown to be consistently accurate when analysed in the context of volume acquisitive crimes such as burglary and vehicle thefts (Pitcher and Johnson, 2011). The concept 'flagging' (Farrell and Pease, 2017;Ferguson, 2011;Lui, 2008 andSagovsky andJohnson, 2007), which is outlined within NRT, indicates that an offender will target victims based on their perceived low level of risk. This theory is born from rational choice theory (RCT) (Cornish and Clarke, 1987) which effectively argues that criminals make calculated decisions whether to commit a crime or not based on a complex assessment of factors including the risk of apprehension, ease of commission, and financial reward. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Certain aspects of optimal forager theory (OFT), which is drawn from ecology, have shown positive results in predicting areas at risk of future domestic burglary offending. This led to police services developing analysis methods that embraced OFT to underpin their deployment of resources to prevent or reduce domestic burglary. There has been limited examination, using quantitative approaches, of how individual police services have implemented such crime reduction schemes. This study broadens this literature by qualitatively exploring OFT strategies within 5 police services. By interviewing participants involved in the programs the study gathers views and perspectives of the implementations, identifying many positive by-products of the strategies. In contrast, factors affecting the implementation and application of the theoretical framework are also identified. Both good and bad are discussed in the context of their practical implications for police services globally looking to implement crime reduction plans that embrace OFT.
... Pour les années 2015 à 2018 pour la ville de Montréal, 36 219 introductions par effraction ont été enregistrées, soit une moyenne d'un peu plus de 9000 par année. Les données ouvertes de la ville associent chacun des événements àune localisation assez précise, soit l'intersection la plus proche ; une localisation plus précise permettrait possiblement d'identifier les domiciles et les personnes impliqués, ce qui est non seulement une menace à la vie privée des gens, mais aussi une invitation potentielle à la « revictimisation »(Farrell & Pease, 2017). Environ 96% des introductions par effraction ont pu être géocodées avec succès (n = 34 774), ce qui est bien au-delà du seuil minimal de 85% identifié parRatcliffe (2004) ; la majorité des cas qui ont dû être exclus l'ont été parce que l'adresse n'était pas bien identifiée dans la banque de données d'origine.Ainsi, les données du tableau 1 montrent une distribution fortement asymétrique puisqu'aucun cambriolage n'avait été rapporté dans 63,7% des unités géographiques(n=15 090)et que le maximum est de 77 cambriolages pour la période à l'étude. ...
Article
Full-text available
Le cambriolage est un crime particulièrement fréquent dans les grandes villes. L’objectif de ce projet est d’évaluer la présence d’un lien statistique entre la répartition de cambriolages et la propriété de chiens dans les domiciles de la ville de Montréal pour les années 2015 à 2018. Cette étude vise à croiser des données en sources ouvertes de la ville quant au nombre de cambriolages, du registre municipal des animaux de compagnie, ainsi que des données du recensement. Des analyses bivariées et multivariées, telles que les corrélations et une analyse de régression binomiale négative, sont utilisées. Les résultats suggèrent que les chiens ont un fort effet dissuasif sur la commission de cambriolages, présumément en augmentant les coûts du passage à l’acte pour les cambrioleurs potentiels. Cette recherche suggère de nouveau que des moyens de prévention situationnelle peuvent avoir un impact considérable sur le crime.
... In this context, forces must target their available resources more effectively to manage demand (Kearns and Muir, 2019). Research has shown that most demand is attributable to the same offenders, victims and places (Sherman, 2007;Farrell and Pease, 2017), and thus targeting repetition could be an effective use of resources to reduce overall demand (Farrell, 1995;Pease et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Targeting police resources at repeat demand may reduce overall demand. To effectively target resources at repetition requires repeat demand to be accurately measured in police data. Using domestic violence and abuse (DVA) as a case study of repeat demand, this study takes a deep dive into the data and information systems used by one police force to identify the key issues that prevent the effective measurement of repeat DVA-related demand. From observations of the police response to DVA and manual review of 325 DVA case files, four key issues are identified: (1) fragmented units of measurement across multiple information systems; (2) inconsistent recording of personal details; (3) multiple methods of identifying DVA; and (4) the embedding of information in free-text. This paper makes recommendations to improve the measurement of repeat demand in police data, with implications for police practitioners and researchers.
... Static risk is related to the crime opportunity of the area and the flag hypothesis (Tseloni & Pease, 2003), while dynamic risk is related to burglars' rational decision-making model and the boost hypothesis (Bowers & Johnson, 2004). More recently, a degree of interaction between the two hypotheses called the Flag and Boost Interaction (FBI) has been proposed (Farrell & Pease, 2017), in addition to a theory that extends the boost hypothesis to a network approach (Lantz & Ruback, 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
Near-repeat victimization patterns have made predictive models for burglaries possible. While the models have been implemented in different countries, the results obtained have not always been in line with initial expectations; to the point where their real effectiveness has been called into question. The ability to predict crime to improve preventive policing strategies is still under study. This study aims to discover the limitations to and the success of the models that attempt to predict burglaries based on spatiotemporal patterns of the risk of break-ins spreading in geographic proximity to the initial break-ins. A spatiotemporal log-Gaussian Cox process is contemplated to model the generic near-repeat victimization scenario and adjusted using the Integrated Nested Laplace Approximation (INLA) methodology. This approach is highly suitable for studying and describing the near-repeat phenomenon. However, predictions obtained with INLA are quite monotonous, of low variability and do not reproduce well the local and short-term dynamics of burglaries for predictive purposes. The conclusion is that predictive models cannot be restricted exclusively to distance decay risk, but they must be designed to detect other types of spatiotemporal patterns which, among other possibilities, open up the possibility of correlating distant events and clusters. Although other studies have already highlighted this problem, the proposal here is to go one step further and clearly extend the near-repeat spatial patterns to achieve better prediction results.
... Theories within this family focus on explaining the interaction between a motivated offender and the opportunities for committing crimes created by the environment. The most important premise of this perspective is that crime is not randomly distributed but, rather, it is highly concentrated in time and space [21,22] and among offenders and victims [23,24], and that opportunities for crime in the environment also vary over time and space. ...
Article
Full-text available
Albatrosses are among the most threatened seabird species. Often entangled in gillnets or hooked while longline fishing gear is being set, albatrosses are affected by fishing. This is assumed to be especially true in cases where illegal longline fishing vessels are involved, as they are less likely to implement the bycatch mitigation measures implemented to reduce the risk of albatrosses being caught on their hooks. This is the assumption that was tested in the current study, which uses environmental criminology as its guiding theoretical framework. Using the spatial units of one-half-degree by one-half-degree longitude/latitude cells, this research examined the patterns of concentration of potentially illegal longlining efforts and their relationships to commercially sought-out and illegally caught (i.e., CRAAVED—concealable, removable, abundant, accessible, valuable, enjoyable, disposable) fish species concentrations, as well as their effects on the average risk of albatrosses. The results indicated that (a) potentially illegal longlining activity is spatially concentrated; (b) this concentration is exhibited in areas with the highest concentrations of the presence of CRAAVED fish; and (c) the average risk score of albatrosses, as measured by their International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) Red List status, is significantly higher in the areas where illegal longlining vessels are found controlling for the activities of legal longlining vessels. These findings provide strong grounding that illegal longline fishing poses a particularly serious threat to the survival of albatrosses. These activities, however, are not randomly spread across the vast oceans, but rather are highly spatially concentrated. Therefore, the bird conservation lobby should work closely with regional fisheries management organizations to devise and implement targeted interventions aimed at reducing potential illegal longline fishing, which, in turn, will likely have positive effects on albatrosses. ---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- Article here: https://www.mdpi.com/2076-2615/12/7/887/htm
Article
Full-text available
The British Crime Survey (BCS) has made a major contribution to the understanding of repeat and chronic victimisation. BCS evidence on repeats has led to a range of theoretical and methodological developments capable of informing crime control and victim services. The present paper shows that BCS counting conventions mask the extent of chronic victimisation in most official BCS reports. Crucially, where a victim reports multiple linked and similar events, the series is capped at an arbitrary maximum of five incidents. By thus truncating the long statistical "tail" of victimisation, the incidence of personal crime was reduced by at least a third in every BCS sweep since 2001-2002, and by 52% in the 2005-2006 BCS. The incidence of property crime is reduced by up to a quarter, and by 15% in the 2005-2006 BCS. The contribution of harms against those frequently victimised to total crime suffered is thus revealed as even more important than hitherto acknowledged, and personal crime is revealed as constituting a higher proportion of all crime suffered. Acknowledgements This is a revised version of an article presented at the conference to mark Twenty Five
Book
Full-text available
This report pulls together a number of research results from a variety of sources, much of it carried out with Home Office support. The subject of the report is ‘repeat victimisation’ – the paper describes the extent to which victims or places are repeatedly subject to crime and speculates about the implications for prevention. In relation to some offences the repeated vulnerability of particular individuals is self evident – domestic violence is probably the most obvious example. But in relation to other crimes, such as domestic burglary, attacks on schools or car crime the extent to which repetition occurs is far from obvious but clearly shown in the report. Some of the research had been lying in the academic arena for a long time – but its practical significance for prevention and for policing had not been appreciated. The reduction of repeat victimisation in its several manifestations offers a challenge to the police and their partners in crime prevention. The report is intended to provoke discussion and preventive action across a wide field.
Book
Full-text available
Preventing crime and preventing repeat victimisation are priority issues for the criminal justice system in 1999. Numerous research studies have shown that a small minority of offenders is responsible for a large proportion of all offences recorded by the police and, further, that a minority of victims experiences the majority of crime. It is well known that crime incidents are not uniformly distributed across geographic areas. Some neighbourhoods experience more violent crimes than others, and understanding this dynamic is valuable in formulating crime prevention and law enforcement strategies. The Australian Institute of Criminology’s Research and Public Policy Series no. 15, Repeat Victimisation in Australia by Satyanshu Mukherjee and Carlos Carcach, showed that over half of all property crimes (e.g. break and enter, attempted break and enter and motor vehicle theft) occurred in just over a quarter of all households. Households that experienced three or more incidents (10 per cent of all victim households) during the year accounted for 25 per cent of all incidents. The same study showed that about two-thirds of personal crimes (assault, sexual assault and robbery) are experienced by 41 per cent of victims. It also made the point that if, as a crime prevention measure, we wish to target 1000 randomly selected unvictimised households, we may on an average prevent 83 household crimes; but if we select 1000 previously victimised households, we may be able to prevent 286 household crimes. Australian research complements international research on repeat victimisation and the paper reproduced here, written by two leading British researchers, was originally published for the National Institute of Justice (NIJ), Washington DC. The Australian Institute of Criminology is pleased to be able to bring this type of international work to the attention of Australian readers, and sees great value in cross-national fertilisation.
Article
Full-text available
Burglary in England and Wales fell by 67 % between 1993 and 2008/09. This study examines whether this fall was equitable across different population segments (with respect to their socio-economic characteristics) and area types. In particular, it estimates the extent of burglary falls and any changes in the victimisation divide across socio-economic (population) groups taking into account group composition. To this end, it compares their burglary incidence rates based on burglary count models of the 1994 and 2008/09 Crime Survey for England and Wales data. The results show that some socio-economic groups experienced inequitable burglary falls, and relative to others continue to experience burglaries at higher rates after the crime drop than before.
Chapter
Crime statistics in England and Wales have a long history, with the first national crime figures being collected by the Home Office in 1857. Their use extends well beyond being simply an operational tool for the police to allocate officers to crime problems. Since their earliest inception they have been seen as being important in providing both a local and national picture of criminal activity that informs the public and Parliament as to trends in crime (see Home Office, 1993). They have also become key information in determining government policy on crime and allocating central resources to the police and others who combat crime.
Chapter
The present chapter seeks to link two of the central facts concerning victimization by crime in the Western world. The first is that the burden of crime is borne very unequally across areas and within areas across households and individuals (Tseloni et al., 2010). The second is that there has been a very substantial cross-national drop in crime as captured by victimization surveys (van Dijk et al., 2007) (Farrell et al., 2010). The authors seek to establish whether the crime drop has resulted in a more or less equitable distribution of crime across households. Inequality of victimization challenges distributive justice. Harms as well as goods should be distributed equitably. Changes in inequality would suggest whether we should regard the crime drop as unequivocally benign (inequality reducing or neutral) or have reservations about its benefits (inequality increasing). The possible outcomes of the analysis have differing implications for criminal justice in general and policing in particular. There is already evidence that policing concentration at least in England and Wales is not proportionate to the presenting crime problem (Ross & Pease, 2008), and reasons have been suggested for this, the writers’ favoured account being labelled the “winter in Florida, summer in Alaska” paradox (Townsley & Pease, 2002).
Book
Crime prevention should be rational and should be based on the best possible evidence. Decision-makers should weigh heavily any available evidence on what works best. How can a program that has produced no discernable evidence of effectiveness, as shown through numerous evaluations, be considered for implementation? Unfortunately, this happens all the time. Evidence-based crime prevention attempts to overcome this and other obstacles by ensuring that the best available evidence is considered in any decision to implement a program designed to prevent crime. This book is about evidence-based crime prevention. A project of the Campbell Collaboration Crime and Justice Group, Preventing Crime brings together the leading scientific evidence on what works best for a wide range of interventions organized around four important domains in criminology: at-risk children, offenders, victims, and places. It is the first book to assess the effectiveness of criminological interventions using the most rigorous review methodology of the systematic review. It is an indispensable guide to the leading scientific evidence on what works best to prevent crime.