ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Abstract and Figures

Individual differences in the mere willingness to think analytically has been shown to predict religious disbelief. Recently, however, it has been argued that analytic thinkers are not actually less religious; rather, the putative association may be a result of religiosity typically being measured after analytic thinking (an order effect). In light of this possibility, we report four studies in which a negative correlation between religious belief and performance on analytic thinking measures is found when religious belief is measured in a separate session. We also performed a meta-analysis on all previously published studies on the topic along with our four new studies (N = 15,078, k = 31), focusing specifically on the association between performance on the Cognitive Reflection Test (the most widely used individual difference measure of analytic thinking) and religious belief. This meta-analysis revealed an overall negative correlation (r) of -.18, 95% CI [-.21, -.16]. Although this correlation is modest, self-identified atheists (N = 133) scored 18.7% higher than religiously affiliated individuals (N = 597) on a composite measure of analytic thinking administered across our four new studies (d = .72). Our results indicate that the association between analytic thinking and religious disbelief is not caused by a simple order effect. There is good evidence that atheists and agnostics are more reflective than religious believers.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Atheists and Agnostics Are More Reflective
than Religious Believers: Four Empirical
Studies and a Meta-Analysis
Gordon Pennycook
1
*, Robert M. Ross
2,3
, Derek J. Koehler
1
, Jonathan A. Fugelsang
1
1Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Canada, 2Department of Psychology, Royal
Holloway, University of London, London, United Kingdom, 3ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its
Disorders, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
*gpennyco@uwaterloo.ca
Abstract
Individual differences in the mere willingness to think analytically has been shown to predict
religious disbelief. Recently, however, it has been argued that analytic thinkers are not actu-
ally less religious; rather, the putative association may be a result of religiosity typically
being measured after analytic thinking (an order effect). In light of this possibility, we report
four studies in which a negative correlation between religious belief and performance on
analytic thinking measures is found when religious belief is measured in a separate session.
We also performed a meta-analysis on all previously published studies on the topic along
with our four new studies (N= 15,078, k= 31), focusing specifically on the association
between performance on the Cognitive Reflection Test (the most widely used individual dif-
ference measure of analytic thinking) and religious belief. This meta-analysis revealed an
overall negative correlation (r) of -.18, 95% CI [-.21, -.16]. Although this correlation is mod-
est, self-identified atheists (N= 133) scored 18.7% higher than religiously affiliated individu-
als (N= 597) on a composite measure of analytic thinking administered across our four new
studies (d= .72). Our results indicate that the association between analytic thinking and reli-
gious disbelief is not caused by a simple order effect. There is good evidence that atheists
and agnostics are more reflective than religious believers.
Introduction
Dual-process theories distinguish between two fundamentally different types of cognitive pro-
cesses [1]: Type 1 processes that are intuitive and autonomously cued and Type 2 processes
that are reflective and require working memory. One of the most important insights that has
emerged from the dual-process literature is that the distinction between intuition and reflection
is of consequence to more than just researchers interested in thinking and reasoning [2]. For
example, the propensity to engage analytic reasoning (as distinct, conceptually, from cognitive
ability) predicts paranormal disbelief [3,4], acceptance of science [5,6], less traditional moral
values and judgments [7,8], less reliance on Smartphones as an external source of information
[9], and a lowered receptivity to bullshit [10]. These results indicate that the interplay between
intuitive and analytic processes is an important component of human cognition. The degree to
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 1/18
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Pennycook G, Ross RM, Koehler DJ,
Fugelsang JA (2016) Atheists and Agnostics Are
More Reflective than Religious Believers: Four
Empirical Studies and a Meta-Analysis. PLoS ONE
11(4): e0153039. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039
Editor: Michiel van Elk, University of Amsterdam,
NETHERLANDS
Received: November 24, 2015
Accepted: March 21, 2016
Published: April 7, 2016
Copyright: © 2016 Pennycook et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits
unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any
medium, provided the original author and source are
credited.
Data Availability Statement: All relevant data are
within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding: This research was supported by a
Graduate Scholarship from the Natural Sciences and
Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC)
to GP.
Competing Interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
which analytic (Type 2) processes influence reasoning and decision making in lab studies is at
least somewhat predictive of specific theoretically grounded real world outcomes [2].
Most importantly for the present investigation, there is now a good deal of evidence that
analytic thinking disposition (analytic cognitive style) is negatively associated with religious
belief [11]. For example, religious believers perform worse than non-believers on cognitive
tests that cue incorrect intuitive responses [4,12,13]. Consider, for instance, the bat-and-ball
problem from the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT; [14]): A bat and a ball cost $1.10 in total.
The bat costs $1.00 more than the ball. How much does the ball cost?The majority of partici-
pants respond 10 centsto this question (e.g., 64.9% in [15]). This is the response that typi-
cally comes to mind upon an initial read of the problem, but it is incorrect. If the ball is $0.10,
then the bat must be $1.10 and in total they would be $1.20. To recognize that the intuitive
response is incorrect, the participant must be willing to stop and think analytically about the
problem despite having what seems to be a plausible intuitive response. Performance on the
CRT is therefore thought to index, at least to some degree, a willingness or propensity to engage
Type 2/analytic processing [14,16,17,18]. Naturally, it also requires some degree of cognitive
ability (and, in particular, numeracy) to solve the arithmetic involved in each of the problems
[19]. As a consequence, a number of studies have also demonstrated that there is a negative
association between religious belief and CRT performance (and performance on related tasks)
even after controlling for measures of cognitive ability and intelligence ([4,7,13,20], but see
[21]). This negative association also remains robust in regression analyses that control for vari-
ous demographic factors (e.g., age, sex, education; see [11] for a review). Furthermore, religious
believers spend less time reasoning when given problems in a lab study [20,22], as would be
expected if they are less willing to engage slow, deliberative reasoning processes. Finally, there
is experimental evidence that inducing an analytic or reflective mindset at least temporarily
decreases self-reported religious belief [12,13,23].
These results were challenged in a recent paper by Finley, Tang, and Schmeichel [24]. Spe-
cifically, they hypothesized that the association between analytic thinking and religious belief
depends on the order in which analytic thinking and religious belief measures are presented. In
a high powered experiment, the authors replicated the negative association between religious
belief and CRT performance when the CRT was administered immediately prior to the reli-
gious belief measure. However, they did not find an association when the CRT was adminis-
tered after the measure of religious belief. This is a potentially important finding because many
of the previously cited studies measured analytic thinking before religious belief (e.g., [4,7,12,
20]).
Importantly, Finley et al. do not question the claim that analytic thinking decreases religious
belief, which is supported by experimental evidence [12,13,23]. Rather, they argue that more
analytic thinkers are not necessarily less religious(abstract, p.1). According to Finley et al., the
idea that analytic thinking decreases religious belief at the state level does not necessarily con-
tradict the claim that they are not associated at the trait level. Experiments in which analytic
thinking decreases religious belief serve only as an existence proofit is still quite possible that
analytic thinking is not a meaningful component of religious cognition in most peoples every-
day lives. In other words, the modal analytic thinking disposition may not be sufficiently ana-
lytic to be of consequence for religious belief. This line of reasoning seems to contradict the
wealth of data that indicates that analytic thinking does have consequences for our everyday
lives (as summarized above; see [2] for a review), but religious belief may be an exception.
Finley et al. argue that asking participants to first indicate their religious belief and then
answer reasoning problems is the purest way to test whether there is a genuine association
between the two variables. In support of this contention, the authors point to previous research
wherein moral judgment was only associated with CRT performance if the CRT was given
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 2/18
prior to the moral judgment task [25]. It may be that the CRT (and, presumably, related mea-
sures) induces an analytic mindset, which then somehow interacts with self-reported religious
belief. Finley et al. are, unfortunately, not entirely clear about why they make this prediction.
They state that (p. 2): By having participants complete the CRT prior to reporting religious
beliefs, Gervais and Norenzayan likely activated an analytic mindset in participants who
answered analytically (i.e., correctly), temporarily suppressing religious beliefs. As a result, the
strength of the relationship between individual differences in analytic thinking and religious
beliefs may have been inflated by first activating the analytic system, rather than representing a
pure (i.e., confound free) relationship between individual differences in analytic thinking and
religious beliefs.Finley et al. qualify their claim by arguing that only those participants who
are particularly analytic in the first place would be put in an analytic mindset by the CRT.
However, if this is the case, only those participants who have religious beliefs in the first place
would have their religious beliefs suppressed. Presumably, then, the differences between reli-
gious disbelievers (those scoring near zero on a religious belief measure) and the various levels
of religious believers would decrease if the CRT is presented first. Or, perhaps, there is a more
complex interaction that requires a necessary level of analytic cognitive style for the CRT to
induce an analytic mindset and that only affects some levels of religious belief and not others.
These speculations illustrate that the mechanisms proposed by Finley et al. are not at all
straightforward. Even if the association between CRT and religious belief is only evident when
religious belief is measured first, it is not clear why this would be the case.
Given this uncertainty, the key question is whether one or the other presentation order is
more likely to reflect the actual association (or lack thereof) between analytic thinking and reli-
gious belief. There is some extant data that indicates that, contrary to Finley et al.s claims, the
apparent lack of correlation when religious belief was measured first in their study is the excep-
tion, not the rule. First, six published studies (2 of which were published before Finley et al.)
had participants indicate their religious belief prior to solving the CRT and found significant
associations [26,27,28,29]. In each case, however, the additional tasks were included in-
between the religiosity measure and the CRT. This may explain the discrepancy between these
studies and Finley et al. (although, of course, they cannot be accounted for given Finley et al.s
hypothesis about scale administration order).
Second, there is evidence for the association from large surveys with intervening tasks sepa-
rating analytic thinking and religious belief measures (e.g., [7,10,13,30]). Finley et al. disputed
this evidence by suggesting that these surveys may have included other measures that could
have induced an analytic mindset. We are not convinced. A number of very different interven-
ing tasks have been used, including, for example, a moral values questionnaire [7], a bullshit
receptivity scale and ontological confusions measure [10], and demographic questions [30].
Regardless, it is possible that the CRT induces an analytic mindset that persists during these
intervening tasks.
Third, some investigations of analytic thinking and religiosity have included categorical var-
iables that should, in theory, be less susceptible to contextual or experimental modification. For
example, Pennycook et al. [4] found an association between CRT performance and the type of
God belief (or disbelief). Namely, those who believed in a conventional personalGod scored
the lowest and those who lacked any belief in God (i.e., atheists) scored the highest on the CRT.
It seems unlikely that an analytic mindset would be sufficient to make a personal God theist
into an atheist in the context of a single study session.
Finally, Pennycook et al. [22] did not measure religious belief in the same study session as
analytic thinking and nonetheless found a negative association between the variables. This is
inarguably the purest way to investigate a possible association. For this, Pennycook et al.s par-
ticipants completed a religious belief scale in a separate mass testingsurvey that opened for
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 3/18
participation at the beginning of the semester. The mass testing survey included a number of
scales that were submitted by numerous research groups in the same department (e.g., bore-
dom proneness, a Big Five personality scale), but no analytic thinking measures. Importantly,
the order of the scales was randomized for each participant, with the exception of a boredom
proneness questionnaire that always came first and an aggression questionnaire that always
came last. In this case, religious belief was measured prior to analytic thinking and, more
importantly, in the context of an entirely different study.
Pennycook et al. [22] also reported an association between response time on a base-rate
neglect task and self-reported religious affiliation. This question was completed in a pre-
screenquestionnaire that was separate from both the analytic thinking tasks and the religious
belief scale (Note that although the pre-screen questionnaire is typically used for data screen-
ing, we did not restrict participation in the discussed study [22] or in any of the new studies
reported here). For this, participants simply selected which of a series of religious affiliations or
disaffiliations they most strongly identified with (e.g., Christian, Muslim, Agnostic, Atheist,
None). The pre-screen questionnaire only included demographic questions of this sort and the
religious affiliation question came after questions about ethnic identification. Regardless, as
mentioned above, it seems unlikely that an analytic mindset (if it were somehow present)
would cause someone who identifies as a Christian to suddenly identify as an atheist (for
example).
These results represent a strong challenge to Finley et al.s claim that there is no genuine
association between analytic thinking and religious disbelief. It is unlikely that participants
were in a particularly analytic mindset when indicating their level of religiosity in the mass test-
ing and pre-screen surveys, yet the association was present in each of these studies. Nonethe-
less, it may still be the case that the link between analytic thought and religious belief is more
tenuous than previously reported(p. 1). As such, we report four additional studies in which
analytic thinking and religious belief were measured in separate sessions. In these studies, reli-
gious belief was measured in a mass-testing survey administered to university students at the
beginning of four different academic semesters. Participants also indicated their religious affili-
ation (or lack thereof) in a separate pre-screen questionnaire. Participants who completed
these questionnaires were then permitted to sign up for an online study with a battery of cogni-
tive tasks. However, when this permission was granted there was no connection made between
the online study and the mass testing or pre-screen surveys (i.e., the online study was referred
to as a Thinking Styles and Reasoningstudy and neither the mass testing nor pre-screen ses-
sions were mentioned). Finding a negative correlation between performance on analytic think-
ing and religious belief and affiliation across each of the four studies would constitute strong
evidence for a veridical trait-level relation. The scales that were administered directly before
the religious belief measures differed across the four studies (see S1 Text for a breakdown) and
their order within the mass testing survey was always randomized. As such, the likelihood that
any association can be explained by an unexpected confound (which, in our view, is low in the
first place) decreases with the increasing number of studies.
Method
Ethics statement
These studies were approved by a University of Waterloo Research Ethics Committee. Partici-
pants signed and received separate consent forms for the mass testing and analytic thinking
surveys, which were all completed online. All participants indicated their willingness to consent
via button press. Participants received course credit for the mass testing and analytic thinking
surveys.
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 4/18
Participants
Participants completed a series of reasoning problems and cognitive ability measures as part of
a larger project on the everyday consequences of analytic thinking (see [2]). This includes, but
is not limited to, religious belief. All participants were University of Waterloo students who
signed up for a study on thinking styles and reasoningonline through the participant pool.
The only exclusion criteria was that participants had to have completed the department-wide
mass-testing and pre-screen questionnaires prior to signing up for the thinking styles study
(not every student in the participant pool completes one or both of these questionnaires). Par-
ticipants were not permitted to sign up for the study more than once (i.e., someone who partic-
ipated in Study 1 was not permitted to sign up for Studies 24 in the subsequent semesters).
Demographic characteristics of the participants can be found in Table 1.
Study 1. We had complete data for 381 participants. At the end of the analytic thinking
survey (but not in the mass testing or pre-screen surveys), participants were given an attention
check question. The same instruction check was used for all four studies, with some slight vari-
ations (see below). For this, they were presented with a list of common activities and, in the
instruction box, it read: Below is a list of leisure activities. If you are reading this, please choose
the otherbox below and write I read the instructions.Nine participants failed the instruc-
tion check and were excluded from subsequent analysis. This left us with 372 participants (see
Table 1). We also asked participants if they had seen the CRT before and 77 participants
(20.7%) responded affirmatively. Their data were retained but analyses are reported with and
without these participants. Study 1 was completed in the winter term of 2013. A subset of the
data from Study 1, 3, and 4 were previously published in an investigation of CRT scoring strate-
gies [15]. For this, participants who completed the analytic thinking survey were permitted to
complete a second survey with analytic thinking disposition measures and this subset of partic-
ipants are included in Pennycook et al. [15]. In the current studies, the full sample is included
and additional data from thinking disposition questionnaires is reported in S2 Text. The results
for the thinking disposition questionnaires parallel our performance-based findings.
Study 2. We had complete data for 158 participants. Nine participants failed the instruc-
tion check and were excluded from subsequent analysis. This left us with 149 participants (see
Table 1), 20.7% (N= 31) of which responded affirmatively when asked if they have seen the
CRT before. Study 2 was completed in the spring term of 2013.
Study 3. We had complete data for 406 participants. The attention check was made more
difficult for Studies 3 and 4 by inserting an introductory screen prior to the instruction check
that said: For the final part of the study, we are interested in the types of things that you do in
your spare time. This had a large effect: 127 participants failed and were excluded from subse-
quent analysis. This left us with 279 participants (see Table 1), 21.5% (N= 60) of which
responded affirmatively when asked if they have seen the CRT before. Study 3 was completed
Table 1. Demographics for full participant samples (i.e., excluding those who failed the attention
check) in Studies 14.
Study 1 Study 2 Study 3 Study 4
N
females
265 99 205 192
N
males
107 50 73 75
N
total
372 149 279
a
267
Age (SD) 20.3 (3.9) 21.7 (4.9) 20.2 (3.9) 20.7 (5.2)
a
One participant did not indicate their gender.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039.t001
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 5/18
in the fall term of 2013. Components of this data set were also previously published in an inves-
tigation of analytic thinking and smartphone use [9].
Study 4. We had complete data for 398 participants. One hundred and thirty one partici-
pants failed the instruction check and were excluded from subsequent analysis. This left us
with 267 participants (see Table 1), 25.1% (N= 67) of which responded affirmatively when
asked if they have seen the CRT before. Study 4 was completed in the winter term of 2014.
Materials
All items can be found in S3 Text. A breakdown of the materials for each study can be found in
Table 2. Base-rate neglect problems, used in Studies 1 and 2, were replaced with a longer heu-
ristics and biases battery in Studies 3 and 4.
Religious beliefs. The religious belief scale from the mass testing survey included statements
about eight conventional religious beliefs (Pennycook et al. [22], Study 2): heaven, hell, miracles,
afterlife, angels, demons, soul, and the devil/Satan. Participants indicated their agreement/disagree-
ment with the statements (where agreement meant that they held the belief in question) on the fol-
lowing 5-point scale: 1) I strongly disagree, 2) I disagree, 3) I dont know, 4) I agree, 5) I strongly
agree. The scale had good internal consistency: Cronbachsα= 0.94 in each of the four studies and
in the combined data set. Participants also indicated the typeofGodthattheybelievedinona7
point scale [4]: 1) A personal God, 2) God as an impersonal force, 3) A God who created every-
thing, but does not intervene in human affairs [Deism], 4) DontknowwhetherornotanyGods
exist [Negative Agnostic], 5) DontknowwhetherornotanyGodsexistandnooneelsedoes
either [Positive Agnostic], 6) I dont believe in Gods of any sort [Negative Atheist], and 7) I believe
that God does not exist [Positive Atheist]. To ease exposition, a theism measure was created by
combining theists (options 13), agnostics (options 4 & 5), and atheists (options 6 & 7).
For the religious affiliation question in the pre-screen survey, participants were presented
with a list of religious affiliations and asked to select the option that they most strongly identi-
fied with. The list included the following options: Agnostic, Atheist, Bahai, Buddhist, Chinese
Traditional, Christian, Christian (specifically Catholic), Christian (specifically Protestant),
Hindu, Humanist, Jewish, Muslim, No religion, Sikh, Taoist, and Other/not listed. Across the
entire data set (i.e., Studies 14 combined), 13.3% (N= 142) of the sample selected agnostic,
12.5% (N= 133) selected atheist, 16% (N= 171) of the sample chose no religion, 42%
(N= 448) of the sample selected one of the three Christian options, 14.8% (N= 149) chose a
non-Christian religious affiliation. The remaining 2.2% (N= 24) did not provide a response
and were excluded from the affiliation analysis.
Table 2. Materials for Studies 14. CRT = Cognitive Reflection Test.
Study 1 Study 2 Study 3 Study 4
Religiosity Religious Belief Scale XXXX
God Type XXXX
Religious Afliation XXXX
Analytic Cognitive Style CRT (original) XXXX
CRT (additional) X
a
XX
Base-Rate Neglect X X
Heuristics/Biases X X
Cognitive Ability Numeracy XXXX
Wordsum XXXX
a
The additional CRT questions in Study 1 (N= 3) differed from those used in Studies 3 and 4 (N= 4; see S3 Text.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039.t002
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 6/18
Analytic cognitive style. Multiple measures of analytic cognitive style were included
across the four studies. Each measure is intended to cue an incorrect intuitive response that
requires additional analytic processing to override. The original 3-item CRT [14] was included
in each study. Additional CRT items were added for Studies 1, 3, and 4. Three items were
added for Study 1 and four items were added for Studies 3 and 4 [17]. In Studies 1 and 2, we
also included 6 incongruent base-rate neglect problems [22]. These problems were intermixed
with 6 congruent and 6 neutral problems. The following is an incongruent problem [31]:
In a study 1000 people were tested. Among the participants there were 995 nurses and 5
doctors. Paul is a randomly chosen participant of this study. Paul is 34 years old. He lives in
a beautiful home in a posh suburb. He is well spoken and very interested in politics. He
invests a lot of time in his career. What is most likely?
(a). Paul is a nurse.
(b). Paul is a doctor.
Base-rate neglect refers to the propensity for individuals to underweight or ignore the base-
rate information (i.e., 995 nurses/5 doctors) in lieu of the more intuitive stereotypical informa-
tion (i.e., Paul more closely resembles the stereotype of a doctor than a nurse). Incongruent
problems contain a conflict between base-rate and stereotype (as above) whereas both sources
of information suggest the same response for congruent problems. Neutral problems do not
contain stereotypes in the personality description. The proportion of base-rate responses for
incongruent problems has been shown to correlate negatively with religious belief in prior
work [4,22]. Finally, in Studies 3 and 4, a 14-item battery of heuristics and biases problems
was administered [9,10,16,17]. The battery included problems such as the conjunction fallacy
and the gamblers fallacy.
Cognitive ability. Measures of cognitive ability were included as control variables in each
of the four studies. For this, a 12-item verbal intelligence test (the Wordsum[32]) was
administered in each study. Participants also completed a 3-item numeracy test [33] in Studies
1, 3, and 4. This was increased to a 5-item test in Study 2 [34].
Procedure
Mass testing. The religious belief scale was administered in online mass testing surveys
with a number of different scales. These scales differed across the four studies (see S1 Text for a
breakdown), but their order within the mass testing survey was always randomized.
Pre-screen. The religious affiliation question always came after a set of demographic ques-
tions taken in an online pre-screen questionnaire.
Analytic thinking survey. Participants first completed the base-rate neglect/heuristics and
biases problems (depending on which study, see Table 2), followed by numeracy and the CRT.
The Wordsum was always administered last.
Order. Participants had to complete the mass testing and pre-screen surveys before they
were eligible to sign up for the primary analytic thinking survey. The vast majority of partici-
pants completed the analytic thinking survey on a different day than the mass testing and/or
pre-screen surveys. However, a relatively small proportion did complete the analytic thinking
survey on the same day as the mass testing (8.5% of the sample) or pre-screen (3.9% of the sam-
ple) surveys. Nonetheless, there was nothing linking the analytic thinking survey to any indi-
vidual measure within the mass testing or pre-screen batteries and the results are essentially
identical for participants who completed the surveys on the same day (see S4 Text). We there-
fore retained the full sample in the following analyses.
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 7/18
Results
Descriptive statistics for cognitive variables can be found in Table 3. Data for the original
3-item CRT are reported with (CRT
1
) and without (CRT
2
) participants who indicated seeing
the measure before. An analytic cognitive style (ACS) measure was computed by taking the
mean of the original CRT (including participants who had seen it before, CRT
1
), and, when
appropriate, the extended CRT measure (CRT
3
), base-rate neglect, and heuristics/biases. A cor-
relation matrix for all cognitive variables collapsing across study can be found in Table 4.
Pearson product-moment correlations (r) between the religious belief scale (taken in the
mass-testing survey) and performance on cognitive tests is reported in Table 5. Consistent with
previous research, religious belief was significantly negatively correlated with every analytic
cognitive style measure in each of the four studies and in the combined data set (rs ranging
from -.16 to -.26). This association was strongest for the composite ACS measure (rs ranging
from -.20 to -.29). The negative correlation between the composite ACS measure and religious
belief in the combined data set (N= 1,065) was -.26, 95% confidence interval (CI) [-.32, -.20].
This is in the middle of the range reported in the three original studies [4,12,13] using the
Table 3. Mean proportion correct and associated standard deviations and skewness (in brackets: SD /
Skew
) for cognitive variables.
Study 1 Study 2 Study 3 Study 4 Combined
CRT
1
.39 (.37 /
.42
) .40 (.39 /
.41
) .37 (.36 /
.46
) .43 (.38 /
.28
) .40 (.37 /
.40
)
CRT
2
.38 (.36 /
.46
) .40 (.39 /
.43
) .36 (.36 /
.51
) .42 (.39 /
.32
) .39 (.37 /
.43
)
CRT
3
.36 (.33 /
.53
) - .41 (.33 /
.35
) .45 (.32 /
.26
) .40 (.33 /
.38
)
Base-Rate Neglect .35 (.36 /
.72
) .38 (.34 /
.58
) - - .36 (.35 /
.68
)
Heuristics/ Biases - - .52 (.18 /
.44
) .53 (.17 /
.19
) .52 (.17 /
.32
)
ACS .37 (.27 /
.50
) .39 (.29 /
.33
) .43 (.24 /
.56
) .47 (.24 /
.34
) .41 (.26 /
.38
)
Numeracy .73 (.30 /
-.78
) .80 (.24 /
-1.12
) .73 (.28 /
-.78
) .76 (.30 /
-1.01
) .75 (.29 /
-.89
)
Wordsum .59 (.18 /
-.06
) .63 (.17 /
-.17
) .60 (.17 /
.15
) .62 (.18 /
-.12
) .60 (.18 /
-.05
)
CRT = Cognitive Reection Test
CRT
1
= Accuracy on original 3-item CRT
CRT
2
= Excludes participants who indicated seeing the CRT before
CRT
3
= Accuracy on additional CRT problems; ACS = Analytic Cognitive Style (mean of CRT
1
, CRT
3
, Base-Rate Neglect, Heuristics/Biases).
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039.t003
Table 4. Correlations (r) among cognitive variables, collapsing across all four studies.
1 2 345 67
1. CRT
1
-
2. CRT
2
--
3. CRT
3
.57***
(918)
.57***
(884)
-
4. Base-Rate Neglect .26***
(520)
.23***
(506)
.23***
(372)
-
5. Heuristics/Biases .51***
(546)
.50***
(523)
.48***
(546)
--
6. Numeracy .42***
(1066)
.42***
(1029)
.38***
(918)
.21***
(521)
.41***
(546)
-
7. Wordsum .38***
(1066)
.37***
(1029)
.35***
(918)
.22***
(521)
.41***
(546)
.30***
(1067)
-
***indicates p<.001.
CRT = Cognitive Reection Test
CRT
1
= Accuracy on original 3-item CRT
CRT
2
= Excludes participants who indicated seeing the CRT before
CRT
3
= Accuracy on additional CRT problems. Nfor each correlation listed in brackets.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039.t004
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 8/18
CRT (rs ranging from -.18 to -.33), as would be expected if those studies were measuring a gen-
uine correlation between religious belief and analytic cognitive style. An interesting additional
observation is that excluding participants who had previously seen the CRT did not have an
effect on the association with religious belief (both rs = .22 in the combined data set).
The negative association between religious belief and cognitive ability was less consistent.
Both numeracy and verbal intelligence (Wordsum) were significantly negatively correlated
with religious belief in two of four studies and in the combined data set. To further investigate
the associations between cognitive style, cognitive ability, and religious belief, we ran a regres-
sion analysis on the combined data set with religious belief as the dependent variable and ACS,
numeracy, and verbal intelligence as predictors. Analytic cognitive style (β= -.24, p<.001,
95% CI [-.17, -.31]) and verbal intelligence (β= -.07, p= .026, 95% CI [-.01, -.14]) emerged as
significant independent predictors whereas numeracy was not significant (β= .03, p= .459,
95% CI [-.04, .09]). These results did not change when dummy variables for Study were
included in the regression model (Analytic cognitive style: β= -.24, p<.001, 95% CI [-.17,
-.31]; verbal intelligence: β= -.07, p= .031, 95% CI [-.01, -.14]; numeracy: β= .03, p= .407,
95% CI [-.04, .09]).
To analyze the potential difference between theists and non-theists (agnostics and atheists),
we ran individual one-way ANOVAs for each cognitive measure. There was a main effect of
theism in each case, all Fs>4.28, ps<.015, and for the composite ACS measure (see Fig 1), F
(2, 1061) = 22.29, MSE = .065, p<.001, ƞ
2
= .04, indicating increasing performance as level of
theism decreased (i.e., from theist to agnostic to atheist). A post-hoc Tukey HSD test revealed
significant differences between each of the groups, with agnostics scoring higher than theists
(p= .001) and atheists scoring higher than agnostics (p= .01). Atheists scored 13.5% higher
than theists on the ACS measures, t(768) = 6.40, SE = .02, p<.001, d= .52.
We turn next to the religious affiliation question administered in the pre-screen surveys.
For this, we created four groups using the self-reported affiliations. We focused on the com-
bined data for this analysis to ensure that there was a sufficient sample size in each group. All
participants who indicated a religious affiliation were put in a single religiously affiliated
group (N= 597). Three non-religious groups were then created based on an increasing level of
disaffiliation. Namely, those who indicated having no religion (None) were put in a group
(N= 171) and self-identified agnostics (N= 142) and atheists (N= 133) were put into separate
Table 5. Correlations (r) between religious belief (taken in a mass-testing survey) and performance on cognitive tests.
CRT
1
CRT
2
CRT
3
Base-Rate Neglect Heuristics/Biases ACS Numeracy Wordsum
Study 1 -.26***
(372)
-.24***
(361)
-.17**
(372)
-.23***
(372)
- -.29***
(372)
-.13*
(372)
-.17**
(372)
Study 2 -.21*
(148)
-.22**
(145)
- -.25**
(149)
- -.29***
(149)
-.17*
(149)
-.10
(149)
Study 3 -.17**
(277)
-.16*
(267)
-.16**
(277)
- -.16**
(277)
-.20**
(277)
-.08
(277)
-.10
(277)
Study 4 -.23***
(267)
-.25***
(254)
-.21**
(267)
- -.21**
(267)
-.26***
(267)
-.05
(267)
-.26***
(267)
Combined -.22***
(1064)
-.22***
(1027)
-.18***
(916)
-.23***
(521)
-.18***
(544)
-.26***
(1065)
-.11**
(1065)
-.17***
(1065)
***indicates p<.001
**indicates p <.01
*indicates p <.05.
CRT = Cognitive Reection Test
CRT
1
= Accuracy on original 3-item CRT
CRT
2
= Excludes participants who indicated seeing the CRT before
CRT
3
= Accuracy on additional CRT problems; ACS = Analytic Cognitive Style (mean of CRT
1
, CRT
3
, Base-Rate Neglect, Heuristics/Biases). Nfor each
correlation listed in brackets.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039.t005
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 9/18
groups. Individual one-way ANOVAs revealed that performance on every cognitive measure
significantly differed across the four religious affiliation groups (all Fs>5.84, all ps.001).
Focusing specifically on the composite ACS measure, there was a robust difference across
groups, F(3, 1039) = 25.17, MSE = .064, p<.001, ƞ
2
= .07. As is evident from Fig 2, atheists
scored the highest, followed by agnostics, nones, and finally, the religiously affiliated. A post-
hoc Tukey HSD test revealed that the religiously affiliated and noneswere a homogeneous
subset and the agnostics and atheists were a second homogeneous subset (ps<.05). In other
words, the religiously disaffiliated (atheists and agnostics) scored higher than the religiously
affiliated and the religiously apathetic (nones). This is not a small effect: atheists scored
18.7% higher on the ACS composite than the religiously affiliated, t(728) = 7.69, SE = .02, p<
.001, d= .72.
Meta-Analysis
The results from these four studies provide strong support for the claim that atheists and
agnostics are genuinely more reflective than are religious believers. Nonetheless, it may be the
case that the size of the correlation between analytic thinking and religious belief reported in
earlier studies has been inflated due to the CRT being presented before the religiosity questions.
To investigate, we summarized the results of studies that have reported a zero-order correlation
Fig 1. Mean accuracy on Analytic Cognitive Style (ACS) measures as a function of theism (taken in a
mass testing survey). Ns = 570, 294, 200 (respectively).
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039.g001
Fig 2. Mean accuracy on Analytic Cognitive Style (ACS) measures as a function of religious affiliation
(taken in a pre-screen survey). Ns = 597, 171, 142, 133 (respectively).
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039.g002
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 10 / 18
between at least one behavioral measure of analytic thinking (e.g., the CRT) and religiosity
(Table 6). The order in which the analytic thinking/religiosity measures were administered for
each of the studies is also identified in Table 6.
As a procedure for identifying candidate studies, the primary author manually searched
each article that cited any of the three original CRT/religiosity studies [4,12,13] via Google
Scholar. Thirty-five studies were identified, including Finley et al.s[24] two conditions (which
we treat as two studies for the purpose of this analysis), the four studies from the present paper,
an unpublished study from a Masters thesis [26], and 8 studies (across 2 articles, [28,29]) that
were published after Finley et al. Thirty-one of these studies found a statistically significant
negative association between analytic thinking and religious belief, and four did not. However,
two of these non-significant results (rs = -.15 and -.23 from [22] and [29], respectively) were
within the range of previous work but had relatively small samples. The only two strong excep-
tions are from Finley et al. [24] and McCutcheon et al. [36]. Arguably, three large-sample stud-
ies in which the correlation between CRT performance and religiosity was significant, but quite
modest could also be considered partial exceptions [5,30]. However, in all three cases there
were reasons why the correlation may have been attenuated. In the case of Browne et al. [30]
(rs = -.08 and -.11), the CRT was administered in a telephone interview and, likely as a conse-
quence, their participants did particularly poorly (Mean accuracy = 0.46/3) thereby restricting
the range. Similarly, Gervais[5] samples (rs = -.09 and -.10) scored particularly low on the
CRT (Means = 0.69 and 0.72 in Studies 1 and 2, respectively). Moreover, Gervais sampled from
undergraduate students at the University of Kentucky who were highly religious (Mean belief
in God was 5.88/7 in Study 1 and 76.7/100 in Study 2), indicating that the attenuated correla-
tions may be the result of the restricted range for both the CRT and religious belief measure.
We re-analyzed Gervaisdata (which is available online; see Table 6 notes) and found that self-
identified agnostics/atheists (N= 69) scored almost 20% higher on the CRT (Mean = 1.20,
SD = 1.17) than did the religiously affiliated (N= 517, Mean = 0.64, SD = 0.99), t(584) = 4.34,
SE = .13, p<.001, d= .52. This is similar to the religious affiliation results from our four new
studies.
We used meta-analysis to estimate the overall effect size of the association between religios-
ity and CRT scores. We focus specifically on the CRT as it is the most common measure across
these studies and was the measure that Finley et al. employed. For the studies that used more
than one religiosity measure, we chose the measure that was most closely related to religious
belief (e.g., God, supernatural agents, faith). This is because the theoretical association between
analytic thinking and religiosity pertains specifically to supernatural beliefs and not necessarily
religious participation or practice [11]. Finally, three studies used an intuitive scoring
approach for the CRT (see Table 6) in which performance was scored according to the number
of intuitive (modal) responses instead of the number of correct responses. Given the strong
correlation between the outcomes of these scoring techniques (e.g., r= -.85 in [15]), we simply
reversed the sign of the correlation in the meta-analysis for the three relevant cases.
We converted rscores into Fishers z-scores to estimate uncertainty in effect sizes, and
back-transformed Fishers z-scores to rscores for interpretation [39]. We conducted a random
effects meta-analysis using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis version 3.3.070 [40,41] and exam-
ined the data for publication bias using a funnel plot and Eggers regression test for funnel plot
asymmetry [42].
Fig 3 shows a forest plot for the meta-analysis. The analysis indicates that there is a negative
association between CRT score and religious belief, r= -.183 (95% CI [-.208, -.157],
N= 15,078, k= 31). It is noteworthy that this overall effect size is the same as was reported in
the very first study on the topic by Shenhav, Rand, and Greene [13]. Nonetheless, there was sig-
nificant heterogeneity among the studies (Q= 64.16, p<.001, I
2
= 53.24%), which indicates
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 11 / 18
Table 6. Summary of studies reporting a correlation (r) between a behavioral measure of analytic thinkingand religiosity (variously measured).
Significant correlations are in bold.
Reference Study Analytic thinking measure Religiosity measure rN
Shenhav et al. 1*CRT (intuitive scoring) God .18
#
882
(2012) [13] Convinced of Gods existence .15
a
Immortal souls .14
Belief change .19
2*CRT God -.18
#
321
Pennycook et al. 1*CRT Religious belief scale -.33
#
181
(2012) [4] Base-rate neglect -.19
2*CRT Religious belief scale -.29
#
267
Base-rate neglect -.31
Gervais & 1*CRT Intrinsic religiosity -.22 179
Norenzayan Intuitive religious belief -.15
(2012) [12] Supernatural agents -.18
#
Pennycook et al. (2013) [20]1*Belief bias syllogisms Religious belief scale -.46 91
Kahan (2013)
b
1
§
CRT Importance of religion -.15
#
1750
[27] Prayer frequency -.12
Razmyar & 1*CRT Overall religiosity -.09 150
Reeve (2013)
c
Overall spirituality -.19
[21] Prayer frequency -.19
Extrinsic religiosity -.20
Intrinsic religiosity -.24
Fundamentalism -.10
Scriptural acceptance -.17
#
Piazza & Sousa (2014) [35]3*CRT (intuitive scoring) Overall religiosity .28
#
192
Pennycook et al. 1*CRT Religious belief scale -.23
#
505
(2014a) [7] Base-rate neglect -.16
Pennycook et al. 1*Base-rate neglect Religious belief scale -.28 78
(2014b) [22]2
ŧ
CRT Religious belief scale -.26
#
198
Base-rate neglect -.29 200
3
ŧ
Base-rate neglect (rapid-response) Religious belief scale -.15 89
Browne et al. 1*CRT Strong faith -.11
#
1137
(2014)
d
[30] Spiritual thinking -.08
Byrd (2014)
e
[26]1
§
CRT (intuitive scoring) Theism .14
#
412
McCutcheon et 1
f
CRT Intrinsic religiosity .04
#
164
al. (2014) [36] Belief bias syllogisms -.02
Baron et al. (2015) [37]4*CRT/ Belief bias syllogisms (combined) God determines morality -.32
#
96
Gervais
g
(2015) 1*CRT God -.10
#
787
[5]2*CRT God -.09
#
596
Pennycook et al. 1*CRT Religious belief scale -.21
#
279
(2015) [10] Heuristics & Biases battery -.20
2*Heuristics & Biases battery Religious belief scale -.34 187
Finley et al. CRT CRT Intrinsic religiosity -.17 410
(2015) [24] First*Intuitive religious belief -.23
Supernatural agents -.19
#
Belief CRT Intrinsic religiosity .04 410
First
§
Intuitive religious belief <.01
Supernatural agents -.03
#
(Continued)
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 12 / 18
Table 6. (Continued)
Reference Study Analytic thinking measure Religiosity measure rN
Lindeman & Lipsanen (2016) [28]1
§
CRT Religious belief scale -.22
#
3044
Jack et al. (in 1
§
CRT God -.15
#
236
press) [29]2
§
CRT God -.25
#
233
3*CRT God -.22
#
159
4
§
CRT God -.24
#
527
5
ŧ
CRT God -.23
#
69
6*CRT God -.16
#
459
8*CRT God -.17
#
371
Current study 1
ŧ
CRT Religious belief scale -.26
#
372
Base-rate neglect -.23
2
ŧ
CRT Religious belief scale -.21
#
148
Base-rate neglect -.25 149
3
ŧ
CRT Religious belief scale -.17
#
277
Heuristics/biases -.16
4
ŧ
CRT Religious belief scale -.23
#
267
Heuristics/biases -.21
a
Value is a point biserial correlation coefcient (dichotomous variable).
b
These values were computed by the present authors using Kahans (2013) [27] data, which were available online through the Society of Judgment and
Decision Making website (http://journal.sjdm.org/vol8.4.html).
c
Some of these measures of religiosity relate to aspects of religious practice and commitment and not religious belief (see [11]).
d
The CRT was administered via phone interview in this study and performance was exceptionally low. This may explain the attenuated correlations.
e
This analysis excludes participants who had previous knowledge of the CRT. Around half of the sample includes philosophers either with a PhD or who
were in a PhD program at the time of the study. Participants in this study were given the CRT before the theism measure, but with a personality task in-
between.
f
The measures were completed in a paper-and-pencil study and the order of the pages was varied (no order analyses were reported).
g
These values were computed by the present authors using Gervais(2015) [5] data, which were available online through the authors website (http://
willgervais.com/journal-articles/). Participants with missing data for any CRT item were removed from analysis.
*Indicates that the religious belief measure was administered after the analytic thinking measure.
§
Indicates that the religious belief measure was administered before the analytic thinking measure.
ŧ
Indicates that the religious belief measure was administered in a separate session as the analytic thinking measure.
#
Indicates that the correlation was included in the meta-analysis.
Note: This table does not include correlations between religious belief and self-report measures of analytic thinking disposition (e.g., [38]).
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039.t006
Fig 3. Forest plot of random effect meta-analysis showing effect sizes (r) for the association between
religious belief scales and performance on the CRT.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039.g003
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 13 / 18
that the precise magnitude of the overall effect size should be interpreted with some degree of
caution. This may (at least partially) be the result of the large differences in religiosity measures
along with the restricted range of CRT scores in some of the studies (as discussed above).
Interestingly, a subgroup analysis of the 7 studies in which the religiosity measure was
administered prior to the CRT (including Finley et al.s belief first condition, see Table 6) pro-
duced a significant overall effect, r= -.174 (95% CI [-.227, -.120], N= 6,612, k= 7). The same
was true for the 17 studies in which the CRT was administered first, r= -.183 (95% CI [-.216,
-.150], N= 6,971, k= 17), and the 7 studies in which religiosity was measured in a separate ses-
sion, r= -.228 (95% CI [-.290, -.163], N= 1,331, k= 7). Given the small number of studies in
these subgroup analyses, the precise magnitude of the correlations should not be considered
reliable nor should the small differences between them be strongly interpreted. The purpose of
these subgroup analyses is to demonstrate that the negative association between CRT perfor-
mance and religious belief is far more robust than implied by Finley et al. This may be because
the majority of studies that measured religiosity in the same session as the CRT (regardless of
order, excepting Finley et al.) included additional seemingly unrelated intervening measures,
thereby masking the goal of the study.
Since our meta-analysis focused on published data (along with one Masters thesis), it is pos-
sible that the overall effect size is inflated by publication bias. Since the correlation between
CRT performance and religiosity is negative, publication bias should emerge as a gap in the
bottom right region of a funnel plot (see Fig 4). Visual inspection reveals that the funnel plot
for this meta-analysis is relatively symmetric. Consistent with this, Eggers regression test for
funnel plot asymmetry was not statistically significant (t= 0.74, SE = 0.61, p= .234), indicating
little evidence for publication bias in this collection of studies.
General Discussion
A negative association between analytic thinking and religious belief has been reported in sev-
eral studies [11]. We replicated this association in four studies, totaling over 1,000 participants.
Crucially, this association was evident despite the fact that religiosity and analytic thinking
were measured in separate sessions. These results contradict Finley et al.s[24] hypothesis that
the association between religiosity and analytic thinking requires participants to be put in an
analytic mindset when reporting their religiosity. In each of these four studies, our religious
belief scale was administered in a mass testing survey along with a variety of scales (presented
in a random order for each participant; see S1 Text). Analytic thinking was also predictive of
categorical theism and religious affiliation variables (administered in mass testing and pre-
screen surveys, respectively).
Fig 4. Funnel plot of standard error by FishersZ.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039.g004
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 14 / 18
We also performed a meta-analysis to quantitatively examine the robustness of the relation-
ship between analytic thinking and religious belief. This meta-analysis revealed evidence for a
negative association between performance on the Cognitive Reflection Test and religious belief
when considering all published studies (r= -.18). Although this effect size is small, it is similar
to effect sizes for other cognitive factors often considered important in the development of reli-
gious belief. To take a few examples, researchers have found statistically significant associations
between religious or theistic belief and mentalizing (r= .10, [43]), teleological thinking (r= .12
to .20, [43]), anthropomorphism (r= .05 to.10, [43]), empathizing (r= .21, [44]), ontological
confusions (r= .22 to .35, [10,44]), and dualism (r= .41, [43]). Moreover, although the overall
correlation is relatively small, self-identified atheists scored 18.7% higher than religiously affili-
ated individuals on a composite measure of analytic thinking in the combined analysis of our
four new studies (d= .72). This analysis was paralleled in Gervais[5] Study 2 (as reported
above, see also [45]).
The results summarized in Table 6 seem to indicate that full religious belief scales (as used
here, see also [4,7,10,20,22,28]) may be preferable to single-item or more general religiosity
measures. Moreover, the results of the four new studies reported here indicate that composite
measures of analytic thinking may prove to be better predictors of religious belief than the
3-item Cognitive Reflection Test, which is by far the most common measure used in this litera-
ture. Finally, given the robust differences between self-reported atheists/agnostics and religious
believers, samples that do not contain a sufficient number of religious disbelievers may not pro-
duce strong negative associations between religious belief and performance on analytic think-
ing measures. This is an area that requires further exploration.
Finley et al. framed their discussion in terms of explaining why religious belief might corre-
late with analytic thinking when the CRT is administered prior to questions about religiosity.
Our results indicate that a more appropriate question might be why there was no correlation
when religiosity was measured first by Finley et al. In our initial investigation of the relation-
ship between analytic thinking and religiosity [4], we deliberately measured religiosity last due
to concerns that demand characteristics might influence how participants respond to other
questions. In particular, we suspect that the object of the study might become transparent to
some participants when religiosity and analytic thinking are measured in proximity to each
other and without any cover story. Given the claim that the CRT and related measures are sen-
sitive to the disposition to think analytically [18], we reasoned that religious beliefs (which are
presumably more stable) should be administered second (although there is evidence that the
strength of particular religious beliefs is affected by analytic primes [12,13]). Put another way,
Finley et al.s failure to replicate might have occurred because participants correctly guessed
that the study had something to do with religion by the time they were given the CRT. This
may have motivated religious participants to perform well on the CRT, thereby diminishing
the influence of analytic thinking disposition (i.e., the motivation to think when not otherwise
compelled to do so). This conjecture is supported by the fact that prior studies have shown a
negative correlation between CRT performance and religious belief even when the CRT came
at the end of the study (see Table 6). For example, Byrd [26] found a correlation between the
CRT and theism when participants were given a personality test after the religious belief ques-
tion but before the CRT, which may have masked the goal of the study. It should nonetheless
be noted that Finley et al. did not find a significant difference in CRT performance between
order conditions. If demand characteristics explain Finley et al.s failure to replicate, the mech-
anisms may be more complicated than outlined here. For example, some religious participants
may be motivated by the demand characteristic whereas others may simply give up on the task.
Future research is needed to come to any definitive conclusions.
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 15 / 18
Conclusion
Finley et al. [24] hypothesised that there should only be a correlation between analytic thinking
and religious belief when participants are put in an analytic thinking mindset prior to reporting
their level of religious belief. Across four new studies, we found that this is not the case. There
was a consistent negative association between performance on analytic thinking measures and
religious belief even when the two measures were administered in separate surveys. Moreover,
there was an association between analytic thinking and categorical religious belief variables:
Self-identified atheists and agnostics scored higher on various measures of analytic cognitive
style than did religious believers. A summary and meta-analysis of prior work indicates that
Finley et al.s failed replication is one of only two notable exceptions across 35 studies with over
15,000 participants. These results reinforce prior work and indicate that, contra Finley et al.,
there is a genuine association between analytic thinking and religious disbelief.
Supporting Information
S1 Text. Mass testing.
(XLSX)
S2 Text. Results for thinking disposition scales.
(DOCX)
S3 Text. Materials.
(DOCX)
S4 Text. Analysis of same-day/different-day participants.
(DOCX)
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: GP. Performed the experiments: GP. Analyzed the
data: GP RR. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: GP DK JF. Wrote the paper: GP
RR DK JF. Performed meta-analysis: RR.
References
1. Evans J, St. B. T., Stanovich KE (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the
debate. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 8, 223241.
2. Pennycook G., Fugelsang J. A., & Koehler D. J. (2015). Everyday consequences of analytic thinking.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 425432.
3. Cheyne J.A. & Pennycook G. (2013). Sleep paralysis post-episode distress: Modeling potential effects
of episode characteristics, general psychological distress, beliefs, and cognitive style. Clinical Psycho-
logical Science, 1, 135148.
4. Pennycook G., Cheyne J.A., Seli P., Koehler D.J. & Fugelsang J.A. (2012). Analytic cognitive style pre-
dicts religious and paranormal belief. Cognition, 123, 335346. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.003
PMID: 22481051
5. Gervais W. Override the controversy: Analytic thinking predicts endorsement of evolution. Cognition.
2015 May; 142: 312321. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.011 PMID: 26072277
6. Shtulman, A., & McCallum, K. (2014). Cognitive reflection predicts science understanding. Proceedings
of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 29372942.
7. Pennycook G., Cheyne J.A., Barr N., Koehler D.J. & Fugelsang J.A. (2014a). The role of analytic think-
ing in moral judgements and values. Thinking & Reasoning, 20, 188214.
8. Royzman E. B., Landy J. F., & Goodwin G. P. (2014). Are good reasoners more incest-friendly? Trait
cognitive reflection predicts selective moralization in a sample of American adults. Judgment and Deci-
sion Making, 9, 176190
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 16 / 18
9. Barr N., Pennycook G., Stolz J.A., & Fugelsang J.A. (2015). The brain in your pocket: Evidence that
Smartphones are used to supplant thinking. Computers in Human Behavior, 48, 473480.
10. Pennycook G., Cheyne J.A., Barr N., Koehler D.J. & Fugelsang J.A. (2015) On the reception and detec-
tion of pseudo-profound bullshit. Judgment and Decision Making, 10, 549563.
11. Pennycook G. (2014). Evidence that analytic cognitive style influences religious belief: Comment on
Razmyar and Reeve (2014). Intelligence, 43, 2126.
12. Gervais W. M., & Norenzayan A. (2012). Analytic thinking promotes religious disbelief. Science, 336,
493496. doi: 10.1126/science.1215647 PMID: 22539725
13. Shenhav A., Rand D. G., & Greene J. D. (2012). Divine intuition: Cognitive style influences belief in
god. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 423428.
14. Frederick S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. The Journal of Economic Perspectives,
19, 2542.
15. Pennycook G., Cheyne J.A., Koehler D.J. & Fugelsang J.A. (2015). Is the cognitive reflection test a
measure of reflection and intuition? Behavior Research Methods. doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0576-1
16. Toplak M. V., West R. F., & Stanovich K. E. (2011). The Cognitive Reflection Test as a predictor of per-
formance on heuristics-and-biases tasks. Memory & Cognition, 39, 12751289.
17. Toplak M. V., West R. F., & Stanovich K. E. (2014). Assessing miserly information processing: An
expansion of the Cognitive Reflection Test. Thinking & Reasoning, 20, 147168.
18. Pennycook G. & Ross R. M. (2016). Commentary on Cognitive reflection vs. calculation in decision
making. Frontiers in Psychology.
19. Sinayev A., & Peters E. (2015). Cognitive reflection vs. calculation in decision making. Frontiers in Psy-
chology, 6, 532. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00532 PMID: 25999877
20. Pennycook G., Cheyne J.A., Koehler D.J. & Fugelsang J.A. (2013). Belief bias during reasoning among
religious believers and skeptics. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 20, 806811.
21. Razmyar S. & Reeve C. L. (2013). Individual differences in religiosity as a function of cognitive ability
and style. Intelligence, 667673.
22. Pennycook G., Cheyne J.A., Barr N., Koehler D.J. & Fugelsang J.A. (2014). Cognitive style and religi-
osity: The role of conflict detection. Memory & Cognition, 42, 110.
23. Yilmaz O., Karadöller D. Z., & Sofuoglu G. (2016). Analytic thinking, religion and prejudice: An experi-
mental test of the dual-process model of mind. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion.
doi: 10.1080/10508619.2016.1151117
24. Finley A. J., Tang D., Schmeichel B. J. (2015). Revisiting the relationship between individual differ-
ences in analytic thinking and religious belief: Evidence that measurement order moderates their
inverse correlation. PLoS ONE 10(9): e0138922. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138922 PMID: 26402334
25. Paxton JM, Ungar L, Greene JD. Reflection and reasoning in moral judgment. Cogn Sci. 2012 Jan; 36
(1): 16377. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01210.x PMID: 22049931
26. Byrd. N. (2014). Intuitive and reflective responses in philosophy. University of Colorado. (unpublished
Masters Thesis).
27. Kahan D. (2013). Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection. Judgment & Decision Mak-
ing, 8, 407424.
28. Lindeman M., & Lipsanen J. (2016). Diverse cognitive profiles of religious believers and nonbelievers.
The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10508619.
2015.1091695
29. Jack A. I., Friedman J. P., Boyatzis R. E., & Taylor S. N. (in press). Why do you believe in God? Rela-
tionships between religious belief, analytic thinking, mentalizing and moral concern. PLoS ONE.
30. Browne M, Pennycook G, Goodwin B, McHenry M. Reflective minds and open hearts: Cognitive style
and personality predict religiosity and spiritual thinking in a community sample. Eur J Soc Psychol.
2014; 44: 736742. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2059
31. De Neys W., & Glumicic T. (2008). Conflict monitoring in dual process theories of thinking. Cognition,
106, 12481299. PMID: 17631876
32. Malhotra, N., Krosnick, J. A., & Haertel, E. (2007). The Psychometric Properties of the GSS Wordsum
Vocabulary Test (GSS Methodology Report No. 111). Chicago: NORC.
33. Schwartz L. M., Woloshin S., Black W. C., & Welch H. G. (1997). The role of numeracy in understanding
the benefit of screening mammography. Annals of Internal Medicine, 127(11), 966972. PMID:
9412301
34. Lipkus I. M., Samsa G., & Rimer B. K. (2001). General performance on a numeracy scale among highly
educated samples. Medical Decision Making, 21, 3744. PMID: 11206945
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 17 / 18
35. Piazza J. & Sousa P. (2014). Religiosity, political orientation, and consequentialist moral thinking.
Social Psychology & Personality Science, 5, 334342.
36. McCutcheon L. E., Lowinger R., Wong M., & Jenkins W. (2014). Is analytic thinking related to celebrity
worship and disbelief in religion? North American Journal of Psychology, 16, 453462.
37. Baron J., Scott S., Fincher K., & Metz S. E. (2015). Why does the Cognitive Reflection Test (sometimes)
predict utilitarian moral judgment (and other things)? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cog-
nition, 4, 265284.
38. Svedholm A. M., & Lindeman M. (2013). The separate roles of the reflective mind and involuntary inhibi-
tory control in gatekeeping paranormal beliefs and the underlying intuitive confusions. British Journal of
Psychology, 3, 303319.
39. Hedges L. V., & Olkin I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
40. Borenstein M., Hedges L. V., Higgins J. P. T., & Rothstein H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis.
Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
41. Borenstein M., Hedges L. V., Higgins J. P. T., & Rothstein H. R. (2014). Comprehensive Meta-Analysis
Version 3.3.070. Englewood, NJ: Biostat.
42. Egger M., Smith G. D., Schneider M., & Minder C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple,
graphical test. BMJ, 315, 629634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 PMID: 9310563
43. Willard A. K., & Norenzayan A. (2013). Cognitive biases explain religious belief, paranormal belief, and
belief in lifes purpose. Cognition, 129, 379391. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.07.016 PMID:
23974049
44. Lindeman M., Svedholm-Häkkinen A. M., & Lipsanen J. (2015). Ontological confusions but not menta-
lizing abilities predict religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in supernatural purpose. Cognition,
134, 6376. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.008 PMID: 25460380
45. Da Silva, S., Matsushita, R., Seifert, G., & De Carvalho, M. (unpublished). Atheists score higher on cog-
nitive reflection tests. Munich Personal RePEc Archive. Available online: https://mpra.ub.uni-
muenchen.de/68451/. MPRA Paper No. 68451, posted December 20, 2015.
Analytic Thinking and Religious Disbelief
PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0153039 April 7, 2016 18 / 18
... Indeed, several prior studies support this notion. For example, those who perform better in analytical thinking tasks lean toward disbelieving religious explanations 1 (Gervais & Norenzayan, 2012;Pennycook et al., 2016). Nonreligious participants perform better in cognitive reflection as well as cognitive flexibility tests (Zmigrod et al., 2019) and are more likely to solve base-rate problems (Pennycook et al., 2016) compared with religious individuals. ...
... For example, those who perform better in analytical thinking tasks lean toward disbelieving religious explanations 1 (Gervais & Norenzayan, 2012;Pennycook et al., 2016). Nonreligious participants perform better in cognitive reflection as well as cognitive flexibility tests (Zmigrod et al., 2019) and are more likely to solve base-rate problems (Pennycook et al., 2016) compared with religious individuals. In addition, a meta-analytical study (k = 83) concludes that religiosity correlates negatively with cognitive ability (pooled correlation: −.20 ≤ r ≤ −.23), evidencing that more intelligent individuals are less likely to be religious (Zuckerman et al., 2013(Zuckerman et al., , 2020. ...
Article
Full-text available
Science and religion are influential social forces, and their interplay has been subject to many public and scholarly debates. The present article addresses how people mentally conceptualize the relationship between science and religion and how these conceptualizations can be systematized. To that end, we provide a comprehensive, integrative review of the pertinent literature. Moreover, we discuss how cognitive (in particular, epistemic beliefs) and motivational factors (in particular, epistemic needs, identity, and moral beliefs), as well as personality and contextual factors (e.g., rearing practices and cross-cultural exposure), are related to these mental conceptualizations. And finally, we provide a flowchart detailing the psychological processes leading to these mental conceptualizations. A comprehensive understanding of how individuals perceive the science–religion relationship is interesting in and of itself and practically relevant for managing societal challenges, such as science denial.
... Of course, anyone who has studied religion knows that highly reflective believers exist. As such they may wonder whether these reflective religionists simply do not make it into the studies that find reflective thinking correlating with atheism or agnosticism (Pennycook et al., 2016). However, even studies including academic philosophers find moderate correlations between reflection and atheism (Byrd, 2023b). ...
Preprint
Many studies have found correlations between reflective thinking and disbelief in God or lower levels of religiosity — so-called analytic atheism. Some data even detect analytic atheism in highly reflective people like philosophers. However, there is growing evidence that the most widely used tests of reflection confound reflection with ancillary abilities such as numeracy. Moreover, some studies do not detect analytic atheism in every country. Further, experimentally encouraging reflection has made some non-believers more open to believing in God. Finally, there is growing evidence of low data quality from one of the most common sources of online research participants. These prior results indicate that analytic atheism may be less than universal or partially explained by confounding factors. To address these possibilities, we developed better measures of religiosity and reflection, controlled for more confounds, and studied both paid and ad-recruited participants (N > 70,000). All four studies detected signs of analytic atheism above and beyond confounds. We also discovered analytic apostasy: the better a person performed on reflection tests, the greater their odds of losing their religion since childhood — even when controlling for confounds. Religious conversion was rare, but the final study recruited enough converts to find that the odds of becoming religious since childhood were probably unrelated to reflection, suggesting reflection’s relationships to conversion and deconversion are asymmetric. Detected relationships were usually small, but few failed to reach conventional levels of significance, indicating that reflective thinking may be a reliable albeit marginal predictor of apostasy.
... Research suggests that intelligent people are more likely to think analytically which in turns predicts lower religiosity, or alternatively, that both intelligence and analytical thinking measures tap into cognitive ability (Zuckerman et al., 2020). Indeed, analytical thinking tends to correlate negatively with religious belief (Pennycook et al., 2016;Stagnaro et al., 2019;Ståhl and van Prooijen, 2021;Yilmaz, 2021), though there is some evidence that the magnitude of this correlation varies crossculturally (Gervais et al., 2018). The causal direction of the relation between religiosity and analytical thinking is unclear (Villanueva et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
In many Western societies, religious people (particularly Christians) are underrepresented in STEM fields, relative to their numbers in the general population. We review existing literature that supports two broad types of explanations for religious underrepresentation in STEM, focusing primarily on Christians in the US. First, Christianity and science may actually conflict or be perceived by Christians to conflict with one another, which reduces Christians' engagement in STEM fields. Second, science and scientists may be portrayed, and Christians stereotyped, in ways that lead Christians to believe they would not belong in STEM. We argue that it is critical to further investigate these explanations, as doing so will shed light on how to broaden participation and engagement in science among a sizeable chunk of the population. We conclude our review by discussing promising directions for future research and implications for non-Christian religions, as well as in contexts outside the West.
... Enfin, il semble exister une relation inverse entre le raisonnement analytique et le dogmatisme [131]. Certains travaux en psychologie cognitive suggèrent que des niveaux plus élevés d'intelligence analytique, ainsi que les manipulations qui font appel à la pensée analytique, encouragent la révision des croyances en permettant à un individu d'évaluer des affirmations contradictoires et d'envisager des alternatives dans son raisonnement [132,133]. En outre, les personnes dotées d'une plus grande intelligence fluide, qui est corrélée à la capacité de la mémoire de travail [134,135], peuvent être mieux à même de garder à l'esprit des possibilités alternatives et donc plus à même de faire face à l'incertitude [136]. Ces considérations suggèrent que la capacité de raisonnement analytique est susceptible d'être associée à la flexibilité des croyances et à la capacité de les réviser. ...
Article
Full-text available
L’empathie joue un rôle fondamental dans la motivation à vouloir le bien-être des autres. Cependant, elle a des coûts cognitifs, émotionnels et énergétiques. Elle est limitée et fragile. Cet article intègre des connaissances en provenance de la théorie de l’évolution, des neurosciences, de la psychologie et de l’économie comportementale pour démontrer pourquoi et comment l’empathie est non-consciemment et rapidement modulée par divers signaux sociaux et facteurs situationnels. L’empathie n’est donc pas toujours une source d’information fiable dans la prise de décision morale. Ce cadre théorique des neurosciences sociales explique que la prise de décision guidée par l’empathie seule peut parfois éroder nos valeurs éthiques. Clarifier le rôle de l’empathie dans les processus de décision morale a des implications dans les domaines du droit, de la santé et des politiques publiques.
Article
Full-text available
Beliefs in supernatural agents or religious beliefs are pervasive, yet there are individual differences in such beliefs. Although various factors have been proposed as relevant, recent research has increasingly emphasized the importance of cultural learning, showing that enthusiastic religious behavior (credibility enhancing displays; CREDs) from parents predicts increased religious beliefs among their children. In addition to this kin-biased learning, Gervais and Najle (2015) analyzed data from the World Values Survey to demonstrate that the number of adults who show religious CREDs is also an important predictor of people’s beliefs, indicating that individuals develop their religious beliefs through conformist learning. This pre-registration study aimed to replicate and extend these findings by analyzing data from the International Social Survey Programme (ISSP), which is another large social survey. We examined the generalizability of the results by analyzing multigenerational samples. Multilevel regression and signal detection analyses revealed that the presence of both kin-biased and conformist learning cues was significantly associated with respondents’ religious beliefs. Moreover, they suggested tension between the two cultural learning cues, thereby suggesting that the effect of kin-biased learning on religious beliefs becomes stronger (weaker) when the cue for conformist learning is unclear (clear). These results support the idea that these two types of cultural learning are crucial to the development of religious beliefs.
Preprint
Full-text available
This paper presents the outcomes of a study focused on the Polish adaptation of the Survey of Scientifically Unaccepted Beliefs (SSUB; Irwin & Marks, 2013), designed to gauge individual differences in paranormal beliefs. Notably, the Polish version exhibited robust internal consistency, evidenced by Cronbach's alpha coefficients of 0.87 for the New Age Beliefs scale and 0.90 for the Traditional Religious Beliefs scale. Moreover, it demonstrated excellent test-retest stability. Subsequent to conducting confirmatory factor analysis, adjustments were made to streamline the scale for enhanced model fit. To validate the questionnaire, a range of measures were utilized to evaluate characteristics potentially linked to paranormal beliefs. The findings revealed positive associations between paranormal beliefs and various cognitive tendencies such as conspiracy thinking, magic ideation, superstitious thinking, bullshit receptivity, and irrationality. Furthermore, correlations with facets of the Big Five personality traits were identified. In summary, the Polish adaptation of SSUB exhibited satisfactory psychometric properties, thereby establishing its utility in assessing paranormal beliefs.
Article
Psychologists are paying increasing attention to a distinction between two ways of thinking. Cognitive psychologists discern between non-reflective “intuitive” and critical reflective “analytic” thinking. Cultural psychologists discern between context-focused “holistic” and object-focused “analytic” thinking. Both find the former strongly correlated with religious beliefs and Asian cultures, the latter with secular beliefs and Euro-American cultures. Yet, recent studies convincingly suggest: first, that analytic thinking does not just relate to secular beliefs but also to alternative beliefs that straddle the boundaries between secular and religious worldviews; second, that critical reflective thinking includes both the holistic context-focus of Asian religions and the analytic object-focus of European philosophies and sciences. This article supports these recent studies in the psychology of religion based on recent studies from the history and sociology of religion and a discourse analysis of interviews with members of a small-scale twenty-first-century spiritual group, as an example. We show that people who identify as spiritual in a metamodern context use both holistic and analytic thinking in creating alternative worldviews, which dialectically reconcile beliefs that many within modern western cultures would consider contradictory. We conclude that this both/and logic challenges theories and stereotypes about “secular versus religious” and “eastern versus western” thinking.
Article
Full-text available
Our moral sense is a biological adaptation specific to our species. It has evolved to facilitate group living, regulate social interactions, and promote cooperation beyond kinship. Beliefs intensely associated with moral values can motivate positive societal progress. However, moral convictions can also lead to dogmatism, antagonistic attitudes, a polarized political climate, and violent collective actions. This article draws on theories and empirical evidence from anthropology, neuroscience, cognitive science, social psychology, economics, and political science to explain why and how moralization increases the strength of beliefs and attitudes –in certainty and importance. Moral convictions are perceived as absolute, universal, and definite beliefs or principles. They engage the brain circuit associated with reward processing and valuation, which motivates and reinforces behavior. Moral commitments act as signals to conform to the rules of one’s coalition, which causes predictable cognitive biases that benefit the self and the group. Moral convictions are held with great confidence. This leads to confirmation bias, which means that people are unwilling to seek or accept corrective information no matter how accurate the content of their beliefs. Once an issue is moralized, it becomes more challenging to engage in a cost-benefit analysis. A deeper understanding of attitudes, values, and moralization dynamics may allow social scientists and policymakers to identify and support pathways away from extremism and to strengthen and sustain engagement in benevolent forms of collective action.
Chapter
Deepfakes combine the latest in machine learning and Artificial Intelligence (AI) to create hyper-realistic audio-visual forgeries. Deepfakes materialise from a specific type of deep learning—hence the name—in which sets of algorithms compete in a generative adversarial network or GAN (Goodfellow et al. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2014). Deep learning is a form of AI in which sets of algorithms (or neural networks) learn to simulate rules and replicate patterns by analysing large data sets. The generator algorithm creates content modelled on source material, which is already readily available (social media content, e.g.), whilst the discriminator algorithm seeks to detect flaws on the fake. This is an iterative process allowing for rapid improvements which address the flaws, meaning the GAN is able to produce highly realistic fake video content. This field of AI continues to advance at a rapid rate thus it is ever easier to create sophisticated and compelling videos. This has sparked concerns from politicians and international organisations about how these types of videos may be weaponised for malicious ends. These fears have fed into the post-truth era in the second decade of the early twenty-first century, where social media and information platforms have become increasingly uncanny spaces creating “abtruth” environments that, often toxify individual and collective memory to, destabilise the idea of provable facts and the nature of the “real” world.
Article
Full-text available
The dual-process theory of thought rests on the co-existence of two different thinking modalities: a quick, automatic, and associative process opposed to a slow, thoughtful, and deliberative process. The increasing interest in determining the neural foundation of the dual-process distinction has yielded mixed results, also given the difficulty of applying the fMRI standard approach to tasks usually employed in the cognitive literature. We report an activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis to investigate the neural foundation of the dual-process theory of thought. Eligible studies allowed for the identification of cerebral areas associated with dual-process theory-based tasks without differentiating between fast and slow thinking. The ALE algorithm converged on the medial frontal cortex, superior frontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, insula, and left inferior frontal gyrus. These structures partially overlap with the cerebral areas recurrently reported in the literature about the neural basis of the dual-process distinction, where the PARCS theory-based interpretation emphasizes the role of the right inferior gyrus. The results confirm the potential (but still almost unexplored) common ground between the dual-process literature and the cognitive control literature.
Thesis
Full-text available
Cognitive scientists have revealed systematic errors in human reasoning. There is disagreement about what these errors indicate about human rationality, but one upshot seems clear: human reasoning does not seem to fit traditional views of human rationality. This concern about rationality has made its way through various fields and has recently caught the attention of philosophers. The concern is that if philosophers are prone to systematic errors in reasoning, then the integrity of philosophy would be threatened. In this paper, I present some of the more famous work in cognitive science that has marshaled this concern. Then I present reasons to think that those with training in philosophy will be less prone to certain systematic errors in reasoning. The suggestion is that if philosophers could be shown to be less prone to such errors, then the worries about the integrity of philosophy could be constrained. Then I present evidence that, according to performance on the CRT (Frederick 2005), those who have benefited from training and selection in philosophy are indeed less prone to one kind of systematic error: irrationally arbitrating between intuitive and reflective responses. Nonetheless, philosophers are not entirely immune to this systematic error, and their proclivity for this error is statistically related to their responses to a variety of philosophical questions. So, while the evidence herein puts constraints on the worries about the integrity of philosophy, it by no means eliminates these worries. The conclusion, then, is that the present evidence offers prima facie reasons to ascribe a mitigated privilege to philosophers' ability to rationally arbitrate between intuitive and reflective responses.
Article
Full-text available
Prior work has established that analytic thinking is associated with disbelief in God, whereas religious and spiritual beliefs have been positively linked to social and emotional cognition. However, social and emotional cognition can be subdivided into a number of distinct dimensions, and some work suggests that analytic thinking is in tension with some aspects of social-emotional cognition. This leaves open two questions. First, is belief linked to social and emotional cognition in general, or a specific dimension in particular? Second, does the negative relationship between belief and analytic thinking still hold after relationships with social and emotional cognition are taken into account? We report eight hypothesis-driven studies which examine these questions. These studies are guided by a theoretical model which focuses on the distinct social and emotional processing deficits associated with autism spectrum disorders (mentalizing) and psychopathy (moral concern). To our knowledge no other study has investigated both of these dimensions of social and emotion cognition alongside analytic thinking. We find that religious belief is robustly positively associated with moral concern (4 measures), and that at least part of the negative association between belief and analytic thinking (2 measures) can be explained by a negative correlation between moral concern and analytic thinking. Using nine different measures of mentalizing, we found no evidence of a relationship between mentalizing and religious or spiritual belief. These findings challenge the theoretical view that religious and spiritual beliefs are linked to the perception of agency, and suggest that gender differences in religious belief can be explained by differences in moral concern. These findings are consistent with the opposing domains hypothesis, according to which brain areas associated with moral concern and analytic thinking are in tension.
Article
Full-text available
Sinayev and Peters (2015; hereafter SP namely, that the propensity or disposition to think analytically plays an important role in CRT performance (Pennycook et al., 2015b). We discuss recent empirical evidence that supports the claim that the CRT is more than just a measure of numeracy or, more generally, cognitive ability.
Article
Full-text available
We administrate the cognitive reflection test devised by Frederick to a sample of 483 undergraduates and discriminate the sample to consider selected demographic characteristics. For the sake of robustness, we take two extra versions that present cues for removing the automatic (but wrong) answers suggested by the test. We find a participant's gender and religious attitude to matter for the test performance on the three versions. Males score significantly higher than females , and so do atheists of either gender. While the former result replicates a previous finding that is now reasonably well established, the latter is new. The fact that atheists score higher agrees with the literature showing that belief is an automatic manifestation of the mind and its default mode. Disbelieving seems to require deliberative cognitive ability. Such results are verified by an extra sample of 81 participants using Google Docs questionnaires via the Internet.
Article
Full-text available
Although bullshit is common in everyday life and has attracted attention from philosophers, its reception (critical or ingenuous) has not, to our knowledge, been subject to empirical investigation. Here we focus on pseudo-profound bullshit, which consists of seemingly impressive assertions that are presented as true and meaningful but are actually vacuous. We presented participants with bullshit statements consisting of buzzwords randomly organized into statements with syntactic structure but no discernible meaning (e.g., “Wholeness quiets infinite phenomena”). Across multiple studies, the propensity to judge bullshit statements as profound was associated with a variety of conceptually relevant variables (e.g., intuitive cognitive style, supernatural belief). Parallel associations were less evident among profundity judgments for more conventionally profound (e.g., “A wet person does not fear the rain”) or mundane (e.g., “Newborn babies require constant attention”) statements. These results support the idea that some people are more receptive to this type of bullshit and that detecting it is not merely a matter of indiscriminate skepticism but rather a discernment of deceptive vagueness in otherwise impressive sounding claims. Our results also suggest that a bias toward accepting statements as true may be an important component of pseudo-profound bullshit receptivity.
Preprint
Cognitive scientists have revealed systematic errors in human reasoning. There is disagreement about what these errors indicate about human rationality, but one upshot seems clear: human reasoning does not seem to fit traditional views of human rationality. This concern about rationality has made its way through various fields and has recently caught the attention of philosophers. The concern is that if philosophers are prone to systematic errors in reasoning, then the integrity of philosophy would be threatened. In this paper, I present some of the more famous work in cognitive science that has marshaled this concern. Then I present reasons to think that those with training in philosophy will be less prone to certain systematic errors in reasoning. The suggestion is that if philosophers could be shown to be less prone to such errors, then the worries about the integrity of philosophy could be constrained. Then I present evidence that, according to performance on the CRT (Frederick 2005), those who have benefited from training and selection in philosophy are indeed less prone to one kind of systematic error: irrationally arbitrating between intuitive and reflective responses. Nonetheless, philosophers are not entirely immune to this systematic error, and their proclivity for this error is statistically related to their responses to a variety of philosophical questions. So, while the evidence herein puts constraints on the worries about the integrity of philosophy, it by no means eliminates these worries. The conclusion, then, is that the present evidence offers prima facie reasons to ascribe a mitigated privilege to philosophers' ability to rationally arbitrate between intuitive and reflective responses.
Article
Dual-process models of the mind, as well as the relation between analytic thinking and religious belief, have aroused interest in recent years. However, few studies have examined this relation experimentally. We predicted that religious belief might be one of the causes of prejudice while analytic thinking reduces both. The first experiment replicated, in a mostly Muslim sample, past research showing that analytic thinking promotes religious disbelief. The second experiment investigated the effect of Muslim religious priming and analytic priming on prejudice and showed that, while the former significantly increased the total prejudice score, the latter had an effect only on anti-gay prejudice. Thus, the findings partially support our proposed pattern of relationships in that analytic thinking might be one of the cognitive factors that prevents prejudice whereas religious belief might be the one that increases it.
Article
The factors that make people religious or atheistic are currently much debated, and empirical evidence is limited. Further, the possibility that both religious believers and nonbelievers comprise diverse subtypes with different characteristics has seldom been considered. In this study, characteristics of religious believers (N = 984) and nonbelievers (N = 1,000) were analyzed with latent class analyses. These analyses identified five religious subgroups and five nonbeliever subgroups that differed in analytical thinking, empathizing, mechanistic cognition (i.e., systemizing), and autistic and schizotypal traits. The results highlight the strengths and limitations of current theoretical arguments about religious beliefs.