ArticlePDF Available

Empathizing, systemizing, and career choice in Brazil: Sex differences and individual variation among areas of study

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Individual variation in sexually dimorphic cognitive capacities for understanding people (empathizing) and things (systemizing) is related to career choice: individuals in careers dominated by women tend to exhibit higher empathy than individuals in male-dominated careers, who tend to score higher in systemizing. We tested this pattern in a Brazilian population. In Study I, using University public data (1980–2015), we found that exact sciences attracted significantly more males, whereas humanities and bio-sciences attracted more females throughout the 35-year period. Further, during the time period studied, there was a consistent growth of interest in both men and women in studying both exact sciences and humanities. In Study II, using the GLM analysis of an undergraduate sample of 248 men and 325 women, we replicated the sex differences in empathizing and systemizing, and found that, regardless of sex, individuals in humanities and bio-sciences score higher on empathizing and lower on systemizing than those in exact sciences. These results corroborate the sexually dimorphic pattern in career choice and in empathizing-systemizing, and show the importance of cognitive style as one of the factors related to university majors.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Empathizing, systemizing, and career choice in Brazil: Sex differences
and individual variation among areas of study
Marco Antonio Corrêa Varella , José Henrique Benedetti Piccoli Ferreira, Kamila Janaina Pereira,
Vera Silvia Raad Bussab, Jaroslava Varella Valentova
Department of Experimental Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Sao Paulo, Av. Prof. Mello Moraes 1721, Sao Paulo 05508-030, SP, Brazil
abstractarticle info
Article history:
Received 30 October 2015
Received in revised form 10 March 2016
Accepted 18 March 2016
Available online xxxx
Individual variation in sexually dimorphic cognitive capacities for understanding people (empathizing) and
things (systemizing) is related to career choice: individuals in careers dominated by women tend to exhibit
higher empathy than individuals in male-dominated careers, who tend to score higherin systemizing. We tested
this pattern in a Brazilian population. In Study I, using University public data (19802015), we found that exact
sciences attracted signicantly more males, whereas humanities and bio-sciences attracted more females
throughout the 35-year period. Further, during the time period studied, there wasa consistent growth of interest
in both men and women in studying bothexact sciences and humanities. In StudyII, using the GLM analysisof an
undergraduate sample of 248 men and 325 women, we replicated the sex differences in empathizing and sys-
temizing,and found that, regardless of sex,individuals in humanities and bio-sciences score higheron empathiz-
ing and lower on systemizing than those in exact sciences. These results corroborate the sexually dimorphic
pattern in career choice and in empathizing-systemizing, and show the importance of cognitive style as one of
the factors related to university majors.
© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords:
Empathizing
Systemizing
Study area
Cognition
Sex differences
Career choice
1. Introduction
At least once in a lifetime most people make a study/career
choice and this decision has the potential to inuence one's life
outcomes. Many factors are found to inuence career choice, such
as competitiveness (Buser, Niederle, & Oosterbeek, 2014), accul-
turation, family background, the level of condence and interest
in the given eld (Tang, Fouad, & Smith, 1999), and sex (Halpern
et al., 2007).
In spite of intrasexual individual variation, men and women have, on
average, different occupational interests (Halpern et al., 2007). A recent
meta-analysis (Su, Rounds, & Armstrong, 2009) including more than
503 thousand individuals showed that on the thingspeople dimension,
men on average indicated higher preferences for working with things
than women, whereas women on average preferred more working with
people. The effect size of this difference was large (Cohen's d= 0.93).
This study also found that on the hexagonal RIASEC model (Holland,
1997), men showed stronger interests for Realistic (working with things
and gadgets or working outdoors; d= 0.84) and Investigative (interest
in science, including mathematics, physical and social sciences, and bio-
logical and medical sciences; d= 0.26) types of careers, whereas
women showed stronger interests in the dimensions of Artistic (interest
in creative expression, including writing and the visual and performing
arts; d= 0.35), Social (interest in helping people, d=0.68),andConven-
tional (interest in working in well-structured environments, especially
business settings; d= 0.33). Similar sex differences were found in a
world-wide BBC study sampling more than 255 thousand participants
online (Manning, Reimers, Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, & Fink, 2010).
One of the factors that has been found to inuence both the sex
difference and the intrasexual individual variation in study/career pref-
erence/choice is the cognitive style related to empathizing and system-
izing abilities (e.g. Billington, Baron-Cohen, & Wheelwright, 2007;
Wright, Eaton, & Skagerberg, 2015). Every individual possesses some
degree of cognitive capacities for understanding both people/feelings
and things/systems. Empathizing is dened as an ability that enables
people to understand and better deal with a person's emotions and
thoughts (Baron-Cohen, 2002; Baron-Cohen, Knickmeyer, & Belmonte,
2005; Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 2004), and it allows for compre-
hension and prediction of the social world (Baron-Cohen, Richler,
Bisarya, Gurunathan & Wheelwright, 2003). Systemizing permits
individuals to comprehend or construct systems (Baron-Cohen, 2002;
Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 157164
Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: macvarella@gmail.com (M.A. Corrêa Varella),
jh.benedetti@gmail.com (J.H. Benedetti Piccoli Ferreira), kamilajpereira@gmail.com
(K.J. Pereira), vsbussab@gmail.com (V.S. Raad Bussab), jaroslava@usp.br (J. Varella
Valentova).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.058
0191-8869/© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Personality and Individual Differences
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid
Baron-Cohen et al., 2005), and it enables understanding and foreseeing
the inanimate world (Baron-Cohen et al., 2003).
Empathizing and systemizing differ, on average, between men and
women: women tend to score higher on empathizing than men (d=
.55; Wright et al., 2015), and men tend to score higher on systemizing
than women (d= .50; Baron-Cohen, 2002; Wakabayashi et al., 2007;
Wright et al., 2015). Therefore, individuals with more empathic cogni-
tive style are supposed to prefer career areas that have proportionally
more women, and generally require understanding of other people.
On the other hand, individuals higher on systemizing are supposed to
prefer careers that have high proportion of men, and require compre-
hension of inanimate systems.
Billington et al. (2007) replicated sex differences in areas of study,
showing that 70.1% of the humanities students in the UK were
women and 59.1% of the science students were men. They also repli-
cated the sex differences in empathizing and systemizing, with
women scoring, on average, higher on the rst, and men on the lat-
ter. Moreover, this study reported that, regardless of sex, individuals
higher on systemizing were more likely to enter physical sciences,
and individuals higher on empathizing had higher probability of
choosing to study humanities. In thesameline,twostudiescarried
out in the UK (Wheelwright et al., 2006) and Belgium (Focquaert,
Steven, Wolford, Colden, & Gazzaniga, 2007) also found that individ-
uals in the humanities possessed a cognitive style that is more
empathizing-driven than systemizing-driven, while individuals in
sciences possessed rather the systemizing-driven cognitive style.
Finally, Manning et al. (2010) reported that in a cross-cultural sam-
ple of mostly white North American and Western European popula-
tions both men and women who scored higher on systematizing
compared to empathizing tended to work in occupations with
lower proportion of females.
However, there is still lack of evidence as to whether empathizing-
systemizing and study/career choice are related with each other
in other than North American and Western European populations.
A few studies on a Portuguese speaking population (Brazil and
Portugal) reported sex differences in empathy and/or systemizing
similar to the previous research (e.g., Milfont, Coelho, Pessoa, &
Gouveia, 2012; Rodrigues, Gonçalves, Lopes, & Santos, 2010). We
might thus expect that there would be a similar link between
empathizing-systemizing and career or study choice in this popula-
tion. Interestingly, for socio-historical reasons, the areas of studies
in Brazil are divided differently than in the previously studied coun-
tries. In particular, humanities and social sciences are treated togeth-
er as humanities, whereas sciences are divided into bio-sciences and
exact sciences. Little is known about how bio-sciences t into the
systemizing-empathy cognitive model. Brazil thus offers a unique
opportunity to investigate this question.
1.1. Aims
The main aim of this study was to test whether individual variation
in empathizing and systemizing cognitive styles would be related to
choice of study area, that have, on average, different frequency of male
and female students. Here we focused not only on humanities and
hard sciences, but also on biology. To do this, we rstly tested frequency
of men and women applyingfor undergraduate study areas divided into
three main areas humanities, biological and exact sciences on two
Brazilian samples. To obtain representative data, we analyzed a large
public dataset containing information about the number of male and fe-
male applicants for thethree study areas between 1980 until 2015. On a
smaller sample of men and women, we further replicated this aim using
a questionnaire method, and tested further for sex differences in empa-
thy and systemizing. Finally, we tested whether students from human-
ities, biological and exact sciences differ in their self-reported empathy
and systemizing, irrespective of their sex.
2. Study I
2.1. Methods
To test for differences in frequency of men and women applying for
undergraduate study areas, we gathered public data available online
from the website of the University Foundation for University Entrance
(Fundação Universitária para o Vestibular, FUVEST). FUVEST is a Brazilian
autonomous institution connected to the University of Sao Paulo (USP),
that runs the entrance examinations to USP, and in the eighties also the
entrance examinations for University of Campinas (UNICAMP) and Sao
Paulo State University (UNESP), two other public Sao Paulo State univer-
sities. The foundation possesses statistics about the number of men and
women who applied for each undergraduate course and each grand
study area from 1980 until 2015, except between 198687 and 1991
94 when the sex of the students was not recorded. By this method, we ob-
tained data from 30 years, for a total of 4,046,205 undergraduate appli-
cants (Table 1,Fig. 1).
FUVEST divides the undergraduate programs into humanities (such
as history, arts, music, law, economy, journalism, pedagogy, languages,
tourism, philosophy, geography, etc.), bio-sciences (such as sports,
psychology, pharmacy, veterinary, medicine, odontology, biology,
phonoaudiology, nursing, nutrition, etc.), and exact sciences (such as
civil, electric, mechanic engineering, statistics, computation, geology,
mathematics, physics, chemistry, etc.). Thus, the applicants were divid-
ed into these 3 main study areas (Table 1,Fig. 1).
The demographic data of students applying for undergraduate
courses offer a powerful measure of motivation and capability to follow
an area of career, given that it is data about real-life choices. FUVEST's
exam is considered the most competitive entrance examination in the
country, and one of the most demanding, usually lasting several days.
In Brazil, the best and most important universities, such as the universi-
ties included in this study, are public, either federal or state, the offered
education is of high quality, and no tuition is charged.This explains why
these universities are the most desired places to study and hence, it is
very difcult to be admitted.
2.2. Analyzes and results
Spearman's non-parametric correlations showed a positive correla-
tion between year and number of female applicants in humanities
(ρ= .698, N = 30, p b.001) and exact sciences (ρ=.709,N=30,
pb.001), while there was no correlation in bio-sciences (ρ=.313,
N = 30, p = .092). Similarly, in men year positively correlated with
number of applicants in humanities (ρ= .442, N = 30, p = .014) and
exact sciences (ρ= .411, N = 30, p = .024), but negatively in bio-
sciences (ρ=.433, N = 30, p = .017). Thus, year entered into the
subsequent analyses as a covariate. To further explore these results,
we computed percentages of male and female applicants for each
year, and ran non-parametric correlations between the year and sex
proportion. The correlations were signicant and positive for percent-
age of female applicants (and thus negative for percentage of male
applicants) in all three areas, namely Humanities (ρ= .436, N = 30,
p = .016), Bio-sciences (ρ=.723,N=30,pb.001) and Exact sciences
(ρ=.826,N=30,pb.001).
To analyze the possible differences between men and women ap-
plying for different study areas, we performed a multivariate General
Linear Model (GLM) with number of male and female applicants for
each study area as dependent variables, sex as a factor (coded as 1 =
males, 2 = females), and year as a covariate. The test of Between-
Subject Effects revealed strong effect of sex in all three areas, namely
humanities (F = 32.065, df = 1, 59, p b.001, η
p
2
= .360), bio-sciences
(F = 256.705, df = 1, 59, p b.001, η
p
2
= .818), and exact sciences
(F = 217.776, df = 1, 59, p b.001, η
p
2
= .793). Exact sciences
attracted signicantly more males, whereas humanities and bio-
sciences attracted more females throughout the 35 years period.
158 M.A. Corrêa Varella et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 157164
3. Study II
3.1. Methods
3.1.1. Participants
Two hundred and forty eight men (mean age, 21.6 years, SD = 4.16)
and 325 women (mean age, 21.2 years, SD = 4.58) were recruited for
this study from students at the University of Sao Paulo (USP). Men
and women did not differ in age (t = 1.201, df = 571, p = .23), and
were recruited from different faculties with the aim to acquire a broad
sample across various study elds, in order to test the main hypothesis.
We used the FUVEST 2014 handbook (2013) for applicants to divide
the undergraduate programs into humanities, bio-sciences, and exact
sciences. The distribution of men and women in the three main study
areas is given in Table 2.
3.1.2. Procedure and questionnaires
Participants answered a questionnaire containingbasic demograph-
ic data (such as sex, age, undergraduate course studying) and the full
version of the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and the Systemizing Quotient
(SQ) questionnaires (Baron-Cohen et al., 2003). We used the Brazilian
Portuguese version of the questionnaires (Baron-Cohen, 2004). Both
the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and the Systemizing Quotient (SQ) ques-
tionnaires have 60 statements each, 40 of them are experimental (an
example of the EQ I easily perceive when someone wants to participate
in a conversation, an example of the SQ The functioning of machines
fascinates me), and 20 items are controls (e.g., I dream in the majority
of nights). The answers are given on a verbally described scale, from
strongly disagree, partially disagree to partially agree, and strongly
agree. We scored the questionnaires as suggested by Baron-Cohen
et al. (2003), so that the higher the score on Systemizing quotient the
higher the cognitive capacity of systemizing, and the higher the score
on Empathy quotient, the higher the cognitive capacity for empathy.
The data were gathered between 2005 and 2006 by distributing printed
questionnaires to students in their classrooms after approval of the
teaching professor. Participants voluntarily signed the consent term
and lled in the anonymous questionnaire, which took approximately
20 min. When nished, they put the questionnaires into a sealed box.
3.2. Analyses
A KolmogorovSmirnov test showed violations from normality in
the scores of systemizing and empathy in women (KS = .08, df =
392, p b.001; KS = .06, df = 392, p = .004, respectively), while the
data were normally distributed in men (KS = .05, df = 314, p =
.200; KS = .05, df = 314, p = .074, respectively). In women, the sys-
temizing scores were mildly positively skewed (skewness = .69,
SE = .12), while the empathy scores were mildly negatively skewed
(skewness = .36, SE = 12). Therefore, we used non-parametric corre-
lations to test a possible relationship between age and Empathy and
Systemizing quotient scores. Because in both men and women, age pos-
itively correlated with the Systemizing score (Spearman's rho = .20,
N=310,pb.001; Spearman's rho = .22, N = 389, p b.001, respective-
ly), we controlled for age in the subsequent analysis of the Systemizing
score. There was no signicant correlation between age and Empathy
score (Spearman's rho = .10, N = 311, p = .09, Spearman's
rho = .06, N = 387, p = .24), and we have thus not controlled for
age in this analysis. To test the main hypothesis, we ran two separate
Generalized linear models (GZLM), that is considered a robust test to-
wards mild violations of normality (Ng & Cribbie, 2016). The scores of
Systemizing and Empathy entered as dependent variables, sex and
area of study as factors,and age as a covariate in the case of Systemizing
score.
3.3. Results
The GZLM for Empathy quotient score was signicant (Likelihood
Ratio Chi-Square = 109,185, df = 5, p b.001) (Fig. 2). The test of
model effects showed signicant inuence of sex (Wald Chi-
Square = 5.09, p = .024), area of study (Wald Chi-Square = 84.33,
pb.001), and also interaction between sex and area of study (Wald
Chi-Square = 12.80, p = .002). Estimated marginal means with
Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons showed that
women scored signicantly higher on Empathy (mean = 39.5,
SE = .69, 95% CI: 38.1940.89) than men (mean = 37.3, SE = .725,
95% CI: 35.8638.70) (Mean difference: 2.26, SE = 1.00, p = .024).
Further, individuals from humanities (mean = 41.0, SE = .82, 95%
CI: 39.442.6) as well as from bio-sciences (mean = 42.6, SE = .81,
95% CI: 41.044.2) scored higher on Empathy than those from
exact sciences (mean = 31.6, SE = .96, 95% CI: 29.7733.53)
(p's b.001) (Mean difference: 9.34, SE = 1.26, p b.001; Mean differ-
ence: 10.94, SE = 1.26, p b.001, respectively). There was no differ-
ence in Empathy score between bio-sciences and humanities
(Mean difference: 1.60, SE = 1.15, p = .50). Interaction between
sex and area of study showed that women scored higher on Empathy
thanmenintheareaofbio-sciences (Mean difference: 5.06, SE =
1.62, p = .026), but there was only a trend in the same direction in
humanities (Mean difference: 4.77, SE = 1.64, p = .054) and no dif-
ference between men and women in exact sciences (Mean differ-
ence: 3.07, SE = 1.91, p = 1.000). Further, men from exact sciences
scored signicantly lower on Empathy than men from both human-
ities (Mean difference: 5.41, SE = 1.70, p = .022) and bio-sciences
(Mean difference: 6.87, SE = 1.79, p = .002). Men from bio-
sciences and humanities didn't differ from each other (Mean differ-
ence: 1.45, SE = 1.83, p = 1.000). Similarly, women from exact
sciences scored signicantly lower on Empathy than women from
both humanities (Mean difference: 13.27, SE = 1.86, p b.001) and
bio-sciences (Mean difference: 15.01, SE = 1.75, p b.001), and
women from bio-sciences and humanities didn't differ from each
other (Mean difference: 1.74, SE = 1.40, p = 1.000).
The GZLM for Systemizing score was also signicant (Likelihood
Ratio Chi-Square = 110.3, df = 6, p b.001) (Fig. 3). The test of model
effects showed signicant inuence of sex (Wald Chi-Square = 68.5,
pb.001), area of study (Wald Chi-Square = 9.1, p = .01), age (Wald
Table 1
Total number (N) of applicants and mean number of applicants per year (Mean N) divided by sex and study area.
Humanities Bio-sciences Exact sciences Total
Total N of applicants Mean N (SD) Total N of applicants Mean N (SD) Total N of applicants Mean N (SD) Total N of applicants Mean N (SD)
Men 765,884 25,529
(5625)
449,121 14,971
(2136)
681,492 22,716
(4965)
1,896,497 63,217
(7826)
Women 974,903 32,497
(6759)
929,308 30,977
(5099)
245,497 8183
(3204)
2,149,708 71,657
(12,453)
Cohen's d1.121 4.095 3.478 .812
% of overlap 57.51% 4.06% 8.20% 68.47%
Total 1,740,787 29,013 1,378,429 22,974 926,989 15,450 4,046,205 67,437
159M.A. Corrêa Varella et al. / Personalityand Individual Differences 97 (2016) 157164
Fig. 1. Number of male and female applicants for humanities (a), bio-sciences (b), and exactsciences (c) between 1980 and 2015.
160 M.A. Corrêa Varella et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 157164
Chi-Square = 16.9, p b.001). There was no signicant interaction be-
tween sex and area of study(Wald Chi-Square = .41, p = .81). Estimat-
ed marginal means with Bonferroni correction for multiple
comparisons, controlled for age, showed that men scored signicantly
higher on Systemizing (mean = 33.1, SE = .71, 95% CI: 31.834.5)
than women (mean = 25.1, SE = .67, 95% CI: 23.826.4) (Mean differ-
ence: 8.06, SE = .97, p b.001). Further, individuals from humanities
(mean = 27.5, SE = .80, 95% CI: 26.029.1) scored lower on Systemiz-
ing than those from exact sciences (mean = 31.2, SE = .94, 95% CI:
29.433.0) (Mean difference: 3.68, SE = 1.23, p = .008). There was
no difference between bio-sciences (mean = 28.6, SE = .80, 95% CI:
27.130.2) and humanities (Mean difference: 1.12, SE = 1.13, p =
.96) or exact sciences (Mean difference: 2.57, SE = 1.24, p = .12).
Non-parametric correlations showed a positive association between
Systemizing and Empathy scoresin men (Kendall's tau= .173, N = 244,
pb.001), but not in women (Kendall's tau = .074, N = 325, p = .052).
When age was controlled for, this association was even strongerin men
(r = .275, N = 237, p b.001), and reached signicance also in women
(r = .117, N = 318, p = .04). One-way Fisher r-to-z transformation
showed a signicant difference between these two correlation coef-
cients (Fisher's Z = 1.91, p = .03), which suggests that reports on
Empathy and Systemizing are more disparate in women than in men.
To investigate this result in more detail, we subtracted the Systemizing
score from the Empathy score for each participant. The signicant
difference between men and women in the absolute differences (U =
54.29, N = 569, p b.001) suggested that the relative Empathy versus
Systemizing difference is higher in women than in men. Further, to
test the difference and direction in reported Empathy and Systemizing,
we ran a non-parametric Wilcoxon signed rank tests for men and
women separately. In women, the Empathy score was signicantly
higher than Systemizing (W = 2 728.0, N = 325, p b.001), and the
same was truefor men (W = 9 693.5, N = 244, p b.001). Thus, although
women scored, on average, higher on Empathythan men, both sexes re-
ported higher Empathy compared to Systemizing. This result applied to
most of the sub-groups, namely for men from humanities (W = 592.0,
N=82,pb.001), men from bio-sciences (W = 606.0, N = 69, p b.001),
men from exact sciences (W = 2 335.500, N = 90, p = .045), women
from humanities (W = 179.5, N = 108, p b.001), and women from
bio-sciences (W = 139.0, N = 160, p b.001). The only group, where
the mean score of Systemizing did not signicantly differ from the
mean score of Empathy, were women from the exact sciences (W =
532.0, N = 54, p = .22).
4. Discussion
This studyaimed to explore how sex and the chosen area of study re-
late to the cognitive style of empathizing and systemizing in a Brazilian
population. First,we have shown that women more frequently apply for
and are enrolled in studies of humanities and bio-sciences areas than
men, whereas men more frequently apply for and study exact sciences.
Further, in line with previous studies (e.g., Baron-Cohen et al., 2003;
Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 2004), Brazilian women scored, on aver-
age, higher than men in Empathy, while the opposite pattern appeared
in Systemizing. Finally, irrespective of sex, individuals higher on empa-
thy and lower on systemizing are more frequently enrolled in humani-
ties and bio-sciences, while individuals higher on systemizing more
frequently study exact sciences. Interestingly, besides replicating the
ndings of previous literature on different populations (e.g. Billington
et al., 2007; Focquaert et al., 2007; Manning et al., 2010; Wheelwright
et al., 2006), we showed that in at least one scientic area, here bio-
sciences, women and individuals higher on empathizing over-
represent men and individuals higher on systemizing.
In Study I, using a non-obtrusive method based on a public database
of a representative long-term sample mostly from the University of Sao
Paulo (USP), we showed a consistent sex difference between men and
women in the choice of study area. In particular, women applied
signicantly morefrequently than men for undergraduate majors in hu-
manities and bio-sciences, while men applied more than women for
exact sciences. The effect sizes for all three areas were large, and in
Exact sciences and Bio-sciences there was a very small percentage of
overlap between the sexes. Also, we found that over the last 35 years,
number of both men and women applying for humanities and exact sci-
ences grew, while the interest for studying biological sciences dropped
in male applicants, andstayed relatively stable in female applicants. Fur-
ther, against a stereotypical opinion that women are not interested in
sciences, we clearly showed that women are indeed interested, and
their interest is growing throughout the decades (see Fig. 1). However,
despite the continuous increase of women in exact sciences, the predict-
ed sex difference remains. Interestingly, interest of both men and
women in studying humanities also grew over time, which can be
given by the fact that the eld of humanities grew itself, and thus con-
tinually offers a growing number of its sub-areas.
In Study II, we have replicated the sex differences on a smaller sample
of actual students of different areas at USP. Further, in line with previous
studies, we have encountered sex differences in cognitive styles dened
as systemizing-empathizing: women scored higher on Empathy than
men, while the opposite applied to Systemizing. These differences had
medium and large effect sizes, and overlap of more than 80% and more
than 70% between the group distributions, respectively. These nd-
ings add cross-cultural support for the theory of cognitive sex differ-
ences in empathizing-systemizing (Baron-Cohen et al., 2003).
Moreover, we showed that individuals who study humanities and
bio-sciences show higher empathizing and lower systemizing cogni-
tive styles than individuals who study exact sciences. This is in line
with the few previous studies investigating whether the cognitive
style inuences choice of study area (Billington et al., 2007;
Focquaert et al., 2007; Manning et al., 2010; Wheelwright et al.,
2006). Interestingly, women in bio-sciences scored higher on Empa-
thythanmeninthesamearea,whilewehavenotencountereddif-
ferences between men and women in the exact sciences and
humanities. This is probably because men in the humanities are rel-
atively higher on Empathy, while women in the exact sciences are
relatively lower on Empathy, and the sex difference thus diminishes.
Further, we showed that most participants scored higher on
Empathy than Systemizing. This is in line with previous studies
(e.g., Billington et al., 2007), and can be interpreted within the
evolutionary frame of a highly social species. Arguably, intra-
species selective pressures are usually stronger than inter-species
Table 2
The mean scores (and SD) of Empathy and Systemizing of men and women in the three
main study areas, humanities, bio-sciences and exact sciences.
Nof
participants
Mean Empathy
score (SD)
Mean Systemizing
score (SD)
Humanities Men 82 38.82 (12.20) 31.07 (12.73)
Women 107 43.30 (11.85) 23.82 (11.28)
Cohen's d.373 .654
% of overlap 85.21% 74.37%
Total 189 41.36 (12.17) 26.97 (12.43)
Bio-sciences Men 66 39.92 (10.01) 32.77 (12.80)
Women 158 45.16 (9.63) 23.92 (9.69)
Cohen's d.534 .780
% of overlap 78.95% 69.65%
Total 224 43.62 (10.01) 26.53 (11.41)
Exact sciences Men 90 33.46 (11.49) 35.96 (10.66)
Women 53 30.36 (14.45) 27.87 (10.54)
Cohen's d.237 .763
% of overlap 90.57% 70.28%
Total 143 32.31 (12.71) 28.33 (12.03)
Total Men 314 37.39 (11.23) 34.25 (11.79)
Women 392 42.51 (12.02) 25.99 (11.40)
Cohen's d.440 .713
% of overlap 82.59% 72.15%
Total 706 40.23 (11.94) 29.66 (12.27)
161M.A. Corrêa Varella et al. / Personalityand Individual Differences 97 (2016) 157164
or ecological pressures, and thus in highly social species, such as
most primates including humans, it is expected that understanding
other conspecics rather than objects might have led to higher re-
productive success and survival. For example, as shown by Seyfarth
and Cheney (2013), perception of others' internal states and the
more complex Theory of Mind are adaptive in a wide array of
non-human and human primates, because such abilities support
formation and maintenance of social bonds, leading to higher
Fig. 2. Differences in Empathy scores between men and women, and individuals from different study areas (error bars: +/2SE).
Fig. 3. Differences in Systemizing scores between men and women, and individuals from different study areas (error bars: +/2SE).
162 M.A. Corrêa Varella et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 157164
reproductive success. Moreover, seeing into other's individual
head facilitates anticipating social dangers, such as expulsion
from a social group, or direct attacks from other group members,
thus increasing chances of survival and, consequently, chances for
reproduction.
Furthermore, although both men and women tend to have higher
empathy than systemizing, we showed that the discrepancy between
systemizing and empathizing cognitive styles is greater in women
than in men, which is in linewith a recent study based on Chinese pop-
ulation (Zheng & Zheng, 2015). This can be partly caused by prenatal
hormonal action. On average, males experience higher levels of prenatal
testosterone than females (Beck-Peccoz et al., 1991), and levels of fetal
testosterone were shown to be positively linked to systemizing capaci-
ties (Auyeung et al., 2006), while negatively to empathizing capacities
(Knickmeyer, Baron-Cohen, Raggatt, Taylor, & Hackett, 2006). Despite
higher levels of fetal testosterone, males have relatively high empathiz-
ing capacities. From the evolutionary perspective, increased empathy in
males is connected to many human-specic social skills, such as rela-
tively high paternal care (Fernandez-Duque, Valeggia, & Mendoza,
2009), tendency to monogamy and pair-bonding (Fletcher, Simpson,
Campbell, & Overall, 2015), among others, that are characteristics
connected to an overall decrease of sexual dimorphism in modern
humans compared to its ancestor species (Grabowski, Hatala, Jungers,
&Richmond,2015).
We also showed that systemizing tends to increase with age in
both sexes, while empathy remains more stable throughout the
lifespan. An increase in systemizing with age might indicate training
in scientic and analytic thinking among students at university, who
start their studies with intuitions and later develop their critical and
analytical thinking. This would need to be tested on a non-university
population, and in particular within a more age heterogeneous pop-
ulation, because both men and women studying science can be rela-
tively more competitive, which can be connected to higher levels of
actual testosterone. Higher testosterone leads to higher competitive-
ness and higher competitiveness increases testosterone (e.g. Booth,
Granger, Mazur, & Kivlighan, 2006).
Although this study used two different methods, it was not a longi-
tudinal study, and we thus cannot say whether individuals higher on
Systemizing seek to studyexact sciences, and people higheron Empathy
seek to study humanities and biology, or whether individuals develop
such cognitive styles as a consequence of the area they study. On the
other hand, our rst study showed sex differences among people who
applied for the given study area, and since sex differences between
men and women in Systemizing and Empathy have been reported
consistently across studies and samples, we can conclude that the rst
explanation is likely the case.
A limitation is that we did not have information about applicants' or
our questionnaire participants' sexual orientation. Sexual orientation of
the participants might interfere with the results, because previous studies
showed that homosexual men and womenare,onaverage,moreinterest-
ed in professions mostly preferred by heterosexual individuals of the op-
posite sex (Ellis, Ratnasingam, & Wheeler, 2012). Similarly, homosexual
men score higher on empathy and lower on systemizing than heterosex-
ual men (Zheng & Zheng, 2015). We might thus expect that, for example,
there would be a higher percentage of homosexual men in humanities
and bio-sciences than in exact sciences, while the opposite might be
true for homosexual women. Future studies should thus control for sexual
orientation, and other factors, such as for how long participants have been
studying, and sample different populations from different cultures.
5. Conclusions
To conclude, women's and men's interests and participation in uni-
versity studies is growing, and there are still clear differences between
men and women in their interests. Women have more widespread
cognitive styles and academic interests, ranging from humanities to
bio-sciences, while men focus more on exact sciences. Most important-
ly, it seems that it is not only the sex of the individuals, but also intra-
sexual variance in cognitive tendencies that inuences the choices of
studies, and consequently careers, as demonstrated in previous studies
(Billington et al., 2007; Focquaert et al., 2007; Manning et al., 2010;
Wheelwright et al., 2006).
Acknowledgments
We thank to Nathalia Nabor and other undergraduate students for
their help with data collection and transcription, and to the workers of
FUVEST who kindly helped us to nd and sort the oldest public data
on entrance exams. We owe our great thanks also to Jerry A. Hogan
(University of Toronto) for English proofreading and valuable
comments.
References
Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Chapman, E ., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., & Hackett, G.
(2006). Foetal testosterone and the child systemizing quotient. European Journal of
Endocrinology,155(Suppl. 1), S123S130.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences,6(6), 248254. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01904-6.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2004). A diferença essencial: A verdade sobre o cérebro de homens e
mulheres. Rio de Janeiro: O bjetiva.
Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of
adults with Asperger Syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differ-
ences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,34(2), 163175. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00.
Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N.,& Wheelwright, S. (2003). The sys-
temizing quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-func-
tioning autism, and normal sex differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Society, B: Biological Sciences,358(1430), 361374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.
2002.1206.
Baron-Cohen, S., Knickmeyer, R. C., & Belmonte,M. K. (2005). Sex differences in thebrain:
Implications for explaining autism.Sc ience,310(5749), 819823. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1126/science.1115455.
Beck-Peccoz, P., Padmanabhan, V., Baggiani, A. M., Cortelazzi, D., Buscaglia, M.,
Medri, G., ... Beitins, I. Z. (1991). Maturation of hypothalamicpituitarygo-
nadal function in normal human fetuses: Circulating levels of gonadotropins,
their common alpha-subunit and free testosterone, and discrepancy between
immunological and biological activities of circulating follicle-stimulating hor-
mone. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism,73, 525532.
Billington, J., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2007). Cognitive style predicts entry
into physical sciences and humanities: Questionn aire and performa nce tests of
empathy and systemizing. Learning and Individual Differences,17(3), 260268.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2007.02.004.
Booth, A., Granger, D. A., Mazur, A., & Kivlighan, K. T. (2006). Testosterone and social
behavior. Social Forces,85(1), 167191.
Buser, T., Niederle, M., & Oosterbeek, H. (2014). Gender, competitiveness, and career
choices. The Quarterly Journal of Economics,129(3), 14091447. http://dx.doi.org/10.
3386/w18576.
Ellis, L., Ratnasingam, M., & Wheeler, M. (2012). Gender, sexual orientation, and occupa-
tional interests: Evidence of their interrelatedness. Personality and Individual
Differences,53,6469. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.02.008.
Fernandez-Duque, E.,Valeggia, C. R., & Mendoza,S. P. (2009). The biology of paternal care
in human and nonhuman primates. Annual Review of Anthropology,38,115130.
Fletcher, G. J., Simpson, J. A., Campbell, L., & Overall, N. C. (2015). Pair-bonding, romantic
love, and evolution the curious case of Homo sapiens. Perspectives on Psychological
Science,10(1), 2036.
Focquaert, F., Steven,M. S., Wolford, G. L.,Colden, A., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2007). Empathiz-
ing and systemizing cognitive traits in the sciences and humanities. Personality and
Individual Differences,43(3), 619625. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.01.004.
Fundação Universitária para o Vestibular(2013). Fuvest 2014: Manual do Candidato. Re-
trieved from http://download.uol.com.br/vestibular2/manual/fuvest2014.pdf
Grabowski, M., Hatala,K. G., Jungers, W.L., & Richmond, B. G. (2015). Body massestimates
of hominin fossils and the evolution ofhuman body size. Journal of Human Evolution,
85,7593.
Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., & Gernsbacher, M. A.
(2007). The science of sex differences in science and mathematics. Psychological
Science in the Public Interest,8(1), 151. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1529-1006.
2007.00032.x.
Holland, J. L. (1997). Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and
work environments (3rd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Knickmeyer, R., Baron-Cohen, S., Raggatt, P., Taylor, K., & Hackett, G. (2006). Fetal testos-
terone and empathy. Hormones and Behavior,49,282292.
Manning, J. T., Reimers, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Fink, B. (2010). Sexu-
ally dimorphic traits (digit ratio, body height, systemizingempathizing scores)
and gender segregation between occupations: Evidence from the BBC internet
study. Personality and Individual Differences,49(5), 511515. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.015.
163M.A. Corrêa Varella et al. / Personalityand Individual Differences 97 (2016) 157164
Milfont,T.L.,Coelho,J.A.P.d.M.,Pessoa,V.S.,&Gouveia,V.V.(2012).Sexo,estilos
cognitivos e mapas cognitivos: Avaliando suas relações na população geral.
Estudos de Psicologia (Natal),17(1), 2533. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-
294X2012000100004.
Ng, V. K., & Cribbie, R. A. (2016). Using the Gamma Generalized Linear Model for model-
ing continuous, skewed and heteroscedastic outcomes in psychology. Current
Psychology,111.
Rodrigues, J., Gonçalves, G., Lopes, A., & Santos, J. (2010). Quociente de sistematização:
Uma análise exploratória. Psychologica,52(1), 4154 Retrieved from http://iduc.uc.
pt/index.php/psychologica/article/view/989/438
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2013). Afliation, empathy, and the origins of theory
of mind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,110(Supplement 2),
1034910356.
Su, R., Rounds, J., & Armstrong, P. I. (2009). Men and things, women and people: A meta-
analysis of sex differences in interests. Psychological Bulletin,135(6), 859884. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017364.
Tang, M., Fouad, N. A., & Smith, P. L. (1999). Asian Americans' career choices: A path
model to examine factors inuencing their career choices. Journal of Vocational
Behavior,54(1), 142157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jvbe.1998.1651.
Wakabayashi, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Uchiyama, T., Yoshida, Y., Kuroda, M., & Wheelwright,
S. (2007). Empathizing and systemizing in adults with and without autism spectrum
conditions: Cross-cultural stability. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
37(10), 18231832. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0316-6.
Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Goldenfeld, N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., Smith, R., ...
Wakabayashi, A. (2006). Predicting autism spectrum quotient (AQ) fromthe system-
izing quotient-revised (SQ-R) and empathy quotient (EQ). Brain Research,1079(1),
4756. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.012.
Wright, D. B., Eaton, A. A., & Skagerberg, E. (2015). Occupational segregation and psycho-
logical gender differences: How empathizing and systemizing help explain the distri-
bution of men andwomen into (some) occupations. Journal of Res earch in Personalit y,
54,3039. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.06.004.
Zheng, L., & Zheng, Y. (2015). Sex and sexual orientation differences in empathizing-
systemizing cognitive styles in China. Personality and Individual Differences,87,
267271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.08.014.
164 M.A. Corrêa Varella et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 97 (2016) 157164
... El IAB comprende 21 reactivos con cuatro opciones de respuesta y explica el 56% de la varianza (α = 0.93). El nivel de ansiedad se consideró por su puntaje: mínimo (0-5), leve (6)(7)(8)(9)(10)(11)(12)(13)(14)(15), moderado (16)(17)(18)(19)(20)(21)(22)(23)(24)(25)(26)(27)(28)(29)(30) y grave . La escala BIEPS-A comprende 12 reactivos con tres opciones de respuesta y explica el 60% de la varianza (α = 0.79). ...
... Se detectó un efecto principal para el factor año de formación, con menor puntaje en el IAB obtenido por estudiantes del último año de formación en comparación con el de estudiantes de primer y segundo años, con F(3, 313) = 3.58, p = 0.01, η p 2 = 0.03 y DHS p < 0.01 (Tabla 1). Se identificaron cuatro niveles diferenciados de posesión de empatía: bajo (19)(20)(21)(22)(23)(24)(25)(26)(27)(28)(29)(30)(31)(32)(33)(34), medio (35)(36)(37), alto La dimensión 1 del análisis de correspondencias múltiples explicó el 31.28% de la varianza y se definió por el nivel de ansiedad (MD: 0.47). ...
... El principal resultado de este estudio fue que un alto nivel de empatía se asocia a niveles moderado a grave de ansiedad y a bajo nivel de bienestar psicológico en estudiantes del primer año de formación de la carrera de medicina. La empatía fue alta o muy alta en el 58.7% de los estudiantes evaluados, lo cual puede deberse a la naturaleza de la carrera, que requiere contacto con terceros y por ello tiende a ser frecuentemente elegida por individuos con mayores habilidades empáticas 26 . En concordancia con la literatura, las mujeres obtuvieron puntajes superiores en la evaluación de la empatía 27 , lo cual se ha explicado por influencias evolutivas, neurobiológicas y sociales que derivan en diferentes estilos cognitivos 28 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Resumen Antecedentes: La expresión de empatía mejora la calidad de la atención a la salud, y por ello se sugiere su instrucción du-rante los años de formación académica de estudiantes de medicina. No obstante, la expresión de empatía también puede ocasionar ansiedad y perjudicar el bienestar psicológico. Objetivo: Identificar perfiles que describan la relación entre empatía, ansiedad y bienestar psicológico en estudiantes mexicanos de medicina. Método: Estudio transversal en estudiantes mexi-canos de medicina que completaron la Escala Multidimensional de Empatía, el Inventario de Ansiedad de Beck y la Escala de Bienestar Psicológico para Adultos. Resultados: Las mujeres presentaron mayor empatía y ansiedad, y menor bienestar psicológico (p < 0.01). La ansiedad en estudiantes del último año de formación fue menor que en los estudiantes de primer y segundo año (p < 0.01). Se detectaron cuatro perfiles: 1) hombres, ansiedad mínima, empatía baja; 2) estudiantes de primer año de formación, ansiedad moderada a grave, bienestar psicológico bajo, empatía alta; 3) último año de formación, ansiedad leve, bienestar psicológico adecuado, empatía media; 4) mujeres, segundo y tercer años de formación, muy alta empatía. Conclusiones: Los estudiantes de medicina del primer año de formación constituyen una población de riesgo. Son necesarias estrategias de atención para este grupo estudiantil. Palabras clave: Salud mental. Análisis de correspondencias múltiples. Estudiantes de medicina. Perfil psicológico. Abstract Background: The expression of empathy improves the quality of health care, which is why its instruction is suggested during the years of academic training of medical students. However, expressing empathy can also cause anxiety and impair psychological well-being. Objective: To identify profiles that describe the relationship between empathy, anxiety and psychological well-being in Mexican medical students. Method: Cross-sectional study in Mexican medical students completed the Multidi-mensional Empathy Scale, the Beck Anxiety Inventory, and the Psychological Well-Being Scale for Adults. Results: Women presented greater empathy and anxiety and lower psychological well-being (p < 0.01). Anxiety in students of the last year of training was lower compared to students of the first and second year (p < 0.01). Four profiles were detected: 1) men, minimal anxiety, low empathy; 2) first-year students, moderate to severe anxiety, low psychological well-being, high empathy; 3) last
... El IAB comprende 21 reactivos con cuatro opciones de respuesta y explica el 56% de la varianza (α = 0.93). El nivel de ansiedad se consideró por su puntaje: mínimo (0-5), leve (6)(7)(8)(9)(10)(11)(12)(13)(14)(15), moderado (16)(17)(18)(19)(20)(21)(22)(23)(24)(25)(26)(27)(28)(29)(30) y grave . La escala BIEPS-A comprende 12 reactivos con tres opciones de respuesta y explica el 60% de la varianza (α = 0.79). ...
... Se detectó un efecto principal para el factor año de formación, con menor puntaje en el IAB obtenido por estudiantes del último año de formación en comparación con el de estudiantes de primer y segundo años, con F(3, 313) = 3.58, p = 0.01, η p 2 = 0.03 y DHS p < 0.01 (Tabla 1). Se identificaron cuatro niveles diferenciados de posesión de empatía: bajo (19)(20)(21)(22)(23)(24)(25)(26)(27)(28)(29)(30)(31)(32)(33)(34), medio (35)(36)(37), alto ...
... El principal resultado de este estudio fue que un alto nivel de empatía se asocia a niveles moderado a grave de ansiedad y a bajo nivel de bienestar psicológico en estudiantes del primer año de formación de la carrera de medicina. La empatía fue alta o muy alta en el 58.7% de los estudiantes evaluados, lo cual puede deberse a la naturaleza de la carrera, que requiere contacto con terceros y por ello tiende a ser frecuentemente elegida por individuos con mayores habilidades empáticas 26 . En concordancia con la literatura, las mujeres obtuvieron puntajes superiores en la evaluación de la empatía 27 , lo cual se ha explicado por influencias evolutivas, neurobiológicas y sociales que derivan en diferentes estilos cognitivos 28 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The expression of empathy improves the quality of health care, which is why its instruction is suggested during the years of academic training of medical students. However, expressing empathy can also cause anxiety and impair psychological well-being. Objective: To identify profiles that describe the relationship between empathy, anxiety and psychological well-being in Mexican medical students. Method: Cross-sectional study in Mexican medical students completed the Multidimensional Empathy Scale, the Beck Anxiety Inventory, and the Psychological Well-Being Scale for Adults. Results: Women presented greater empathy and anxiety and lower psychological well-being (p < 0.01). Anxiety in students of the last year of training was lower compared to students of the first and second year (p < 0.01). Four profiles were detected: 1) men, minimal anxiety, low empathy; 2) first-year students, moderate to severe anxiety, low psychological well-being, high empathy; 3) last year of training, mild anxiety, adequate psychological well-being, medium empathy; 4) women, second and third year of training, very high empathy. Conclusions: Medical students in their first year of training constitute a population at risk. Attention strategies are necessary for this student group.
... Not only the financial aspect but many other factors are found to influence career choice in Brazil, such as competitiveness, acculturation, family background, the level of confidence and interest in a given field, and sex (Varella et al., 2016). A study by Varella et al. (2016) explored how sex and the chosen area of study relate to the cognitive style of empathizing and systemizing in a Brazilian population. ...
... Not only the financial aspect but many other factors are found to influence career choice in Brazil, such as competitiveness, acculturation, family background, the level of confidence and interest in a given field, and sex (Varella et al., 2016). A study by Varella et al. (2016) explored how sex and the chosen area of study relate to the cognitive style of empathizing and systemizing in a Brazilian population. They showed that Brazilian women more frequently apply for and are enrolled in studies of humanities and biosciences areas than men, whereas men more frequently apply for and study exact sciences (Varella et al., 2016). ...
... A study by Varella et al. (2016) explored how sex and the chosen area of study relate to the cognitive style of empathizing and systemizing in a Brazilian population. They showed that Brazilian women more frequently apply for and are enrolled in studies of humanities and biosciences areas than men, whereas men more frequently apply for and study exact sciences (Varella et al., 2016). Based on the systemizing-empathy cognitive model, in Varella et al.'s study, Brazilian women scored, on average, higher than men in empathy, whereas the opposite pattern appeared in systemizing. ...
Article
Purpose Speech-language pathologists (SLPs) are professionals marked by occupational gender segregation, with speech-language pathology jobs being dominated by women worldwide. In the United States, male participation in leadership and prominent positions within the profession has been gradually increasing, but the proportion of men compared to women is still very low. This study aimed to verify the prevalence of Brazilian men who are SLPs, assess the participation of male SLPs in leadership positions within the profession, and compare this gender distribution with that of other countries. Method In this retrospective, descriptive cross-sectional study, we considered information contained in public databases and information obtained through utilizing the Brazilian Access to Information Law. Male representation in Brazil was assessed considering the distribution of active male speech-language pathology professionals, participation in collegiates of the Brazilian federal and regional speech-language pathology councils, participation in the board of the Brazilian Society of Speech-Language Pathology, participation in editorial boards, authorship in Brazilian journals, and prevalence in public health care. Besides the data gathered in Brazil, for comparison purposes, data on scientific representation in the United States and that of speech-language pathology societies in 40 countries were collected. Results Males represent 4.8% of the Brazilian active speech-language pathology professionals. Representation in public health care was low (3.0%), which reveals a significant gender disparity. Male participation in leadership positions averaged at around 14%–25%, which can illustrate a potential growth in the field of male professionals. Conclusions The gender disparity seen globally remains present in Brazilian SLPs. The findings of this study can benefit the profession by recognizing this inequality and stimulating the search for solutions that favor gender equality considering Brazilian sociocultural features.
... If artistic motivation were not domain specific, there would be no major difference in prioritization of artistic extra-class activities between those applying for artistic careers and those not applying for artistic careers. The career choice is influenced by many factors, including individual cognitive style (Varella, Ferreira, Pereira, Bussab, & Valentova, 2016) and particularly artistic career-choice is specific by relatively higher intrinsic than extrinsic motives (Varella, 2021). To test this hypothesis, we performed a multivariate General Linear Model (GLM) with Bonferroni post-hoc comparison for main effects and Partial Eta-squared (η p 2 ) as effect size estimators during the entire available time period. ...
... Finally, we tested the temporal stability of the prioritization of artistic extra-class leisure activities by analyzing whether there were any temporal trends in the overall artistic leisure engagement. Following Varella et al. (2016) and Varella (2021), the correlation between the series of years throughout the available period and the percentage of prioritization artistic extra-class activities indicates its temporal stability or instability. If the tendency to prioritize artistic leisure activities was temporally instable there would be positive or negative significant correlations between the time period and the percentage of artistic prioritization. ...
Article
Full-text available
Artistic behavior as aesthetically enhancing activities is conceptualized as a functionally autonomous activity within the evolved human behavioral repertoire. Accordingly, it should be intrinsically motivated, and it might also be expected to be temporally stable and domain specific. Preferential freely-pursued activities reflect intrinsic motivation and offer a valuable measure of artistic motivation. We used a large decades-long real-life public Brazilian data set from university applications to test these ideas. We analysed data on extra-class activities from 674.699 late-adolescents applying for university courses between 1987 and 2004, mostly between 17 and 19 years of age; approximately half men and half women. We found that 27% of individuals reported that Artistic/cultural activities were the leisure-time activity they participated in most frequently, and 32% reported they spent the longest period of free-time doing Artistic-activities (theater/cinema, music, dance, art-craft/plastic arts). Interestingly, from this whole sample, only less than 3% actually applied for artistic careers, which suggests that the prevalence of prioritizing artistic activities is higher than commonly assumed and includes not only professional artists, but also many hobbyists, amateurs and dedicated fans. Further, artistic careers applicants prioritize art almost three times more than the total of applicants, suggesting its specificity. After controlling for inconsistency of answer options during the period, prioritizing both Artistic/cultural and Artistic-activities remained temporally stable, as predicted. Despite limitations, overall results supported the hypotheses that artistic behavior is more intrinsically motivated, domain specific, and temporally stable. This plausibly demonstrates that artistic propensity has at least partly an evolved nature.
... En cambio, el segundo se presenta comúnmente en las mujeres, e implica una mayor habilidad empática y un menor interés por pensamientos sistematizantes. Se ha demostrado que estos estilos cognitivos influyen en los intereses académicos de los estudiantes universitarios 26 . Es posible que las diferencias entre los perfiles 2 y 3 sean explicadas por la ausencia de contacto con pacientes durante el periodo preclínico de formación y su posterior involucramiento con estos durante el periodo clínico. ...
... Si bien esto no fue considerado en el presente estudio, existen datos que indican que los estudiantes de ingeniería muestran un menor nivel de empatía en las dimensiones fantasía y toma de perspectiva del IRI, en comparación con los estudiantes de psicología y de trabajo social 30 . Partiendo de este resultado y de los datos presentados por Varella et al. 26 , podría esperarse también un menor nivel de empatía en estudiantes de ingeniería en comparación con estudiantes de medicina. Futuros estudios deberán someter a verificación empírica esta hipótesis. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: To identify the association between general empathy and medical empathy. Detect predictors of the level of medical empathy from general empathy. To determine the psychosocial profile that describes the relationship between general empathy, medical empathy, and demographic and academic characteristics in medical students. Method: Descriptive cross-sectional design. Medical students completed the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) and the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy-student version (JSPE-S). Results: Relatively high levels of general and medical empathy were detected. The total score of the IRI and the JSPE-S and their dimensions correlated positively (r = 0.14-0.52), except for the dimension personal distress of the IRI. The score of the IRI dimension empathic concern was the best predictor of the JSPE-S score and its dimensions (b = 0.27-0.54). Four profiles were detected: 1) men, preference for technology-oriented specialty, less empathy; 2) pre-clinical period students, less empathy; 3) students of the clinical period, greater empathy; and 4) women, preference for patient-oriented specialty, greater empathy. Conclusions: General and medical empathy are associated. Differentiated empathic profiles will allow the design of instructional strategies in empathy according to the specific needs of each one.
... It is about empathy and compassion fatigue. There is evidence that, regardless of their gender, people with greater empathic capacity prefer careers such as medicine and nursing [105], which plays a crucial role during their professional life, particularly in the healthcare professional-patient relationship [106]. However, a high level of empathy can lead to burnout syndrome and compassion fatigue [107]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to use latent profile analysis to identify specific profiles of burnout syndrome in combination with work engagement and to identify whether job satisfaction, psychological well-being, and other sociodemographic and work variables affect the probability of presenting a profile of burnout syndrome and low work enthusiasm. A total of 355 healthcare professionals completed the Spanish Burnout Inventory, the Utrecht Work Engagement Scale, the Job Satisfaction Scale, and the Psychological Well-Being Scale for Adults. Latent profile analysis identified four profiles: (1) burnout with high indolence (BwHIn); (2) burnout with low indolence (BwLIn); (3) high engagement, low burnout (HeLb); and (4) in the process of burning out (IPB). Multivariate logistic regression showed that a second job in a government healthcare institution; a shift other than the morning shift; being divorced, separated or widowed; and workload are predictors of burnout profiles with respect to the HeLb profile. These data are useful for designing intervention strategies according to the needs and characteristics of each type of burnout profile.
... It is about empathy and compassion fatigue. There is evidence that, regardless of their gender, people with greater empathic capacity prefer careers such as medicine and nursing [104], which play a crucial role during their professional life, particularly in the healthcare professional-patient relationship [105]. However, a high level of empathy can lead to burnout syndrome and compassion fatigue [106]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to use latent profile analysis to identify specific profiles of burnout syndrome in combination with work engagement and to identify whether job satisfaction, psychological well-being, and other sociodemographic and work variables affect the probability of presenting a profile of burnout syndrome and low work enthusiasm. A total of 355 healthcare professionals completed the Spanish Burnout Inventory, the Utrecht Work Engagement Scale, the Job Satisfaction Scale, and the Psychological Well-Being Scale for Adults. Latent profile analysis identified 4 profiles: 1) burnout with high indolence (BwHIn); 2) burnout with low indolence (BwLIn); 3) high engagement, low burnout (HeLb); and 4) in the process of burning out (IPB). Multivariate logistic regression showed that a second job in a government health care institution; a shift other than the morning shift; being divorced, separated or widowed; and work load are predictors of burnout profiles with respect to the HeLb profile. These data are useful for designing intervention strategies according to the needs and characteristics of each type of burnout profile.
... Empathy, caring, and risk-aversion are features often more strongly linked to women than men and therefore hypothesized to be the driving factors behind female leaders' public health responses. In contrast, there is a stronger perceived association of features like being obstinate and risk-seeking with men than women (Christov-Moore et al., 2014;Corrêa Varella et al., 2016) 1 . Many leadership stereotypes are masculine and male leadership is often perceived as more successful than female leadership (Koenig et al., 2011). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic sparked media attention surrounding the supposedly superior leadership of women in public health crises. This notion may have resulted from associating women with risk-aversion and men with risk-seeking. Men's leadership is often viewed as superior, but the pandemic's unique challenges might have led to stereotypically risk-averse women being viewed as superior. We conducted three experiments on evaluating pandemic strategies, investigating the effect of the politician's gender and the participants' political ideology. A risk-averse strategy was generally viewed more favorably. Moreover, ideology is a stronger predictor than the politician's gender. Conservative participants favored a risk-seeking strategy, while liberal participants favored a risk-averse strategy. Participants adhered to gender stereotypes of risk-seeking/aversion but did not project them onto political leaders.
Article
Occupations are segregated with respect to sex, even in modern, egalitarian societies. There are strong pressures to eliminate segregation and therefore strong reasons to correctly theorize why segregation persists. The dominant view underpinning most public policies is essentially that environmental factors nudge women and men into different occupational paths. Nudging, however, ignores research suggesting that psychological traits that influence occupational choice differs between women and men, on average. Some of the most well-documented and persistent average sex differences between men and women suggest that the taken-for-granted assumption that an egalitarian society would exhibit a more or less equal distribution of men and women across the occupational landscape may be mistaken. Rather, models of occupational choice informed by individual differences in preferences, broadly understood, would help us better explain how men and women behave in the labor market. Differences in occupational preferences will affect choices. Therefore, differences in proportions of women and men across professions may be in line with an egalitarian society and the well-being and best interest of both men and women in society.
Article
Full-text available
Some researchers in psychology have ordinarily relied on traditional linear models when assessing the relationship between predictor(s) and a continuous outcome, even when the assumptions of the traditional model (e.g., normality, homoscedasticity) are not satisfied. Of those who abandon the traditional linear model, some opt for robust versions of the ANOVA and regression statistics that usually focus on relationships for the typical or average case instead of trying to model relationships for the full range of relevant cases. Generalized linear models, on the other hand, model the relationships among variables using all available and relevant data and can be appropriate under certain conditions of non-normality and heteroscedasticity. In this paper, we summarize the advantages and limitations of using generalized linear models with continuous outcomes and provide two simplified examples that highlight the methodology involved in selecting, comparing, and interpreting models for positively skewed outcomes and certain heteroscedastic relationships.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined the extent to which men and women differ regarding systemizing and construction of cognitive maps. Participants were 214 people (57% women) from João Pessoa (Brazil), with mean age of 29.3 years. They were asked to list the Brazilian Federation Unities (BFUs) and their capitals, to indicate in a sketch map the BFUs, to estimate the distances between capitals, and to answer the systemizing quotient test and demographic questions. Results showed that men and women differ with respect to the number of BFUs-capital correctly listed, and on the systemizing score, with men showing more right answers and being more systemizing than women. Men and women did not differ in locating the BFUs in the map, but men had more correct answers overall. In terms of distance estimation once again men showed better spatial representation than women. These findings indicate the influence of sex and systematizing on the representation of cognitive maps.
Article
Full-text available
The present study examined sex and sexual orientation differences in empathizing-systemizing (E-S) in a Chinese sample of 239 heterosexual men, 266 heterosexual women, 492 gay men, and 430 lesbian women recruited via the Internet. Empathizing and systemizing were assessed using two different abbreviated 8-item scales derived from longer original scales. There was a significant sex difference in S, with heterosexual men scoring significantly higher on S than heterosexual women, but there was no significant sex difference in E. For men, there were significant sexual orientation differences in E and S. Heterosexual men scored significantly higher on S than gay men, and gay men scored significantly higher on E than heterosexual men. For women, there were no significant differences in E and S between heterosexual and lesbian women. The results indicate a cross-cultural consistency of sex differences in S, while suggesting that E is influenced by culture.
Article
Full-text available
Este artigo relata em que medida homens e mulheres se diferenciam quanto ao estilo cognitivo de sistematização e à habilidade de construção de mapas cognitivos. Participaram da pesquisa 214 pessoas (57% mulheres) de João Pessoa (PB), com idade média de 29,3 anos. Elas foram requeridas a listar as Unidades da Federação (UFs) e capitais brasileiras, indicar em mapa esquemático as UFs do país, estimar as distâncias entre capitais e responder o quociente de sistematização. Os resultados indicaram que homens e mulheres se diferenciam em relação ao número de acertos entre capitais e à pontuação em sistematização; em média, os homens apresentam mais acertos e são mais sistematizadores. Em relação à estimativa de distâncias, verificou-se uma configuração espacial mais adequada para as pessoas do sexo masculino. Concluindo, o sexo e o estilo cognitivo de sistematização influenciam a construção de mapas cognitivos.
Article
Full-text available
This article evaluates a thesis containing three interconnected propositions. First, romantic love is a "commitment device" for motivating pair-bonding in humans. Second, pair-bonding facilitated the idiosyncratic life history of hominins, helping to provide the massive investment required to rear children. Third, managing long-term pair bonds (along with family relationships) facilitated the evolution of social intelligence and cooperative skills. We evaluate this thesis by integrating evidence from a broad range of scientific disciplines. First, consistent with the claim that romantic love is an evolved commitment device, our review suggests that it is universal; suppresses mate-search mechanisms; has specific behavioral, hormonal, and neuropsychological signatures; and is linked to better health and survival. Second, we consider challenges to this thesis posed by the existence of arranged marriage, polygyny, divorce, and infidelity. Third, we show how the intimate relationship mind seems to be built to regulate and monitor relationships. Fourth, we review comparative evidence concerning links among mating systems, reproductive biology, and brain size. Finally, we discuss evidence regarding the evolutionary timing of shifts to pair-bonding in hominins. We conclude there is interdisciplinary support for the claim that romantic love and pair-bonding, along with alloparenting, played critical roles in the evolution of Homo sapiens. © The Author(s) 2014.
Article
Full-text available
The proportion of men and women workers varies among occupation types. There are several factors that may contribute to occupational segregation by gender. Using a large U.S. sample (n = 2149), we examine the extent to which occupational segregation can be attributed to gender differences in empathizing and systematizing: psychological dimensions which theorists argue represent meaningful differences between men and women. Of the eight occupational categories for which employee gender and occupation type were associated at the p < .01 level, four of these--Construction, Professional/Scientific/Technical fields, Management, and Education--were partially mediated by systemizing and/or empathizing scores, which typically accounted for 10-20% of the observed gender differences. For other areas, like Health, gender differences were not mediated by either measure.
Article
Full-text available
This report documents gender and sexual orientation differences in occupational interests for a large sample of North American college students. The students rated their interests in 26 different occupations. Seventeen of the occupations were of greater interest to males and the remaining nine appealed more to females. Regarding sexual orientation, male homosexuals expressed significantly more interest in all nine female-preferred occupations than did heterosexual males; and, in most cases, bisexual males expressed preferences that were intermediate in this regard. For females, sexual orientation was related to interests in eight occupations. Six of these were in the direction of female homosexuals (or bisexuals) preferring more male-typical occupations. The remaining two occupations – those of accountant and head of a corporation – were actually of greater interest to heterosexual females than to homosexual (or bisexual) females. We conclude that (a) pronounced gender differences exist in people’s interests in many occupations, and (b) there are substantial tendencies for homosexuals (and bisexuals to a lesser degree) to have occupational interests that gravitate toward what is typical of the opposite gender rather than their own gender. These “contra-sex” occupational interests among non-heterosexuals were considerably more pronounced in males than in females.
Article
Full-text available
Gender differences in competitiveness have been hypothesized as a potential explanation for gender differences in education and labor market outcomes. We examine the predictive power of a standard laboratory experimental measure of competitiveness for the later important choice of academic track of secondary school students in the Netherlands. Although boys and girls display similar levels of academic ability, boys choose substantially more prestigious academic tracks, where more prestigious tracks are more math- and science-intensive. Our experimental measure shows that boys are also substantially more competitive than girls. We find that competitiveness is strongly positively correlated with choosing more prestigious academic tracks even conditional on academic ability. Most important, we find that the gender difference in competitiveness accounts for a substantial portion (about 20%) of the gender difference in track choice. JEL Codes: C9, I20, J24, J16.