ArticlePDF Available

The herbicide atrazine induces hyperactivity and compromises tadpole detection of predator chemical cues

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The ability to detect chemical cues is often critical for freshwater organisms to avoid predation and find food and mates. In particular, reduced activity and avoidance of chemical cues signaling predation risk are generally adaptive behaviors that reduce prey encounter rates with predators. Here, we examined the effects of the common herbicide atrazine on the ability of Cuban tree frog (Osteopilus septentrionalis) tadpoles to detect and respond to chemical cues from larval dragonfly (Libellulidae sp.) predators. Tadpoles exposed to an estimated environmental concentration of atrazine (calculated using US EPA software; measured concentration: 178 µg/L) were significantly hyperactive relative to those exposed to solvent control. Additionally, control tadpoles significantly avoided predator chemical cues, but tadpoles exposed to atrazine did not. These results are consistent with previous studies that have demonstrated that ecologically relevant concentrations of atrazine can induce hyperactivity and impair the olfactory abilities of other freshwater vertebrates. We call for additional studies examining the role of chemical contaminants in disrupting chemical communication and the quantification of subsequent impacts on the fitness and population dynamics of wildlife. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Environmental Toxicology
THE HERBICIDE ATRAZINE INDUCES HYPERACTIVITY AND COMPROMISES TADPOLE
DETECTION OF PREDATOR CHEMICAL CUES
MACKENZIE EHRSAM,SARAH A. KNUTIE, and JASON R. ROHR*
Department of Integrative Biology, University of South Florida, Tampa, Florida, USA
(Submitted 31 August 2015; Returned for Revision 9 November 2015; Accepted 20 January 2016)
Abstract: The ability to detect chemical cues is often critical for freshwater organisms to avoid predation and nd food and mates. In
particular, reduced activity and avoidance of chemical cues signaling predation risk are generally adaptive behaviors that reduce prey
encounter rates with predators. The present study examined the effects of the common herbicide atrazine on the ability of Cuban tree frog
(Osteopilus septentrionalis) tadpoles to detect and respond to chemical cues from larval dragony(Libellulidae sp.) predators. Tadpoles
exposed to an estimated environmental concentration of atrazine (calculated using US Environmental Protection Agency software;
measured concentration, 178 mg/L) were signicantly hyperactive relative to those exposed to solvent control. In addition, control
tadpoles signicantly avoided predator chemical cues, but tadpoles exposed to atrazine did not. These results are consistent with previous
studies that have demonstrated that ecologically relevant concentrations of atrazine can induce hyperactivity and impair the olfactory
abilities of other freshwater vertebrates. The authors call for additional studies examining the role of chemical contaminants in disrupting
chemical communication and the quantication of subsequent impacts on the tness and population dynamics of wildlife. Environ
Toxicol Chem 2016;9999:16. #2016 SETAC
Keywords: Amphibian Behavior Infodisruption Olfaction Predation
INTRODUCTION
Infodisruption, dened as chemical contaminants disrupting
communication within or among organisms, has 2 modes of
expression: 1) an external interorganismal disruption that causes
a breakdown in detection or production of chemical signals
between senders and receivers, and 2) an internal intra-
organismal disruption that can affect cell-to-cell communica-
tion [1]. Several chemical contaminants are infodisruptors, and
both internal and external infodisruption can affect organisms
health [1,2]. For many organisms, external infodisruption can
interfere with locating predators, mates, and food [1,2].
Similarly, endocrine disruptors, a type of internal organismal
infodisruptor (which eventually can also affect communication
between the sexes) [1], have been implicated in the collapse
of wildlife populations [3]. Unlike internal infodisruptors
(e.g., endocrine disruptors), external infodisruptors that
affect communication among individuals have not been well
studied [1].
For freshwater organisms, chemical cues are often cru-
cial [46]. This is because visual and auditory cues tend to
attenuate rapidly through water. In addition, turbidity in
freshwater ecosystems can further make visual cues unreliable.
Consequently, most freshwater organisms rely heavily on
chemical communication to avoid predation and nd food and
mates [7,8], which is why external interorganismal infodisrup-
tors have the potential to adversely affect the tness of
freshwater organisms [1,2].
One of the most common chemical contaminants in
freshwater ecosystems is the widely used herbicide 2-chloro-
4-(ethylamino)-6-(isopropylamino)-S-triazine (atrazine) [9].
Because of its heavy use, persistence, and mobility, atrazine is
found commonly in freshwater environments where many sh
and amphibians develop [10,11]. Atrazine has a variety of effects
on freshwater organisms, including sh and amphibians. For
example, atrazine has been reported to affect behavior [12,13],
physiology, and growth [14,15]; elevate mortality [1618];
increase infections and suppresses immunity [1922]; and induce
community-wide, indirect effects [2326]. A recent meta-
analysis revealed that all of these effects appear to be general,
with the exception of elevated mortality [9].
More specically, atrazine has been reported as both an
intraorganismal and an interorganismal infodisruptor. Atrazine
has been documented as an endocrine disruptor of freshwater
sh and amphibians [9,27,28]. In addition, several studies
provide evidence that exposure to ecologically relevant
concentrations of atrazine can reduce the olfactory abilities of
sh and craysh [2935]. For example, atrazine impaired
olfaction in salmon, with adverse effects on imprinting, homing,
and migratory behaviors [34]. Other studies have shown that
atrazine exposure decreased the response of male salmon to
reproductive priming hormones released in the urine of female
salmon [29,33] and decreased the response of goldsh to
chemical cues signaling predation [32]. Several sh studies
using electro-olfactograms, which measure the changing
electrical potentials of the olfactory epithelium, found that
short term, low-level exposure to atrazine reduced electro-
olfactogram responses and olfactory-mediated behav-
iors [30,31]. In contrast, olfaction of American toad (Anaxyrus
americanus) tadpoles was not signicantly affected by
atrazine [36]. Additional studies are needed on amphibians to
assess whether atrazine generally has adverse effects on
olfaction in freshwater vertebrates. In the present study, we
evaluated whether exposing Cuban tree frog (Osteopilus
septentrionalis) tadpoles to the estimated environmental
concentration of atrazine affects their abilities to detect
chemical cues from predatory dragony larvae (Libellulidae
sp.) and to exhibit appropriate antipredator behaviors. Given
* Address correspondence to jasonrohr@gmail.com
Published online 22 January 2016 in Wiley Online Library
(wileyonlinelibrary.com).
DOI: 10.1002/etc.3377
Environmental Toxicology and Chemistry, Vol. 9999, No. 9999, pp. 1–6, 2016
#2016 SETAC
Printed in the USA
1
that there are more studies suggesting that atrazine is an
infodisruptor than not, we hypothesized that exposure to
atrazine would decrease tadpole detection of chemical cues
from predators.
Background on field concentrations of atrazine
Although atrazine has been banned in Europe, it remains one
of the most commonly used pesticides in the United States and
the rest of the world, where it is used heavily in corn and
sorghum production [9]. Atrazine is relatively persistent, with
an aqueous photolysis half-life of 335 d under natural light and
neutral pH and half-lives that can exceed 3 mo in mescosms [9].
Atrazine is also relatively mobile, regularly entering water
bodies with runoff and rain inputs [9]. Atrazine has been
detected in rain or air from European and US sites more than any
other currently used pesticide [37].
In the present study, we tested the effects of the estimated
environmental concentration (EEC) of atrazine on tadpole
olfaction. The EEC is the concentration estimated to enter a
standardized farm pond that is a standardized distance from the
application site (based on particular soil properties) [23]. The
EEC was calculated using US Environmental Protection
Agency (USEPA) GENEEC software (based on applications
to corn) and is the concentration used in registering
pesticides [23]. The measured concentration to which tadpoles
were exposed was 178 mg/L of atrazine.
To place this concentration in an ecological context, we
provide background on measured concentrations of atrazine in
the environment. The maximum reported wet deposition of
atrazine is 154 mg/L from Iowa precipitation [38]. Data on
atrazine concentrations in surface water are more abundant for
lotic (streams and rivers) than lentic (lakes, ponds, wetlands,
ditches) systems, primarily because of the extensive stream
monitoring conducted by the US Geological Survey National
Water Quality Assessment project [39]. Atrazine has been
detected in streams at 200 mg/L and repeatedly has been
detected in streams above 100 mg/L [39]. However, more
amphibians develop in lentic than lotic systems, where water is
not replenished and chemicals can concentrate as lentic systems
dry. Rohr and McCoy [9] reported that maximum reported
atrazine concentrations in lentic systems are often 2.5 to 10
times higher than maximum concentrations in lotic systems. For
example, atrazine concentrations in lentic surface waters have
been reported at 131 mg/L in Mississippi (USA) [40], 681 mg/L
in Ontario (Canada) [41], 850 mg/L in Florida (USA) [42],
1068 mg/L in Kansas (USA) [43], 1096 mg/L in Nebraska
(USA) [44], and 2300 mg/L in Iowa (USA) [45]. These ndings
seem to belie probabilistic aquatic ecological risk assessments
for atrazine that suggest that concentrations near or above the
EEC are extremely rare [46] and demonstrate that measured
concentrations of atrazine near and well above the EEC are
widespread. In summary, the EEC is the concentration the
USEPA estimates to enter farm ponds regularly and is not a
worst-case scenario, because it was exceeded in at least 4 states
in the United States and in Canada. As such, the concentration of
atrazine we test in the present study is ecologically relevant
based both on regulatory standards and measured eld
concentrations.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Tadpole rearing
Cuban tree frog tadpoles were collected from the Botanical
Gardens at the University of South Florida in Tampa, Florida.
Once in the lab, tadpoles were maintained under laboratory
conditions at 22 8C and a 12:12-h light:dark cycle. Eighty
tadpoles (Gosner stages ranging from 25 to 41; mean standard
error [SE], 34.00 0.53) were kept individually in 250 mL of
pond water in a standard 0.47-L glass mason jar. One-half the
tadpoles were then exposed to 7.81 mL of either acetone or a
3.26-mg/mL stock solution of atrazine dissolved in acetone, to
achieve an estimated actual concentration of 178 mg/L of
atrazine (measured with an Abraxis enzyme-linked immuno-
sorbent assay microtiter plate kit; Abraxis). This is the estimated
environmental concentration of atrazine for farm ponds.
Tadpoles were exposed to acetone control or atrazine for 7 d.
They were fed agarose discs containing spirulina and aked sh
food ad libitum, and we checked survival daily.
Predator cue collection
Aquatic dragony larvae regularly prey on frog tadpoles [24]
and are often the dominant predator in temporary pond
ecosystems. We collected 12 Libellulidae dragony larvae
from Spectrum Pond at the University of South Florida and
maintained them in an 11.36-L aquarium lled with 5.68 L
of dechlorinated tap water for 24 h. This tank also contained
12 Cuban tree frog tadpoles to provide food for the dragony
larvae. The water in this tank served as our predator cue source.
Behavioral observations
For behavioral observations, 20 plastic shoeboxes were laid
out in a 5-by-4 grid, and each was lled with 2 L of pond water.
Observations were double blind, as the observer did not have
access to the treatment groups of the individual tadpoles at the
time of the observations. One tadpole was placed in each
shoebox; 10 of the tadpoles were exposed to acetone control,
and the other 10 were exposed to atrazine. Scan samples were
conducted for 8 min before cues were added, recording the
location of tadpoles in each shoebox (left or right side of
the arena) and whether individual tadpoles were moving.
After the initial 8-min period, a repeat pipetter was used to
add 3 mL of either a control (water) or experimental predator
cue to 1 side of the container, while an equal amount of water
was added to the opposite side of the container. Cue and control
solutions were added carefully and simultaneously so that
the liquid ran down the side of the container to minimize
disturbance. One-half the predator cue addition in each
treatment occurred on the right side of the container, and
one-half was added to the left side. Thus, one-half the atrazine-
exposed and one-half the acetone-exposed tadpoles received
predator cue on 1 side of the container and the other tadpoles
received water on both sides of the container. Having water on
both sides allowed us to quantify activity levels in the absence of
predator cue.
Immediately after cue additions, which took approximately
3 min, we performed scan samples for 20 min, again recording
the location and activity of tadpoles. Given that 1 experimenter
could only observe 20 tadpoles at a time, and we wanted the
same experimenter making all observations, we conducted 3
additional temporal blocks in sequence, so that all 80 tadpoles
were exposed to the cues in a timeframe of approximately 4 h.
Immediately after observations in the shoeboxes were complete,
all tadpoles were returned to their mason jars containing their
assigned chemical treatment. The next day, the same procedure
was repeated for all 80 tadpoles, except that any tadpole that was
exposed to the predator cue on day 1 received water on both
sides of the arena on day 2 and vice versa. This experimental
design allowed us to use each tadpole as its own internal control
2Environ Toxicol Chem 9999, 2016 M. Ehrsam et al.
for both activity and locational preference. After the 2 d of
observations were complete, tadpoles were euthanized with
MS-222 solution. We then recorded the snoutvent length, mass
(g), and Gosner developmental stage [21] of each tadpole and
preserved tadpoles individually in 70% ethanol.
To determine the mean time it took the predator chemical
cues to diffuse across the test chamber, in separate trials, we
added 5 drops of food coloring to a randomly selected end of
5 test chambers each containing a tadpole. Time was recorded
when the dye had diffused to the halfway point of the tank and
again when the dye reached the opposite end of the tank.
Data analysis
We rst transformed the data, giving observations on the
same side of the predator cue a value of 1 and on the opposite
side as the cue a value of 1. Hence, negative values refer to
avoidance and positive values refer to attraction. If a tadpole
was moving during an observation, it received a 1, and if it was
not moving it received a 0. Hence, the average of the activity
data reects the proportion of observations where the tadpole
was moving.
Next, we conducted separate 2-way analyses of variance
(ANOVAs) on average location and activity before any
olfactory cues were added, testing for main and interactive
effects of the atrazine and predator treatments. This allowed us
to determine whether there were any differences between
treatments before the predator cues were added. We knew that
the predator cue would eventually diffuse across the entire test
arena within the 30-min trial. Thus, we were expecting to
detect an atrazine-by-predator-by-time interaction, because we
hypothesized that the solvent-exposed but not atrazine-exposed
animals would avoid the predator chemical cue early in the trial
when there was a clear gradient, but avoidance would eventually
subside once the cue fully dispersed. To conduct these analyses,
we averaged the location and activity data for each of the
four 5-min intervals of the 20-min trials. This enhanced our
likelihood of meeting the assumptions of ANOVA (because of
the central limit theorem). We then conducted a repeated
measures ANOVA with individual as a random effect (so we
could compare location and activity of each individual in the
presence and absence of predator cues) and tested for the main
and interactive effects of atrazine treatment, predator treatment,
and time on location and activity responses. Thirteen tadpoles
died during this experiment and thus were excluded from any
statistical analyses. All analyses were conducted using Statistica
6.0 (Statsoft).
RESULTS
Before any predator cues were added, the tadpoles showed
no signicant difference in their average location in the atrazine
or predator cue treatments (p>0.05; Table 1 and Figure 1A).
After the predator cue was introduced, tadpoles exposed to
solvent, but not those exposed to atrazine, signicantly avoided
the predator chemical cue for the rst 10 min of observations but
exhibited no avoidance during the last 10 min (Figure 2). This
Table 1. Results of analyses of variance on tadpolelocation before and after
predator cue additions
Effect df F p
Pre-predator cue
Intercept 1 1.64 0.204
Atrazine 1 0.74 0.392
Error 67
Predator 1 0.02 0.883
Predator atrazine 1 0.44 0.507
Error 67
Post-predator cue
Intercept 1 0.21 0.648
Atrazine 1 1.82 0.182
Error 67
Time 3 0.37 0.778
Time atrazine 3 0.76 0.519
Error 201
Predator 1 0.67 0.416
Predator atrazine 1 0.12 0.730
Error 67
Time predator 3 2.10 0.102
Time predator atrazine 3 3.20 0.024
Error 201
Figure 1. Effect of atrazine on mean (1 standard error [SE]) tadpole
location before predator cue addition (A). Effect of atrazine (B) and predator
cue (C) treatments on mean (1 SE) activity of tadpoles before and after
predator cue introductions.
Atrazine reduces detection of predator chemical cues Environ Toxicol Chem 9999, 2016 3
resulted in a signicant atrazine-by-predator cue-by-time
interaction (F
3,201
¼3.20, p¼0.024; Table 1). It took an
average of 8.8 0.97 min (SE; n¼5) for the food coloring to
diffuse fully across the test chamber. Hence, tadpole avoidance
of the predator cue seemed to last for approximately as long as
the cue gradient persisted.
Before any predator cues were added, tadpoles exposed to
atrazine were signicantly more active than tadpoles exposed to
solvent control (F
1,67
¼7.84, p¼0.007; Figure 1B); but activity
levels did not differ signicantly between tadpoles assigned to
receive predator cue or not (Table 2). After the predator cue
was introduced, tadpoles exposed to atrazine remained signi-
cantly more active than tadpoles exposed to solvent control
(F
1,67
¼7.13, p¼0.009; Figure 1B). The tadpoles, however, did
not signicantly reduce their activity in response to the predator
cue (Figure 1C) but did tend to exhibit less activity later than
earlier in the trial (F
3,201
¼4.45, p¼0.005; Figure 1C). There
were no signicant statistical interactions on tadpole activity
(Table 2).
DISCUSSION
We found evidence that atrazine exposure impairs the ability
of Cuban tree frog tadpoles to detect chemical cues from
predators. Tadpoles exposed to solvent control signicantly
avoided predator chemical cues, whereas tadpoles exposed
to atrazine did not (Figure 2). In addition, we revealed that
exposure to ecologically relevant concentrations of atrazine
caused hyperactivity in Cuban tree frog tadpoles compared with
tadpoles exposed to solvent control (Figure 1B). These results
have many similarities with other studies examining the effects
of atrazine on the behavior and olfactory abilities of amphibians
and sh.
Several other studies have shown that ecologically relevant
concentrations of atrazine affect the motor activity of sh and
amphibians [9]. After just 1 d of exposure to environmentally
relevant concentrations of atrazine, red drum sh larva
exhibited hyperactivity in the form of high swimming speeds,
erratic swimming paths, and increased energy use and metabolic
rates [47]. Another study investigating the sublethal effects of
atrazine in goldsh found that short-term exposures to low
atrazine concentrations caused activity increases, including
elevated burst swimming and surfacing activities [32]. Sala-
mander larva exposed to various concentrations of atrazine
exhibited higher activity than control larvae [11,14], and these
effects persisted for several months after atrazine exposure
ceased [12,13].
Our nding that atrazine exposure impairs the ability of
Cuban tree frog tadpoles to detect chemical cues that predators
release is consistent with several studies showing that ecolo-
gically relevant concentrations of atrazine can compromise the
olfactory abilities of sh and craysh [2935]. However, the
present studys results are inconsistent with the only other study
that examined atrazine-related infodisruption in tadpoles [36].
Importantly, every study that detected atrazine-associated
infodisruption tested for infodisruption while the sh, craysh,
or amphibian was being exposed to atrazine [2935]. The single
study that did not detect infodisruption associated with atrazine-
exposed tadpoles transferred the tadpoles to atrazine-free water,
provided a 30-min acclimation period, and then introduced
chemical cues to elicit behavioral responses [36]. If the effect of
atrazine on the olfactory organs of vertebrates is short-lived,
Figure 2. The relationship between time since predator (Pred) cue addition and exposure to atrazine (Atr) or solvent (Solv) on the mean location (1 standard
error [SE] to reduce overlap of SE bars) of tadpoles. Positive values represent attraction to the predator cue, and negative values represent avoidance of the
predator cue.
Table 2. Results of analyses of variance on tadpole activity before and after
predator cue additions
Effect df F p
Pre-predator cue
Intercept 1 370.63 <0.001
Atrazine 1 7.84 0.007
Error 67
Predator 1 0.30 0.583
Predator atrazine 1 1.04 0.313
Error 67
Post-predator cue
Intercept 1 167.63 <0.001
Atrazine 1 7.13 0.009
Error 67
Time 3 4.45 0.005
Time atrazine 3 0.29 0.834
Error 201
Predator 1 2.05 0.157
Predator atrazine 1 2.67 0.107
Error 67
Time predator 3 0.51 0.674
Time predator atrazine 3 0.45 0.717
Error 201
4Environ Toxicol Chem 9999, 2016 M. Ehrsam et al.
then 30 min exposure to clean water might have been sufcient
time to rinse the atrazine from the olfactory organ, allow for at
least partial recovery of olfactory function, or prevent the
detection of any atrazine-induced olfactory impairment.
However, we cannot rule out species-level differences in
sensitivity to atrazine or other differences among the experi-
ments as explanations for the differences in their results. We
encourage future studies to evaluate species-level variation in
olfactory sensitivity to atrazine and how both durations and
concentrations of atrazine exposure affect amphibian olfaction.
Amphibians are in decline globally [48,49], and although it is
theoretically possible that atrazine-induced infodisruption could
contribute to these declines, we caution against extrapolating
these results to the population viability of amphibians in the wild
for several reasons. First, if the effects of atrazine are found to be
short-lived, it might suggest that any infodisruption might not
have substantial impacts on wildlife populations. Hence,
quantifying the duration of atrazine effects on olfaction would
offer insight into both discrepancies in the results among studies
and the likelihood that any infodisruption would have
population-level impacts. Second, if the olfaction of predators
is equally or more impaired by atrazine than the olfaction of
amphibian prey, then any infodisruption could benet the prey
more than the predators. It is important, therefore, to understand
the net effects of pesticides on speciesinteractions before
reliably extrapolating to effects in nature [19]. Finally, there are
many factors that are not captured in a laboratory study that
could affect the effects of atrazine in the wild, such as variation
in temperature and organic material on which pesticides can
bind [18]. Regardless of how any atrazine-induced infodisrup-
tion inuences populations of vertebrates in the wild, the present
studys results contribute to the weight of evidence that atrazine
is, indeed, an interorganismal infodisruptor of vertebrates.
We encourage further studies to more thoroughly quantify
the consequences of such infodisruption and whether they are
capable of contributing to declines of biodiversity.
AcknowledgmentWe thank B. Roznik and C. Gabor for help and support.
This research was supported by grants from the National Science
Foundation (EF-1241889), National Institutes of Health (R01GM109499,
R01TW010286), US Department of Agriculture (NRI 2006-01370, 2009-
35102-0543), and US Environmental Protection Agency (CAREER
83518801) to J. Rohr.
Data availabilityData are available by contacting the corresponding
author (jasonrohr@gmail.com).
REFERENCES
1. Lurling M, Scheffer M. 2007. Info-disruption: Pollution and the transfer
of chemical information between organisms. Trends Ecol Evol
22:374379.
2. Schmidt KA, Dall SRX, van Gils JA. 2010. The ecology of information:
An overview on the ecological signicance of making informed
decisions. Oikos 119:304316.
3. Kidd KA, Blancheld PJ, Mills KH, Palace VP, Evans RE, Lazorchak
JM, Flick RW. 2007. Collapse of a sh population after exposure to a
synthetic estrogen. P Natl Acad Sci USA 104:88978901.
4. Rohr JR, Madison DM, Sullivan AM. 2002. Sex differences and
seasonal trade-offs in response to injured and non-injured conspecics
in red-spotted newts, Notophthalmus viridescens.Behav Ecol Sociobiol
52:385393.
5. Chivers DP, Smith RJF. 1998. Chemical alarm signalling in aquatic
predator-prey systems: A review and prospectus. Ecoscience
5:338352.
6. Rohr JR, Park D, Sullivan AM, McKenna M, Propper CR, Madison
DM. 2005. Operational sex ratio in newts: Field responses and
characterization of a constituent chemical cue. Behav Ecol 16:286
293.
7. Rohr JR, Madison DM, Sullivan AM. 2003. On temporal variation and
conicting selection pressures: A test of theory using newts. Ecology
84:18161826.
8. Kats LB, Dill LM. 1998. The scent of death: Chemosensory assessment
of predation risk by prey animals. Ecoscience 5:361394.
9. Rohr JR, McCoy KA. 2010. A qualitative meta-analysis reveals
consistent effects of atrazine on freshwater sh and amphibians.
Environ Health Persp 18:2032.
10. Knutson MG, Richardson WB, Reineke DM, Gray BR, Parmelee JR,
Weick SE. 2004. Agricultural ponds support amphibian populations.
Ecol Appl 14:669684.
11. Rohr JR, Elskus AA, Shepherd BS, Crowley PH, McCarthy TM,
Niedzwiecki JH, Sager T, Sih A, Palmer BD. 2003. Lethal and sublethal
effects of atrazine, carbaryl, endosulfan, and octylphenol on the
streamside salamander, Ambystoma barbouri.Environ Toxicol Chem
22:23852392.
12. Rohr JR, Palmer BD. 2005. Aquatic herbicide exposure increases
salamander desiccation risk eight months later in a terrestrial
environment. Environ Toxicol Chem 24:12531258.
13. Rohr JR, Palmer BD. 2013. Climate change, multiple stressors, and the
decline of ectotherms. Conserv Biol 27:741751.
14. Rohr JR, Elskus AA, Shepherd BS, Crowley PH, McCarthy TM,
Niedzwiecki JH, Sager T, Sih A, Palmer BD. 2004. Multiple stressors
and salamanders: Effects of an herbicide, food limitation, and
hydroperiod. Ecol Appl 14:10281040.
15. Larson DL, McDonald S, Fivizzani AJ, Newton WE, Hamilton SJ.
1998. Effects of the herbicide atrazine on Ambystoma tigrinum
metamorphosis: Duration, larval growth, and hormonal response. Phys
Zool 71:671679.
16. Rohr JR, Sager T, Sesterhenn TM, Palmer BD. 2006. Exposure,
postexposure, and density-mediated effects of atrazine on amphibians:
Breaking down net effects into their parts. Environ Health Persp
114:4650.
17. Storrs SI, Kiesecker JM. 2004. Survivorship patterns of larval
amphibians exposed to low concentrations of atrazine. Environ Health
Persp 112:10541057.
18. Rohr JR, Sesterhenn TM, Stieha C. 2011. Will climate change
reduce the effects of a pesticide on amphibians? Partitioning effects
on exposure and susceptibility to pollution. Glob Change Biol
17:657666.
19. Rohr JR, Raffel TR, Sessions SK, Hudson PJ. 2008. Understanding the
net effects of pesticides on amphibian trematode infections. Ecol Appl
18:17431753.
20. Kerby JL, Storfer A. 2009. Combined effects of atrazine and
chlorpyrifos on susceptibility of the tiger salamander to Ambystoma
tigrinum virus. EcoHealth 6:9198.
21. Rohr JR, Schotthoefer AM, Raffel TR, Carrick HJ, Halstead N,
Hoverman JT, Johnson CM, Johnson LB, Lieske C, Piwoni MD, Schoff
PK, Beasley VR. 2008. Agrochemicals increase trematode infections in
a declining amphibian species. Nature 455:12351239.
22. Rohr JR, Raffel TR, Halstead NT, McMahon TA, Johnson SA,
Boughton RK, Martin LB. 2013. Early-life exposure to a herbicide has
enduring effects on pathogen-induced mortality. P Roy Soc Lond B Bio
280:20131502.
23. Halstead NT, McMahon TA, Johnson SA, Raffel TR, Romansic JM,
Crumrine PW, Rohr JR. 2014. Community ecology theory predicts the
effects of agrochemical mixtures on aquatic biodiversity and ecosystem
properties. Ecol Lett 17:932941.
24. Rohr JR, Crumrine PW. 2005. Effects of an herbicide and an insecticide
on pond community structure and processes. Ecol Appl 15:11351147.
25. Staley ZR, Rohr JR, Harwood VJ. 2011. Test of direct and indirect
effects of agrochemicals on the survival of fecal indicator bacteria. Appl
Environ Microbiol 77:87658774.
26. Staley ZR, Rohr JR, Senkbeil JK, Harwood VJ. 2014. Agrochemicals
indirectly increase survival of E. coli O157: H7 and indicator bacteria
by reducing ecosystem services. Ecol Appl 24:19451953.
27. Hayes TB, Khoury V, Narayan A, Nazir M, Park A, Brown T, Adame L,
Chan E, Buchholz D, Stueve T, Gallipeau S. 2010. Atrazine induces
complete feminization and chemical castration in male African clawed
frogs (Xenopus laevis). P Natl Acad Sci USA 107:46124617.
28. Langlois VS, Carew AC, Pauli BD, Wade MG, Cooke GM, Trudeau
VL. 2010. Low levels of the herbicide atrazine alter sex ratios and
reduce metamorphic success in Rana pipiens tadpoles raised in outdoor
mesocosms. Environ Health Persp 118:552557.
29. Moore A, Waring CP. 1998. Mechanistic effects of a triazine pesticide
on reproductive endocrine function in mature male Atlantic salmon
(Salmo salar L.) parr. Pestic Biochem Phys 62:4150.
Atrazine reduces detection of predator chemical cues Environ Toxicol Chem 9999, 2016 5
30. Tierney KB, Ross PS, Jarrard HE, Delaney KR, Kennedy CJ. 2006.
Changes in juvenile coho salmon electro-olfactogram during and after
short-term exposure to current-use pesticides. Environ Toxicol Chem
25:28092817.
31. Tierney KB, Singh CR, Ross PS, Kennedy CJ. 2007. Relating olfactory
neurotoxicity to altered olfactory-mediated behaviors in rainbow
trout exposed to three currently-used pesticides. Aquat Toxicol
81:5564.
32. Saglio P, Trijasse S. 1998. Behavioral responses to atrazine and diuron
in goldsh. Arch Environ Contam Toxicol 35:484491.
33. Moore A, Lower N. 2001. The impact of two pesticides on olfactory-
mediated endocrine function in mature male Atlantic salmon (Salmo
salar L.) parr. Comp Biochem Phys B 129:269276.
34. Moore A, Lower N, Mayer I, Greenwood L. 2007. The impact
of a pesticide on migratory activity and olfactory function in
Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar L.) smolts. Aquaculture 273:350
359.
35. Belanger RM, Peters TJ, Sabhapathy GS, Khan S, Katta J, Abraham
NK. 2015. Atrazine exposure affects the ability of craysh (Orconectes
rusticus) to localize a food odor source. Arch Environ Contam Toxicol
68:636645.
36. Rohr JR, Swan A, Raffel TR, Hudson PJ. 2009. Parasites, info-
disruption, and the ecology of fear. Oecologia 159:447454.
37. van Dijk HFG, Guicherit R. 1999. Atmospheric dispersion of current-
use pesticides: A review of the evidence from monitoring studies. Water
Air Soil Poll 115:2170.
38. Hateld JL, Wesley CK, Prueger JH, Pfeiffer RL. 1996. Herbicide and
nitrate distribution in central Iowa rainfall. J Environ Qual 25:259
264.
39. US Geological Survey. National Water Quality Assessment (NAWQA)
Program. cited 2015 November 24]. Available from: http://cida.usgs.
gov/nawqa_www/nawqa_data_redirect.html?p¼nawqa
40. Kroger R, Moore MT, Brandt JR. 2012. Current- and past-use pesticide
prevalence in drainage ditches in the Lower Mississippi Alluvial
Valley. Pest Manag Sci 68:303312.
41. Frank R, Braun HE, Ripley BD, Clegg BS. 1990. Contamination of
rural ponds with pesticide, 197185, Ontario, Canada. Bull Environ
Contam Toxicol 44:401409.
42. Graves GA, Wan YS, Fike DL. 2004. Water quality characteristics of
storm water from major land uses in South Florida. J Am Water Resour
As 40:14051419.
43. Kadoum AM, Mock DE. 1978. Herbicide and insecticide residues in
tailwater pits: Water and pit bottom soil from irrigated corn and
sorghum elds. J Agri Food Chem 26:4550.
44. Watanabe H, Watermeier NL, Steichen JM, Barnes P, Phong TK. 2007.
Impacts of tillage and application methods on atrazine and alachlor
losses from upland elds. Weed Biol Manag 7:4454.
45. Kolpin DW, SneckFahrer D, Hallberg GR, Libra RD. 1997. Temporal
trends of selected agricultural chemicals in Iowas groundwater, 1982
1995: Are things getting better? J Environ Qual 26:10071017.
46. Giddings JM, Anderson TA, Hall LW, Hosmer AJ, Kendall RJ,
Richards RP, Solomon KR, Williams WM. 2005. Atrazine in North
American Surface Waters: A Probabilistic Aquatic Ecological Risk
Assessment. Society for Envrionmental Toxicology and Chemistry,
Pensacola, FL, USA.
47. Alvarez MD, Fuiman LA. 2005. Environmental levels of atrazine and
its degradation products impair survival skills and growth of red drum
larvae. Aquat Toxicol 74:229241.
48. Rohr JR, Raffel TR. 2010. Linking global climate and temperature
variability to widespread amphibian declines putatively caused by
disease. P Natl Acad Sci USA 107:82698274.
49. Wake DB, Vredenburg VT. 2008. Are we in the midst of the sixth mass
extinction? A view from the world of amphibians. P Natl Acad Sci USA
105:1146611473.
6Environ Toxicol Chem 9999, 2016 M. Ehrsam et al.
... Freshwater organisms specially rely on chemical communication for their survival and most of their behaviors. ATR compromises the olfactory abilities of amphibians and fish, affecting their behavior, impairing detection of food source odors, possible mates, and predators, altering their physiology, growth, survival, and reproduction (Ehrsam et al., 2016;Tierney et al., 2007;Moore and Waring, 1998;Moore and Lower, 2001;Hussein et al., 1996;Belanger et al., 2016). For instance, in the Cuban tree frog tadpoles ATR exposure impairs the detection of chemical cues of predators (Ehrsam et al., 2016). ...
... ATR compromises the olfactory abilities of amphibians and fish, affecting their behavior, impairing detection of food source odors, possible mates, and predators, altering their physiology, growth, survival, and reproduction (Ehrsam et al., 2016;Tierney et al., 2007;Moore and Waring, 1998;Moore and Lower, 2001;Hussein et al., 1996;Belanger et al., 2016). For instance, in the Cuban tree frog tadpoles ATR exposure impairs the detection of chemical cues of predators (Ehrsam et al., 2016). In salmons, ATR exposure decreases the response of the male to reproductive hormones released in the urine of female salmon, promoting adverse effects on imprinting, homing, and migratory behaviors (Moore and Waring, 1998;Moore and Lower, 2001). ...
... Also, pesticides may enter the body via the digestive tract and initiate synucleinopathy in the gut, which may later spread to the brain (Braak et al., 2006). Several studies evaluating the toxicity of ATR exposure on olfaction have been performed on vertebrates, such as fish, crayfish, and frogs (Ehrsam et al., 2016;Tierney et al., 2007;Moore and Waring, 1998;Moore and Lower, 2001;Hussein et al., 1996;Belanger et al., 2016). In fish, low-level exposure to ATR changes the electrical response within the olfactory epithelium, measured by electro-olfactogram, and impairs the olfactory preference/avoidance to some amino acids (Tierney et al., 2007). ...
... In the 100 mg/L treatment, tadpoles moved significantly more without predator cues, which is the expected result if ammonium nitrate has no effect on the sensory system of the toads. The lack of significance in the rest of the treatments, however, could be due to the fact that as a result of the ammonium nitrate tadpoles could not detect the presence of predator cues in the water and exhibited no difference in behavior, which has been demonstrated in previous studies that exposed tadpoles to chemicals in the water [36]. For the Control treatment, we can argue that, as with R. dalmatina, these are the largest tadpoles in and as such have no significant decrease in movement due to predator cue detection. ...
... ATZ as an endocrine disruptor has also been shown to cause sex specific behavioral outcomes in various model organisms. A study in tadpoles observed hyperactivity in ATZ exposed tadpoles, and these tadpoles were also less likely to avoid predatory chemical cues (Ehrsam et al. 2016). A gestational ATZ exposure in C57BL/6 mice found dams had decreased novel object recognition and displayed hyperactivity, while juvenile offspring were hyperactive with an increase in anxiety-like behavior (Lin et al. 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
Atrazine (ATZ) is an herbicide commonly used on crops in the Midwestern US and other select global regions. The US Environmental Protection Agency ATZ regulatory limit is 3 parts per billion (ppb; μg/L), but this limit is often exceeded. ATZ has a long half-life, is a common contaminant of drinking water sources, and is indicated as an endocrine disrupting chemical in multiple species. The zebrafish was used to test the hypothesis that an embryonic parental ATZ exposure alters protein levels leading to modifications in morphology and behavior in developing progeny. Zebrafish embryos (F1) were collected from adults (F0) exposed to 0, 0.3, 3, or 30 ppb ATZ during embryogenesis. Differential proteomics, morphology, and behavior assays were completed with offspring aged 120 or 144 h with no additional chemical treatment. Proteomic analysis identified differential expression of proteins associated with neurological development and disease; and organ and organismal morphology, development, and injury, specifically the skeletomuscular system. Head length and ratio of head length to total length was significantly increased in the F1 of 0.3 and 30 ppb ATZ groups (p < 0.05). Based on molecular pathway alterations, further craniofacial morphology assessment found decreased distance for cartilaginous structures, decreased surface area and distance between saccular otoliths, and a more posteriorly positioned notochord (p < 0.05), indicating delayed ossification and skeletal growth. The visual motor response assay showed hyperactivity in progeny of the 30 ppb treatment group for distance moved and of the 0.3 and 30 ppb treatment groups for time spent moving (p < 0.05). Due to the changes in saccular otoliths, an acoustic startle assay was completed and showed decreased response in the 0.3 and 30 ppb treatments (p < 0.05). These findings suggest that a single embryonic parental exposure alters cellular pathways in their progeny that lead to perturbations in craniofacial development and behavior.
... Given this disparity in results, we would recommend that further study is needed to investigate the effects of 17β-trenbolone on amphibian behaviour before we can make any definitive conclusions about the impacts of this pollutant on amphibians, more generally. However, whilst literature investigating the effects of 17β-trenbolone on tadpole behaviour is scarce, other studies have, in agreement with the present study, found that tadpoles exposed to chemical pollutants (including EDCs) can exhibit higher levels of activity, and following a predator threat, reduce activity and swimming speed (Ehrsam et al., 2016;Gabor et al., 2019;Rohr and McCoy, 2010;Sievers et al., 2018b). The differences between our present and previous study (Orford et al., 2022) could therefore be due to species-specific responses to chemical exposure. ...
Article
Endocrine-disrupting chemicals-compounds that directly interfere with the endocrine system of exposed animals-are insidious environmental pollutants that can disrupt hormone function, even at very low concentrations. The dramatic impacts that some endocrine-disrupting chemicals can have on the reproductive development of wildlife are well documented. However, the potential of endocrine-disrupting chemicals to disrupt animal behaviour has received far less attention, despite the important links between behavioural processes and population-level fitness. Accordingly, we investigated the impacts of 14 and 21-day exposure to two environmentally realistic levels of 17β-trenbolone (4.6 and 11.2 ng/L), a potent endocrine-disrupting steroid and agricultural pollutant, on growth and behaviour in tadpoles of an anuran amphibian, the southern brown tree frog (Litoria ewingii). We found that 17β-trenbolone altered morphology, baseline activity and responses to a predatory threat, but did not affect anxiety-like behaviours in a scototaxis assay. Specifically, we found that tadpoles exposed to our high-17β-trenbolone treatment were significantly longer and heavier at 14 and 21 days. We also found that tadpoles exposed to 17β-trenbolone showed higher levels of baseline activity, and significantly reduced their activity following a simulated predator strike. These results provide insights into the wider repercussions of agricultural pollutants on key developmental and behavioural traits in aquatic species, and demonstrate the importance of behavioural studies in the ecotoxicological field.
... Moreover, chemical cues are usually more effective in aquatic environments (Ferland-Raymond et al. 2010), particularly in turbid ones (Ferrari et al. 2010). However, the presence of other chemical compounds in the water can impair cue detection or risk assessment, which may consequently affect the prey response to avoid the predator (Lürling and Scheffer 2007;Ehrsam et al. 2016). ...
Article
The capacity to identify predator chemical cues is extremely advantageous as it allows prey to avoid the predation sequence from the beginning. However, for aquatic organisms, identification can be constrained by the presence of other substances, such as plant chemical compounds. Despite its ecological implications, there is a lack of knowledge on the potential chemical interference of sympatric plants to the surrounding aquatic fauna. In this context, our study aims to understand the consequences that chemical interference can entail in the anti-predator responses of tadpoles of the Cope's toad (Rhinella diptycha). We conducted an outdoor experiment, where we compared the anti-predator responses of R. diptycha tadpoles to a natural predator (giant water bug) with and without adding chemicals of a potentially toxic native plant (Microlobius foetidus) to the water. Tadpoles showed an increase in grouping behaviour and a reduction in activity in the predator treatment. Moreover, our results indicate that the chemical compounds of the sympatric plant modified tadpole behavioural responses, disrupting grouping behaviour while maintaining reduced activity. These findings help understand the complexity of chemical communication in aquatic habitats and the consequences on animal-plant interactions and conservation.
... Cuban tree frogs appear to be highly tolerant to variation in environmental stressors such as acidic water [76], temperature [77], and elevated salinity [78]. However, they do not appear to be generally more tolerant than native species to chemical contaminants including atrazine [79] and the fungicide Diversity 2023, 15, 215 7 of 10 chlorothalonil [14]. Cuban tree frogs exposed to agrochemicals can also experience increased susceptibility to the chytrid fungus well after exposure [80]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Exposure to agrochemicals can have lethal and sublethal effects on amphibians. Most toxicology studies only examine exposure during the aquatic larval stage. Survival of the juvenile stage is the most important for population persistence and it is critical to understand the potential impacts of exposure during this life stage. We investigated how short-term exposure to triclopyr, an herbicide commonly used in forestry management, might impact several juvenile traits. To determine if juveniles perceived exposure as an environmental stressor, we measured their release of corticosterone. We also examined dispersal traits by measuring foraging and hopping behavior. We found no evidence that exposure negatively impacted these traits or was a stressor. Our results provide a preliminary assessment of the potential impact of triclopyr on juvenile amphibians, but we recommend additional research on the effects of agrochemicals on juvenile amphibians.
Article
Animals that live in human‐impacted landscapes experience an onslaught of novel stimuli that may interfere with natural communication pathways. During mating, this interference may alter the criteria deployed to assess potential mates as males and females find they must shift their focus and emphasize alternative sensory modalities. The wolf spider, Tigrosa helluo (Araneae, Lycosidae) is common in agricultural fields where commercial formulations of herbicides with glyphosate as the active ingredient are regularly applied. With the development of genetically resistant crops, glyphosate‐based herbicides have become among the most widely used and heavily applied agricultural chemicals in the world. In a laboratory experiment, we explored the effects of this herbicide on male—female interactions during courtship, mating, and sexual cannibalism. We expected that it might impact assessment such that there was a shift in the features that were important to the outcome of mating interactions. When herbicide was present, female body condition, an indication of recent feeding success, became important to mating success. This result was, in part, due to higher rates of sexual cannibalism in parings of males and females with low body condition values. The leg raises and pedipalp waves that males perform in courtship were not affected by herbicide nor were they related to mating success but, across all treatments, they were negatively correlated with lytic activity of females as measured just prior to pairing. This result indicates that males detected this aspect of the female's physiology and that a strong immune response made the female less attractive, possibly because it indicated a current or recent infection. Taken together, these results verify that a common herbicide shifts mating criteria used by an agribiont spider and, separate from the herbicide, the immune status of females affects the reactions of courting males.
Chapter
Semiochemicals, such as sex pheromones or predator kairomones, trigger inter- and intraspecific interactions among animals. With more than 7500 species, frogs (Anura) are a highly diverse group with complex life cycles and adaptations to aquatic and terrestrial habitats. In this review we give a comprehensive and numerical overview about the research conducted concerning semiochemical use in different anuran live stages. Analysing more than 300 studies, we found that chemical communication and kairomone recognition are best studied in tadpoles. Tadpoles of different species are well known to react for example to damage-released alarm substances or predator kairomones. This knowledge has been used in some species to elope more complex research questions about learning mechanisms or the impact of chemical pollution. But also juvenile frogs and adults have been in the focus of semiochemical research, e.g., analysing the recognition of predator kairomones, conspecific semiochemicals, substances involved in parental care, or potential courtship pheromones. However, compared to tadpoles the research efforts in these life stages are still scarce. Furthermore, the species studied regarding semiochemical use do not remotely reflect the diversity of anuran species known today. Future studies of anuran chemical communication should provide additional insight to the role of semiochemicals across different life stages and species.
Article
Organisms face energetic challenges of climate change in combination with suites of natural and anthropogenic stressors. In particular, chemical contaminant exposure has neurotoxic, endocrine-disrupting, and behavioral effects which may additively or interactively combine with challenges associated with climate change. We used a literature review across animal taxa and contaminant classes, but focused on Arctic endotherms and contaminants important in Arctic ecosystems, to demonstrate potential for interactive effects across five bioenergetic domains: (1) energy supply, (2) energy demand, (3) energy storage, (4) energy allocation tradeoffs, and (5) energy management strategies; and involving four climate change-sensitive environmental stressors: changes in resource availability, temperature, predation risk, and parasitism. Identified examples included relatively equal numbers of synergistic and antagonistic interactions. Synergies are often suggested to be particularly problematic, since they magnify biological effects. However, we emphasize that antagonistic effects on bioenergetic traits can be equally problematic, since they can reflect dampening of beneficial responses and result in negative synergistic effects on fitness. Our review also highlights that empirical demonstrations remain limited, especially in endotherms. Elucidating the nature of climate change-by-contaminant interactive effects on bioenergetic traits will build toward determining overall outcomes for energy balance and fitness. Progressing to determine critical species, life stages, and target areas in which transformative effects arise will aid in forecasting broad-scale bioenergetic outcomes under global change scenarios.
Article
Full-text available
Proc. R. Soc. B 280, 20131502. (7 December 2013; Published online 23 October 2013) (doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.1502). In the second paragraph of the ‘Material and methods’ section, the concentration of atrazine is incorrectly listed as 65.9 ± 3.48 mg l⁻¹, mean ± s.e. The concentration should be listed as 65.9 ± 3.48 µg l⁻¹, mean ± s.e. • © 2014 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved.
Article
Full-text available
Virtually all species live within complex food webs, and many of these organisms are exposed to contaminants. However, we know little about how community processes, such as competition and predation, influence susceptibility to contaminants or how different types of contaminants shape communities. The objective of our study was to determine how realistic concentrations of the herbicide atrazine and the insecticide endosulfan influence the structure of a pond community when the presence of common community members was manipulated. We employed a factorial design in mesocosms to evaluate the effects of pesticide treatments (25 μg/L of atrazine, 10 μg/L of endosulfan, solvent control; two pulses separated by two weeks) and the presence or absence of wood frog tadpoles (Rana sylvatica), adult snails (Planorbella trivolvis), and caged dragonfly larvae (Anax junius) on a freshwater community. Tadpoles, snails, and chironomid larvae, Polypedilum sp. (Dipterans), all competed for periphyton. As a result, tadpoles reduced the survival, mass, and reproduction of snails; snails reduced the growth, development, inactivity, and dragonfly avoidance of tadpoles; snails and tadpoles reduced the abundance of chironomid larvae; and chironomid larvae reduced snail mass. The adverse effect of snails on tadpole growth and behavior was greater in the presence of the caged tadpole predator, A. junius. Neither pesticide affected dragonfly survival, but endosulfan directly reduced zooplankton (Daphnia), and atrazine indirectly reduced chironomid abundance. Atrazine also directly decreased periphyton, and endosulfan decimated chironomid larvae, resulting in indirect increases and decreases in competition for both snails and tadpoles, respectively. Consequently, relative to endosulfan, atrazine tended to decrease snail mass and reproduction and reduce tadpole mass, development, inactivity, refuge use, and dragonfly avoidance. However, the indirect effects of pesticides depended upon the presence of heterospecifics. The indirect benefit of endosulfan on snail mass was greater in the presence of caged dragonfly larvae, and endosulfan's indirect benefit on tadpole mass was greater in the absence of snails. The effect of pesticides on tadpole activity depended on both caged dragonflies and snails. Thus, environmentally realistic concentrations of pesticides directly and indirectly shaped species responses and community composition, but the initial composition of the community influenced these pesticide effects. These results emphasize the importance of quantifying the effects of contaminants within complex natural communities.
Article
Full-text available
Storm water and agricultural runoff frequently contain agrochemicals, fecal indicator bacteria (FIB), and zoonotic pathogens. Entry of such contaminants into aquatic ecosystems may affect ecology and human health. This study tested the hypothesis that the herbicide atrazine and the fungicide chlorothalonil indirectly affect the survival of FIB (Escherichia coli and Enterococcus faecalis) and a pathogen (E. coli O157:H7) by altering densities of protozoan predators or by altering competition from autochthonous bacteria. Streptomycin-resistant E. coli, En. faecalis, and E. coli O157:H7 were added to microcosms composed of Florida river water containing natural protozoan and bacterial populations. FIB, pathogen, and protozoan densities were monitored over six days. Known metabolic inhibitors, cycloheximide and streptomycin, were used to inhibit autochthonous protozoa or bacteria, respectively. The inhibitors made it possible to isolate the effects of predation or competition on survival of allochthonous bacteria, and each treatment increased the survival of FIB and pathogens. Chlorothalonil's effect was similar to that of cycloheximide, significantly reducing protozoan densities and elevating densities of FIB and pathogens relative to the control. Atrazine treatment did not affect protozoan densities, but, through an effect on competition, resulted in significantly greater densities of En. faecalis and E. coli O157:H7. Hence, by reducing predaceous protozoa and bacterial competitors that facilitate purifying water bodies of FIBs and human pathogens, chlorothalonil and atrazine indirectly diminished an ecosystem service of fresh water.
Article
Full-text available
Environmental pollutants, found in aquatic ecosystems, have been shown to have an effect on olfactory-mediated behaviors including feeding, mate attraction, and other important social behaviors. Crayfish are polytrophic, meaning that they feed on and become prey for all levels of the aquatic food web as well as are also important for the transfer of energy between benthic and terrestrial food webs. Because crayfish are a keystone species, it is important to investigate any factors that may affect their population size. Crayfish are active at night and rely heavily on their sensory appendages (e.g., antennulues, maxillipeds, and pereopods) to localize food sources. In this experiment, we investigated the effects of atrazine (ATR) exposure on the chemosensory responses of male and female crayfish to food odors. We exposed crayfish to environmentally relevant, sublethal levels of ATR [80 ppb (µg/L)] for 72 h and then examined the behavioral responses of both ATR-treated and control crayfish to food odor delivered from one end of a test arena. We used Noldus Ethovision XT software to measure odor localization and locomotory behaviors of crayfish in response to food (fish) odor. We found that control crayfish spent more time in the proximal region of the test arena and at the odor source compared with ATR-treated crayfish. Furthermore, there were no differences in the time spent moving and not moving, total distance travelled in the tank, and walking speed (cm/s) when control and ATR-treated crayfish were compared. Overall, this indicates that acute ATR exposure alters chemosensory abilities of crayfish, whereas overall motor function remains unchanged.
Article
Full-text available
Ecosystems are often exposed to mixtures of chemical contaminants, but the scientific community lacks a theoretical framework to predict the effects of mixtures on biodiversity and ecosystem properties. We conducted a freshwater mesocosm experiment to examine the effects of pairwise agrochemical mixtures [fertiliser, herbicide (atrazine), insecticide (malathion) and fungicide (chlorothalonil)] on 24 species- and seven ecosystem-level responses. As postulated, the responses of biodiversity and ecosystem properties to agrochemicals alone and in mixtures was predictable by integrating information on each functional group's (1) sensitivity to the chemicals (direct effects), (2) reproductive rates (recovery rates), (3) interaction strength with other functional groups (indirect effects) and (4) links to ecosystem properties. These results show that community ecology theory holds promise for predicting the effects of contaminant mixtures on biodiversity and ecosystem services and yields recommendations on which types of agrochemicals to apply together and separately to reduce their impacts on aquatic ecosystems.
Article
Full-text available
Exposure to stressors at formative stages in the development of wildlife and humans can have enduring effects on health. Understanding which, when and how stressors cause enduring health effects is crucial because these stressors might then be avoided or mitigated during formative stages to prevent lasting increases in disease susceptibility. Nevertheless, the impact of early-life exposure to stressors on the ability of hosts to resist and tolerate infections has yet to be thoroughly investigated. Here, we show that early-life, 6-day exposure to the herbicide atrazine (mean ± s.e.: 65.9±3.48 µg l(-1)) increased frog mortality 46 days after atrazine exposure (post-metamorphosis), but only when frogs were challenged with a chytrid fungus implicated in global amphibian declines. Previous atrazine exposure did not affect resistance of infection (fungal load). Rather, early-life exposure to atrazine altered growth and development, which resulted in exposure to chytrid at more susceptible developmental stages and sizes, and reduced tolerance of infection, elevating mortality risk at an equivalent fungal burden to frogs unexposed to atrazine. Moreover, there was no evidence of recovery from atrazine exposure. Hence, reducing early-life exposure of amphibians to atrazine could reduce lasting increases in the risk of mortality from a disease associated with worldwide amphibian declines. More generally, these findings highlight that a better understanding of how stressors cause enduring effects on disease susceptibility could facilitate disease prevention in wildlife and humans, an approach that is often more cost-effective and efficient than reactive medicine.
Article
Operational sex ratio (OSR) has been traditionally thought of as a force imposing competition for mates rather than also a cue used to regulate the intrasexual competition individuals encounter. To assess whether eastern red-spotted newts, Notophthalmus viridescens, could appropriately compare OSRs, we quantified field responses to traps containing four males, a sexually receptive female, four males plus a female, or nothing as a control. Early in the breeding season, males from two populations chose competitive mating opportunities over no mating opportunity at all, but generally preferred less competitive mating prospects. Later in the breeding season, as the OSR of newt populations becomes more male biased, males accordingly increased their acceptance of intrasexual competition. Females avoided groups of four males, and for both sexes, avoidance of male-biased courting groups increased their probability of amplexus courtship. We then isolated an approximately 33-kD protein from male cloacal glands that was used by males to compare OSRs. To our knowledge, this protein represents the first isolated and characterized component of an olfactory cue used to evaluate OSR. These results support two important principles regarding mating systems: (1) OSR can somewhat paradoxically be both the source imposing competition for mates and the source used to reduce it, and (2) analogous to the sex in short supply often being ''choosy'' selecting mates, the sex in excess (here, males) appears to be choosy about its acceptance of intrasexual competition.
Article
Since 1982, the Iowa Groundwater Monitoring (IGWM) Program has been used to sample untreated groundwater from Iowa municipal wells for selected agricultural chemicals. This long-term database was used to determine if concentrations of select agricultural chemicals in groundwater have changed with time. Nitrate, alachlor [2-chloro-2'-6'-diethyl-N-(methoxymethyl)- acetanilide], atrazine (2-chloro-4-ethylamino-6-isopropylaminos-s-triazine), cyanazine [2-[[4-chloro-6-(ethylamino)-1,3,5-triazin-2-yl]amino]-2- methylpropionitrite)], and metolachlor [2-chloro-N-(2-ethyl-6-methylphenyl)- N-(2-methoxy-1-methylethyl) acetamide] were selected for this temporal analysis of the data. Conclusive temporal changes in frequency of detection and median chemical concentrations were found only for atrazine (decrease) and metolachlor (increase). The greatest temporal chemical changes occurred in the shallowest wells and in alluvial aquifers-both relating to groups of wells generally having the youngest groundwater age. The temporal patterns found for atrazine and metolachlor are consistent with their patterns of chemical use and/or application rates and are suggestive of a causal relation. Only continued data collection, however, will indicate if the trends in chemical concentrations described here represent long-term temporal patterns or only short-term changes in groundwater. No definitive answers could be made in regards to the question of overall improvements in groundwater quality with respect to agricultural chemical contamination and time, due to the inherent problems with the simplistic measurement of overall severity (summation of alachlor + atrazine + cyanazine + metolachlor concentrations) examined for this study. To adequately determine if there is an actual decreasing trend in the overall severity of contamination (improving groundwater quality), the collection of additional water-chemistry data and the investigation of other measures of severity are needed.
Article
Herbicides are detected in rainfall; however, these are a small fraction of the total applied. This study was designed to evaluate monthly and annual variation in atrazine (6-chloro-N-ethyl-Nâ²-(1-methylethyl)-1,3,5-triazine-2,4-diamine), alachlor (2-chloro-N-(2,6-diethylphenyl)-N-(methoxymethyl)acetamide), metolachlor (2-chloro-N-(2-ethyl-6-methylphenyl)-N-(2-methoxy-1-methylethyl)acetamide), and NOâ-N concentrations in rainfall over Walnut Creek watershed south of Ames, IA. The study began in 1991 and continued through 1994. Within the watershed, two wet/dry precipitation samplers were positioned 4 km apart. Detections varied during the year with >90% of the herbicide detections occurring in April through early July. Concentrations varied among events from nondetectable amounts to concentrations of 154 μg L⁻¹, which occurred when atrazine was applied during an extremely humid day immediately followed by rainfall of 100 μg m⁻² representing