ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Acoustic signals are an important part in the behaviour of many species and may play a key role in speciation. However, little is known about the importance of natural selection on the evolution of such signals. Acoustics signals are the main communication channel for most anuran species, and background noise from streams is a constant source of masking interference for species reproducing in these environments. Herein, we test if the noise of flowing water habitats has favoured advertisement calls with higher dominant frequencies in frogs. Phylogenetic generalized least square model analysis revealed a significant influence of reproductive environment and body size on dominant frequency, with no significant interaction between habitat and body size. While stream breeders call at higher dominant frequencies, this acoustic parameter is inversely correlated with body size in both environments. We discuss the biological consequences of long-term adaptive shift in this acoustic parameter and possi- ble tradeoffs with other evolutionary forces.
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Background noise as a selective pressure:
stream-breeding anurans call at higher frequencies
David Lucas hr
1
&Gustavo Brant Paterno
1
&Felipe Camurugi
2
&Flora Acuña Jun
3
&
Adrian Antonio Garda
1,4
Received: 18 June 2015 / Accepted: 30 November 2015
#Gesellschaft für Biologische Systematik 2015
Abstract Acoustic signals are an important part in the behav-
iour of many species and may play a key role in speciation.
However, little is known about the importance of natural se-
lection on the evolution of such signals. Acoustics signals are
the main communication channel for most anuran species, and
background noise from streams is a constant source of
masking interference for species reproducing in these environ-
ments. Herein, we test if the noise of flowing water habitats
has favoured advertisement calls with higher dominant fre-
quencies in frogs. Phylogenetic generalized least square mod-
el analysis revealed a significant influence of reproductive
environment and body size on dominant frequency, with no
significant interaction between habitat and body size. While
stream breeders call at higher dominant frequencies, this
acoustic parameter is inversely correlated with body size in
both environments. We discuss the biologicalconsequences of
long-term adaptive shift in this acoustic parameter and possi-
ble trade-offs with other evolutionary forces.
Keywords Acoustic communication .Advertisement call .
Comparative methods .Evolution .Masking interference
Introduction
Acoustic signals are a fundamental part in the communication
system of many species, having evolved independently several
times in different clades (Gerhardt and Huber 2002). Stochastic
processes, pleiotropic effects, and sexual and natural selection
may drive the evolution of sound communication in animals
(Wilkins et al. 2013). The role of stochastic evolution has been
quantified contrasting molecular and acoustic variation with
the aid of recent phylogenetic hypotheses (Goicoechea et al.
2010). Morphological and physiological constrains affect
signal characteristics, and selective pressures on these may lead
to pleiotropic signal divergence (Podos 2001). Sexual selection
is the best-studied evolutionary force shaping acoustic signal
evolution. Females may show strong preference for specific
acoustic parameters, resulting in differential mating success
(Ritchie 1996) and ultimately leading to species divergence.
In contrast, much less is known about the role of natural selec-
tion on acoustic signal evolution (Wilkins et al. 2013).
Background noise is one of the main constraints on acous-
tic communication, limiting the active space of every natural
communication system (Brumm 2013). Short duration noise
is circumvented by plastic responses, whereas more predict-
able and constant noise should result in long-term adaptive
processes (Brumm 2013). While short-term plastic responses
to background noise have been fairly well studied, less is
known about long-term adaptive processes in constantly noisy
environments (Brumm and Slabbekoorn 2005).
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article
(doi:10.1007/s13127-015-0256-0) contains supplementary material,
which is available to authorized users.
*David Lucas Röhr
davidlucasr@yahoo.com.br
1
Programa de Pós-graduação em Ecologia, Universidade Federal do
Rio Grande do Norte, Lagoa Nova, 59072-970 Natal, RN, Brazil
2
Programa de s-Graduação em Ciências Biológicas (Zoologia),
Departamento de Sistemática e Ecologia, Centro de Ciências Exatas e
da Natureza, Universidade Federal da Paraíba, João
Pessoa 58059-900, PB, Brazil
3
Departamento de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Estadual de
Feira de Santana, BR 116, Km 03, Campus Universitário,
44031-460 Feira de Santana, BA, Brazil
4
Departamento de Botânica e Zoologia, Centro de Biociências,
Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Campus
Universitário, Lagoa Nova, 59078-900 Natal, RN, Brazil
Org Divers Evol
DOI 10.1007/s13127-015-0256-0
Stream noise is characterized by constancy, often high inten-
sities, and emphasized energy in low frequency bands (Goutte
et al. 2013) that potentially overlap with low frequency anuran
vocalizations (Wells 2010). Hence, higher frequencies should
be favoured in stream breeders by reducing the energy expen-
diture needed to diminish interference by increasing intensity.
Indeed, flowing water noise pressure level is one of the best
predictors of anuran community composition in the vicinity of
streams (Goutte et al. 2013) and communicating in high
frequencies near streams improves signal detection and
discrimination (Boonman and Kurniati 2011). Still, the role of
flowing water noise as a selectivepressureonanuranadvertise-
ment calls is contentious (Boeckle et al. 2009;Hoskinetal.
2009). One analysis using 110 species in five families found
that stream species use slightly higher dominant frequencies,
but this trend vanished in analyses controlling for body size and
phylogeny (Vargas-Salinas and Amézquita 2014).
Herein, we test if masking interference from low frequency
noise of streams has favoured advertisement calls with higher
dominant frequencies in frogs reproducing near these environ-
ments. To do so, we gathered information on calls of 509
species from 31 frog families in all biogeographic realms.
We test this hypothesis using phylogenetic comparative
methods controlling for adult male body sizes.
Materials and methods
We constructed a database composed of mean advertisement
call dominant frequencies (the frequency band with the
greatest amount of energy) and maximum male body sizes
(snout-vent length (SVL)) reported for each species from data
available inthe literature. We searched for species reproducing
exclusively in flowing or still waters (leaving out species that
use both habitats and species which reproduce independently
from water bodies) and included in the phylogeny proposed
by Pyron and Wiens (2011). In order to achieve a large data
set, we used practical and pre-established criteria for data in-
clusion. For multiple literature hits on the same species, we
included the most recently reported mean dominant frequency
and the overall largest male body size. However, to ensure that
these criteria do not include a bias in the analyses, we tested a
random subset of the data demonstrating that there is no sig-
nificant difference in dominant frequency between older and
recent publications and that maximum and mean SVL are
highly correlated (more than 98 %) (Supporting Information
Sects. 2.4 and 2.5). For a few species where authors reported a
dominant frequency range, we used the average between max-
imum and minimum values. In rare cases where the publica-
tion did not report values for dominant frequency but included
a spectrogram with a straight and clearly identifiable empha-
sized spectrum, we included a visual estimation of this
parameter.
We evaluated the phylogenetic signal of our data set using
Blombergs K, which varies from zero to infinity and indicates
the strength of phylogenetic signal under Brownian motion
model of evolution (Blomberg et al. 2003). Next, we used a
phylogenetic generalized least square model (PGLS), which
takes into account the nonindependence of observations due
to phylogeny and assumes a Brownian motion model of evo-
lution (Freckleton et al. 2002). We used the dominant frequen-
cy as the response variable and reproduction habitat (still/
flowing) and SVL as the explanatory variables to test if dom-
inant frequency was affected by reproduction environment.
Dominant frequencies and body sizes were log transformed
(natural logarithm) before the analysis. To optimize branch
length transformation, the lambda value was set by maximum
likelihood (Orme et al. 2012). All statistical analyses were
performed in R 3.1.2 using the packages Caper (Orme et al.
2012) and Picante (Kembel et al. 2010).
Results
We complied a dataset of 509 species representing 31 of the 54
currently recognized anuran families (see Supporting
Information Sect. 2 for phylogenetic tree and dataset; see
Appendix 1for complete table with references). Stream-
reproducing species (N= 177) have a mean dominant frequen-
cy of 3.37 ± 2.04 kHz (range 0.4215.97) and a mean SVL of
41.7 ± 20 mm (range 20138), while still water reproducing
species (N= 332) average dominant frequency and SVL were
2.18 ± 1.26 kHz (range 0.189.17) and 51.2 ± 29.3 mm (range
15245), respectively.
Dominant frequency (K= 0.37, Z variance = 4.16,
p<0.001) and SVL (K= 0.44, Z variance = 4.45, p<
0.001; Table 1) presented a significant phylogenetic signal.
PGLS analysis revealed a strong influence of reproductive
environment and body size on dominant frequency (R
2
=
0.38), with no significant interaction between habitat and
body size (Table 2;Fig.1; see Sect. 4.5 in Supporting
Information for model diagnostic). While stream breeders call
with higher frequencies than still water species, dominant fre-
quency decreases with increasing body size in both environ-
ments (β=0.874, standard error = 0.052). Model residuals
Tabl e 1 Phylogenetic signal for dominant frequency (DF) and body
size (SVL) calculated through BlombergsK
Source K PIC.mean PIC.rdn.mean pvalue
logDF 0.3660 0.00754 0.02918 0.001
logSVL 0.4387 0.00321 0.01501 0.001
Residuals 0.1002 0.00013 0.00016 0.173
From Blomberg et al. 2003
D.L. Röhr et al.
showed a nonsignificant phylogenetic signal (K= 0.055,
Z variance = 1.301, p=0.888).
Discussion
Frogs reproducing in streams use higher dominant frequen-
cies, suggesting that advertisement calls have evolved to di-
minish masking interference from flowing waters background
noise by reducing spectral overlap. This hypothesis was pre-
viously corroborated in studies on a single community
(Preininger et al. 2007) and one specific genus (Boeckle
et al. 2009) but contradicted in another analysis (Hoskin
et al. 2009). Nevertheless, all these studies used a limited
taxonomic sampling and did not account for phylogeny.
Conversely, an analysis including a wider taxonomic sam-
pling found that, although stream breeders call with higher
average dominant frequencies, this difference is not signifi-
cant when controlling for phylogeny and body size (Vargas-
Salinas and Amézquita 2014). The authors found that stream
breeders were significantly smaller and attributed this differ-
ence to habitat filtering (size selected for higher dominant
frequency), natural selection favouring small size in those
habitats (reduced pressure from evaporative loss), or both
(Vargas-Salinas and Amézquita 2014). The dataset used by
the authors was one fifth of the one used in our present anal-
ysis (110 species), included only five anuran families (and
hence a significantly smaller number of phylogenetic con-
trasts), and was restricted to New World frogs, mostly from
the Amazon region. Indeed, while few species reproduce in
flowing waters in the Amazon region, about half of the species
in southeast Asia, for example, are riparian and develop in
streams (Zimmerman and Simberloff 1996), a biogeographic
bias that is likely to influence the results. The current study
encompasses a much larger interspecific variation in body size
(230 mm vs 123 mm in their study) and a larger overlap in
body size between the two categories of reproductive environ-
ment (118 mm vs 47 mm). Therefore, the families and species
chosen in Vargas-Salinas and Amézquita (2014)surveydonot
adequately represent size differences among reproductive
habitats for all anurans, making it difficult to disentangle the
effects of body size and environment.
Although environmental influence on dominant frequency
is highly significant in our analysis, its effect is small when
compared to body size (Table 2). This is expected considering
the inverse relationship between vocal apparatus mass and call
frequency, which makes the variation in this acoustic param-
eter limited by morphological constraints. Indeed, our com-
plete PGLS model accounts for about 40 % of the variation in
this parameter, and other selective forces might be important
(see below). Moreover, the importance of environmental
noise as a selective pressure can vary among different anuran
clades, and future studies should focus on more restrictive
groups (such as a single family) with better representation
of its species and accurate measures of sound pressure levels.
Even though the environment is not the main driver of dom-
inant frequency variation, there is a mean difference of nearly
1200 Hz between environments. Thus, considering the impor-
tance of this parameter for anuran reproduction (Gerhardt
and Huber 2002), its biological relevance should not be
overlooked.
Tabl e 2 ANOVA table for the phylogenetic generalized least square
model {log(DF) habitat × log(SLV)} evaluating the effects of body size
(SVL) and habitat on advertisement call dominant frequencies
Source df SQ MSQ Fpvalue
Habitat 1 0.0764 0.0764 33.0 <0.001
SVL 1 0.6461 0.6461 279.4 <0.001
Habitat × SVL 1 0.0008 0.0008 0.3 0.5674
Residuals 505 1.1675 0.0023 ––
2.5 2.7 2.9 3.1 3.3 3.5 3.7 3.9 4.1 4.3 4.5 4.7 4.9 5.1 5.3 5.5
5.0
5.5
6.0
6.5
7.0
7.5
8.0
8.5
9.0
9.5
10.0
Lo
g
snout−vent len
g
th (lnSVL)
Log dominant frequency (lnDF)
Flowing water (C = 11.05)
Still water (C = 10.86)
Y = −0.87x + C
Fig. 1 Relationship between
dominant frequency and body
size (SVL) for species calling in
still and flowing waters (n=509).
Lines represent PGLS regressions
Stream background noise and anuran communication
In large species, even an evolutionary increase in dominant
frequency, within physical constraints on sound production
mechanisms, may still not overcome the emphasized spectrum
from background stream noise to obtain release from masking
interference. Furthermore, because advertisement calls are
crucial for anuran reproductive behaviour, other evolutionary
forces besides pleiotropic effects of body size and noise inter-
ference might be involved in the establishment of dominant
frequency differences. For instance, some species/clades may
evolve different strategies to cope with such interference, such
as visual communication (Starnberger et al. 2014;Hödland
Amézquita 2001).
Several complex trade-offs among selective pressures may
be involved in shaping anuran vocalization. Call attractiveness
to females and detectability may be selected by opposing
forces in streams. Females may show increased phonotaxis
for low or median values of dominant frequency, leading to
directional or stabilizing selection (Gerhardt (1991), but see
Gerhardt and Schwartz (2001) for further discussion on fe-
male preference for dominant frequency). During aggressive
acoustic encounters, dominant frequency might be determi-
nant for the outcome (Davies and Halliday 1978), and territo-
rial males may lower call frequency in the presence of in-
truders (Wagner 1989; Bee and Bowling 2002). Hence, males
near streams may face a trade-off between the need to increase
call frequency to enhance signal detection at the expense of
reducing attractiveness and overall recognition.
Additionally, a trade-off between sound propagation and
detectability in forested stream environments is also expected.
Low-frequency calls are more efficient in habitats with many
physical barriers compared to higher frequencies (Ey and
Fischer 2009). Thus, species reproducing in forest streams
should face opposite selective pressures, where low dominant
frequencies suffer less attenuation and degradation, but high
dominant frequencies experience less masking interference.
Furthermore, community composition may promote addition-
al limits and selective pressures by driving the evolution of
anuran advertisement call dominant frequencies in two dis-
tinct manners. First, the presence of sympatric phylogenetical-
ly related taxa with similar vocalizations may lead to sexual
character displacement to decrease hybridization probability
(Lemmon 2009). Second, in highly diverse acoustic habitats,
calls may evolve to fill different acoustic niches and spectral
silent windows should be favoured (Chek et al. 2003). In
either case, dominant frequency changes favoured by these
scenarios could reinforce or counterbalance the selective
forces of flowing water masking interference.
Although background noise is probably the main differ-
ence in the acoustic scenario between still and flowing water
habitats, these environments also vary in a myriad of other
factors that might affect its acoustic community and should
be considered in future studies, such as (1) community of
sound-guided predators (Ryan and Tuttle 1983), (2)
vegetation coverage which might act as propagation barriers
(Ey and Fischer 2009), (3) sympatric species with prominent
acoustic signals (Chek et al. 2003), and (4) tadpole develop-
ment environment leading to differences in adult body size
and steroid hormones (Wells 2010).
Considering all the different evolutionary processes acting
upon anuran advertisement calls, the importance of stream back-
ground noise as a selective pressure is expected to vary between
clades, especially considering that stream colonization took
place many times independently and the evolutionary time un-
der this condition varies. For example, when only the three most
representative families with species from both habitats were
tested separately, the habitat effect was not significant for one
family, while the effect of size was very different between the
two others (Supporting Information Sect. 4.7.2). Therefore, our
study reveals a general pattern for anurans (broad phylogenetic
scale), while selection by background noise might vary between
clades with contrasting evolutionary histories.
Finally, even with this complex evolutionary scenario, we
found a significant trend for anuran species calling near
streams to use higher dominant frequencies. Other advertise-
ment call characteristics may respond similarly. For example,
sound intensity and call rate are expected to be higher in frogs
reproducing in stream habitats and using calls with dominant
frequencies similar to surrounding noise. Patterns for other
variables, such as call duration and complexity, are less clear.
Testing predictions for these variables, however, is much
harder because of the lack of appropriate descriptions in the
literature. Background noise from streams is clearly determi-
nant for the evolution of anuran advertisement calls, and fu-
ture work should explore the generality of these results for
other groups of animals.
Acknowledgments We thank Carlos Roberto Fonseca, Alex Pyron,
Pablo Martinez, Marcelo Gehara, and Frank Burbrink for suggestions
on the manuscript and fruitful discussions. AAG and FAJ thank National
Counsel of Technological and Scientific Development - CNPq for finan-
cial support (Universal # 473503/2012-3 and #305704/2013-3, respec-
tively).
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
interest.
Ethical approval This article does not contain any studies with human
participants or animals performed by any of the authors.
References
Bee, M. A., & Bowling, A. C. (2002). Socially mediated pitch alteration
by territorial male Bullfrogs, Rana catesbeiana. Journal of
Herpetology, 36(1), 140143.
Blomberg,S.P.,Garland,T.,&Ives,A.R.(2003).Testingfor
phylogenetic signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are
D.L. Röhr et al.
more labile. Evolution, 57(4), 717745. doi:10.1111/j.0014-
3820.2003.tb00285.x.
Boeckle, M., Preininger, D., & Hödl, W. (2009). Communication in noisy
environments I: acoustic signals of Staurois latopalmatus Boulenger
1887. Herpetologica, 65(2), 154165. doi:10.1655/07-071r1.1.
Boonman, A., & Kurniati, H. (2011). Evolution of high-frequency com-
munication in frogs. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 13(2), 197
207.
Brumm, H. (2013). Animal Communication and Noise (Vol. 2).
Heidelberg: Springer.
Brumm, H., & Slabbekoorn, H. (2005). Acoustic communication in
noise. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 35,151209.
Chek, A. A., Bogart, J. P., & Lougheed, S. C. (2003). Mating signal
partitioning in multi-species assemblages: a null model test using
frogs. Ecology Letters, 6(3), 235247. doi:10.1046/j.1461-0248.
2003.00420.x.
Davies, N. B., & Halliday, T. R. (1978). Deep croaks and fighting assess-
ment in toads Bufo bufo. Nature, 274(5672), 683685. doi:10.1038/
274683a0.
Ey, E., & Fischer, J. (2009). The Bacoustic adaptation hypothesis^-a
review of the evidence from birds, anurans and mammals.
Bioacoustics, 19(1-2), 2148.
Freckleton, R. P., Harvey, P. H., & Pagel, M. (2002). Phylogenetic anal-
ysis and comparative data: a test and review of evidence. American
Naturalist, 160(6), 712726. doi:10.1086/343873.
Gerhardt, H. C. (1991). Female mate choise in treefrogs: static and dy-
namic acoustic criteria. Animal Behaviour, 42,615635.
Gerhardt, H. C., & Huber, F. (2002). Acoustic communication in insects
and anurans: common problems and diverse solutions.Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Gerhardt, H. C., & Schwartz, J. J. (2001). Auditory tuning and frequency
preferences in anurans. In M. J. Ryan (Ed.), Anuran communication
(pp. 7385). Washington: Smithsonian Institution Press.
Goicoechea, N., De La Riva, I., & Padial, J. M. (2010). Recovering
phylogenetic signal from frog mating calls. Zoologica Scripta,
39(2), 141154.
Goutte, S.,Dubois, A., & Legendre, F. (2013). The importance of ambient
sound level to characterise anuran habitat. Plos One, 8(10), e78020.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078020.
Hödl, W., & Amézquita, A. (2001). Visual signaling in anuran amphib-
ians. In M. J. Ryan (Ed.), Anuran Communication (pp. 121141).
Washington: Smithsonian Institution Press.
Hoskin, C. J., James, S., & Grigg, G. C. (2009). Ecology and taxonomy-
driven deviations in the frog call-body size relationship across the
diverse Australian frog fauna. Journal of Zoology, 278(1), 3641.
doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.2009.00550.x.
Kembel, S. W., Cowan, P. D., Helmus, M. R., Cornwell, W. K., Morlon,
H., Ackerly, D. D., et al. (2010). Picante: R tools for integrating
phylogenies and ecology. Bioinformatics, 26(11), 14631464. doi:
10.1093/bioinformatics/btq166.
Lemmon, E. M. (2009). Diversification of conspecific signals in sympat-
ry: geographic overlap drives multidimensional reproductive char-
acter displacement in frogs. Evolution, 63(5), 11551170.
Orme, C. D. L., Freckleton, R. P., Thomas, G. H., Petzoldt, T., Fritz, S. A.,
Isaac, N., et al. (2012). Caper: comparative analyses of phyloge-
netics and evolution in R. R package version 0.5.
Podos, J. (2001). Correlated evolution of morphologyand vocal signal
structure in Darwins Fnches. Nature, 409,185188.
Preininger, D., Boeckle, M., & Hödl, W. (2007). Comparison of anuran
acoustic communities of two habitat types in the Danum Valley
Conservation Area, Sabah, Malaysia. Salamandra, 43(3), 129138.
Pyron, R. A., & Wiens, J. J. (2011). A large-scale phylogeny of Amphibia
including over 2800 species, and a revised classification of extant
frogs, salamanders, and caecilians. Molecular Phylogenetics and
Evolution, 61(2), 543583. doi:10.1016/j.ympev.2011.06.012.
Ritchie, M. G. (1996). The shape of female mating preferences.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93, 14628
14631.
Ryan, M. J., & Tuttle, M. D. (1983). The ability of the frog-eating bat to
discriminate among novel and potentially poisonous frog species
using acoustic cues. Animal Behaviour, 31(3), 827833.
Starnberger, I., Preininger, D., & Hödl, W. (2014). From uni- to
multimodality: towards an integrative view on anuran communica-
tion. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 200,777787.
Vargas-Salinas, F., & Amézquita, A. (2014). Abiotic noise, call frequency
and stream-breeding anuran assemblages. Evolutionary Ecology,
28(2), 341359. doi:10.1007/s10682-013-9675-6.
Wagner, W. E., Jr. (1989). Fighting, assessment, and frequency alteration
in Blanchards cricket frog. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology,
25(6), 429436.
Wells, K. D. (2010). The ecology and behavior of amphibians. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Wilkins, M. R., Seddon, N., & Safran, R. J. (2013). Evolutionary diver-
gence in acoustic signals: causes and consequences. Trends in
Ecology & Evolution, 28(3), 156166. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2012.10.
002.
Zimmerman, B. L., & Simberloff, D. (1996). An historical interpretation
of habitat use by frogs in a Central Amazonian Forest. Journal of
Biogeography, 23,2746.
Stream background noise and anuran communication
... In the long term, some species might also be able to adapt or even evolve in response to noise exposure [11]. Several large-scale studies showed a strong correlation between dominant frequency (DF) and background noise, while temporal call features were not found to be shaped by the habitat conditions where the signaller was vocalizing [10,17,18]. Frequency modulation (FM) and harmonic traits might also be affected by noisy environments [10,12]. ...
... For body size, we used the male snout-vent length (SVL). In some cases, SVL reports are ranges, and we uniformly chose the maximum SVL, as this measurement correlates highly (i.e., 98% in anurans) with the mean SVL values [18]. For habitat characteristics, we recorded breeding sites for each taxon based on their exclusive use of standing (lentic) water or running (lotic) water (i.e., excluded taxa that utilize both). ...
... An interaction term 'Environment x SVL' was included to determine if calling environment and SVL influence call parameters. Prior to the analysis, DF and SVL data were ln transformed as in previous studies [17,18]. Moreover, we set the λ values by maximum likelihood to optimize branch-length transformations in the model. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The acoustic adaptation hypothesis (AAH) states that signals should evolve towards an optimal transmission of the intended information from senders to intended receivers given the environmental constraints of the medium that they traverse. To date, most AAH studies have focused on the effect of stratified vegetation on signal propagation. These studies, based on the AAH, predict that acoustic signals should experience less attenuation and degradation where habitats are less acoustically complex. Here, we explored this effect by including an environmental noise dimension to test some AAH predictions in two clades of widespread amphibians (Bufonidae and Ranidae) that actively use acoustic signals for communication. By using data from 106 species in these clades, we focused on the characterization of the differences in dominant frequency (DF) and frequency contour (i.e., frequency modulation [FM] and harmonic performances) of mating calls and compared them between species that inhabit flowing-water or still-water environments. Results After including temperature, body size, habitat type and phylogenetic relationships, we found that DF differences among species were explained mostly by body size and habitat structure. We also showed that species living in lentic habitats tend to have advertisement calls characterized by well-defined FM and harmonics. Likewise, our results suggest that flowing-water habitats can constrain the evolutionary trajectories of the frequency-contour traits of advertisement calls in these anurans. Conclusions Our results may support AAH predictions in frogs that vocalize in noisy habitats because flowing-water environments often produce persistent ambient noise. For instance, these anurans tend to generate vocalizations with less well-defined FM and harmonic traits. These findings may help us understand how noise in the environment can influence natural selection as it shapes acoustic signals in affected species.
... When accounting for phylogenetic relationships, our results showed that DF was negatively related to SVL. A similar observation was made by Röhr et al. (2016); they found that the call frequency of anurans is significantly dependent on body size, and that DF decreases when SVL increases. ...
... The acoustic characteristics of anurans are significantly influenced by many environmental factors, especially background noise, which is the most important difference between anurans that breed in stagnant water and those that breed in flowing water (Bee & Swanson, 2007;Vargas-Salinas & Amézquita, 2014;Röhr et al., 2016;Köhler et al., 2017). Most of the noise produced by flowing water is concentrated at low frequencies (Brumm & Slabbekoorn, 2005;Brumm & Slater, 2006), high-frequency noises could increase the signal-to-noise ratio and reduce the masking of acoustic signals of anurans that breed in flowing water (Dubois & Martens, 1984;Narins et al., 2004). ...
... However, Preininger et al. (2007) and Boeckle et al. (2009) found that anurans that breed in flowing water have a higher DF. We eliminated the impact of SVL and phylogenetic relationships, and the results showed that the DF of the 53 anuran species in China was significantly higher in flowing water than in stagnant water, which is similar to the findings of Röhr et al. (2016). Thus, we conclude that phylogenetic effects, SVL, and the water type of breeding habitats have a combined effect on advertisement calls in anurans. ...
Article
Full-text available
Vocalization plays an important role in the communication of anurans. In this study, the advertisement calls of male Fejervarya multistriata obtained in Lishui, Zhejiang, China during the breeding season were recorded. Their note number (NN), note duration (ND), note interval (NI), call duration (CD), pitch (PIT), call intensity (CIT) and dominant frequency (DF) were analyzed. The calls of F. multistriata are composed of one to 38 notes, and calls composed of fewer than ten notes have the highest frequency. Male frogs produced calls ranging from 1201 Hz to 3357 Hz with two DFs (1412.49 Hz and 2953.89 Hz). By comparing the differences among individual calls, it was found that the within-individual coefficients of variation (CV W ) and among-individual coefficients of variation (CV A ) for NN, NI, CD, PIT and DF were more than 10%, whereas that of CIT was less than 5%. The CV A /CV W ratios indicate that ND is important for sexual selection, whereas NN, NI CD, PIT, CIT, and DF are important for individual recognition. Phylogenetic generalized least squares analysis showed that phylogenetic signals affect DF vs. snout-vent length (SVL) and CD of anurans in China, and accounting for phylogenetic signals, DF was negatively correlated with SVL. DF was found to be higher in anurans that breed in flowing water than in those that breed in stagnant water, after eliminating the effects of phylogeny and SVL. Therefore, we conclude that phylogenetic effects, SVL, and the water type of breeding habitats have a combined impact on the advertisement calls in anurans.
... Reported effects of noise include behavioral changes, physiological stress, and the masking of communication signals (Brumm and Slabbekoorn 2005;Barber et al. 2010;Tennessen and Langkilde 2016;Simmons and Narins 2018). In most anuran species, acoustic communication plays a critical role in reproductive success, and adverse effects of auditory masking produced by natural abiotic and biotic sources of noise are known in this group (e.g., Schwartz et al. 2001;Sun and Narins 2005;Bee and Swanson 2007;Cunnington and Fahrig 2010;Vargas-Salinas et al. 2014;Röhr et al. 2016). In noisy conditions, senders must adapt their acoustic signals to improve the transfer of information to receivers, which often reduces its effectiveness (Warren et al. 2006). ...
... However, the variations appear to be slight (<125 Hz) or non-existent in species with call frequencies greater than approximately 1.5 kHz (e.g., Lengagne 2008;Parris et (Figure 2) may not directly mask them. It is known that individuals living in chronically noisy environments may exhibit calls with higher dominant frequencies than expected to avoid auditory masking (Röhr et al. 2016;Goutte et al. 2018). However, increase the call frequency in response to anthropogenic noise is not a common response in anuran species, which show variable behavior regarding this property (Roca et al. 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
The increasing expansion of urban areas leads to the emergence of new noisy environments that can affect animal communication. Calls play a crucial role in the mating displays of anurans, and the negative impact of anthropogenic noise-induced auditory masking has been reported in several species. We investigated the acoustic variation in 96 males (n = 971 calls) of the treefrog Boana pulchella across acoustically undisturbed sites (AUS) and different noise conditions, associated with urban areas (URBAN) and roads (ROAD), in Central Argentina. We analyzed the effect of anthropogenic noise conditions on six temporal (call duration, intercall interval, first and second note duration, internote interval, call rate) and three spectral (dominant frequency of first and second note, frequency difference between them) call properties. The effects of temperature and size on acoustical variables were controlled. We observed differences in all call attributes among the noise conditions, except for intercall interval. Males exposed to URBAN and ROAD exhibited significant changes in call duration, dominant frequency of the second note, and frequency difference between them. URBAN males had longest internote interval, while ROAD individuals displayed increased first and second note duration and call rates. Interestingly, ROAD males exhibited immediate changes in call rate in direct response to passing heavy vehicles. Our study emphasizes the impact of anthropogenic noise on the acoustic characteristics of B. pulchella calls. Understanding how animals adapt to noisy environments is crucial in mitigating the adverse effects of urbanization on their communication systems. Future investigations should explore whether the observed call adjustments are effective in avoiding or mitigating the negative consequences of anthropogenic noise on reproductive success.
... Noise is an inescapable component of animal environments, produced by biotic sources, geophysical factors, and anthropogenic sources (Pijanowski et al. 2011), and its prevalence has selected for traits that allow animals to maintain effective communication and cue recognition in noisy conditions (Wiley and Richards 1978;Endler 1992;Wiley 2006Wiley , 2015. Natural forms of acoustic noise have been shown to shape signal frequency (Kirschel et al. 2009;Röhr et al. 2016), auditory physiology (Schmidt et al. 2011), and other aspects of communication behavior (Schwartz and Wells 1983;Greenfield 1988). The ecological effects of noise are especially important in the context of a changing Anthropocene, as human activity patterns have significantly increased ambient noise levels and introduced novel forms of noise across the landscape (Barber et al. 2010;Shannon et al. 2016). ...
Chapter
Here we provide an overview of work related to anthropogenically produced substrate-borne vibrational noise. We review the marine and terrestrial vibrational noise literature base, focusing upon the species studied, the increasing research attention, and the findings of latest papers. We highlight the key sources of vibrational noise, how noise may be measured and mitigated (both by humans and by animal receivers), and how we can test for the potential impacts of noise sources. We present two case studies of previously untested species, the first relating to vibrational sensitivity of barnacles, and the second relating to activity patterns of the Madagascar hissing cockroach under vibrational noise. Currently it is difficult to draw firm conclusions on the effects of vibrational noise, given the few studies in both environments. However, effects seen to date include interference with signaling, pair formation and parental care, in addition to activity changes, and an increase of stress-related behaviors. Notably the aquatic research base lags behind the terrestrial, with the vibrational sensing capabilities of most benthic organisms largely unknown currently. We highlight vibrational noise as an area that requires more research attention both on the land and in the sea.
... In insects, a study found that Grasshoppers raised under noisy conditions produced songs with higher maximum-frequency as adults, suggesting plasticity in this spectral property of sound (Lampe et al., 2014). In anurans, the frequency of call is conditioned by morphological constraints, such as body size, and is considered a parameter conserved between generations, nevertheless, individuals that live in environments with constant abiotic noise call at higher dominant frequencies than expected, suggesting evolutionary changes (Goutte et al., 2016;Röhr et al., 2016). In birds, evolutionary changes are also reported, proposing that biotic noise has shaped certain bird sound (Dubois and Martens, 1984;Brumm and Slabbekoorn, 2005), but changes are also suggested due to vocal plasticity (Gross et al., 2010;Bermúdez-Cuamatzin et al., 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Acoustic communication is a way of information exchange between individuals, and it is used by several animal species. Therefore, the detection, recognition and correct understanding of acoustic signals are key factors in effective communication. The priority of acoustic communication is effectiveness rather than perfection, being effective avoids affecting the sound-based communication system of the species. One of the factors that can affect effective communication is the overlap in time and frequency during signal transmission, known as signal masking. One type of sound that can cause masking is anthropogenic noise, which is currently increasing due to urban growth and consequently motorized transportation and machinery. When exposed to anthropogenic noise, animals can use compensatory mechanisms to deal with sound masking, such as the modification of acoustic parameters of their acoustic signal. Here, we performed a meta-analysis investigating whether different taxa have a general tendency for changes in acoustic parameters due to anthropogenic noise, we used taxa and acoustic parameters available in the literature that met the minimum criteria to perform a meta-analysis. We hypothesized that animals exposed to anthropogenic noise use compensation mechanisms, such as changes in dominant, maximum or minimum frequencies, call duration, note duration and call rate to deal with masking. We performed a meta-analysis, which synthesized information from 73 studies comprising 82 species of three taxa: insects, anurans and birds. Our results showed that in the presence of anthropogenic noise, insects did not change the acoustic parameters, while anurans increased call amplitude and birds increased dominant frequency, minimum and maximum frequencies, note duration and amplitude of their songs. The different responses of the groups to anthropogenic noise may be related to their particularities in the production and reception of sound or to the differences in the acoustic parameters considered between the taxa and also the lack of studies in some taxa.
... A similar power spectrum is expected for anthropogenic noise in cities and alongside roads (Slabbekoorn et al., 2018). Bell, 2020), but diverse ecological factors are capable of promoting the present spatial segregation of the body size of species (Nevo, 1973;Duellman & Thomas, 1996;Morrison & Hero, 2003;Wells, 2007;Campos et al., 2017), and one of these factors is the level of abiotic noise in the habitat (Goosem et al., 2007;Röhr et al., 2016). In fact, anuran assemblages alongside streams tend to exhibit lower average values and less variability in male body size than anuran assemblages located away from streams (Preininger et al., 2007;Boeckle et al., 2009;Carvajal-Castro & Vargas-Salinas, 2016). ...
Article
The abiotic noise of streams can mask the acoustic signals of anurans with a large body size calling at low frequencies, but not the signals emitted by anurans with a small body size calling at high frequencies. As a consequence, the body size of species in assemblages alongside streams is, on average, lower and less variable than that of assemblages away from streams. Given that the body size in anurans is frequently related to life-history traits, it is expected that functional diversity (FD) will be lower in anuran assemblages alongside streams than in assemblages away from streams. We calculated and compared FD, based on six functional traits, for anuran species in seven localities in different biogeographical regions in the Neotropics. In five lowland localities, FD was lower in assemblages alongside streams than in assemblages away from streams. However, the reverse trend was found in two Andean localities. Noise from streams, acting as an environmental filter, could promote low FD because taxa whose phenotype differs from an optimal type (high call frequency, small body size and associated traits) are excluded from riparian places. However, such habitat filtering could be stronger and affect more anurans in lowland assemblages than in those at medium elevation.
... Adjusting temporal and spectral characteristics of acoustic signals to reduce masking by noise have been reported in several vertebrates (see Warren et al., 2006; Barber et al., 2010). In most anuran species, the acoustic com- munication plays a critical role in reproduction and adverse effects of auditory masking produced by natural abiotic and biotic sources of noise are known in this group (e.g., Gerhardt and Klump, 1988;Schwartz et al., 2001;Bee and Swanson, 2007;Röhr et al., 2016). Particularly, conspecific choruses produce limiting communication conditions because near acoustic sources simultaneously generate competing calls with a high degree of temporal and spectral overlap and increased noise levels interfering with reproductive behavior ( Schwartz et al., 2001;Wollerman and Wiley, 2002; Richardson and Lengagne, 2010). ...
Article
Successful reproduction in most anurans is associated to acoustic communication and negative effects of auditory masking by natural biotic and anthropogenic noise are known in this group. However, the potential additive effect of both noise types has been scarcely studied. We examine in situ the acoustic variation of 135 males (n=975 calls) of Odontophrynus americanus in areas from Central Argentina with (TN) and without traffic noise (N-TN) and in presence (Ch) or absence (N-Ch) of chorus. The effect of noise condition on four call properties (call duration [CD], intercall interval [ICI], pulse rate [PR] and dominant frequency [DF]) were analyzed using linear mixed models (LMM and GLMM). A Principal Component Analysis (PCA) was performed to identify the acoustical properties that best accounted for variation among all possible noise conditions (N-TN/N-Ch; N-TN/Ch; TN/N-Ch and TN/Ch). PR and DF showed significant higher values in TN than N-TN sites while a significant increase in ICI and a decrease in DF were found in chorus situation. Analyzing combined effects, PR significantly increased in N-TN/N-Ch condition and decreased under TN/Ch. ICI was significantly higher in N-TN/N-Ch condition. PCA showed a separation of males calling in noisy condition mainly associated to higher values of DF and CD. The acoustic variation observed in noisy environments could have implications in the reproductive strategies of the individuals although new studies should be conducted to analyze the effect of this variation on the recognition and choice by females. Our outcomes highlight the importance of conducting in situ behavioral studies considering the additive effects of different environmental noise sources.
Article
Full-text available
Human activities worldwide are increasingly releasing low-frequency noise into the environment. Anthropogenic noise imposes a novel stress for wild animals and has become an increasing global concern. Many animals have been found to mitigate noise disturbance by modifying their acoustic parameters, yet the calling behaviours of species that breed near airports have not been explored in detail, especially for amphibians. Here, we measured the environmental noise of a swamp close to the runway at Haikou Meilan Airport in China. We also recorded the calls of four anuran species that reproduce in this swamp, and we measured the sound pressure levels of each focal species. These frogs vary in their call amplitude and possess disparate spectral and temporal traits. We show that two frog species characterized by lower frequency calls increase their call frequency when an airplane takes off. Moreover, three anuran species characterized by higher call rates significantly reduce their call rates (or call efforts) in response to airplane flyby. These results suggest that animals’ behavioural adaptation to human-made noise may be species specific. Because call frequency and call effort have close relationships with reproductive success, we deduce that such noise-induced behaviour adjustments may influence the fitness of taxa that depend on communication by sound.
Article
Mate choice is a natural selection mechanism whereby the traits of select members of a population are passed on to their offspring. We hypothesized that, for stream‐dwelling anurans, females might prefer males with smaller body size and higher call frequency as their vocal calls are more conspicuous in the species’ habitat featuring intense but predominantly low‐frequency stream noise. To test this hypothesis, we first measured the body size of amplectant male concave‐eared torrent frogs ( Odorrana tormota ) in the field, a species with demonstrated ability of ultrasonic communication. Our field observation showed that, on average, amplectant males had smaller body size compared to that of non‐amplectant males ( P < 0.05), an unusual finding among anurans. We next performed two‐choice amplexus experiments by pairing a female with two males in an aquarium indoors. Results of laboratory experiments similarly revealed female’s preference for males with smaller body sizes. Additionally, we recorded and analyzed the advertisement calls of 33 males and confirmed that their call frequency was inversely related to the body size as expected. Together, our studies showed that, for O. tormota , smaller males with higher call frequencies had higher mating success over bigger rivals, that is, females prefer smaller males.
Article
Full-text available
Several animal groups depend on acoustic communication to survive and reproduce, bringing attention to the possible negative effects of increasing anthropogenic noise. We examined the role of acoustical communication on the small-scale distribution of Scinax nebulosus, and how this distribution is affected by acoustic noise. We performed playback experiments to estimate theoretical male-male distances and compared them to measured male-male distances. If sound does, in fact, dictate chorus spatial structure, our models should successfully predict frog’s small-scale distribution in the field. By definition, communication should prompt a behavioral response, and playback experiments were designed to determine the intensity of a courtship call necessary to prompt the emission of territorial calls by males (call shift threshold – CST). If noise alters CST, our models should be able to predict its effect on small-scale distribution. Our results showed a positive correlation between experimental and theoretical distances, indicating that our theoretical model is capable of predicting male-male distances. Noise levels altered CST, suggesting that small-scale frog distribution is in fact affected by noise. Our results support the view that anuran vocalizations have a central role in their spatial structuring, and also that noise can interfere with small-scale distribution, causing aggregations to become denser.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Two species of frog are currently known to produce high-frequency or even partly ultrasonic vocalizations, Odorrana tormota and Huia cavitympanum. Both possess special adaptations to their hearing system that extend their hearing into the high-frequency range. Typical stream-dwelling species of frog are not known to use high frequencies. Goal: To investigate the environmental conditions that may have led to the evolution of very high-frequency communication in some frog species. Method: Study auditory communication in Huia masonii, a frog of habitats next to fast-flowing streams on the island of Java, Indonesia. Study the tympanums of all members of the genus Huia. Measure the acoustic characteristics of fast-flowing water. Calculate water noise levels (amplitudes) at different distances from streamside. Results: All four members of the genus Huia possess a modified tympanal membrane, suggesting that high-frequency communication is present in all members. All frogs using high-frequency communication live along turbulent perennial streams. They all appear to be solitary callers and experience low encounter rates between partners. The high frequencies used by Huia improve signal detection and discrimination but only within 103 m of a noisy river; they are unsuited for long-range detection. Beyond 103 m of noisy rivers, communication using normal (low) frequencies is more advantageous.
Article
Full-text available
Environmental noise can be an important selective force modulating signal evolution in species with acoustic communication. Many anuran species breed alongside streams; hence, the sound produced by the flowing water is an important source of noise for acoustic communication. Since calling is physiologically very expensive in anurans, and communication is essential for reproduction, we expected adaptations that reduce envi-ronmental masking effects and allow acoustic communication in streamside breeders. This basic assumption of the acoustic adaptation hypothesis has not been yet evaluated at a large phylogenetic scale. We combined ahistorical and phylogenetic methods to test whether anuran species that breed alongside streams call at higher frequencies than species that breed away from streams. We compiled primary and secondary data on body size, breeding habitat, and the dominant frequency of the advertisement call for 110 species; 40 of them breed alongside streams and 70 away from streams. Call frequency was slightly higher and body size was significantly smaller in streamside breeding species. After controlling for the effects of body size and phylogenetic signal, only differences in body size persisted between species breeding at both kinds of habitats. Our data suggest that habitat filtering rather than acoustic adaptation explains the high call frequency of stream breeders. Species with large body size, pleiotropically constrained to utter low-frequency calls, would have succeeded less often in establishing viable populations alongside streams, due to the masking effect of low-frequency noise. Thus, small species calling at relatively high frequencies would be more common there. Although our data do not preclude adaptations
Article
Full-text available
Undeniably, acoustic signals are the predominant mode of communication in frogs and toads. Acoustically active species are found throughout the vast diversity of anuran families. However, additional or alternative signal modalities have gained increasing attention. In several anurans, seismic, visual and chemical communications have convergently evolved due to ecological constraints such as noisy environments. The production of a visual cue, like the inevitably moving vocal sac of acoustically advertising males, is emphasized by conspicuously coloured throats. Limb movements accompanied by dynamic displays of bright colours are additional examples of striking visual signals independent of vocalizations. In some multimodal anuran communication systems, the acoustic component acts as an alert signal, which alters the receiver attention to the following visual display. Recent findings of colourful glands on vocal sacs, producing volatile species-specific scent bouquets suggest the possibility of integration of acoustic, visual and chemical cues in species recognition and mate choice. The combination of signal components facilitates a broadened display repertoire in challenging environmental conditions. Thus, the complexity of the communication systems of frogs and toads may have been underestimated.
Article
Full-text available
Habitat characterisation is a pivotal step of any animal ecology study. The choice of variables used to describe habitats is crucial and need to be relevant to the ecology and behaviour of the species, in order to reflect biologically meaningful distribution patterns. In many species, acoustic communication is critical to individuals' interactions, and it is expected that ambient acoustic conditions impact their local distribution. Yet, classic animal ecology rarely integrates an acoustic dimension in habitat descriptions. Here we show that ambient sound pressure level (SPL) is a strong predictor of calling site selection in acoustically active frog species. In comparison to six other habitat-related variables (i.e. air and water temperature, depth, width and slope of the stream, substrate), SPL had the most important explanatory power in microhabitat selection for the 34 sampled species. Ambient noise was particularly useful in differentiating two stream-associated guilds: torrents and calmer streams dwelling species. Guild definitions were strongly supported by SPL, whereas slope, which is commonly used in stream-associated habitat, had a weak explanatory power. Moreover, slope measures are non-standardized across studies and are difficult to assess at small scale. We argue that including an acoustic descriptor will improve habitat-species analyses for many acoustically active taxa. SPL integrates habitat topology and temporal information (such as weather and hour of the day, for example) and is a simple and precise measure. We suggest that habitat description in animal ecology should include an acoustic measure such as noise level because it may explain previously misunderstood distribution patterns.
Book
The study of animal communication has led to significant progress in our general understanding of motor and sensory systems, evolution, and speciation. However, one often neglected aspect is that signal exchange in every modality is constrained by noise, be it in the transmission channel or in the nervous system. This book analyses whether and how animals can cope with such constraints, and explores the implications that noise has for our understanding of animal communication. It is written by leading biologists working on different taxa including insects, fish, amphibians, lizards, birds, and mammals. In addition to this broad taxonomic approach, the chapters also cover a wide array of research disciplines: from the mechanisms of signal production and perception, to the behavioural ecology of signalling, the evolution of animal communication, and conservation issues. This volume promotes the integration of the knowledge gained by the diverse approaches to the study of animal communication and, at the same time, highlights particularly interesting fields of current and future research.
Data
Environmental noise can be an important selective force modulating signal evolution in species with acoustic communication. Many anuran species breed alongside streams; hence, the sound produced by the flowing water is an important source of noise for acoustic communication. Since calling is physiologically very expensive in anurans, and communication is essential for reproduction, we expected adaptations that reduce environmental masking effects and allow acoustic communication in streamside breeders. This basic assumption of the acoustic adaptation hypothesis has not been yet evaluated at a large phylogenetic scale. We combined ahistorical and phylogenetic methods to test whether anuran species that breed alongside streams call at higher frequencies than species that breed away from streams. We compiled primary and secondary data on body size, breeding habitat, and the dominant frequency of the advertisement call for 110 species; 40 of them breed alongside streams and 70 away from streams. Call frequency was slightly higher and body size was significantly smaller in streamside breeding species. After controlling for the effects of body size and phylogenetic signal, only differences in body size persisted between species breeding at both kinds of habitats. Our data suggest that habitat filtering rather than acoustic adaptation explains the high call frequency of stream breeders. Species with large body size, pleiotropically constrained to utter low-frequency calls, would have succeeded less often in establishing viable populations alongside streams, due to the masking effect of low-frequency noise. Thus, small species calling at relatively high frequencies would be more common there. Although our data do not preclude adaptations to noisy habitats in some anuran species, they do not provide support for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis at a wider phylogenetic scale.