ArticlePDF Available

Resonance in conversational second stories: A dialogic resource for stance taking

Authors:

Abstract

This study investigates resonance (Du Bois 2003, 2007) in second stories as a method of anchoring the second telling to the previous telling and as a resource of stance taking. It takes a closer look at the exact ways in which second stories are structurally shaped through initial (first) tellings by examining the resonating elements (of the first stories) that the second tellers recycle in their second stories, i.e., resonating lexico-syntactic, structural, prosodic, and semantic elements. Tellers legitimize, firstly, their tellings by tying back to the previous story and, secondly, their stance by positioning themselves in view of the stance displayed in the previous story. The resonating second story as such can manifest the recipients' interpretation of what they make of the first story and provide a new angle on the first telling, either reinforcing, redefining, or rejecting the gist of the initial telling. Resonance in second stories manifests the fundamental social cohesion that conversationalists uphold, among other ways, by tying back to each other's words.
1860–7330/12/0032–0525 Text & Talk 32–4(2012),pp.525 – 545
Online1860–7349 DOI10.1515/text-2012-0025
©WalterdeGruyter
Resonance in conversational second stories:
a dialogic resource for stance taking
MAARIT SIROMAA
Abstract
This study investigates resonance (Du Bois 2003, 2007) in second stories as
a method of anchoring the second telling to the previous telling and as a
resource of stance taking. It takes a closer look at the exact ways in which
second stories are structurally shaped through initial ( rst) tellings by examin-
ing the resonating elements (of the rst stories) that the second tellers recycle
in their second stories, i.e., resonating lexico-syntactic, structural, prosodic,
and semantic elements. Tellers legitimize, rstly, their tellings by tying back to
the previous story and, secondly, their stance by positioning themselves in view
of the stance displayed in the previous story. The resonating second story as
such can manifest the recipientsinterpretation of what they make of the rst
story and provide a new angle on the rst telling, either reinforcing, redening,
or rejecting the gist of the initial telling. Resonance in second stories manifests
the fundamental social cohesion that conversationalists uphold, among other
ways, by tying back to each other’s words.
Keywords: resonance; second story; stance taking; storytelling; direct
reported speech (DRS); voicing.
1. Introduction
Thisstudyinvestigatesresonanceinsecondstoriesasamethodofanchoring
thesecond tellingtotheprevioustellingandasaresourceofstancetaking.
I examine two cases of rst story–second story pairs in naturally occurring
ScottishEnglishconversation.
1
Iexploretheintersubjectiveroleofresonance
(DuBois2003, 2007)indisplaying socialcohesion.Inthe analysis,Ihigh-
lighttheconnectionsbetweenthose resonating elements and patterns ofthe
initialorrststoriesthatthesecondtellersrecycleintheirsecondstories.
2
The
analysisshowsthatthetwostoriesareanchoredtoeachotherbyresonating
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
526 Maarit Siromaa
lexico-syntactic,structural,semantic,andprosodicelementsincorresponding
positionsintherst story–second story pairs. Indeed,therststory–second
storypairherereferstothereciprocalunitoftwoconsecutivenarrativesonthe
sametopictoldbydifferentspeakers.
More specically, the prosodic features under investigation in this paper
includetheprosodicandparalinguisticeffectsthatareemployedinthevoicing
ofreportedspeech(Couper-KuhlenandSelting1996).Theauditoryprosodic
analysis in this study concentrates on resonance between instances of the
resoundingofothersvoiceindirectreportedspeech(DRS),showingthatthe
practice of the voicing of reported speech is recycled by the second teller.
Although the type and quality of the voicing is not necessarily the same,
employingtheactualpracticeofvoicedDRSisresonant.Thendingsofthis
studysupportaclaimthatco-conversationalistsmayrespondtovoicedDRSin
theinitialtellingbyproducingasubsequentvoicedDRSintheirsecondtelling
(Niemelä2005,2011).
Resonatingelementshavebeenshowntobeconsequentialintermsofstance
taking.Thepresentstudyshedsnewlightonthephenomenonof“chimingin”
(Couper-Kuhlen1999),there-soundingofothersvoice.Inotherwords,con-
versation participants respond to voiced reported speech produced by other
interlocutorsbychimingin,whichisonewayofindicatingthat“thattheyare
orientingtoabitoftalkasthereportedspeechofagureratherthanascur-
rentspeaker’sownwords”(Couper-Kuhlen1999:4).Szczepek(2001)givesa
detailedaccountofhowspeakersdothis,examiningthewayinwhichinter-
locutorsrepeattheprosodicparametersofotherspeakers,includingintonation
contour,pitchregister,pitchjumps,volume,andspeechrate.Szczepek(2001:
41)putsforwardaclaimthatinsomecases“[p]rosodicorientationthusseems
to create a bridge between two turns that could not be achieved by verbal
means alone.” Couper-Kuhlen (1999: 11) further shows that the prosodic
chiminginofanotherconversationparticipantfunctions,ontheonehand,asa
sign of understanding that the teller is speaking in some particular “other
voice”and,ontheotherhand,asasignofco-alignmentwiththestanceofthe
teller.
Thepresentstudylendssupporttotheviewthatresonantvoicingcanfunc-
tionasadisplayofcongruentstance.Resonatingelementsdonotautomati-
callyindicateacongruentstance(GoodwinandGoodwin1987b;Kärkkäinen
2003).However,allthesecondstoriesinthispaperhappentobecongruousin
relationtothersttellings.Thisstudyalsodiscussesthesequentialorganiza-
tionofresonatingvoicedreportedspeech.Voicedenactmentsbystoryrecipi-
entsdisplayasharedstanceandconstituteoneappropriateresponsetovoiced
directreportingswithinastorytellingsequence.Aresonantinstanceofvoiced
directreportingis therefore an orderlyphenomenonininteractionand a se-
quentiallyrelevantpracticeofstancetaking.
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
Resonance in conversational second stories 527
Iapplyintheanalysistheconversation-analyticmethodofdetailedsequen-
tialandlinguisticanalysisofface-to-faceinteraction(seeSacksetal.1974;
GoodwinandHeritage1991;HutchbyandWooft1998).Ifurtherdrawfrom
thendingsofstudiesoninteractiveandintersubjectivestancetaking(seeDu
Bois2003, 2007; Kärkkäinen 2006, 2007;Englebretson 2007).To date, the
conceptofstoryandtheactivityofstorytellinghavebeenwidelycoveredin
conversation-analytic and other research elds by numerous scholars (see
LabovandWaletzky1967;Goodwin1982;Sacks1986,1992;Lerner1992;
Schegloff 1997; Thornborrow and Coates 2005; Georgakopoulos 2007). A
greatnumberofcontemporarylinguisticresearchdoneonstories,narratives,
andstorytellingreliesontheclassicworkbyLabovandWaletzky(1967)on
narrativestructure.Accordingtotheauthors,astoryorapieceofnarrative,or
whattheyrefertoastheminimalnarrative,musthaveatleasttwoconsecutive
narrativeclauses.Inotherwords,astorymusthaveatleastonetemporaljunc-
tureofevents.
Recentapproachestostories,especiallyintheeldoflinguisticsandsociol-
ogy,emphasizetheunxednatureofthenarrativeinadiscourseenvironment.
Sacks(1992)rstdrawsattentiontoconversationalstoriesasextendedturns
of talk. Producing these situated turns requires negotiation and cooperation
betweentheconversationparticipants.A conversationalstoryishereunder-
stoodasanextended,structuredturnoftalk,whichistypicallytakenbyone
teller,togiveareportofaneventinchronologicalordertooneormorerecipi-
ents in order to perform a social action, e.g., a complaint, a solicitation of
empathy,oranexpressionofsolidarity.Thispaperappliestheoverallstruc-
turalunitsofthestory,i.e.,prefacesequence;tellingsequence,whichincludes
orientation,complication,evaluation,highpoint(orig.climax),andresolution;
andresponsesequence,combiningselectivelytheviewsofSacks(1992)and
LabovandWaletzky(1967)onstorystructure.
Goodwin(2002:25)characterizesstorytellingasaninteractiveprocessin
which“tellersprovidestoryrecipientswithinterpretivetemplatesthattheyuse
tomonitortheeventsbeingreported(frequentlythoughbynomeansalways
markedashavingoccurredinthepast)prospectivelyinordertolocatewhen
thestoryarrivesatitsclimax,theplacewhererecipientsareexpectedtopro-
videaresponsetoit.”Herestorytellingissimilarlyseenasarelevantinterac-
tionalpracticeofstancetakingonitsown,asitisoftenaresultofnegotiating
about some previously introduced topic.As Jefferson(1978: 220) suggests,
something in the ow of the conversation triggers a response from a co-
participantintheformofarststory,whichthenallowsandinvitesfurther
co-participationintheformofrecipientevaluationsandfurtherstorytelling.
3
Second stories (Sacks 1992: 765; Schegloff 1992: 206; Tainio 1996: 16;
Routarinne1997:152; Norrick 2000: 112; Ochs andCapps2001:209–210;
Ikeda2003:90;Arminen2004:320;Hsieh2004:39)indeedhaveareciprocal
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
528 Maarit Siromaa
relationshipwithandaretoldimmediatelyaftertherststory.InSacks’s(1992:
259)understanding,peoplefeelfrustratedifthey,uponhearingastory,havea
similarexperiencetotellbutonlyrememberitthenextday,forexample.
Conversationalists’desiretosharesimilarexperiencesthusmanifestsitself
insecondstories.Attingsecondstoryasaresponsetotheinitialstoryillus-
trateshowandtowhatdegreethetellersympathizesandagreeswiththepre-
cedingstoryandadjustsherstancerelativetothestanceoftherststoryteller.
Ithasthusbeenproposedthatsharedstance,alignment,afliation,andunder-
standingbetweenparticipantscanbedisplayedbyproducingasecondstory,
4
buthowexactlythisisdoneonlexico-syntactic,prosodic,andstructurallevels
hasnotbeenresearchedinanydetail.Thisstudyinvestigatesthecorrespond-
ing resonating actions in the consequent tellings. Moreover, the two stories
exhibitparallelismnotonlyinformbutalsoinfunction.Thatis,theinstances
of voiced DRS in rst and second stories match each other on a functional
level,i.e.,theyappearinthestoryhighpoint,andonaformallevel,i.e.,they
sharematchinglexico-syntacticandprosodicfeatures.
2. Applyingearlierapproachestoparallelismandstancetaking
totheanalysisofsecondstories
Thecurrentsectiondiscussessomeoftheearlierresearchonparallelismand
resonanceandontheintersubjectiveprocessofstancetakingininteractionand
outlinesthemethodofanalysisusedinSection3.Someofthepreviousstudies
havetoucheduponlexical,grammatical,andprosodicresonanceinconversa-
tional or institutional second stories; however, none offermorethan a brief
glanceatthephenomenonofresonatingsecondstories.Thetheoreticalback-
groundisthereforeoutlinedbypresentinganexampleofarststory−second
storypair,implementingtheearlierliteratureonresonanceandstancetaking
foradetailedanalysisoftheresonatingelementsinthetellings.
Parallelism in discourse has been observed and documented by previous
researchonthelevelsoflexis,grammar,andsoundpatterns(seeSilverstein
1984; Tannen 1987; Johnstone 1994; Couper-Kuhlen 1999; Du Bois 2003,
2007;Laury2005;Kärkkäinen2006).GoodwinandGoodwin(1987a)intheir
workonarguinghavediscussedanddocumentedformattyingwhichrefersto
one aspect of parallelism in discourse from a conversation-analytic vantage
point.Theydescribeformattyingasaturn-designfeatureinwhichelements
of previous talk are being recycled to formulate new turns. Goodwin and
Goodwin(1987a:216)furthersuggeststhatinaseriesofargumentativemoves,
“participants frequently tie not only to the type of action produced by last
speaker but also to the particulars of its wording.” The observations by
Goodwin and Goodwin relate to a technique of tying a previous story to a
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
Resonance in conversational second stories 529
secondbywayofthesame-signicanceprocedureintroducedbyRyave(1978:
127)inwhichasucceedingstorytellerorganizesthestory“intermsofasig-
nicancestatementwhichalsoservestoformulateaprecedingstory.”
Theabove-mentionedstudiesemphasizetherecyclingofcertainelements
ofapreviousspeecheventandusingthemasafoundationuponwhichcon-
secutiveturnsrest.Anward’s (2005:28)analysisconcursthat whenever the
recycledelementsarere-uttered,theyappearinanewsequentialcontextand
thereforeareinherentlydifferentinform.OchsandCapps(2001:210)further
concludethatsecondstoriesare thematically and structurally shaped bythe
earliertellingand“provideatemplateforinterpretingtherststory.”Byway
ofexaminingtheparallelisminrststory–secondstorypairs,itispossibleto
takeacloserlookathowexactlysecondstoriesarestructurallyshapedbyear-
liertellings.
Thetermresonance is applied hereinthecurrentanalysisof consecutive
rststory–secondstorypairs,notonlybecauseitisparticularlyusefulforthe
descriptionoftheprocessesofstancetaking,butalsobecauseitdrawsapracti-
calallusiontotheresonanceofsound,i.e.,“thereinforcementorprolongation
ofsoundbyreection”(s.v.resonance,OED1989).Thetermoriginatesfrom
DuBois’s(2007)theoryondialogicsyntaxandisunderstoodbyhimasapro-
cessofactivating“afnityacrossutterances.”
Resonancehasbeendemonstratedtoreachoverarelativelyshortdistance,
i.e.,inthenextturnandtheimmediatelysubsequentturns.However,Arppe’s
(2004:71)analysisshowsthatresonatingpatternscanariseatvariouspointsof
adialogueandnotonlyincloseadjacencypairssuchasassessmentpairs,for
example.Thecurrentstudyattendstothewell-foundedevidenceprovidedby
Arppe(2004)andAnward(2005)fortrackingandidentifyinganactivecon-
nectionbetween resonatingelementsalsoinlongersequencesofinteraction
(i.e.,storyroundsandothersequencesofstorytelling).
Thefollowingexample(1)istakenfromSacks’s(1992:262)lectures,and
itsbriefillustrativeanalysisrepresentsthewaytherestofthecurrentpaperis
organized.Thebestwaytoshedlightontheactivationoftheresonatingcon-
nectionsbetweenstoriesistoapplythenotionofresonancetothetranscriptof
Sacks’sexample.Theanalysisisconductedintwoparts,outliningthemethod
ofanalysisinSection3.Irstanalyzetheresonatingelementsintermsoftheir
location within the structural story frame. That is, I identify and track the
dialogicprocessofresonanceinconcurrentstructuralunitsofthestories,i.e.,
prefacesequence,tellingsequencewhichincludesorientation,complication,
evaluation, high point and resolution, and response sequence (Labov and
Waletzky 1967; Sacks 1992). I then analyze the resonating lexico-syntactic
featuresbydisplayingtheresonatingturnsintheformofadetaileddiagraph
(seeDuBois2007),whichisatoolthatcanbeusedtomapouttheresonating
elementsinsequencesofdialogue.
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
530 Maarit Siromaa
K (whose gender cannot be deduced from the transcript) starts telling
abouthavingatalkwithhisfatherandRproducesatting,consecutivehypo-
thetical constructed explanation for the rst story about a prison warden.
Sacks does not treat R’s response to the rst telling as a second story per
se but sees the constructed explanation as “similar to a telling of a second
story” (Sacks 1992: 263), making it adequate for the purposes of this
demonstration.
(1) Are you mad at me (Sacks1992;seeappendixfortranscriptiondetails)
01 K: Ihadaveryintellectualtalkwithmyfather.
02 R: Ahhowpleasant.
03 K: Yes.Anditturnedoutverygood.
04 R: (From)theoldman’sviewpoint?
05 K: No!Inmyviewpoint.He-agreed.Foronceinhislife.
06 R: Heagreedwithwhat?
07 K: Oh,Idon’tknowIjust-
08 R: heh(He//agreed)hehh
09 K: He-hegroundedme‘causeofmygrades,see.I-Iknow,I
10 deservedtobegrounded,so-
11 R: Noyou//don’t!
12 K: I-
13 K: Ididthough.Withtwofails?You-you’reboundtobe
14 averagingto–you-
15 R: Butchudon’twannabegrounded.
16 K: Isuredo,because–uhwellw-westartingtalkin’//(an’
17 hesaid-)
18 R: Ohyouwanthimtocontrol.
19→ K: No.No.He-hesaysuhareyoumadatmebecauseI
20 groundedju?
21 R: ehhehheh
22 K: Hesezuh–(1.0)–Wellthenwhatareyousohap-orwh-
23 uhunhappyabout?
24 R: heh//Youknowwhatheremindsmeof ?
25 K: IgoOhnogoodreason,//y’know,I-
26 R: Y’knowwhatheremindsmeof,whenthewardentakesthe
27→ guytothegaschamberhesezyernotmadatmepersonally
28 hehh
29 J: Hellno!
30 R: hehhh
31 K: hhhhh
32 R: It’sthesametypeofda(h)mnsituationhehhImeanit’s
33 notmyfaultthatthesocietycouldn’t--
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
Resonance in conversational second stories 531
Thesequentialplacementoftheresonatinglines19and27isidenticalinthe
overallstructuralstoryframe,representingthedirectreportedspeech(DRS)in
thehighpointofthestory,whichincidentallyisafrequentsiteforreported
speech(CliftandHolt2007).Laury’s(2005:185)workonresonatingactions
suggeststhatwhenspeakersrecyclelinguisticelementsthey“canbeseenas
‘doingthesamething’bothinteractionallyandsyntactically,astheydevelop
theirmutualstancetowardthetopicunderdiscussion.”Likewiseinexample
(1),thecorrespondinghighpointsinthestorytellingaresimilarinform,mean-
ing,andfunction,i.e.,theyperformsimilarinteractionaltasks.Theexample
willbeanalyzedinmoredetailbelow.
Sacks (1992: 261) differentiates the kinds of responses that hearers give
to rst stories, namely remembered second stories and hypothetical second
stories,claimingthataconstructedsecondstorymaytthescopeoftherst
tellingevenbetterthananactualrememberedsecondstorybecauseofrestric-
tionsthatarecausedbywhathappened in, and what is remembered of, the
actualeventthatisbeingreported.
Theanalysisoftherststory−secondstorypairinexample(1)illustratesthe
signicanceofresponsestorststoriesandsupportsthendingsofprevious
research (see Du Bois 2003, 2007; Niemelä 2005, 2011; Kärkkäinen 2006,
2007;Englebretson2007;Keisanen2007;Rauniomaa2007;andHaddington
2007)inestablishingthatthestance-takingprocessisindeedinteractiveand
intersubjectiveinnature.Thetwoconsecutivestoriesofexample(1)partici-
pateintheintersubjectivestance-takingprocessthatemergesoutoftheprevi-
ousinteractionandgraduallydevelopsintheongoingtellingeventofthetwo
speakers,namelyKandR.
Krstevaluatesthestanceobject(DuBois2007),namelythejustication
ofhisfatherinictingpunishmentonhimbecauseofpoorgrades.Hepositions
himselfasagreeingwith his fathers actions by stating on lines 9 and10,I
deserved to be grounded,whichsuggeststhatKtakesapositiveandaccepting
stancetowardhisfathergroundinghim.Kfurtherreportsthathisfatherasked
himifhewasangryathimbecauseofthepunishmentbystatingonlines19
and20,He says uh are you mad at me because I grounded you?.Online25,K
beginsareportwhichestablishesthatheisnotangryathisfatherperseandis
unhappy for no good reason. K’s report is interrupted by R, who evaluates
theconductofandtakesanincongruentstancetowardbothK’sfatherandK
himself.RrstdrawsaparallelbetweenthebehaviorofK’sfatherandthat
ofaprisonwardenwhotakestheconvicttothegaschamber.Hefurthertopi-
calizes the explanatory, comparative, and evaluative functions of his hypo-
theticalstorybystatingonline32,It’s the same type of da(h)mn situation[the
conductofK’sfatherandthatofaprisonwarden].Revaluatesthestanceob-
ject,namelythejusticationofK’sfatherinictingpunishmentonK,bycom-
paringthedisciplinaryactionsofK’sfathertocapitalpunishment.Hedoesthis
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
532 Maarit Siromaa
byassigningtothehypotheticalprisonwardenanimaginaryreportedspeech
sequenceonline27yer not mad at me personallythatresonateswiththatof
K’sfatherHe says uh are you mad at me because I grounded you?online19.
Thehypotheticalsecondstoryisproducedinsequentiallyandinteractionally
relevantposition:itimmediatelyfollowsandprovidesaninterpretationofthe
earliertelling.
R disaligns with K, drawing a parallel between the behavior of K’s father
andanimaginaryprisonwarden,whichismanifestedintheresonatingDRS
utterancesonlines19and 27. Both sentences start with matching reporting
clauseshe (says / sez)andarefollowedbyayes–noquestion (are) you (not)
mad at me.K’squestionispositive;R’sresponse,he sez yer not mad at me,is
negativeinform.K’sreportingcontinueswithasubordinatedclausebecause
I grounded u which semantically resonates with R’s subsequent adverbial
personally.TheparallelinstancesofDRSarethelinguisticconstructionsthat
carryforwardtheinteractionalprocessofthestancetaking.Kärkkäinen(2006:
720),along withDuBois(2007),seesresonanceacrossspeakersasone re-
source of stance taking, and further concurs that two resonating utterances
maymanifestadifferentiationofstances.R’sDRSutteranceismodeledafter
thatofK’s,butthedivergentstancesaremadeexplicitintheparticipants’dis-
aligningevaluationsofthebehaviorofK’sfather.Onlines9and10, Kutters
He- he grounded me ‘cause of my grades, see. I- I know, I deserved to be
grounded, so-.Online11,RgivesaresponseNo you don’t!,claimingthatK
does not deserve to be grounded. On line 15 he further claims that K does
notwanttobegroundedeither,But chu don’t wanna be grounded.However,in
K’sunderstanding,hedeservestobegroundedbecauseofhisgrades,andhe
andhisfathernallyseeeyetoeyeonsomething,whichmakesthesituationa
positiveone.RhoweverclaimsthatKdoesnotwanttobegroundedandthat
KandhisfatherarenotinagreementbutratherKisbeingoppressedbyhis
father.Thus,thesimilarresonatingelementsdisplaydivergentviewsanddis-
closethedifferencesinindividualstances.Inthisexample,bothstoriesevalu-
atethesocialphenomenonofpunishmentandthejusticationofinictingiton
individualsfornotconformingtotherulesofthefamilyunitor,onalarger
scale,society.
ThedifferentiationofstancesofKandRisthatKacceptsthedisciplinary
actionshisfatherinictsonhimoutofnecessity,becauseofK’spoorgrades.
RontheotherhandevaluatesthedisciplinaryactionsofK’sfatherinanega-
Figure1. Diagraph of lexico-syntactic resonance in high points
19 K: He- he says are you mad at me because I groundedju?
27 R: he sez yer not mad at me personally
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
Resonance in conversational second stories 533
tivelight.HedisplaysK’sfatherasacharacterwhoostensiblyrelieveshim
oftheresponsibilityofinictingpunishmentonhissonbecauseheissimply
“doinghisjob.”AtthesametimeRimplicitlyjudgestheseeminginevitability
ofthepunishmentandridiculesK’sfathersclaimofnothavingachoicein
thematter.R’s stancedisalignswiththatofK’sandhe makesitexplicitby
sarcasticallycomparingadomesticpunishmenttoasevereinstitutionaldeath
sentence.
3. Resonatingelementsbetweenrstandsecondstories
Thissectionprovidesfurtheranalysesoftheresonatingelementsbetweenrst
andsecondstories.Thesymmetryinstorystructureswillbeshownrst.Then
thelexico-syntactic,semantic,andprosodicparallelismwillbecomparedand
analyzedin two further rst story–second story pairs.The data used in this
paper consist of audio-recorded transcripts of naturally occurring informal
conversationalinteractionsbetweennativespeakersofScottishEnglish.The
dataextractsaretakenoutofaconversationbetweentwoclosefriends,Carrie
andStephen,recordedinGlasgowin2004.
Ithasbeenestablishedthatconversationalstoriesareafrequentphenome-
non in face-to-face interactions. Not all conversational stories are followed
byaresonatingsecondstory;however,many conversational stories may be
followedbyoneormoresecondstories.Aquantitativeanalysisofhowregu-
larlyresonatingsecondstoriesoccurinvarioustypesofconversationaldatais
outsidethescopeofthisstudy.But,basedoninvestigatingthephenomenait
seemsthatsecondstoriesoccurmoreofteninsituationswheretheconversa-
tionparticipantsarefamilymembers,closefriends,oracquaintances.Itiscru-
cialtoacknowledgethatthespeakersintheexamplesofthispaperarerather
wellacquainted.Therefore,infutureresearchitwouldbeinterestingtoinves-
tigateresonatingfeaturesbetweenstorieswithinamoreheterogeneousgroup
ofspeakers.
3.1. Resonance in the corresponding structural units of the story pairs
Tofurtherillustratetheresonatingfeaturesatdifferentpointsofthestorystruc-
ture,thefollowingexamplesshowtworststory–secondstorypairsthathave
resonatingelementsincorrespondingstructuralunits.Forinstance,thepreface
sequencesoftherstandsecondtellingsmaydisplayaresonantconnection,
whichisillustratedintheanalysisbypresentingtranscriptsofthegivensec-
tionsofthetellings.
Example(2) showsresonancein thehighpoints oftherststoryandthe
secondstory.StephenandCarriearediscussingStephen’sdifcultytosleepon
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
534 Maarit Siromaa
Sunday nights. Stephen has been having trouble sleeping and he gives an
accountofatypicalSundaynight.Heexplainshowhetriestopreparehimself
forbedbutneverthelesshasdifcultybeingabletofallasleep.
(2) I can’t sleep(Glasgowdata2004)
a. Stephen’sstoryhighpoint
71 Stephen: andsoI’msittinggoing,
72 (1.3)
73 @Ican’tbelieve,
74 (.)it’shalfpastfourinthemorning,
75→ andIhavegottogetup,
76→ togotoworktomorrow/@.
77 (0.5)
78 Stephen: uhhuh,
79 (.)uh[huh].
(83linesomitted)
b. Carrie’sstoryhighpoint
162 Carrie: ((SCREAM))
163 anditwaslike,
164 @argh,
165→ Ihavetogetupforwork/@.
166 (.)ehheh.
167 (0.9)
Stephendescribeshisfutileeffortstofallasleepingreatdetailandtowardthe
endofhistelling,onlines73–76,heassignsavoicedDRSutteranceI can’t
believe it’s half past four in the morning and I have got to get up to go to work
tomorrowtohimselfasthecharacterinthestory.HisDRSsequenceconsti-
tutesthehighpointofthestoryandservesasaniconicanimatedrepresenta-
tionofhisdesperatecondition.Althoughthetellerisquotinghisownwords,
theothervoicecomesoffasseparatefromthemainbulkofthetelling.The
animatedvoicing(Couper-Kuhlen1999)ofhisutterancedepictsutterirrita-
tionanddisbelief:theDRSutteranceisaclearerandtighterarticulationand
hasaslowerpace,andthedistinctarticulationoftheDRSutteranceseparates
itfromthesurroundingturns.Bytellingtherststory,Stephenportrayshim-
selfassomeonewhosuffersfrom not being able to fall asleep at night and
positionshimselfinviewofthestanceobject,thetroublewithsleeping.Itis
notable that Carrie rst of all evaluates the stance object from her point of
viewinhersecondstorybydescribingasimilareventofhavinghersleeping
patterndisruptedbyanightmare.Towardtheendofhertelling,shealsopro-
duces,inheraccount,aresonantturntoStephen’sDRSsequence.Sherecycles
someofthefeaturesinStephen’sDRSbyproducingasimilarbutshorterand
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
Resonance in conversational second stories 535
slightlyreformulatedversionargh I have to get up for work onlines164and
165.
Althoughtheprosodicparameters,suchasintonationcontour,pitchregister,
pitchjumps,volume,andspeechrate,ofthereportedspeechsequencehavenot
been measured, the auditory analysis reveals that there is a notable shift in
thosevalues.Intermsofprosody, her DRS utterance is higher in pitch and
louderthanthesurroundingtalk.Italsohasabreathyqualitytoit.Allinall,the
distinctvoicinggivestheimpressionthatthespeakerisfacingapanickysitu-
ation,anditalsodepictsfrustrationandirritation.Niemelä(2005,2011)shows
thatco-conversationalistsoftenrespondtovoicedDRSbyproducing asub-
sequentDRSsequencethatappliesvoicingthatiseithersimilartoorsome-
howcongruentwiththeprevioussequence(cf.“chimingin”inCouper-Kuhlen
1999).ThecurrentexamplesfurtherconcurthatavoicedDRSsequenceofthe
rst speaker does not only echo in the immediately following turns of the
recipientbutalsoasfarasinthesecondstoryprovidedbythesecondspeaker
(83linesintervenebetweenthetwosequences).Moreover,itisnotnecessarily
the exact same voice quality that speakers tie back to over such a lengthy
periodbuttheactualpracticeofproducingavoicedDRSutterance.
BothDRSutterancesareprecededbyslightly differentreportingclauses.
Online 71,Stephenutters and so I’m sitting going,which clearlyidenties
Stephenhimself asreportedspeaker ofthefollowingDRSsequence.Carrie
howeverstatesand it was likeonline163,whichimpliesthatamoreimper-
sonalaccountistofollow.It referstotheactualsituation,whichdetachesCar-
rie’spersonfromtheeventbymakingitmoreuniversallyapplicable.More-
over, it was like suggests that the following report will evaluate the actual
situation,orthestanceobject.TheDRSutteranceprovidesaroughimageof
the general state of mind of a sleep-deprived person from Carrie’s point of
view. In the second story, she displays aspects of herunderstanding of Ste-
phen’srststory(Schegloff1992:206),makingexplicitthatshealignswith
Stephen’stellingandtakesacongruentstance.However,shealsoprovidesa
unique,personalangletotheproblem,contributingtothemutualunderstand-
ingthatStephenandCarriehaveofthetroublewithsleeping.Conversational
storytellingisaninteractionalprocessinwhichparticipantscandeployawide
rangeofstance-takingpractices.ByapplyingareformulatedversionofSte-
phen’sDRSsequenceinhersecondstory,CarriealignswithStephen,placing
herintheexactsamepositionasStepheninhisrststory.Carrieadjustsher
ownstanceonthetroublewithsleepingbylettingtheprosodicandlinguistic
featuresofherownDRSutteranceresonatewiththeDRSsequenceintherst
story.Shethusexpressessolidarityandasharedstancewiththetellerofthe
rststory.
Bywayoftellingresonatingsecondstories,i.e.,modelingthemafterprevi-
ousstories,tellerspositionthemselvesinregardtotherststory’sstance.The
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
536 Maarit Siromaa
secondtellercomplements the story oftheinitialstorytellerby producing a
telling that ts the scope of the initial story, reversing teller/recipient roles.
AccordingtoKärkkäinen(2006:700),resonatingelementsdonotnecessarily
denoteafliationorsharedstance.Here,however,CarrieandStephendisplay
asharedstanceandparticipateinredeningandreinforcingtheirjointunder-
standingof thesocialphenomenonof“troublewithsleeping”andacknowl-
edgingitaspartoftherealmofhumanexperiencebyproducingaresonating
rststory–secondstorypair.
Example(2)presentedarststory−secondstorypairwithresonatinghigh
pointsofthestory.Example(3)presentsasinglerststory–secondstorypair
withresonatingprefacesandhighpoints.Thefourresonantelementsresonate
not only across but also within the stories. That is, in this type of “cross-
resonance”bothprefacesareresonantwithbothhighpoints,andviceversa.
The following example illustrates all relevant sections of the transcripts,
namelytheprefacesandhighpointsofbothtellings.
Stephen,Carrie,andtheirmutualfriendLisahavebeenoutdrinkingafew
nightsearlier.Stephenintroducesthistopicbyrequestingaconrmationfrom
CarriethatLisaactuallyhasbeenfeelingsickafterthenightinquestion.After
receivingconrmationfromCarrie,Stephenstartstelling(therststory)about
thepeculiarcircumstancesofthemorningfollowingtheirnightout:hehad
gonetosleepandwokenupfullyclothed.
(3) I woke up naked (Glasgowdata2004)
a1. Stephen’srststorypreface(storyintroductionviaanannouncementon
line41):
34 Stephen: [soLisawassick]?
35 (0.7)
36 Carrie: ay,
37 shesendmeatextgoing,
38 <@Iwassick,
39 Ihadagreatnightthough,
40 thanksverymu[ch@>.=
41→ Stephen: [Iwokeupin]bedwithallmyclotheson.
(14linesomitted)
a2. Stephen’srststoryhighpoint:
55 Stephen: that’sthelastthingIremember.=
56 =Iwokeupinbedwithallthefuckingclothes_
57 likejumperon,
Stephenrstdrawsattentiontothenightinquestiononline34,So Lisa was
sick?,requestingforconrmationfromCarriethattheirmutualfriendLisahad
beenfeelingsickaftertheirnightout.Stephen’srequestforconrmationinter-
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
Resonance in conversational second stories 537
ruptsCarrie’stalkaboutanothersubjectandintroducesanewtopic.Stephen
receivesfromCarrieaconrmationthatLisahadindeedbeensick,ay online
36, and a short recap of text messaging between Carrie and Lisa on lines
37– 40:she send me a text going, I was sick, I had a great night though, thanks
very much.Hethenlaunchesintostorytellingviaanannouncementonline41
I woke up in bed with all my clothes on,thusreservingalongerturntotella
(initial)storyofhavinghadanunusualwakening.Stephenevaluatesthestance
object,namelytheextraordinarycircumstancesofwakingupthemorningfol-
lowinganightofheavydrinking,bydescribinginmoredetailtheeventsofthe
previousnight.Towardtheendofhistelling,henallyprovidesahighpoint
onlines56and57,I woke up in bed with all the fucking clothes_like jumper
on,whichisaresonatingreformulationoftheprefaceofhisstory.Stephen’s
storyprefacefunctionsasagambitthatletstherecipientknowwhattoexpect
andthatisredeemedbythehighpointatthecompletionofhistelling(Sacks
1992).
CarriestartsherowntellingrightafterStephen’sstoryandproclaimsonline
63,I totally woke up in time,statingthatsheindeedhadmanagedtowakeup,
“respectably”,intime.
(3)
b1. Carrie’ssecondstorypreface:
63→ Carrie: [Itotallywokeupintime].
64 Stephen: andIwasn’tduetogetupunti]l,
65 (1.1)
66 Stephen: eighto’clock,
67 orsomethinglikethat.
68 (0.4)
69 Carrie: I-Itotallyhadthatwakingupreallywell,
TheprefaceofCarrie’stelling(online63)resonateswiththeprefaceandhigh
pointofStephen’stellingonline41andonlines56and57.
Carriethenproceedstotellthesecondstoryinwhichshereferstoanother
timewhenshehadhadasimilarexperiencetothatofStephen’sofwakingup
inunusualcircumstances.
(3)
b2. Carrie’ssecondstoryhighpoint:
70 Ithinkthislasttime,
71 IhadbeenoutwithNeal,
72 intheAthletegig,
73 (0.5)
74→ Carrie: Iwokeupnake[d],
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
538 Maarit Siromaa
75 Stephen: [eh].
76 Carrie: likethreehourslateforwork.
Carriearrivesatthehighpointofherstoryonline74bystating,I woke up
naked. Bytellingtheresonantsecondstory, Carrie aligns with Stephen and
evaluatesthestanceobjectfromherpointofview.Thehighpointofherstory
onlines74and76,I woke up naked like three hours late for work,resonates
withboththeprefaceandhighpointofStephen’sstoryandtheprefaceofher
ownstory.
Thetwostoriesarecompactentitiesthataretightlyanchoredtoeachother
bytheirresonating elements.Thecurrent example demonstratesstorytelling
sequencesas“dynamicallyconstructedspeechevents”(Goodwin1997).The
otherparticipantsofthetellingeventcommentonthestoryoftheinitialstory-
tellerbyproducingexclamations,explicitevaluationsor“animations”thatt
thescopeoftheinitialstory,andinthesecondstorythoseteller/recipientroles
arereversed.Bythetimetheparticipantsreachthesecondstory,theysharea
widerunderstandingofthelocalstance-takingphenomenon.Thesecondstory
ispromptedbytherst,andtherststoryprovidestherelevanceofthesecond
story.
Both stories here comment on the social phenomenon of staying out late
drinkingatnightanditsconsequences,suchaswakingupinunusualcircum-
stancesthenextmorning.Herebothhighpointsofthestoryareparallelstate-
mentsthatdisplayastonishmentanddismaycausedbywakingupwithallor
noclotheson.Coates(2001:81)furtherclaimsthatthecapacityof“sequential
story-tellingtotestifytotheclosenessofparticipantsmeansthatitcanbea
powerful way of ‘doing’ friendship.” Carrie thus displays togetherness and
buildsherstanceonthatofStephen’s,aligningwiththestancedisplayedinthe
rststory.Moreover,sheidentiesherownbehaviorwiththesocialphenom-
enonof (confessing to) unruly and rebellious but “cool” behavior of young
adults.InthecurrentexampleStephenandCarrieconstructareciprocalstance-
takingenvironmentinwhichtheresonatingelementsfunctionasonemeansof
takingastance,reinforcingand(re)deningtheirunderstandingregardingthe
phenomenaofthesocialrealm.
Allthreeexamplesaboveshowthatsecondtellersrecyclelexical,syntactic,
andsemanticelementsoftherststoriesinthesecondtellings,usetherst
tellingasareferencepointofstancetaking,buildingtheirownstanceaccord-
ingtowhathasbeensaidinthersttelling.Secondtellersthusestablishcohe-
sionwiththerststoryandlegitimizethesecondtellingasappropriateinthe
given circumstances and at the given moment in interaction. In the current
examplesthetellers,viareciprocalevaluationandstancetaking,participatein
thereshaping,reinforcing,and(re)constructingoftheunderstandingpartici-
pantshaveofthesocialphenomenainquestion.
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
Resonance in conversational second stories 539
3.2. Lexico-syntactic resonance
Thediagraphsinthegures(Du Bois2007:160)are“usefulforpresenting
dialogicrelations”betweenturnsoftalk.Theysimplylineupthecorrespond-
inggrammaticalelementsoneontopoftheotherandmakevisibletheparal-
lelismbetweenany two or more turns. Here, Figures 2, 3,and4 shed light
on the resonating elements of the previous examples mainly on the lexico-
syntacticlevelbutalsoonthelevelofprosody,lendingsupporttotheanalysis
ofSection3.1.
Figure 2 deals with the example of the rst story−second story pair by
StevenandCarrieon“thetroublewithsleeping”(example[2],I can’t sleep).
Figure2showsthehighpointsoftherstandsecondtelling,oneontopof
theother,exemplifyingthewayinwhichsecondstorytellersinexample(2)
recyclenotonlythelexicalandgrammaticalelementsbutalsothevoicingof
therststory,i.e.,theutteranceshaveasimilarlexico-syntacticaswellaspro-
sodicdesign.
TheDRSutterancesproducedbyStephenonlines75and76,I have got to get
up to go to work tomorrow,andbyCarrieonline165,I have to get up for work,
simultaneouslyexhibitseveralparallelgrammaticalitems.Firstofall,thepro-
noun I takes identical perspectives in both sequences. The following verb
phrasehave (got) to get up inbothturnsembodiestheduressofeachoccasion
of“havingtogetup”afterasleeplessnight.Alsothefollowingadjuncts(to go
to) / ( for) workareparallelintheirfunction.Intermsofprosody,bothturns
carryananimatedvoicequalitythatdepictsfrustrationandirritation.
Here,inthersttelling,StephenproducesaDRSutterancethatdisplaysa
reportofhisownthoughtsandstance.Correspondingly,inthesecondstory,
CarrieuttersaresonantDRSutterancewhichdisplaysareportofherthoughts
andstance.Theseresonantturnsareshortbutveryimportantsectionsofthe
tellings.Afterall,theyarethehighpointsthatembodythegistofthestories.
Whatismore,theresonancebetweenthemaccentuatesthealignmentandcon-
gruentstanceofthetwostoriesand,foritspart,anchorsthetellingstogether.
Thendingsbasedontheexamplesofthecurrentstudywouldseemtosuggest
thatresonantturnsareparticularlycommonatthehighpointsoftellings;con-
sequently,theycouldbeconsideredtorepresentafocalpointintermsofnd-
ingresonanceinstorytelling.
Figure2. Diagraph of lexico-syntactic and prosodic resonance in high points
075 Stephen: and I have got to get up,
076 Stephen: to go to work tomorrow/@.
165 Carrie: @I have to get up for work/@.
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
540 Maarit Siromaa
Therstandsecondstoryprefacesandhighpointsinexample(3)alsohave
a resonating lexico-syntactic structure. The resonating elements not only
anchorthetwotellingstogetherbutalsoprovideasmoothtransitionfromthe
rststorytothesecond.Figure3makesvisiblethedialogicresonancebetween
the rst story−second story pair by Stephen and Carrie on “waking up in
unusualcircumstancesafteranightoutonthetown.”
Figure 3 highlights active afnity between turns that are produced by one
speakerwithinasingletelling.Forinstance,Stephen’srststoryprefaceon
line 41, I woke up in bed with all my clothes on, is a declarative sentence,
whichisalmostidenticalwiththesubsequenthighpointofthesamestoryon
lines56and57,I woke up in bed with all the fucking clothes_ like jumper on.
Moreover,inthesecondstory,Carrieproducesaresonantutteranceasthehigh
pointofherstoryonlines74and76,I woke up naked like three hours late for
work.Alsoher story preface on line 63, I totally woke up in time, contains
manyofthesamelexico-syntacticfeatures.Thesubjectandtheverbphraseof
allutterancesarethesameexceptfortheaddedadverbialtotallyinCarrie’s
secondstorypreface.
Theadverbialphrasesfollowingtheverbelementalldescribethesomehow
peculiar circumstances in which the tellers woke up. Stephen’s with all my
clothes onandwith all my fucking clothes_like jumper onandCarrie’snaked
resonateinthattheyallcommentontheunusualamountorlackofclothingat
themomentofwakingup.Carrie’sadverbialphraseoftimein timeonline63
alsoresonateswiththepreviousadverbialphrase with all my ( fucking) clothes
(on/_like jumper on) onlines41and56 –57inthatitalsodescribestheunusual
circumstancesofactuallywakingup in timeafteranightout.Finally,thehigh
pointofCarrie’stelling,naked, like three hours late for work,onlines74and
76resonateswithbothStephen’sandherownpreviousutterances:rstwith
the unusual amount or lack of clothing at night time and second with the
unusualtimeofwakingup.
Theexamplesinthispapersuggestthatresonanceisagradientfeaturein
rststory–secondstorypairs.Alldiagraphsshowthatcorrespondingturnsin
therstandsecondstoriesmaybemoreorlessparallelintermsoftheirlexis,
syntax,semanticcontent,andprosody.Itisoutsidethescopeofthisstudyto
41 Stephen:[I woke up in] bed with all my clothes on.
56 Stephen: I woke up in bed with all the fucking clothes_
57 like jumper on,
63 Carrie: [I totally woke up in time].
74 Carrie: I woke up nake[d],
76 Carrie: like three hours late for work.
Figure3. Diagraph of Lexico-syntactic resonance in prefaces and high points
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
Resonance in conversational second stories 541
draw conclusions on whether some elements resonate more frequently with
priortalkandactionthanothers. Therefore,itwouldbeworthwhiletoinvesti-
gateinfurtherstudieswhattheconsequencesareofsomesecondtellingsreso-
natingwiththepreviousmorerobustlythanothers.
Alsoitwouldseemthatelementsthatarenotincorrespondingsequential
positionswithinthestorystructuremayindeedresonatewitheachother,e.g.,
therststoryprefacemayresonatewiththerstandsecondstoryhighpoints.
5
Thoseresonatingturnscreatethematiccohesionbetweenandacrossthetell-
ings.Theyalsodisplaythespeakers’stancesandprovideapointofreference
forfurtherstancetaking.
4. Conclusion
Thecurrentpapercontributestothestudyofhowsecondstoriesproduceforms
ofintersubjectivitythroughuseofparallelstructure.Itexaminesintersubjec-
tiveandinteractivestance taking in conversationalstorytelling,focusingon
thesymmetryoflinguisticandprosodicelements,i.e.,resoundingoftheother
voice,betweenconversationparticipants.Thedatashowthattellerssystemati-
callytiebacktoprevioustellings and model new turns on old ones. Corre-
sponding actions in rst and second stories resonate with each other on a
structural-functionallevel,e.g.,inthestoryhighpoint,andonaformallevel,
e.g., in the lexico-syntax andprosody. Resonating elementscreate cohesion
betweenthestoriesbyanchoringthesecondtellingtotherstandthusprovide
therelevanceofthesecondstory.Also,resonancehasbeenpreviouslymainly
treatedasresourcesthatbridgeoverarelativelyshortdistanceintheunfolding
sequenceofturns(i.e.,alocalphenomenon).However,thispapershowsthatit
canextendoverlongerdistancesaswell.
Formalresonanceappearsinalloftheexamplesofthisstudy;however,reso-
natingvoicinginDRSutterancesonlyappearedinexample(2).Thendings
implythatsecondtellerstiebacktosomeratherthanalloftheformalelements
ofthersttellingdependingontheindividualtelling.Inotherwords,interac-
tantscanbeselective.Similarfunctionalresonanceappearsinalloftheex-
amplesinthisstudy.Especiallythehighpointsoftheconsecutivetellingsseem
toberesonant.Further,in example(2),fourresonantelementsresonatenot
onlyacrossbutalsowithinthestories,presentingacaseof“cross-resonance”
inwhichbothprefacesareresonantwithbothhighpoints,andviceversa.
Conversationaliststellstoriesininteractionallyandsequentiallyappropriate
momentstotakedivergentorconvergentstances.Onanindividuallevel,sec-
ondstoriesenabletheconversationaliststogainaccesstoeachothersexperi-
ences,emotions,andstances.Tyingbacktotherststorywhiletellingasec-
ondisahighlyintersubjectivephenomenon.Thus,theresonatingsecondstory
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
542 Maarit Siromaa
assuchcanmanifesttherecipients’interpretationofwhattheymakeoftherst
storyandprovideanewangleonthersttelling,eitherreinforcing,redening,
orrejectingthegistoftheinitialtelling.Resonatinglexico-syntactic,prosodic,
andsemanticelementsarethesynapsesthatfunctionasmeanstoactivatethis
reciprocalrelationshipbetweenarststory–secondstorypair.Firstly,tellers
legitimizetheirtellingsbytyingbacktothepreviousstory.Secondly,thetell-
ersdisplayand legitimize their stancebypositioningthemselvesin view of
thestancedisplayedinthepreviousstory.Bythetimetheparticipantsreach
the end of the second story, they share a wider understanding of the social
phenomenonbeingassessed.Resonanceinsecondstoriesmanifeststhefunda-
mentalsocialcohesionthatconversationalistsuphold,amongotherways,by
tyingbacktoeachotherswords.
Appendix:Transcriptionkey
For the purpose of transcribing examples (2) and (3) in this paper, I have
compiledatranscriptionkey,chieyfollowingthenotationthatisbasedonthe
CA style transcriptionsystem(Jefferson1985). Example(1)isanunaltered
originaltranscription(Sacks1992).Theboldtypefaceisusedtohighlightthe
sequencesunderexaminationintheexamples,whereastheitalictypefaceis
usedtodifferentiateextractsofexamplesfromthebodytext.
. terminalcontour:falling
yes_ continuativecontour:levelpitch
, continuativecontour:slightrise
! stronglyanimatedtone,pitchmovementinanydirection
[ ] overlapping
= latching
(.) micropause(lessthan0.2sec)
(2.0) lengthofpauseinapproximateseconds
ye- truncatedword
YES increasedvolume
hhh audiblebreath
ye(hh)s within-speechaspiration,laughter
(yes) uncertainhearing,transcribersbestguess
@yes/@ changeinvoicequality,usuallyreportedspeech
((SCREAM)) describingparalinguisticfeaturesoftalk
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
Resonance in conversational second stories 543
Notes
1. SeeStoryrounds(Sacks1992;Tannen1984:100).
2. Theterm“initial”isapurelyanalytictoolhere;rststoriesarisefrominteractionandaretrig-
geredbysomethingintheowoftheconversation(seeJefferson1978).
3. Seesame-signicanceprocedure(Ryave1978:127).
4. Mydataandpreviousresearch(e.g.,Sacks1992)showthatsecondstoriesdonotnecessarily
agreewiththepreviousstory.
5. Judgingbyexamples(2) and (3)above,itwould seem thatthereisapattern of resonance
betweentherststoryprefaceandhighpointandthesecondstoryhighpoint,buttheclaim
remainsrathersupercialbasedonsuchasmallsampleofdata.
References
Anward,J.2005.Lexemerecycled.Howcategoriesemergefrominteraction.Logos and Language
2.31– 46.
Arminen,I.2004.Secondstories:Thesalienceofinterpersonalcommunicationformutualhelpin
AlcoholicsAnonymous.Journal of Pragmatics 36. 319–347.
Arppe,H.2004.I don’t think so at all” — Negotiating a topic in collaboration: A sequence of
assessments.Helsinki:UniversityofHelsinkiprograduthesis.
Coates,J.2001.“My mind iswithyou”:Story sequence in thetalkofmalefriends. Narrative
Inquiry 11(1).81–101.
Couper-Kuhlen, E. 1999. Coherent voicing: On prosody in conversational reported speech. In
W.Bublitz&U.Lenk(eds.),Coherence in spoken and written discourse: How to create it and
how to describe it,11–32.Amsterdam&Philadelphia:JohnBenjamins.
Couper-Kuhlen,E.&M.Selting.1996.Towardsaninteractionalperspectiveonprosodyanda
prosodicperspectiveoninteraction.InE.Couper-Kuhlen&M.Selting(eds.),Prosody in con-
versation, 11–56.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.
DuBois,J.2003.Resonanceandstructuremapping.Invitedtalkatseminar“Stancetaking,dia-
logicsyntaxandinteraction,”UniversityofOulu,4September.
DuBois,J.2007.Thestancetriangle.InR.Englebretson(ed.),Stancetaking in discourse: The
intersubjectivity of interaction.Amsterdam&Philadelphia:JohnBenjamins.
Englebretson, R. (ed.). 2007. Stancetaking in discourse: Subjectivity, evaluation, interaction.
Amsterdam&Philadelphia:JohnBenjamins.
Georgakopoulos,A.2007.Small stories, interaction and identities.Amsterdam&Philadelphia:
JohnBenjamins.
Goodwin,C.2002.Timeinaction.Current Anthropology 43.19–35.
Goodwin,C.&M.Goodwin.1987a.Children’sarguing.InS.Philips,S.Steele&C.Tanz(eds.),
Language, gender, and sex in comparative perspective, 200 –248.Cambridge:CambridgeUni-
versityPress.
Goodwin,C.&M.Goodwin.1987b.Concurrentoperationsontalk:Notesontheinteractiveorga-
nizationofassessments(withCharlesGoodwin).IPrA Papers inPragmatics 1.1–55.
Goodwin, C. & J. Heritage. 1991. Conversation analysis. Annual Review of Anthropology 19.
283–307.
Goodwin, M. 1982. “Instigating”: Storytelling as social process. American Ethnologist 9.
674 – 695.
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
544 Maarit Siromaa
Goodwin,M.1997.Byplay:Negotiatingevaluationinstory-telling.InG.R.Guy,J.Baugh,D.
Schiffrin&C.Feagin(eds.),Towards a social science of language: Papers in honour of William
Labov, vol.2,77–102.Amsterdam&Philadephia:JohnBenjamins.
Haddington,P.2007.Positioningandalignmentasactivitiesofstancetakinginnewsinterviews.In
R.Englebretson(ed.),Stancetaking in discourse: Subjectivity, evaluation, interaction, 283–317.
Amsterdam&Philadelphia:JohnBenjamins.
Holt,E.andClift,R.2007.Reportingtalk:reportedspeechininteraction.Studies in Interactional
Sociolinguistics.Cambridge,UK:CambridgeUniversityPress.
Hsieh,E.2004.Storiesinactionandthedialogicmanagementofidentities:Storytellingintrans-
plantsupportgroupmeetings.Research on Language and Social Interaction 37(1).39–70.
Hutchby,I.&R.Wooftt.1998.Conversation analysis. Principles, practices and applications.
Cambridge:PolityPress.
Ikeda,E.2003.Socializingmissionaryideologiesthroughnarrative.Texas Linguistic Forum47.
81–95.ProceedingsoftheEleventhAnnualSymposiumaboutLanguageandSociety,Austin,
TX,11–13April.
Jefferson,G.1978.Sequentialaspectsofstorytellingininteraction.InJ.Schenkein(ed.),Studies
in the organization of conversational interaction,219–248.NewYork:Academic.
Jefferson,G.1985.Anexerciseinthetranscriptionandanalysisoflaughter.InT.VanDijk(ed.),
Handbook of discourse analysis, vol. 3: Discourse and dialogue, 25–34.London:Academic
Press.
Johnstone, B. (ed.). 1994. Repetition in discourse: Interdisciplinary perspectives, vols. 1–2.
Norwood,NJ:Ablex.
Kärkkäinen, E. 2003. “Isshe vicious ordense?” Dialogic practices of stance taking in spoken
English. In T. Nakayama, T. Ono & H. Tao (eds.), Santa Barbara papers in linguistics 12.
Recent studies in empirical approaches to language, 47– 65. Santa Barbara: University of
California.
Kärkkäinen,E.2006.Stancetakinginconversation:Fromsubjectivitytointersubjectivity.Text &
Talk26(6).699–731.
Kärkkäinen,E.2007.TheroleofI guessinconversationalstancetaking.InR.Englebretson(ed.),
Stancetaking in discourse:Subjectivity, evaluation, interaction, 183–219.Amsterdam&Phila-
delphia:JohnBenjamins.
Keisanen,T.2007.Stancetakingasaninteractionalactivity:Challengingthepriorspeaker.InR.
Englebretson(ed.),Stancetaking in discourse:Subjectivity, evaluation, interaction,253–281.
Amsterdam&Philadelphia:JohnBenjamins.
Labov,W.&J.Waletzky.1967.Narrativeanalysis:Oralversionsofpersonalexperience.Journal
of Narrative and Life History 7(1– 4). 3–38.
Laury, R. 2005. Dialogic syntax andtheemergenceoftopicsininteraction.TRANEL ( Travaux
neuchâtelois de linguistique)41.165–189.
Lerner,G.1992.Assistedstory-telling:Deployingsharedknowledgeasapracticalmatter.Quali-
tative Sociology 15(3).247–271.
Niemelä,M.2005.Voiceddirectreportedspeechinconversationalstorytelling.SKYJournal of
Linguistics 18.197–221.
Niemelä,M.2011.Resonance in storytelling: Verbal, prosodic and embodied practices of stance
taking(ActaUniversitatisOuluensisB95).Oulu:UniversityofOulu.
Norrick,N.2000.Conversational narrative: Storytelling in everyday talk.Amsterdam&Philadel-
phia:JohnBenjamins.
Ochs,E.&J.Capps.2001.Living narrative. Creating lives in everyday storytelling.Cambridge,
MA&London:HarvardUniversityPress.
OED. 1989. “resonance”. OED online. Oxford University Press. http://dictionary.oed.com/
(accessed25January2012).
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
Resonance in conversational second stories 545
Rauniomaa,M.2007.StancemarkersinspokenFinnish:minun mielestäandminustainassess-
ments.InR.Englebretson(ed.),Stancetaking in discourse: Subjectivity, evaluation, interaction,
221–252.Amsterdam&Philadelphia:JohnBenjamins.
Routarinne, S. 1997. Kertomuksen rakentaminen [Constructing a conversational story]. In L.
Tainio(ed.),Keskusteluanalyysin perusteet, 138–155. Tampere:Vastapaino.
Ryave,A.1978.Ontheachievementofaseries ofstories.InJ.Schenkein(ed.),Studies in the
organization of the conversational interaction, 113–132. NewYork:Academic.
Sacks,H.1986.Someconsiderationsofastorytoldinordinaryconversations.InU.Qasthoff&
E.Gulich(eds.),Narrativeanalysis:Aninterdisciplinarydialogue.[Specialissue].Poetics 15.
127–138.
Sacks,H.1992.Lectures on conversation,G.Jefferson(ed.).Oxford:Blackwell.
Sacks,H.,E.Schegloff&G.Jefferson.1974.Asimplestsystematicsfortheorganizationofturn-
takingforconversation.Language 50.696 –735.
Schegloff,E.1992.Inanothercontext.InC.Goodwin &A. Duranti,Rethinking context: Lan-
guage as an interactive phenomenon(StudiesintheSocialandCulturalFoundationsofLan-
guage11.)NewYork:CambridgeUniversityPress.
Schegloff,E.1997.Narrativeanalysis:Thirtyyearslater.InM.Bamberg(ed.),Oralversionsof
personalexperience:Threedecadesofnarrativeanalysis.[Specialissue].Journal of Narrative
and Life History 7(1– 4).97–105.
Silverstein,M.1984.Onthe pragmatic “poetry”ofprose:Parallelism,repetition,andcohesive
structureinthetimecourseofdyadicconversation.InD.Schiffrin(ed.),Meaning, form, and use
in context: Linguistic applications,181–199.Washington,DC:GeorgetownUniversityPress.
Szczepek,B.2001.Prosodicorientationinspokeninteraction.InLiSt — Interaction and Linguistic
Structures 27.1–45.
Tainio,L.1996.Arkikertomuksetkulttuurisentietämyksenvälittäjinä[Mundanestoriesasmedia-
torsofculturalknowledge].Naistutkimus9(2).11–29.
Tannen,D.1984.Conversational style. Analyzing talk among friends.Norwood,NJ:Ablex.
Tannen,D.1987.Repetitioninconversation:Towardapoeticsoftalk.Language 63(3).574 – 605.
Thornborrow,J.&J.Coates(eds).2005.The sociolinguistics of narrative.Amsterdam&Philadel-
phia:JohnBenjamins.
MaaritSiromaa(néeNiemelä)receivedherPh.D.inresonanceinstorytellingfromtheUniversity
ofOulu,Finland,andiscurrentlyworkingthereasapost-doctoralresearcher.Herresearchinter-
ests include situated resourcesand practices of social interaction.Address for correspondence:
EnglishPhilology,P.O.Box1000,FI-90014UniversityofOulu,Finlandmaarit.siromaa@oulu.
.
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
Brought to you by | University of Oulu
Authenticated
Download Date | 3/4/15 8:45 AM
... The first story is shaped and mapped onto the responding story, since "tellers provide story recipients with interpretive templates that they use to monitor the events being reported […] prospectively in order to locate when the story arrives at its climax, the place where recipients are expected to provide a response to it" (Goodwin 2002: 27). This interrelation between first and second stories, together with the reciprocity that is often displayed in collective storytelling, depends upon the practice of stance-taking, which means adopting a specific point of view publicly (Simoraa 2012;Kääntä and Lehtinen 2016). Stance-taking is both a linguistic and a social act, and it always involves an evaluation, whether explicit or implicit (inferred) (Du Bois 2007). ...
... Both resolutive stories and parallel assessments are second stories that resonate with first stories, 6 but they are modelled and shaped in different ways with regard to the first poster's stance. A revealing aspect of how response stories resonate is the interactive practice of recycling key lexical, semantic, and syntactic material from the first to the second story (Kärkkäinen 2006;Simoraa 2012). ...
... As Simoraa (2012) contends, these resonating elements forge cohesion between the first and the second story. Indeed, the response story in (4b) is anchored in that told in (4a), in such a way that the first narrative sets the parameters that determine the relevance of the second. ...
Article
Full-text available
Online peer support groups encourage individuals to tell their stories and to find validation and emotional comfort when reading about the stories of others. Coincidently, lived experiences are the kind of knowledge applied to solicit and to deliver peer advice. This study examines the relationship between storytelling and advice in an English speaking online forum that provides support for those with an eating disorder (ED). The results revealed a range of different types of narratives within the data, from more elaborate testimonials of the ED and the process of recovery to brief personal passages responding to the first poster. The Labovian narrative structure appeared in a number of the first stories, whereas two main configurations, contingent upon the kind of response offered, emerged in second stories: parallel assessments (or snapshots) and success stories. Parallel assessments constituted self-centred stories and did not include any advice provision. Success stories, instead, became an essential component of the advice-giving act since they were remedial. The solution proposed by responders to the problem posed by the first poster was organized either to offer tips (that is, a series of practical recommendations to address a specific ED or recovery issue) or to deliver thoughtful advice through a resolutive story that introduced the state of recovery as a real possibility. Both parallel assessments and resolutive stories included contrasting resonances in relation to the first story. Resolutive stories encompassed resonating elements whose meanings were transformed and (re)signified from the positioning of a subject moving towards recovery. However, snapshots echoed specific key expressions from the initiating post. The goal was to display alignment with the first teller by describing a similar I-perspective experience. Taken together, the individual small stories contributed to the co-construction of a multiple-lived story with regard to the ED in the online community.
... Die Konversationsanalyse, die grundsätzlich ein großes Interesse an interaktionsbasierten Hilfskontexten wie etwa der Therapie oder der Kommunikation zwischen Ärzt*innen und Patient*innen hat, hat bislang kaum Arbeiten zur Kommunikation in Selbsthilfegruppen hervorgebracht. Eine namhafte Ausnahme stellen die Arbeiten von Ilkka Arminen zur Kommunikation der Anonymen Alkoholiker (AA) dar (Arminen, 1998a (Ryave, 1978;Selting, 2012;Siromaa, 2012). Eine Geschichte, lässt sich verknappt sagen, macht eine Geschichte erwartbar, und eine Folgeerzählung dient dazu, die erste Geschichte zu ratifizieren, zu deuten und ihre Relevanz weiterzuspinnen (Sacks, 1995, Vol II, S. 249). ...
... Indem sie die zentrale Aussage der ersten Erzählung aufgreifen und sich inhaltlich und formal an dieser ausrichten, demonstrieren sie darüber hinaus performativ das Verstandenhaben auf eine Weise, die die reine Bestätigung nicht leisten kann. Abfolgen von Geschichten dienen damit nicht nur dem Fortgang einer Interaktion, sondern auch der Identifikation und dem sozialen Zusammenhalt (Siromaa, 2012;Theobald & Reynolds, 2015). Diese Eigenschaften von verknüpften (»clumped«, Sacks, 1995, Vol II, S. 250) Erzählungen lassen sich in der Situation der Selbsthilfegruppe für den Austausch fruchtbar machen. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Viele pädagogische und andere Arbeitsfelder definieren sich über »Helfen« als zentralen Handlungsmodus. Aber trotz breiter sozial- und erziehungswissenschaftlicher Diskussion bleibt das Helfen als Praxis theoretisch unterbestimmt. Der interdisziplinäre Band versammelt Beiträge zu organisierten Formen der Hilfegewährung, die für den Wohlfahrtsstaat kennzeichnend sind. Qualitative empirische Studien zeichnen die Praktiken des institutionalisierten Helfens und die Verschränkungen mit ihren organisationalen Strukturen nach. Dieser Blick eröffnet Perspektiven auf die Methodizität des Helfens als widersprüchliches Phänomen: trotz aller Vorgaben und Konzepte beruht es im Kern auf konkreten Begegnungen.
... Die rekonstruierte Ordnung ist insofern einzigartig, als sie sich in einer spezifischen Situation entfaltet hat; sie ist aber gleichzeitig in der Lage, Aussagen darüber zu treffen, woran sich Mitglieder in Situationen wie dieser orientieren, was sie für angemessen und relevant oder unangemessen und sanktionierbar halten und was es ist, dass sie gemeinsam regelhaft herstellen. Aus diesem Grund geben auch detaillierte Einzelfall-Format beschrieben, das gerade informeller Interaktion zu Kohärenz und Themenprogression verhilft (Ryave, 1978;Selting, 2012;Siromaa, 2012). Eine Geschichte, lässt sich verknappt sagen, macht eine Geschichte erwartbar, und eine Folgeerzählung dient dazu, die erste Geschichte zu ratifizieren, zu deuten und ihre Relevanz weiterzuspinnen (Sacks, 1995, Vol II, S. 249). ...
... Indem sie die zentrale Aussage der ersten Erzählung aufgreifen und sich inhaltlich und formal an dieser ausrichten, demonstrieren sie darüber hinaus performativ das Verstandenhaben auf eine Weise, die die reine Bestätigung nicht leisten kann. Abfolgen von Geschichten dienen damit nicht nur dem Fortgang einer Interaktion, sondern auch der Identifikation und dem sozialen Zusammenhalt (Siromaa, 2012;Theobald & Reynolds, 2015). Diese Eigenschaften von verknüpften (»clumped«, Sacks, 1995, Vol II, S. 250) Erzählungen lassen sich in der Situation der Selbsthilfegruppe für den Austausch fruchtbar machen. ...
Book
Full-text available
Viele pädagogische und andere Arbeitsfelder definieren sich über »Helfen« als zentralen Handlungsmodus. Aber trotz breiter sozial- und erziehungswissenschaftlicher Diskussion bleibt das Helfen als Praxis theoretisch unterbestimmt. Der interdisziplinäre Band versammelt Beiträge zu organisierten Formen der Hilfegewährung, die für den Wohlfahrtsstaat kennzeichnend sind. Qualitative empirische Studien zeichnen die Praktiken des institutionalisierten Helfens und die Verschränkungen mit ihren organisationalen Strukturen nach. Dieser Blick eröffnet Perspektiven auf die Methodizität des Helfens als widersprüchliches Phänomen: trotz aller Vorgaben und Konzepte beruht es im Kern auf konkreten Begegnungen.
... Die Konversationsanalyse, die grundsätzlich ein großes Interesse an interaktionsbasierten Hilfskontexten wie etwa der Therapie oder der Kommunikation zwischen Ärzt*innen und Patient*innen hat, hat bislang kaum Arbeiten zur Kommunikation in Selbsthilfegruppen hervorgebracht. Eine namhafte Ausnahme stellen die Arbeiten von Ilkka Arminen zur Kommunikation der Anonymen Alkoholiker (AA) dar (Arminen, 1998a (Ryave, 1978;Selting, 2012;Siromaa, 2012). Eine Geschichte, lässt sich verknappt sagen, macht eine Geschichte erwartbar, und eine Folgeerzählung dient dazu, die erste Geschichte zu ratifizieren, zu deuten und ihre Relevanz weiterzuspinnen (Sacks, 1995, Vol II, S. 249). ...
... Indem sie die zentrale Aussage der ersten Erzählung aufgreifen und sich inhaltlich und formal an dieser ausrichten, demonstrieren sie darüber hinaus performativ das Verstandenhaben auf eine Weise, die die reine Bestätigung nicht leisten kann. Abfolgen von Geschichten dienen damit nicht nur dem Fortgang einer Interaktion, sondern auch der Identifikation und dem sozialen Zusammenhalt (Siromaa, 2012;Theobald & Reynolds, 2015). Diese Eigenschaften von verknüpften (»clumped«, Sacks, 1995, Vol II, S. 250) Erzählungen lassen sich in der Situation der Selbsthilfegruppe für den Austausch fruchtbar machen. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Der Beitrag untersucht anhand von Gesprächsaufnahmen in einer Suchtselbsthilfegruppe, wie Gruppenmitglieder das Format der Erzählung einsetzen, um irritierende Erlebnisse füreinander einzuordnen. Durch die Aneinanderreihung von ähnlichen, aber spezifisch anderen Erzählungen gelingt es ihnen, eine gemeinsame Perspektive auf das Erlebte einzunehmen und es als Fall-von-etwas zu normalisieren. Dabei wird deutlich, dass die Gruppe gleichermaßen daran arbeitet, individuellen Perspektiven einen Ort zu geben, wie auch gemeinsame Orientierungspunkte herauszuarbeiten und zu stabilisieren. Das Helfen in der Selbsthilfegruppe unterscheidet sich demnach von organisationalem Helfen darin, dass die Bearbeitung von Themen nicht unidirektional ist, sondern auch der Selbstvergewisserung und Reflexion aller Gruppenmitglieder dient. /////////////////////////////// This text investigates recordings from a support group on addiction, showing how members use narratives to make sense of disturbing experiences. By stringing together similar, but specifically different stories they manage to establish a shared perspective on what happened and to normalize it as a case-of-something. It becomes clear that the group works to provide a space for individual perspectives as much as it strives to build and stabilize shared interpretations. Helping in support groups thus differs from organizational support in that dealing with relevant topics is not a unidirectional effort but allows all group members to reflect on and reaffirm their own positions.
Article
Analyzing 20 comments posted in response to YouTube videos wherein two Asian American young women share their “lunchbox moment” stories, or first-person past-oriented accounts of how their (white) classmates at school reacted negatively to food that they brought from home for lunch, we demonstrate how posters collaboratively transform individual offline experiences of marginalization and difference into online moments of inclusion, solidarity, and shared identity. Integrating research on “second stories” ( Sacks 1992 ), “story rounds” ( Tannen 2005 ), online storytelling ( Page 2011 , 2018 ), and online-offline interconnections (e.g., Bolander and Locher 2020 ), we show how commenters of diverse backgrounds accomplish “adequation” ( Bucholtz and Hall 2005 ) between their different minority identities in how they convey their own lunchbox moment stories. By using metadiscursive terms (e.g., “story”), “constructed dialogue” ( Tannen 2007 ), ethnic category mentions, heritage languages, familiar address terms (e.g., first name), and emojis, YouTube posters create inclusion online and across cultural, ethnic, and spaciotemporal lines.
Chapter
Previous studies have indicated that self-disclosure post messages are prevalent in online self-help groups. These studies tend to employ quantitative analytical approaches to reveal the correlations between self-disclosure post messages and their responses in terms of frequency and specific content. However, the functions of self-disclosure post messages have been overlooked. This chapter conducts discourse analysis to explore how participants utilize self-disclosure post messages to achieve various communicative purposes in online self-help groups for anxiety and depression (OSGADs) from personal, textual, and interpersonal perspectives. The results show that self-disclosure is multifunctional in the discourse. Identified functions of self-disclosure post messages include distancing from problems, releasing emotions, showing empathy/understanding, giving advice, encouraging more disclosure, and normalizing experiences. This chapter argues that self-disclosure post messages are significant in the online communication of social support, as they enable participants to offer emotional, informational, and network support simultaneously.
Article
Speaking on one's own behalf and asserting one's entitlement to assess oneself are regular features of interaction, but participants often sound for others through practices like responsive animation, through which they are seen to be temporarily “doing being” others in a responsive slot. In this paper we study a collection of responsive animations consisting primarily of non-lexical vocalisations with gestural ensembles produced in contexts where territorial rights are in tension. We focus on environments where a participant engages in self-deprecating disclosures around past or projected negative happenings that may be shared by a co-participant (albeit experienced independently) and who animates an aspect of these in response. We describe how co-participants sound for each other by deploying animations that instead of minimising deprecating components, actually amplify them through a transformed and creative vocal and/or visual demonstration of a jointly negotiated (shared) attribute or experience. These animations create brief moments of heightened involvement and other-attentiveness before transitioning to a new/next order of conversational business.
Article
Revealing one’s evaluation towards a shared target of stance will likely set off a chain of reactions among all participants in an interaction. This interactive activity widely recognized as stancetaking has attracted the attention of researchers in a variety of fields of inquiry. This paper intends to enrich this line of research by revealing details of stancetaking as an evolving process. It proposes to do so by recognizing two separate layers relevant for stance progression. The first is the external layer where participants physically exchange utterances in order to negotiate their stances. The second is the internal layer where each participant interacts with his/her own internalized and internalizing knowledge. To demonstrate these points, I will analyze excerpts of English conversations between unacquainted speakers who experienced a common major incident in their daily lives (an earthquake and a false missile alarm incident). I will also use a conversation in Thai to demonstrate how a speaker indexes her changing evaluation toward a third person by alternating different third person pronouns.
Article
In this paper we offer a longitudinal Conversation Analysis of talk lasting 18 months between a father and son, which reveals changes in the child’s level of Interactional Competence (IC). We propose an index of developing IC based upon Sacks’ distinction between “invited” and “volunteered” stories. While stories have a “socialization function” we suggest stories may be tracked in terms of IC also. What has been called “today narrative” routines (“What did you do at school today”, etc.), initiated by the father, predominate in a series of conversations. The analysis reveals how the child is encouraged to take extended turns through the father’s questions and comments, which are developed into a storytelling sequence. We observe that the child’s responses to the initial inquiries become more elaborate over time. Furthermore, there is a gradual resistance to the invited story format because it inhibits how the child organizes his stories. Volunteered stories obtain a more personal ‘voice’. The paper showcases the nexus between socialization and interactional competence.
Book
Full-text available
Die gesellschaftliche Bedeutung populärkultureller Formate wie der TV-Serie hängt nicht nur von den ästhetischen Medienprodukten selbst ab, sondern auch von ihrer kommunikativen Verarbeitung in der Interaktion. Dabei bieten Serien in hohem Maße ein identitäts- und gemeinschaftsstiftendes Potential, dem sich die vorliegende Studie aus gesprächsanalytischer Perspektive widmet. Anhand von videographierten Gesprächen über Serien und Serienrezeption zwischen Schüler/-innen wird erörtert, wie sich die Teilnehmenden miteinander vergemeinschaften und voneinander abgrenzen, in welchem Zusammenhang dabei interaktive Bewertungskonstruktionen und der relative Wissensstatus stehen und welchen Einfluss die medialen und ästhetischen Ressourcen der Serien für interaktionale Positionierungspraktiken haben. Umfangreiche Detailanalysen werden in einem Analysemodell zusammengeführt, das dabei helfen kann, sowohl die Komplexität von Serienkommunikation als auch die sozialisatorische Relevanz von Serienrezeption in jugendlichen Peer-Groups besser zu verstehen. Die Arbeit schließt an zentrale Forschungslinien der Konversations- und Diskursanalyse an und bietet Anknüpfungspunkte für künftige Untersuchungen zu medienbezogenen Interaktionen. Open Access verfügbar: https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/9783110727845/html
Article
The abstract for this document is available on CSA Illumina.To view the Abstract, click the Abstract button above the document title.
Chapter
This chapter highlights the notion of a series of stories, which can be articulated by considering the possibly implied analytical problem. For stories to obtain the series-of-stories status, conversational participants need to listen to and analyze an in-progress story in such a manner as to permit them, upon the completion of the present storyteller's story, to construct their own story by utilizing the results of their prior analytic attention, in such a way as to assure that their succeeding story is seen as a successive story with definite, observable relationships to the previous story. An implication of these considerations is that the occurrence of a series of stories, as a conversational phenomenon, is not a preordained, pre-given matter; it is not guaranteed independent of the occasions on which the stories are told but resides in the succeeding storyteller's situated achievement.