ArticlePDF Available

Behavioral Biases of Individual Investors: The Effect of Anchoring

Authors:

Abstract

The objective of this paper is to investigate the presence of the anchoring bias in the financial decision making of individual investors. A survey study is carried out to find out how the studied bias affects the investment behavior on the Tunisian stock market. The survey is for exploratory purpose and it is based on multiple factorial correspondence analyses. The results reveal that Tunisian investors do not suffer from the anchoring bias.
Eurasian Journal of Social Sciences, 3(1), 2015, 13-19
DOI: 10.15604/ejss.2015.03.01.002
EURASIAN JOURNAL OF SOCIAL SCIENCES
http://www.eurasianpublications.com
I am grateful to participants of the 14th EBES conference for valuable comments and suggestions, but
reserve to myself the responsibility for any errors.
BEHAVIORAL BIASES OF INDIVIDUAL INVESTORS: THE EFFECT OF
ANCHORING
Salma Zaiane
FSEG Tunis, Tunisia. Email: zaiane_salma@yahoo.com
Abstract
The objective of this paper is to investigate the presence of the anchoring bias in the financial
decision making of individual investors. A survey study is carried out to find out how the studied
bias affects the investment behavior on the Tunisian stock market. The survey is for exploratory
purpose and it is based on multiple factorial correspondence analyses. The results reveal that
Tunisian investors do not suffer from the anchoring bias.
Keywords: Behavioral Finance, Anchoring, Individual Investors, Emergent Market
JEL Classification: G12
1. Introduction
The scandals that have occurred in recent years and the crashes and successive financial
crises that characterize modern economies, including the current financial meltdown from the
subprime, lead us to question the functioning of financial markets. Researchers try to
understand the attitudes of investors, often influenced by mental routines, errors in judgments or
even emotional factors. Obviously, this leads one to doubt the efficiency of financial markets,
that is to say, their ability to control the policies of the firms and to allocate the capital optimally.
Kahneman and Tversky (1974; 1979) propose an alternative study focusing on behavioral
evidence in total opposition to the rationality of investors which follows the theory of financial
markets. Indeed, investors are not fully rational and their demand for risky financial assets is
affected by their beliefs or their feelings, which are clearly not justified by economic
fundamentals. They are thus prey to several biases that affect their logical reasoning, and push
them to commit errors in thinking.
Empirical work and recent experimental research have confirmed that the errors of
judgments made by individuals affect the behavior of security prices on financial markets. In
fact, investors do not necessarily follow objective notions of financial loss or gain calculated
mathematically. A key way, in which investors are victims, is the anchoring bias according to
which people tend to rely on the first piece of information offered (the "anchor") in making
judgments or taking decisions.
In this paper, we seek to better understand the human behavior that governs the
dynamics of financial markets, studied through investor anchoring on the Tunisian stock market.
For that purpose, we use a questionnaire developed and administered to a Tunisian sample of
individual investors. The rest of the paper is organized as follows: Section II presents a review
Salma Zaiane / Eurasian Journal of Social Sciences, 3(1), 2015, 13-19
14
of the literature of the anchoring bias, and Section III presents the assumptions of our work.
Empirical validation is described in Section IV and Section V is devoted to present the empirical
results and their interpretation. Finally, Section VI contains the summary and the conclusion.
2. Literature Review
Anchoring describes the common human tendency to rely too heavily, or „anchor‟ on one trait or
piece of information when making decisions. When presented with new information, the
investors tend to be slow to change or the value scale is fixed or anchored by recent
observations. They are expecting the trend of earning is to remain with historical trend, which
may lead to possible under reactions to trend changes.
“In many situations, people make estimates by starting from an initial value that is
adjusted to yield the final answer. The initial value, or starting point, may be suggested by the
formulation of the problem, or it may be the result of a partial computation. In either case,
adjustments are typically insufficient (Slovic and Lichtenstein, 1971). That is, different starting
points yield different estimates, which are biased toward the initial values. We call this
phenomenon anchoring (Tversky and Kahneman, 1974, p. 1128).
According to Esch et al. (2009), anchoring refers to a biased judgment of a stimulus
based on an initial assessment of another stimulus and an insufficient adjustment away from
that initial assessment”. This means that an earlier presented value affects people when they
are to estimate an unknown quantity, which then will be close to the value that was considered
before the estimation. An example of the anchoring effect is how you get influenced by the
asking price when buying a house (Kahneman, 2011). A higher asking price will influence you to
value the house higher than you would have done if the asking price was lower. According to
Kahneman (2011), any number that you are asked to consider as a possible solution to an
estimation problem will induce an anchoring effect.
Numeric judgments under uncertainty are the most observed anchoring effects, since a
lot of studies have been done in this field (Esch et al. 2009). However, the anchoring effect of a
judgment does not have to be a numeric one (Cohen and Reed, 2006), but is a general
phenomenon (Soman and Chattopadhyay, 2007). Hence, every time individuals form an image
about a stimulus while another stimulus is present, this image may be subject to anchoring
effects (Esch et al. 2009).
Anchoring is produced by two different mechanisms, where one occurs in System 1 and
one in System 2. In System 1, anchoring is an automatic manifestation, which occurs by a
priming effect. In System 2, anchoring instead occurs in a conscious activity of adjustment.
However, there is in most cases no corresponding subjective experience in anchoring. This
effect is therefore often perceived by people as unbelievable (Kahneman, 2011).
Forty years of psychological anchoring studies have found the anchoring bias to be
fairly robust against experimental variations (Furnham and Boo, 2011). Contrasting this view,
recent economic field experiments on anchoring in price valuations find only moderate effects
(Simonson and Drolet, 2004; Bergman et al. 2010; Tufano, 2010; Alevy et al. 2011; Fudenberg
et al. 2012; Maniadis et al. 2014). These results support the notion of market conditions
correcting irrational consequences of individual heuristics. Therefore, rationality-increasing
teamwork as a ubiquitous form of decision-making in actual markets might be an additional filter
for biased decisions previously overlooked in experimental studies (Meub and Till, 2014).
3. Empirical Studies
3.1. Objective
The aim of our empirical studies is to test the existence of the anchoring bias on a sample of
individual investors on the Tunisian stock market, to study if they are victims of this bias in
making their decisions. For that, we conducted a questionnaire survey. Indeed, the psychology,
which can be defined as "the science of behavior”, must be taken into account by a method of
investigation which can well describe the characteristics of the investor. The questionnaire
Salma Zaiane / Eurasian Journal of Social Sciences, 3(1), 2015, 13-19
15
appears to be a useful tool in determining how individual errors affect aggregate behavior. We
will particularly understand how the decisions of many individual investors are incorporated into
prices on financial markets.
3.2. Data
The subjects are targeted on the individual private stock investors in Tunis1. We addressed our
questionnaire to 150 Tunisian investors2. We used two methods of data collection (face to face
interviews and mail survey). We got a response rate of 83% and a final sample of 125 investors.
The survey was conducted in July 2008. The face-to-face interviews3 allowed us to respond
directly to questions that respondents were asked about the issue itself. It also allowed us to
better control the representativeness of the sample. Furthermore, we avoided expressing any
opinion or any form of approval or disapproval, to avoid influencing the respondent.
3.3. Profile of Respondents
Table 1 reports summary statistics for our sample of investors grouped by gender, age,
education and business position. 73.6% of the subjects who responded to the questionnaire
were men. This is easily understood since the number of men is higher than the number of
women investing in the Tunisian stock market. A greater number of subjects (35.2%) were
aged around 35~49 while 30.4% were aged between 25 and 34 years. 44% of the subjects
have a bachelor degree while 44.8% have a master degree and above. We remark according to
our sample, that the higher the degree of education, the more we invest in the stock market.
Moreover, the proportion of executives is very high. In fact, they represent almost half of our
sample (48%). Finally, most of the respondents belonged to the middle-income class with a
monthly income between 600 and 2000 dinars4.
Table 1. Profile of respondents
Variables
Response (in %)
Gender
Male
73.6
Female
26.4
-
-
-
Age
<25
12.8
25-34
30.4
35-49
35.2
50-60
12.8
>60
8.8
Education*
low
11.2
Middle
44.0
High
44.8
-
-
Income**
Low
23.3
Middle
58.4
High
18.4
-
-
Business
position
Merchant,
Artisan,
Entrepreneur
6.4
Executive,
Higher
intellectual
profession
48.0
Middle
management
20.8
Employee
8.0
Student
9.6
Notes: *The education of low: high school or lower; middle: bachelor; high: master and above.
**The income of low: < 600 dinars; middle: [from 600 dinars to 2000 dinars]; high: > 2000 dinars.
1 We note that commercial agents working at the front offices in stock market intermediary houses help as
to contact the investors.
2 Several questionnaires were omitted since too many questions had been left unanswered.
3 Face to face interviews represent 70% of total interviews. We chose to perform our investigation on the
big Tunis (Tunis, Ben Arous, Ariana), because the population of the big Tunis is heterogeneous and
diversified and therefore, it gives us a greater depth of information.
4 100 Tunisian Dinars = 54.15 US Dollars as of 14/12/2014.
Salma Zaiane / Eurasian Journal of Social Sciences, 3(1), 2015, 13-19
16
3.4. Methodology
For our study, we used the “Sphinx” software (trial version, V5). This allowed us to design the
questionnaire, to register the responses, and especially to process and analyze the data. We
did not take missing data into consideration. Indeed, the terms "no answers" do not appear in
the results: It could be either a deliberate refusal to answer certain questions or accidental
omissions.
The anchoring bias is studied through the following four questions. For each question,
one response modality is considered symptomatic of the psychological bias. If we accumulate
three typical responses, we confirm the presence of the latter. We create a code for each
question (variable) and each modality. This involves defining a label, that is to say an abstract in
a smaller number of characters. Each theme is associated with a number. For example, the first
question is associated with the code "Loss value1. The coding variable is given in Table 2.
Table 2. Coding variable
Loss value
When you lose money on a value you :
Loss value1 : never reinvest on it
Loss value2 : Try to regain with it quickly
Loss value3 : look from time to time to see
the evolution of its price without doing
anything
Information after
analysis
If the day after you bought a security you
learn information that challenges your
analysis, you :
Information after analysis1 : abandon
your analysis and, if appropriate, resell the
security
Information after analysis2 : wait till
another information comes consolidate one
or the other of the positions
Information after analysis3 : put in
perspective the scope of the information
First idea
You think in stock exchange the
first idea:
First idea1 : is always good
First idea2 : should never be followed
First idea3: is often good when it comes to
selling and bad when it comes to buying
Comparison
Over the long term, you prefer to buy a
security that will be
undervalued compared to:
Comparison1 : its direct competitor
Comparison2: its sector
Comparison3: the overall market
Table 3 summarizes the anchoring symptoms that will be developed later.
Table 3. Anchoring symptoms
Code
Presence of the representativeness bias
Loss value1
Under-reaction to new information
Information after analysis3
Importance of the first intuition
First idea1
Presence of the confirmation bias (numerical anchoring)
Comparison1
After this coding, the data were entered on the Sphinx software. Finally, we presented
the results of the analysis.
Salma Zaiane / Eurasian Journal of Social Sciences, 3(1), 2015, 13-19
17
4. Results
Tables 4, 5, 6 and 7 report the results of the univariate analysis of the various variables of the
anchoring bias. The symptomatic modality of the bias is set in gray.
Table 4. Loss value Table 5. Information after analysis
Loss value
Number
of
citations
%
Information after analysis
Number
of citations
%
Loss value1
14
11.5%
Information after analysis1
31
25.0%
Loss value2
44
36.1%
Information after analysis2
53
42.7%
Loss value3
64
52.5%
Information after analysis3
40
32.3%
Total
122
100%
Total
124
100%
Table 6. First idea Table 7. Comparison
First idea
Number of citations
%
Comparison
Number of citations
%
First idea1
44
37.6%
Comparison1
10
8.1%
First idea2
29
24.8%
Comparison2
46
37.1%
First idea3
44
37.6%
Comparison3
68
54.8%
Total
17
100%
Total
124
100%
We note from Table 4 that only 11.5% of the respondents prefer not to invest in a
security that decreases substantially without any reason. In contrast, the majority of the subjects
(52.5%) preferred to watch the evolution of its price from time to time without doing anything.
This is not consistent with the representativeness bias that stipulates that past events are
typical and representative of the future.
Table 5 shows that 42.7% of the respondents, when they learn information that calls
into question their analysis, expect other information to come consolidating one or the other
position. In contrast, 32.3% of the subjects put the scope of that information into perspective.
This does not confirm the results of Bernard and Abarbanell (1992) and Barberis, Shleifer and
Vishny (1998) that argue that the anchoring is at the origin of the under-reaction of investors to
new information.
We note from Table 6 that 37.6% of the respondents consider that while trading the first
idea is always good. The investor sentiment seems to be influenced by the first intuition
(Bazerman, 2004; Chapman and Johnson, 2002; Epley, 2004). In Fact, They tend to
overestimate the information that are in line with their first idea and to underestimate those that
oppose it. Thus, they will more likely buy the security, when they had a favorable first
impression and vice versa. In contrast, 37.6% of them assume that the first idea is often good
when it comes to selling and bad when it comes to buying. Therefore, buying and selling
decisions are not controlled by objective assessments, with the risks that entail (Mangot, 2004).
Table 7 shows that the majority of respondents (54.8%) prefer to buy undervalued
securities as compared to the overall market over the long term. In contrast, only 8.1% of them
consider the price of the competitor as an anchor. They present the numerical anchoring bias. It
is a declination of numerical confirmation bias. It reflects the tendency to focus on a number and
then use it as a reference when making an estimate (Mangot, 2004). This reasoning can be
misleading. The less we know the market price, the more vulnerable it is to numerical anchor.
Thus, the numbers (PER in this case) can affect the valuation of securities (Thomas and
Morwitz, 2007). Therefore, we can get the values when the PER is lower (higher) than their
comparable to buy in a bull market (bearish). Thus, valuations may deviate considerably from
their historical standards. We conclude from the results of Tables 4, 5, 6 and 7 the absence of
Salma Zaiane / Eurasian Journal of Social Sciences, 3(1), 2015, 13-19
18
the anchoring bias. Indeed, none of the symptomatic responses of the anchor bias is checked
on the four variables studied5.
5. Conclusion
Human decision making does not seem to conform to rationality and market efficiency, but
exhibits certain behavioral biases that are clearly counter-productive from the financial
perspective.
In this paper, we tested the presence of the anchoring bias on the Tunisian stock
market. For that, we administered a questionnaire to a group of individual investors, to consider
whether they are victims of this bias in their decision making. The results indicate that individual
investors on the Tunisian stock exchange don‟t suffer from the anchoring bias. In fact, they are
not subject to the representativeness and the confirmation biases. Moreover, they don‟t under-
react to new information.
Besides, another interesting study could be made from the same research framework; it
is to test the presence of other psychological biases such as herding, loss aversion and mental
accounting. Further research should also investigate anchoring in the context of an
experimental approach focusing on individual investment (Matsumoto et al. 2013). Also, further
research could examine the relation between anchoring and overconfidence (Heywood-Smith et
al. 2008).
References
Alevy, J.E. Landry C.E. and List, J.A., 2011, Field experiments on anchoring of economic
valuations. University of Alaska Anchorage, Department of Economics, Working Paper,
1-02.
Barberis, N., Shleifer, N., and Vishny, J., 1998. A model of investor sentiment. Journal of
Financial Economics, 49, pp.307-345.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-405X(98)00027-0
Bazerman, M.H., 2004. Judgement in managerial decision making. 3rd Edition. Toronto: John
Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Bergman, O., Ellingsen, T., Johannesson, M. and Svensson, C., 2010, Anchoring and cognitive
ability. Economics Letters, 107 (1), pp.66-68.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2009.12.028
Bernard, V.L. and Abarbanell, J.S., 1992, Tests of analysts' overreaction/underreaction to
earnings information as an explanation for anomalous stock price behavior. Journal of
Finance, 47, pp.1181-1207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.1992.tb04010.x
Chapman, G.B. and Johnson, E.J., 2002. Incorporating the irrelevant: Anchors in judgments of
belief and value. In: T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, and D. Kahneman eds. Heuristics and biases:
The psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press,
pp.120-138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511808098.008
Epley, N., 2004. A tale of tuned decks? Anchoring as adjustment and anchoring as activation. In:
D.J. Koehler and N. Harvey eds. The Blackwell handbook of judgement and decision
making. Oxford: Blackwell, pp.240-256. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470752937.ch12
Cohen, J.B. and Reed, A., 2006. A multiple pathway anchoring and adjustment (MPAA) model
of attitude generation and recruitment. Journal of Consumer Research, 33, pp.1-15.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/504121
Esch, F.R., Schmitt, B.H., and Redler, J., 2009. The brand anchoring effect: A judgment bias
resulting from brand awareness and temporary accessibility. Psychology & Marketing,
26(4), pp.383396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mar.20278
5 We don‟t take into consideration the variable "first idea" because it shows the same percentage (37.6%)
for two terms modalities.
Salma Zaiane / Eurasian Journal of Social Sciences, 3(1), 2015, 13-19
19
Fudenberg, D., Levine, D.K., and Maniadis, Z., 2012. On the robustness of anchoring effects in
WTP and WTA experiments. American Economic Journal: Microeconomics, 4, pp.131-
145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/mic.4.2.131
Furnham, A. and Boo, H.C., 2011 A literature review of the anchoring effect. The Journal of
Socio-Economics, 40, pp.35-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2010.10.008
Heywood-Smith, SA.B., Welsh, M.B., and Begg, S.H., 2008. Cognitive errors in estimation:
Does anchoring cause overconfidence? At the Society of Petroleum Engineers 84th
Annual Technical Conference and Exhibition, Denver, Colorado.
Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A., 1974. Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases.
Science, 185, pp.1124-1131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.185.4157.1124
Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A., 1979. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk.
Econometrica, 47(2), pp.263-293. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1914185
Kahneman, D., 2011. Thinking fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Maniadis, Z., Tufano, F., and List, L.A., 2012. One swallow does not make a summer: How
economists (Mis) use experimental methods and their results. Unpublished Working
Paper.
Mangot, M., 2004. Les comportements en bourse, 6 erreurs psychologiques qui coûtent cher
(Behavioral in stock exchange : 6 psychological errors that cost a lot). Paris: Gualino
Editeur.
Matsumoto, A.S., Fernandes, JL.B., Ferreira, I.K., and Chagas, P.C., 2013, Behavioral finance:
A study of affect heuristic and anchoring in decision making of individual investors.
SSRN Working Paper.
Meub, L. and Till, T., 2014. An experimental study on social anchoring. Cege Discussion Paper,
196 March.
Slovic, P. and Lichtenstein, S., 1971. Comparison of Bayesian and regression approaches to
the study of information processing in judgment. Organ. Behav. Hum. Perform. 6,
pp.649-744. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0030-5073(71)90033-X
Soman, WK.D. and Chattopadhyay, A., 2007. On the perceived value of money: The reference
dependence of currency numerosity effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(1),
pp.1-10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/513041
Simonson, I. and Drolet, A., 2004. Anchoring effects on consumers‟ willingness to pay and
willingness to accept. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(3), pp. 681-690.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/425103
Thomas, M. and Morwitz, V., 2007, Heuristics in numerical cognition: Implications for pricing.
Johnson School Research Paper Series, 1-08.
Tufano, F., 2010. Are true‟ preferences revealed in repeated markets? An experimental
demonstration of context dependent valuations. Experimental Economics, 13(1), pp.1-
13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10683-009-9226-8
... І, мабуть, найбажаніший для держави ефект, якого можна досягнути відносно платників податків -позитивне та раціональне податкове якоріння: формування усталеного погляду та підходів, тобто суспільної думки, що полягає в активній участі в оподаткуванні. Водночас прийняття рішень платником не завжди відповідає раціональності й очікуваній ефективності, виявляючи при цьому певні поведінкові упередження, які, в більшості випадків, є протипродуктивними з фінансової точки зору (Zaiane, 2015). ...
Article
In the light of the COVID-19 pandemic, more and more attention of Ukrainian scientists is drawn to non-traditional factors stimulating the development and harmonization of the tax system, which should include behavioral aspects. The purpose of the article was to outline the latter in the conditions of functioning of the tax system of Ukraine. The influence of behavioral factors was assessed based on the expected results of their activation. It is noted that ignoring the above factors encourages taxpayers to make irrational decisions and violations of tax legislation, which is expressed in tax evasion or avoidance. However, the same aspects can be potentially effective in harmonizing the current tax system. To outline the current behavioral aspects of the impact on taxation in Ukraine, the existing results of the tax system were studied, the basic factors influencing them were identified and the desired qualitative effects in the future were outlined. I claim that сurrently, the tax system of Ukraine is characterized by the predominance in the structure of revenues of the Consolidated Budget of Ukraine of the share of indirect taxes over direct ones. And although at the end of 2020 this figure, compared to 1992, decreased by 3.4%, the inflated tax burden is felt by all taxpayers. In addition, excessive fiscalization of the economy does not promote investment activities that could improve the country's economic climate. Outlining the range of main factors that can be used to adjust the economic behavior of taxpayers, as well as factors that directly affect the taxpayer's decision, made it possible to form a list of active behavioral aspects relevant in modern conditions for harmonization of Ukraine's tax system. Among them, there is the transfer of part of the financial obligations to perform direct functions of the state to the payers themselves, in particular by directing funds from the excise tax on fuel for road repairs and rehabilitation, strengthening equity trends by increasing taxation of capital and luxury goods, setting progressive rates taxes, granting benefits and preferences to certain categories of payers; adjusting the preferences of taxpayers through the level of taxation. Under general condition that the state modifies the tax targets from fiscal to incentive, the result should be a new model of taxpayers' behavior.
... In addition to general-purpose subconscious heuristics (i.e. representativeness, availability, framing and anchoringadjustment), investors (entrepreneurs or venture capitals dealing with an opportunity) sometimes use other cognitive biases such as overconfidence, status quo, the gambler's fallacy and hindsight bias (Rakesh, 2013;Zaiane, 2015;Venkatapathy and Sultana, 2016). The attitudes toward the risk incurred by the investor and several other psychological factors play a key role in the evaluation process. ...
Article
Purpose This study aims to explore the heuristics applied by tech entrepreneurs in the Middle East during the opportunity evaluation process. Design/methodology/approach A multiple case-based methodology was applied, which consisted of semi-structured interviews with entrepreneurial experts from different cities in the Middle East. Qualitative data analysis was then performed with inductive thematic coding using the Eisenhardt method. Findings The results suggest that entrepreneurs mostly use six heuristics to evaluate opportunities quickly. Three of them are related to the opportunity as an abstract idea, and three are connected with the person (s) involved in the opportunity. In addition, entrepreneurs in the Middle East were more interested in the personal characteristics of the opportunity presenter than in the opportunity itself. Research limitations/implications Identifying the heuristics applied by experts may neglect the perspective of the community of entrepreneurs as a whole. Hence, future research should target a wider segment of entrepreneurs. Furthermore, the effect of applying such heuristics on the strategic growth of startups remains an open question. Practical implications The identified heuristics are aligned with the hands-on approach of entrepreneurship and can be applied as a decision-making technique for aspiring entrepreneurs who seek to succeed in this region. Originality/value This study explores the under-examined topic of heuristics in opportunity evaluation within the regional context of the Middle East, which has also been scarcely investigated. It sheds light on the importance of cultural factors in identifying the cognitive shortcuts used in a business context.
... Several studies like Muhammad and Noshaba (2015), Priya and Seema (2015) and Salma (2015) focused on the effect of anchoring bias on any risky investment decision. They highlighted the reasons behind market inadequacy. ...
Conference Paper
In the current capitalist world, several theoretical and conceptual studies have established that man is effortlessly irrational in nature at the time of making investment decisions. Behavioral finance concentrates on how psychological effects can affect market outcomes. Thus, this concept is gaining more significance as it helps in elucidating the difference between the actual behavior and the expectations of a resourceful and rational investor behavior. Therefore, it is important to understand the behavior due to the uncertainty in the Indian economy. The present study examines the significance of behavioral finance in filling the gap between the actual decisions and influencing variables by comparing the different heuristics prevailing in India (Bengaluru city) with the help of various types of investors. Moderating effects are used, and technical anomalies are examined based on different heuristic concepts like anchoring and adjustment, herd mentality, overconfidence, outcome bias and self-control bias. The type of investors chosen for this study are conservative, moderate, aggressive, and retired investors. India is chosen as it is a developing country and the economy is prone to misfortunes arising out of the impacts, that is, volatility in the stock market and mispricing of the securities. A statistical study is carried out to evaluate the impact of heuristics on investment decision making and the results show that there is a significant difference between some of the heuristics adopted and the various types of investors' decision making.
... This shows that anchoring has no significant effect on investment decision, thus H1 is rejected. This result is consistent with Zaiane (2015). It can be concluded that investors do not make investment decisions based on the first information that is anchored in investors' minds. ...
Article
Full-text available
Gold presents as important role in an investor's portfolio because it provides stability of returns and favorable opportunity to improve investor wealth. Investment decision making depend on intrinsic factor investor behavior. Therefore, this study aims to investigate the impact of behavioral finance toward investment decision in gold instrument. Anchoring, availability bias, information asymmetry, representative bias, and risk aversion are chosen as behavioral finance factors. This study is conducted in Batam City Indonesia with 143 respondents and analysed using multiple linear regression analysis. Findings reveal behavioral factors such as information asymmetry and availability bias have significant impact on investment decision. However, the other independent variables do not have significant impact on investment decision.
... On this basis, a questionnaire was developed through an extensive review of literature. At this stage, some relevant studies were consulted and questionnaire Kudryavtsev et al. (2013), Nofsinger (2014, Prosad (2014), Ricciardi and Simon (2000), Statman (2014), Tseng (2013) and Zaiane (2015). Excluding the cover page that was used to introduce this research to the respondents, the developed questionnaire was structured into Sections A and B. Section A contains various nominal questions about the respondents' profiles, whereas Section B asks several ordinal questions regarding the influence of five psychological biases investigated in this study. ...
Article
Purpose Guided by several pioneered studies, the purpose of this paper is to comprehensively investigate the investment behaviours of Malaysian retail and institutional investors in an attempt to identify whether the influence of psychological biases is equally applicable to investor divides. Design/methodology/approach The researchers have adopted a quantitative research design by way of survey methodology to obtain data from institutional and retail investors in Malaysia. In addition, the authors have mainly employed second-order measurement invariance analysis to uncover the difference across investor divides. Findings The tests of measurement invariance at the model level indicate an insignificant difference between institutional investors and retail investors. The post hoc test (at the path level) reveals that institutional and retail investors are similar with respect to representative heuristic, overconfidence bias and anchoring bias; though the results also show that they are different with respect to religious bias and herding bias. Research limitations/implications Based on the findings of this study, it is generally not logical to assume that institutional investors completely behave rational during investment decisions. Besides, future researchers are called upon to directly compare the investment decisions of institutional and retail investors with respect to whether the influence of psychological biases is equally applicable to them, particularly on the investigated psychological biases and other psychological biases that are not covered in this study. Originality/value This study has offered insight into whether the influence of psychological biases is equally applicable to institutional and retail investors in Malaysia using second-order measurement invariance analysis. This study is unique in context and the approach it has adopted.
... On this basis, a questionnaire was developed through an extensive review of literature. At this stage, some relevant studies were consulted and questionnaire Kudryavtsev et al. (2013), Nofsinger (2014, Prosad (2014), Ricciardi and Simon (2000), Statman (2014), Tseng (2013) and Zaiane (2015). Excluding the cover page that was used to introduce this research to the respondents, the developed questionnaire was structured into Sections A and B. Section A contains various nominal questions about the respondents' profiles, whereas Section B asks several ordinal questions regarding the influence of five psychological biases investigated in this study. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose This paper aims to ascertain whether halal certification mark, halal brand quality and halal awareness influence Nigerian consumers when making buying decisions. Design/methodology/approach The researchers reflect on the newly collected data to shed light on the above issues from the perspective of Nigerian consumers. To this end, a questionnaire was developed and used to collect data from 282 respondents. The data collected were analyzed using both descriptive and inferential statistics. Findings This study found that halal certification mark and halal brand quality are the most influential factors that contributed to the consumers’ buying decisions in Nigeria. Originality/value Based on the findings of this study, the researchers have argued that more efforts are needed in the area of halal awareness in Nigeria. Similarly, the study argues that halal brand quality should always be held at the esteemed position. Based on the study’s findings, the authors have been able to fill the literature gap, particularly in the context of the Nigerian halal industry.
Article
Full-text available
This study presents evidence towards the existence of behavioural biases while analysing the impact of such biases on the investors’ investment performance in India, the USA, and the UK. Further, the study aims to investigate the moderating role of financial literacy on the relationship between behavioural biases and investment performance. It reveals that behavioural biases like affect heuristic, herding and status quo positively impact the investors’ investment performance amongst Indian investors whereas loss aversion, overconfidence, availability, and representativeness biases positively impact the investors’ investment performance amongst UK investors; and affect heuristic bias amongst USA investors. The findings divulge the negative impact of anchoring bias and mental accounting amongst Indian investors; availability bias, disposition effect, and overconfidence bias amongst USA investors; and affect heuristic and herding amongst UK investors. The study finds that a few of the behavioural biases impact investment performance positively, though less often. Hence investors should be cautious as these biases generally lead to irrational decision-making and might adversely influence the investors’ investment performance in the long run. The study further concludes that financial literacy significantly moderates the relationship between anchoring bias and individuals’ investment performance in India and affect heuristic in the USA. Interestingly, in the UK scenario, no significant moderating impact of financial literacy was found between any of the behavioral biases and investment performance.
Article
Full-text available
Behavioral finance, a field that combines the psychology of investors with economics and other social sciences, has gained huge popularity in the recent past due to the volatility and complexity of the stock market. Therefore, the aim of the study is to review the literature on behavioral finance and suggest areas for future research. To conduct a systematic literature review, research papers published between 1990 and 2021 were considered for the study. They were assessed on different parameters, such as time frame, journal, country, type of data, statistical tool, sampling techniques, behavioral biases, and their impact. Further, the proposed research model was established using Pareto analysis. Behavioral biases and their impact have been identified. Also, the gaps and the future research area have been proposed. Based on the identified biases, a model has been proposed depicting the relationship between behavioral biases and investment decisions. The study will help academicians and future researchers develop an understanding of behavioral finance in different dimensions of the work done in last two decades. Despite the copious literature available on the subject, the study is distinctive in its nature of conducting a literature review using a quality tool, "Pareto Analysis"
Article
Full-text available
The standard investment theory assumes that the investors are all uniform and invest with the same motives. Sentiment among investors can affect the returns of the stocks and the stocks preferred by retail investors are sensitive to even minor changes in investor sentiment (Kumar and Lee). Behavioral Finance provides an alternative explanation for the behavior that traditional economics fails to account for. Not all investors can be expected to react in the same manner to all the biases; some investors may show fewer predispositions towards certain biases. A better understanding of how economic decisions are made will help investors to make more optimal decisions. The research further delves into an investigation of whether the proposed groups of aversion, mental accounting, attitudinal and interpretational biases hold true in reality.
Article
Full-text available
We reexamine the effects of the anchoring manipulation of Ariely, Loewenstein, and Prelec (2003) on the evaluation of common market goods and find very weak anchoring effects. We perform the same manipulation on the evaluation of binary lotteries, and find no anchoring effects at all. This suggests limits on the robustness of anchoring effects. (JEL C91, D12, D44)
Article
The objective of this article is to investigate the presence of affect and anchoring biases in the financial decision making of individual investors. Another parallel objective is to verify whether the gender factor (male and female) or financial knowledge interfere with the presence of these biases. Considering the results of an innovative experiment, we found support to the existence of such biases. It was verified that there was no significant difference regarding men and women with respect to being attached, anchored or attached and anchored to their investments. The women in this sample were slightly more anchored. Finally, it was observed that the knowledge factor interfered marginally in affect, in addition to partially attenuating the anchoring bias.
Article
Many decisions are based on beliefs concerning the likelihood of uncertain events such as the outcome of an election, the guilt of a defendant, or the future value of the dollar. Occasionally, beliefs concerning uncertain events are expressed in numerical form as odds or subjective probabilities. In general, the heuristics are quite useful, but sometimes they lead to severe and systematic errors. The subjective assessment of probability resembles the subjective assessment of physical quantities such as distance or size. These judgments are all based on data of limited validity, which are processed according to heuristic rules. However, the reliance on this rule leads to systematic errors in the estimation of distance. This chapter describes three heuristics that are employed in making judgments under uncertainty. The first is representativeness, which is usually employed when people are asked to judge the probability that an object or event belongs to a class or event. The second is the availability of instances or scenarios, which is often employed when people are asked to assess the frequency of a class or the plausibility of a particular development, and the third is adjustment from an anchor, which is usually employed in numerical prediction when a relevant value is available.
Article
We examine the susceptibility of consumers' willingness-to-pay (WTP) and willingness-to-accept (WTA) judgments to influence by arbitrary anchors. The results of four studies suggest that, although circumstantial differences between buying and selling decisions can lead to systematic differences in susceptibility to anchoring effects, the principles governing the impact of anchoring on WTP and WTA judgments are essentially the same. In particular, robust anchoring effects are observed when the anchor relates to the focal source of uncertainty. Further, WTP and WTA judgments are most susceptible to anchoring (and endowment) effects under uncertainty about the desire to trade. The implications of this research for our understanding of anchoring and endowment effects are discussed.
Article
The anchoring-and-adjustment heuristic has been studied in numerous experimental settings and is increasingly drawn upon to explain systematically biased decisions in economic areas as diverse as auctions, real estate pricing, sports betting and forecasting. In these cases, anchors result from publicly observable and aggregated decisions of other market participants. However, experimental studies have neglected this social dimension by focusing on external, experimenter-provided anchors in purely individualistic settings. We present a novel experimental design with a socially derived anchor, monetary incentives for unbiased decisions and feedback on performance to more accurately implement market conditions. Despite these factors, we find robust effects for the social anchor, an increased bias for higher cognitive load, and only weak learning effects. Finally, a comparison to a neutral, external anchor shows that the social context increases the bias, which we ascribe to conformity pressure. Our results support the assumption that anchoring remains a valid explanation for systematically biased decisions within market contexts.