ArticlePDF Available

Effects of the axis of rotation and primordially solicited limb of high level athletes in a mental rotation task

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

A recent set of studies has investigated the selective effects of particular physical activities that require full-body rotations, such as gymnastics and wrestling (Moreau, Clerc, Mansy-Dannay, & Guerrien, 2012; Steggemann, Engbert, & Weigelt, 2011), and demonstrated that practicing these activities imparts a clear advantage in in-plane body rotation performance. Other athletes, such as handball and soccer players, whose activities do require body rotations may have more experience with in-depth rotations. The present study examined the effect of two components that are differently solicited in sport practices on the mental rotation ability: the rotation axis (in-plane, in-depth) and the predominantly used limb (arms, legs). Handball players, soccer players, and gymnasts were asked to rotate handball and soccer strike images mentally, which were presented in different in-plane and in-depth orientations. The results revealed that handball and soccer players performed the in-depth rotations faster than in-plane rotations; however, the two rotation axes did not differ in gymnasts. In addition, soccer players performed the mental rotations of handball strike images slower. Our findings suggest that the development of mental rotation tasks that involve the major components of a physical activity allows and is necessary for specifying the links between this activity and the mental rotation performance.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Effects of the axis of rotation and primordially
solicited limb of high level athletes in a mental
rotation task
Hamdi Habacha
, Laure Lejeune-Poutrain
1
, Nicolas Margas
1
,
Corinne Molinaro
1
Normandie Université, France, UNICAEN, CesamS, F-14032 Caen, France
article info
Article history:
PsycINFO classification:
2906
Keywords:
Mental body rotation
Physical activity
Embodiment
Limb
Rotation axis
abstract
A recent set of studies has investigated the selective effects of partic-
ular physical activities that require full-body rotations, such as gym-
nastics and wrestling (Moreau, Clerc, Mansy-Dannay, & Guerrien,
2012; Steggemann, Engbert,& Weigelt,2011), and demonstrated that
practicing these activities imparts a clear advantage in in-plane body
rotation performance. Other athletes, such as handball and soccer
players, whose activities do require body rotations may have more
experience with in-depth rotations. The present study examined
the effect of two components that are differently solicited in sport
practices on the mental rotation ability: the rotation axis (in-plane,
in-depth) and the predominantly used limb (arms, legs). Handball
players, soccer players, and gymnasts were asked to rotate handball
and soccer strikeimages mentally, which were presentedin different
in-planeand in-depth orientations.The results revealedthat handball
and soccer players performed the in-depth rotations faster than in-
plane rotations; however, the two rotation axes did not differ in gym-
nasts. In addition, soccer players performed the mental rotations of
handball strikeimages slower. Our findings suggest that the develop-
ment of mental rotation tasks that involve the major componentsof a
physical activity allows and is necessary for specifying the links
between this activity and the mental rotation performance.
Ó2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2014.06.002
0167-9457/Ó2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Corresponding author. Tel.: +33 02 31 56 72 65.
E-mail address: hamdi.habacha@gmail.com (H. Habacha).
1
Tel.: +33 02 31 56 72 65.
Human Movement Science 37 (2014) 58–68
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Human Movement Science
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/humov
1. Introduction
Mental rotation is an important cognitive ability that permits the mental rotation of representa-
tions of two- or three-dimensional objects. In a well-known study, Shepard and Metzler (1971)
asked participants to judge whether two rotated 3D cube figures depicted identical or different
objects and observed that the response time (RT) was linearly proportional to the angle of rotation
from the original position. This result suggested that participants mentally rotated one item in ref-
erence to the other and that the degree of rotation of an object from the original directly correlates
with the length of time required for a participant to judge if the two items are identical or different.
This mental rotation of abstract objects induces an object-based transformation, for which the rela-
tionship between the environment and the egocentric frame of the observer remains fixed, while
each of their relations to the object reference frame are updated (Amorim, Isableu, & Jarraya,
2006; Steggemann et al., 2011). This type of mental transformation classically generates a linear
increase in the RTs as the angular disparity increases (Cooper, 1975; Shepard & Metzler, 1971;
Steggemann et al., 2011). Other mental rotation tasks require egocentric perspective transforma-
tions, for which the relationship between the environment and the object reference frames remains
fixed, while each of their relations to the egocentric reference frame of the observer are updated
(Amorim et al., 2006; Jola & Mast, 2005; Steggemann et al., 2011). In these mental body rotation
tasks, participants are asked to judge the laterality of full human body postures with one arm out-
stretched. In contrast to the mental rotation of abstracts objects, RTs are generally independent of
rotation angle (Wraga, 2003; Wraga, Creem, & Proffitt, 2000; Wraga, Creem-Regehr, & Proffitt,
2004; Zacks, Mires, Tversky, & Hazeltine, 2002). Some studies of mental body rotations showed con-
stant RTs at low angles but a sudden increase with greater angles (Graf, 1994; Keehner, Guerin,
Miller, Turk, & Hegarty, 2006; Kozhevnikov & Hegarty, 2001; Michelon & Zacks, 2006). These find-
ings show that the classical linear increase in the RTs with increasing rotation angles is not inevi-
table in mental body rotation tasks; even if some studies showed a linear increase in the RTs
with increasing rotation angles, the slopes were less steep than those reported for abstract objects
rotation (Easton & Sholl, 1995; Parsons, 1987; Rieser, 1989).
The above-mentioned differences in RT patterns depend on the spatial frames of reference involved
in the mental rotation processes, i.e., object-related vs. egocentric frames of reference (Preuss, Harris,
& Mast, 2013; Zacks et al., 2002). Using human body postures, Parsons (1987) showed a correlation
between the time required for participants to imagine themselves in the position of the human-body
stimulus and the time required to make a handedness judgment of the same figure during a mental
body rotation task. He proposed that the participants performed the task by imagining themselves
in the position of the body figure (i.e., egocentric perspective transformation). Therefore, this egocen-
tric mental transformation would require embodied transformations and more specifically, the map-
ping of body axes (head–feet, front–back, and left–right) onto the stimuli defined as a spatial
embodiment prior to performing the mental perspective transformation, which is defined as motoric
embodiment (Amorim et al., 2006; Jola & Mast, 2005; Parsons, 1987; Steggemann et al., 2011). Thus,
this embodied process relies on one’s long-term knowledge of body structure (Amorim et al., 2006)
and can be modulated by the motor processes because mental rotation rates are slower for impossible
body postures compared to possible ones (Petit & Harris, 2005).
The influence of motor processes on the mental rotation performance is another noteworthy
issue. The first study to investigate the relationship between motor processes and mental rotation
was proposed by Wexler, Kosslyn, and Berthoz (1998), who asked participants to simultaneously
perform manual and mental rotations of a joystick and observed the slower rotation times when
manual and mental rotations were performed in opposite directions compared to when the rotation
directions were identical. In accordance with the authors’ hypothesis, this result suggests that men-
tal rotation processes represent covert motor rotation. Similarly, Wohlschläger and Wohlschläger’s
research (1998) demonstrated that concordant directions in simultaneous manual and mental
rotations facilitated mental rotation; conversely, discordant rotation directions inhibited mental
rotation. These results suggest a common process that controls both overt and covert object
reorientation dynamics.
H. Habacha et al. / Human Movement Science 37 (2014) 58–68 59
The influence of motor tasks on mental rotation encouraged some authors to investigate the effect
of a wider motor activity (i.e., physical activity) on mental rotation performance. A recent study by
Pietsch and Jansen (2012) reported that students who study sports and athletics exhibit better mental
rotation abilities than students in science education. In addition, physical activity, such as juggling,
was found to enhance performances in a mental object rotation task (Jansen, Titze, & Heil, 2009).
Undergraduate students performed better in a mental object rotation task after 10 months of training
that included highly coordinated and rotational movements, such as wrestling, than after a period of
training of the same duration that did not include these movements, such as running (Moreau et al.,
2012). Taken together, these studies revealed that training or the practice of physical activities
influences mental rotation performance.
Further studies focused on the effect of an extended practice of physical activities that involve full
body rotations by investigating the mental body rotation performances among rotational experts.
Ozel, Larue, and Molinaro (2002) first compared the mental rotation performances of experts in phys-
ical body rotations (i.e., gymnasts) and athletes who rarely execute physical body rotations in their
activity to those of non-athletes. The results showed that the athletes performed better than the
non-athletes, but no difference was observed between the two groups of athletes. This study showed
a general effect of physical practice, but failed to highlight an effect of the frequent execution of phys-
ical body rotations on the mental rotation performances. This shortcoming may be due to the nature of
the task used in their study (i.e., mental object rotation), which required object-based transformations.
The object-based transformation is not an embodied process and does not allow the implementation
of one’s motor expertise during a mental rotation task (Jola & Mast, 2005; Steggemann et al., 2011).
Steggemann et al. (2011) highlighted that the selective effect of motor expertise can be observed in
a mental body rotation task that requires egocentric perspective transformations. In their study, gym-
nasts, which were defined as full body rotation experts, outperformed other athletes in a left–right
judgment task (i.e., perspective transformation task) that involved body images that were presented
in 135°and 180°in-plane orientations. This selective effect would illustrate the specific expertise of
gymnasts with extreme in-plane body orientations (Steggemann et al., 2011). However, different
results were obtained in Jola and Mast’s study (2005) for another type of motor experts (i.e., dancers).
In their study, Jola and Mast asked expert dancers and non-experts to solve a mental body rotation
task using body postures rotated in-plane. The RTs and error rates did not significantly differ between
experts and non-experts. The authors suggested that the test stimuli (i.e., line drawings of the human
body rotated in picture plane) would not encourage the use of dancers’ specific expertise, which is the
movement around a different rotational axis (e.g., the longitudinal body axis during pirouettes).
Various physical activities appear to differentially affect the mental rotation abilities. One way to
better understand the interplay between motor expertise and mental rotation is to investigate the
effects of specific components of the physical activity in a mental body rotation task. In the current
study, we focused on the rotation axis and the primordially solicited limb during practice in soccer
and handball players.
In their study, Steggemann et al. (2011) considered handball and soccer players to be non-experts
of rotational movements because their activities do not require in-plane body rotations. While in-
plane orientations beyond 90°are indeed rare in handball and soccer practice, orientations below
90°are quite frequent. In addition, soccer and handball players constantly experience in-depth
rotations because their activities involve various twists in the air that require rotations around the
longitudinal body axis. To our knowledge, only one recent study compared the mental rotation task
performance between soccer players and non-athletes in a task that involved cubes and human body
figures as stimuli (Jansen, Lehmann, & Van Doren, 2012). The results revealed that soccer players per-
formed faster than non-athletes in mental rotation tasks that involved embodiment (i.e., human body
figures). This finding seems to support the idea that egocentric skills are developed in this physical
activity, even if the activity is classically known as an activity with a high exocentric component
(Jansen et al., 2012).
Because practicing movements around one axis may create difficulties in performing the same
movements around another axis (Jola & Mast, 2005) and in-plane rotations and in-depth rotations
partly involve different cognitive processes (ter Horst, van Lier, & Steenbergen, 2010), we
hypothesized that the sensorimotor experience related to the rotation axes that are largely engaged
60 H. Habacha et al. / Human Movement Science 37 (2014) 58–68
in a physical activity could influence the mental rotation performance. Consequently, we investigated
the effect of the rotation axis by comparing performances in a mental rotation task using images of
handball and soccer movements that involved the rotation of two body axes (in-depth and in-plane)
between high level handball players, soccer players and gymnasts.
Another specific feature of soccer and handball that may potentially influence mental rotation is
the extensive use of two different limbs. This feature is often neglected in mental rotation tasks.
Because the mental representation of the body is continuously updated with regard to its position
or movements of body parts (Ionta & Blanke, 2009), handball players are expected to develop an
increased perceptual sensitivity to the movements of their arms, and the same can be expected in
soccer players for movements of their legs. Gymnasts are expected to develop a similar perceptual
sensitivity in their arms and legs because gymnastics involves both of these body parts. As most
mental rotation studies use hand laterality judgments, which is a task that does not take into account
the limbs that are mainly involved in certain physical activities (i.e., as a more exclusive use of the legs
in soccer), we investigated whether the predominant use of the arms or legs influences the mental
rotation performance in a laterality judgment task.
We hypothesized that differences in the performance between different groups of athletes will sug-
gest that these components influence mental rotational processes via sensorimotor experience related
to the physical activity if the mental rotation tasks include components that are specific to the studied
physical activities. More precisely, we expected that handball and soccer players, who are frequently
exposed to in-depth rotations, would judge the in-depth rotated images more quickly than in-plane
rotated images in the present study. We also expected that the amount of time required for gymnasts
to judge in-depth or in-plane rotated images would not significantly differ. In addition, different RT
patterns that depend on the solicited limb during each physical activity will yield information regard-
ing the importance of this component in mental rotation.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
A total of 55 males (M
age
= 22.8 years, SD = 2.9 years) voluntarily participated in the experiment. All
participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Each participant was unaware of the purpose
of the experiment and provided informed consent before being tested.
Three groups of athletes were formed: 19 soccer players, 21 handball players and 15 gymnasts.
Table 1 depicts the mean age and training experience of the three groups. The inclusion criteria for
each group required that participants were currently practicing their activity and that they trained
regularly over the last six years (for at least 8 h per week). In addition, each participant reported
having no practical experience in the other groups’ activity or at the most, only minimal experience
from school lessons. The three groups did not significantly differ in age, F(2,52) = 0.959, p= .390 or
mean training experience, F(2,52) = 1.347, p= .269.
2.2. Stimuli
The stimuli used in the current study included images of handball and soccer strike moves being
executed with either the left or right arms or legs. The images were rotated in eight orientations
(0°,45°,90°, 135°, 180°, 225°, 270°, and 315°) about two axes: in picture plane and in-depth.
Table 1
Mean age and training experience of the three groups of athletes.
Age Training experience
Mean (Years) SD (Years) Mean (Years) SD (Years)
Soccer players 23.6 2.9 12.8 2.7
Handball players 22.3 3.2 12.1 2.8
Gymnasts 22.6 2.6 13.6 2.3
H. Habacha et al. / Human Movement Science 37 (2014) 58–68 61
Images of left and right strike moves were produced using mirror images of each other such that
each left–right pair of stimuli was identical except for the change in position. Accordingly, 64 different
stimuli (2 sports 2 laterality 2 rotation axes 8 orientations) were generated (Fig. 1). The images
were 15 cm in size and presented on the center of a black background. The stimuli were displayed on a
personal computer via proprietary software developed by our laboratory. This software program was
developed to record the accuracy of each response as well as the response time (i.e., from stimulus
onset to button-press).
2.3. Procedure and task
The participants were seated in front of a computer at a distance of 60 cm in darkened and
separate rooms of a gymnasium. For the 20 training trials, the experimenter remained in the
room to ensure that participants followed the instructions and to provide task instructions as
necessary. After training, the experimenter left the room and was absent for the duration of the test
period.
Participants were asked to judge as quickly and accurately as possible whether the left or right arm
was performing the strike move that was presented using their index finger to press either the left or a
right button on the keyboard. The left and right buttons were color-coded and labeled as ‘‘left strike’’
and ‘‘right strike’’. The order of the test trials was randomized using the proprietary software.
Moreover, each orientation was displayed three times but not more than twice successively, such that
no image of the same sport (handball or soccer strike) was presented in more than three consecutive
trials. This approach resulted in a total of 192 trials (64 stimuli three representations of each
orientation). The trials were divided into two test blocks of 96 trials each.
Each trial began with a blank screen presented for 2000 ms, after which a black fixation cross
appeared for 500 ms. After fixation, the test image was presented for a maximum of 5000 ms, and
the next trial began if a response was given.
Fig. 1. Examples of stimuli corresponding to a handball strike and soccer strike rotated over 135°in-plane and in-depth: RS)
Right Strike (right hand outstretched); LS) Left Strike (left hand outstretched).
62 H. Habacha et al. / Human Movement Science 37 (2014) 58–68
2.4. Data analysis
Only data from correct responses were included in the analyses, and RT outliers were excluded
(faster than 300 ms and slower than 3000 ms) according to previous studies of mental rotation using
human body figures (Amorim et al., 2006; Steggemann et al., 2011).
Two ANOVAs were conducted on the reaction time (RTs) and error rates data using the rotation axis
(in-plane, in-depth), sport move (handball strike, soccer strike), and orientation (0°,45°,90°, 135°,
180°) as within-subject factors and sport (handball, soccer, gymnastics) as a between-subject factor.
We computed descriptive data, Bonferroni post hoc tests and deviation contrasts of RTs to test for
linear trends.
3. Results
3.1. Response times (RTs)
The results revealed a significant main effect of orientation on the mental rotation performance,
F(4,208) = 13.816, p< .001,
g
2
= .21. A contrast analysis showed a linear increase in the RTs as the rota-
tion angle increased, F(1,52) = 17.403, p< .001,
g
2
= .25. The two-way interaction between orientation
and rotation axis reached significance, F(4,208) = 14.700, p< .001,
g
2
= .22. Separate contrasts revealed
a significant linear increase in the RTs as the in-plane rotations increased, F(1,52) = 19.239, p< .001,
g
2
= .27. Such a linear trend was absent when the stimuli were rotated in-depth, F(1,52) = 0.730,
p= .397,
g
2
= .01 (Fig. 2).
The sport (i.e., soccer, handball, gymnastic) did not exert a significant effect, F(2,52) = 2.111,
p= .131,
g
2
= .07. As expected, the rotation axis influenced the mental rotation performances,
F(1,52) = 16.868, p< .001,
g
2
= .25, such that shorter RTs were observed for in-depth rotations
(M= 930 ms, SD = 28) than for in-plane rotations (M= 1008 ms, SD = 35). More interestingly, a signif-
icant two-way interaction between the rotation axis and sport was found, F(2,52) = 4.899, p< .05,
g
2
= .16, which revealed that soccer players and handball players performed faster for in-depth rota-
tions than for in-plane rotations, p< .01 and p< .001. The same interaction was not significant for
gymnasts (Fig. 3).
In addition, the sport movement exerted a significant main effect, F(1,52) = 21.057, p< .001,
g
2
= .29. The RTs for soccer strikes (M= 944 ms, SD = 28) were shorter than for handball strikes
(M= 994 ms, SD = 34). The two-way interaction between the factors ‘‘sport move’’ and ‘‘sport’’ was sig-
nificant, F(2,52) = 14.574, p< .001,
g
2
= .36. Soccer players performed faster for soccer strike images
Fig. 2. Response time according to the two-way interaction between orientation and rotation axis.
H. Habacha et al. / Human Movement Science 37 (2014) 58–68 63
than for handball strike images, p< .001. However, this difference was not significant in gymnasts and
handball players (Fig. 4). Moreover, the two-way interaction between ‘‘sport move’’ and the rotation
axis reached significance, F(1,52) = 4.737, p< .05,
g
2
= .08. Post hoc tests revealed that the significant
difference in the RTs between soccer strikes and handball strikes was greater for in-plane rotations
than for in-depth rotations.
The two-way interaction between the sport movement and orientation reached significance,
F(4,208) = 10.183, p< .001,
g
2
= .16. Separate contrasts revealed a linear increase in the RTs as the
rotation angles of the soccer strike stimuli, F(1,52) = 9.730, p= .01,
g
2
= .16, and the handball strike
stimuli, F(1,52) = 20.445, p< .001,
g
2
= .28, increased. However, the handball strike stimuli showed
steeper increases in the RTs for increasing rotation angles than soccer strike stimuli, t(54) = 3.744,
p< .001.
3.2. Error rates
The error rates were included in the analyses to quantify the response accuracy. A significant
effect of the orientation was observed, F(4,204) = 7.432, p< .001,
g
2
= .13. A contrast analysis
Fig. 3. Response time according to the two-way interaction between rotation axis and sport.
Fig. 4. Response time according to the two-way interaction between sport move and sport.
64 H. Habacha et al. / Human Movement Science 37 (2014) 58–68
showed a linear increase in the error rates as the rotation angle increased, F(1,52) = 9.844, p< .01,
g
2
= .16. The interaction between the orientation and rotation axis reached significance,
F(4,204) = 9.529, p< .001,
g
2
= .16. Separate contrasts revealed a significant linear increase in the
error rates as the in-plane rotations increased, F(1,52) = 20.564, p< .001,
g
2
= .28. Such a linear
increase was not observed when the stimuli were rotated in-depth, F(1,52) = 1.256, p= .268,
g
2
= .024 (Fig. 5).
The main effect of the rotation axis reached significance, F(1,52) = 21.510, p< .001,
g
2
= .30.
In-plane rotations were significantly more error prone (M= 4.63%) compared to in-depth rotations
(M= 2.74%). More interestingly, the interaction between the rotation axis and sport was significant,
F(2,51) = 5.213, p< .01,
g
2
= .17. Post hoc tests revealed that handball and soccer players committed
significantly more errors for in-plane rotations than for in-depth rotations, p< .01, while the error
rates of gymnasts were equal for in-plane and in-depth rotations (Fig. 6). In addition, handball players
committed more errors than gymnasts for in-plane rotations, p< .05, and soccer players also
committed more errors than gymnasts for in-plane rotations but the difference was not significant,
p= .063.
Fig. 5. Error rates according to the two-way interaction between orientation and rotation axis.
Fig. 6. Error rates according to the two-way interaction between rotation axis and sport.
H. Habacha et al. / Human Movement Science 37 (2014) 58–68 65
4. Discussion
In the present study, we investigated the effect of the specific components of motor expertise on
the mental rotation ability by studying the influence of the rotation-axis and the limb predominantly
involved during a mental body rotation task. To address this aim, handball players, soccer players and
gymnasts, whose physical activity differently involved these components, performed a mental rota-
tion task using full body posture stimuli that depicted handball strike moves (arm movement) and
soccer strike moves (leg movement) that were rotated over different in-plane and in-depth
orientations.
The results showed a linear trend of the RTs and error rates as a function of the rotation angle. As
mentioned previously, the literature did not show consistent RT and error rate patterns in perspective
transformation tasks contrary to object-based transformation tasks. Interestingly, the RTs and error
rates for in-plane rotations, but not for in-depth rotations, showed a linear trend, suggesting different
mental transformations according to the rotation axis. The in-plane rotations revealed the classical
linear increase in the RTs and error rates. We suspect that perspective transformations to adopt
in-plane body orientations rely mainly on mental rotation processes (Parsons, 1987; Wraga, 2003;
Wraga et al., 2000, 2004; Zacks et al., 2002). However, the RT and error rate patterns for in-depth rota-
tions were clearly distinguishable from the classical linear increase reported for object rotation, which
suggests that the perspective transformation to adopt in-depth orientations relies more on embodied
perspective transformations (Creem et al., 2001; Parsons, 1987; Zacks et al., 2002). However, the
preference for different strategies according to the rotation axis cannot be completely specified based
on the current findings and thus warrants further investigation.
The mental rotations were faster for in-depth-rotated stimuli than for in-plane-rotated stimuli.
This result is consistent with the experience gained both in everyday life and during physical practice
because body rotations around the longitudinal body axis (in-depth) are more frequent than rotations
over the sagittal body axis (in-plane). Aligning the mental representation of one’s body with the in-
depth rotated stimuli leads to faster mental rotation because the participant may perform a simple
rotation over the longitudinal body axis until it is aligned with the stimulus or may only drag the men-
tal image of his body forward to align it with the largest orientations, corresponding to back views of
the body. In-depth rotations would thus rely more on perspective translation, which is easier and
requires less processing time than perspective rotation (Creem-Regehr, 2003; Rieser, 1989). Aligning
one’s mental body image with a human figure rotated in-plane necessitated additional rotations over
the sagittal body axes and therefore required more time to be completed.
We expected mental rotation processes to be influenced by the specific components of given phys-
ical activities, such as the frequency of body rotations around specific axes. We hypothesized that soc-
cer and handball players will perform better using in-depth-rotated stimuli and that gymnasts would
perform equally well in in-plane and in-depth rotations because they execute movements around
both body axes over a wide range of orientations, including upside-down body positions. The results
confirmed this hypothesis because soccer and handball players performed in-depth rotations faster
than in-plane rotations and no difference was observed between the two ration axes for gymnasts.
This finding supports a close relationship between the physical movement and mental execution
(Decety, Jeannerod, & Prablanc, 1989; Decety & Michel, 1989; Gerardin et al., 2000; Jeannerod,
2001; Roland, Skinhøj, Lassen, & Larsen, 1980; Stephan et al., 1995) and highlights that the specific
components of a physical activity influence the mental rotation performance. However, contrary to
the study by Steggemann et al. (2011), the group of gymnasts was not significantly faster than the
other athletes (i.e., handball and soccer players) during in-plane rotations. Given that soccer and
handball players committed more errors for in-plane rotations than gymnasts, the most plausible
explanation is that soccer and handball players used other strategies based on the visual appearance
of the stimuli (Jola & Mast, 2005) or engaged fast guesses leading to similar RTs than those of gymnasts
while generating more errors. Another explanation relies on the fact that some activities in the
non-expert group studied by Steggemann and colleagues (track and field, rowing, and swimming)
developed poorer spatial abilities than handball and soccer.
66 H. Habacha et al. / Human Movement Science 37 (2014) 58–68
The type of stimulus also influences the mental rotation performances: mental rotations were
slower for handball strike moves than for soccer strike moves. More importantly, among the three
groups of athletes, only soccer players performed significantly slower for handball strike move. This
finding partially supports our hypothesis regarding the specific effects of physical activity on mental
rotation, because the least use of arms in soccer players appears to influence their mental rotation
performance. Indeed, a mental rotation task that requires the athlete to mentally adopt a full body
posture, including arm movements, may be difficult for soccer players. Despite a slight difference,
handball players did not perform slower for soccer strike moves than handball strike moves. This find-
ing may be explained by the fact that handball players use their legs to defend or perform technical
gestures, such as to feint to dodge the opponent, and these movements may provide a certain type
of expertise with leg movements. As expected, gymnasts performed the two types of strike moves
equally well because gymnastics involve the movement of both the arms and legs.
Independent of sport, the current study allowed the investigation of the influence of additional
unaddressed stimuli features and their combination on the mental body rotation performances in a
single study. To our knowledge, previous studies have not investigated the influence of human body
orientations ranging from 0 degrees to 180 degrees around two rotation axes on the mental body rota-
tion ability in the same participants. The results showed that the mental rotation of handball strikes
required more time than that of soccer strikes, especially for in-plane rotations. Previous studies have
demonstrated that RT patterns change as a result of modifying the participant’s posture (de Lange,
Helmich, & Toni, 2006; Helmich, de Lange, Bloem, & Toni, 2007; Ionta & Blanke, 2009; Ionta,
Fourkas, Fiorio, & Aglioti, 2007; Parsons, 1994; Sirigu & Duhamel, 2001). In the current study, aligning
the mental image of one’s own body with the handball strike figure may require simulated hand
movements, which are subject to increased biomechanical constraints because the hand is placed
on the keyboard. Thus, transformations around more than one body axis are necessary to align the
hand with that of the handball figure. However, the alignment of the mental image of one’s leg with
that of the stimulus figure may require a simple transformation and thus less time.
The present study confirms a relationship between the mental imagery of movement and skill.
More specifically, it demonstrated that some important components of a physical activity (such as
the rotation axis and limb movements) influence the mental rotation performance. While the body
rotation axes clearly influenced the mental rotation performance, the influence of the predominant
limb was partially evidenced in the present study. Indeed, the mental rotations of human body figures
with an outstretched arm may cause difficulties for soccer players. In addition, the similar RTs
observed in handball players for handball and soccer moves suggests that arm and leg movements
may be more important in handball practice than initially supposed. Other sports that require the
excessive use of one limb would be interesting to study to assess the contribution of the limb to men-
tal rotation. To establish further the links between specific motor skills and mental rotation in sports,
an accurate description of the physical activity and the development of mental rotation tasks that
involve manipulation of the major components of this physical activity are needed.
References
Amorim, M. A., Isableu, B., & Jarraya, M. (2006). Embodied spatial transformations: ‘‘Body analogy’’ for the mental rotation of
objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 327–347.
Cooper, L. A. (1975). Mental rotation of random two-dimensional shapes. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 20–43.
Creem, S. H., Downs, T. H., Wraga, M., Harrington, G. S., Proffitt, D. R., & Downs, J. H. (2001). An fMRI study of imagined
self-rotation. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 1, 239–249.
Creem-Regehr, S. H. (2003). Updating space during imagined self and array translations. Memory and Cognition, 31, 941–952.
Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Prablanc, C. (1989). The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioural Brain Research, 34,
35–42.
Decety, J., & Michel, F. (1989). Comparative analysis of actual and mental movement times in two graphic tasks. Brain and
Cognition, 11, 87–97.
de Lange, F. P., Helmich, R. C., & Toni, I. (2006). Posture influences motor imagery: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 33, 609–617.
Easton, R. D., & Sholl, M. J. (1995). Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 483–500.
Gerardin, E., Sirigu, A., Lehericy, S., Poline, J. B., Gaymard, B., Marsault, C., et al (2000). Partially overlapping neural networks for
real and imagined hand movements. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 1093–1104.
Graf, R. (1994). Self-rotation and spatial reference: The psychology of partner-centred localisations. Frankfurt, Germany: Peter Lang.
H. Habacha et al. / Human Movement Science 37 (2014) 58–68 67
Helmich, R. C., de Lange, F. P., Bloem, B. R., & Toni, I. (2007). Cerebral compensation during motor imagery in Parkinson’s disease.
Neuropsychologia, 45, 2201–2215.
Ionta, S., Fourkas, A. D., Fiorio, M., & Aglioti, S. M. (2007). The influence of hands posture on mental rotation of hands and feet.
Experimental Brain Research, 183, 1–7.
Ionta, S., & Blanke, O. (2009). Differential influence of hands posture on mental rotation of hands and feet in left and right
handers. Experimental Brain Research, 195, 207–217.
Jansen, P., Titze, C., & Heil, M. (2009). The influence of juggling on mental rotation performance. International Journal of Sport
Psychology, 40, 351–359.
Jansen, P., Lehmann, J., & Van Doren, J. (2012). Mental rotation performance in male soccer players. PLoS One, 7, e48620.
Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14, S103–S109.
Jola, C., & Mast, F. W. (2005). Mental object rotation and egocentric body rotation: Two dissociable processes? Spatial Cognition
and Computation, 5, 217–237.
Keehner, M., Guerin, S. A., Miller, M. B., Turk, D. J., & Hegarty, M. (2006). Modulation of neural activity by angle of rotation during
imagined spatial transformations. NeuroImage, 33, 391–398.
Kozhevnikov, M., & Hegarty, M. (2001). A dissociation between object manipulation spatial ability and spatial orientation
ability. Memory & Cognition, 29, 745–756.
Moreau, D., Clerc, J., Mansy-Dannay, A., & Guerrien, A. (2012). Enhancing spatial ability through sport practice: Evidence for an
effect of motor training on mental rotation performance. Journal of Individual Differences, 33, 83–88.
Michelon, P., & Zacks, J. M. (2006). Two kinds of visual perspective taking. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 327–337.
Ozel, S., Larue, J., & Molinaro, C. (2002). Relation between sport activity and mental rotation: Comparison of three groups of
subjects. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 95, 1141–1154.
Parsons, L. M. (1987). Imagined spatial transformation of one’s body. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 172–191.
Parsons, L. M. (1994). Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 709–730.
Petit, L. S., & Harris, I. M. (2005). Anatomical limitations in mental transformations of body parts. Visual Cognition, 12, 737–758.
Pietsch, S., & Jansen, P. (2012). Different mental rotation performance in students of music, sports and education science.
Learning and Individual Differences, 2, 159–163.
Preuss, N., Harris, L., & Mast, F. W. (2013). Allocentric visual cues influence mental transformation of bodies. Journal of Vision, 13,
1–10.
Rieser, J. J. (1989). Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1157–1165.
Roland, P. E., Skinhøj, E., Lassen, N. A., & Larsen, B. (1980). Different cortical areas in man in organization of voluntary
movements in extrapersonal space. Journal of Neurophysiology, 43, 137–150.
Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–703.
Sirigu, A., & Duhamel, J. R. (2001). Motor and visual imagery as two complementary but neurally dissociable mental processes.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 910–919.
Steggemann, Y., Engbert, K., & Weigelt, M. (2011). Selective effects of motor expertise in mental body rotation tasks: Comparing
object-based and perspective transformations. Brain and Cognition, 76, 97–105.
Stephan, K. M., Fink, G. R., Passingham, R. E., Silbersweig, D., Ceballos-Baumann, A. O., Frith, C. D., et al (1995). Functional
anatomy of the mental representation of upper extremity movements in healthy subjects. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73,
373–386.
ter Horst, A. C., van Lier, R., & Steenbergen, B. (2010). Mental rotation task of hands: Differential influence number of rotational
axes. Experimental Brain Research, 203, 347–354.
Wexler, M., Kosslyn, S., & Berthoz, A. (1998). Motor processes in mental rotation. Cognition, 68, 77–94.
Wohlschläger, A., & Wohlschläger, A. (1998). Mental and manual rotation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
and Performance, 24, 397–412.
Wraga, M. (2003). Thinking outside the body: An advantage for spatial updating during imagined versus physical self-rotation.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 993–1005.
Wraga, M., Creem, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2000). Updating displays after imagined object and viewer rotations. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 151–168.
Wraga, M., Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2004). Spatial updating of virtual displays during self- and display rotation.
Memory & Cognition, 32, 399–415.
Zacks, J. M., Mires, J., Tversky, B., & Hazeltine, E. (2002). Mental spatial transformation of objects and perspective. Spatial
Cognition and Computation, 2, 315–332.
68 H. Habacha et al. / Human Movement Science 37 (2014) 58–68
... Research has demonstrated that individuals with motor expertise have an advantage of this very expertise when performing mental rotation tasks (Jansen and Lehmann, 2013;Habacha et al., 2014Habacha et al., , 2022Schmidt et al., 2016;Weigelt and Memmert, 2021;Kamruddin, 2022). For instance, Habacha et al. (2014) conducted a study comparing table tennis players, who frequently perform and observe rapid hand movements, with soccer players, who lack experts in hand movements, in a mental rotation task using their hands. ...
... Research has demonstrated that individuals with motor expertise have an advantage of this very expertise when performing mental rotation tasks (Jansen and Lehmann, 2013;Habacha et al., 2014Habacha et al., , 2022Schmidt et al., 2016;Weigelt and Memmert, 2021;Kamruddin, 2022). For instance, Habacha et al. (2014) conducted a study comparing table tennis players, who frequently perform and observe rapid hand movements, with soccer players, who lack experts in hand movements, in a mental rotation task using their hands. The authors found that table tennis players exhibited faster mental rotation of their hands and had lower response times for object-based transformations. ...
... This familiarity potentially enhances performance on egocentric mental rotation tasks. Habacha et al. (2014) discovered a selective effect of motor expertise, as evidenced by the superior performance of sports experts. This was attributed to the increased embodiment of spatial transformations, as indicated by a higher correlation between the mental rotation task and the sports situation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and objectives The experience obtained from motor expertise may contribute to and enhance the development of particular visuo-spatial abilities. This exploratory and preliminary study compares the response times of a mental rotation task with egocentric and object-based transformation instructions between soccer players of varying performance levels and gymnasts. Methods Fifty-six male participants were grouped based on their sports experience. Soccer-specific novices (SS-N: n = 19; age = 15.9 ± 0.87), soccer-specific experts (SS-E: n = 17; age = 16.4 ± 0.70), gymnastic-specific experts (GS-E: n = 10; age = 16.6 ± 1.71), and gymnastic-specific novices (GS-N: n = 10; age = 16.0 ± 1.63) were recruited to perform a perceptual task (recognition of soccer-specific poses) and mental rotation tasks with different stimuli (soccer-specific poses, cubes, line-drawings of hands, letters). Results During the perceptual task with instructions on egocentric transformation and soccer-specific poses, we observed that gymnasts had longer response times than soccer players. Our findings also suggest that experts correctly identified most of the poses in terms of accuracy. In the mental rotation task with object-based transformation, gymnasts processed all stimuli, even the soccer-specific poses, more accurately than both soccer groups. Conclusion Our results suggest that gymnasts’ motor expertise plays a role in their performance on mental rotation tasks involving both egocentric and object-based transformations, regardless of the stimuli presented.
... However, this is not the case for other physical activities; the covert transfer of motor strategies is dependent on the movement-related specificities of motor experience (Habacha, Lejeune-Poutrain, Margas & Molinaro, 2014b). That is, other motor experts could apprehend mental rotation tasks differently, compared to wrestlers; their specific experience should encourage them to use their favorite strategies developed through years of practice leading to selective effects on mental rotation performance (Habacha et al., 2014b;Habacha, Lejeune-Poutrain & Molinaro, 2017;Habacha, Molinaro, Tabben & Lejeune-Poutrain, 2014c). ...
... However, this is not the case for other physical activities; the covert transfer of motor strategies is dependent on the movement-related specificities of motor experience (Habacha, Lejeune-Poutrain, Margas & Molinaro, 2014b). That is, other motor experts could apprehend mental rotation tasks differently, compared to wrestlers; their specific experience should encourage them to use their favorite strategies developed through years of practice leading to selective effects on mental rotation performance (Habacha et al., 2014b;Habacha, Lejeune-Poutrain & Molinaro, 2017;Habacha, Molinaro, Tabben & Lejeune-Poutrain, 2014c). For example, gymnasts do not manipulate external "objects" (i.e., opponents) during practice and, unlike wrestlers, they plan future actions without taking into account experience with manipulation of external objects. ...
... In addition to confirming the effect of wrestling expertise on the mental manipulation of abstract objects (Moreau, 2012(Moreau, , 2013Moreau et al., 2011), the present findings confirm our hypothesis that only a sensorimotor experience specific to hand manipulation of external "objects" will imply the use of motor-based strategy in mental rotation of abstract objects. This specific covert transfer of motor strategies is in accordance with the findings of Habacha and colleagues, suggesting that motor experts do not apprehend spatial tasks similarly; the transfer of their sensorimotor experience is movement-related and is dependent on the tasks' context and stimuli (Habacha et al., 2014b(Habacha et al., , 2017(Habacha et al., , 2014a. Even if gymnasts are experts in mental rotation, the effect of gymnastics experience seems to be body-movement related. ...
Article
The involvement of motor processes in mental rotation is experience-dependent: different levels of expertise in sensorimotor interactions lead to different strategies in mental rotation. In the present study, wrestlers, gymnasts, and nonathletes physically rotated objects that were either light (wooden) or heavy (lead) but otherwise having the same sizes and shapes. They then performed a mental rotation task using photographs of these objects in which the material and therefore the weight was visible. I hypothesized that wrestlers would rely more heavily on experience-based sensorimotor strategies in performing mental rotation because during their athletic practice they not only manipulate external “objects” (i.e., their opponent) but also have to plan future actions taking into account past experience of these “objects” (for example their weight). All participants reported that lead objects were harder to physically rotate than wooden ones. However, only wrestlers mentally rotated lead objects more slowly than wooden ones—as they would if they were physically rotating them—suggesting the involvement of motor processes. These findings show that the involvement of motor processes in mental rotation depends on specific rather than mere sensorimotor experience.
... More recently, a body of research highlighted selective effects of sport practice on embodied mental rotation performance (Habacha, Lejeune-Poutrain, & Molinaro, 2017;Steggemann, Engbert, & Weigelt, 2011). That is, athletes practicing different sports involving different movements seem to use different strategies to solve the same mental rotation task (Habacha, Lejeune-Poutrain, Margas, & Molinaro, 2014;Steggemann et al., 2011). Their sensorimotor experience is movement related and shapes the way they apprehend these tasks. ...
... In addition, participants in mental body rotation tasks classically chose the shortest path to align their body representation with the stimuli (Parsons, 1987;Zartor, Mikheev, & Afanasiev, 2010). Thus, mental rotation studies using a mental body rotation task usually computed mean RTs of angular disparities for which the shortest rotation path between stimulus and target is the same (e.g., 45°-315°, 90°-270°, 135°-225°) (Habacha, Lejeune-Poutrain, et al., 2014;Habacha et al., 2017;Steggemann et al., 2011). Accordingly, the 135° and the 225° used in the present study represent similar rotation paths or amounts (e.g., not increasing), explaining the flat RT means as a function of rotation angle. ...
Article
The purpose of the present study was to examine the effect of different levels of exercise intensity on mental rotation performance in gymnasts versus nongymnasts. A group of elite gymnasts and a group of nongymnasts performed a mental body rotation task at rest and then performed the same task preceded by short bouts of intense exercise at 60%, 80%, 100%, and 120% of their maximum aerobic speed. The analyses of response times showed that gymnasts performed the mental rotation task faster after bouts of intense exercise than in rest condition, but nongymnasts performed equally in rest and after exercise. This finding highlights the specific physical expertise as a variable that can affect the influence of exercise on cognitive processing.
... During the test, the mean correct rate and reaction time from presentation of the pictures to pushing of the buttons were recorded. Test-retest reliability is 0.91 [32][33][34] (Figure 1). Faster reaction times and more correct responses in the Mental Rotation test represent better spatio-temporal abilities [32][33][34]. ...
... Test-retest reliability is 0.91 [32][33][34] (Figure 1). Faster reaction times and more correct responses in the Mental Rotation test represent better spatio-temporal abilities [32][33][34]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The esports industry is increasing in popularity and is now played at the professional level. We hypothesized that esports players may have a significant advantage over the general population in terms of psychological and cognitive characteristics, which may be similar to that of professional baseball players. We recruited three participant groups: esports players (n = 55), pro-baseball players (n = 57), and age- and sex-matched healthy comparison subjects (n = 60). We assessed psychological status using the Korean versions of Temperament and Character Inventory and State and Trait Anxiety Inventory and cognitive functions using the modified Tower of London, Emotional Perception, and Mental Rotation tests. Esports players had similar psychological characteristics to pro-baseball players (higher novelty seeking [p < 0.01 *, ŋ = 0.818], self-directedness [p < 0.01 *, ŋ = 0.757], and self-transcendence scores [p < 0.01 *, ŋ = 0.853], and decreased state anxiety scores [p < 0.01 *, ŋ = 0.808]), which differed from those of the general population. However, esports players showed higher working memory [p < 0.01 *, ŋ = 0.823] and slower emotional perception than pro-baseball players [p < 0.01 *, ŋ = 0.812]. In conclusion, esports and pro-baseball players had similar psychological but different cognitive characteristics.
... Contemporary cognitive science research on mental rotation continuously provides insights into the underlying processes of spatial perception and representational manipulation [4], revealing noteworthy associations between mental rotation and proficiencies in diverse domains. Notably, mental rotation ability has demonstrated its potential as a precise predictor of athletic performance [5]. Moreover, students' visuospatial abilities, encompassing mental rotation, have been found to facilitate cognitive operations in spatial contexts, thereby contributing to academic achievements in subjects such as mathematics and chemistry [6][7][8]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mental rotation tasks have been widely used to assess individuals’ spatial cognition and the ability to mentally manipulate objects. This study employed a computerized adaptive training method to investigate the behavioral performance of participants of different genders in mental rotation tasks with different rotation angles before and after training. A total of 44 Chinese university students participated in the experiment, with the experimental group undergoing a five-day mental rotation training program. During the training phase, a three-down/one-up staircase procedure was used to adjust the stimulus levels (response time) based on participants’ responses. The results showed that the training had a facilitative effect on the mental rotation ability of both male and female participants, and it was able to eliminate the gender differences in mental rotation performance. Regarding the angles, we observed that the improvement in the angles involved in the training was significantly higher compared to untrained angles. However, no significant differences in improvement were found among the three trained angles. In summary, these findings demonstrate the effectiveness of computerized adaptive training methods in improving mental rotation ability and highlight the influence of gender and angles on learning outcomes.
... Even though there is some evidence for better general cognitive skills in elite athletes (Voss, Kramer, Basak, Prakash, & Roberts, 2010), creating experimental tasks and conditions that allow the expert advantage to emerge appears critical (Ericsson & Smith, 1991). Habacha and colleagues took into account this factor with the direct manipulation of stimuli and task conditions and showed that athletes do not perform mental rotation tasks uniformly (Habacha, Lejeune-Poutrain, Margas, & Molinaro, 2014a;Habacha, Lejeune-Poutrain, & Molinaro, 2017;Habacha, Molinaro, Tabben, & Lejeune-Poutrain, 2014b). Specifically, physical skills specific to each sport appear to selectively influence mental rotation strategies and performance. ...
... The interaction effect between PlaneDir and PA showed more homogeneous log RTs from one plane to another in the Art group than in the Sed and in the Fut groups (Fig. 2a), a finding which is consistent with the results of a previous study with only male subjects performing a MRt of left-right decision 30 . Due to the specificities of the PA the Art subjects were probably more accustomed to body rotations in the frontal and sagittal planes than the Sed and the Fut subjects, while each of the Art, Sed and Fut subjects had a long-term experience of such rotations in the horizontal plane. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mental rotation (MR) is a spatial skill considered to be a key-component of intellectual ability. Studies have suggested that the response time (RT) in a MR task (MRt) might be influenced, with possible gender differences, by the practice of a physical activity (PA) and depending on the plane, direction, degrees of the MR and the frame of reference to perform it. The present study aimed at examining the respective influences of all these variables on the RT by developing a linear mixed-effect model from the RTs varying according to the MR plane, direction, degrees and frame of reference. The MRt was performed by 96 males and females, all undergraduate students, distributed in three groups (sedentary subjects, artistic gymnasts, and futsal players). The results showed that only gender had a main effect (faster log RT in males), probably task-dependent. The other variables interacted among them showing that: (a) the log RT may be influenced by rotations experienced during PA, in particular during the locomotion on a horizontal ground and (b) such influence mainly depends on the compatibility of the physical rotations experienced with the plane and the degrees of the MRt.
... Les tâches à réaliser (reproduire les mouvements d'un avatar) peuvent en outre susciter une identification à l'avatar favorisant une stratégie motrice. Pour autant, des travaux ont montré que la pratique d'activités physiques sollicitant les capacités spatiales, e.g., la lutte, suscite un progrès significatif dans des tâches de rotation mentale appelant à comparer des structures à base de cubes (e.g., Habacha, Lejeune-Poutrain, Margas & Molinaro, 2014). On dispose en outre de résultats indiquant qu'il pourrait en être de même de la pratique de la danse (e.g., Jansen, Kellner & Rieder, 2013) 21 . ...
Article
Full-text available
Haute École Pédagogique du canton de Vaud Mots clés : jeux vidéo actifs, plateforme Play Lü ® , éducation physique, mathématiques Résumé : Des travaux scientifiques ont mis en exergue des liens étroits entre le mouvement humain et la cognition numérique. Ils plaident ainsi en faveur de la mise en place, à l'école, de croisements entre éducation physique et mathématiques. L'émergence de jeux vidéo actifs pourrait favoriser de tels croisements, possibilité que le présent article envisage dans le cas de la plateforme Play Lü ® , en appui sur la littérature quant aux relations entre arithmétique, rotation mentale et motricité.
... It has a test-retest reliability of 0.79 (Figure 1). Faster reaction times and more correct responses in the mental rotation test represent better spatiotemporal visual attention (32). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Complex post-traumatic stress disorder (C-PTSD) is characterized by the typical symptoms of PTSD, in addition to affective dysregulation, negative self-concept, and disturbances in interpersonal relationships. Children and adolescents with C-PTSD have been reported to have deficits in emotional and cognitive functions. We hypothesized that the following are associated with the severity of C-PTSD symptoms: (1) adolescents with C-PTSD who show deficits in emotional perception and cognitive functions, including executive function and attention; and (2) deficits in neurocognitive functions. Methods: Information on 69 adolescents with PTSD, aged 10–19 years, was gathered from seven shelters. All participants were assessed using complete clinical scales, including the C-PTSD Interview and Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scales, and neurocognitive function tests, including the emotional perception, mental rotation, and modified Tower of London tests. Results: Adolescents with C-PTSD were more likely to have a history of sexual assault, dissociation, and self-harm than those with PTSD. The total and subscale scores of the C-PTSD Interview Scale in adolescents with C-PTSD were higher than that in adolescents with PTSD. In addition, neurocognitive functions, including emotional perception, attention, and working memory, were correlated with the severity of C-PTSD symptoms. Discussion: Adolescents with C-PTSD experienced more serious clinical symptoms and showed more deficits in neurocognitive functions than adolescents with PTSD. Clinicians should pay careful attention toward the emotional and neurocognitive functions when assessing and treating patients with C-PTSD.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Aerobic exercise may be associated with changes in brain activity within the default mode network (DMN) and dorsal attention network (DAN). We hypothesized that changes in functional connectivity (FC) within the DMN and DAN might be most effectively activated by moderate-intensity exercise. Methods: Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging scans and visuospatial attention tests after resting were performed before and after each of moderate- and high-intensity aerobic exercises (10 min each) in 15 healthy male volunteers. Results: The reaction time during the attention test increased significantly, and the rate of correct responses decreased from moderate-intensity exercise condition to high-intensity exercise condition. FC within the DMN under high-intensity exercise condition was higher than that under pre-exercise and moderate-intensity exercise conditions. FC within the DAN under moderate-intensity exercise condition was the highest, whereas FC between the DMN and DAN under moderate-intensity exercise condition was the lowest. Changes in cognitive domain functions were associated with changes in FC between the DMN and DAN. Conclusion: Our results support the inverted-U hypothesis of maximum arousal efficacy during moderate exercise. Both cognitive domains, namely, the attention system and brain activity domains, may be better under moderate-intensity exercise than under high-intensity exercise.
Article
Full-text available
The influence of juggling on mental rotation performance in children Study aim: To assess the influence of juggling training on mental rotation performance in children. Material and methods: Two groups of girls aged 6 - 14 years were studied: experimental (EG; n = 26) and control (CG; n = 24). All girls solved a mental rotation task with 3-D block figures on computer screen (pre-test). After the initial test, EG girls participated in juggling training for 3 months; the CG girls participated in light strength training with theraband stretch bands. After 3 months, all girls solved the mental rotation task again (post-test). The post-pre differences in the mental rotation performance were recorded. Results: Children who learned juggling performed the mental rotation task significantly (p<0.05 - 0.01) faster, in terms of reaction time, at non-zero angular disparity than their mates who were strength-trained. Conclusions: Since mental rotation skills enhance spatial imagination, problem solving and mathematical skills, it may be assumed that juggling training enhances also other cognitive domains and is worth implementing in the education process.
Article
Full-text available
Identifying a human body stimulus involves mentally rotating an embodied spatial representation of one's body (motoric embodiment) and projecting it onto the stimulus (spatial embodiment). Interactions between these two processes (spatial and motoric embodiment) may thus reveal cues about the underlying reference frames. The allocentric visual reference frame, and hence the perceived orientation of the body relative to gravity, was modulated using the York Tumbling Room, a fully furnished cubic room with strong directional cues that can be rotated around a participant's roll axis. Sixteen participants were seated upright (relative to gravity) in the Tumbling Room and made judgments about body and hand stimuli that were presented in the frontal plane at orientations of 0°, 90°, 180° (upside down), or 270° relative to them. Body stimuli have an intrinsic visual polarity relative to the environment whereas hands do not. Simultaneously the room was oriented 0°, 90°, 180° (upside down), or 270° relative to gravity resulting in sixteen combinations of orientations. Body stimuli were more accurately identified when room and body stimuli were aligned. However, such congruency did not facilitate identifying hand stimuli. We conclude that static allocentric visual cues can affect embodiment and hence performance in an egocentric mental transformation task. Reaction times to identify either hands or bodies showed no dependence on room orientation.
Article
Full-text available
It is the main goal of this study to investigate the visual-spatial cognition in male soccer players. Forty males (20 soccer players and 20 non-athletes) solved a chronometric mental rotation task with both cubed and embodied figures (human figures, body postures). The results confirm previous results that all participants had a lower mental rotation speed for cube figures compared to embodied figures and a higher error rate for cube figures, but only at angular disparities greater than 90°. It is a new finding that soccer-players showed a faster reaction time for embodied stimuli. Because rotation speed did not differ between soccer-players and non-athletes this finding cannot be attributed to the mental rotation process itself but instead to differences in one of the following processes which are involved in a mental rotation task: the encoding process, the maintanence of readiness, or the motor process. The results are discussed against the background of the influence on longterm physical activity on mental rotation and the context of embodied cognition.
Article
Related perceptual, motor, and cognitive performances were examined to reveal the accuracy of the properties of action spontaneously represented when mentally simulating moving one's hand. The kinematic configuration of the body represented and transformed in mental simulations was not fixed or canonical but corresponded to one's current configuration. Mental simulation time mimicked movement time for natural efficient movement from a posture midway between each of the hand's joint limits into many other postures. Equal time was required for simulated and real movements into more common, comfortable postures; shorter but proportional time was required for simulated movement than real movement into less common postures that involved longer trajectories, coordinated activity at more joints, motion near extremes of joint limits, and uncomfortable kinesthetic sensations. The findings suggest that sensorimotor structures support mental simulations of actions.
Article
The relation between mental and manual rotation was investigated in 2 experiments. Experiment 1 compared the response times (RTs) of mental rotation about 4 axes in space with the RTs shown in the same task when participants were allowed to reorient the stimuli by means of rotational hand movements. For the 3 Cartesian axes, RT functions were quantitatively indistinguishable. Experiment 2 investigated interference between mental rotation and 4 kinds of simultaneously executed hand movements that did not reorient the stimuli. Interference was observed only when axes of manual and mental rotation coincided in space. Regardless of the hand used, concordant rotational directions facilitated, whereas discordant directions inhibited, mental rotation. The results suggest that mental object rotation and rotatory object manipulation share a common process that is thought to control the dynamics of both imagined and actually performed object reorientation.
Article
In this study the effect of long-term physical and musical activity on spatial cognitive performance, measured by mental rotation performance, is investigated in detail. Mental rotation performance is the ability to rotate a three-dimensional object using the imagination. Three groups, each consisting of 40 students, and divided by the subjects, music, sports, and education, solved a psychometrical mental rotation task with three-dimensional block figures. The results showed a better mental rotation performance for music and sports students compared to the education students. Furthermore, the well known gender difference favoring males was found for both sports and education students but not for music students.
Article
Two experiments investigated whether possible and impossible body postures influence mental rotation processes differently considering that anatomical limitations constrain the way in which subjects perform mental transformations of body parts. In Experiment 1, mental rotation was performed on two stimuli presented simultaneously. Both possible and impossible body postures elicited mental rotation, although the mental rotation rate was slower for impossible postures. In Experiment 2, only one stimulus was presented at a time and subjects decided whether it represented a correct body-part configuration or not. A typical mental rotation function was only present for correct body-part configurations. The results are discussed in terms of the familiarity of the stimuli. Unfamiliar stimuli (physically impossible) are rotated via local representations of their parts, whilst global representations are used for rotating familiar (anatomically correct) stimuli.
Article
Two experiments are reported in which Ss were required to determine whether a random, angular form, presented at any of a number of picture-plane orientations, was a "standard" or "reflected" version. Average time required to make this determination increased linearly with the angular departure of the form from a previously learned orientation. The slope and intercept of the reaction-time (RT) function were virtually constant, regardless of the perceptual complexity of the test form and the orientation selected for initial learning. When Ss were informed in advance as to the identity and the orientation of the upcoming test form and, further, were permitted to indicate when they were prepared for its external presentation, RT for determining the version of the form was constant for all test-form orientations. However, the time needed to prepare for the test-form presentation increased linearly with the angular departure of the form from the learned orientation. It is argued that the processes both of preparing for and of responding to a disoriented test form consist of the mental rotation of an image, and that both sorts of mental rotation (pre-stimulus and post-stimulus) are carried out at essentially the same constant rate.
Article
Recently, researchers have begun to investigate how nonspatial perceptual information is extracted into visual working memory (VWM), focusing particularly on object-based encoding (OBE). That is, whenever even one feature-dimension is selected for entry into VWM, the others are also extracted automatically. While there is evidence supporting robust OBE in VWM, some researchers have argued that it is restricted to certain conditions, suggesting that OBE might be weak. The current study analyzed the experimental differences between prior studies revealing OBE and the ones that failed, and suggested that there were three critical differences in the experimental settings. Studies supporting robust OBE predominantly were conducted by probing an "irrelevant-change distracting effect," in which a change of stored irrelevant-feature dramatically affects performance. To examine whether OBE in VWM is robust or weak, we manipulated these three aspects under the irrelevant-change distracting effect to check whether OBE could be erased. In three experiments, we found similar degrees of the distracting effect between the experimental condition (controlling these factors) and the control condition; this suggests that these factors do not affect OBE. We conclude that robust OBE exists in VWM.