ArticlePDF Available

The use of scent-detection dogs

Authors:
  • The Ministry for the Environment

Abstract

Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) can detect substances at much lower concentrations than humans (Thorne, 1995) and their area of olfactory epithelium (18 to 150 cm2 ; Dodd and Squirrel, 1980, cited in Thorne, 1995) is much greater than that of humans (3 cm2; Albone, 1984). Dogs are used by humans to locate a range of substances because of their superior olfactory acuity. This paper reviews the use of scent-detection dogs to detect non-biological scents (explosives, chemical contaminants, illegal drugs) and biological scents (human odours, animal scents) and their role in conservation.
97
Focus Volume 58 (4) : April, 2005
Irish Veterinary Journal
CONTINUING EDUCATION Volume 59 (2) : February, 2006
Irish Veterinary Journal
Detection dogs for non-biological scents
Dogs trained to detect explosives and land mines are now the largest
group of working scent-detection dogs in the world (Gazit and Terkel,
2003). They are considered to be the most reliable, versatile and cost
efficient explosives-detectors (Furton and Myers, 2001; Lorenzo et al.,
2003). The ability of dogs to locate their target scents while ignoring
the many non-target scents encountered in their search environments
(e.g., airports) is claimed to be better than that of instruments (Furton
and Myers, 2001). There are over 100 million laid land mines around
the world. They block access to productive land, curb economic
growth, and kill and maim people (McLean, 2001). Mine-detection
dogs search for buried land mines and are used to confirm that areas
are free from mines (Phelan and Webb, 2003). They are trained to
detect the explosive chemicals in land mines but also to recognise the
scent of tripwires (Fjellanger, 2003; Hayter, 2003). Experts believe that
the detection abilities of land mine-detection dogs are superior to all
comparable methods (Bach and McLean, 2003).
Accelerant-detection dogs are trained to locate the residual scent of
flammable products used as accelerants by arsonists and to ignore
the smell of pyrolysis products such as burned carpet or wood (Katz
and Midkiff, 1998). Dogs find vestiges of accelerants at fire scenes
more quickly and precisely than humans (Kurz et al., 1994). When
dogs are used to locate accelerants, fewer samples from a scene
need to be submitted for analysis, and this improves the efficiency
of investigations and saves time and money (Tindall and Lothridge,
1995; Katz and Midkiff, 1998). Dogs can detect extremely low volumes
(5.0 to 0.005μL) of accelerants, levels which are at or beyond the
sensitivity of laboratory techniques and equipment (Kurz et al., 1994;
Tindall and Lothridge, 1995; Kurz et al., 1996).
Dogs can be trained to identify areas contaminated with hazardous
chemicals, such as toluene (Arner et al., 1986). They are capable
of locating very small (0.1g) quantities of these chemicals over
large distances where instruments have failed to detect them. This
improves human safety by identifying the outer limits of a polluted
area before dangerously high levels of toxins are encountered and
can determine point sources for more efficient sampling (Arner et al.,
1986). Organochlorine residues have been found in beef exports from
Australia and dogs are now used routinely to detect aldrin, dieldrin,
and DDT contamination on farmland. The level of contamination in
the soil can be very low (1 part per million and less) (Crook, 2000)
and trained dogs can identify point sources of organochlorines with
sensitivity levels of up to 99%. Using dogs saves time and reduces
the number of soil samples required to identify contaminated sites
(Crook, 2000).
Dogs are used by customs services to find illegal drugs including
cocaine, heroin, methamphetamine and marijuana (Lorenzo et al.,
2003) and are used routinely to screen the millions of people and
items crossing international borders through airports, seaports and
by post (Adams and Johnson, 1994; Rouhi, 1997). Drug-detection dogs
are also used by police and in schools and workplaces to detect and
deter the use and trading of illicit substances (Ritz, 1994).
Detection dogs for biological scents
Human scents
Dogs are able to identify an individual’s scent even when it is mixed
with the scent of another person or with other strong smelling
substances (Kalmus, 1955). Police in some countries use dogs to
identify criminals by matching the scent of a perpetrator at a crime
scene to the scent of a suspect. To some police forces this is the
The use of scent-detection dogs
Clare Browne1, Kevin Stafford2 and Robin Fordham1
1 Ecology Group, Institute of Natural Resources, Massey University, Palmerston North, New Zealand
2 Institute of Veterinary, Animal and Biomedical Sciences, Massey University, Palmerston North, New Zealand
Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) can detect substances at much lower concentrations than humans (Thorne, 1995) and their area of
olfactory epithelium (18 to 150 cm2 ; Dodd and Squirrel, 1980, cited in Thorne, 1995) is much greater than that of humans (3 cm2;
Albone, 1984). Dogs are used by humans to locate a range of substances because of their superior olfactory acuity. This paper reviews
the use of scent-detection dogs to detect non-biological scents (explosives, chemical contaminants, illegal drugs) and biological scents
(human odours, animal scents) and their role in conservation.
Dogs have
amazing
capacities to
detect
scents.
Author for Correspondence:
Clare Browne
Ecology Group
Institute of Natural Resources
Massey University
Palmerston North
New Zealand
Tel: +64 6 355 9235
Email: clare_browne@yahoo.co.nz
most valuable task a police dog can perform but it is controversial
(Schoon, 1997). Because the information provided by dogs in ‘scent
identification line-ups’ is used as evidence in court (Schoon, 1996),
its reliability has been investigated in several studies. Results indicate
that, with sufficient training, dogs are capable of matching scents from
different parts of the same human body (Schoon and De Bruin, 1994;
Settle et al., 1994). In addition, dogs can follow trails of human scent
through busy urban centres 48 hours after they were laid with 77.5%
average success (Harvey and Harvey, 2003).
Dogs trained for search and rescue are used to search for missing
people, avalanche victims, survivors at disaster sites (such as
earthquakes, floods and plane crashes) and drowned persons (Fenton,
1992; Hebard, 1993). Cadaver-detection dogs are trained to find
decomposing human bodies (Lasseter et al., 2003) and are used to
locate the victims of misadventure. Cadaver dogs are trained to find
traces of human corpses, such as skeletal remains or fluid and tissue
remnants, on the surface, buried underground, or in water (Fenton,
1992; Lasseter et al., 2003). Cadaver dogs can rapidly search large
areas for human remains, saving a considerable amount of human time
and effort (Komar, 1999). Detection rates of cadaver dogs range from
30% to 81% in field trials (Komar, 1999; Lasseter et al., 2003).
Scent detection dogs can aid in the diagnosis of some types of
cancer. They can detect the odour of melanoma cells and that of
urine from people with bladder cancer, with accuracy levels of 100%
and 41%, respectively (Pickel et al., 2001; Pickel et al., 2004; Willis et
al., 2004). Cancerous cells may produce volatile chemicals, enabling
their detection by dogs (Pickel et al., 2004). Edney (1993) described
the behaviour of 37 dogs that responded to their owners’ epileptic
events. Of these dogs, 57% displayed characteristic behaviours prior
to a seizure and 68% performed similar behaviours during a seizure.
Activities of the dogs prior to the onset of a human seizure were
predominantly attention-seeking such as barking, jumping up and
becoming overly attentive; while the behaviour of the dogs reacting
during their owners’ seizures were mainly described as protective,
including sitting and staying beside their owners. Dogs trained to alert
their owners to impending epileptic attacks were able to consistently
indicate to their owners that a seizure was imminent, with warning
times ranging from 10 to 45 minutes (Strong et al., 1999; Brown and
Strong, 2001). It has been suggested that dogs are able to detect
scents exuded by their owners before the epileptic fit and sense
behavioural changes in their owners at this time (Edney, 1993).
More than a third of people with diabetes reported that their
dogs react to their hypoglycaemic attacks (Lim et al., 1992; cited in
Chen et al., 2000). Three case studies described dogs detecting a
hypoglycaemic attack before their owners had noticed any symptoms
(Chen et al., 2000). The dogs displayed a range of abnormal behaviours
prior to and during their owners’ hypoglycaemia, including running
and hiding, barking and preventing the owner from leaving the house.
None of the dogs described resumed normal behaviours until their
owners had eaten food to correct blood glucose concentrations. The
mechanisms by which dogs detect changes in human blood glucose
levels are unknown, but it is suspected that the dogs recognise
olfactory changes attributed to increased sweating, possibly combined
with muscle tremors and behavioural changes (Chen et al., 2000).
Clare Browne’s
dog Apple sniffs
a dead tuatara,
a reptile used in
training.
Volume 59 (2) : February, 2006 CONTINUING EDUCATION
Irish Veterinary Journal
98
99
Animal scents
Dogs are used for biosecurity purposes in a variety of circumstances,
including containment and border control. Dogs are used in Guam, for
example, to search outward-bound cargo for brown tree snakes (Boiga
irregularis) and prevent accidental introduction of this pest elsewhere
(Engeman et al., 1998a; Engeman et al., 1998b). These snake-detection
dogs have an average location rate of 62% (Engeman et al., 2002).
Dogs can locate insects that damage plants. The red palm weevil
(Rhynchophorus ferrugineus) can inflict severe damage on date palms
(Phoenix dactylifera L.), the most important fruit crop in the Middle
East (Nakash et al., 2000). Affected trees are extremely difficult to
find, but can be saved if identified in the early stages of infestation
(Nakash et al., 2000). Nakash et al. (2000) reported that two dogs
were trained to respond to the secretions of infested trees and
produced very high success rates in initial tests. Dogs can also be
trained to detect the egg masses of gypsy moths (Porthetria dispar
L.) which are laid close to the ground in leaf litter or debris and are
particularly hard to find (Wallner and Ellis, 1976). Two dogs evaluated
at searching for egg masses had a combined average detection rate of
73%, with the results showing a strong correlation between one dog’s
number of indications and egg mass density (Wallner and Ellis, 1976).
There is potential for calibrating and using a dog to estimate egg mass
density by the number located within a specific time period.
In the USA subterranean termite damage and control are estimated
to cost up to US$2 billion per annum (Culliney and Grace, 2000).
Early infestations are often impossible to detect visually and can cause
significant damage before they are discovered (Brooks et al., 2003).
Trained termite-detection dogs can locate eastern subterranean
termites (Reticulitermes flavipes Kollar) with average success rates of
over 95%, and can discriminate between termites, other insects (ants
and cockroaches) and termite-damaged wood (Brooks et al., 2003).
When the ability of dogs to detect western subterranean termites
(Reticulitermes hesperus Banks) was compared with electronic odour
detection devices, the dogs correctly identified 98% of artificially
set-up infestations while the electronic device had a low detection
rate (Lewis et al., 1997). However, the dogs also produced 28%
false positives, where there was no infestation, although this may be
attributable to training techniques (Brooks et al., 2003).
Screwworms (Cochliomyia hominivorax) are obligate parasites that
can kill warm-blooded animals and cause significant economic losses
(Welch, 1990). A dog trained to detect both screwworm pupae
and screwworm-infested wounds on animals had an extremely high
success rate (99.7%) at finding them (Welch, 1990).
Dogs may even be used to detect microorganisms. Some
cyanobacteria species in commercial catfish ponds produce odorous
compounds which accumulate in the flesh of the fish, causing an
unpleasant flavour (Shelby et al., 2004). The cost of rejecting affected
fish ranges from $15 to 23 million annually for catfish producers in
the United States (Hanson, 2003; cited in Shelby et al., 2004). Shelby
et al. (2004) showed that dogs could identify the two most common
‘off-flavours’, 2-methylisoborneol and geosmin, in pond water samples
with high levels of accuracy. Three dogs detected the off-flavours at
levels of 1μg/L with 79% to 93% accuracy and 10ng/L with 37% to 49%
success. Trained dogs are a practical method of detecting off-flavours
and are a reliable alternative to chemical analysis or human taste-
testers (Shelby et al., 2004). Microbial growth in buildings can have
detrimental effects on human health and cause costly deterioration
of construction materials. The initial detection of microbial growth
is extremely difficult and Kauhanen et al. (2002) tested the efficacy
of dogs trained to find rot fungi, typical building moulds and bacteria.
They found that their two study dogs were able to locate 75% of
hidden microbial growth samples.
Dogs can identify dairy cows that are in oestrus from the scent of
vaginal fluid, urine, milk and blood plasma, with accuracies ranging
from 78% to 99% (Kiddy et al., 1978; Kiddy et al., 1984). Dogs can also
discriminate between the milk of cows in pre-oestrus, oestrus and
dioestrus (Hawk et al., 1984).
Detection dogs used for conservation
Dogs are used to locate and monitor a number of endangered
mammals and birds and are a comparatively unobtrusive method for
researchers and conservationists to use when studying rare species.
Dogs can offer safer methods of studying potentially dangerous
animals, reduce some sample collection biases and decrease the
time spent searching for animals. It is often difficult to collect
information on endangered species due to their low densities and
the large, remote areas they commonly inhabit. The use of scat
(animal droppings)-detection dogs is becoming increasingly popular in
many countries due to the problems inherent in traditional methods
of researching threatened species. Mark-recapture techniques
and attaching radio-tracking devices, for example, are invasive and
potentially harmful to the animals (Long et al., 2002). Using dogs to
find scats is a non-invasive method of studying rare animal populations,
and it can increase sample numbers while reducing collection bias
(Wasser et al., 2004). The information that can be extracted from
scats is comparable to data provided by traditional methods.
Applying molecular techniques to scats provides information on the
species, sex, individual identity, diet and parasitology of animals (Kohn
and Wayne, 1997; Mills et al., 2000). Reproductive and stress hormones
from scats can indicate reproductive productivity and impacts of
disturbance on physiological condition (Wasser et al., 2000; Wasser
et al., 2004). By systematically sampling scats over a large geographic
area, population characteristics such as sex ratio, relatedness, habitat
and home ranges may be estimated (Kohn and Wayne, 1997; Kohn
et al., 1999; Wasser et al., 2004). Scats may provide more information
and be a more accessible source of DNA than materials such as hair,
skin, feathers, nails, bones, or saliva (Kohn and Wayne, 1997). The
distribution of animals determined by dog-assisted scat sampling has
been found to correspond well with methods such as hair sampling
and GPS radio-tracking (Wasser et al., 2004).
Apple at work in
the New Zealand
countryside.
Focus Volume 58 (4) : April, 2005
Irish Veterinary Journal
CONTINUING EDUCATION Volume 59 (2) : February, 2006
Irish Veterinary Journal
101
Dogs are used to locate bears in North America for management of
game populations and conservation purposes. A study by Wasser et al.
(2004) described the use of scat-detection dogs to assess the impacts
of human disturbances on black bear (Ursus americanus) and grizzly
bear (Ursus arctos horribilis) populations in Canada. The dogs were
trained to locate bear scats along transects within a 5,200km2 area
and DNA was extracted from the scats to determine species and
individual identities. By using scat-detection dogs, Wasser et al. (2004)
were able to effectively and non-invasively identify land use patterns
for both black and grizzly bears. Mark-recapture methods, using dogs
trained to locate bear scent along transect routes, are also used to
estimate bear population in North America (Akenson et al., 2001); and
dogs can be trained to discriminate between black and grizzly bear
scats, reducing the need for laboratory tests (Hurt et al., 2000).
Dogs trained to find the scats of endangered San Joaquin kit foxes
(Vulpes macrotis mutica) in the US are more efficient than humans at
finding scats for demographic and population studies (Smith et al.,
2003). Trained dogs are able to find up to four times more kit fox
scats along transects than an experienced person, and even the dogs’
worst detection rate in difficult scenting conditions was as good
as that of humans (Smith and Ralls, 2001; Smith et al., 2003). Dogs
searching for kit fox scats must distinguish them from coyote (Canis
latrans), skunk (Mephitis mephitis) and badger (Taxidea taxus) scats, and
have been found to be 100% correct in their species identification
(Smith and Ralls, 2001; Smith et al., 2003). Kit fox latrines (areas where
one or more individuals repeatedly defaecate) can also be found by
dogs (Ralls and Smith, 2004). As the cost of extracting DNA from
faecal samples and using laboratory methods to determine species is
expensive, this extremely accurate species identification ability of scat-
detection dogs saves thousands of dollars.
Biologists studying the endangered amur tiger (Panther tigris altaica)
in Russia use dogs to identify individual tigers. The dogs identify the
tigers by smelling the collected urine and scat samples and matching
them to a reference collection of known tigers (L. Kerley, personal
communication, 2004). The movements of individual tigers are
monitored using a combination of observation, conventional tracking
and the dog-identified scats (Kerley, 2003). However, information
on the population dynamics of the tigers can be obtained by using
the dogs alone. Tigers new to the area can also be identified by this
method (L. Kerley, personal communication, 2004) and two dogs used
in this project have proved to have accuracy rates of 89% and 96%
(Kerley, 2003).
Trained dogs assist researchers studying ringed seals (Phoca hispida) in
the North American Arctic. Dogs have been relied on to locate these
seals in a number of studies, which assessed the impacts of human
activity and industry on the seals, examined possible links between
lair characteristics and predation success, and obtained measures of
territory size (Lydersen and Gjertz, 1986; Smith, 1987; Furgal et al.,
1996). Specially trained dogs can locate, by scent, subnivean (beneath
the snow) lairs and breathing holes on the ice shelf at distances over
1.5km, through drifted snow up to 2m deep, and in winds of up to 46
km/hour (Smith, 1987).
Dogs traditionally used for hunting game birds are now frequently
employed to locate birds and help carry out studies on threatened
species. Yellow rails (Coturnicops noveboracensis), for example, are
classified as a vulnerable species in Quebec (Robert and Laporte,
1997). Because their patchy, localised distribution makes them
extremely difficult to locate, study, or catch, dogs have been used to
find their nests during research projects (Robert and Laporte, 1997).
Management programs of rare avian species have also benefited
from dogs’ innate behaviour. Border collies, for example, were used
to help capture endangered aleutian canada geese (Branta canadensis
leucopareia) in Alaska for relocation to predator-free islands (Shute,
1990). The terrain of the island inhabited by the geese made catching
them extremely dangerous for humans, and many researchers and
geese sustained injuries. The use of dogs not only made the exercise
much safer, but also much more efficient. Scientists took three weeks
to catch 120 geese; two dogs were able to round up 143 in four days
(Shute, 1990).
Dogs have been used in New Zealand for more than a 100 years
to locate a number of endangered species, such as blue duck
(Hymenolaimus malacorhynchos), kiwi (Apteryx spp.) and kakapo
(Strigops habroptilus) (Browne, 2005). Reliable kiwi-detection dogs are
considered essential to kiwi field research because the birds are so
difficult to locate (Colbourne, 1992).
Surveys of bird carcasses can be used to estimate mortality caused by
disease, poisoning or pollution (Homan et al., 2001). Quick recovery
of carcasses before decomposition or scavenging takes place is
important to obtain accurate population estimates. Homan et al.
(2001) compared the searching efficiency of humans and dogs looking
for house sparrow (Passer domesticus) carcasses amongst vegetation.
They found that dogs were significantly more efficient at detecting
avian carcasses than humans, finding twice as many, even at very low
carcass densities.
Summary
Dogs are reliable and efficient scent-detectors. Numerous studies
have established dogs’ proficiency at locating an extremely wide range
of scents. Trained dogs can significantly reduce the amount of time
spent searching for a target object, chemical or species. Often more
sensitive, reliable and practical than electronic scent-detection devices,
dogs are also easy and cheap to train and put into action. Scent-
detection dogs make a significant contribution to the conservation
programmes of many endangered species. In the future we can
expect to see dogs involved more widely in chemical detection,
conservation and disease diagnosis, both human and veterinary. The
major restriction to the use of trained scent-detection dogs appears
to be human imagination.
References
Adams, G.J. and Johnson, K.G. (1994). Sleep, work, and the effects
of shift work in drug detector dogs Canis familiaris. Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 41: 115-126.
Akenson, J.J., Henjum, M.G., Wertz, T.L. and Craddock, T.J.
(2001). Use of dogs and mark-recapture techniques to estimate
American Black Bear density in northeastern Oregon. Ursus 12: 203-
210.
Albone, E.S. (1984). Mammalian semiochemistry: the investigation of
chemical signals between mammals. Chichester: Wiley. 360 pp
Arner, L.D., Johnson, G.R. and Skovronek, H.S. (1986).
Delineating toxic areas by canine olfaction. Journal of Hazardous
Materials 13: 375-381.
Focus Volume 58 (4) : April, 2005
Irish Veterinary Journal
CONTINUING EDUCATION Volume 59 (2) : February, 2006
Irish Veterinary Journal
Bach, H. and McLean, I.G. (2003). Remote explosive scent tracing
(REST), genuine or a paper tiger? Journal of Mine Action 7: 75-82.
Brooks, S.E., Oi, F.M. and Koehler, P.G. (2003). Ability of canine
termite detectors to locate live termites and discriminate them from
non-termite material. Journal of Economic Entomology 96: 1259-1266.
Brown, S.W. and Strong, V. (2001). The use of seizure-alert dogs.
Seizure 10: 39-41.
Browne, C.M. (2005). The Use of Dogs to Detect New Zealand
Reptile Scents. Unpublished Master of Science thesis, Massey
University, Palmerston North, New Zealand.
Chen, M., Daly, M., Natt, Susie and Williams, G. (2000). Non-
invasive detection of hypoglycaemia using a novel, fully biocompatible
and patient-friendly alarm system. British Medical Journal 321: 1565-
1566.
Colbourne, R. (1992). Little spotted kiwi (Apteryx owenii): recruitment
and behaviour of juveniles on Kapiti Island, New Zealand. Journal of the
Royal Society of New Zealand 22: 321-328.
Croo k, A. (2000). Use of odour detection dogs in residue
management programs. Asian-Australasian Journal of Animal Sciences
13: 219-219.
Culliney, T.W. and Grace, J.K. (2000). Prospects for the biological
control of subterranean termites (Isoptera: Rhinotermitidae), with
special reference to Coptotermes formosanus. Bulletin of Entomological
Research 90: 9-21.
Edney, A. (1993).Dogs and human epilepsy. Veterinary Record 132:
337-338.
Engeman, R.M., Rodriquez, D.V., Linnell, M.A. and Pitzler,
M.E. (1998a). A review of the case histories of the brown tree snakes
(Boiga irregularis) located by detector dogs on Guam. International
Biodeterioration and Biodegradation 42: 161-165.
Engeman, R.M., Vice, D.S., Rodriguez, D.V., Gruver, K.S.,
Santos, W.S. and Pitzler, M.E. (1998b). Effectiveness of the
detector dogs used for deterring the dispersal of brown tree snakes.
Pacific Conservation Biology 4: 256-260.
Engeman, R.M., Vice, D.S., York, D. and Gruver, K.S. (2002).
Sustained elevation of the effectiveness of detector dogs for locating
brown tree snakes in cargo outbound from Guam. International
Biodeterioration and Biodegradation 49: 101-106.
Fenton, V. (1992). The use of dogs in search, rescue and recovery.
Journal of Wilderness Medicine 3: 292-300.
Fjellanger, R. (2003). The REST concept. In: Mine Detection Dogs:
Training, Operations and Odour Detection. Edited by I.G. McLean. Geneva:
GICHD. pp53-107.
Furgal, C.M., Innes, S. and Kovacs, K.M. (1996). Characteristics
of ringed seal, Phoca hispida, subnivean structures and breeding habitat
and their effects on predation. Canadian Journal of Zoology 74: 858-874.
Furton, K.G. and Myers, L.J. (2001). The scientific foundation and
efficacy of the use of canines as chemical detectors for explosives.
Talanta 54: 487-500.
Gazit, I. and Terkel, J. (2003). Explosives detection by sniffer dogs
following strenuous physical activity. Applied Animal Behaviour Science
81: 149-161.
Harvey, L.M. and Harvey, J.W. (2003). Reliability of bloodhounds in
criminal investigations. Journal of Forensic Sciences 48: 811-816.
Hawk, H.W., Conley, H.H. and Kiddy, C.A. (1984). Estrus-related
odors in milk detected by trained dogs. Journal of Dairy Science 67:
392-397.
Hayter, D. (2003). Training dogs to detect tripwires. In: Mine Detection
Dogs: Training, Operations and Odour Detection. Edited by I.G. McLean.
Geneva: GICHD. pp109-138.
Hebard, C. (1993). Use of search and rescue dogs. Journal of the
American Veterinary Medical Association 203: 999-1001.
Homan, H.J., Linz, G. and Peer, B.D. (2001). Dogs increase
recovery of passerine carcasses in dense vegetation. Wildlife Society
Bulletin 29: 292-296.
Hurt, A., Davenport, B. and Greene, E. (2000). Training dogs to
distinguish between black bear (Ursus americanus) and grizzly bear
(Ursus arctos) faeces. University of Montana Under-Graduate Biology
Journal. Accessed: http://ibscore.dbs.umt.edu/journal/Articles_all/2000/
Hurt.htm
Kalmus, H. (1955). The discrimination by the nose of the dog of
individual human odours and in particular of the odour of twins. British
Journal of Animal Behaviour 3: 25-31.
Katz, S.R. and Midkiff, C.R. (1998). Unconfirmed canine accelerant
detection: a reliability issue in court. Journal of Forensic Sciences 43:
329-333.
Kauhanen, E., Harri, M., Nevalainen, A. and Nevalainen, T.
(2002). Validity of detection of microbial growth in buildings by trained
dogs. Environment International 28: 153-157.
Kerley, L. (2003). Scent dog monitoring of Amur tigers - II. A final
report to Save the Tiger Fund. Lazovsky State Nature Zapovednik,
Lazo. 7 p.
Kiddy, C.A., Mitchell, D.S., Bolt, D.J. and Hawk, H.W. (1978).
Detection of estrus-related odors in cows by trained dogs. Biology of
Reproduction 19: 389-395.
Kiddy, C.A., Mitchell, D.S. and Hawk, H.W. (1984). Estrus-related
odors in body fluids of dairy cows. Journal of Dairy Science 67: 388-391.
Kohn, M.H. and Wayne, R.K. (1997). Facts from faeces revisited.
Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 12: 223-227.
Kohn, M.H., York, E.C., Kamradt, D.A., Haught, G., Sauvajot,
R.M. and Wayne, R.K. (1999). Estimating population size by
genotyping faeces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B -
Biological Sciences 266: 657-663.
Komar, D. (1999). The use of cadaver dogs in locating scattered,
scavenged human remains: preliminary field test results. Journal of
Forensic Sciences 44: 405-408.
Kurz, M.E., Billard, M., Rettig, M., Augustiniak, J., Lange, J.,
Larsen, M., Warrick, R., Mohns, T., Bora, R., Broadus, K.,
Hartke, G., Glover, B. ,Tankersley, D. and Marcouiller, J. (1994).
Evaluation of canines for accelerant detection at fire scenes. Journal of
Forensic Sciences 39: 1528-1536.
Kurz, M.E., Schultz, S., Griffith, J., Broadus, K., Sparks,
J., Dabdoub, G. and Brock, J. (1990). Effect of background
interference on accelerant detection by canines. Journal of Forensic
Sciences 41: 868-873.
Lasseter, A.E., Jacobi, K.P., Farley, R. and Hensel, L. (2003).
Cadaver dog and handler team capabilities in the recovery of buried
human remains in the southeastern United States. Journal of Forensic
Sciences 48: 617-621.
Lewis, V.R., Fouche, C.F. and Lemaster, R.L. (1997). Evaluation
of dog-assisted searches and electronic odor devices for detecting the
western subterranean termite. Forest Products Journal 47: 79-84.
Long, R.A., Donovan, T.M., MacKay, P., Zielinski, W.J. and
Buzas, J.S. (2002). Scat-sniffing dogs as a tool for studying forest
Volume 59 (2) : February, 2006 CONTINUING EDUCATION
Irish Veterinary Journal
102
104
carnivores in Vermont. Presented at Carnivores 2002 Monterey,
California. [Abstract only]
Lorenzo, N., Wan, T.L., Harper, R.J., Hsu, Y.-L., Chow, M., Rose,
S. and Furton, K.G. (2003). Laboratory and field experiments used
to identify Canis lupus var. familiaris active odor signature chemicals
from drugs, explosives, and humans. Analytical and Bioanalytical
Chemistry 376: 1212-1224.
Lydersen, C. and Gjertz, I. (1986). Studies of the ringed seal Phoca-
hispida in its breeding habitat in Kongsfjorden Svalbard Arctic ocean.
Polar Research 4: 57-64.
McLean, I.G. (2001). What the dog’s nose knows. Journal of Mine
Action 5: 108-109.
Mills, L.S., Citta, J.J., Lair, K.P., Schwartz, M.K. and Tallmon,
D.A. (2000). Estimating animal abundance using noninvasive DNA
sampling: promise and pitfalls. Ecological Applications 10: 283-294.
Nakash, J., Osem, Y. and Kehat, M. (2000). A suggestion to
use dogs for detecting red palm weevil (Rhynchophorus ferrugineus)
infestation in date palms in Israel. Phytoparasitica 28: 153-155.
Phelan, J.M. and Webb, S.W. (2003). Chemical sensing for buried
landmines: fundamental processes influencing trace chemical detection.
In: Mine Detection Dogs: Training, Operations and Odour Detection. Edited
by I.G. McLean. Geneva: GICHD. pp 209-285.
Pickel, D., Manucy, G.P., Walker, D.B., Hall, S.B. and Walker,
J.C. (2004). Evidence for canine olfactory detection of melanoma.
Applied Animal Behaviour Science 89: 107-116.
Pickel, D.P., Cognetta, A.B., Manucy, G.P., Walker, D.B., Hall,
S.B. and Walker, J.C. (2001). Preliminary evidence of canine
olfactory detection of melanoma. Presented at the 23rd Annual
Meeting of Association for Chemoreception Sciences, Sarasota,
Florida.
Ralls, K. and Smith, D.A. (2004). Latrine use by San Joaquin kit
foxes (Vulpes macrotis mutica) and coyotes (Canis latrans). Western North
American Naturalist 64: 544-547.
Reindl, S., Higgins, K., Whitelaw, A., Hurt, A. and Shivik, J.
(2004). Efficacy of detection dogs to determine presence/absence at a
black-footed ferret reintroduction site. Final report. 5 p.
Ritz, D. (1994). The canine connection. Security Management 38: 34-
38.
Robert, M. and Laporte, P. (1997). Field techniques for studying
breeding yellow rails. Journal of Field Ornithology 68: 56-63.
Rouhi, A.M. (1997). Detecting illegal substances. Chemical and
Engineering News 75: 24-29.
Schoon, G.A.A. and De Bruin, J.C. (1994). The ability of dogs to
recognize and cross-match human odours. Forensic Science International
69: 111-118.
Schoon, G.A.A. (1996). Scent identification lineups by dogs (Canis
familiaris): experimental design and forensic application. Applied Animal
Behaviour Science 49: 257-267.
Schoon, G.A.A. (1997). Scent identifications by dogs (Canis familiaris):
a new experimental design. Behaviour 134: 531-550.
Settle, R.H., Sommerville, B.A., McCormick, J. and Broom,
D.M. (1994). Human scent matching using specially trained dogs.
Animal Behaviour 48: 1443-1448.
Shelby, R.A., Schrader, K.K., Tucker, A., Klesius, P.H. and
Myers, L.J. (2004). Detection of catfish off-flavour compounds by
trained dogs. Aquaculture Research 35: 888-892.
Shute, N. (1990). Dogging rare geese to save them. National Wildlife
28: 22.
Smith, D.A. and Ralls, K. (2001). Canine assistants for
conservationists. Science 291: 435.
Smith, D.A., Ralls, K., Hurt, A., Adams, B., Parker, M.,
Davenport, B., Smith, M.C. and Maldonado, J.E. (2003).
Detection and accuracy rates of dogs trained to find scats of San
Joaquin kit foxes (Vulpes macrotis mutica). Animal Conservation 6: 339-
346.
Smith, T.G. (1987). The ringed seal Phoca hispida of the Canadian
western Arctic. Canadian Bulletin of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 216:
1-81.
Strong, V., Brown, S.W. and Walker, R. (1999). Seizure-alert dogs
- fact or fiction? Seizure 8: 62-65.
Thorne, C. (1995). Feeding behaviour of domestic dogs and the
role of experience. In: The Domestic Dog: its Evolution, Behaviour and
Interactions with People. Edited by J. Serpell. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press. pp 103-114.
Tindall, R. and Lothridge, K. (1995). An evaluation of 42 accelerant
detection canine teams. Journal of Forensic Sciences 40: 561-564.
Wallner, W.E. and Ellis, T.L. (1976). Olfactory detection of gypsy
moth pheromone and egg masses by domestic canines. Environmental
Entomology 5: 183-186.
Wasser, S.K., Hunt, K.E., Brown, J.L., Cooper, K., Crockett,
C.M., Bechert, U., Millspaugh, J.J., Larson, S. and Monfort, S.L.
(2000). A generalized faecal glucocorticoid assay for use in a diverse
array of nondomestic mammalian and avian species. General and
Comparative Endocrinology 120: 260-275.
Wasser, S.K., Davenport, B., Ramage, E.R., Hunt, K.E., Parker,
M., Clarke, C. and Stenhouse, G. (2004). Scat detection dogs in
wildlife research and management: application to grizzly and black
bears in the Yellowhead Ecosystem, Alberta, Canada. Canadian Journal
of Zoology 82: 475-492.
Welch , J.B. (1990). A detector dog for screwworms (Diptera:
Calliphoridae). Journal of Economic Entomology 83: 1932-1934.
Willis, C.M., Church, S.M., Guest, C.M., Cook, W.A., McCarthy,
N., Bransbury, A.J., Church, M.R.T. and Church, J.C.T. (2004).
Olfactory detection of human bladder cancer by dogs: proof of
principle study. British Medical Journal 329: 712-716.
Volume 59 (2) : February, 2006 CONTINUING EDUCATION
Irish Veterinary Journal
... Dating back to the early 1700's, scent trained dogs were being used by monks at the St. Bernard Hospice in Switzerland to search for lost or stranded travelers (Barwig et al., 1986). While scent-trained dogs are still widely used for such search and rescue purposes, they are becoming increasingly used to locate cryptic objects (Browne et al., 2006). Some of the best known examples include the use of detection dogs to expose and locate drugs, firearms, contraband plants and foods, and even cancer (Adamkiewicz et al., 2013;Browne et al., 2006;McCulloch et al., 2006;Pickel et al., 2004;Wasser et al., 2004;Witherington et al., 2017). ...
... While scent-trained dogs are still widely used for such search and rescue purposes, they are becoming increasingly used to locate cryptic objects (Browne et al., 2006). Some of the best known examples include the use of detection dogs to expose and locate drugs, firearms, contraband plants and foods, and even cancer (Adamkiewicz et al., 2013;Browne et al., 2006;McCulloch et al., 2006;Pickel et al., 2004;Wasser et al., 2004;Witherington et al., 2017). ...
... Often, frogs can only be located when adults return to breeding sites. Detection dogs have demonstrated their value in detecting the presence and/or absence of other taxa by indicating on either live scent or scat (Arandjelovic et al., 2015;Browne et al., 2006;Colbourne, 1992). We suggest that conservation detection dogs could play a valuable role identifying the presence and/or absence of amphibians during early life stages post reintroduction and recommend further investigation into the feasibility of incorporating detection dogs into recovery plans. ...
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, the potential to locate endangered animals using scent trained detection dogs (conservation detection dogs) has gained attention. Among vertebrates, conservation detection dogs have demonstrated a remarkable capacity to detect the scent of endangered mammals, reptiles, and birds, but their use in detecting amphibians is only beginning to be explored. A lack of work in this area is surprising given that amphibians are declining faster than any other vertebrate taxa. Moreover, amphibians are generally small, highly cryptic and breed in complex habitats, making them difficult to locate for the purpose of monitoring or establishing conservation breeding programs. This study aimed to provide a preliminary investigation into whether conservation detection dogs can be imprinted on the scent of the critically endangered Baw Baw frog (Philoria frosti) under captive conditions, and then effectively trained to locate wild frogs in their complex natural habitat. Two conservation detection dogs were trained to identify and locate P. frosti scent under controlled conditions before assessing their ability to locate wild P. frosti. Both conservation detection dogs were effective at locating P. frosti scent under controlled conditions, and also demonstrated an ability to detect live frogs under natural conditions. From an applied perspective, our findings provide new evidence that conservation detection dogs are capable of learning to detect the scent of small, cryptic anuran species. They also indicate that detection dogs are capable of locating frogs in highly complex forest habitat, confirming their untapped potential to aid in the management of imperiled species that have previously proven difficult to detect, monitor, and protect. We discuss the limitations of our approach and provide recommendations to help direct future amphibian conservation detection dog research.
... Dogs have an advanced olfactory system that is 10,000-100,000 times more sensitive than that of humans (Walker et al., 2003(Walker et al., , 2006Sankaran et al., 2012). Due to their acute olfactory abilities and trainability, humans use dogs for a wide range of scent-detection tasks (Browne et al., 2006). For example, dogs have been used to detect explosives, illicit substances, missing persons, cadavers, and various diseases (Browne et al., 2006;Helton, 2009). ...
... Due to their acute olfactory abilities and trainability, humans use dogs for a wide range of scent-detection tasks (Browne et al., 2006). For example, dogs have been used to detect explosives, illicit substances, missing persons, cadavers, and various diseases (Browne et al., 2006;Helton, 2009). Over the past century, dogs have also been used for conservation purposes, and have successfully detected numerous endangered or invasive species (Hill and Hill, 1987;Browne et al., 2006;Beebe et al., 2016). ...
... For example, dogs have been used to detect explosives, illicit substances, missing persons, cadavers, and various diseases (Browne et al., 2006;Helton, 2009). Over the past century, dogs have also been used for conservation purposes, and have successfully detected numerous endangered or invasive species (Hill and Hill, 1987;Browne et al., 2006;Beebe et al., 2016). ...
... Dogs have searched for numerous targets including accelerants, hazardous chemicals, explosives, illegal drugs, human diseases such as cancer, diabetes and epilepsy, live humans, cadavers and more (Browne et al., 2006;Kokocińska-Kusiak et al., 2021;Rooney et al., 2019;Whitehouse-Tedd et al., 2021;Woollett et al., 2013). ...
... Their use is non-invasive which protects environmental and wildlife welfare and is preferable to capture-recapture methods (Browne et al., 2006;Grimm-Seyfarth & Klenke, 2018;Kerley, 2010;Richards, 2018). Across many circumstances, CDD are faster, can find more samples and cover greater distances during a survey than other methods (Browne et al., 2006;Grimm-Seyfarth & Klenke, 2018;Kerley, 2010;MacKay et al., 2008;Stanhope & Sloan, 2019). ...
... Their use is non-invasive which protects environmental and wildlife welfare and is preferable to capture-recapture methods (Browne et al., 2006;Grimm-Seyfarth & Klenke, 2018;Kerley, 2010;Richards, 2018). Across many circumstances, CDD are faster, can find more samples and cover greater distances during a survey than other methods (Browne et al., 2006;Grimm-Seyfarth & Klenke, 2018;Kerley, 2010;MacKay et al., 2008;Stanhope & Sloan, 2019). For example, Mathews et al. (2013) found that when comparing humans and CDD during searches for bat carcasses at wind turbine sites, CDD took on average 40 min to conduct a search versus humans taking 2 h and 46 min and CDD found 75% of targets versus humans finding 20%. ...
Article
Full-text available
Conservation detection dogs (CDD) use their exceptional olfactory abilities to assist a wide range of conservation projects through the detection of target specimens or species. CDD are generally quicker, can cover wider areas and find more samples than humans and other analytical tools. However, their efficacy varies between studies; methodological and procedural standardisation in the field is lacking. Considering the cost of deploying a CDD team and the limited financial resources within conservation, it is vital that their performance is quantified and reliable. This review aims to summarise what is currently known about the use of scent detection dogs in conservation and elucidate which factors affect efficacy. We describe the efficacy of CDD across species and situational contexts like training and fieldwork. Reported sensitivities (i.e. the proportion of target samples found out of total available) ranged from 23.8% to 100% and precision rates (i.e. proportion of alerts that are true positives) from 27% to 100%. CDD are consistently shown to be better than other techniques, but performance varies substantially across the literature. There is no consistent difference in efficacy between training, testing and fieldwork, hence we need to understand the factors affecting this. We highlight the key variables that can alter CDD performance. External effects include target odour, training methods, sample management, search methodology, environment and the CDD handler. Internal effects include dog breed, personality, diet, age and health. Unfortunately, much of the research fails to provide adequate information on the dogs, handlers, training, experience and samples. This results in an inability to determine precisely why an individual study has high or low efficacy. It is clear that CDDs can be effective and applied to possibly limitless conservation scenarios, but moving forward researchers must provide more consistent and detailed methodologies so that comparisons can be conducted, results are more easily replicated and progress can be made in standardising CDD work.
... The canine nose has some 250 million olfactory cells in addition to supporting cells and basal cells, in the olfactory epithelium which covers about 18 to 150 cm 2 (Browne et al., 2006;Sjaastad et al., 2016). In humans, the olfactory epithelium covers about 5 cm 2 and contains about 5 million olfactory cells (Sjaastad et al., 2016). ...
Article
Full-text available
Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) have excellent olfactory processing capabilities that are utilized widely in human society e.g., working with customs, police, and army; their scent detection is also used in guarding, hunting, mold-sniffing, searching for missing people or animals, and facilitating the life of the disabled. Sniffing and searching for odors is a natural, species-typical behavior and essential for the dog's welfare. While taking advantage of this canine ability widely, we understand its foundations and implications quite poorly. We can improve animal welfare by better understanding their olfactory world. In this review, we outline the olfactory processing of dogs in the nervous system, summarize the current knowledge of scent detection and differentiation; the effect of odors on the dogs’ cognitive and emotional processes and the dog-human bond; and consider the methodological advancements that could be developed further to aid in our understanding of the canine world of odors.
... DDs have their grounds based on knowledge from various scientific fields, such as psychology, veterinary medicine, chemistry, and biology, gathered in law enforcement through canine detection units in their daily work activities [6]. The employment of these dogs includes functions critical to society, such as detecting explosives and bombs [7]. Several security agencies worldwide have established sniffer dog units and have been dogs includes functions critical to society, such as detecting explosives and bombs Several security agencies worldwide have established sniffer dog units and have b developing their EDD teams to address the increasing threat posed by terrorist gro As a result, most sniffer dogs are primarily trained for explosive detection in so countries in response to the global terrorism issue [8]. Figure 1 shows the EDD "K from the Brazilian Federal Police in real action, inspecting the Presidential Rolls Ro during the security inspection protocols for the inauguration of the President of Republic of Brazil on 1 January 2011. ...
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary This review analyzes the scientific literature regarding explosive detection dogs (EDDs), focusing on animal personality profile, selection, training methods, employment, and performance. Critical aspects of EDDs were addressed to improve the success of working with dogs in explosive detection and expand knowledge in a very sensitive area. Dissemination of knowledge on the employment and technical training of EDDs is essential to prevent catastrophes caused by explosives and is essential to save lives worldwide. Abstract Terrorist organizations have compelled security authorities of every nation to make an increasingly significant commitment toward mitigating the risk of mass casualties and severe financial and property damages. As a result, various security measures have been implemented, including the use of advanced equipment and an uptick in intelligence activities. One of the most effective tools that has yielded outstanding results is the use of explosive detection dogs (EDDs). The nature of EDDs demands a high level of sensitivity given the inherent danger and severity of real threat situations that may involve the risk of explosion. Moreover, the operating procedures for EDDs are unique and distinguishable from other forms of detection. We conducted a review to ensure a comprehensive understanding of the subject, highlighting the EDDs’ personality profile, selection, training methods, performance, and employment, incorporating insights from diverse fields, conducting an analysis, and presenting a perspective on using EDDs to prevent explosion threats.
... Preliminary conclusions can be drawn from statistical analysis, suggesting that Geosmin, 2-methylisoborneol, halomethane, and bromoorganics may serve as potential odour fingerprints of tap water. Studies have already confirmed the effective recognition ability of sniffer dogs for Geosmin and 2-methylisoborneol (Browne et al. 2006). ...
Article
Full-text available
Effective control of water leakage is a critical aspect for ensuring the high-quality development of the water sector. In recent years, the utilization of sniffer dogs in water leakage detection has emerged as a promising technology, progressing from laboratory experiments to small-scale real-world applications. Leveraging their remarkable ability to trace chlorine, sniffer dogs have demonstrated an impressive accuracy and high efficiency in detecting underground pipe leaks. This mini-review comprehensively examines the advancements in sniffer dog usage for leak detection, provides an overview of various application methods, and elucidates the conditions and limitations associated with each approach. It also delves into the management mechanisms of sniffer dogs, offering a comparative analysis of different management models. Lastly, this review addresses the challenges inherent in applying sniffer dogs to water leak detection, poses pertinent research questions concerning sniffer dog training and the expansion of odour fingerprint, and presents a forward-looking perspective on the subject.
Article
Full-text available
Conservation detection dog handler teams (CDDHTs) offer many potential benefits to the world of conservation. Seabird populations are an important component of marine ecosystems. However, they are threatened by several anthropogenic activities, including the introduction of invasive species. Although CDDHT can support seabird conservation through invasive species management efforts and population assessments, they are under-utilised. A lack of methodological standardisation within CDDHT work and the under-publishing of their use within seabird research leads to difficulties in conducting new CDDHT seabird-related studies due to an inability to learn from previous research. This study aimed to address these shortcomings by investigating the techniques and methods used by those actively working with, or planning to work with, CDDHT on a seabird project to better understand them, and propose best practices in the field. Seven professionals who have used, or will use, CDDHT as part of a seabird project (four handlers, three ecologists/researchers) participated in structured written surveys which were thematically analysed. Five superordinate themes emerged from the survey data: Training, Location, Role of Handler, Wildlife Considerations, and Dog Selection Criteria, with the first two themes having several subordinate themes. A summary of best practices was developed from the findings, with notable recommendations including preparation across all project elements, networking with other professionals, and making judgments on the use of techniques like discrimination and field trials based on the specific project and dog(s). These results can serve to benefit future seabird studies involving CDDHT as well as supporting the development of standardisation in the CDDHT field.
Chapter
Dogs as research assistants in wildlife conservation have been used since the twentieth century. The experimental and quasi-experimental research on their efficiency, accuracy, and reliability is more recent and does not exceed a few decades. We start by discussing the potential and overlooked conceptual and methodological contributions of psychophysics to the training and assessment of wildlife conservation canines. We then briefly discuss issues around transects in search and question the validity and relevance of this anthropocentric approach. The remainder of this chapter is a comprehensive and critical review of the literature. An important part of this review is a summary table (appendix) of the main research we collated identifying the specific goals, species, methods, and the overall results and conclusions of each peer-reviewed publication.
Article
Full-text available
From 1993-1995, we captured Yellow Rails (Coturnicops noveboracensis) and searched for their nests in southern Quebec. All rails were caught at night, when males call constantly, by attracting birds with imitated calls (waiting) or by approaching birds and immobilizing them with a powerful beam of light (approaching). We were successful 183 times, capturing and recapturing 111 individuals. Most captures (66.6%) were made by waiting, 9.3% by approaching, and 24.0% by approaching after an unsuccessful attempt by waiting. Capture success was higher when we combined waiting and approaching than when we used either waiting or approaching alone. In 1994 - 1995, capture success was 24% higher when we used the combination technique, compared to attempts made with the waiting technique alone. We captured 81.6% of rails in those years and up to seven in a single night. The combination of waiting and approaching was more effective in 1994 - 1995 than in 1993 (X2 = 7.20; P < 0.01), possibly owing to experience acquired in 1993 and to a larger net being used the last two years. In 1994-1995, waiting was more effective on rails captured for the first time than on rails previously captured (X2 = 7.66; P < 0.01), probably because birds previously caught are warier. We searched for nests with the help of a German short-haired pointer and a French pointer. In 1994, they searched for 18.5 and 7.8 h, respectively, and in 1995 the German short-haired searched for 8.8 h. Only the German short-haired pointer found nests, three in 3 h in 1994 and two in 2.5 h in 1995. We believe the effectiveness of this type of search depends on the dog's abilities and training, the dog's handler, and probably weather conditions.
Article
Full-text available
Laboratory blind trials were conducted to evaluate the ability of beagles and an electronic odor device to detect termites in wood. In the first test, pine blocks artificially infested with either 0, 5, 50, or 200 workers of the western subterranean termite, Reticulitermes hesperus Banks, were randomly presented to five beagles and one electronic odor device. Blocks were presented one at a time to beagles and the electronic odor device. The beagles correctly identified 81 percent of the blocks while the electronic odor device correctly identified 48 percent. A second laboratory test comparing five additional electronic odor devices from the same manufacturer resulted in a slightly higher correctly identified value, 62 percent. Beagles performed best for blocks containing 50 or more termites. However, the percentage of misidentified controls (false positives) for beagles was high, 28 percent. The electronic odor device did not demonstrate statistically significant detection ability. Although beagles were almost perfect (49 of 50 blocks) in detecting termites in blocks with 50 or more individuals, neither detection method was reliable with control samples or with samples with a low number of termites. The implications of these findings to the use of either of these termite detection methods are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
The accidental introduction of the Brown Tree Snake Boiga irregularis to Guam has resulted in the extirpation of most of the island's native terrestrial vertebrates, has created a health hazard to infants and children, and has resulted in economic losses. Cargo inspections using teams of handlers and their detector dogs form a last line of defense for preventing Brown Tree Snake dispersal from Guam. To assess the efficacy of the teams of handlers and their dogs for locating stowed Brown Tree Snakes, we planted Brown Tree Snakes (in escape-proof containers) in cargo without the knowledge of the handlers inspecting the cargo. We found that when an observer attended the inspection to monitor procedures, 80% of the planted snakes were located. Without an attending observer present, 70% of the planted snakes were discovered, but only after such plantings had become a routine procedure. Prior to the routine planting of snakes, efficacy was nearly 50% less (38%). The reasons some planted snakes were missed by the dog teams were split between: an insufficient search pattern by the handler, or the dog giving no discernable indication that a snake was present.
Article
We estimated black bear (Ursus americanus) population density in the Blue Mountains of northeastern Oregon based on summer mark-recapture surveys in 1996 and 1997. We developed a mark-recapture technique to estimate black bear density using houndsmen with dogs to detect bear scent on driving transects. We conducted 53 surveys and recorded 72 instances where dogs detected bear scent (a strike). We used strike frequency as a bear density index. Strike frequency did not differ between years; dogs detected bear scent at a rate of 1.47 strikes/20 km in 1996 and 1.18 strikes/20 km in 1997. We recorded each scent detection and pursued every bear to determine if the bear was marked or unmarked. On 55 occasions bears were treed after being pursued by dogs from transect routes; 33 of these were marked and 22 were unmarked. Applying the NOREMARK software, we estimated 59 bears in 1996 and 48 bears in 1997 in the 234-km2 survey area. We calculated a density of 25.2 bears/100 km2 in 1996 and 20.5 bears/100 km2 in 1997 in the survey area. This compares with a minimum known population density based on 3 years of marking bears prior to this study of 10.8 bears/100 km2 in the 485 km2 capture area that encompassed the survey area. We believe this technique holds promise as a practical tool for wildlife managers. Potential applications of this technique are to produce an index of black bear population trends or, in combination with other techniques, to more accurately assess density of a black bear population. The advantages, considerations, costs, concerns, and limitations of this technique are discussed.
Article
Canines trained to alert to traces of flammable liquids at a fire scene are useful to identify locations to collect samples for laboratory analysis. In some instances, no samples are collected or laboratory testing of samples collected following a canine scent alert fails to identify a residual flammable liquid and potential accelerant. In these, an attempt may be made, through testimony of the dog's handler, to introduce at trial, information regarding the canine alert to indicate the presence of an ignitable liquid at the scene. Canine handlers contend that the dog has greater sensitivity to typical accelerants than laboratory techniques but scientists counter that, while sensitive, the specificity of canine detection is unknown. Unverified canine indications have been used in a number of cases and challenges to several of these have reached the appellate level. Examination of court decisions on admissibility of canine alerts shows that they have been as varied as the arguments pro and con.
Article
It is estimated that over 200 accelerant detection canines (ADCs) are currently assisting in fire investigations throughout the United States. On many occasions, their ability and reliability have been called into question. The Pinellas County Forensic Laboratory evaluated 42 accelerant detection canine teams in their ability to discriminate between common accelerants and pyrolysis products, to detect common accelerants at low concentrations, to precisely locate accelerants and, to detect different classes of accelerants. Ultimately, the accuracy, dependability and overall effectiveness varied from canine to canine and handler to handler and appeared to be somewhat limited by the canine's training, handling and maintenance. While most of the canine teams performed extremely well and could be an asset to fire investigation, some proved to be unreliable. A universal endorsement or condemnation of all accelerant detection canines could not be made; however, endorsements of specific canine teams and trainers were possible. Every working canine team should be evaluated independently. Routine testing is imperative to establish the canine abilities and limitations.
Article
Unlocking The Secrets Of Supersniffing Dogs In Sydney, Australia, Jamie begins a busy day with grooming, exercise, and a health check. He then travels to the airport for the day's first task, inspecting cargo from a number of jumbo jets parked on the tarmac. From the airport, he moves to the location where mail is imported to check half a million or so articles. Next, he goes to the waterfront to search a ship—and the day isn't over yet. Jamie, an athletic, black Labrador retriever trained by the Australian Customs Service to search for narcotics, epitomizes the unrivaled ability of dogs to locate objects through their sense of smell. Yet, it is not fully understood how dogs do what they do and why they are so good at it. Basic studies of canine olfaction and breeding are helping researchers figure out the secrets of the dog's exquisite detection capabilities. They are also generating knowledge ...