ArticlePDF Available

Academic integrity: Online classes compared to face-to-face classes

Authors:

Abstract

Trends toward an increase in online courses suggest the need for more research on differing levels of cheating and other acts of academic disintegrity as compared to face-to-face classes. We surveyed 639 students in both types of classes. Students felt it was easier to cheat in online classes than face-to-face classes. For students taking both online and face-to-face classes, we found that cheating occurred more frequently in online classes. However, students who took only online classes were less likely to cheat than students who took only face-to face classes. The relationship of age to taking online classes and cheating offered an explanation for the contradictory finding. Sex differences in cheating behavior were absent.
Academic Integrity: Online Classes Compared to
Face-to-Face Classes
Arden Miller and Adena D. Young-Jones
Trends toward an increase in online courses suggest the need for more research on
differing levels of cheating and other acts of academic disintegrity as compared to
face-to-face classes. We surveyed 639 students in both types of classes. Students felt
it was easier to cheat in online classes than face-to-face classes. For students taking
both online and face-to-face classes, we found that cheating occurred more frequently
in online classes. However, students who took only online classes were less likely
to cheat than students who took only face-to face classes. The relationship of age
to taking online classes and cheating offered an explanation for the contradictory
nding. Sex differences in cheating behavior were absent.
Arden Miller and Adena D. Young-Jones,
Psychology Department, Missouri State Uni-
versity.
Correspondence regarding this article should
be addressed to Arden Miller at ArdenMiller@
MissouriState.edu.
Paper presented at the meeting of the South-
western Psychological Association, April, 2012,
Oklahoma City, OK.
Since 2003, online enrollments have
grown 358%, and 31% of students now take
at least one course online (Allen & Seaman,
2011). But this research also shows that
about a third of academic leaders perceive
online outcomes to be inferior to traditional
classes and that faculty members have misgiv-
ings about online classes. These misgivings
include lack of course comparability, more
opportunities to cheat in online classes, and
a greater attraction to students whose goal
is to cheat (Bailey & Bailey, 2011). Young-
berg’s (2012) commentary in the Chronicle
of Higher Education argues that the number
one reason why online education will not
replace college is “It’s too easy to cheat.”
The majority of faculty (64%) and students
(57%) believe it is easier to cheat in online
classes (Kennedy, 2000).
Despite this common belief, there is a
lack of adequate research comparing aca-
demic disintegrity online (OL) to face-to-face
(FF) classes. Existing research has found
higher levels of cheating in online classes
(Lanier, 2006). But others have found lower
levels of cheating in online classes (Hart
& Morgan, 2010; Kidwell & Kent, 2008,
Stuber-McEwen, Wisely, & Hoggatt, 2009) or
cheating levels comparable to other research
studies of FF classes (Grijalwa, 2006, Watson
& Sottile, 2010). But comparing ndings
to other studies that estimate cheating in
traditional classes, as Grijalwa did, is a weak
methodology not suited to hypothesis testing.
Research has found lower levels of cheat-
ing in online classes may have been subject
to volunteer biases that inuence ndings. In
Hart’s (Hart & Morgan, 2010) study, the 44
participating students from traditional classes
represented 44% of the cohort, while the 330
students from online classes represented only
16% of the cohort. Similarly, Stuber-McEwen
et al. had a 100% response rate with face-
to-face students completing the survey in
class. Online students were emailed a link
from instructors, but the authors apparently
didn’t know how many were sent the emailed
in order to report a response rate. However,
surveys done through email links with no
incentive typically have a very low response
rate; often less than 10% (Miller, Shoptaugh,
& Parkerson, 2008). Research shows that
volunteerism is related to higher levels of
altruism (Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1975), and
thus may be selective for lower levels of cheat-
ing (Miller, et al., 2008). The online class
2/ Journal of Instructional Psychology, Vol. 39, No. 3
cheating assessments, having lower response
rates, would be more signicantly reduced
by volunteer biases. Similarly, Kidwell and
Kent (2008) had a much higher response rate
among traditional students (42%) relative to
online students (24.8%).
When comparing online students to
traditional students, the glaring differences
in attributes of online versus face-to-face
students that are plausible explanations for
any differences are often missed. Residential
students are more likely to be in the 18-22
range, while non-traditional and older stu-
dents are more likely to be enrolled in online
classes. For example, Dutton, Dutton, and
Perry (2002) found that the average age of
their students in lecture classes was 22.5 as
compared to the average age in online classes
of 27.6. Previous research has demonstrated
that older students are less likely to cheat
(Miller, Shoptaugh, & Parkerson, 2008). Past
research has also indicated that, in general,
undergraduates members of Greek social
organizations tend to cheat more (Iyer &
Eastman, 2006), and these students are likely
to be traditional, face-to-face students. A
variety of other attribute characteristics that
may differ between online and face-to-face
students could be determining factors behind
the inconsistency of ndings regarding cheat-
ing in online and face-to-face classes.
What seems to be missing from these
comparison studies is the fact that many stu-
dents take both sorts of classes. The benet
of surveying these students resides in the
control of attribute differences between online
and traditional classes, making the students
their own control. Our present research will
consider differences in cheating during online
and face-to-face classes for students enrolled
in both types of classes. We will also consider
between subject comparisons for students
having only one type class.
Additionally, we investigated whether
there were differences in online and face-
to-face student’s perceptions of how severe
consequences should be and beliefs about the
student’s responsibility to prevent cheating.
Previous research has found students who
cheat more believe consequences should
be less severe (Kufahl, Shoptaugh, Miller,
& Levesque, 2005) and demonstrate lower
levels of Academic Integrity Responsibility
(Miller, Shoptaugh, & Wooldrige, 2011).
Academic Integrity Responsibility (AIR) is
the extent to which it is believed that students
are responsible for deterrence of cheating
in coursework. Low scores indicate the
belief that promotion of academic integrity
is primarily or solely the responsibility of
the teacher.
The purpose of this study was to compare
online course cheating to face-to-face course
cheating using between subjects (students
enrolled in only one type) and within sub-
jects (students enrolled in both online and
face-to-face classes) comparisons, with an
established survey (Miller, Shoptaugh, &
Wooldridge, 2011). The survey has extra
items added to accommodate differences in
cheating that occur in an online class. Ad-
ditionally, comparisons will be made on the
AIR (Miller, et al., 2011).
Method
Participants
Participants were 531 undergraduates
and 108 graduate students from two south-
midwest universities. Extra credit was given
as determined by their individual instructor.
While 144 were solicited through an introduc-
tory psychology pool at one university, 279
participants from the same university and
214 participants from the second university
volunteered with varied incentives offered by
their instructor. Students were sent to a web
page that provided the consent form, with
consent acknowledged by entering the survey
web form. Median age was 22 with a range
from 17 to 56 with 67.5% of participants
being female. We received 639 responses.
Participants were fairly evenly distributed
across college class. Of these, 289 had both
types of classes, 246 had only face-to-face
E . . / 3
Table 1
Individual Cheating Item (1 = never) Differences Within Students Having Both Types of
Classes and Between Groups for Students with Only One Type of Class
Both
Types One Type
OL*FF OL*FF
1. Turning in work done by someone else. 1.06 1.07 1.01*1.09
2. UNauthorized use of the text or other book in answer-
ing items on a test, quiz, or other assessment. 1.45*1.14 1.42 1.20
3. UNauthorized use of a web search or other digital me-
dia in answering items on a test, quiz, or other assessment. 1.50*1.22 1.38 1.26
4. Writing or providing a paper or assignment for another
student. 1.17 1.12 1.06*1.25
5. Receiving help on an assignment that exceeds that
which would be acceptable to the teacher. 1.39 1.32 1.11*1.53
6. Getting questions or answers from someone who has
already taken a test. 1.34 1.41 1.06*1.50
7. Providing questions or answers to a student who will be
taking the test at a later time. 1.35 1.39 1.10*1.54
8. Helping someone else cheat during a quiz or exam. 1.20*1.12 1.07*1.23
9. Copying or getting help from another student during a
quiz or exam. 1.23 1.16 1.11 1.24
10. Paraphrasing (copying with rewording) a sentence
from a written or internet source without footnoting or
referencing it in the paper.
1.47*1.36 1.36*1.60
11. Copying a sentence directly from a written or internet
source without quotes and proper referencing. 1.20 1.20 1.14*1.31
12. Turning in a paper obtained in large part from a term
paper “mill” or website. 1.06 1.07 1.00*1.06
13. Using unpermitted crib notes (or cheat sheets) during
a test. 1.20*1.11 1.10 1.13
14. Altering a graded test and submitting it (as misgraded)
for extra credit. 1.07 1.05 1.00*1.07
15. Turning in a paper copied, at least in part, from an-
other student’s paper. 1.10 1.08 1.02 1.07
16. Using a false excuse to obtain an extension on a due
date or to take a test at a different time. 1.19 1.14 1.06*1.24
17. Participating in the exchange or sharing of a stolen
copy of the test. 1.08 1.05 1.02 1.09
18. Turning in a paper that you originally wrote for
another class without awareness of the professor regarding
its previous use.
1.14 1.16 1.04*1.20
* = p < .01 two tailed
4/ Journal of Instructional Psychology, Vol. 39, No. 3
classes, and 104 had only online classes.
Procedures
All items were completed in an html
formatted web survey. At the outset it was
made explicit that all responses were entirely
anonymous. The anonymous survey included
18 items to address categories of cheating
with choices of: “never”, “once”, “more than
once”, or “frequently”. With permission,
these items were derived from McCabe’s
surveys that have been widely used (McCabe
& Trevino, 1993). However the items have
evolved through two research studies (Miller,
Shoptaugh, & Parkerson, 2008; Miller,
Shoptaugh, & Wooldridge, 2011) and were
updated to address both online and face-to-
face classes, see Table 1. Participants also
completed a ve–item survey to assess Aca-
demic Integrity Responsibility (AIR) (Miller,
Shoptaugh, & Wooldridge, 2011). Students
were asked how often they witnessed cheat-
ing in the past year using the same choices
as above and whether they thought it was
easier to cheat in online classes (1 = strongly
disagree, 5 = strongly agree). Students also
gave their sex, age, class, and GPA.
After the anonymous survey was com-
pleted, students were taken to a new web
form which allowed them to enter their names
into a second database in order to receive
participation credit.
Results
Frequency of Cheating
While 15.7% disagreed, 57.2% agreed
that is easier to cheat in online classes. We
analyzed the accuracy of that belief in two
ways. Within-subject comparisons were made
with students having both types of classes
followed by between-subjects comparisons
for students having only online (OL) or only
face-to-face (FF) classes.
Table 2
Differences in Online Only, Face-to-face Only, and Student with Both Types of Classes
Class Type
Online
(N = 104)
Face-to-Face
(N = 246)
Both
(N = 289)
Variable M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Self-reported Cheating * 2.52 (4.45)a4.66 (6.18)b4.81 (6.44)b
Age ** 28.8(7.90) a 21.2(5.36) b 23.65(5.65) c
AIR (p < .05) 16.6(4.84) a 15.1(4.44) b 15.44(4.40) a b
Online Cheating is easier ** 3.08(1.31) a 3.88(1.13) b 3.72(1.20) b
Witnessed cheating** 1.74(1.06) a 2.29(1.11) b 1.99(1.03) b
* Difference signicant at the p < .01 level
** Difference signicant at the p < .001 level
abc Means with the same letter do not differ on Scheffe test
E . . / 5
Students taking both types of classes
reported more cheating in OL classes, M =
4.15 than in FF classes, M = 3.15, t (288) =
4.35, p < .001. The fact that these subjects
took signicantly more FF credits, M = 21.9,
than OL credits, M = 17.8, t (289) = -6.73, p
< .001, demonstrates that cheating frequency
ndings cannot be explained by differences
in number of credit hours completed. To the
contrary it raises the possibility that these
differences could be underestimated.
Secondly, we made between subject
comparisons for students having only one
type of class. We found lower rates of cheat-
ing in the only-OL students, M = 2.52, than
in only-FF students, M = 4.66, t(265.4) =
-3.64, p < .001. Number of hours were not
signicantly different, t(348) = -1.85.
Since the between subjects ndings
differed from within subjects ndings, we
explored the most obvious attribute difference
between online only and face-to-face only:
age. Our introduction reviewed the evidence
that online students are older on the average
and that older students cheat less. When
age was entered into the regression alone,
the standardized regression coefcient was
substantial, b* = -.235, t(346) = -4.51, p <
.001. When entering class type second in the
regression, the effects the differences between
the two groups was no longer signicant, b*
= .069, t(345) = 1.16.
Literature often describes cheating data
in percentages who have cheated. Fewer OL
only students cheated, 51.9% than FF only
students 71.5%, χ2(1) = 12.49, p < .001.
This is likely due to the older age of the OL
only students. For students with both types of
classes, we compared cheating within subjects
and found more students had cheated OL
classes, 64.7%, than in FF classes, 49.1%,
χ2(1) = 14.3, p < .001.
An item by item view of differences
for each type of cheating behavior in Table
1 shows how these specic behaviors dif-
fer in OL and FF classes. In general there
appears to be more unauthorized use of the
crib notes, text, and web searches in online
courses for students taking both types of
classes. However, students in only face-to-
face classes are more likely to use someone
else’s work or provide it to another student,
receive improper help in completing an as-
signment, get questions from those who have
taken the test and give questions to others,
and misuse the internet relative to students
who take only online classes.
Differences in Online, Face-to-Face, and
Students with Both
To conduct an analysis of variance
comparing the three groups, a cheating score
for students with both types of classes was
counted as their highest cheating rate for either
the OL courses or the FF courses. Signicant
ndings were explored using Scheffe post hoc
tests. Students in OL courses cheated less than
others, F(2, 636) = 5.90, p < .01, see Table
2. Students taking OL classes were older,
F(2, 633) = 59.31, p < .001 and witnessed
less cheating in the past year, F(2, 636) =
10.9, p < .001. They were more inclined to
take responsibility for the integrity environ-
ment, scoring higher on Academic Integrity
Responsibility (AIR), F(2, 635) = 4.11, p <
.05. OL-only students were less likely to
believe that it is easy to cheat in OL than in
FF classes, F(2, 628) = 16.3, p < .001.
Sex Differences
There were no signicant sex differences
or interactions with sex for any measures of
cheating behavior. Females scored higher
on AIR, M = 15.9 than males, M =14.73,
t(634) = -2.95.
To consider arguments that differences in
ndings on sex often follow from differences
in populations, we analyzed sex differences
in cheating for each student source. While
there was a non-signicant trend for males,
M = 5.33, to cheat more than females, M =
3.95, in the population from the second uni-
versity, t(212) =1.58, the opposite marginally
signicant trend, females cheating more, M =
6/ Journal of Instructional Psychology, Vol. 39, No. 3
5.96 than males, M = 3.95, was found among
introductory psychology students at the rst
university, t(142) =1.68, p < .10, with no
such trends in the second population at the
rst university, t(277) = .32.
Other Correlations
Older students were less likely to cheat,
more likely to take responsibility for academic
integrity, perceived consequences should be
more severe, and witnessed less cheating, see
Table 3. This table shows a variety of correla-
tions relevant to understanding cheating in OL
and FF classes. Higher Academic Integrity
Responsibility is related to a preference for
more severe consequences, less cheating, and
less witnessing of cheating.
Discussion
Within the academic community, it is
commonly believed that cheating is more
likely to occur in online classes than face-
to-face classes. Such pervasive notions exist
despite a lack within the literature to support
this comparative idea. Our study builds on
previous research, which has attempted to
compare OL and FF cheating, by using a
between subjects and within subjects design
of participants taking both types of classes (n
= 289), only FF (n = 246), and only OL (n =
104). While the overall consensus agreed that
cheating is easier in online classes (57.2%),
there is a level of complexity to this asser-
tion. Specically, our ndings indicate that
students taking both types of classes are
more likely to cheat in their online classes.
However, a seemingly contradictory nding
occurred when we considered students who
only took OL or only FF classes, because stu-
dents who took only OL classes cheated less
than other students. The ndings showed that
the population who take only online classes
are older, take more Academic Integrity
Responsibility, and cheat less.
The present research supports previ-
ous ndings that cheating occurs within the
academic setting. However, specic cheating
behaviors differ for students taking both types
of classes and only FF courses. Students in
both types of classes were signicantly more
likely to report the usage of cheat sheets
during tests, paraphrasing without proper
Table 3
Correlations Between Selected Variables
AIR 2 3 4 5 6
1. AIR - -.275* .369* -.339* -.116*
-.083
2. Cheating - -.276* .376* .150*
0.058
3. Consequence - -.137* 0
0.018
4. Witnessed - .198*
-0.062
5. OL cheating easier -
-0.077
6. GPA
-
* p < .01
E . . / 7
citation, assisting others cheat, and unauthor-
ized use of text or web in answering items.
An overlap occurs for only FF students in
helping someone else cheat and paraphrasing
without appropriate citations. Additionally,
only FF students are more likely to turn in
work done by someone else, complete work
for someone else, give/receive inappropriate
help, use a false excuse, or submit previous
work in subsequent classes.
The pattern of correlations suggests
that there is a culture or social component
to cheating. Students who cheat more also
witness more cheating and do not perceive
they have any role in reducing cheating. This
could suggest acceptance of cheating in many
academic subcultures. Findings of higher
rates of cheating in fraternities and sororities
supports the notion of disintegrity-accepting
subcultures (Iyer & Eastman, 2006).
While some studies report males cheat
more than females, and a sex differences is
often presumed, many studies, including this
one, failed to nd sex differences in cheating.
Miller, et al. (2008) argue that the differences
in these ndings occur primarily due to sex
differences in volunteerism and as these dif-
ferences are very small and unreliable; sex
should not be considered a signicant factor in
cheating behavior. The fact that three different
sources for participants resulted in minimal
but diverse sex differences underscores the
weakness of any expectations about cheating
behavior as a function of sex.
While we found signicant results in the
present study, limitations exist regarding the
nature of sampling. Participants volunteered
for extra credit points; individuals who desire
extra credit may have different characteristics
than those who do not wish to participate.
While using non-volunteers is ethically
problematic, varieties in incentive strength
may inuence the responding population (cf.,
Miller et al, 2008). Additional research should
also extend the understanding of disintegrity
subcultures and explore methods to prevent
such disintegrity. As there is an increasing
trend toward online courses, extended re-
search within this domain is necessary.
An additional weakness resides in the
selection of disintegity survey items. The
more comprehensive the survey, the higher
the rates of cheating that are typically reported
(Miller et al., 2008). If the survey were more
comprehensive in covering forms of cheating
common in one type of class than in another,
this could generate signicant differences in
cheating rates. Particularly when we consider
differences in how students might cheat in an
online class, attention must be paid to com-
prehensive coverage in surveying disintegrity.
It is common in the literature to report
cheating as percentages of students who have
cheated and we included that statistic in our
results. Although that is useful for compar-
ing the results of different studies, it can be
misleading. If a treatment reduced students
cheating from 12 times per semester to one
or two times per semester, it would not im-
pact the percentage who have cheated. Yet
treatments to prevent frequent cheating are
probably more important than a treatment
that affects a person who would cheat once.
Unfortunately, a common metric, while desir-
able for discussions, is not very practical for
testing hypotheses.
Regardless of teaching modality, educa-
tors should be aware that cheating occurs at
rather high levels. Overall, despite perpetual
reminders that disintegrity is not acceptable, it
is actually quite common within the academic
setting. Deterrence of cheating in online
classes requires attention to new strategies
that may be different from conventional
classes. It appears that professors must be
as, or more, vigilant in addressing cheating
in online classes.
References
Allen, I., & Seaman, J. (2011). Going the
Distance: Online Education in the USA 2011
Wellesley MA: Babson Survey Research Group
Bailey, W. C., & Bailey, S. (2011). Do online
and lecture students view cheating differently?
Review of Business Research, 11(5), 33-45.
8/ Journal of Instructional Psychology, Vol. 39, No. 3
Dutton, J., Dutton, M., & Perry, J. (2002).
How do online students differ from lecture
students? Journal of Asynchronous Learning
Networks, 6(1), 1-20.
Grijalva, T. C., (2006). Academic honesty
and online courses. College Student Journal,
40, 180-185.
Hart, L., & Morgan, L. (2010) Academic
Integrity in an online registered nurse to baccalau-
reate in nursing program. Journal of Continuing
Education in Nursing, 41, 498-505.
Iyer, R. & Eastman, J. K. (2006). Academic
dishonesty: Are business students different from
other college students? Journal of Education for
Business, 82(2), 101-110.
Kennedy, K. (2000). Academic dishonesty
and distance learning: Student and faculty views.
College Student Journal, 34, 309-314.
Kidwell, L. A. & Kent, J. (2008). Integrity
at a distance: A study of academic misconduct
among university students on and off campus.
Accounting Education: An International Journal,
17, Supplement, S3-S16.
Kufahl, L., Shoptaugh, C., Miller, A., &
Levesque, C. (March, 2005). An evaluation of aca-
demic honesty attitudes, behaviors and correlates.
Paper presented at the meeting of the Southwestern
Psychological Association, Memphis, TN.
Lanier, M. M. (2006). Academic integrity
and distance learning. Journal of Criminal Justice
Education, 17, 244-261.
McCabe, D. L., & Trevino, L. K. (1993).
Academic dishonesty: Honor codes and other
contextual inuences. Journal of Higher Educa-
tion, 64, 522-538.
Miller, A.T., Shoptaugh, C, & Parkerson, A.
(2008). Underreporting of cheating in research
using volunteer college students. College Student
Journal, 42, 326-339.
Miller, A.T., Shoptaugh, C, & Wooldridge, J.
(2011). Reasons not to cheat, academic-integrity
responsibility, and frequency of cheating. The
Journal of Experimental Education, 79(2), 169-
184.
Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R.L. (1975). The
Volunteer Subject. New York: Wiley.
Stuber-McEwen, D., Wiseley, P., & Hog-
gatt, S. (2009) Point, click, and cheat: frequency
and type of academic dishonesty in the virtual
classroom. Online Journal of Distance Learning
Administration, 12 (3), retrieved from http://www.
westga.edu/~distance/ojdla/fall123/stuber123.
html.
Watson, G. & Sottile, J. (2010) Cheating in
the digital age: Do students cheat more in online
courses? Online Journal of Distance Learning
Administration, 13(1), retrieved from http://
www.westga.edu/~distance/ojdla/spring131/
watson131.pdf
Youngberg, D. (2012, August 13). Why
online education won’t replace college—Yet.
The Chronicle of Higher Education, retrieved
from http://chronicle.com/article/Why-Online-
Education-Wont/133531/?cid=wc&amp;utm_
source=wc&amp;utm_medium=en.
... Prior to the pandemic the problem of academic dishonesty in online courses has long been an issue of concern (Peterson, 2019; Kolowich, 2016, McCabe et al., 2012, King et al., 2009, Lanier, 2006. Some authors claim that cheating is more prevalent in online than in live courses (Young, 2012, Miller & Young-Jones, 2012. We may therefore assume that 'online scores are likely inflated by cheating' (Dendir & Maxwell, 2020). ...
... There is 'prevailing disparity between the amount of actual cheating and the perception of academic dishonesty' (Watson & Sottile 2010). The reasons why students decide to cheat are numerous and depend on gender, personal needs, age and cultural rules (Miller & Young-Jones, 2012, Yu Niiya et al., 2008, Humbarger & DeVaney, 2005, Kohlberg, 1971).Since the start of the pandemic universities have been struggling with finding effective ways to minimize cheating during online exams. Different authors have suggested using various deterrents to decrease academic dishonesty. ...
... In fact, 14 of the 30 outliers with very high scores on the online test were females. Gender does not seem to play a role in cheating behavior (Miller & Young-Jones, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
Universities have long been struggling with academic dishonesty in both online and pen-and-paper examinations. Different authors have suggested various deterrents to decrease cheating during exams. The aim of this study is to investigate how the online environment affects academic dishonesty during online exams and to compare students’ behaviour during written and paper exams. The hypotheses tested is that there is a significant difference between mean values of the results achieved on pen-and-paper tests and online tests. The research adopted a cross-sectional study design. The Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test confirmed the hypothesis as it showed that the scores on the online exam (mean rank = 35.9) were statistically higher than the ones on the pen-and-paper test (mean rank = 28.3), Z= -3.311, p=0.001 with a small effect size r = 0.29. This could be due to the test format and insufficient proctoring technology. Online cheating could be minimized by giving priority to formative assessment, by raising students’ awareness of the negative consequences of academic dishonesty, and implementing more sophisticated technologies to track students’ behavior during online exams. Additionally, multiple-choice questions should be replaced by open-ended questions. Finally, a speaking section could be added to online tests, which is to be passed successfully in order for students to receive a passing grade for the EFL course.
... One possible explanation for this finding is that online-only students previously tended to be older, more mature, and more motivated and therefore less likely to engage in academic dishonesty (Kidwell and Kent 2008;Ladyshewsky 2015). Conversely, students enrolled in both online and face-to-face courses believe more cheating occurs in online environments (Miller and Young-Jones 2012). The pandemic induced shift to online learning affected all of higher education, not just those selfselecting into online programs, necessitating a reexamination of these attitudes and findings. ...
... Turning to general student perceptions of academic dishonesty, in Table 4, Panel A, 63 percent (37) participants agree that academic dishonesty is more prevalent during online exams (mean of 3.68, t-statistic ¼ 4.84, p < 0.01). This result complements similarly reported prior findings (King et al. 2009;Watson and Sottile 2010;Miller and Young-Jones 2012). Students are also asked whether they know of a peer(s) who engaged in academic dishonesty while taking an exam, and 29 percent (17) of respondents are aware. ...
Article
As accounting programs increase their online offerings, understanding the challenges of maintaining academic integrity online is crucial. This study documents an emerging method of online academic dishonesty—on-demand services from academic resource sites (ARS) such as Chegg.com. ARS are web-based repositories of textbook problems, homework solutions, etc., and many of them employ subject-matter experts to answer questions in real time, potentially during active exams. In periods of fewer online exam safeguards, 13–25 percent of intermediate accounting students are identified as using Chegg during exams. Corroborating evidence shows an anomalous improvement in student performance in online exams with minimal safeguards, which is attenuated by an increase in mitigation policies. Survey responses confirm that students are familiar with and use ARS, including 10 percent who acknowledge use during quizzes or exams. These findings help formulate suggestions about practices educators can employ to decrease pervasive use of ARS in online learning. JEL Classifications: M49.
... Lanier (2006) found that cheating was more prevalent in online courses, with Watson and Sottile (2010) finding that students were four times more likely to cheat in online classes than in person. While more investigation among specific student populations is needed (Holden et al., 2021), there is evidence that online assessments provide more opportunities for academic cheating (Miller & Young-Jones, 2012). Online assessment may result in increased plagiaristic behaviours (Clarke et al., 2023), while access to the Internet for students has been described as precipitating a loss of control over assessment integrity (St-Onge et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
In this article we report on a study of higher education students’ (N = 256) perceptions on the willingness, pressure, and frequency of their peers to cheat in online assessments at an Australian university in Singapore during the COVID-19 induced Online Teaching and Assessment period (COTA). MANOVA was used to identify the differences in perception between COTA and In- Person Teaching and Assessment (IPTA), as well as differences between academic disciplines and stages of study. The findings demonstrate that students perceived an increase across all areas of online cheating during COTA, and that these perceptions varied significantly by discipline but not by stage of study. Inductive qualitative thematic analysis was then used to explore the reasons behind the perceived increases, identifying themes related to anonymity, material access, pressure to achieve, lack of consequences, and peer group access. The implications of this research offer deeper insight into assessment security, design, and student concerns during emergency online teaching periods which can inform institutional policies in the future.
... Many health professions programs changed to elements of online learning during and post COVID (Kumar et al., 2021;Naciri et al., 2021;Schmutz et al., 2021). Results are mixed as to which may be more effective for student, but issues around student motivation, engagement and academic integrity may be relevant (Miller & Young-Jones, 2012;Platt et al., 2014). This will be an area to watch as more studies report on the effect of online learning compared to face-to-face learning in general, as well as with distributed practice and retrieval practice. ...
Article
Full-text available
To determine the effect of distributed practice (spacing out of study over time) and retrieval practice (recalling information from memory) on academic grades in health professions education and to summarise a range of interventional variables that may affect study outcomes. A systematic search of seven databases in November 2022 which were screened according to predefined inclusion criteria. The Medical Education Research Study Quality Instrument (MERSQI) and Newcastle-Ottawa Scale-Education (NOS-E) were used to critically appraise eligible articles. A summary of interventional variables includes article content type, strategy type, assessment type and delay and statistical significance. Of 1818 records retrieved, 56 were eligible for inclusion and included a total of 63 experiments. Of these studies, 43 demonstrated significant benefits of distributed practice and/or retrieval practice over control and comparison groups. Included studies averaged 12.23 out of 18 on the MERSQI and averaged 4.55 out of 6 on the NOS-E. Study designs were heterogeneous with a variety of interventions, comparison groups and assessment types. Distributed practice and retrieval practice are effective at improving academic grades in health professions education. Future study quality can be improved by validating the assessment instruments, to demonstrate the reliability of outcome measures. Increasing the number of institutions included in future studies may improve the diversity of represented study participants and may enhance study quality. Future studies should consider measuring and reporting time on task which may clarify the effectiveness of distributed practice and retrieval practice. The stakes of the assessments, which may affect student motivation and therefore outcomes, should also be considered.
Chapter
The outbreak of the Covid-19 pandemic in 2020 triggered a paradigm shift from in-person to online teaching, learning and assessment procedures to ensure the continuity of academic activities. Generally, the shift to online learning forced higher education institutions to become particularly innovative regarding online assessments. Securing online assessments against academic misconduct was a major concern as the online assessment practices conducted in the sector during the Covid-19 lockdown were questioned in many research studies that drew attention to the issues of quality and integrity in online assessments. Therefore, proctoring software was used frequently during the Covid-19 pandemic, as most universities shifted temporarily to online learning. However, remote, online assessment and the use of proctoring software are unlikely to disappear completely, even though most students have returned to in-person learning at universities. For the study on which this chapter was based, journal articles and book chapters that were focused on the use of proctoring software in higher education were reviewed to answer two research questions raised in the study. To ensure in-depth research, 10,136 journals articles we searched and six reports were identified that satisfied the criteria for inclusion. The findings of the analysis of these studies showed that institutions of higher education are relying on the potential of technologies to proctor online examinations. However, there are still some challenges relating to the process of online proctoring. Based on these findings, some recommendations are made.
Article
Full-text available
Online teaching has gained more momentum since the outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic. While this mode offers many benefits, one major concern is maintaining academic integrity, as online instruction can provide more opportunities for cheating. This study aimed to explore students’ attitudes toward cheating in online assessments (OAs) and any potential differences based on gender and nationality. Since our purpose was to perform a cross-cultural examination of cheating behaviors in an academic environment, we conducted the study in culturally diverse countries. The participants were 629 university students from Iran, Romania, and Lebanon. They completed a questionnaire about academic integrity in OAs. The results showed that 60% of the participants had no negative views on cheating in OAs, 58.5% admitted to cheating in OAs themselves, and 85% viewed OAs as less reliable than in-person assessments. During OAs, the most common ways of cheating included using notes on paper, relying on course materials, and sharing answers through social media and messaging apps. The main motivations for cheating included stress, time constraints, and the desire to achieve a higher grade, while factors that deterred cheating included moral and social stigma and the rights of other students. The study found no significant difference in attitudes toward cheating in OAs between male and female participants, but there were significant differences between students of different nationalities (p < 0.05).
Article
Full-text available
La Universidad Estatal a Distancia es una universidad pública de educación superior a distancia, donde actividades como la aplicación de las pruebas (exámenes) y tutorías eran presenciales; mientras que la entrega de tareas, el seguimiento a personas estudiantes, entre otras, se llevaban a cabo a través de plataformas virtuales. En el contexto de la Educación Remota de Emergencia (ERE) por la situación sanitaria mundial, se migraron todas las actividades anteriores a la virtualidad. El objetivo de esta experiencia consistió en identificar los tipos de deshonestidad académica más comunes en las asignaturas de Química y los resultados de una encuesta realizada a las personas estudiantes sobre el tema, para la presentación de acciones como medio de mitigación, disminución y reflexión de este fenómeno. La identificación de las actividades de deshonestidad académica se realizó a través de comparación de tareas y pruebas, denuncias por parte de estudiantes o terceros y experiencia docente. Las acciones llevadas a cabo por parte del área de Química consistieron en capacitación docente, modificación de bancos de ítems y diversificación de los tipos de evaluaciones. En ausencia de valores como la ética, la honestidad y el respeto a la propiedad intelectual, es necesario incluirlos como ejes trasversales en las asignaturas y realizar estrategias como campañas de honestidad e inclusión de talleres o asignaturas de ética en la malla curricular de las asignaturas, como medio para prevenir o minimizar estas acciones de deshonestidad académica en profeso de formación
Article
Full-text available
"Going the Distance: Online Education in the United States, 2011" is the ninth annual report on the state of online learning in U.S. higher education. The survey is designed, administered and analyzed by the Babson Survey Research Group. Data collection is conducted in partnership with the College Board. This year's study, like those for the previous eight years, is aimed at answering fundamental questions about the nature and extent of online education. Based on responses from more than 2,500 colleges and universities, the study addresses: Is online learning strategic? After remaining steady for several years, the proportion of chief academic officers saying that online education is critical to their long-term strategy took an upward turn in both 2010 and 2011. Sixty-five percent of all reporting institutions said that online learning was a critical part of their long-term strategy, a small increase from sixty-three percent in 2010. The year-to-year change was greatest among the for-profit institutions, which increased from fifty-one percent agreeing in 2009 to sixty-nine percent in 2011. For-profit institutions are the most likely to have included online learning as a part of their strategic plan. (Contains 4 footnotes.) [For the previous report, "Class Differences: Online Education in the United States, 2010," see ED529952.]
Article
Full-text available
Academic dishonesty is an issue of concern for teachers, students, and institutions of higher education. It is often perceived that because students and faculty do not interact directly in web-based classes, cheating will be more abundant than that which would be observed in a traditional classroom setting. In this paper we provide initial evidence of the magnitude of cheating in online courses. To estimate cheating in a single online class, we merge data from a student randomized response survey on cheating behavior with class-specific information provided by faculty. For our sample of students in a large public university, we find evidence that academic dishonesty in a single online class is no more pervasive than in traditional classrooms. We attribute this finding to the way online courses are designed, which may reduce the need for cheating, and that panic cheating, a typical form of cheating found in traditional classes, is less likely to occur in online classes.
Article
Full-text available
The latest trend in academia has been the rapid and large growth of online or distance learning courses. There are numerous benefits both for students as well as for the institutions. Despite the increasing reliance on this pedagogy, little research attention has focused on the potential for academic dishonesty. This study surveyed 1,262 students at a large, state-funded university and examined the prevalence of cheating in traditional lecture courses and online courses. The findings indicate that cheating was much more prevalent in online classes compared to traditional lecture courses. Moreover, results showed significant differences based on a number of demographic variables. The paper concludes with a discussion of policy suggestions and research recommendations.
Article
Full-text available
Academic integrity and misconduct have been the subject of increased interest in universities and for the public at large. Many studies have examined cheating behaviours to determine which forms of misconduct are most prevalent, which students perceive to be most serious, which academic disciplines have higher cheating rates, and what factors influence a student's propensity to cheat. Such research has taken place in traditional colleges and universities where students study on campus and have regular contact with other students and educators. However, the increasing popularity of distance education has raised new concerns over academic integrity among students not on campus. This paper reports on a study that explored academic misconduct amongst the student cohort at an Australian university with an extensive distance education program. Using a survey instrument previously developed in the USA, students were asked about a number of types of academic misconduct, their prevalence, and their seriousness. The study found that distance students are far less likely to engage in academic misconduct. Reasons for this finding are explored within the paper.
Article
Cheating is a major concern on many college campuses. For example, Davis, Grover, Becker, and McGregor (1992) reported that between 40% and 60% of their student respondents reported cheating on at least one examination. The 1990s also witnessed the unprecedented growth of distance learning and Web-based courses. Because students and faculty do not interact directly in such classes, they offer a unique venue for academic dishonesty. The present project explored student and faculty views concerning cheating and distance learning. The results indicated that both faculty and students believe it is easier to cheat in distance learning classes. Additional factors that impact the perceived ease of cheating in these classes are evaluated. (PDF) Academic dishonesty and distance learning: Student and faculty views. Available from: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/236158886_Academic_dishonesty_and_distance_learning_Student_and_faculty_views [accessed Dec 17 2020].
Article
The authors investigated the relations among reasons students gave for why they would not cheat in response to a cheating vignette, self-reported cheating, and the extent to which students take responsibility for promoting academic integrity. The authors surveyed 1,086 graduate and undergraduate students. Students who said they would not cheat because of punitive consequences were more likely to report that they cheated in classes and took less responsibility for promoting academic integrity. Students whose reasons related to the value of learning, personal character, and/or it being simply not right reported less cheating and took more responsibility for academic integrity. Academic-integrity responsibility correlated with less cheating. Results are discussed in terms of the effectiveness of punishment and the significance of internalizing integrity standards.
Article
In this article, the authors investigated academic dishonesty and how business students stand on the issue as compared with other college students. They found in their study that nonbusiness stu-dents are more likely to cheat than are busi-ness students. In general, students who are members of Greek social organizations, undergraduates, male, and have low self-esteem typically engage in higher levels of academic dishonesty. Only employment and innovativeness had an overall significant influence on academic dishonesty. any people in the educational system are concerned with the problem of academic dishonesty and the rate at which it is increasing (McCabe & Trevino, 1997; Park, 2003; Pullen, Ortloff, Casey, & Payne, 2000; Williams & Hosek, 2003). The estimate of how many students cheat varies dra-matically. McCabe and Trevino (1997) offer a range from 13% to 95%, and Park states that at least 50% of students cheat. In business literature, Kidwell, Wozniak, and Laurel (2003), and Chap-man, Davis, Toy, and Wright (2004) found that 75% of students reported cheating. Their findings are similar to the 63% found by Nonis and Swift (1998). Finally, there is concern that academic dishonesty is increasing because technology makes it easier for students to cheat (Born, 2003; Park; Scanlon, 2004). Academic dishonesty occurs in dif-ferent countries, with both undergradu-ate and graduate students, and in public and private schools of all sizes (Park, 2003). Even for schools with honor sys-tems, the number of code violations for cheating has increased since the mid-90s (Auer & Krupar, 2001). There also are multiple reasons why students cheat, and students rationalize and downplay the cheating done by themselves and their peers (Park). This issue of academic dishonesty is critical for business schools because it seems to mirror the growing concerns of ethical problems in the business community (Chapman et al., 2004; Kidwell et al., 2003). Those who cheat in college are more likely to cheat on the job (Swift & Nonis, 1998). Thus, there is an increased need for business schools to address academic dishonesty because what students learn as accept-able behavior in the classroom impacts their expectations of what is acceptable professionally. Furthermore, the costs for not addressing this issue are enor-mous (Kidwell et al.; Rawwas, Al-Khatib, & Vitell, 2004; Williams & Hosek, 2003). A concern of business faculty is what factors influence cheating (per-sonal, contextual, or situational). In this study, we tested a series of hypotheses with the aim of describing the academically dishonest student, and whether business students differ from nonbusiness students in terms of academic dishonesty.
Article
This study has two primary objectives. First, we want to know how students who enroll in online classes differ from their peers in traditional lecture classes. Our second objective involves both exploring what factors influence performance among online students, as well as whether those factors differ for online and lecture students. Our comparisons are of two large sections of a course in computer programming for which almost the only difference was that one section consisted of on-campus lectures, and the other section was online. We find that online students do differ from lecture students in a number of important characteristics. However, when we examine class performance and course completion, we find that the factors which influence performance seem to have a stronger impact on lecture students, but we cannot reject the hypothesis that factor coefficients are the same for the two groups.
Article
The number of nursing programs offering online courses continues to expand. This is a relatively new method of instruction that has not been extensively evaluated. Academic integrity in the online classroom is one area of concern. This study compared academic integrity in both an online and a traditional classroom registered nurse to baccalaureate in nursing (RN-BSN) program. A comparative descriptive design was used to evaluate academic integrity in the two RN-BSN cohorts. The traditional classroom RN-BSN students reported higher levels of cheating compared with the online students. Self-reported cheating behaviors were higher among younger students in the traditional classroom. This study did not support contemporary concerns that cheating is more prevalent in online courses.