ChapterPDF Available

Complementarity

Authors:
Markey, P. M., & Markey, C. N. (2009). Complementarity. In H. Reis & S.
Sprecher (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Human Relationships, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications.
Complementarity
Within the context of social relationships, the term complementarity has two
distinct and separate meanings. Of these two definitions, complementarity most
commonly refers to the premise that people tend to seek out other individuals with
characteristics that are different yet complementary from their own (a concept sometimes
called negative assortative mating). A second definition of complementarity is derived
from Interpersonal Theory and refers to the principle that during any dyadic interaction,
the behaviors of one person tend to elicit or constrain the behaviors of the other, and vice
versa (a concept sometimes called interpersonal complementarity). This entry describes
both of these definitions and provides an overview of the research examining
complementarity.
Negative Assortative Mating
The notion that people are generally attracted to persons who possess
characteristics that are dissimilar from their own is a prominent belief in popular culture
and is reflected in the common expression “opposites attract.” It is thought that such
differences are sought because it allows couples to divide tasks and pursue goals which
are consistent with each member’s personality without much conflict. For example, a
couple might experience little conflict if one member is motivated or driven by social
rewards (e.g., remembers birthdays, focuses on raising children, etc.) and the other is
focused on material rewards (e.g., earning a high income).
Although the notion of complementarity tends to be a common belief among lay
persons, research examining assortative mating suggests that people tend to be attracted
to others who are similar to themselves. It has been argued that such similarity might be
sought because it produces feelings of attachment, fosters a sense of equality, and allows
each partner to enhance or reinforce the self-concept of the (similar) other. Research has
found that men and women alike have a propensity to desire romantic partners similar to
themselves on traits such as extraversion, agreeableness, emotional stability,
conscientiousness, and openness to experience. Studies have also found preferences for
romantic partners who are similar with respect to certain demographic characteristics
such as age, ethnicity, religious background, height, weight, socioeconomic status,
values, political orientation, and physical qualities. As noted by David Buss, and in
contrast to the popular definition of complementarity, the notion that people desire
homogamy is one of the most replicated findings in human mating research.
Not only have studies suggested that individuals tend to desire a person who is
similar (i.e., not complementary) to themselves, but such similarity is also related to
relationship quality. Researchers have repeatedly found that similarity between romantic
partners on various characteristics (e.g., age, socioeconomic status, personality, etc.) is
linked to lower levels of divorce and higher levels of marital quality. Taken together, past
research suggests that this kind of complementarity is not often sought in romantic
partners nor does it typically lead to satisfying relationships.
Interpersonal Complementarity
Within the context of Interpersonal Theory the term complementarity refers to the
principle that an individual’s behavior tends to alter the behavior of his or her interaction
partner. This implies that interpersonal behaviors carry information regarding how the
other should respond, and thus, interpersonal behaviors can encourage or restrain
subsequent behavior from others. Although this notion of complementarity seems fairly
straightforward, there have been various models presented to define exactly how
complementarity occurs. Arguably, the two most common models are Robert Carson’s
model of complementarity and Jerry Wiggins’ model of complementarity. Both of these
models utilize the dimensions of warmth and dominance, which are often conceptualized
as the primary components of social behavior, in order to define complementarity.
Carson’s model of complementarity predicts that dominant or submissive
behaviors encourage the opposite behavior in interaction partners (i.e., dominance invites
submission, and submission invites dominance) and warm or cold behaviors encourage
similar response in interaction partners (i.e., warmth invites warmth, and coldness invites
coldness). For example, if person A acts in a warm and dominant manner, the likely
response of person B would be to complement this style of behavior by acting in a warm
and submissive manner. Wiggins’ model applies social exchange theory to the
dimensions of warmth and dominance in order to predict behaviors that are
complementary. This definition suggests that every behavior carries with it information
that grants or denies status (to the self and to the other) and grants or denies love (to the
self and to the other) and a complementarity behavior is its logical match. For example, a
dominant behavior tends to grant both status and love to the self, but only grants love
without status to the other. The complementary behavior would therefore be a warm
response, which tends to grant love without status to the self and both love and status to
the other.
Past research examining which of these two models best predicts interpersonal
behavior has been somewhat mixed. Researchers have found that the videotaped
behaviors of randomly paired strangers tend to occur in a manner predicted by Carson’s
model of complementarity. However, others have suggested that support for Carson’s
model only occurs along the warmth dimensions (i.e., dominance does not induce
submission, and vice versa). For example, researchers have found that participants who
interacted with a confederate coached to act in either a dominant or warm manner tended
to act is a similar manner in regard to warmth, but the dominant behavior of the
confederate failed to evoke submission in the participants.
In sum, complementarity is most frequently defined as the contention that people
tend to seek out other individuals with characteristics that are different from their own,
but is also associated with the distinctly different notion that, during any dyadic
interaction, the behaviors of both persons tend to elicit or constrain the behaviors of each
other.
Patrick M. Markey; Villanova University
Charlotte N. Markey; Rutgers University
See Also
Personality Traits, Effects on Relationships; Interpersonal Influence; Interpersonal
Attraction
Further Readings
Carson, R. (1969). Interaction concepts of personality. Chicago: Aldine.
Markey, P. M., & Markey, C. N. (2007). Romantic ideals, romantic obtainment and
relationship experiences: The complementarity of interpersonal traits among
romantic partners. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 24, 517-534.
Sadler, P., & Woody, E. (2003). Is who you are who you you’re talking to? Interpersonal
style and complementarity in mixed-sex interactions. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 84, 80 – 96.
Tracey, T. J. (2004). Levels of interpersonal complementarity: A simplex representation.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30 (9), 1211 – 1225.
... To answer this question, we first consider the possibility that congruence between partners' sexual preferences fosters satisfying sexual encounters. The appeal of this approach is evident in the long-standing interest in the roles of similarity and complementarity in close relationships (Markey & Markey, 2009;Montoya, Horton, & Kirchner, 2008). A different perspective proposes that sexual satisfaction is a consequence of motivated perception of the sexual relationship. ...
... Winch, Ktsanes, and Ktsanes (1954) defined complementarity as a match on characteristics that are different in kind yet interrelated, such that when combined, the need or goal underlying each characteristic can be gratified (e.g., one partner is dominant and the other is submissive). Winch et al.'s formulations have received little support, with reviews of the literature concluding that complementarity plays little role in mate selection or relationship satisfaction (Markey & Markey, 2009). ...
... Moreover, when the 10-item version of the similarity index (computed using the same 10 items comprised by the complementarity index) was substituted for the 29-item version of the similarity index, complementarity retained its significant effect on sexual satisfaction and the effect of similarity dropped to near-zero. This result provides a novel contribution to the existing literature in which there is little evidence for complementarity in couples, much less any role of complementarity in predicting satisfaction (Markey & Markey, 2009;White & Hatcher, 1984). Why might complementarity be influential in the sexual domain but not in other aspects of couples lives? ...
Article
Full-text available
We examined whether sexual similarity, complementarity, and perceptual accuracy exist within a large sample of heterosexual couples. Partners exhibited similarity and complementarity of sexual preferences, and they perceived each other's preferences with a considerable degree of accuracy; these effects were greater than found in randomly assigned pseudocouples. In addition, people overperceived sexual similarity and complementarity, and they overperceived the accuracy with which their partner knew their preferences. We suggest these three forms of overperception are the result of motivated cognitive processes in service of sexual relationship maintenance. Using actor-partner interdependence modeling (Kenny, Kashy, & Cook, 2006), overperception predicted sexual satisfaction, as did complementarity, but similarity and accuracy did not. These findings indicate that motivated cognition may foster sexual satisfaction, as theories of motivated relationship cognitions suggest.
... If their desires are similar or complementary, their sex life presumably will have more of what they each want and less of what they do not want. The longstanding interest in couple similarity and complementarity makes this explanation compelling (e.g., Karney & Bradbury, 1995;Markey & Markey, 2009). However, perhaps their sexual satisfaction is a product of motivated beliefs about their sex life, just as other beliefs about their relationship may reflect the need to feel securely connected to each other (Murray, 1999). ...
... By this approach, someone who tends to be dominant should get along well with a submissive partner, as has been found in heterosexual and lesbian couples (e.g., Markey & Markey, 2013). However, beyond the traits of dominance and submission, this formulation of complementarity has found little support, and literature reviews often conclude that complementarity plays little role in mate choice or relationship satisfaction (e.g., Markey & Markey, 2009). ...
Article
We examined whether similarity, complementarity, accuracy, and positive illusions exist within the sex lives of same-sex romantic couples. Partners had similar and complementary sexual desires and they perceived each other's desires with considerable accuracy; these effects were greater than in randomly matched pseudocouples. As evidence of positive sexual illusions, people overperceived sexual similarity and complementarity, and they overperceived the accuracy with which their partner knew their desires. Using actor-partner interdependence models (D. A. Kenny, D. A. Kashy, & W. L. Cook, 2006), similarity, complementarity, and positive illusions predicted sexual satisfaction, but a partner's actual accuracy did not. In parallel with earlier findings from heterosexual couples, this work indicates that positive sexual illusions are motivated cognitive processes that benefit sexual satisfaction, as theories of relationship maintenance suggest.
... When the interpersonal style of one member of the dyad elicits the interpersonal style of the other member and vice versa, they are said to have complementary interpersonal styles. There are many definitions of complementarity (Markey & Markey, 2009b). However, this study defines complementarity in a manner consistent with interpersonal theory (Carson 1969;see Figure 3; Leary, 1957). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
This study examined the importance of interpersonal styles and complementarity as predictors of friendship quality among same-sex friends. One-hundred and twenty-eight women (64 friendship dyads) and eighty men (40 friendship dyads) each completed interpersonal circumplex ratings of their friend and a measure of friendship quality. Using actor-partner interdependence models, it was found that dominant individuals tended to report high levels of friendship quality. Further, female friends tended to be similar in warmth, while male friends were not. However, contrary to previous research, complementarity was not a significant predictor of friendship quality. Results are discussed in terms of how friendships might differ from other types of relationships.
... The results indicated that, in the case of perceived ideal similarity, the actor effects were statistically significant for both partners, on all five dimensions, while the partner effect was significant only for four dimensions (intimacy/loyalty being the exception). These results are consistent with other studies showing that similarity (of personality, facial traits etc.) influences relationship satisfaction (Lutz-Zois, Bradley, Mihalik & Moorman-Eavers, 2006) and the fact that ideal similarity (although not measured with the ideal scales of Fletcher et al., 1999) has an influence on relationship satisfaction (Acitelli et al., 2001;Markey & Markey, 2009). The present study extends on previous knowledge by incorporating five ideal dimensions considered to be relevant from an evolutionary point of view (warmth/trustworthiness, vitality/attractiveness, status/resources, intimacy/loyalty, and passion) and by considering not only the influence of similarity on own relationship satisfaction, but also on partner relationship satisfaction. ...
Article
Full-text available
Recenzie-Thomas Piketty, Le capital au XXIe siècle, Éditions du Seuil, Paris, 2013
... The results indicated that, in the case of perceived ideal similarity, the actor effects were statistically significant for both partners, on all five dimensions, while the partner effect was significant only for four dimensions (intimacy/loyalty being the exception). These results are consistent with other studies showing that similarity (of personality, facial traits etc.) influences relationship satisfaction (Lutz-Zois, Bradley, Mihalik & Moorman-Eavers, 2006) and the fact that ideal similarity (although not measured with the ideal scales of Fletcher et al., 1999) has an influence on relationship satisfaction (Acitelli et al., 2001;Markey & Markey, 2009). The present study extends on previous knowledge by incorporating five ideal dimensions considered to be relevant from an evolutionary point of view (warmth/trustworthiness, vitality/attractiveness, status/resources, intimacy/loyalty, and passion) and by considering not only the influence of similarity on own relationship satisfaction, but also on partner relationship satisfaction. ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent research and theory suggest that people use ideal standards (organized around five major dimensions: warmth/trustworthiness, vitality/attractiveness, status/resource, intimacy loyalty, and passion) to evaluate their intimate partner and relationships. Guided by the ideal standards model (Simpson, Fletcher & Campbell, 2001), we examined actor and partner effects of perceived ideal similarity and communication of ideals on relationship satisfaction, using the actor‑partner interdependence model (APIM and APIMeM) and data from a sample of 100 couples. The results indicated that perceived ideal similarity predicted the individual's own relationship satisfaction (all dimensions), and the relationship satisfaction of his or her partner (four dimensions: warmth/ trustworthiness, vitality/attractiveness, status/resources, and passion). In the case of communication of ideals, the partner effect was statistically significant for all dimensions, while the actor effect was significant for three dimensions: status/resources, intimacy/loyalty, and passion. Moreover, we tested whether communication of ideals mediate the effect of perceived ideal similarity on relationship satisfaction. We found support for the full mediation hypothesis for the warmth/trustworthiness and status/resources dimensions. In dyadic data analysis we assume data interdependence, due to the fact, that in a close relationship, partners influence each other's cognitions, emotions, and behaviors. This idea is underlined in Ideals Standards Model, which analyse the content and functions of partner and relationship ideals in intimate relationships. Campbell, Overall, Rubin and Lackenbauer (2013) found that people are more satisfied not only when they perceive their partners as closely matching their own ideal standards (an actor effect), but also when they match the ideal standards held by their partner (a partner effect). Guided by the ideal standards model (Simpson et al., 2001), we examined actor and partner effects of perceived ideal similarity and communication of ideals on relationship satisfaction. Firstly, we assessed the relationship between similarity of ideals and relationship satisfaction, using the Actor‑Partner Interdependence Model (APIM), and secondly we
... The concept of the interpersonal circle has had significant successes in accurately examining interpersonal relating (for reviews see Hatcher & Rogers, 2009;Horowitz, Turan, Wilson, & Zolotsev, 2008) and the validity of the scales for measuring the "Dimensions of the Interpersonal circle" have been well tested across a variety of domains (Ansell, Kurtz & DeMoor, 2011). It has been replicated across a range of interpersonal dynamics including traditional relationships such as sexual partners (Markey & Markey, 2009), interpersonal values (Locke, 2000) and interpersonal problems (Horowitz, Rosenburg, & Bartholomew, 1993). It has also been applied to criminal interpersonal dynamics such as tactics used by child sex offenders to secure and manipulate victims (Bennell, Alison, Stein, Alison and Canter, 2001), behavioral coherence in group robbery (Porter & Alison, 2006), and group sex offense behavior (Porter & Alison, 2004). ...
Article
Full-text available
Principal component analysis of an operational field sample of 181 police interrogations with terrorist suspects identified five counter interrogation factors: passive (refusing to look at interviewers, remaining silent); passive verbal (monosyllabic response, claiming lack of memory); verbal (discussing an unrelated topic, providing well known information, providing a scripted response) with two single item components: retraction of previous statements and no comment. Analysis revealed significant differences in the use of counter interrogation tactics between terrorist groups, with paramilitary suspects using more passive, verbal and no comment tactics than right wing and international terrorists. International terrorists made significantly more use of retraction tactics than right wing and paramilitary groups.
... In 1. Although there are many definitions of complementarity (Markey & Markey, 2009b), the current research defines complementarity in a manner consistent with interpersonal theory (Carson, 1969;Kiesler, 1983;Leary, 1957). This definition allows complementary behaviors to potentially be opposite (e.g., a dominant behavior might complement a submissive behavior) or similar (e.g., a warm behavior might complement a warm behavior). ...
Article
This study extended previous research on complementarity through the examination of female same‐gender romantic dyads. One‐hundred and forty‐four women (72 couples) completed interpersonal circumplex ratings of their romantic partner and a relationship measure of love and harmony. Results indicated that high levels of relationship quality were reported by participants who were warm and submissive or who had partners who possessed these characteristics. Additionally, members of female same‐gender couples tended to complement each other in terms of dominance but not warmth. However, consistent with past research stressing the unique importance female same‐gender couples tend to prescribe to relationship equality, dyads that contained members who were equivalent in terms of dominance tended to experience high levels of relationship quality.
... The concept of the interpersonal circle has had significant successes in accurately examining interpersonal relating (for reviews see Hatcher & Rogers, 2009;Horowitz, Turan, Wilson, & Zolotsev, 2008) and the validity of the scales for measuring the "Dimensions of the Interpersonal circle" have been well tested across a variety of domains (Ansell, Kurtz & DeMoor, 2011). It has been replicated across a range of interpersonal dynamics including traditional relationships such as sexual partners (Markey & Markey, 2009), interpersonal values (Locke, 2000) and interpersonal problems (Horowitz, Rosenburg, & Bartholomew, 1993). It has also been applied to criminal interpersonal dynamics such as tactics used by child sex offenders to secure and manipulate victims (Bennell, Alison, Stein, Alison and Canter, 2001), behavioral coherence in group robbery (Porter & Alison, 2006), and group sex offense behavior (Porter & Alison, 2004). ...
Article
Full-text available
This field observation examines 58 police interrogators’ rapport-based behaviors with terrorist suspects; specifically, whether rapport helps elicit meaningful intelligence and information. The Observing Rapport-Based Interpersonal Techniques (ORBIT; Alison, Alison, Elntib & Noone, 2012) is a coding framework with 3 elements. The first 2 measures are as follows: (i) 5 strategies adopted from the motivational interviewing (Miller & Rollnick, 2009) literature in the counseling domain: autonomy, acceptance, adaptation, empathy, and evocation and (ii) an “Interpersonal Behavior Circle” (adopted from Interpersonal theories, Leary, 1957) for coding interpersonal interactions between interrogator and suspect along 2 orthogonal dimensions (authoritative-passive and challenging-cooperative); where each quadrant has an interpersonally adaptive and maladaptive variant. The third (outcome) measure of ORBIT includes a measure of evidentially useful information (the “interview yield”) and considers the extent to which suspects reveal information pertaining to capability, opportunity and motive as well as evidence relevant to people, actions, locations and times. Data included 418 video interviews (representing 288 hours of footage), with all suspects subsequently convicted for a variety of terrorist offenses. Structural equation modeling revealed that motivational interviewing was positively associated with adaptive interpersonal behavior from the suspect, which, in turn, increased interview yield. Conversely, even minimal expression of maladaptive interpersonal interrogator behavior increased maladaptive interviewee behavior as well as directly reducing yield. The study provides the first well-defined and empirically validated analysis of the benefits of a rapport-based, interpersonally skilled approach to interviewing terrorists in an operational field setting. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved)
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any references for this publication.